Dear Editors,
Impairment in working memory, the short-term encoding and maintenance of information, is a core deficit in schizophrenia that may represent an endophenotype. Though an important treatment target, no current pharmacologic interventions significantly improve working memory (Kahn and Keefe, 2013). Animal studies suggest that the hypothalamic neuropeptide oxytocin modulates memory processes (Chini et al., 2014), but oxytocin’s ability to ameliorate working memory deficits in schizophrenia has been tested in only two small studies (Feifel et al., 2012; Michalopoulou et al., 2015) that yielded divergent findings.
To clarify whether oxytocin administration can acutely improve working memory in schizophrenia, we conducted a randomized, placebo-controlled, crossover study enrolling 70 clinically stable outpatients with schizophrenia and 80 age-matched neurotypical controls (Table 1). Participants had no major medical illnesses, no history of a substance use disorder within the past 6 months, and negative urine toxicology screens on testing days. We did not match groups on education, as decreased educational attainment is a consequence of schizophrenia (Resnick, 1992). All participants provided written consent and the study protocol was approved by the University of California, San Francisco Institutional Review Board. We administered 40 IU of intranasal oxytocin (Syntocinon, Novartis) or matched placebo on two testing days separated by at least two weeks. Approximately 90 minutes following drug administration on each day, participants completed the Letter Number Sequence (LNS) task, a measure of working memory from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-III; Wechsler, 1997) that requires no specific academic skills.
Table 1.
Patients (n=70) | Controls (n=80) | Patients vs. Controls | |
---|---|---|---|
Sex | 19 female (37%) | 25 female (31%) | |
Mean (SD) |
|||
Age | 43.4 (14.5) | 40.8 (14.2) | p = 0.27 |
Education years | 14.4 (3.0) | 15.9 (2.2) | p < 0.001 |
PANSS score | |||
Positive | 13.5 (5.1) | - | - |
Negative | 13.9 (4.7) | - | - |
General | 26.2 (6.7) | - | - |
LNS raw score | PL: 14.06 (4.21) | PL: 16.01 (3.07) | |
OT: 14.26 (4.29) | OT: 16.13 (2.89) |
We used mixed-effects models to test whether oxytocin improved LNS performance, regressing LNS scores on a drug x group interaction with participant included as a random effect. We found a main effect of group, indicating that—as expected—patients performed worse overall (b = −1.91; CI = −3.02, −0.81; p = 0.001). However, neither the drug x group interaction (b = −0.09; CI = −0.81, 0.64; p = 0.81) nor the main effect of drug (b = −0.15; CI = −0.21, 0.52; p = 0.41) were significant. Given this null result, we conducted two follow-up analyses to clarify our findings: (1) There is evidence of differential effects of oxytocin in males and females (for a review see Bradley and Woolley, 2017); thus, we examined whether sex moderated oxytocin’s effects on working memory. We regressed LNS scores on a group x sex interaction and a drug x sex interaction with participant included as a random effect. We did not include a 3-way interaction given the null interaction previously obtained. Neither the group x sex nor drug x sex interaction were significant (both p > 0.25). (2) To improve the inference of our non-significant null hypothesis significance test p-values, we applied equivalence testing using the two one-sided test (TOST) procedure (Lakens et al., 2018). The TOST procedure allows for detection of the absence of a meaningful oxytocin effect using equivalence bounds, which we set based on calculations that our study had 80% power to detect an effect size of at least d = 0.34. The equivalence test was significant, t(69) = −2.140, p = 0.018, given bounds of d = −0.34 to d = 0.34 and an alpha of 0.05. Thus, we can reject the null hypothesis that oxytocin is associated with a meaningful difference in LNS performance in our patient sample.
Our finding that oxytocin did not modulate working memory performance among patients is in line with one previous study (Feifel et al., 2012) that failed to find an effect of a two-week course of oxytocin on working memory, but inconsistent with another study (Michalopoulou et al., 2015) that reported positive effects after a single dose. There are at least two possible explanations for these divergent findings. First, capturing oxytocin effects on working memory may be task-dependent. The LNS used here and by Feifel et al. (2012) tests visual spatial working memory and processing speed (Crowe, 2016), while the Digit Span task used by Michalopoulou et al. (2015) involves a significant immediate memory component (Hill et al., 2010). Second, different dosages of oxytocin and different administration protocols may lead to divergent results. Michalopoulou et al. (2015) administered a lower single dose (24 IU) than us, while Feifel et al. (2012) administered 20 IU twice daily for one week and then 40 IU twice daily for two weeks. While dose-response curves for intranasal oxytocin are not well-established, behavioral effects are known to vary with dosage, timing, and chronicity of administration (Bradley and Woolley, 2017).
Given the lack of clarity regarding oxytocin’s therapeutic benefit in schizophrenia, we believe that it is critical to disseminate negative findings that might guide future investigations. To our knowledge, this is the largest study to examine the acute effect of intranasal oxytocin on working memory in people with schizophrenia. Our results suggest that oxytocin is unlikely to improve the working memory deficits associated with schizophrenia.
Acknowledgements
We are grateful to the participants who gave their time to this study.
Footnotes
Conflict of interest
All authors declare no conflicts of interest in relation to the subject of this study.
References
- Bradley ER, Woolley JD, 2017. Oxytocin effects in schizophrenia: Reconciling mixed findings and moving forward. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 80, 36–56. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.05.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chini B, Leonzino M, Braida D, Sala M, 2014. Learning About Oxytocin: Pharmacologic and Behavioral Issues. Biological Psychiatry 76, 360–366. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.08.029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crowe SF, 2016. Does the Letter Number Sequencing Task Measure Anything More Than Digit Span? Assessment 7, 113–117. 10.1177/107319110000700202 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feifel D, Macdonald K, Cobb P, Minassian A, 2012. Adjunctive intranasal oxytocin improves verbal memory in people with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Hill BD, Elliott EM, Shelton JT, Pella RD, O’Jile JR, Gouvier WD, 2010. Can we improve the clinical assessment of working memory? An evaluation of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Third Edition using a working memory criterion construct. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 32, 315–323. 10.1080/13803390903032529 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kahn RS, Keefe RSE, 2013. Schizophrenia is a cognitive illness: time for a change in focus. JAMA Psychiatry 70, 1107–1112. 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.155 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lakens D, Scheel AM, Isager PM, 2018. Equivalence Testing for Psychological Research: A Tutorial. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science 10.1177/2515245918770963 [DOI]
- Michalopoulou PG, Averbeck BB, Kalpakidou AK, Evans S, Bobin T, Kapur S, Shergill SS, 2015. The effects of a single dose of oxytocin on working memory in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 162, 62–63. 10.1016/j.schres.2014.12.029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Resnick SM, 1992. Matching for Education in Studies of Schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 49, 246 10.1001/archpsyc.1992.01820030078011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D, 1997. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-IIIuk).