Skip to main content
Springer logoLink to Springer
. 2019 Jun 20;55(7):811–822. doi: 10.1007/s00127-019-01734-6

Epidemiology of loneliness in a cohort of UK mental health community crisis service users

Jingyi Wang 1, Brynmor Lloyd-Evans 1, Louise Marston 2, Ruimin Ma 1, Farhana Mann 1, Francesca Solmi 1, Sonia Johnson 1,3,
PMCID: PMC7303089  PMID: 31222412

Abstract

Purpose

Loneliness is an important issue for mental health service users. However, it has not been a particularly prominent focus of recent mental health research. This paper aimed to explore the severity of loneliness among people leaving mental health community crisis services, and to identify factors associated with loneliness.

Methods

A total of 399 participants experiencing mental health crises recruited for a research trial from community crisis services were included in this cross-sectional study. They completed the eight-item measure of the University of California at Los Angeles Loneliness Scale and a set of instruments assessing socio-demographic, psychosocial, and psychiatric variables.

Results

Severity of loneliness was high among people leaving community crisis services. Longer years since first contact with mental health services (2–10 years, coefficient = 1.83, 95% CI 0.49–3.16; more than 10 years, coefficient = 1.91, 95% CI 0.46–3.36) and more severe affective symptoms (coefficient = 0.32, 95% CI 0.23–0.40) were associated with greater loneliness, whereas bigger social network size (coefficient = − 0.56, 95% CI − 0.76 to − 0.36) and greater social capital (coefficient = − 0.16, 95% CI − 0.31 to − 0.003) were associated with less severe loneliness.

Conclusions

This paper supports a view that people experiencing mental health crises often report relatively severe loneliness, and that loneliness tends to become more severe during the course of illness. A greater awareness of loneliness among mental health professionals may be beneficial. Loneliness is a potential focus of the development of interventions to improve the lives and outcomes of people with significant mental health problems.

Keywords: Loneliness, Prevalence, Correlates, Mental health

Introduction

Loneliness can be defined as a negative emotional state that occurs when there is a subjective discrepancy between the desired and actual social relationships [13]. Chronic loneliness is experienced by approximately 10–15% of the general population across all ages [4]. Feelings of loneliness have been found to be more prevalent among people with mental health problems than in the general population [5, 6]. For people with depression, cross-sectional studies have found up to 40% of respondents feeling lonely all or most of the time [7], with an 11-fold increase in the odds of loneliness compared to adults with no mental disorder [8]. In a comparison between people with psychosis and a general population sample with similar demographic characteristics, the prevalence of loneliness among people with psychosis was 80% compared with 35% in the general population [9].

Evidence suggests that, in the general population, loneliness is associated with risk factors such as being a victim of domestic violence, lack of employment, not being married/partnered, being a young or an older adult (compared with middle-aged individuals), and impaired self-reported health [6, 10]. The limited available research evidence suggests that there may be some specific correlates of loneliness among people with mental health problems. Severe mental illness is known to hamper the development of social skills and negatively influence people’s ability to establish and sustain relationships [11, 12]. Depression and loneliness appear to have a circular relationship, and people with depressive symptoms sometimes have an urge to avoid social contact and isolate themselves from others [13, 14]. Stigma and social exclusion are also believed to be important causes of loneliness among people with significant mental health problems. They have an obvious effect on breaking an individual’s social ties through rejection, avoidance, and distancing from other people as well as discrimination in various areas of life [15, 16]. While some individual findings have been reported, the factors associated with loneliness have rarely been systematically studied and are not well established among people with mental health problems. Identifying these factors could help to design loneliness interventions for mental health service users in a personalised way, targeting people at particular risk of being lonely, and they may also provide pointers to potential mechanisms.

Our goal in this paper is to advance understanding of variables associated with loneliness among people experiencing mental health crises through an investigation of patterns of loneliness among people leaving the care of Crisis Resolution Teams (CRTs). CRTs, also known as home treatment teams, are a form of community crisis care intended to reduce the use of inpatient services. They offer rapid assessment to people with mental health crises and refer them to the most suitable services [17]. CRTs also provide intensive home treatment for individuals who are suitable for community-based treatment as an alternative to inpatient care [17]. CRT users are a clinically very mixed group of secondary mental health service users including a wide range of diagnoses and a mixture of long-term and short-term illness. Therefore, they represent an ideal group for researchers to begin to understand the extent of loneliness and the factors associated with it in people with relatively severe mental health problems. Our primary research aims were to: (1) assess the severity of loneliness among people leaving CRTs; and (2) to identify factors independently associated with loneliness among CRT users.

Methods

Study sample

The sample was taken from patients receiving care from CRTs in six NHS Trusts, which covered inner city, suburban, and more rural regions. The sample was recruited to participate in a trial of peer-delivered self-management for the CORE study (CRT Optimisation and RElapse prevention) which has already been reported [18]: this paper describes secondary analyses from the participant baseline data. The CORE study protocol was approved by the London Camden and Islington Research Ethics Committee and approvals from Research and Development departments and participating services in involved NHS Trusts were obtained. The participants had received support from a CRT team in one of the six Trusts for no less than a week and had capacity to provide written informed consent. Exclusion criteria were: (1) presenting such a high level of risk to other people that clinical staff considered that assessment on mental health service premise might not be safe for researchers, (2) not living in the catchment area, (3) not understanding English sufficiently to complete the study assessments, and (4) having been discharged from the CRT more than 1 month. After screening 3054 service users, 1697 people were eligible to participate. Among the excluded 1357 service users, 179 did not have capacity, 336 were assessed as posing too high a risk to participate, 175 were outside the recruitment area, 111 had language barriers, 121 received support from CRTs for less than 7 days, 146 were discharged from hospital rather than CRTs, and 289 had other reasons. Of the eligible participants, 401 (23.6%) were recruited and completed the assessment. The main reasons for not taking part in the study were declining to participate, not being contactable, and running out of time (interviews needed to be completed within a month of crisis team discharge). However, two participants then withdrew consent for their data to be used and so were excluded, which resulted in the study sample of 399 participants available for analyses. The sample included only those in the main trial and not the pilot sample for the CORE study.

Measures

Here, we describe the measures used for the analyses reported in this study.

Loneliness

An eight-item short-form measure of the University of California at Los Angeles (UCLA) Loneliness Scale (ULS-8) [19] provided a measure of perceived loneliness. The scale was derived from the 20-item UCLA Loneliness Scale [20] by Hays and DiMatteo, and has a high level of validity and reliability [19, 21, 22]. Both the frequency and intensity of feelings of loneliness in important aspects of life are covered by the unidimensional scale (e.g., “How often do you feel that you lack companionship?”). To reduce response bias, the word ‘lonely’ never appears in the instrument [23]. Items are rated on a four-point scale: (1) Never, (2) Rarely, (3) Sometimes, and (4) Always, with higher total scores indicating greater loneliness.

Socio-demographic variables

Information regarding the individual’s social and demographic characteristics was recorded, including: age, gender, ethnic background, whether born in the UK, accommodation and living situation, contact with children, educational attainment, and employment.

Psychosocial variables

Social network size was assessed using two items from the Lubben Social Network Scale (LSNS-6) [24]. LSNS-6 is a six-item measure of social contact with family and friends, which showed high internal consistency, stable factor structures, and high correlations with criterion variables [24]. In this paper, we used the two scale items which assessed amount of social contact with family and friends (total score 0–10; higher total score indicating larger social network size), ignoring the other four items which were related to the perceived quality of social relationships and overlapped conceptually with loneliness. Social capital was assessed with the Health and Lifestyles Survey Social Capital Questionnaire [25], a six-item measure of neighbourhood social capital regarding enjoyment of living, personal safety, neighbours looking after each other, facilities for children, local transport, and leisure facilities. This gives a minimum social capital score of -6 and a maximum of 6, with higher total scores indicating greater neighbourhood social capital.

Psychiatric variables

The Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS 4.0) [26], a 24-item scale of psychiatric symptom severity, was used to rate participants’ present condition. Possible scores vary from 24 to 168 with lower scores indicating less severe psychopathology. Good-to-excellent inter-rater reliability of the BPRS has been confirmed in the existing studies [2629]. Following guidance based on factor analysis of the BPRS [30], three subscales were derived for analyses in this paper due to their potential relevance to loneliness: affect subscale (anxiety, depression, suicidality, and guilt), positive symptoms subscale (grandiosity, suspiciousness, hallucinations, and unusual thought content), and negative symptoms subscale (blunted affect, emotional withdrawal, and motor retardation). In addition, number of psychiatric inpatient hospitalisations in a lifetime, number of years, since first contact with mental health services, and participants’ baseline clinical diagnoses were collected.

Procedures

Potentially eligible participants were initially identified by clinicians in CRTs. They were then contacted by clinical staff in CRTs or other community mental health services to introduce the study and establish willingness to be called by study researchers for further discussion. Those who were willing to be contacted by researchers were informed of the content and procedures of the study and were invited to participate. All participants provided their informed consent. After written consent was received, study baseline measures were completed with all consenting participants as a structured interview. Diagnoses of mental health problems were collected from patient clinical records by researchers at the end of recruitment.

Statistical analyses

All data were checked for missing values. The loneliness scale (ULS-8) was completed by all the respondents with 8 (2.0%) having 1–2 items missing. The number of participants who had missing items on the other measures ranged from 1 (0.3%) to 7 (1.8%). As the percentage of missing data was low, case mean substitution was used to replace the missing items with the subject’s mean score based on the items that were present for that subject [31]. This method was only used for ULS-8, BPRS subscales, and social capital, as this technique is appropriate to measures where all items are indicators of a specific concept or construct [31] and the items are parallel and approximately interchangeable [32]. As case mean substitution was reported not to result in highly biased estimates only when less than 25% of item scores and less than 10% of cases are missing [33], 8 cases with more than 25% missing data on BPRS subscales or social capital measures were removed from the corresponding analyses.

Descriptive statistics were obtained for all variables. For variables not normally distributed, medians and interquartile ranges were reported; otherwise, means and standard deviations were reported. With respect to categorical variables, descriptive statistics were reported as frequencies and percentages within each category. A series of univariate linear regression analyses were carried out with baseline loneliness score as dependent variable and the independent variables separately. The residuals of the models were checked; the models were good fit, so variable transformations were not conducted.

Three multivariable linear regression models were carried out to examine the association between loneliness scores and the three sets of explanatory variables. Blocks of independent variables associated at the p < 0.25 level with the outcome variable in univariate linear regression were entered into the multivariable regression models in the following order: (1) socio-demographic variables (age, whether born in the UK, accommodation, employment, and living situation); (2) socio-demographic and psychosocial variables (social network size and social capital); and (3) socio-demographic, psychosocial, and psychiatric variables (number of psychiatric inpatient hospitalisations, number of years since first contact with mental health services, affective symptoms, positive symptoms, negative symptoms, and clinical diagnoses). Psychosis was used as a reference in clinical diagnoses, because the majority of CRT users are people with psychosis, and its association with loneliness may be different from other affective disorders.

All analyses were performed using Stata version 12.1. P values of less than 0.05 were considered significant.

Results

Sample characteristics

The sample at baseline consisted of 399 participants (40.2% male). The median age was 40.0 (IQR 29.9–50.0), with the youngest respondent being 18 years of age and the oldest 75. 53.8% of participants were White British. A large proportion of the sample, 89.7%, had independent accommodation, 46.5% were currently living with a partner or with family, and 16.8% were living with dependent children. In terms of education and employment, 27.4% had a degree, whilst 27.3% were in regular employment. More than one in four participants had a diagnosis of schizophrenia or psychosis (27.0%), 16.3% were diagnosed with bipolar affective disorder/manic episode, 35.0% suffered from depressive/anxiety disorders, 13.3% from personality disorders, and 8.4% from other disorders. The characteristics of respondents are described in Table 1.

Table 1.

Socio-demographic, psychosocial, and psychiatric characteristics of crisis resolution team users

Characteristic N (%) or mean (SD) or median (IQR)
Age (years) 40.0 (29.9–50.0)
Gender
 Male 160 (40.2%)
 Female 238 (59.8%)
Ethnic background
 White British 214 (53.8%)
 White other 40 (10.1%)
 Black/Black British 80 (20.1%)
 Asian/Asian British 37 (9.3%)
 Mixed 27 (6.8%)
Born in the UK
 No 89 (22.7%)
 Yes 304 (77.4%)
Housing
 Independent accommodation 357 (89.7%)
 Other 41 (10.3%)
Contact with children under 16
 Living with dependent children 67 (16.8%)
 Other 332 (83.2%)
Education attainment
 No qualifications 76 (19.1%)
 Other qualifications 213 (53.5%)
 Degree 109 (27.4%)
Employment
 No 257 (64.4%)
 In voluntary, protected, or sheltered work 33 (8.3%)
 In regular employment 109 (27.3%)
Living with a partner or with family
 No 213 (53.5%)
 Yes 185 (46.5%)
 Loneliness (8–32) 22 (19–25)
 Social network size (0–10) 4.9 (2.3)
 Social capital (− 6 to 6) 3 (0–5)
Number of psychiatric inpatient hospitalisations
 Never 148 (37.1%)
 Once 86 (21.6%)
 2–5 times 102 (25.6%)
 More than 5 times 63 (15.8%)
Number of years since first contact with mental health services
 Less than 3 months 67 (16.8%)
 3 months–1 year 39 (9.8%)
 1–2 years 28 (7.0%)
 2–10 years 126 (31.7%)
 More than 10 years 138 (34.7%)
 Affective symptoms (4–28) 12 (8–17)
 Positive symptoms (4–28) 5 (4–8)
 Negative symptoms (3–21) 4 (3–6)
Clinical diagnosis
 Psychosis (schizophrenia and other non-affective psychosis) 106 (27.0%)
 Bipolar affective disorder/manic episode 64 (16.3%)
 Depressive/anxiety disorders 137 (35.0%)
 Personality disorders 52 (13.3%)
 Other disorders 33 (8.4%)

For instruments (loneliness, social network size, social capital, affective symptoms, positive symptoms, and negative symptoms), range of scores is indicated between brackets

N number of participants, SD standard deviation, IQR interquartile range

Severity of loneliness at baseline among people leaving CRTs

The descriptive statistics for ULS-8 items are summarised in Table 2. Over 70% of participants sometimes or always felt that they lacked companionship, left out, isolated from others, unhappy being so withdrawn, and that people were around them but not with them. Data about the frequency and intensity of loneliness experiences were summed to provide a total loneliness score from 8 to 32. The mean total score was 21.9 (SD = 5.0); the total score exhibited slight negative skew. Thus, the median score, 22 (IQR 19–25), is reported in Table 1. Although there is no standard threshold for severe loneliness for ULS-8, a score of 24 is a potential cut-off as it equates to answering “sometimes” to every question. In our sample, 30.6% of participants had a total score over 24 and could be considered as having severe loneliness.

Table 2.

Descriptive statistics for ULS-8 items

Item Never
N (%)
Rarely
N (%)
Sometimes
N (%)
Always
N (%)
1. How often do you feel that you lack companionship? 40 (10.0) 57 (14.3) 205 (51.4) 97 (24.3)
2. How often do you feel that there is no one you can turn to? 60 (15.0) 66 (16.5) 195 (48.9) 78 (19.6)
3. How often do you feel that you are an outgoing person? 59 (14.8) 108 (27.1) 164 (41.1) 68 (17.0)
4. How often do you feel left out? 41 (10.3) 73 (18.3) 194 (48.6) 91 (22.8)
5. How often do you feel isolated from others? 32 (8.0) 61 (15.3) 199 (49.9) 107 (26.8)
6. How often can you find companionship when you want it? 37 (9.3) 71 (17.8) 179 (44.9) 112 (28.1)
7. How often do you feel unhappy being so withdrawn? 34 (8.5) 57 (14.3) 176 (44.1) 132 (33.1)
8. How often do you feel people are around you but not with you? 35 (8.8) 55 (13.8) 195 (48.9) 114 (28.6)

ULS-8 UCLA Loneliness Scale-8, N number of participants

Factors associated with loneliness in people experiencing mental health crises

The results of univariate linear regression analyses investigating factors associated with baseline loneliness are shown in Table 3. Feelings of loneliness were more intense in people for whom it had been 2–10 years since first contact with mental health services compared to those in contact with services for less than 3 months. Greater severity of loneliness was also associated with more severe affective symptoms, positive symptoms or negative symptoms, and with diagnosis of depressive/anxiety disorder, personality disorder, or other disorders as opposed to psychosis. Lower levels of loneliness were associated with bigger social network size, more neighbourhood social capital, and having been hospitalised more than once (with the never hospitalised as a reference group).

Table 3.

Results of univariate linear regression analyses for factors associated with baseline loneliness

Variables Coefficienta 95% CI P valueb
Socio-demographic variables
Age (years) − 0.03 − 0.07 to 0.01 0.11
Gender (0 = male; 1 = female) 0.39 − 0.62 to 1.39 0.45
Ethnic background
 White British Reference
White other − 0.13 − 1.82 to 1.57 0.88
 Black/Black British − 0.44 − 1.73 to 0.85 0.50
 Asian/Asian British 0.62 − 1.13 to 2.37 0.49
 Mixed − 0.83 − 2.84 to 1.18 0.42
Born in the UK (0 = no, 1 = yes) 1.13 − 0.04 to 2.31 0.06
Housing (0 = other, 1 = independent accommodation) − 1.08 − 2.70 to 0.53 0.19
Contact with children under 16 (0 = other; 1 = living with dependent children) − 0.73 − 2.05 to 0.58 0.27
Education attainment
 No qualifications Reference
 Other qualifications − 0.11 − 1.42 to 1.20 0.87
 Degree 0.23 − 1.23 to 1.70 0.76
Employment
 No Reference
 In voluntary, protected or sheltered work − 0.25 − 2.06 to 1.56 0.79
 In regular employment − 1.00 − 2.12 to 0.12 0.08
Living with a partner or with family (0 = no; 1 = yes) − 0.94 − 1.93 to 0.04 0.06
Psychosocial variables
Social network size (two items from LSNS-6) − 0.79 − 1.00 to − 0.59 < 0.001
Social capital (HLSSC) − 0.52 − 0.68 to − 0.36 < 0.001
Psychiatric variables
Number of psychiatric inpatient hospitalisations
 Never Reference
 Once − 1.29 − 2.60 to 0.03 0.06
 2–5 times − 1.46 − 2.71 to − 0.21 0.02
 More than 5 times − 2.13 − 3.59 to − 0.67 0.004
Number of years since first contact with mental health services
 Less than 3 months Reference
 3 months–1 year 1.37 − 0.59 to 3.34 0.17
 1–2 years 1.80 − 0.39 to 4.00 0.11
 2–10 years 1.58 0.10 to 3.05 0.04
 More than 10 years 1.23 − 0.23 to 2.68 0.10
Affective symptoms (four items from BPRS) 0.45 0.38 to 0.53 < 0.001
Positive symptoms (four items from BPRS) 0.24 0.12 to 0.35 < 0.001
Negative symptoms (three items from BPRS) 0.38 0.17 to 0.60 0.001
Clinical diagnosis
 Psychosis (schizophrenia and other non-affective psychosis) Reference
 Bipolar affective disorder/manic episode − 0.85 − 2.37 to 0.67 0.27
 Depressive/anxiety disorders 1.97 0.73 to 3.21 0.002
 Personality disorders 2.60 0.98 to 4.23 0.002
 Other disorders 1.93 0.01 to 3.84 0.048

CI confidence interval, LSNS-6 Lubben Social Network Scale-6, HLSSC Health and Lifestyles Survey Social Capital Questionnaire, BPRS Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale

aNegative regression coefficient = less loneliness

bSignificant p values printed in bold

Table 4 presents the results of multivariable linear regression analyses for factors associated with baseline loneliness. Model 1, including only socio-demographic variables, explained 2.0% of the variance in loneliness. The only factor significantly associated with greater loneliness was younger age. After adding psychosocial variables to socio-demographics in model 2, the amount of variance explained rose to 18.1%. In this model, greater loneliness was related to younger age, being born in the UK, smaller social network size, and lower neighbourhood social capital. Model 3, where all the three blocks of independent variables were entered into the regression equation, explained a total of 36.2% of the variation in loneliness. In this final model, more severe feelings of loneliness proved to be significantly associated with more than 2 years since first contact with mental health services (compared with fewer than 3 months), and with more severe affective symptoms. Lower levels of loneliness were associated with bigger social network size, greater neighbourhood social capital, and more than 5 psychiatric inpatient hospitalisations compared with no hospitalisations. After adjusting for clinical variables, the association of loneliness with age was no longer significant and the association with being born in the UK became borderline significant.

Table 4.

Results of multivariable linear regression analyses for factors associated with baseline lonelinessa

Variables Model 1 Model 2 Model 3
Coefficientb 95% CI P valuec Coefficient 95% CI P value Coefficient 95% CI p value
Socio-demographic variables
Age (years) − 0.04 − 0.08 to − 0.001 0.045 − 0.05 − 0.09 to − 0.02 0.01 − 0.03 − 0.06 to 0.01 0.17
Born in the UK (0 = no; 1 = yes) 0.98 − 0.21 to 2.16 0.11 1.12 0.03 to 2.22 0.045 1.00 − 0.02 to 2.02 0.05
Housing (0 = other; 1 = independent accommodation) − 0.56 − 2.21 to 1.09 0.51 0.12 − 1.42 to 1.66 0.88 0.45 − 1.00 to 1.90 0.54
Employment
 No Reference
 In voluntary, protected, or sheltered work − 0.32 − 2.16 to 1.52 0.73 0.23 − 1.45 to 1.90 0.79 0.66 − 0.85 to 2.17 0.39
 In regular employment − 1.00 − 2.15 to 0.15 0.09 − 0.04 − 1.11 to 1.03 0.94 − 0.16 − 1.19 to 0.87 0.76
Living with a partner or with family (0 = no; 1 = yes) − 0.87 − 1.89 to 0.15 0.10 − 0.36 − 1.30 to 0.58 0.46 − 0.47 − 1.37 to 0.42 0.30
Psychosocial variables
Social network size (two items from LSNS-6) − 0.71 − 0.93 to − 0.50 < 0.001 − 0.56 − 0.76 to − 0.36 < 0.001
Social capital (HLSSC) − 0.37 − 0.53 to − 0.20 < 0.001 − 0.16 − 0.31 to − 0.003 0.046
Psychiatric variables
Number of psychiatric inpatient hospitalisations
 Never Reference
 Once − 0.97 − 2.09 to 0.16 0.09
 2–5 times − 1.21 − 2.49 to 0.06 0.06
 More than 5 times − 1.82 − 3.38 to − 0.26 0.02
Number of years since first contact with mental health services
 Less than 3 months Reference
 3 months–1 year 1.39 − 0.30 to 3.09 0.11
 1–2 years 1.91 − 0.01 to 3.82 0.05
 2–10 years 1.83 0.49 to 3.16 0.01
 More than 10 years 1.91 0.46 to 3.36 0.01
Affective symptoms (four items from BPRS) 0.32 0.23 to 0.40 < 0.001
Positive symptoms (four items from BPRS) 0.10 − 0.01 to 0.21 0.07
Negative symptoms (three items from BPRS) 0.18 − 0.02 to 0.38 0.07
Clinical diagnosis
 Psychosis Reference
 Bipolar affective disorder/manic episode − 0.60 − 2.00 to 0.80 0.40
 Depressive/anxiety disorders 0.95 − 0.39 to 2.29 0.17
 Personality disorders − 0.01 − 1.47 to 1.46 0.99
 Other disorders 0.39 − 1.41 to 2.18 0.67
Radj2 0.020 0.181 0.362

CI confidence interval, LSNS-6 Lubben Social Network Scale-6, HLSSC Health and Lifestyles Survey Social Capital Questionnaire, BPRS Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale, Radj2 = adjusted-R2

aUsing multivariable linear regression analyses with loneliness score at baseline as dependent variable and factors with p < 0.25 in univariate linear regression as independent variables

bNegative regression coefficient = less loneliness

cSignificant p values printed in bold

Discussion

When compared with studies which used the same loneliness scale, levels of loneliness in this sample of CRT users seemed to be considerably greater among people experiencing mental health crises (M = 21.9, SD = 5.0) than among young adults in two community-based samples of 19–39 years old from the 1996 Niagara Young Adult Study (M 15.78–16.08 and SD 5.08–5.27) [34] and Italian community-dwelling older adults of 65–89 years old from the Act on Ageing project (M = 13.1, SD = 6.9) [35], although their age ranges did not match the ages of our sample (18–75 years old). Since the mean score for loneliness in our sample was much higher than that in the two general population samples mentioned and this finding was consistent with existing research [9, 36], it is reasonable to conclude that feelings of loneliness were more prevalent amongst mental health community crisis service users than in the general population. In mental health context, the severity of loneliness in our sample (M = 21.9, SD = 5.0) was comparable to adults with social anxiety disorder (M 23.68–25.07 and SD 2.79–4.73) [37] and adults with autism spectrum disorders (M = 20.9 and SD = 4.7) [38].

A second aim of this paper was to identify factors independently associated with loneliness among individuals experiencing mental health crises. In the first multivariable regression model with socio-demographic characteristics as explanatory variables, younger age was associated with increased loneliness. This result coheres with the BBC’s Loneliness Experiment which reported 16–24 years old as the group who feel loneliest among 55,000 members of the British public [39]. However, only 2% of the variance in loneliness was explained in the first model, and in the final model, loneliness did not have significant association with any of the socio-demographics after adjusting for psychosocial and psychiatric variables. This finding is consistent with the previous research in people with psychosis [9, 36, 40], although socio-demographic characteristics have been reported as linked to individual differences in loneliness in studies of general population [41, 42]. It is possible that the social impact of having a significant mental health problem is sufficiently severe that it overrides other socio-demographic factors which might, otherwise, be expected to make a difference in loneliness.

As psychosocial variables, social network size and neighbourhood social capital were found to be inversely related to the severity of loneliness, explaining 18.1% of the variance in loneliness together with socio-demographic factors. The finding coheres with the existing studies showing that objective measures of social relations and perception of community-level structures or characteristics may affect the severity of loneliness [43, 44], although the direction of causality cannot be inferred. Communities with poor social capital may predispose an individual to loneliness, or lonely people may find it more difficult to recognise community social resources. As the proportion of variance explained is relatively low, the findings also suggest that there are some major influences on loneliness that are not captured by social network size and neighbourhood social capital.

After adding psychiatric variables to the final regression model, the explained variance in loneliness increased to 36.2%. It is surprising that having had more than five psychiatric admissions was associated with less severe loneliness. This result is contradictory to the previous research which reported that a greater number of psychiatric inpatient admissions were related to more intense feelings of loneliness [36]. As we explored associations between loneliness and many variables, this may be just a chance finding. It is also possible that peer support developed during psychiatric hospitalisations and, perhaps, lasted beyond admission, or that having multiple admissions might be a marker for a group of people who were relatively intensively engaged with services and so more likely to have their needs addressed. If this finding is replicated in other studies, future research could include mixed method exploration of whether there are potential mechanisms to underpin an association between hospitalisations and social support and loneliness. It is more intuitively understandable that longer years since first contact with mental health services were related to greater loneliness. People may withdraw from social connections or lose intimate or confiding relationships cumulatively through the course of an enduring mental illness. It may also be that people’s positive social identities and hopefulness about restoring relationships erode over time, which may increase feelings of loneliness. The emotional impact of relapse may be greater every time people experience mental health relapse and this may impact on feelings of loneliness. More severe affective symptoms (a summary score including symptoms of anxiety, depression, suicidality, and guilt) were associated with greater loneliness, while severity of positive symptoms or that of negative symptoms of psychosis had no association with loneliness. A recent systematic review found contradictory results about the relationship between loneliness and psychotic symptomatology [45], but findings from a meta-analysis reported a moderate association between loneliness and psychosis [46]. The relationship between depressive symptoms and loneliness was stronger and more consistent in the previous studies. Adults with depressive episode were around 11 times more likely to feel lonely compared to those with no mental disorder [8]. Greater loneliness was also reported to be associated with more severe depressive symptoms and poorer remission from depression in a systematic review [47]. Masi and colleagues [48] described a regulatory loop model of loneliness, in which people who are lonely tend to have negative and biased social cognitions [49]. These cognitions are likely to get people involved in behavioural confirmation processes which generate more negative social interactions and elicit further confirmation of their poor social value, and, finally, result in greater loneliness [48, 49]. People with depression also tend to have negative bias in thinking processes which leads to negative patterns of behaviour [50]. The two factors together with biological processes, stressors, and interpersonal factors interact with one another and form a “negative downward loop” which pushes people further into depression [51]. Therefore, a plausible hypothesis is that loneliness and depression may be involved in a double feedback loop reinforcing each other [52]. In the multivariable model, loneliness was not associated with participants’ diagnoses. A possible explanation is that the impact of these diagnoses on loneliness was largely due to the differences in severity of symptoms. We used the BPRS to rate participants’ current symptoms which appeared to have a greater effect on feelings of loneliness than their long-term diagnoses.

Limitations of the study

The results of this study should be interpreted with a number of limitations in mind. First, the cross-sectional design of this paper makes it impossible to examine causal relationships between loneliness and its correlates. Second, the participants were assessed immediately post-crisis which is a very particular time. They may experience more severe loneliness during the long-term recovery journey than during crisis. Third, the recruitment rate of 23.6% of identified eligible potential participants was low and might have resulted in some degree of selection bias. The low recruitment rate in our sample may be related to the mental health crises that the service users had just experienced before recruitment, which leads to the concern that people with severe symptoms may disproportionately fall into the nonresponse group, and, thus, results should be interpreted with some caution. Finally, our sample may underrepresent people who were unwilling to participate in a trial generally, or to whom the specific intervention which we were offering did not appeal. This might introduce some biases that might be absent in a straightforward epidemiological study.

Clinical and policy implications

The present study has some important implications for practice and policy. The high prevalence of loneliness is a serious problem for which we need to seek solutions considering its adverse physical and mental health consequences as well as its impact on resource use [47, 53]. Although loneliness is common in mental health community crisis service users, it may be overlooked in clinical environment due to lack of awareness among practitioners, or due to the practitioners feeling, they may not be able to address any needs identified, or avoiding the subject for fear it will be perceived as intrusive or upsetting by the service users. They may also not consider that it is their responsibility to help with this [54]. It may be helpful for mental health professionals to screen for the severity of loneliness to identify people at risk, given that loneliness may influence mental health in both immediate and long term [8, 36, 55, 56]. The study also showed that individuals with severe affective symptoms or with a long history of mental illnesses were especially prone to loneliness and thus need further attention from clinicians. A validated short screening tool can potentially provide practitioners a time and cost-effective approach to identify people in need, e.g., the De Jong Gierveld 6-Item Loneliness Scale [57] and the UCLA 3-Item Loneliness Scale [58] which are short and possess robust psychometric properties. Evaluation is helpful to show that practitioners are sincerely helping people who turn to the services and even a simple question asking people whether they are lonely may make an impact.

It is important to create a culture in mental health services where loneliness is viewed as a valid area to discuss and try to tackle, and for practitioners to work with service users to maximise available support from families and local community resources to address loneliness. More importantly, the high prevalence of loneliness among service users is not only an individual but also a community and societal level problem, and macrosocial factors have been proposed as significant determinants of levels and content of social relationships [59]. Therefore, it is imperative to advance public awareness of the importance of reducing loneliness, and ultimately to urge policy level changes to support disadvantaged populations. The Campaign to End Loneliness took the first steps towards diminishing loneliness and inspiring national, regional, and local organisations, and people to deal with its health threat in older age [60]. The Campaign utilises a partnership approach, and joint projects are delivered by various organisations to raise awareness of loneliness, identify and develop effective loneliness interventions, and establish a reliable lifelong network of support [60]. However, the Campaign did not specifically focus on mental health service users and has not been robustly evaluated. Therefore, interventions targeting mental health service users need to be developed and tested, considering the great social impact of having mental health problems. A similar collaborative approach could be conducted to inspire organisations and people to tackle loneliness issues as a mental health priority at the level of communities and societies, which might be the most effective approach. Nonetheless, we would need to be confident that mental health service users, for whom stigma and social exclusion are problems, will also benefit from similar approaches designed for the general population.

Research implications

In terms of research implications, scientists should be trying to understand the phenomenon of loneliness and potential strategies to address it more from service users’ perspective. For example, the most widely used loneliness measures were initially designed for the general population [61, 62]. We cannot be sure whether these measures are perceived by service users as a meaningful reflection of their experiences. Perhaps, new measures, respectively, targeting children/young people and adults could be produced and interventions could be designed in collaboration with service users. In addition, more longitudinal research is needed to untangle the direction of effect in the relationship between loneliness and poor outcomes as well as the mechanisms through which loneliness affects mental health. We also need more longitudinal research to understand the course of loneliness for people with severe mental illness. Loneliness is not such a serious problem if it is short term: this study suggests that loneliness may typically get worse during the course of someone’s contact with mental health services, but understanding the extent of chronic loneliness and the characteristics of those affected by mental health crises is necessary to inform appropriate interventions.

To conclude, this study supports a view that people experiencing mental health crises have more severe loneliness than the general population. The quantitative investigation also highlighted the strong association between greater loneliness and small social network size, limited social capital, more severe affective symptoms, and long-term mental illness history. Therefore, interventions targeting loneliness and mental health care professionals should pay more attention to people at particular risk of being lonely.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to acknowledge financial support for the CORE Study from the National Institute for Health Research under its Programme Grants for Applied Research programme (Reference RP-PG-0109-10078). The contents of the manuscript are the sole responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the official views of the NHS, the National Institute for Health Research, or the Department of Health. We are grateful to participants who completed the assessment, to staff from the six NHS Trusts where participants were recruited, and to the CORE research team who collected the baseline data. SJ and BLE are partly supported by the NIHR Mental Health Policy Research Unit and the NIHR UCLH Biomedical Research Centre. SJ leads and BLE is a co-applicant in the UKRI Loneliness and Social Isolation in Mental Health Network.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Footnotes

This article is part of the focused issue ‘Loneliness: contemporary insights on causes, correlates, and consequences’.

References

  • 1.Peplau LA, Perlman D. Loneliness: a sourcebook of current theory, research, and therapy. New York: Wiley; 1982. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Peplau LA, Perlman D. Blueprint for a social psychological theory of loneliness. In: Cook M, Wilson G, editors. Love and attraction. England: Pergamon, Oxford; 1979. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Sermat V. Sources of loneliness. Essence. 1978;2(4):271–276. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Jopling K, Sserwanja I. Loneliness across the life course: a rapid review of the evidence. UK Branch: Calouste Gulbenkian Foundation; 2016. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Borge L, Martinsen EW, Ruud T, Watne O, Friis S. Quality of life, loneliness, and social contact among long-term psychiatric patients. Psychiatr Serv. 1999;50(1):81–84. doi: 10.1176/ps.50.1.81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Lauder W, Sharkey S, Mummery K. A community survey of loneliness. J Adv Nurs. 2004;46(1):88–94. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2648.2003.02968.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Victor CR, Yang KM. The prevalence of loneliness among adults: a case study of the United Kingdom. J Psychol. 2012;146(1–2):85–104. doi: 10.1080/00223980.2011.613875. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Meltzer H, Bebbington P, Dennis MS, Jenkins R, McManus S, Brugha TS. Feelings of loneliness among adults with mental disorder. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2013;48(1):5–13. doi: 10.1007/s00127-012-0515-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Badcock JC, Shah S, Mackinnon A, Stain HJ, Galletly C, Jablensky A, Morgan VA. Loneliness in psychotic disorders and its association with cognitive function and symptom profile. Schizophr Res. 2015;169(1–3):268–273. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2015.10.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Richard A, Rohrmann S, Vandeleur CL, Schmid M, Barth J, Eichholzer M. Loneliness is adversely associated with physical and mental health and lifestyle factors: results from a Swiss national survey. PLoS One. 2017;12(7):e0181442. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0181442. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Hamilton NG, Ponzoha CA, Cutler DL, Weigel RM. Social networks and negative versus positive symptoms of Schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 1989;15(4):625–633. doi: 10.1093/schbul/15.4.625. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Bradshaw T, Haddock G. Is befriending by trained volunteers of value to people suffering from long-term mental illness? J Adv Nurs. 1998;27(4):713–720. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2648.1998.00618.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Dill JC, Anderson CA. Loneliness, shyness, and depression: the etiology and interrelationships of everyday problems in living. In: Joiner T, Coyne JC, editors. The interactional nature of depression: advances in interpersonal approaches. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 1999. pp. 93–125. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Weeks DG, Michela JL, Peplau LA, Bragg ME. Relation between loneliness and depression: a structural equation analysis. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1980;39(6):1238–1244. doi: 10.1037/h0077709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Perese EF, Wolf M. Combating loneliness among persons with severe mental illness: social network interventions’ characteristics, effectiveness, and applicability. Issue Mental Health Nurs. 2005;26(6):591–609. doi: 10.1080/01612840590959425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Morgan C, Burns T, Fitzpatrick R, Pinfold V, Priebe S. Social exclusion and mental health: conceptual and methodological review. Br J Psychiatry. 2007;191:477–483. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.106.034942. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Do H. Crisis resolution/home treatment teams. The Mental Health Policy Implementation Guide. London: Department of Health; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Johnson S, Lamb D, Marston L, Osborn D, Mason O, Henderson C, Ambler G, Milton A, Davidson M, Christoforou M, Sullivan S, Hunter R, Hindle D, Paterson B, Leverton M, Piotrowski J, Forsyth R, Mosse L, Goater N, Kelly K, Lean M, Pilling S, Morant N, Lloyd-Evans B. Peer-supported self-management for people discharged from a mental health crisis team: a randomised controlled trial. Lancet (Lond Engl) 2018;392(10145):409–418. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(18)31470-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Hays RD, DiMatteo MR. A short-form measure of loneliness. J Pers Assess. 1987;51(1):69–81. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa5101_6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Russell D, Peplau LA, Cutrona CE. The revised UCLA Loneliness Scale: concurrent and discriminant validity evidence. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1980;39(3):472–480. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.39.3.472. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Wu C-H, Yao G. Psychometric analysis of the short-form UCLA Loneliness Scale (ULS-8) in Taiwanese undergraduate students. Pers Individ Differ. 2008;44(8):1762–1771. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2008.02.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Dogan T, Cotok NA, Tekin EG, Elsevier Science BV (2011) Reliability and validity of the Turkish Version of the UCLA Loneliness Scale (ULS-8) among university students. In: 3rd World conference on educational sciences, vol 15, procedia social and behavioral sciences, pp 2058–2062. 10.1016/j.sbspro.2011.04.053
  • 23.Cramer KM, Barry JE. Conceptualizations and measures of loneliness: a comparison of subscales. Pers Individ Differ. 1999;27(3):491–502. doi: 10.1016/s0191-8869(98)00257-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Lubben J, Blozik E, Gillmann G, Iliffe S, Kruse WV, Beck JC, Stuck AE. Performance of an abbreviated version of the Lubben Social Network Scale among three European community-dwelling older adult populations. Gerontologist. 2006;46(4):503–513. doi: 10.1093/geront/46.4.503. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Authority HE . Health and lifestyles: a survey of the UK population—Part 1. London: Health Education Authority; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Ventura J, Green MF, Shaner A, Liberman RP. Training and quality assurance with the brief psychiatric rating-scale—the drift busters. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 1993;3(4):221–244. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Bech P, Larsen JK, Andersen J. The BPRS—pshchometric developments. Psychopharmacol Bull. 1988;24(1):118–121. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Tarell JD, Schulz SC. Nursing assessment using the BRPS—a structured interview. Psychopharmacol Bull. 1988;24(1):105–111. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Roncone R, Ventura J, Impallomeni M, Falloon IRH, Morosini PL, Chiaravalle E, Casacchia M. Reliability of an Italian standardized and expanded Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS 4.0) in raters with high vs. low clinical experience. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1999;100(3):229–236. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1999.tb10850.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Dazzi F, Shafer A, Lauriola M. Meta-analysis of the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale—Expanded (BPRS-E) structure and arguments for a new version. J Psychiatr Res. 2016;81:140–151. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2016.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Fox-Wasylyshyn SM, El-Masri MM. Handling missing data in self-report measures. Res Nurs Health. 2005;28(6):488–495. doi: 10.1002/nur.20100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Newman DA. Missing data: five practical guidelines. Org Res Methods. 2014;17(4):372–411. doi: 10.1177/1094428114548590. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Eekhout I, de Vet HCW, Twisk JWR, Brand JPL, de Boer MR, Heymans MW. Missing data in a multi-item instrument were best handled by multiple imputation at the item score level. J Clin Epidemiol. 2014;67(3):335–342. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2013.09.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Bonin MF, McCreary DR, Sadava SW. Problem drinking behavior in two community-based samples of adults: influence of gender, coping, loneliness, and depression. Psychol Addict Behav. 2000;14(2):151–161. doi: 10.1037/0893-164x.14.2.151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Mulasso A, Roppolo M, Giannotta F, Rabaglietti E (2016) Associations of frailty and psychosocial factors with autonomy in daily activities: a cross-sectional study in Italian community-dwelling older adults. Clin Interv Aging. 10.2147/cia.s95162 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 36.Chrostek A, Grygiel P, Anczewska M, Wciorka J, Switaj P. The intensity and correlates of the feelings of loneliness in people with psychosis. Compr Psychiat. 2016;70:190–199. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2016.07.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Mazurek MO. Loneliness, friendship, and well-being in adults with autism spectrum disorders. Autism. 2014;18(3):223–232. doi: 10.1177/1362361312474121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Jazaieri H, Goldin PR, Werner K, Ziv M, Gross JJ. A Randomized Trial of MBSR versus aerobic exercise for social anxiety disorder. J Clin Psychol. 2012;68(7):715–731. doi: 10.1002/jclp.21863. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Hammond C, Qualter P, Victor C, Barreto M (2018) Who feels lonely? The results of the world’s largest loneliness study. BBC Radio 4 website. https://www.bbc.co.uk/programmes/articles/2yzhfv4DvqVp5nZyxBD8G23/who-feels-lonely-the-results-of-the-world-s-largest-loneliness-study. Accessed 22 Feb 2019
  • 40.Shioda A, Tadaka E, Okochi A. Loneliness and related factors among people with schizophrenia in Japan: a cross-sectional study. J Psychiatr Ment Health Nurs. 2016;23(6–7):399–408. doi: 10.1111/jpm.12318. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Victor CR, Scambler S, Bowling A, Bond J. The prevalence of and risk factors for, loneliness in later life: a survey of older people in Great Britain. Ageing Soc. 2005;25:357–375. doi: 10.1017/s0144686x04003332. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Cohen-Mansfield J, Hazan H, Lerman Y, Shalom V. Correlates and predictors of loneliness in older-adults: a review of quantitative results informed by qualitative insights. Int Psychogeriatr. 2016;28(4):557–576. doi: 10.1017/s1041610215001532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Gierveld JD, Keating N, Fast JE. Determinants of loneliness among older adults in Canada. Can J Aging Rev Can Vieil. 2015;34(2):125–136. doi: 10.1017/s0714980815000070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Coll-Planas L, Gomez GD, Bonilla P, Masat T, Puig T, Monteserin R. Promoting social capital to alleviate loneliness and improve health among older people in Spain. Health Soc Care Community. 2017;25(1):145–157. doi: 10.1111/hsc.12284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Lim MH, Gleeson JFM, Alvarez-Jimenez M, Penn DL. Loneliness in psychosis: a systematic review. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2018;53(3):221–238. doi: 10.1007/s00127-018-1482-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Michalska da Rocha B, Rhodes S, Vasilopoulou E, Hutton P. Loneliness in psychosis: a meta-analytical review. Schizophr Bull. 2018;44(1):114–125. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbx036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Wang JY, Mann F, Lloyd-Evans B, Ma RM, Johnson S. Associations between loneliness and perceived social support and outcomes of mental health problems: a systematic review. BMC Psychiatry. 2018;18(1):156. doi: 10.1186/s12888-018-1736-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Masi CM, Chen HY, Hawkley LC, Cacioppo JT. A meta-analysis of interventions to reduce loneliness. Personal Soc Psychol Rev. 2011;15(3):219–266. doi: 10.1177/1088868310377394. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Cacioppo JT, Hawkley LC. Perceived social isolation and cognition. Trends Cognitive Sci. 2009;13(10):447–454. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.06.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Beck AT. Depression: causes and treatment. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press; 1967. [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Schotte CKW, Van den Bossche B, De Doncker D, Claes S, Cosyns P. A biopsychosocial model as a guide for psychoeducation and treatment of depression. Depress Anxiety. 2006;23(5):312–324. doi: 10.1002/da.20177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Holwerda T, Deeg D, Beekman A, Schoevers R, Van Tilburg T, Schutter N. Loneliness as a social marker for depression. Gerontologist. 2016;56:37. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Holt-Lunstad J, Smith TB, Baker M, Harris T, Stephenson D. Loneliness and social isolation as risk factors for mortality: a meta-analytic review. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2015;10(2):227–237. doi: 10.1177/1745691614568352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Pinfold V, Sweet D, Porter I, Quinn C, Byng R, Griffiths C, Billsborough J, Enki DG, Chandler R, Webber M, Larsen J, Carpenter J, Huxley P (2015) Health services and delivery research. In: Improving community health networks for people with severe mental illness: a case study investigation. NIHR Journals Library, Southampton (UK). 10.3310/hsdr03050 [PubMed]
  • 55.Holvast F, Burger H, de Waal MMW, van Marwijk HWJ, Comijs HC, Verhaak PFM. Loneliness is associated with poor prognosis in late-life depression: longitudinal analysis of the Netherlands study of depression in older persons. J Affect Disord. 2015;185:1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.06.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.van Beljouw IM, Verhaak PF, Cuijpers P, van Marwijk HW, Penninx BW. The course of untreated anxiety and depression, and determinants of poor one-year outcome: a one-year cohort study. BMC Psychiatry. 2010;10:86. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-10-86. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Gierveld JD, Van Tilburg T. A 6-item scale for overall, emotional, and social loneliness—confirmatory tests on survey data. Res Aging. 2006;28(5):582–598. doi: 10.1177/0164027506289723. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Hughes ME, Waite LJ, Hawkley LC, Cacioppo JT. A short scale for measuring loneliness in large surveys—results from two population-based studies. Res Aging. 2004;26(6):655–672. doi: 10.1177/0164027504268574. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.House JS, Umberson D, Landis KR. Structures and processes of social support. Ann Rev Sociol. 1988;14:293–318. doi: 10.1146/annurev.so.14.080188.001453. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Ferguson L. The campaign to end loneliness. Work Older People. 2011;15(2):66–70. doi: 10.1108/13663661111144772. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Russell D, Peplau LA, Cutrona CE. The revised Ucla Loneliness Scale—concurrent and discriminant validity evidence. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1980;39(3):472–480. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.39.3.472. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.de Jong-Gierveld J, Kamphuis F. The development of a Rasch-Type Loneliness Scale. Appl Psychol Meas. 1985;9(3):289–299. doi: 10.1177/014662168500900307. [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology are provided here courtesy of Springer

RESOURCES