Skip to main content
Maternal & Child Nutrition logoLink to Maternal & Child Nutrition
. 2020 Feb 12;16(3):e12967. doi: 10.1111/mcn.12967

Household food insecurity and early childhood development: Systematic review and meta‐analysis

Klébya Hellen Dantas de Oliveira 1,, Géssica Mercia de Almeida 1, Muriel Bauermann Gubert 1, Amanda Souza Moura 1, Ana Maria Spaniol 1, Daphne C Hernandez 2, Rafael Pérez‐Escamilla 3, Gabriela Buccini 3
PMCID: PMC7296813  PMID: 32052571

Abstract

Household food insecurity (HFI) is a powerful stressor negatively associated with early childhood development (ECD). However, no comprehensive review has examined the association of HFI and ECD. Therefore, this systematic review and meta‐analysis investigated the association between HFI and ECD domains and subdomains in children under 5 years old. Peer‐reviewed and grey literature were systematically searched in electronic databases with no year or language restrictions. Studies were eligible if they assessed the association between HFI and one or more ECD domains. Data were extracted using a standard predefined protocol. Meta‐analysis was performed, and the heterogeneity across studies was explored. Nineteen studies were included in the systematic review and 14 in the meta‐analysis. Of the studies, 15 were from high income countries (HICs) and four from low–middle income countries (LMICs). For developmental risk and the cognitive/math and cognitive/school readiness and reading subdomains, the only studies available were conducted in HICs. The meta‐analysis showed that HFI was associated with developmental risk (OR 1.28; 95% CI [1.14, 1.45]), cognitive/vocabulary (OR 0.94; 95% CI [0.90, 0.98]), and cognitive/math (OR 0.84; 95% CI [0.73, 0.96]). HFI was marginally associated with cognitive/school readiness and reading (OR 0.91; 95% CI [0.82, 1.00]) and motor development (OR; 0.91, 95% CI [0.80, 1.04]). HFI was associated with poor ECD in children under 5 years old. Specifically, HFI was associated with developmental risk and poor math skills in studies conducted in HICs and with poor vocabulary skills in studies conducted in both HICs and LMICs. Prospective studies examining HFI and ECD are needed in LMICs.

Keywords: early childhood development, high and low–middle income countries, household food insecurity, infant, meta‐analysis, systematic review, toddlers


Key messages

  • Household food insecurity (HFI) is associated with poor early childhood development (ECD) in children under 5 years old.

  • HFI is associated with developmental risk and poor math skills in high income countries (HICs) and with poor vocabulary skills in both HICs and low–middle income countries (LMICs).

  • Standardized methods to assess ECD and clear definitions of ECD domains are necessary to allow global‐level comparability.

1. INTRODUCTION

Adverse childhood experiences, such as poverty, food insecurity, family stress, neglect, and racial disparities, are risk factors for suboptimal early childhood development (ECD) (Knowles, Rabinowich, Ettinger de Cuba, Cutts, & Chilton, 2016). Specifically, studies have found that household food insecurity (HFI) is a powerful stressor with important implications for cognitive—including vocabulary, memory, attention, problem‐solving, analytical, and academic skills (Hobbs & King, 2018; Johnson & Markowitz, 2018); language—including comprehensive and expressive verbal communication (Saha et al., 2010); motor—including control and coordination of gross and fine movements (Hernandez & Jacknowitz, 2009; Milner, Fiorella, Mattah, Bukusi, & Fernald, 2018); socio‐emotional—including the abilities to regulate emotional responses, behavioural problems, and social interactions (Gill, Koleilat, & Whaley, 2018; Hobbs & King, 2018; Milner et al., 2018; Whitaker, Phillips, & Orzol, 2006); and developmental risk—including poor performance in non‐specific developmental domains (Black et al., 2012; Rose‐Jacobs et al., 2008).

HFI is defined as the lack of physical, social, and economic access to sufficient, safe, and nutritious food to meet the dietary needs and food preferences for an active and healthy life (Food and Agriculture Organization, 2013). HFI can be experienced at different levels of severity (mild, moderate, or severe), and more severe and longer exposure to HFI leads to higher odds that HFI will negatively affect ECD outcomes (Jyoti, Frongillo, & Jones, 2005).

Previous reviews have reported the association of HFI with poor ECD (Cook & Frank, 2008; Pérez‐Escamilla & Vianna, 2012; Shankar, Chung, & Frank, 2017). Cook and Frank (2008) showed that HFI was associated with developmental risk and negative effects on physical function, academic performance, and impaired social skill development in the United States (US) preschool‐aged and school‐aged children. Pérez‐Escamilla and Vianna (2012) described that in the US, HFI was associated with poor performance in psycho‐emotional, social, and academic indicators, beginning in the early years and through adolescence. Shankar et al. (2017) found in high income countries (HICs) that even marginal HFI was associated with adverse ECD outcomes, including developmental risk and impaired cognitive outcomes in infants and toddlers, as well as with externalizing and internalizing behaviours among preschoolers, school‐aged children, and adolescents.

These previous reviews were restricted to HICs, and the results focused on long‐term outcomes throughout childhood and adolescence (Cook & Frank, 2008; Pérez‐Escamilla & Vianna, 2012; Shankar et al., 2017). These limitations called for conducting a comprehensive review to capture the entire body of evidence in HICs and low–middle income countries (LMICs) as well as to explore the association of HFI and ECD outcomes by ECD domains and subdomains, that is, cognitive (executive function, school readiness, reading, vocabulary, and math), language (communication and language comprehension and expression), motor (fine and gross motor), socio‐emotional (externalizing behaviour and hyperactivity and internalizing behaviour and anxiety) and developmental risk.

Therefore, the aim of this systematic review and meta‐analysis was to investigate the association between HFI and ECD domains and subdomains among children under 5 years old in HICs and LMICs.

2. METHODS

The protocol for this systematic review and meta‐analysis was registered on PROSPERO prior to starting the literature search (CRD42018091435).

2.1. Eligibility criteria

This systematic review followed the guidance of the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta‐Analysis (PRISMA; Moher, Liberati, Tetzlaff, Altman, & Group, 2009). A literature search protocol was prepared on the basis of clearly defined a priori inclusion and exclusion criteria.

Studies evaluating the association between HFI and ECD in infants and toddlers under 5 years old were included. Studies were excluded if they were (a) qualitative or (b) reviews (systematic or not), letters to the editor, books, conference abstracts, study protocols and (c) studies that did not report the association between HFI and ECD. Studies were eligible if they (a) reported the association (coefficient β,and odds ratio) between ECD and HFI and (b) used an experience‐based food insecurity scale to assess HFI (Pérez‐Escamilla, Gubert, Rogers, & Hromi‐Fiedler, 2017). Measuring ECD is complex and consensus on the use of a single tool globally has not been reached. Because tools designed to measure ECD vary widely and may cover a single or multiple ECD domains (Fernald, Prado, Kariger, & Raikes, 2017), we chose to include all studies that measured any domains or subdomains of ECD, regardless of the tool used.

If two or more eligible studies used the same data, only one paper was included, which was selected either because it reported more thoroughly the data or was the most recent publication.

2.2. Exposure

The key exposure was HFI (vs. non‐HFI), assessed using experience‐based food insecurity scales (Pérez‐Escamilla et al., 2017). These scales (Household Food Security Survey Module (HFSSM), Core Food Security Module (CFSM), Household Food Insecurity Access Scale (HFIAS), and Household Food Security Scale (HHFS)) assess HFI based on the experience by the respondents of food deprivation, poor dietary quality, and corresponding coping skills in the household (Pérez‐Escamilla et al., 2017). These scales have been validated and are used in many countries worldwide to measure HFI (Marques, Reichenheim, de Moraes, Antunes, & Salles‐Costa, 2015; Pérez‐Escamilla et al., 2017; Saha et al., 2010).

2.3. Outcome

There is no consensus on the definition of early childhood. The World Health Organization defines early childhood as ages 0–8 years (World Health Organization, 2019). The American Academy of Pediatrics divides early childhood into ages 0–1 (infants), 1–3 (toddlers), and 3–5 (preschoolers) (American Academy of Pediatrics, 2019). Recent publications emphasize the importance of ECD among children under 3 years (Black et al., 2017). In this systematic review, the outcome was defined as ECD in children under 5 years old.

The ECD outcome was classified into five domains based on the conceptual definitions reported by Fernald et al. (2017): (a) cognitive (including five subdomains: cognitive scores, executive function, school readiness and reading, vocabulary, and math), (b) socio‐emotional (including four subdomains: externalizing behaviour, externalizing behaviour/hyperactivity, internalizing behaviour, and internalizing behaviour/anxiety), (c) language (single domain including comprehensive and expressive verbal communication skills), (d) motor (single domain including fine and gross motor skills), and (e) developmental risk (developmental concerns in non‐specific developmental domains).

Studies were allocated into the predefined domains and subdomains, following a consensus process followed by two study co‐authors (MBG and GB). First, the definition of each domain and subdomain was recorded, according to the literature. Then, the studies were allocated into the corresponding domains or subdomains, according to the description reported by the authors. The same study could be allocated to more than one subdomain if results for additional subdomains were available. We chose not to separate fine and gross motor skills into subdomains because only one study made this distinction. The definitions of domains, subdomains, and description according to what was reported in each study are presented in Table 1.

Table 1.

Early childhood development domain definitions and subdomains classification

ECD domain ECD subdomain Study identification ECD outcome Definition reported by the study Tool used to assess ECD outcome

Developmental risk

Developmental risk was defined when the delay was not specific per domain and the evaluation was performed by a global screening test.

Black et al. (2012) Developmental risk Developmental risk was defined as having one or more developmental concerns. Parents' Evaluation of Developmental Status
Rose‐Jacobs et al. (2008) Developmental risk Children with two or more significant concerns are at developmental risk. Parents' Evaluation of Developmental Status
Drennen et al. (2019) Developmental risk Children with two or more concerns were classified as at developmental risk. Parents' Evaluation of Developmental Status

Socio‐emotionalb

Social–emotional skills, temperament, and personal social/adaptive skills

Externalizing behaviour Hobbs & King (2018) Externalizing behaviour problems Externalizing behaviours directed toward others (e.g., aggressive behaviours, whether the child argued a lot, bullied, was disobedient, or destroyed things). Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL/1.5–5)
King (2018) Externalizing behaviour problems Externalizing behaviours (e.g., fights, hits others, or disobedience). Child Behavior Checklist– (CBCL/1.5‐5)
Whitaker et al. (2006) Aggressive Not reported Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL/1.5–5)
Nagata, Gomberg, Hagan, Heyman, & Wojcicki (2018) Oppositional defiant score Not reported Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL/1.5–5)
Gill et al. (2018) High‐frequency discipline Not reported “How many days in a typical week does NAME need to be disciplined for his/her behavior?” item adapted from the Home Observation Measurement of the Environment‐Short Form scale.
Johnson & Markowitz (2018) Conduct problems Conduct problems (e.g., how often child pushes or tantrums). Items drawn from the Preschool and Kindergarten Behavior Scales, 2nd ed. and the Social Skills Rating Scale
Finch, Yousafzai, Rasheed, & Obradović (2018) Externalizing behaviour problems Not reported Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire
Externalizing behaviour/hyperactivity Johnson & Markowitz (2018) Hyperactivity Hyperactivity (e.g., how well child pays attention, resists distraction, sits stilla). Items drawn from the Preschool and Kindergarten Behavior Scales, 2nd ed. and the Social Skills Rating Scale
Nagata et al. (2018) Attention deficit/hyperactivity Not reported Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL/1.5–5)
Whitaker et al. (2006) Inattention/hyperactivity Not reported Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL/1.5–5)
Internalizing behaviour/anxiety Whitaker et al. (2006) Anxious/depressed Not reported Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL/1.5–5)
Nagata et al. (2018) Anxiety Not reported Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL/1.5–5)
Internalizing behaviour Nagata et al. (2018) Affective score Not reported Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL/1.5–5)
Nagata, Gomberg, Hagan, Heyman, & Wojcicki (2018)a Pervasive developmental Pervasive developmental symptoms included mothers reporting that their children avoided eye contact, did not answer when called or when spoken to, showed little affection, had a speech problem, exhibited strange behaviour, or were upset by new situations. Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL/1.5–5)
Milner et al. (2018) Personal social Personal social reflects emotional responses and social interactions. Inventory (Ages and Stages Questionnaire: I)
King (2018) Internalizing behaviours Internalizing behaviours (e.g., withdrawn, shy, secretive, or refuses to talk). Child Behavior Checklist(CBCL/1.5‐5)
Hobbs & King (2018) Internalizing behaviours Internalizing behaviours are negative emotions directed toward self (e.g., whether the child worried, sulked a lot, was shy, or refused to talk). Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL/1.5–5)

Cognitiveb

Cognitive skills and abilities for the executive function/self‐regulation/effortful control, early academic skills, and approaches to learning.

Cognitive scores Hernandez & Jacknowitz (2009)a Cognitive scores Early cognitive and language ability by examining items about early communication skills, expressive and receptive vocabulary, listening comprehension, and early problem‐solving skills. Bayley Short Form‐Research Edition (BSF‐R) Mental Scale
Executive function Obradovíc, Yousafzai, Finch, & Rasheed (2016)a Executive function Executive function skills include inhibitory control (ability to suppress a dominant response in favour of a subdominant response), working memory (ability to hold, update, and manipulate information in the mind over short periods of time), and cognitive flexibility (ability to switch flexibly between two different dimensions). Executive function battery
Obradovíc, Yousafzai, Finch, & Rasheed (2016)a Performance Intelligence Quotient Performance intelligence (e.g., fluid reasoning, spatial processing, perceptual organization, and visual–motor integration). Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence III

School readiness and reading

Hobbs & King (2018) Woodcock–Johnson letter–word identification Not reported Woodcock–Johnson test of achievement letter–word identification subtest
Huang, Potochnick, & Heflin (2018) School readiness in reading assessment score Reading assessment (e.g., phonological awareness, letter recognition and sound knowledge, and word recognition). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test
Johnson & Markowitz (2018) Reading Reading ability evaluated letter and letter–sound knowledge, print conventions, and expressive and receptive vocabulary skills. Measure developed specifically for the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study‐Birth Cohort
Vocabulary Hobbs & King (2018) Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test Children's receptive vocabulary capabilities for standard English and academic readiness. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test–Revised
Obradovíc, Yousafzai, Finch, & Rasheed (2016) Verbal Intelligence Quotient Verbal intelligence (e.g., vocabulary, verbal comprehension, and knowledge about the world). Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence III
Math Huang et al., 2018 School readiness in math assessment score Math assessment (e.g., number sense, counting, operations, and geometry). Test of Early Mathematics Ability–3
Johnson & Markowitz (2018) Math Math skills assessed children's number sense, properties, operations, measurement, and geometry and spatial abilities. Measure developed for the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study‐Birth Cohort
Mickens et al. (2019) Early Mathematics Ability Informal and formal mathematical knowledge. Informal knowledge covers concepts that children learn outside of the context of formal schooling, whereas formal knowledge covers concepts that a child is taught in school. Test of Early Mathematics Ability (TEMA)

Motorb

Control and coordination of gross and fine movements skills

Hernandez & Jacknowitz (2009) Fine and gross motor Fine motor skills (e.g., reaching, grasping, and manipulating small objects) and gross motor skills (e.g., sitting, standing, walking, and balance). Motor skills scale adapted from the Bayley Scales of Infant Development, Second Edition
Milner et al. (2018) Gross motor Gross motor evaluates body and muscle movement, including tasks such as standing, walking, and balancing. Inventory (Ages and Stages Questionnaire: I)

Languageb

Comprehensive and expressive verbal communication skills

Milner, Fiorella, Mattah, Bukusi, & Fernald (2018)a Communication The communication assesses language development and the use of words or sounds to express feelings. Inventory (Ages and Stages Questionnaire: I)
Saha et al. (2010)a Language comprehension Children's ability to comprehend words in different categories (e.g., animals, body parts, or food). Bengali adaptation of MacArthur's Communicative Development Inventory
Saha et al. (2010)a Language expression Children's ability to express words in different categories (e.g., animals, body parts, or food). Bengali adaptation of MacArthur's Communicative Development Inventory

Abbreviation: ECD, early childhood development.

a

Hernandez & Jacknowitz, 2009 (evaluating cognitive scores), Obradovíc et al., 2016 (evaluating executive function and performance intelligence quotient), Nagata et al., 2018 (evaluating pervasive developmental), Milner et al., 2018 (evaluating communication), and Saha et al., 2010 (evaluating language comprehension and language expression) could not be grouped in the ECD domains due to the ECD definition.

b

The four main ECD domains were adapted according the proposed by Fernald et al. (2017).

2.4. Search strategy

Seven electronic bibliographic databases (Medline/PubMed, LILACS, Scopus, Web of Science, Science Direct, EMBASE, and PsycINFO) were systematically searched. In addition, grey literature was searched for on Google Scholar and Open Grey. The search on Google Scholar was limited to the first 400 records, as recommended by previous authors (Pizato, Botelho, Gonçalves, Dutra, & de Carvalho, 2017). All searches were conducted without language or date restrictions, through April 20, 2018, and the search was updated on November 13, 2019. Reference lists of eligible articles were read to identify additional studies related to the topic. Co‐authors (DCH, MBG, RPE, and GB)) were consulted on the inclusion of additional studies that were not captured by the systematic search. This procedure was used to ensure that relevant studies were not left out of the review.

Articles were identified by using the following terms: infant, toddler, food insecurity, food security, and ECD. Descriptors for these terms were identified in English, Portuguese, and Spanish from the Medical Subject Headings (MeSH) terms (Data S1). The searches were conducted independently by two co‐authors (KHDO and GMA). The studies identified from each search were imported into EndNote X8 (Thomson Reuters 2018) prior to removing duplicates.

2.5. Study selection

Articles were excluded if they did not meet the aforementioned criteria (Data S2). Two co‐authors (KHDO and GMA) independently screened the titles and abstracts. Studies were included in the initial screening if they examined the association between HFI and ECD. Next, the articles were independently assessed for eligibility using the predefined inclusion and exclusion criteria defined above. Disagreements were resolved through consensus, or when required, through consultation with a third co‐author (ASM).

2.6. Data extraction

Data were extracted independently by two co‐authors (KHDO and GMA). The data extraction form included study identification (author, year of publication, and country of study, country's income level), name of the primary study and year of data collection, study design and sample size, child's age, HFI (status, tool, child's age when measured, and prevalence), ECD (tool, child's age when measured, and outcomes), effect size of the association between ECD and HFI (odds ratio or coefficient β with respective 95% confidence interval—adjusted or crude), and confounders. When necessary, authors were contacted to provide missing information or additional data to calculate the OR and CI of the association between HFI and ECD.

2.7. Quality assessment of studies

Quality assessment was conducted by reviewing key study design and methodological attributes based on a checklist adapted from the Effective Public Health Project Practice Quality Assessment Tool checklist (“http://www.ephpp.ca/tools.html”) (Armijo‐Olivo, Stiles, Hagen, Biondo, & Cummings, 2012). This quality assessment tool has been used and adapted by other systematic reviews in the maternal and child nutrition field with reliable and consistent results regarding studies quality assessment (Boccolini, Carvalho, & Oliveira, 2015; Buccini, Pérez‐Escamilla, Paulino, Araujo, & Venancio, 2017).

The following six attributes were classified as either “strong,” “moderate,” or “weak”: (a) selection bias, (b) study design, (c) confounding factors, (d) blinding, (e) data collection methods, and (f) withdrawals and dropouts. The questions to assess the following three attributes—confounding factors, blinding, and withdrawals and dropouts—were adapted to capture the quality of observational studies. Confounding factors assessed if there were or not significant differences between HFI group versus household food security (examples of confounders include race, sex, age, and health status). Blinding assessed if the interviewer assessing the ECD outcome was aware of the HFI status of participants. Withdrawals and dropouts assessed if follow‐up losses (in longitudinal studies) or missing data were reported. A roadmap for scoring the six attributes (a–f) is described in Data S3. Study quality was upgraded or downgraded based on the internal validity of the studies. The studies were classified according to the final score of Effective Public Health Project Practice Quality Assessment Tool as “strong quality” if none attribute was weak; “moderate quality” if one of the six attributes was classified as weak; and “weak quality” for studies with more than one attribute classified as weak (Armijo‐Olivo et al., 2012). The original and adapted checklist questions are presented in Data S3.

2.8. Data analysis

2.8.1. Effect size

Effect measures were presented as pooled odds ratios. For studies that summarized the effect size with estimators other than ORs, whenever possible, the estimators were converted to ORs, and the 95% CI was calculated (Deeks, Altman, & Bradburn, 2008). For four studies, the conversion to OR was not possible due to lack of information; therefore, they were excluded from the meta‐analysis (Fuller et al., 2002; Greder, Peng, Doudna, & Sarver, 2017; King, 2018; Zaslow et al., 2009). When adjusted estimates were available, they were included; otherwise, crude estimators were considered (Gill et al., 2018; Obradovíc et al., 2016).

When studies reported two or more ECD outcomes, the findings were included in the meta‐analysis separately, by ECD domain and subdomain. If more than one ECD outcome per domain was presented in the same study, they were grouped according to predefined ECD domains and subdomains definitions. Domain definitions as well as how each study outcome were defined and grouped within subdomains are presented in Table 1.

2.8.2. Meta‐analysis

Effect measures from the meta‐analysis were presented as pooled odds ratios (ORs) by ECD domain (developmental risk and motor) and, when appropriate, by subdomains (cognitive and socio‐emotional). The cognitive domain was represented by three subdomains: cognitive/school readiness and reading, cognitive/math, and cognitive/vocabulary. The socio‐emotional domain was represented by four subdomains: internalizing behaviour, internalizing behaviour/anxiety, externalizing behaviour, and externalizing behaviour/hyperactivity. The language domain was excluded from the meta‐analysis because the two studies identified assessed different aspects of language and could not be combined in a single subdomain.

To interpret meta‐analysis results, high scores for developmental risk and socio‐emotional outcomes (internalizing behaviour, internalizing behaviour/anxiety, externalizing behaviour, and externalizing behaviour/hyperactivity) indicate poor development. On the other hand, low scores for cognition and motor development domains reflect poor development.

Heterogeneity was assessed by the degree of inconsistency (I2 statistic) and its significance (p < .05), using a fixed effect model. If high heterogeneity was found (i.e., I2 > 50%) random‐effects models were used for the analysis (Deeks et al., 2008). Random‐effects models take into account the variability of effects between different studies (Deeks et al., 2008). Meta‐regression was considered for studies with high heterogeneity (i.e., I2 > 50%); however, due to the relatively low number of studies, we were unable to follow this approach. Instead, we conducted a descriptive analysis of possible sources of heterogeneity. Results were not broken down by country income due to the scarcity of studies conducted in LMICs. This decision is unlikely to have biased our results because this source of heterogeneity was considered in the interpretation of the findings, and findings when just including HICs were similar as when also including LMICs. All analyses were conducted using Stata version 14.1 (Stata Corp., College Station, TX, USA).

3. RESULTS

3.1. Characteristics of studies

A total of 3,706 publications were identified in the initial search. After excluding 414 duplicates, a total of 3,292 publications were screened for exclusion criteria by title and abstracts. This resulted in 57 publications for full text reading, including seven from the search update (November 13, 2019). Of those, 38 studies did not meet the eligibility criteria and were excluded. The reasons for exclusion are described in Data S2. Thus, 19 studies were included in this systematic review, and 14 studies were included in the meta‐analysis (Figure 1). In the quality assessment, five studies were classified as weak, eight as moderate and six as strong. Figure 2 indicates that 31.6% of the studies had strong quality. The quality attributes examined that were the weakest were confounding factors followed by selection bias, study design, and withdrawals and dropout.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta‐Analysis flow diagram of systematic review on household food insecurity and early childhood development

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Summary of the risk of bias of the studies included in the systematic review based on the checklist “Effective Public Health Practice Project Quality Assessment Tool”

Table 2 summarizes the main characteristics of the studies included in the systematic review. The systematic review included 15 studies from the US (78.9% of the studies). Only four studies were conducted in LMICs: Kenya (Milner et al., 2018), Pakistan (Finch et al., 2018; Obradovíc et al., 2016), and Bangladesh (Saha et al., 2010). In the majority of studies, HFI and ECD were measured at the same time or in an interval up to 24 months, and six studies did not present this information (Black et al., 2012; Drennen et al., 2019; Gill et al., 2018; Greder et al., 2017; Hobbs & King, 2018; Mickens, 2019). Information regarding HFI and ECD status was usually reported by the mothers. All HFI scales probed for HFI in the context of food deprivation due to lack of money. All scales, but one (HHFS), were directly adapted from the Household Food Security Survey Module(HFSSM).

Table 2.

General characteristics of the studies that evaluated the association between household food insecurity and early childhood development by domains

Study identification

Country's income levele

Name of the primary study

Year of data collection

Study design

Sample sizeg (n)

Quality score Child's age HFI ECD
Status

Tools

Child's age when measured

Tools

Child's age when measured

Developmental riskd

Drennen et al. (2019) United States

High income

Children's HealthWatch

2009–2017

Cross‐sectional

28,184

Weak 4–48 months HFI = low and very low FS

18‐item HFSSM

N/R

PEDS

N/R

Black et al. (2012) United States

High income

Children's HealthWatch

2000–2010

Cross‐sectional

26,950

Weak 4–36 months HFI = low and very low FS

18‐item CFSM

N/R

PEDS

N/R

Rose‐Jacobs et al. (2008) United States

High income

Children's HealthWatch

2004–2005

Cross‐sectional

2,010

Weak 4–36 months HFI = low and very low FS HFI with hunger

18‐item CFSM

4–36 months

PEDS

4–36 months

Socio‐emotionald

Finch et al. (2018) Pakistan

Low–middle income

PEDS trial

2011–2012 to 2013–2014a

Longitudinal

1,302

Strong 24 and 48 months HFI

HFIAS

24 months and 48 months

SDQ 48months. BSID‐III 24months

Gill et al. (2018) United States

High income

WIC

2014

Cross‐sectional

4,125

Weak 6 months–5 years HFI = low and very low FS

6‐item HFSSM

N/R

1 question (adapted from the HOMES‐SF scales) N/R

Hobbs & King (2018) United States

High income

FFCWS

2003 and 2006 1

Longitudinalf

2,046

Moderate 5 years HFI = low and very low FS

18‐item HFSSM

N/R

CBCL N/R

Johnson & Markowitz (2018) United States

High income

ECLS‐B

2001, 2003, and 2005‐2006

Longitudinal

3,700

Strong 9, 24, and 57b months HFI = low and very low FS

18‐item CFSM

9 months, 2 years, and preschool

BSF‐R. Preschool and Kindergarten Behavior Scales Social Skills Rating Scale

9 months, 2 years, and preschool

King (2018) United States

High income

FFCWS

2003 and 2005a

Longitudinal

2,044

Strong 3 and 5 years HFI

18‐item HFSSM

3 and 5 yearsc

CBCL

3 and 5 yearsc

Milner et al. (2018) Kenya

Low–middle income

Panel study on fishing livelihoods, fish consumption, and child nutrition

2012–2015

Longitudinal

304

Moderate <2 years HFI Timing HFI Intensity HFI Duration HFI

9‐item HFIAS

0–3–6–9–12–15–18–21–24 months

Inventory ASQ:I

6–12–18–24 months

Nagata et al. (2018) United States

High income

Hispanic eating and nutrition cohort

2010–2011 to 2012–2013a

Longitudinalf

168

Moderate 4 and 5 years HFI

18‐item HFSSM

4 year

CBCL

5 years

Greder et al. (2017) United States

High income

RFSH

2011 and 2012

Longitudinalf

175

Moderate 18–60 months HFI

6‐item HFSSM

N/R

CBCL

N/R

Zaslow et al. (2009) United States

High income

ECLS‐B

2001–2002 and 2003–2004

Longitudinalf

8,944

Moderate 9 and 24 months HFI = very low FS

18‐item HFSSM

9 months

TAS‐45

24 months

Whitaker et al. (2006) United States High income

FFCWS

2001–2003a

Longitudinalf

2,870

Strong 3 years HFI = low and very low FS

10‐item HFSSM

3 years

CBCL

3 years

Fuller et al. (2002) United States

High income

1998

Longitudinalf

405

Strong 24–42 months FS and hunger index

12‐item CFSM

9, 24, and 48 monthsc

CBCL (adapted)

9, 24, and 48 monthsc

Cognitived

Mickens et al. (2019)

United States

High income

LSCA

2010–2011

Longitudinal

792

Moderate 3–5 years HFI = low and very low FS

8‐item HFSSM

N/R

TEMA

N/R

Hobbs & King (2018)

United States

High income

FFCWS

2003 and 2006 1

Longitudinalf

2,046

Moderate 5 years HFI = low and very low FS

18‐item HFSSM

N/R

PPVT (revised) Woodcock–Johnson test of achievement letter–word identification subtest

N/R

Huang et al. (2018) United States

High income

ECLS‐B

2005–2006a

Longitudinalf

8,900

Moderate 48 months HFI = low and very low FS

18‐item CFSM

48 months

PPVT Test of Early Mathematics Ability‐3

48 months

Johnson & Markowitz (2018) United States

High income

ECLS‐B

2003, and 2005–2006

Longitudinal

3,700

Strong 9, 24, and 57b months HFI = low and very low FS

18‐item CFSM

9 months, 2 years, and preschool

BSF‐R

9 months, 2 years, and preschool

Obradovíc, Yousafzai, Finch, & Rasheed (2016) Pakistan

Low–middle income

PEDS trial

2011–2012 to 2013–2014a

Longitudinalf

8,944

Moderate 24–48 months HFI

HFIAS

24 months

WPPSI‐III inhibitory control task working memory task cognitive flexibility task

48 months

Hernandez & Jacknowitz (2009)

United States

High income

ECLS‐B

2001 and 2003

Longitudinal

7,900

Strong 9 and 24 months HFI = low, very low, and marginally FS Persistent adult HFI, Transitional adult HFI

10‐item HFSSM

9 and 24 months

BSF‐R

9 and 24 months

Zaslow et al. (2009) United States

High income

ECLS‐B

2001–2002 and 2003–2004

Longitudinalf

8,944

Moderate 9 and 24 months HFI = very low FS

18‐item HFSSM

9 months

BSF‐R

24 months

Motord

Milner et al. (2018) Kenya

Low–middle income

Panel study on fishing livelihoods, fish consumption, and child nutrition

2012 to 2015

Longitudinal

304

Moderate <2 years HFI Timing HFI Intensity HFI Duration HFI

9‐item HFIAS

0–3–6‐9–12–15–18–21–24 months

Inventory ASQ: I

6–12–18–24 months

Hernandez & Jacknowitz (2009) United States

High income

ECLS‐B

2001 and 2003

Longitudinal

7,900

Strong 9 and 24 months HFI = low, very low and marginally FS Persistent adult HFI, Transitional adult HFI

10‐item HFSSM

9 and 24 months

BSF‐R

9 and 24 months

Languaged

Milner et al. (2018) Kenya

Low–middle income

Panel study on fishing livelihoods, fish consumption, and child nutrition

2012–2015

Longitudinal

304

Moderate <2 years HFI Timing HFI Intensity HFI Duration HFI

9‐item HFIAS

0–3–6–9–12–15–18–21–24 months

Inventory ASQ:I

6–12–18–24 months

Saha et al. (2010) Bangladesh

Low–middle income

MINIMat

2002 and 2003

Longitudinalf

1,639

Weak 18 months HFI 11‐item HHFS 8 and 30 weeks of pregnancy

MacArthur's CDI (adaptation)

18 months

Abbreviations: ASQ, Ages and Stages Questionnaire; BSF‐R, Bayley Short Form‐Research Edition; BSID‐III, Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development, Third Edition BSID‐III; CBCL, Child Behavior Checklist; CDI, Communicative Development Inventory; CFSM, Core Food Security Module; ECD, early childhood development; ECLS‐B, Early Childhood Longitudinal Study‐Birth Cohort; FFCWS, The Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study; FS, food security; HFI, household food insecurity; HFIAS, Household Food Insecurity Access Scale; HFSSM, Household Food Security Survey Module; HHFS, household food security; HOME‐SF, The Home Observation Measurement of the Environment‐Short Form; LSCA, Longitudinal Child Study of Arizona; MINIMat, Maternal and Infant Nutrition Intervention in Matlab; N/R, not reported; PEDS, Parents Evaluation of Developmental Status; PEDS trial, Pakistan Early Child Development Scale‐Up; PPVT, Peabody Picture Vocabulary test; RFSH, Rural Families Speak about Health; SDQ, Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire; TAS‐45, Toddler Attachment Sort‐45; TEMA, Test of Early Mathematics Ability; WIC, Nutrition Program for Women, Infants, and Children; WPPSI‐III, Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence III.

a

Year of data collection calculated from date provided by the study.

b

Age verified by ECLS website.

c

Child's age when HFI and ECD was measured by the study author.

d

Reported as ECD description adopted in this review.

e

Reported as proposed by The World Bank. Countries and Economies: Income levels, 2019.

f

Studies originally designed as longitudinal presenting cross‐sectional analysis for the purposes of this review were classified within study design as longitudinal.

g

Sample size for the purposes of this review.

Fifteen studies had longitudinal design, however nine of these studies presented cross‐sectional analysis for the purposes of this review (Fuller et al., 2002; Greder et al., 2017; Hobbs & King, 2018; Huang et al., 2018; Nagata et al., 2018; Obradovíc et al., 2016; Saha et al., 2010; Whitaker et al., 2006; Zaslow et al., 2009). Four studies had cross‐sectional design (Black et al., 2012; Drennen et al., 2019; Gill et al., 2018; Rose‐Jacobs et al., 2008). Confounders used to adjusted the association between HFI and ECD were related to the (a) child characteristics (i.e., gender, age, birth weight, race/ethnicity, nutrition status, and breastfeeding history), (b) caregiver characteristics (i.e., maternal education, maternal employment, maternal race/ethnicity, and marital status), and (c) household characteristics (i.e., public benefits receipt, income, size, and number of children).

3.2. Description of HFI and ECD outcomes

The prevalence of HFI in the US among 9‐month‐old infants was 12.5%, according to a nationally representative longitudinal study (Zaslow et al., 2009) and 11.7% at 48 months old (Huang et al., 2018; Table 3). In LMICs, the prevalence of HFI was 33.0% at 24 months and 37.0% at 48 months of age (Obradovíc et al., 2016).

Table 3.

Main results of studies evaluating the association between household food insecurity and early childhood development by domains

Study identification ECD outcomes (% delay or x̅) HFI prevalence ECD (% delay, x̅ or r) by HFI Effect size Effect size (ORs used in the meta‐analysis)
Developmental riskd

Drennen et al. (2019)

United States

11.5%

27.3% overall HFI

14.0% as HFI/child secure

13.3% as HFI/child FI

12.5% HFI/Child Secure 14.0% HFI/Child FI

<13 mo

HFI/child secure ORa 1.12 (0.85–1.47)

HFI/child FI ORa 1.49 (1.11–1.98)

13–24 months

HFI/child secure ORa 1.12 (0.90–1.39)

HFI/child FI ORa 1.22 (0.98–1.53)

25–36 months

HFI/child secure ORa 1.23 (0.98–1.54)

HFI/child FI ORa 1.35 (1.08–1.69)

37–48 months

HFI/child secure ORa 1.32 (1.02–1.72)

HFI/child FI ORa 1.44 (1.12–1.85)

25–36 months

HFI/child secure ORa 1.23 (0.98–1.54)

Black et al., (2012)

United States

15.2%a 24.0% N/R HFI ORa 1.22 (1.22–1.69) HFI ORa 1.22 (1.22–1.69)

Rose‐Jacobs et al. (2008)

United States

14.0%

21.0% HFI

6.0% HFI with hunger

18.0%

HFI ORa 1.76 (1.26–2.46)

HFI without hunger ORa1.77 (1.23–2.56)

HFI ORa 1.76 (1.26–2.46)
Socio‐emotionald

Finch et al., (2018)

Pakistan

Externalizing behaviour problems x̅ = 0.951 SD = 0.523

Prosocial behaviours x̅ = 1.529 SD = 0.363

BSID‐III social–emotional x̅ = 93.709 SD = 18.334

BSID‐III cognitive‐language x̅ = 80.5 SD = 12.793

32.8% HFI N/R

Externalizing behaviour

β 0.187 SE (0.068) Prosocial behaviours

β −0.008 SE (0.061)

Externalizing behaviour ORa 1.21 (1.06–1.38)e

Gill et al., (2018)

United States

High discipline frequency 56.4%

22.7% low FS

9.7% very low FS

67.6% marginal/high FS

High discipline frequency

Low FS = 60.7%

Very low FS = 64.2%

High‐frequency discipline Low FS

ORa 1.27 (1.07, 1.49)

Very low FS

ORa 1.41 (1.11, 1.79)

High‐frequency discipline Low and very low FS

OR 1.38 (1.21–1.58)e

Hobbs & King (2018)

United States

Externalizing behaviours x̅ = 9.6

Internalizing behaviours x̅ = 4.3

19.2% HFI

4.0% very low FS

N/R

Externalizing behaviours 10th β 0.02 (−0.05 to 0.10)

25th β 0.24 (0.12 to 0.36)

50th β 0.27 (0.15 to 0.39)

75th β 0.28 (0.10 to 0.46)

90th β 0.32 (0.08 to 0.57)Internalizing behaviours 10th β 0.28 (0.22 to 0.33)

25th β 0.25 (0.15 to 0.35)

50th β 0.39 (0.26 to 0.52)

75th β 0.43 (0.26 to 0.61)

90th β 0.44 (0.25 to 0.63)

Externalizing behaviours 90th ORa 1.38 (1.08–1.77)e

Internalizing behaviours 90th ORa 1.55 (1.28–1.88)e

Johnson & Markowitz (2018)

United States

Bayley behaviour score at 9 months x̅ = 5.16 SD = 0.99

Bayley behaviour score at 2 years x̅ = 3.41 SD = 0.83

Low FS at 9 months x̅ = 0.15

Very low FS at 9 months x̅ = 0.04

Low FS at 2 years x̅ = 0.11

Very low FS at 2 years x̅ = 0.03

Low FS at preschool x̅ = 0.16

Very low FS at preschool x̅ = 0.05

Bayley behaviour score at 9 months

HFI at 9 months x̅ = 5.13 SD = 0.99

HFI at 2 years x̅ = 5.14 SD = 0.98

HFI at preschool x̅ = 5.13 SD = 1.00

Bayley behaviour score (2 years)

HFI at 9 months x̅ = 3.41 SD = 0.84

HFI at 2 years x̅ = 3.39 SD = 0.84

HFI at 2 years x̅ = 3.38 SD = 0.83

Hyperactivity

Low FS at 2 years

β −0.01 SE (0.09)

Very low FS at 2 years

β 0.41 SE (0.15)

Conduct problems

Low FS at 2 years

β −0.01 SE (0.09)

Very low FS at 2 years

β 0.41 SE (0.18)

Hyperactivity

Low FS at 2 years

ORa 0.99 (0.83–1.18)e

Conduct problems

Low FS at 2 years

ORa 0.99 (0.83–1.18)e

King (2018)

United States

N/R

19.0% HFI

7.0% child HFI

HFI

Externalizing behaviours at 5 year x̅ = 11.9

Internalizing behaviours at 5 year x̅ = 5.9

Child HFI Externalizing behaviours at 5 year x̅ = 12.3

Internalizing behaviours at 5 year x̅ = 5.9

Externalizing behaviours

Child HFI SD 0.07 CSE (0.02)

HFI SD 0.02 CSE (0.01)

Internalizing behaviours

Child HFI SD 0.06 CSE (0.03)

HFI SD = 0.03 CSE (0.01)

Not possible to calculate OR

Milner et al. (2018)

Kenya

N/R

HFI timing

Round 1 x̅= 0.94 SD = 0.51

Round 2 x̅= 0.88 SD = 0.46

Round 3 x̅= 0.81 SD = 0.44

Round 4 x̅=0.83 SD = 0.40

Round 5 x̅= 0.83 SD = 0.43

Round 6 x̅=0.80 SD = 0.45

Round 7 x̅=0.80 SD = 0.41

Round 8 x̅=0.80 SD = 0.44

Round 9 x̅=0.84 SD = 0.47

HFI intensity

Cumulative 24 months x̅=7.54 SD = 2.98

HFI duration

Never 35.0%

1–2 times 33.0%

3–4 times 15.0%

5–6 times 9.0%

≥7 times 7.0%

N/R

Personal social

HFI 3 months ago

β −0.16 (−0.29, −0.031)

HFI current

β 0.032 (−0.11, 0.18)

HFI intensity

β −0.021 (−0.054, 0.012)

HFI duration

β −0.015 (−0.055, 0.026)

Personal social

HFI 3 months ago

ORa 0.85 (0.75–0.97)e

Nagata et al. (2018)

United States

6.0% affective score

5.4% anxiety score

5.4% oppositional defiant score

3.6% attention deficit/hyperactivity score

7.1% pervasive developmental problems

8.6% marginal FS

28.2% low FS

4.9% very low FS

N/R

Affective score

β 0.13 SE (0.07)

Pervasive developmental score

β 0.21 SE (0.10)

Oppositional defiant score

β 0.06 SE (0.08)

Anxiety

ORa 1.02 (0.81–1.28)

Attention deficit/hyperactivity

ORa 0.91 (0.65–1.29)

Affective score

ORa 1.14 (0.99–1.31)e

Oppositional defiant score

ORa 1.06 (0.91–1.24)e

Anxiety

ORa 1.02 (0.81–1.28)

Attention deficit/hyperactivity

ORa 0.91 (0.65–1.29)

Greder et al. (2017)

United States

Internalizing behaviours

T score = 50.25, SD = 11.03 CI 29.00–79.00

Externalizing behaviours T score = 51.34, SD = 11.54 CI 28.00–92.00

Score 1.55, SD = 2.04 CI 0.00–6.00 Internalizing child behaviour r = .23 Externalizing child behaviour r = .35

Externalizing behaviour

β 0.236 p < .05

Not possible to calculate OR

Zaslow et al. (2009)

United States

38.7% insecurely attached

2.6% very low FS

9.9% low FS

N/R

Insecure attachment

β 0.013

Not possible to calculate OR

Whitaker et al. (2006)

United States

10.4% aggressive

12.6% anxious/depressed

10.2% inattention/hyperactivity

21.9% any behaviour problem

17.1% mothers marginally FS

12.2% mothers HFI

3.5% mothers HFI with hunger.

Mothers HFI = 27.4% children HFI

Mothers marginally FS = 0.6% children HFI

Aggressive

14.5% marginally FS

20.8% HFI

Anxious/depressed

18.9% marginally FS

24.6% HFI

Inattention/hyperactivity

13.6% marginally FS

18.6% HFI

Any behaviour problem

29.9% marginally FS

38.6% HFI

Aggressive

Marginally FS

ORa 1.5 (1.1–2.1)

HFI ORa 1.9 (1.4–2.7)

Anxious/depressed

Marginally FS

ORa 1.8 (1.4–2.4)

HFI ORa 2.2 (1.6–3.1)

Inattention/hyperactivity Marginally FS

ORa 1.6 (1.1–2.2)

HFI ORa 1.9 (1.4–2.8)

Any behaviour problem Marginally FS

ORa 1.6 (1.3–2.1)

HFI ORa 2.1 (1.6–2.7)

Aggressive

HFI ORa 1.90 (1.40–2.70)

Anxious/depressed

HFI ORa 2.20 (1.60–3.10)

Inattention/hyperactivity HFI ORa 1.90 (1.40–2.80)

Fuller et al. (2002)

United States

Aggression

Girls x̅ = 13.8, SD = 8.4

Boys x̅ = 15.4 SD = 8.6

Inattentiveness

Girls x̅ = 6.8, SD = 3.9 Boys x̅ = 7.5, SD = 3.4

N/R N/R Aggression β 0.07 p > .05cInattentiveness β 0.03 p > .05c Not possible to calculate OR
Cognitived

Mickens et al. (2019)

United States

Battelle Developmental Inventory

Overall sample x̅: 8.99 SD = 4.58

English sample x̅: 9.92 SD = 4.74

Spanish sample x̅: 7.98 SD = 4.17

English TEMA x̅: 86.51 SD = 11.83

Spanish TEMA x̅: 75.94 SD = 12.72

31.3% N/R

TEMA English

Low FS β −0.02 (−3.30, 2.44)

TEMA Spanish

Low FS β −0.07 (−5.77, 2.15)

TEMA English

Low FS ORa 0.98 (0.04–11.47)e

Hobbs & King (2018)

United States

Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test x̅ = 86.2

Woodcock–Johnson letter–word identification x̅ = 99.9

19.2% HFI

4.0% very low FS

N/R

Peabody vocabulary

10th β −0.01 (−0.20 to 0.19)

25th β −0.11 (−0.26 to 0.05)

50th β −0.16 (−0.29 to −0.02)

75th β −0.19 (−0.33 to −0.04)

90th β −0.14 (−0.24 to −0.04)

Woodcock−Johnson letter–word

10th β −0.16 (−0.30 to −0.01)

25th β −0.01 (−0.16 to 0.14)

50th β −0.07 (−0.18 to 0.05)

75th β −0.03 (−0.16 to 0.10)

90th β −0.03 (−0.08 to 0.14)

Peabody vocabulary

10th ORa 0.99 (0.82–1.21)e

Woodcock−Johnson letter–word

10th ORa 0.85 (0.74–0.99)e

Huang et al. (2018)

United States

N/R

11.7% HFI_US

15.4% HFI_IMM

Children HFI_US

Math test score x̅ = 24.95 Reading test score x̅ = 21.36

Children HFI_IMM

Math test score x̅ = .45 Reading test score x̅ = 19.92

Reading assessment score

HFI_US β 0.39 SE (1.23)

Math assessment score

HFI_US β −0.17 SE (1.11)

Reading assessment score

HFI_IMM

ORa 0.44 (0.10–1.97)e

Math assessment score HFI_IMM

ORa 0.47 (0.09–2.38)e

Johnson & Markowitz, 2018 UnitedStates

Bayley cognitive score at 9 months x̅ = 76.57 SD = 10.00

Bayley cognitive score at 2 years

x̅ = 124.78 SD = 10.12

Low FS at 9 months x̅ =0.15 Very low FS at 9 months x̅ =0.04 Low FS at 2 years x̅ = 0.11 Very low FS at 2 years x̅ = 0.03 Low FS at preschool x̅ =0.16 Very low FS at preschool x̅ =0.05 Bayley cognitive score at 9 months HFI at 9 months x̅ = 76.39 SD = 10.10 HFI at 2 years x̅ = 76.42 SD = 9.90 HFI at preschool x̅ = 76.38 SD = 9.75Bayley cognitive score (2 years) HFI at 9 months x̅ = 124.58 SD = 9.98 HFI at 2 years x̅ = 124.52 SD = 10.32 HFI at preschool x̅ = 124.32 SD = 10.20 Approaches to learning Low FS at 2 years β −0.10 SE (0.08)very low FS at 2 years β −0.34 SE (0.12)Reading Low FS at 2 years β −0.13 SE (0.06)very low FS at 2 years β −0.07 SE (0.11)Math Low FS at 2 years β −0.18 SE (0.07) very low FS at 2 years β −0.27 SE (0.12) Reading Low FS at 2 years eORa 0.88 (0.78–0.99)Math Low FS at 2 years eORa 0.84 (0.73–0.96)

Obradovíc, Yousafzai, Finch, & Rasheed (2016)

Pakistan

N/R 33.0% N/R

EF β 0.022 SE (0.026)c

Verbal IQ β 0.048 SE (0.025)c

Performance IQ β 0.042 SE (0.026)c

Verbal IQ

OR 0.95 (0.91–1.00)e

Hernandez & Jacknowitz, (2009)

United States

N/R

HIF at 24 months

x̅ = 24.54, SEM = 0.09

HIF persistent

x̅ = 24.58, SEM = 0.10

Cognitive scores at 24 months

HFI at 9 months x̅ = 125.66, SEM = 0.55

HFI at 24 months x̅ = 123.93, SEM = 0.70

HFI persistent x̅ = 123.93, SEM = 0.77

Cognitive scores at 24 months

Adult HIF persistent

β −0.87 SE (0.81)

Adult HFI at 9 months

β 0.41 SE (0.54)

Adult HFI at 24 months

β −1.50 SE (0.69)

Zaslow et al., (2009)

United States

High income

Mental development score

x̅ = 34.7 SD = 22.4

2.6% very low FS

9.9% low FS

N/R

3−level FI and mental development

β = 0.015

Not possible to calculate OR
Motord

Milner et al., (2018)

Kenya

N/R

HFI timing

Round 1 x̅ = 0.94 SD = 0.51

Round 2 x̅ = 0.88 SD = 0.46

Round 3 x̅ = 0.81 SD = 0.44

Round 4 x̅ = 0.83 SD = 0.40

Round 5 x̅ = 0.83 SD = 0.43

Round 6 x̅ = 0.80 SD = 0.45

Round 7 x̅ = 0.80 SD = 0.41

Round 8 x̅ = 0.80 SD = 0.44

Round 9 x̅ = 0.84 SD = 0.47

HFI intensity

Cumulative 24 months x̅ = 7.54 SD = 2.98

HFI duration

Never 35%

1–2 times 33.0%

3–4 times 15.0%

5–6 times 9.0%

≥7 times 7.0%

N/R

Gross motor

HFI 3 months ago

β −0.097 (−0.23, 0.036)

HFI current

β −0.050 (−0.18, 0.079)

HFI intensity

β −0.028 (−0.058, 0.0019)

HFI duration

β −0.029 (−0.066, 0.0075)

Gross motor

HFI 3 months ago

ORa 0.91 (0.79–1.03)e

Hernandez & Jacknowitz, (2009)

United States

N/R

HIF at 24 months

x̅ = 24.54, SEM = 0.09

HIF persistent

x̅ = 24.58, SEM = 0.10

Motor scores at 24 months

HFI at 9 months x̅ = 81.65, SEM = 0.29

HFI at 24 months x̅ = 81.48, SEM = 0.36

HFI persistent x̅ = 80.86, SEM = 0.39

Motor scores at 24 months

Adult HIF persistent

β −0.58 SE (0.40)

Adult HFI at 9 months

β 0.10 SE (0.29)

Adult HFI at 24 months

β 0.05 SE (0.35)

Motor scores at 24 months

Adult HFI at 24 months

ORa 1.05 (0.53–2.09)e

Languaged

Milner et al. (2018)

Kenya

N/R

HFI timing

Round 1 x̅ = 0.94 SD = 0.51

Round 2 x̅ = 0.88 SD = 0.46

Round 3 x̅ = 0.81 SD = 0.44

Round 4 x̅ = 0.83 SD = 0.40

Round 5 x̅ = 0.83 SD = 0.43

Round 6 x̅ = 0.80 SD = 0.45

Round 7 x̅ = 0.80 SD = 0.41

Round 8 x̅ = 0.80 SD = 0.44

Round 9 x̅ = 0.84 SD = 0.47

HFI intensity

Cumulative 24 months

x̅ = 7.54 SD = 2.98

HFI duration Never 35%

1–2 times 33.0%

3–4 times 15.0%

5–6 times 9.0%

≥7 times 7.0%

N/R

Communication

HFI 3 months ago

β −0.12 (−0.26, 0.019)

HFI current

β −0.0070 (−0.14, 0.13)

HFI intensity

β −0.022 (−0.057, 0.014)

HFI duration

β −0.020 (−0.063, 0.024)

Saha et al. (2010) Bangladesh

Language expression

M 10.0 IQR (7.0, 15.0)

Language comprehension

M 37.4 SD (7.80)

N/R N/R

Language comprehension

ORa 0.83 (0.92–0.74)b

Language expression

ORa 0.36 (0.37–0.36)b

Abbreviations: CI, confidence intervals; CSE, clustered standard errors; ECD, early childhood development; EF, executive function; FS, Food Security; HFI, Household Food Insecurity; IMM, immigrant; IQ, intelligence quotient; IQR, interquartile range; M, median; N/R, not reported; OR, odds ratio; SD, standard deviation; SE: standard error; TEMA: Test of Early Mathematics Ability; US: United States; x̅: mean.

a

Percentage of ECD outcome calculated by data provided by the study.

b

Effect size calculated per data provided by the study.

c

Effect size not adjusted.

d

Reported as ECD description adopted in this review.

e

Calculated by the authors as recommended by Deeks, Altman, & Bradburn (2001) and Altman (1991).

For the ECD outcomes, the prevalence of developmental risk in the US ranged from 14.0% (Rose‐Jacobs et al., 2008) to 15.2% (Black et al., 2012) among children ages 4 to 36 months and was 11.5% among children ages 4 to 48 months (Drennen et al., 2019). Most studies assessed children's socio‐emotional and cognitive development in the US. Whitaker et al. (2006) evaluated socio‐emotional outcomes in children at 3 years and found high prevalence of inattention/hyperactivity (10.2%), aggressiveness (10.4%), anxiety/depression (12.6%), and overall behavioural disorder (21.9%). Zaslow et al. (2009), investigating children aged 9 and 24 months, found that 38.7% had insecure attachment (i.e., disorganized, avoidant, or ambivalent attachment) (Table 3).

For cognitive outcomes, the studies provided only the average test performance score and can just be interpreted in relation to the minimum and maximum possible range of values or compared with the means of other populations in which the same test is applied. For instance, Zaslow et al. (2009) presented the mean score for mental development from a nationally representative sample of children when they were 24 months old (34.7 ± 22.4, range 0–81.8). Johnson and Markowitz (2018) reported a Bayley cognitive mean score of 124.78 ± 10.12 at 2 years of age (range 40–160). Hobbs and King (2018) found that 5‐year‐old children living in large urban areas of the US had a mean receptive vocabulary score of 86.2 using the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (standard 20–160), and the reading mean score was 99.9 using Woodcock–Johnson letter–word identification (standard 100 ± 15).

3.3. HFI and ECD associations, meta‐analysis, and sources of heterogeneity

The meta‐analysis was performed for the ECD domains and subdomains identified (for definitions, see Table 1). For developmental risk and cognitive/math and cognitive/school readiness and reading, the studies available were conducted in HICs. Cognitive/vocabulary and motor development studies were conducted in both HICs and LMICs.

Fixed‐effects meta‐analysis was performed for developmental risk, motor (fine and gross motor outcomes), cognitive/school readiness and reading, cognitive/vocabulary, and cognitive/math subdomains (Figure 3). HFI was significantly associated with developmental risk (OR 1.28; 95% CI [1.14, 1.45]) and a suboptimal cognitive outcome in math (OR 0.84; 95% CI [0.73, 0.96]) and vocabulary (OR 0.94; 95% CI [0.90, 0.98]) skills. HFI was marginally associated with cognitive/school readiness and reading (OR 0.91; 95% CI [0.82, 1.00]). No association between motor development and HFI was found (0.91 OR; 95% CI [0.80, 1.04]).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Fixed effects of meta‐analysis of studies evaluating the association between household food insecurity and developmental risk (OR > 1 is considered risk factor) and motor and cognitive outcomes (OR < 1 is considered risk factor)

Due to the high heterogeneity found among studies, a random‐effects meta‐analysis was performed for socio‐emotional outcomes: externalizing behaviour (OR 1.25; 95% CI [1.08, 1.44]), externalizing behaviour/hyperactivity (OR 1.18; 95% CI [0.79, 1.77]), internalizing behaviour (OR 1.14; 95% CI [0.83, 1.57]), and internalizing behaviour/anxiety (OR 1.48; 95% CI [0.70, 3.15]) (Figure 4). The high heterogeneity identified was possibly due to the lack of standardized (a) sample size, (b) study quality, (c) tools used to measure ECD, and (d) time lapse between assessing ECD and HFI (Table 4).

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Random effects of meta‐analysis of studies evaluating the association between household food insecurity and socio‐emotional outcomes (OR > 1 is considered risk factor)

Table 4.

Possible sources of heterogeneity by early childhood development socio‐emotional subdomains

Externalizing behaviour

6 studiesa

(I‐squared = 74.7%)

Externalizing behaviour/hyperactivity

3 studiesb

(I‐squared = 83.5%)

Internalizing behaviour

3 studiesc

(I‐squared = 92.7%)

Internalizing behaviour/anxiety

2 studiesd

(I‐squared = 92.9%)

Number of studies Possible sources of heterogeneity Number of studies Possible sources of heterogeneity Number of studies Possible sources of heterogeneity Number of studies Possible sources of heterogeneity
Study design Cross‐sectional 1 Yes 0 No 3 No 0 No
Longitudinal 5 3 0 2
Study quality Strong 3 Yes 2 Yes 0 No 1 Yes
Moderate 2 1 3 1
Weak 1 0 0 0
Sample size ≤1,000 1 Yes 1 Yes 2 Yes 1 Yes
1,001–5,000 5 2 1 1
5,001–10,000 0 0 0 0
≥10,001 0 0 0 0
Country income Low–middle 1 Yes 0 No 1 Yes 0 No
High 5 3 2 2
Informant Mother only 4 Yes 2 Yes 3 No 2 No
Mother and/or caregiver 2 1 0 0
Size effect Adjusted 5 Yes 3 No 3 No 2 No
Crude 1 0 0 0
HFI intensity Very low + low 5 Yes 2 Yes 3 No 2 No
Low 1 1 0 0
HFI tool CFSM/HFSSM 5 Yes 3 No 2 Yes 2 No
HFIAS 1 0 1 0
ECD tool PEDS 0 Yes 0 Yes 0 Yes 0 No
BSF‐R 0 0 0 0
ASQ‐1 0 0 1 0
CBCL 3 2 2 2
Others 3 1 0 0
HFI and ECD evaluation At the same time 3 Yes 1 Yes 2 Yes 1 Yes
≤12 months 2 2 1 1
>12 months 1 0 0 0

Abbreviations: ASQ‐1, Ages and Stages Questionnaire; BSF‐R, Bayley Short Form‐Research Edition; CBCL, Child Behavior Checklist; CFSM, Core Food Security Module; ECD, early childhood development; HFI, household food insecurity; HFIAS, Household Food Insecurity Access Scale; HFSSM, Household Food Security Survey Module; PEDS, Parents Evaluation of Developmental Status.

a

Finch, Yousafzai, Rasheed, & Obradović (2018); Gill et al. (2018); Hobbs & King (2018); Johnson & Markowitz (2018); Nagata et al. (2018); and Whitaker et al. (2006).

b

Johnson & Markowitz (2018); Nagata, Gomberg, Hagan, Heyman, & Wojcicki (2018); and Whitaker et al. (2006).

c

Nagata, Gomberg, Hagan, Heyman, & Wojcicki (2018); Hobbs & King (2018); and Milner, Fiorella, Mattah, Bukusi, & Fernald (2018).

d

Nagata, Gomberg, Hagan, Heyman, & Wojcicki (2018) and Whitaker et al. (2006).

The language domain was assessed in different aspects for only two studies conducted in LMICs; thus, no meta‐analysis was performed. Findings on the language domain individual studies showed that communication at 24 months was not associated with HFI (Milner et al., 2018); however, comprehensive and expressive language skills at 18 months old were significantly associated with HFI (Saha et al., 2010).

4. DISCUSSION

This systematic review found that HFI is associated with poor ECD outcomes among children under 5 years old in both HICs and LMICs. Meta‐analysis showed that HFI was associated with the developmental risk and cognitive outcomes related to vocabulary and math skills. These results are consistent with previous reviews focusing on HICs (Cook & Frank, 2008; Pérez‐Escamilla & Vianna, 2012; Shankar et al., 2017) and reinforce the critical role that adverse experiences, such as HFI, during early childhood have in putting at risk the future of vulnerable children.

We hypothesize that HFI can impact child cognitive development through several pathways. First, HFI is strongly related with poverty (Wight, Kaushal, Waldfogel, & Garfinkel, 2014), and persistent poverty is known to have negative effects on children's cognitive development (Schoon, Jones, Cheng, & Maughan, 2012). Second, HFI is also associated with a poorer diet quality and quantity (Fram, Ritchie, Rosen, & Frongillo, 2015; Prado & Dewey, 2014), resulting in malnutrition (Miller, Murray, Thomson, & Arbour, 2016), which in turn has been associated with impaired brain development (Aboud & Yousafzai, 2016), leading to cognitive deficits, poor school achievement, and high rates of school dropout (Chilton, Chyatte, & Breaux, 2007; Victora et al., 2003; Walker, Chang, Powell, & Grantham‐McGregor, 2005). Third, food insecurity‐related psycho‐emotional stress may lead to difficulties in concentrating and learning (Pérez‐Escamilla & Vianna, 2012). This can also negatively affect caregivers' mental health status leading to less responsive caregiving and early childhood stimulation opportunities at home (Fuller et al., 2002; Pérez‐Escamilla & Vianna, 2012). Hence, we hypothesize that HFI is associated with poverty, poor diet quality, and nutritional deficiencies as well as expose families to stress and anxiety that impairs parent–child interaction, which may put children at developmental risk.

Despite the high heterogeneity, the meta‐analysis results identified HFI to be a risk factor for externalizing behavioural problems, which is consistent with previous studies (Gill et al., 2018; Hobbs & King, 2018; Johnson & Markowitz, 2018; Whitaker et al., 2006). High levels of poverty‐related stressors, such as HFI (Cook et al., 2008; Pérez‐Escamilla & Vianna, 2012), could lead to behaviour problems which in turn can seriously disrupt caregiver–child interactions (McLeod & Fettes, 2007). The high heterogeneity observed among the studies examining socio‐emotional development limits the strength of our conclusions in this domain.

Our meta‐analysis did not find associations between HFI and motor development, which is consistent with previous studies (Hernandez & Jacknowitz, 2009; Milner et al., 2018). Few studies have investigated these outcomes; hence, these findings should be interpreted with caution. Although recent research indicates that factors such as adult–child interactions and caregiving practices contribute to motor development (Adolph, Vereijken, & Shrout, 2003; Kariger et al., 2005; Kuklina, Ramakrishnan, Stein, Barnhart, & Martorell, 2004), the motor domain seems to be less susceptible to environmental influences than the other domains (Fernald et al., 2017). Both genetic and environmental factors are likely to play a key role, but the interactions between environment and genes are complex and not completely understood (Slining, Adair, Goldman, Borja, & Bentley, 2010).

Language and cognitive domains are highly interdependent, because language‐promoting activities support cognitive development (McClure, Cunningham, Bull, Berman, & Allison, 2018). Only two studies examined the association of HFI with language development, thus this relationship needs to be further investigated. In addition, further prospective studies evaluating HFI and cognitive/school readiness and reading need to be conducted to clarify this association.

Our findings should also be interpreted considering some limitations. Studies used different tools to assess HFI and ECD. This lack of uniformity, especially in the measurement of ECD and its domains, limits the interpretation of the data and must be considered when generalizing our findings. On the other hand, HFI was measured using similar experience‐based HFI scales that have been shown to be reliable globally (Marques et al., 2015; Pérez‐Escamilla et al., 2017). HFI experience‐based scales adapted from the HFSSM have been applied in different socio‐economic and cultural contexts and have demonstrated adequate psychometrics, reliability, and validity of the instrument properties (Marques et al., 2015; Pérez‐Escamilla et al., 2017). Our findings call for the development of standardized tools to measure ECD globally, which will allow for greater comparability between studies. Furthermore, future studies need to clearly report when and how HFI and ECD were measured. Child's age at the time of assessment can be a strong confounder when comparing findings across studies (Johnson & Markowitz, 2018); hence, it must be clearly reported.

Studies with different geographic populations and age groups were included in this review, which should be considered in the interpretation of the results. Although we considered performing a meta‐regression to explore the results according to the country's income, due to the very limited number of studies performed on LMICs, this was not possible. Our review and meta‐analysis clearly demonstrate the need to conduct prospective studies to investigate the associations between HFI and ECD in LMICs. Despite these limitations, our meta‐analysis was carefully carried out using random effects models for domains with high heterogeneity across studies (Deeks et al., 2008; Egger, Davey‐Smith, & Altman , 2008). Another limitation is that the majority of studies were cross‐sectional or presenting cross‐sectional analysis for the purposes this review, which precludes drawing causal inferences (Atkins et al., 2004). The sources of bias identified through this systematic review can help guide the design of future studies (Boutron et al., 2019).

To our knowledge, this is the first review and meta‐analysis that identifies HFI as a risk factor for suboptimal ECD, which includes results from LMICs. Our findings suggest that the associations between HFI and ECD can vary across ECD domains. HFI was associated with developmental risk and poor cognitive outcomes, including vocabulary and math skills, among children under 5 years old. This finding supports the need to consider HFI as a key component of integrated ECD national strategies (Pérez‐Escamilla, 2017).

ACKNOWLEDGMENT

All those who contributed to the study are already identified as authors. This study was financed in part by the Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior (CAPES)—Brasil (Finance Code 001).

CONFLICTS OF INTEREST

The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

CONTRIBUTIONS

KHDO, GMA, MBG, ASM, AMS, DCH, RPE, and GB conceptualized and designed the study, reviewed, and revised the manuscript. KHDO and GMA conducted the bibliographic search, collected data, and selected the studies. MBG and GB coordinated and supervised data collection. GMA standardized the data. ASM resolved the disagreements between the first reviewers. AMS reviewed the main study tables. GB carried out the meta‐analysis. KHDO, MBG, GB, and RPE interpreted the results. KHDO, MBG, and GB drafted the initial manuscript. MBG, GB, DCH, and RPE revised the article critically for important intellectual content. KHDO, GMA, MBG, ASM, AMS, DCH, RPE, and GB agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Supporting information

Data S1: Databases and Individualized Truncations of Words

Data S2: Excluded studies in the systematic review

Data S3: Quality Assessment Tool for Quantitative Studies. Original check‐list and adapted items

de Oliveira KHD, de Almeida GM, Gubert MB, et al. Household food insecurity and early childhood development: Systematic review and meta‐analysis. Matern Child Nutr. 2020;16:e12967 10.1111/mcn.12967

REFERENCES

  1. Aboud, F. F. , & Yousafzai, A. K. (2016). Very early childhood development bti—Reproductive, maternal, newborn, and child health: Disease control priorities (3rd ed.). [PubMed]
  2. Adolph, K. E. , Vereijken, B. , & Shrout, P. E. (2003). What changes in infant walking and why. Child Development, 74(2), 475–497. 10.1111/1467-8624.7402011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. American Academy of Pediatrics (2019). Healthy children. Ages & stages. Retrieved from https://www.healthychildren.org/english/ages-stages/pages/default.aspx
  4. Armijo‐Olivo, S. , Stiles, C. R. , Hagen, N. A. , Biondo, P. D. , & Cummings, G. G. (2012). Assessment of study quality for systematic reviews: A comparison of the cochrane collaboration risk of bias tool and the effective public health practice project quality assessment tool: Methodological research. Journal of Evaluation in Clinical Practice, 18(1), 12–18. 10.1111/j.1365-2753.2010.01516.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Atkins, D. , Eccles, M. , Flottorp, S. , Guyatt, G. H. , Henry, D. , Hill, S. , … Schünemann, H. (2004). Systems for grading the quality of evidence and the strength of recommendations I: Critical appraisal of existing approaches The GRADE Working Group. BMC Health Services Research, 4(1), 38 10.1186/1472-6963-4-38 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Black, M. M. , Quigg, A. M. , Cook, J. , Casey, P. H. , Cutts, D. B. , Chilton, M. , … Rose‐Jacobs, R. (2012). WIC participation and attenuation of stress‐related child health risks of household food insecurity and caregiver depressive symptoms. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 166, 444–451. 10.1001/archpediatrics.2012.1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Black, M. M. , Walker, S. P. , Fernald, L. C. , Andersen, C. T. , DiGirolamo, A. M. , Lu, C. , … Devercelli, A. E. (2017). Early childhood development coming of age: Science through the life course. Lancet, 389, 77–90. 10.1016/S0140-6736(16)31389-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Boccolini, C. S. , Carvalho, M. L. D. , & Oliveira, M. I. C. D. (2015). Factors associated with exclusive breastfeeding in the first six months of life in Brazil: A systematic review. Revista de saúde Publica, 49, 91. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Boutron, I. , Page, M. J. , Higgins, J. P. , Altman, D. G. , Lundh, A. , & Hróbjartsson, A. (2019). Considering bias and conflicts of interest among the included studies. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions, 177‐204. [Google Scholar]
  10. Buccini, G. D. S. , Pérez‐Escamilla, R. , Paulino, L. M. , Araujo, C. L. , & Venancio, S. I. (2017). Pacifier use and interruption of exclusive breastfeeding: Systematic review and meta‐analysis. Maternal & Child Nutrition, 13(3), e12384. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Chilton, M. , Chyatte, M. , & Breaux, J. (2007). The negative effects of poverty & food insecurity on child development. Indian Journal of Medical Research, 126(4), 262. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cook, J. T. , & Frank, D. A. (2008). Food security, poverty, and human development in the United States. Annals of the new York Academy of Sciences, 1136, 193–209. 10.1196/annals.1425.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cook, J. T. , Frank, D. A. , Casey, P. H. , Rose‐Jacobs, R. , Black, M. M. , Chilton, M. , … Cutts, D. B. (2008). A brief indicator of household energy security: Associations with food security, child health, and child development in us infants and toddlers. Pediatrics, 122, e867–e875. 10.1542/peds.2008-0286 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Deeks, J. J. , Altman, D. G. , & Bradburn, M. J. (2008). Statistical methods for examining heterogeneity and combining results from several studies in meta‐analysis In Egger M., Smith G. D., & Altman D. G. (Eds.), Systematic reviews in health care. London, UK: BMJ Publishing. [Google Scholar]
  15. Drennen, C. R. , Coleman, S. M. , de Cuba, S. E. , Frank, D. A. , Chilton, M. , Cook, J. T. , … Black, M. M. (2019). Food insecurity, health, and development in children under age four years. Pediatrics, 144(4), e20190824 10.1542/peds.2019-0824 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Egger, M. , Davey‐Smith, G. , & Altman, D. (Eds.). (2008). Systematic reviews in health care: Meta‐analysis in context. London, UK: BMJ Publishing. [Google Scholar]
  17. Food and Agriculture Organization (2013). The state of food insecurity in the world. Rome: Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations. [Google Scholar]
  18. Fernald, L. C. H. , Prado, E. L. , Kariger, P. K. , & Raikes, A. (2017). A toolkit for measuring early childhood development in low and middle income countries. Washington, D.C.: World Bank Group. [Google Scholar]
  19. Finch, J. E. , Yousafzai, A. K. , Rasheed, M. , & Obradović, J. (2018). Measuring and understanding social‐emotional behaviors in preschoolers from rural pakistan. PLoS ONE, 13, e0207807 10.1371/journal.pone.0207807 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Fram, M. S. , Ritchie, L. D. , Rosen, N. , & Frongillo, E. A. (2015. Mar). Child experience of food insecurity is associated with child diet and physical activity. The Journal of Nutrition, 145(3), 499–504. 10.3945/jn.114.194365 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Fuller, B. , Caspary, G. , Kagan, S. L. , Gauthier, C. , Huang, D. S. C. , Carroll, J. , & McCarthy, J. (2002). Does maternal employment influence poor children's social development? Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 17, 470–497. 10.1016/S0885-2006(02)00187-4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  22. Gill, M. , Koleilat, M. , & Whaley, S. E. (2018). The impact of food insecurity on the home emotional environment among low‐income mothers of young children. Maternal and Child Health Journal, 22, 1146–1153. 10.1007/s10995-018-2499-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Greder, K. A. , Peng, C. , Doudna, K. D. , & Sarver, S. L. (2017). Role of family stressors on rural low‐income children's behaviors. In Child Youth Care Forum, 46, 703–720. 10.1007/s10566-017-9401-6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  24. Hernandez, D. C. , & Jacknowitz, A. (2009). Transient, but not persistent, adult food insecurity influences toddler development. Journal of Nutrition, 139, 1517–1524. 10.3945/jn.109.105593 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Hobbs, S. , & King, C. (2018). The unequal impact of food insecurity on cognitive and behavioral outcomes among 5‐year‐old urban children. Journal of Nutrition Education and Behavior, 50, 687–694. 10.1016/j.jneb.2018.04.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Huang, Y. , Potochnick, S. , & Heflin, C. M. (2018). Household food insecurity and early childhood health and cognitive development among children of immigrants. Journal of Family Issues, 39(6), 1465–1497. 10.1177/0192513X17710772 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  27. Johnson, A. D. , & Markowitz, A. J. (2018). Associations between household food insecurity in early childhood and children's kindergarten skills. Child Development, 89, e1–e17. 10.1111/cdev.12764 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Jyoti, D. F. , Frongillo, E. A. , & Jones, S. J. (2005). Food insecurity affects school children's academic performance, weight gain, and social skills. J Nutr., 135(12), 2831–2839. 10.1093/jn/135.12.2831 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Kariger, P. K. , Stoltzfus, R. J. , Olney, D. , Sazawal, S. , Black, R. , Tielsch, J. M. , … Pollitt, E. (2005). Iron deficiency and physical growth predict attainment of walking but not crawling in poorly nourished Zanzibari infants. The Journal of Nutrition, 135(4), 814–819. 10.1093/jn/135.4.814 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. King, C. (2018). Food insecurity and child behavior problems in fragile families. Economics and Human Biology, 28, 14–22. 10.1016/j.ehb.2017.11.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Knowles, M. , Rabinowich, J. , Ettinger de Cuba, S. , Cutts, D. B. , & Chilton, M. (2016). “Do you wanna breathe or eat?”: Parent perspectives on child health consequences of food insecurity, trade‐offs, and toxic stress. Maternal and Child Health Journal, 20, 25–32. 10.1007/s10995-015-1797-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Kuklina, E. V. , Ramakrishnan, U. , Stein, A. D. , Barnhart, H. H. , & Martorell, R. (2004). Growth and diet quality are associated with the attainment of walking in rural Guatemalan infants. The Journal of Nutrition, 134(12), 3296–3300. 10.1093/jn/134.12.3296 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Marques, E. S. , Reichenheim, M. E. , de Moraes, C. L. , Antunes, M. M. , & Salles‐Costa, R. (2015). Household food insecurity: A systematic review of the measuring instruments used in epidemiological studies. Public Health Nutrition, 18(5), 877–892. 10.1017/S136898001400105 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. McClure, C. , Cunningham, M. , Bull, S. , Berman, S. , & Allison, M. A. (2018). Using mobile health to promote early language development: A narrative review. Academic Pediatrics, 18, 850–854. 10.1016/j.acap.2018.07.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. McLeod, J. D. , & Fettes, D. L. (2007). Trajectories of failure: The educational careers of children with mental health problems. AJS; American Journal of Sociology, 113, 653–701. 10.1086/521849 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Mickens, A. J. (2019). The impact of poverty on math readiness. Thesis [Master of Arts in Educational Psychology]. Tucson, Arizona: The University of Arizona. [Google Scholar]
  37. Miller, A. C. , Murray, M. B. , Thomson, D. R. , & Arbour, M. C. (2016). How consistent are associations between stunting and child development? Evidence from a meta‐analysis of associations between stunting and multidimensional child development in fifteen low‐ and middle‐income countries. Public Health Nutrition, 19, 1339–1347. 10.1017/S136898001500227X [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Milner, E. M. , Fiorella, K. J. , Mattah, B. J. , Bukusi, E. , & Fernald, L. C. H. (2018). Timing, intensity, and duration of household food insecurity are associated with early childhood development in kenya. Maternal and Child Nutrition, 14, e12543 10.1111/mcn.12543 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Moher, D. , Liberati, A. , Tetzlaff, J. , Altman, D. G. , & Group, P (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta‐analyses: The PRISMA statement. PLoS Medicine, 6, e1000097–e1000097. 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Nagata, J. M. , Gomberg, S. , Hagan, M. J. , Heyman, M. B. , & Wojcicki, J. M. (2018). Food insecurity is associated with maternal depression and child pervasive developmental symptoms in low‐income latino households. Journal of Hunger and Environmental Nutrition, 14, 1–14. 10.1080/19320248.2018.1434101 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Obradovíc, J. , Yousafzai, A. K. , Finch, J. E. , & Rasheed, M. A. (2016). Maternal scaffolding and home stimulation: Key mediators of early intervention effects on children's cognitive development. Developmental Psychology, 52, 1409–1421. 10.1037/dev0000182 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Pérez‐Escamilla, R. (2017). Food Security and the 2015‐2030 Sustainable Development Goals: From Human to Planetary Health: Perspectives and Opinions. Curr Dev Nutr. 1(7):e000513. doi:10.3945/cdn.117.000513. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Pérez‐Escamilla, R. , Gubert, M. B. , Rogers, B. , & Hromi‐Fiedler, A. (2017). Food security measurement and governance: Assessment of the usefulness of diverse food insecurity indicators for policy makers. Global Food Security, 14, 96–104. 10.1016/j.gfs.2017.06.003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  44. Pérez‐Escamilla, R. , & Vianna, R. (2012). Food insecurity and the behavioral and intellectual development of children: A review of the evidence. Journal of Applied Research on Children, 3(1), 9. [Google Scholar]
  45. Pizato, N. , Botelho, P. B. , Gonçalves, V. S. S. , Dutra, E. S. , & de Carvalho, K. M. B. (2017). Effect of grazing behavior on weight regain post‐bariatric surgery: A systematic review. Nutrients, 9, e1322 10.3390/nu9121322 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Prado, E. L. , & Dewey, K. G. (2014). Nutrition and brain development in early life. Nutrition Reviews, 72, 267–284. 10.1111/nure.12102 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Rose‐Jacobs, R. , Black, M. M. , Casey, P. H. , Cook, J. T. , Cutts, D. B. , Chilton, M. , … Frank, D. A. (2008). Household food insecurity: Associations with at‐risk infant and toddler development. Pediatrics, 121(1), 65–72. 10.1542/peds.2006-3717 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Saha, K. K. , Tofail, F. , Frongillo, E. A. , Rasmussen, K. M. , Arifeen, S. E. , Persson, L. Å. , … Hamadani, J. D. (2010). Household food security is associated with early childhood language development: Results from a longitudinal study in rural bangladesh. Child: Care, Health and Development, 36, 309–316. 10.1111/j.1365-2214.2009.01049.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Schoon, I. , Jones, E. , Cheng, H. , & Maughan, B. (2012). Family hardship, family instability, and cognitive development. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 66(8), 716–722. 10.1136/jech.2010.121228 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Shankar, P. , Chung, R. , & Frank, D. A. (2017). Association of food insecurity with children's behavioral, emotional, and academic outcomes: A systematic review. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 38, 135–150. 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000383 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Slining, M. , Adair, L. S. , Goldman, B. D. , Borja, J. B. , & Bentley, M. (2010). Infant overweight is associated with delayed motor development. The Journal of pediatrics, 157(1), 20–25.e21. 10.1016/j.jpeds.2009.12.054 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Victora, C. G. , Wagstaff, A. , Schellenberg, J. A. , Gwatkin, D. , Claeson, M. , & Habicht, J. P. (2003). Applying an equity lens to child health and mortality: More of the same is not enough. The Lancet, 362(9379), 233–241. 10.1016/S0140-6736(03)13917-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Walker, S. P. , Chang, S. M. , Powell, C. A. , & Grantham‐McGregor, S. M. (2005). Effects of early childhood psychosocial stimulation and nutritional supplementation on cognition and education in growth‐stunted Jamaican children: Prospective cohort study. The Lancet, 366(9499), 1804–1807. 10.1016/S0140-6736(05)67574-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Whitaker, R. C. , Phillips, S. M. , & Orzol, S. M. (2006). Food insecurity and the risks of depression and anxiety in mothers and behavior problems in their preschool‐aged children. Pediatrics, 118, e859–e868. 10.1542/peds.2006-0239 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. World Health Organization . (2019). Maternal, newborn, child and adolescent health. Early child development.
  56. Wight, V. , Kaushal, N. , Waldfogel, J. , & Garfinkel, I. (2014). Understanding the link between poverty and food insecurity among children: Does the definition of poverty matter? Journal of Children and Poverty, 20(1), 1–20. 10.1080/10796126.2014.891973 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Zaslow, M. , Bronte‐Tinkew, J. , Capps, R. , Horowitz, A. , Moore, K. A. , & Weinstein, D. (2009). Food security during infancy: Implications for attachment and mental proficiency in toddlerhood. Maternal and Child Health Journal, 13(1), 66–80. 10.1007/s10995-008-0329-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Data S1: Databases and Individualized Truncations of Words

Data S2: Excluded studies in the systematic review

Data S3: Quality Assessment Tool for Quantitative Studies. Original check‐list and adapted items


Articles from Maternal & Child Nutrition are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES