Abstract
Studies of selective attention typically consider the role of task goals or physical salience, but attention can also be captured by previously reward-associated stimuli, even if they are currently task irrelevant. One theory underlying this value-driven attentional capture (VDAC) is that reward-associated stimulus representations undergo plasticity in sensory cortex, thereby automatically capturing attention during early processing. To test this, we used magnetoencephalography to probe whether stimulus location and identity representations in sensory cortex are modulated by reward learning. We furthermore investigated the time course of these neural effects, and their relationship to behavioral VDAC. Male and female human participants first learned stimulus–reward associations. Next, we measured VDAC in a separate task by presenting these stimuli in the absence of reward contingency and probing their effects on the processing of separate target stimuli presented at different time lags. Using time-resolved multivariate pattern analysis, we found that learned value modulated the spatial selection of previously rewarded stimuli in posterior visual and parietal cortex from ∼260 ms after stimulus onset. This value modulation was related to the strength of participants' behavioral VDAC effect and persisted into subsequent target processing. Importantly, learned value did not influence cortical signatures of early processing (i.e., earlier than ∼200 ms); nor did it influence the decodability of stimulus identity. Our results suggest that VDAC is underpinned by learned value signals that modulate spatial selection throughout posterior visual and parietal cortex. We further suggest that VDAC can occur in the absence of changes in early visual processing in cortex.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Attention is our ability to focus on relevant information at the expense of irrelevant information. It can be affected by previously learned but currently irrelevant stimulus–reward associations, a phenomenon termed “value-driven attentional capture” (VDAC). The neural mechanisms underlying VDAC remain unclear. It has been speculated that reward learning induces visual cortical plasticity, which modulates early visual processing to capture attention. Although we find that learned value modulates spatial signals in visual cortical areas, an effect that correlates with VDAC, we find no relevant signatures of changes in early visual processing in cortex.
Keywords: attention, decoding, learning, magnetoencephalography, reward, value
Introduction
Factors influencing selective attention have been traditionally categorized as stemming either from task demands (e.g., a red book will capture attention if one is searching for red books) or from physical salience (e.g., a red book among blue books will automatically capture attention; Desimone and Duncan, 1995; Egeth and Yantis, 1997; Beck and Kastner, 2009). Recently, reward history (e.g., the learned association of a red logo with a rewarding experience) has also been shown to play an important role in shaping attention (Awh et al., 2012; Anderson, 2013; Chelazzi et al., 2013). This has been demonstrated in experiments using an initial “training phase” in which visual stimuli are associated with different reward outcomes. During a later “testing phase,” participants complete a new, formally unrelated task, which reuses the previously reward-associated stimuli (Anderson et al., 2011). Critically, these stimuli continue to capture attention even in the absence of any reward contingency, physical salience, or task relevance, a phenomenon termed value-driven attentional capture (VDAC; Raymond and O'Brien, 2009; Anderson et al., 2011; Theeuwes and Belopolsky, 2012; Chelazzi et al., 2014). VDAC is a prime example of how attention is influenced by prior experience; understanding its neural mechanisms is therefore instrumental to bridging the fields of learning and attention (Anderson, 2016; Le Pelley et al., 2016; Failing and Theeuwes, 2018).
The behavioral effects of VDAC imply learning-related plasticity, but the nature of these changes remains unclear (Chelazzi et al., 2013; Anderson, 2016; Failing and Theeuwes, 2018). Some have suggested that stimulus feature representations in sensory cortex may undergo reward-mediated plasticity during learning (Anderson, 2016; van Koningsbruggen et al., 2016; Hickey and Peelen, 2017; Failing and Theeuwes, 2018). Strengthened cortical representations could then rapidly capture attention during early visual processing, potentially leading to the behavioral effects of VDAC (Anderson, 2016; Failing and Theeuwes, 2018). However, evidence for the existence of this mechanism is limited (Anderson, 2016; van Koningsbruggen et al., 2016).
Electroencephalography (EEG) studies of VDAC show various types of value modulations in visual processing (Failing and Theeuwes, 2018). The timing of these effects matters (Luck et al., 2000). Early effects may indicate changes in how reward-associated stimuli are initially processed in sensory cortex, consistent with the visual cortical plasticity account (Anderson, 2016; van Koningsbruggen et al., 2016; Failing and Theeuwes, 2018). In contrast, later effects may reflect attentional modulation triggered by these stimuli, consistent with learned value signals originating elsewhere (Foxe and Simpson, 2002; Buffalo et al., 2010; Itthipuripat et al., 2019). Hickey et al. (2010) and Luque et al. (2017) both report an enhancement of the early sensory-evoked P1 component for reward-associated stimuli, suggesting that value modulates early visual processing of these stimuli. In contrast, Qi et al. (2013) report that reward-associated stimuli only evoked the later N2pc component, reflecting an attentional shift toward these stimuli. Others find that reward-associated distracters affect the processing of competing stimuli, reducing the P1, N1, and N2pc (Itthipuripat et al., 2015; MacLean and Giesbrecht, 2015a). Thus, existing evidence on the timing of value effects during VDAC is mixed.
Comparison between studies is additionally challenging because in some studies the task relevance, and therefore the reward history, of stimuli changes across trials (Hickey et al., 2010; Itthipuripat et al., 2015). In other studies, reward-associated stimuli are irrelevant and unrewarded for the entire task (MacLean and Giesbrecht, 2015a; Qi et al., 2013). Furthermore, it is important to isolate effects related to the processing of reward-associated stimuli from those related to the processing of competing target stimuli (Hickey and Peelen, 2015; Itthipuripat et al., 2019).
Finally, most prior studies have examined the spatial discriminability of reward-associated stimuli, for example, by testing for changes in the lateralized P1 and N2pc components (Hickey et al., 2010). However, plasticity-induced strengthening of stimulus representations could also manifest in altered neural discriminability of stimulus identity (Hickey and Peelen, 2015; Poort et al., 2015; LeMessurier and Feldman, 2018). This has not been demonstrated in the context of VDAC using time-resolved methods. Hickey and Peelen (2015) used multivariate pattern analysis (MVPA) to show that fMRI representations of previously reward-associated stimuli are indeed modulated in visual cortex, although the use of fMRI precluded insight into the timing of these effects.
In the current study, we used time-resolved MVPA of magnetoencephalography (MEG) data to probe stimulus location and identity representations in sensory cortex during VDAC. After participants learned stimulus–reward associations, we measured VDAC in a separate task by presenting these stimuli in the absence of reward contingency, and probing their effects on the processing of separate target stimuli. Critically, we included an interval between previously reward-associated stimuli and the imperative targets to isolate the neural processes triggered by the reward stimuli, uncontaminated by target processing. Consistent with VDAC, we found that learned value modulated the spatial selection of previously rewarded stimuli in posterior visual and parietal cortex. However, these effects only emerged from ∼260 ms after stimulus onset, in contrast to previous evidence of early effects. Moreover, learned value did not modulate identity representations of the previously reward-associated stimuli in cortex.
Materials and Methods
Participants.
All experiments were conducted under ethical approval from the Central University Research Ethics Committee of the University of Oxford and with informed consent from participants. We aimed for a sample of 30 human participants from the Oxford community for this experiment. Because of poor behavioral performance or MEG acquisition artifacts, we recruited “replacement” participants as needed, leading to a total sample of 37 participants (age range, 19–34 years; mean = 24.6 years; SD = 4.2 years; 18 females). All participants were right handed and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. One participant was excluded during MEG data acquisition due to inability to follow task instructions, and six participants were excluded before data analysis due to artifacts in the MEG data, leaving 30 participants for analysis.
Study structure.
Participants completed a reward learning task to induce learning of stimulus–reward associations (training phase), and a visual discrimination task (i.e., attention task) to test the effect of these associations on visual attention (testing phase). To maximize learning and increase task engagement, participants completed two sessions of each task in an interleaved order on the same day. Participants were compensated based on their winnings in the reward learning task and were explicitly told that the attention task was not rewarded and was simply a requirement to complete as part of the experiment.
Reward learning task (training phase).
The task was presented using MATLAB (MathWorks) and Psychtoolbox-3 (Kleiner et al., 2007). Participants sat 120 cm from the display, which was a Panasonic PT D7700E translucent back-projection display (54.5 × 43 cm; resolution, 1280 × 1024 pixels; 60 Hz refresh rate). After a 500–1000 ms (randomly jittered) intertrial interval (ITI), two option stimuli (each sized 2.4° × 2.4° visual angle) appeared on screen (Fig. 1a,b). Participants were instructed to maintain fixation until the fixation cross disappeared after 500 ms. After the fixation cross offset, participants had 5000 ms to indicate their choice with a left or right button press. After the choice, a feedback display presented the outcomes of the chosen and unchosen stimuli. To facilitate learning, the background brightness as well as a presented sound effect indicated the outcome reward magnitude. Participants began the next trial with a button press.
There were 10 option stimuli that participants learned about across trials: two high-reward stimuli (250 points), two low-reward stimuli (25 points), and six zero-reward stimuli (0 points; Fig. 1a). One thousand points were equivalent to £1, with participants receiving their winnings at the end of the experiment. A “points bar” at the bottom of the screen indicated how many points they had accumulated for the next £1 and a pound (£) counter indicated the total money won thus far. Reward associations were deterministic. To control for possible stimulus differences between option stimuli, reward associations were counterbalanced across participants using a Latin square design. There were 96 high-reward versus low-reward, 96 high-reward versus zero-reward, and 192 low-reward versus zero-reward choice trials per session, with the latter trial type doubled to equate selection frequency between high- and low-reward stimuli. Option stimuli varied in both colors and shapes, so participants could use either feature or a combination for learning. Trial order was randomized once and presented consistently across participants to minimize differences in learning.
Attention task (testing phase).
The hardware and software setup for stimulus presentation was identical to the reward learning task. The task began with a 500-1000 ms randomly jittered ITI, during which participants had to fixate on a central cross (Fig. 1c). Two of the previously rewarded but currently task-irrelevant reward stimuli then appeared on screen. These could be a pair of high-reward and zero-reward stimuli (high-reward trials), low-reward and zero-reward stimuli (low-reward trials), or two zero-reward stimuli (baseline trials). Following a randomly selected delay of 0, 500, or 1000 ms [the stimulus-onset asynchrony (SOA)], two task-relevant oriented gratings (diameter, 1.2° visual angle; the inner edge was positioned 5.4° away from center) appeared inside each of the reward stimuli. The target grating was oriented to either 0° or 90°. The participant's task was to indicate this orientation with a button-press. The nontarget (distracter) grating was oriented obliquely at 20–70° or 110–160°, randomly selected from 10° bins. Gratings were presented for 200 ms, after which they offset together with the reward stimuli. Participants had up to 1800 ms after grating offset to make their response. Auditory feedback was provided with a high-frequency tone signifying correct trials and a low-frequency tone signifying incorrect trials.
Critically, in high- and low-reward trials, the target grating appeared inside either the high- or low-reward stimuli (congruent trials), or inside the zero-reward stimulus (incongruent trials). In baseline trials, the target grating could appear in either zero-reward stimulus. Reward stimuli and target locations varied randomly between left and right of the fixation cross.
In each session, there were 60 trials per cell in the reward (3) × congruency (2) × SOA (3) condition matrix, with the exception of the baseline condition in which congruency was not a factor. There were 1800 trials in total across both sessions. All trials were fully randomized across participants. Each session was divided into 19 blocks, with 50 trials per block, and each block taking ∼2 min to complete. Participants were instructed to maintain fixation and minimize blinking throughout the duration of a block but were able to take a short break after each block.
In addition to using the SOA as a tool to isolate the processing of reward stimuli, some previous research suggested that VDAC may decrease with increasing SOA duration (Le Pelley et al., 2013). We manipulated the SOA duration in the current experiment to test for this possibility. However, we found no evidence for an effect of SOA on attentional capture at the behavioral level (in reaction time, our a priori measure of interest; see Results; see Fig. 4a). The absence of an SOA effect is consistent with a pilot behavioral study that we conducted (not reported here) and with some previous research (Failing and Theeuwes, 2015). We also found no evidence of neural differences between the 500 and 1000 ms SOA conditions (data not shown). Therefore, we collapse across these two conditions for all neural analyses (see below for decoding methods).
MEG data acquisition and preprocessing.
For both tasks, whole-head MEG data were recorded in a magnetically shielded room using a 306-channel Neuromag VectorView Scanner (102 magnetometers, 204 gradiometers; Elekta) at the Oxford Center for Human Brain Activity (OHBA). Four head position indicator (HPI) coils were placed behind the ears and on top of the forehead to track head movements in the scanner. A magnetic Polhemus Fastrak 3D digitizer pen (Vermont) was used to register the HPI coils and three anatomic landmarks (nasion, and left and right auricular points) and to digitize the head shape by marking 300 points along the scalp. MEG data were sampled at 1000 Hz witha 0.03–330 Hz bandpass filter during signal digitization. Electrocardiography (EKG) and electrooculography (EOG) were recorded using electrodes over the wrists and around the eyes (vertical and horizontal for blinks and saccades), and a forehead ground electrode. Eye movements were also recorded using an EyeLink 1000 infrared eye-tracker (SR Research). Manual responses were collected using a four-button fiber-optic response box.
MEG data were preprocessed using the OHBA Software Library (OSL; https://ohba-analysis.github.io/) and Fieldtrip (Oostenveld et al., 2011). Continuous MEG data were visually inspected to exclude segments containing severe artifacts (e.g., jumps) and to interpolate channels that contained frequent severe artifacts. The Elekta Maxfilter Signal Space Separation algorithm was then applied, together with head movement compensation based on the HPI coil data. In some participants, the trigger signal used to indicate epochs of interest spread into the MEG channels. Therefore, timepoints in the data corresponding to stimulus triggers were linearly interpolated within a 15 ms window around trigger onset; to be consistent, this correction was performed in all participants. Data were then bandpass filtered at 0.1–40 Hz, downsampled to 250 Hz, and epoched from −2000 to 2000 ms relative to grating onset. Eye-blink- and cardiac-related variance were removed by performing independent component analysis (ICA) on the MEG data using the FastICA algorithm and removing any components significantly correlated (using α = 0.05) with vertical EOG and EKG time courses above a threshold of Pearson's r = 0.1. Horizontal EOG, EyeLink, and MEG data were then visually inspected to reject trials containing saccades or other muscle artifacts.
Magnetic resonance imaging data acquisition.
To create subject-specific forward models for MEG source reconstruction, existing structural 3 T magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scans for 16 participants were obtained from previous studies in accordance with data-sharing procedures. In addition, new structural MRI scans were acquired for 19 participants on a separate day at the Oxford Center for Functional MRI of the Brain (FMRIB) using a Siemens Magnetom Prisma 3T scanner, with a T1-weighted MP-RAGE sequence (1 × 1 × 1 mm isotropic voxels; 256 × 234 × 192 grid; echo time, 3.94 ms; inversion time, 912 ms; recovery time, 1900 ms). For these data, participants' noses were included in the field of view to improve registration with the Polhemus head shape and MEG coordinates. MRI data from two participants were not acquired due to early study exclusion and a scheduling issue, respectively. For the latter participant, an average, age-matched (20–24 years old) brain template was used instead (Richards et al., 2016).
Behavioral data analysis.
Behavioral data were analyzed in MATLAB and in R using RStudio (with the ez package; Lawrence, 2016). Accuracy in the reward learning task (Fig. 2a) was quantified as the percentage of choices of the higher reward option and in the attention task as the percentage of correct target-discrimination responses. Reaction time (RT) analyses (Fig. 2b) were performed only on correct trials and using the median as the subject-level summary statistic. Behavioral data were analyzed using repeated-measures ANOVA, followed by pairwise comparisons. Tests were corrected for multiple comparisons using the Holm–Bonferroni method where relevant. Learning curves of accuracy and RT in the reward learning task were computed by smoothing each participant's trialwise data with a 10-trial moving average window within each session and concatenating sessions. Curves for high-reward choices include both high-reward versus zero-reward and high-reward versus low-reward trials.
Multivariate pattern analysis (decoding).
For decoding analyses in both tasks, the main epoch of interest was the 500 ms after reward stimulus onset, which provided a window to analyze early representations of reward-associated stimuli (“options” in the reward learning task, “reward cues” in the attention task) in task-relevant and irrelevant contexts. In the attention task, this epoch in the 500/1000 ms SOA conditions enabled us to examine the response to the reward stimuli separately from the response to target processing. For completeness, we also examined reward stimulus processing in the 0–500 ms after target onset in the 500/1000 and 0 ms SOA conditions.
Decoding was performed on broadband MEG gradiometer data. The true spatial dimensionality of the MEG data after the Maxfilter preprocessing algorithm is ∼64 rather than the 204 gradiometer channels (Woolrich et al., 2011). Therefore, to reduce the number of redundant features for decoding, we first performed principal component analysis on the data to extract a subset of dimensions (70) that explain nearly 100% of the variance. Note that this simply re-expresses the data using a smaller set of features and does not reduce the number of informative dimensions. Using the 70-feature × N-trial × T-time data matrix as input, we conducted multivariate pattern analysis using Mahalanobis distance with fivefold cross-validation, an approach closely related to linear discriminant analysis (LDA; Wolff et al., 2015, 2017; Walther et al., 2016). Specifically, to discriminate between two classes, we split all of the data into five random groups (“folds”) of trials, iteratively using one as a testing set and the remaining as training data (and repeating the splitting procedure 50 times to ensure stable results). We computed Mahalanobis distances between left-out test trials at a given time point and the means of class A and B from the training data at the same time point. Mahalanobis distance is given by the following:
where is the inverse feature covariance estimated on all training data from both classes (per time point, across trials) using a shrinkage estimator (Ledoit and Wolf, 2004). The feature covariance captures the correlated noise between sensors or voxels regardless of condition, and thereby improves decoding reliability (Kriegeskorte et al., 2006; Walther et al., 2016). We then computed the difference between the distances to Class A and Class B, always subtracting the within-class distance from the between-class distance, as follows:
which leads to positive differences with better decodability (Wolff et al., 2017). This was repeated for all trials and timepoints, and results were smoothed across time with a Gaussian smoothing kernel (σ = 16 ms) to obtain a measure of trialwise pattern decodability across time. Note that although this distance measure can be binarized and converted to accuracy over trials (as in LDA), we preserved the parametric distance value as a more informative measure of similarity/dissimilarity between train and test patterns (Walther et al., 2016).
We quantified neural pattern separation for various task parameters during the reward learning and attention tasks, as follows: reward stimulus location (left vs right; location decoding); reward stimulus identity(e.g., high-reward stimulus 1 vs high-reward stimulus 2; identity decoding); and stimulus-independent value (high reward vs low reward; value decoding). Location decoding was performed on data collapsed across stimulus identity, and compared between high-reward and low-reward trials. Identity decoding was performed separately for each location, with the results averaged over location, and compared between high-reward and low-reward trials. For value decoding, we used a cross-generalization approach, in which we trained a classifier using one pair of reward stimuli (e.g., high reward 1 vs low reward 1), and tested performance on another (e.g., high reward 2 vs low reward 2), collapsing across locations. Value decoding was therefore independent of location and identity, and was attributable only to the difference in value between stimuli.
For location and identity decoding in the reward learning task (Fig. 3a–c), the high-reward condition included both high-reward versus low-reward and high-reward versus zero-reward trials. Value decoding analysis (Fig. 3b) excluded the high-reward versus low-reward trials to ensure the interpretability of results; to obtain equal trial numbers between high-reward versus zero-reward and low-reward versus zero-reward trials, we subsampled the latter using an odd/even split. Attention task decoding was performed separately on the 500/1000 ms and the 0 ms SOA trials.
For statistical testing, we averaged pattern decodability across trials for each participant and tested it at the group level against zero or between conditions using cluster-corrected sign permutation tests, with a cluster-forming threshold of α = 0.05 and 10,000 permutations. Baseline data (i.e., before stimulus onset) were not included in permutation testing.
To test how location decoding evolved across trials during the reward learning task as a function of reward (Fig. 3b), for each participant we computed the Spearman rank correlation between trial number and peak location decoding in each trial, separately for high and low reward. We tested the average Fisher z-transformed correlation across participants against zero and between reward conditions using one-sample and paired t tests, respectively. The time of peak decoding was defined based on the group-level decoding results. Note that due to the rejection of trials with artifacts, the number of trials included in this analysis is less than in the learning curves presented in Figure 2
Median split analyses.
To test the relationship between the observed decoding and the attentional capture effect in behavior, we split participant decoding results based on the magnitude of their RT reward × congruency interaction effect across all three SOAs. Decoding differences between groups were tested using independent-samples t statistics and cluster-corrected group permutation tests, in which observed group differences were compared with a null distribution of differences generated from random group partitions (cluster-forming α = 0.05; 10,000 permutations).
Time-resolved correlation analyses.
To complement the median split analyses, we performed time-resolved Spearman rank correlations between the behavioral attentional capture effect and value modulation in the decoding. Correlations were computed at each time point, with statistical significance tested using cluster-corrected permutation tests in which the observed correlation coefficients were compared with a null distribution of coefficients generated from independent shuffles of behavioral and neural participant data (cluster-forming α = 0.05; 10,000 permutations). Additionally, we repeated these analyses after excluding bivariate outliers using a robust multivariate projection method and the box-plot rule (the skipped correlation method in the Robust Correlation Toolbox; Pernet et al., 2012). Outliers were identified and excluded at each time point and were also excluded during permutation testing. Post hoc power was calculated using G*Power, assuming a null hypothesis of no correlation (ρ = 0), α = 0.05, a two-tailed test, and the current sample size of n = 30 (Faul et al., 2009).
Source reconstruction.
Coregistration of MEG sensors and structural MRI was performed using OSL (https://ohba-analysis.github.io/). Structural MRI data were bias corrected and linearly registered to the Montreal Neurologic Institute (MNI) space, enabling source reconstruction and analysis to be completed in this common reference space. The MRI scalp surface was extracted and smoothed, and fiducial points were manually marked. MRI fiducials were registered to the MEG fiducials using linear transformation and subsequently refined using the iterative closest point algorithm applied to the entire MRI scalp surface and set of Polhemus head shape points. Source reconstruction on MEG gradiometer data were performed in Fieldtrip using a single-shell volume conduction model (based on a mesh of the brain–skull boundary; Nolte, 2003), a leadfield matrix generated from a 10 mm-spaced template grid, and linearly constrained minimum variance (LCMV) beamforming (Van Veen et al., 1997). To obtain whole-brain virtual channel data, beamforming filters were applied to time-domain data, and, for each gridpoint, the three-direction dipole data were projected to the direction explaining the most variance using singular value decomposition.
Source-space searchlight decoding.
To visualize brain regions involved in decoding, we performed searchlight decoding on whole-brain virtual channel data in source space in 50 ms steps, from 250 to 500 ms (location decoding), 0 to 500 ms (identity decoding), and 300 to 500 ms (value decoding) after cue onset. Specific time steps were chosen because running the full-time course was computationally intensive and difficult to visualize in both the spatial and temporal domains. These time steps were chosen as they were evenly spaced points corresponding approximately to the significant value modulation of decoding in sensor space or, in the case of identity decoding, to the entirety of the cue epoch. An ∼23-mm-radius searchlight sphere was used to define features (grid points) for decoding. The sphere was enlarged at the edges of the brain to maintain an approximately constant number of grid points per sphere (across all spheres, mean = 62 grid points, SD = 6 grid points). To reduce extensive computation time, the same feature covariance estimate was used for decoding at each time point, which was computed on baseline data (averaged from −100 to −50 ms relative to cue onset). Additionally, the fivefold split into training and testing data was repeated 10 times to obtain stable results. All other aspects of decoding were identical to that described above. For statistical testing, the resultant distance differences were converted into t statistics and were cluster corrected at the whole-brain level using threshold-free cluster enhancement (TFCE) with 10,000 permutations, as implemented in the FSL randomize tool (Smith and Nichols, 2009; Winkler et al., 2014). For visualization, thresholded t value maps were first interpolated to a 2 mm MNI volume using linear interpolation and then interpolated to the Conte69 inflated surface using trilinear interpolation in Fieldtrip (Marcus et al., 2011; Oostenveld et al., 2011).
Univariate analysis of event-related fields.
To complement the decoding analyses and for comparability with prior research, we conducted univariate analysis of the N2pc and P1 components. Data were baseline corrected using a 200 ms prestimulus window. To be optimally sensitive to the potential value modulation of evoked components, we first selected sensors for each component using an orthogonal contrast—activity averaged across high and low reward, and across all SOA conditions—and then tested the value modulation within those sensors of interest (MacLean and Giesbrecht, 2015a; Donohue et al., 2016; Luque et al., 2017).
For the N2pc analysis, we identified sensors of interest for each participant, by computing event-related averages for left and right reward cue trials. We then combined planar gradiometers using the root mean square (rms) approach and computed the difference wave between the cue sides at each sensor. Finally, we identified the five posterior sensors in each hemisphere showing the largest N2pc amplitude averaged within a 200–325 ms poststimulus time window of interest (Itthipuripat et al., 2015). Using these sensors, we compared the strength of the reward–cue-related N2pc between high- and low-reward conditions for the combined 500/1000 ms SOA and the 0 ms SOA conditions. For each reward condition, we computed averages separately for left and right reward cue trials, combined planar gradiometers using rms, averaged across reward cue sides within contralateral and ipsilateral sensors, and computed the sensor-averaged difference between contralateral and ipsilateral conditions (i.e., the N2pc). We then compared the N2pc amplitude, averaged within the 200–325 ms window, between high- and low-reward conditions using a paired t test.
Identifying sensors for the P1 analysis were similar to those described above, except we additionally allowed for variation of the P1 latency across participants by calculating the difference wave for each sensor within a time window identified specifically for that sensor and participant (MacLean and Giesbrecht, 2015a). To identify the individual P1 latencies, we computed an average across all trials; combined planar gradiometers using rms; and, for each sensor, used the findpeaks function from the MATLAB Signal Processing toolbox to identify the latency of the largest peak within a 75–200 ms window (MacLean and Giesbrecht, 2015a; Luque et al., 2017). If no peak was present, we took the latency of the maximum value within that window. The final time window was a 50 ms window around the identified peak (MacLean and Giesbrecht, 2015a; Luque et al., 2017). The mean P1 latency (averaged across sensors and participants) was 156 ms (SD = 16 ms; similar to Luque et al., 2017). We tested for the value modulation of the P1 in a manner identical to that for the N2pc, except that we compared the P1 difference averaged within a 50 ms window around the individual peaks identified earlier.
Data availability.
Analysis scripts and data are available at https://osf.io/6ky8b/.
Results
Reward learning task: learning stimulus–reward contingencies
We first confirmed that participants learned the stimulus–reward contingencies in the reward learning task. Participants won an average of £44.67 (SD = £2.89) of a possible £48. Choice accuracy was sensitive to the value difference between options, with a significant main effect of choice type [high vs zero (H-Z); high vs low (H-L); low vs zero (L-Z)] on accuracy (F(2,58) = 7.43, p = 0.006). Participants were more accurate on H-Z than L-Z trials (t(29) = 4.36, p < 0.001, Cohen's d = 0.8; Fig. 2a). There was no difference between H-Z and H-L (t(29) = 1.37, p = 0.18) or H-L and L-Z trials (t(29) = 2.06, p = 0.1). RT was similarly sensitive to value difference, with a significant main effect of choice type on RT (F(2,58) = 41.4, p < 0.001). Participants were faster on H-Z and H-L choices than L-Z choices (t(29) = 7.69, p < 0.001, Cohen's d = 1.4; and t(29) = 6.07, p < 0.001, Cohen's d = 1.1, respectively; Fig. 2b). There was no difference between H-Z and H-L choices (t(29) = 1.14, p = 0.26).
Previous work has dissociated the effects of irrelevant reward history and reward-independent selection history on attentional capture (MacLean and Giesbrecht, 2015b). The latter phenomenon reflects findings showing that merely selectively attending to a stimulus can affect subsequent attention to it, regardless of reward associations (Awh et al., 2012; MacLean and Giesbrecht, 2015b). To control for selection history, we doubled the number of low-reward versus zero-reward trials to match the frequency of choosing low-reward options with that of high-reward options. We confirmed that choice frequency (i.e., how often participants selected a given reward option) did not differ between high- and low-reward options (note that this is correlated with but not identical to trial-type frequency, which was fixed across participants). There was a main effect of stimulus value on choice frequency (F(2,58) = 721.05, p < 0.001). Participants chose the high- and low-reward options more frequently than the zero-reward stimuli (high vs zero: t(29) = 35, p < 0.001, Cohen's d = 6.39; low vs zero, t(29) = 28.26, p < 0.001, Cohen's d = 5.15). There was no difference between high- and low-reward options (t(29) = 0.6, p = 0.55; Fig. 2c), demonstrating the absence of a choice frequency (i.e., selection history) effect.
Reward learning task: reward stimulus processing in atask-relevant context
Next, we tested the feasibility of time-resolved decoding of reward stimulus representations in the reward learning task, a context in which these stimuli (i.e., options for decision-making) are relevant and rewarded. These analyses focused on the fixation epoch, in which participants saw the options but were not yet able to respond.
We applied multivariate pattern analysis to discriminate between trials in which the higher valued option appeared on the left versus the right of the fixation cross, separately for the high- and low-reward trials (location decoding; Fig. 3a). Given the obvious perceptual differences between left and right option trials, we expected the decoding analysis to reflect this. Crucially, however, these perceptual differences should be identical in high- and low-reward trials, and therefore any differences in decoding between these conditions are attributable solely to the reward associated with each type of option. As expected, for both high- and low-reward options, we could decode the option location from ∼104 and ∼136 ms onward, respectively (each p < 0.001, cluster-corrected permutation test), which is in line with the clear perceptual difference between left and right cue presentation trials. Critically, the decoding was significantly stronger for the high-reward options than the low-reward options from ∼184 ms onward (p < 0.001), indicating that learned value modulated location decodability in this task-relevant context (Fig. 3a). This is consistent with an effect of reward on spatial attention (Anderson, 2016; MacLean et al., 2016; Moore and Zirnsak, 2017).
Given that participants gradually learned to locate and select the higher reward stimulus, we examined how location decoding emerged across trials as a function of associated reward. We extracted trialwise distance differences from the peak time point of maximal decoding in the trial-averaged data in the high- and low-reward conditions (Fig. 3a) and tested for a relationship with trial number. In line with participants' learning, location decoding significantly increased over trials in the high-reward condition (t test on the participant-wise, Fisher z-transformed Spearman rank correlations; t(29) = 8.68, p < 0.001; Fig. 3b, top), but not in the low-reward condition (t(29) = 1.95, p = 0.06; see also Fig. 3b, bottom). Correlations were stronger in the high-reward compared with the low-reward condition (t(29) = 5.7, p < 0.001; Fig. 3b, inset). Thus, the emergence of location decoding across trials also differed as a function of associated reward, consistent with the idea that reward learning gradually shapes selective attention (Chelazzi et al., 2013; Anderson, 2016).
Next, we applied a similar multivariate pattern analysis to discriminate between the two stimulus identities within each reward condition (e.g., high reward 1 vs high reward 2; identity decoding; Fig. 3c). Identity could be decoded from ∼80 and ∼104 ms onward for high- and low-reward stimuli, respectively (p < 0.001, cluster-corrected permutation test), but with no difference between reward conditions (p = 0.417), suggesting that learned value did not modulate the sensory representations of these stimuli in this task (Fig. 3c).
Last, we sought to decode a reward value signal, independent of location or identity. We used a cross-generalization approach, in which we trained a classifier using one pair of reward stimuli (e.g., high 1 vs low 1) and tested performance on another (e.g., high 2 vs low 2), collapsing across locations (Kahnt et al., 2010). Any significant decoding that emerges is therefore independent of location and identity, and so attributable to the difference in value between stimuli (value decoding; Fig. 3d). We found a significant value signal from ∼260 ms onward (p = 0.001, cluster-corrected permutation test), representing the available rewardindependently of stimulus location or identity (Fig. 3d).
Attention task: attentional capture by stimuli associated with reward history
Next, we probed how stimulus–reward associations learned in the reward learning task captured attention in the attention task and whether attentional capture was modulated by reward value and/or by the SOA. We ran a repeated-measures ANOVA on RT with the following three factors: reward (high, low), congruency (congruent, incongruent), and SOA (0, 500, 1000 ms). There was a main effect of congruency (F(1,29) = 12.46, p = 0.001) and a congruency × reward interaction (F(1,29) = 8.9, p = 0.006). Participants were significantly slower in the high-reward incongruent condition relative to the congruent condition (t(29) = 3.77, p = 0.002, Cohen's d = 0.69), with no such difference between the low-reward incongruent and congruent conditions (t(29) = 1.77, p = 0.087, Cohen's d = 0.32). Importantly, although there was a main effect of SOA (F(2,58) = 44.33, p < 0.001; indicative of a general preparatory effect), there were no interactions between SOA and reward or congruency (all F(2,58) < 1, p > 0.3). Thus, VDAC effects in reaction time were consistent across SOAs. For completeness, we tested for reward × congruency effects in each SOA. Across SOA conditions, participants were slower in the high-reward incongruent trials than the congruent trials (0 ms SOA: t(29) = 4.55, p = 0.001, Cohen's d = 0.83; 500 ms SOA: t(29) = 3.15, p = 0.019, Cohen's d = 0.57; 1000 ms SOA: t(29) = 2.8, p = 0.036, Cohen's d = 0.51; Fig. 4a). There was no difference between low-reward incongruent and congruent trials (all t(29) < 2, p > 0.2; Fig. 4a).
A congruency effect on reaction time could be driven both by faster responses in congruent trials, and/or slower responses in incongruent trials, compared with baseline. Figure 4a suggests that both might be the case, and we next tested this explicitly. Given the absence of interactions with SOA, we collapsed across this condition and performed one-sample t tests for each congruency and reward condition. In high-reward trials, participants showed both a congruent effect (t(29) = 3.14, p = 0.004, Cohen's d = 0.57) and an incongruent effect (t(29) = 2.59, p = 0.015, Cohen's d = 0.47), suggesting that they were respectively sped up or slowed down by the presence of a congruent or incongruent high-reward stimulus. These effects were not present in the low-reward conditions (t(29) = 1.2, p = 0.238; and t(29) = 1.38, p = 0.178, respectively).
We then tested whether the effect of congruency on RT decayed over trials, as participants repeatedly encountered the unrewarded and irrelevant stimuli. This would reflect an extinction of the stimulus–reward association as reward becomes unavailable, as widely observed in learning paradigms (Todd et al., 2014). Focusing on the high-reward condition and collapsing across SOAs, we split trial data into two halves per session and computed a difference between congruent and incongruent RTs as an aggregate measure of attentional capture for each half. A session × half repeated-measures ANOVA showed no significant main effects or interactions (all F(1,29) < 2.8, p > 0.11), suggesting that the effect did not reliably extinguish across trials within the timeframe of this experiment (Fig. 4b).
We also analyzed accuracy difference scores. There was a significant main effect of congruency (F(1,29) = 9.25, p = 0.005), a main effect of SOA (F(2,58) = 3.17, p = 0.049), a reward × congruency interaction (F(1,29) = 20.11, p = 0.0001), and a reward × congruency × SOA interaction (F(2,58) = 3.21, p = 0.047). In the high-reward 500 and 1000 ms SOA conditions only, participants were less accurate in the incongruent than in the congruent trials (t(29) = 3.75, p = 0.005, Cohen's d = 0.68; and t(29) = 3.45, p = 0.009, Cohen's d = 0.63). There were no differences in the 0 ms SOA or in any low-reward condition (all t(29) < 2, p > 0.9). Thus, attentional capture also influences accuracy, at least in the longer SOA conditions, with no evidence of a speed–accuracy trade-off.
Reward cue location is decodable in a task-irrelevant context and modulated by value
Next, we probed the neural correlates of the attentional capture effect we observed in behavior. We aimed to identify neural signatures of task-irrelevant reward history, independently of target processing. Similar to the analysis of the reward learning task data, we performed time-resolved decoding of the location and identity of the reward cues, as well as a stimulus-independent value signal. This enabled us to probe the time course of any effects of reward history, such as modulations in early visual processing. Our primary focus was the cue epoch, before the onset of task-relevant gratings, as this period allowed for the analysis of task-irrelevant reward cue processing without the interference of target processing. We specifically focused on the first 500 ms after reward cue onset as this allowed us to collapse across the 500 and 1000 ms SOA trials.
We first looked at location decoding as the most direct correlate of the spatially specific attentional capture that would subsequently impact target processing and related behavior. As before, we found significant decoding of the cue location for both high-reward cues (p < 0.001, cluster-corrected permutation test) and low-reward cues (p < 0.001; Fig. 5a). Critically, this decoding was modulated by reward value, with stronger decoding for high-reward cues compared with low-reward cues starting from ∼260 ms (p < 0.001; Fig. 5a). This effect is solely attributable to the reward history that these stimuli acquired during the reward learning task; in the current context, these stimuli are unrewarded and irrelevant.
We sought to determine the anatomic source of this value modulation. We projected the sensor-space data onto a source-space grid using a LCMV beamformer, and ran the same location decoding analysis within a searchlight sphere across the brain, in 50 ms bins from 250 to 500 ms after cue onset. Value modulation began around the right posterior parietal cortex (PPC), followed by the inferior temporal and visual cortex, and more widespread modulation in posterior parietal cortex, and along the temporal lobe (p < 0.05, whole-brain cluster-corrected using TFCE; Fig. 5b).
We then tested whether this value modulation was related to the value modulation of the behavioral attentional capture effect. We conducted a median split of the participants based on the extent to which their attentional capture was modulated by reward value (i.e., the strength of the reward × congruency interaction effect on RT; Fig. 5d, top). Strong capture participants(i.e., with stronger value modulation of the attentional capture effect) showed accompanying value modulation of the location decoding during the cue epoch starting from ∼244 ms (p < 0.001, cluster-corrected permutation test; Fig. 5c, bottom), while weak capture participants did not (p = 0.206; Fig. 5c, top), with a significant difference between these two groups (p = 0.025, independent-samples, cluster-corrected permutation test; Fig. 5d, bottom). A time-resolved Spearman rank correlation analysis between the behavioral VDAC effect and the neural value modulation across participants mirrored these results (cluster from 348 to 408 ms; average within-cluster: ρ = 0.504, p = 0.031, cluster-corrected permutation test). We also confirmed that these results were not driven by outlying participants using a skipped rank correlation that formally excludes bivariate outliers (cluster from 352 to 416 ms; average within-cluster ρ = 0.508, p = 0.004; for details, see Materials and Methods; Pernet et al., 2012). Finally, we note that according to post hoc power calculations, with the current sample size of n = 30 and α = 0.05, there is 80% power to detect a correlation of ρ = 0.5.
We defined the above strong and weak capture groups based on the extent to which reward value modulates the behavioral effects of attentional capture, but they might also differ in how they learn reward associations (Jahfari and Theeuwes, 2017). We therefore compared their learning curves from the reward learning task, but did not find any significant differences (p = 0.410, independent-samples, cluster-corrected permutation test across trials; Fig. 5d, middle).
Next, we tested whether the cue location decoding persisted into the target epoch, which would suggest that participants maintain traces of the irrelevant reward cue location even while discriminating the oriented gratings. We repeated the location decoding on 500 and 1000 ms SOA trial data, now time locked to the grating onset. We again found significant decoding of cue location for both high-reward cues (p < 0.001, cluster-corrected permutation test) and low-reward cues (p < 0.001), which was also modulated by reward value (p < 0.001; Fig. 6a). Note that the significant decoding before grating onset (the typical “baseline” period) occurs because this period overlaps with the preceding cue epoch. As above, we tested whether this value modulation during the target epoch related to the behavioral attentional capture effect. A median split analysis (described above) showed no significant difference in value modulation of location decoding between strong and weak capture participants (p = 0.283, independent-samples, cluster-corrected permutation test). Strong capture participants showed significant value modulation (p = 0.002, cluster-corrected permutation test), as did weak capture participants (p = 0.049; Fig. 6b). A complementary time-resolved correlation analysis (as above) also showed no significant effects during this time period (Spearman rank correlation: p = 0.336, cluster-corrected permutation test; skipped rank correlation: no data exceeded the cluster-forming threshold).
We also performed the same location decoding analysis in the 0 ms SOA condition, in which reward cues and gratings are presented simultaneously. We found significant decoding of the cue location for high-reward cues (p < 0.001, cluster-corrected permutation test), but not for low-reward cues (p = 0.121), and a significant value modulation from ∼224 ms (p = 0.024; Fig. 6c). As before, we conducted a median split analysis (described above). However, we did not find value modulation of the location decoding when analyzing each group separately (strong capture participants, p = 0.123; weak capture participants, p = 0.519; Fig. 6d). A complementary time-resolved correlation analysis (as above) also showed no significant effects (Spearman rank correlation, p = 0.175; skipped rank correlation, p = 0.427). Note that these analyses should be interpreted with caution because of simultaneous cue and target processing in this condition as well as the reduced within-subject power, with half as many trials here as in the combined 500 and 1000 ms SOA decoding.
We have used here linear multivariate methods as a sensor- and amplitude-agnostic method of analyzing cue-related activity. To ensure that the above results are not specific to our method, and for comparability with prior research, we also conducted univariate analysis of the event-related field (ERF) responses. Based on prior research on VDAC, we focused on value modulation of the early sensory-evoked P1 and the later N2pc components (Hickey et al., 2010; Qi et al., 2013; Itthipuripat et al., 2015; MacLean and Giesbrecht, 2015a; Luque et al., 2017). To be optimally sensitive to value effects, we performed these analyses within time windows of interest based on prior research, and in sensors identified for each participant using an orthogonal contrast (for details, see Materials and Methods; Itthipuripat et al., 2015; MacLean and Giesbrecht, 2015a; Donohue et al., 2016; Luque et al., 2017). Consistent with the location decoding results, we found a value modulation of the N2pc in the 500 and 1000 ms SOA condition (high-reward vs low-reward N2pc amplitude: t(29) = 2.186, p = 0.037; Fig. 7a) and in the 0 ms SOA condition (high-reward vs low-reward N2pc amplitude: t(29) = 2.362, p = 0.025; Fig. 7b). In contrast, we did not observe any value modulation of the P1 component in the 500 and 1000 ms SOA conditions (high-reward vs low-reward P1 lateralization: t(29) = 0.621, p = 0.54; Fig. 7c) or in the 0 ms SOA condition (high-reward vs low-reward P1 lateralization: t(29) = 0.176, p = 0.861; Fig. 7d). Thus, the timing of value modulation effects is consistent across multivariate and univariate analyses, with an absence of value modulation in early time windows (i.e., the evoked P1 component) but with the presence of value modulation in later time windows (i.e., the N2pc component).
No evidence that reward cue identity decoding is modulated by value in a task-irrelevant context
In addition to the location decoding of the reward cues (i.e., reflecting spatial selection), it is possible that decoding of the cue identities (putatively reflecting visual feature representations) would be modulated by learned value (Anderson, 2016; Failing and Theeuwes, 2018). Although we did not observe an effect of reward value on identity decoding during the reward learning task, we nevertheless explored this possibility during the attention task. Although we could reliably decode identity for each type of reward cue (high reward, from ∼128 ms, p < 0.001; low reward, from ∼124 ms, p < 0.001), this was not modulated by value (p = 0.115; Fig. 8a). When we performed a median split analysis by behavior (described above), the strong capture participants did not show any significant value modulation (p = 0.065; Fig. 8b, bottom). Weak capture participants also did not show any value modulation (p = 0.353; Fig. 8b, top). A time-resolved correlation analysis (as above) also showed no significant effects (Spearman rank correlation, p = 0.228; skipped rank correlation, p = 0.334). A similar pattern was observed when decoding in the 0 ms SOA trials, with significant decoding of stimulus identity in high- and low-reward trials (for both, p < 0.001), but no significant value modulation (no data exceeded the cluster-forming threshold; Fig. 8c). Likewise, neither strong nor weak capture participants showed significant value modulation (for both, no data exceeded the cluster-forming threshold candidates; Fig. 8d). A time-resolved correlation analysis (as above) also showed no significant effects during this time period (Spearman rank correlation, p = 0.241; skipped rank correlation, p = 0.158).
It is possible that any effect of reward history on identity decoding is confined to a specific anatomic region (e.g., within the ventral visual stream), which our whole-brain sensor-space analysis may have missed. To test this, we repeated the analysis using source-space searchlight decoding across the brain in 50 ms time steps from cue onset and tested for significant clusters. This approach enabled a more fine-grained subselection of potentially informative signals (i.e., within a searchlight region of interest), relative to the whole-brain sensor-space decoding analysis. There were no significant differences between high- and low-reward trials anywhere in the brain (500 and 1000 ms SOA, p > 0.1; 0 ms SOA, p > 0.209; whole-brain cluster-corrected using TFCE). Thus, we did not find any evidence that reward history modulates stimulus identity representations in either a task-relevant or a task-irrelevant context in the current experiment. Moreover, any hint of an effect is in the opposite direction to that expected, with slightly worse decoding in the high-reward condition.
Decoding stimulus-independent reward value in atask-irrelevant context
Finally, we sought to decode a location- and identity-independent value signal during the attention task, which would provide additional evidence that value information continues to be represented in a task-irrelevant context. Using the same cross-generalization approach as before, we found significant value decoding in the cue epoch, emerging from ∼336 ms (p = 0.003, cluster-corrected permutation test; Fig. 9a). In a median split analysis based on participants' behavior (described above), neither the strong nor the weak capture group showed significant decoding by itself (strong capture, p = 0.086; weak capture, p = 0.143), although the strong capture participants appeared to be driving the overall effect (Fig. 9b). The difference between groups was not significant (no data exceeded the cluster-forming threshold, independent-samples, cluster-corrected permutation test). The absence of significant decoding in either of the two subgroups, together with significant decoding across the entire sample, is most likely explained by a lack of statistical power in the former analysis, rather than a true null effect. A time-resolved correlation analysis (as above) also showed no significant effects during this time period (Spearman rank correlation, p = 0.361, cluster-corrected permutation test; skipped rank correlation, p = 0.480). Source-space searchlight decoding of this value signal across the brain showed significant clusters in bilateral visual, left inferior temporal, and left inferior frontal cortex (p < 0.05, whole-brain cluster-corrected using TFCE; Fig. 9c). In the 0 ms SOA condition, we found no significant value decoding at any time point (p = 0.210, cluster-corrected permutation test).
Discussion
We recorded brain activity using MEG as participants completed a reward learning task to establish stimulus–reward associations, followed by an attention task to measure VDAC. First, we showed a spatially specific behavioral VDAC effect, with reward-associated stimuli respectively speeding or slowing RT in congruent and incongruent trials, consistent with attentional costs and benefits (Posner, 1980; Failing and Theeuwes, 2014). This effect was modulated by reward value, suggesting that it is the associated value that is driving capture, rather than a reward-independent selection history (MacLean and Giesbrecht, 2015b). VDAC was robust even with a delay between reward-associated stimuli and targets (see also Failing and Theeuwes, 2015), allowing us to isolate the processing of reward-associated stimuli. Using MVPA, we found that location decoding of task-irrelevant reward cues was modulated by value from ∼260 ms after stimulus onset. This neural effect was related to behavior: participants who had strong VDAC also had larger value modulation of location decoding. Further, we found that this value modulation extended into the target epoch, consistent with the behavioral effects of VDAC observed in the long SOA conditions (500 and 1000 ms). We found an additional value signal in the long SOA conditions, which was independent of the location or identity of the reward stimuli. In contrast, although we could decode the identity of reward cues, this decoding was not modulated by value. This was the case regardless of SOA condition, whether we focused our analysis on participants showing a strong VDAC effect or whether we performed decoding on whole-brain data or with searchlight analysis.
Value signals and spatial attention
The timing of the value modulation of location decoding is consistent with a lateralized N2pc signal for reward-associated stimuli, and indeed we found a value modulation of the N2pc in a univariate analysis (Hickey et al., 2010; Qi et al., 2013; Itthipuripat et al., 2015). This suggests an effect of value on spatial attention (Woodman and Luck, 1999; Chelazzi et al., 2014; MacLean et al., 2016; Moore and Zirnsak, 2017). Moreover, localization of this effect to the PPC is in line with theories proposing that regions in this area [e.g., the lateral intraparietal area (LIP) in monkeys] integrate diverse signals, including task information and physical salience, to compute the overall attentional priority of stimuli (Bisley and Goldberg, 2010; Zelinsky and Bisley, 2015). Single-unit recording and fMRI studies suggest that learned reward value is similarly integrated in the PPC (Gottlieb and Snyder, 2010; Anderson et al., 2014; Barbaro et al., 2017; Ghazizadeh et al., 2018). For example, LIP neurons encode learned value despite its interference with goal-oriented behavior, and such biases transfer to a task-irrelevant context after extensive training (Peck et al., 2009). Interestingly, after the onset of task-irrelevant reward cues, LIP neurons show sustained increases in firing rate for at least 800 ms (Peck et al., 2009). This dovetails with our findings that value modulation of location decoding extends into the target epoch and that behavioral VDAC effects are also present at long SOAs. The role of learned value in these effects is demonstrated by the relatively weaker location decoding of low-reward cues in both the cue and target epochs, a pattern that mirrors the behavioral results.
The spatial resolution of source localization in the current study was insufficient to confidently distinguish parietal subregions (Corbetta and Shulman, 2002). Our findings are also consistent with studies showing that ventral parts of the PPC (e.g., inferior parietal lobule) are involved in the detection of salient events and attentional orienting (Corbetta and Shulman, 2002; Husain and Nachev, 2007). The processing of reward history in this and other parietal subregions remains an important open question (Frank and Sabatinelli, 2012).
Value signals, VDAC, and the stimulus-onset asynchrony
Although we observed a relationship between the VDAC RT effect and the value modulation of location decoding in the 500/1000 ms SOA condition, this was not apparent in the 0 ms SOA condition. We suggest that this discrepancy stems from practical differences between conditions. Namely, the 0 ms SOA condition involves simultaneous processing of reward cue and target stimuli, which may hamper decoding, and is the primary reason that we included an SOA in the paradigm. Moreover, the 0 ms SOA condition has half as many trials as the combined 500/1000 ms SOA condition, decreasing within-subject power for decoding. Consistent with this interpretation, we did not observe that the VDAC RT effect differed between SOA conditions (Failing and Theeuwes, 2015).
More broadly, we note that cross-subject brain–behavior correlations need to be interpreted with caution (Yarkoni, 2009). Nevertheless, our finding of a link between value modulation of stimulus-driven activity and VDAC effects in RT is consistent with previous studies reporting similar effects using fMRI (Hickey and Peelen, 2015) and EEG (Hickey et al., 2010; Qi et al., 2013).
The latency of value signals and VDAC
The latency of observed value signals is too late to suggest a causal role in modulating visual processing. Attentional modulations arising from frontal and parietal cortex—the putative effects of VDAC—have been shown to already occur by ∼250 ms (Foxe and Simpson, 2002; Buffalo et al., 2010; Anderson, 2016). Indeed, robust behavioral effects are also observed when targets are presented simultaneously to the reward stimuli (e.g., 0 ms SOA condition; but see also Anderson et al., 2011). In these trials, it is likely that any value signal would have to occur earlier than ∼200 ms to causally influence visual processing (Foxe and Simpson, 2002; Buffalo et al., 2010). It is possible that there is an early cortical effect that we cannot detect due to a lack of power or due to the relative insensitivity of MEG to radially oriented dipoles compared with EEG (Ahlfors et al., 2010). As described earlier, some EEG studies have reported value modulation earlier than ∼200 ms (Hickey et al., 2010; MacLean and Giesbrecht, 2015a; Luque et al., 2017).
Differences in task design may explain the discrepancies between studies. Hickey et al. (2010) observed a P1 effect using an intertrial priming paradigm in which the reward history and task relevance of stimuli changed across trials (see also Itthipuripat et al., 2015). Notably, MacLean and Giesbrecht (2015a) did find a P1 effect days after reward learning, but it only occurred in trials in which the reward-associated stimulus directed attention toward the target, suggesting an interaction with task relevance. Similarly, the P1 effect in Luque et al. (2017) occurred in the outcome phase of a reinforcement learning task in which stimuli are task relevant. In contrast, in the current study and in the study by Qi et al. (2013), who also did not find a P1 effect, the reward-associated stimuli were entirely irrelevant in the attention task. Therefore, it is possible that early cortical value signals may only be present, or at least detectable, when they coincide with current or recent task-related attentional modulation (Failing and Theeuwes, 2018).
Implications for the visual cortical plasticity account
The visual cortical plasticity account of VDAC posits that value may be directly encoded in cortex via plasticity of stimulus representations in the ventral visual stream (Anderson, 2016; van Koningsbruggen et al., 2016; Failing and Theeuwes, 2018). High-level object, perceptual, and category learning are known to induce plasticity throughout the ventral visual stream and higher-order areas (Miyashita, 1988; Schoups et al., 2001; Op de Beeck and Baker, 2010; Srihasam et al., 2014; LeMessurier and Feldman, 2018). Such changes may underlie the value modulation of early visual processing during VDAC (e.g., the aforementioned P1 effects), although in the current study we did not find evidence for such early value modulation. Increased decodability of reward-associated stimuli (Hickey and Peelen, 2015) has also been taken as supporting evidence (Failing and Theeuwes, 2018). Importantly, Hickey and Peelen (2015) were decoding object categories. In the current study, we focused on within-category stimulus identity and did not observe the value modulation of identity decoding in either the reward learning or attention task. Moreover, in line with previous research on VDAC (Anderson et al., 2011; Hickey and Peelen, 2015), reward learning in the current study was conducted over a shorter timescale than perceptual learning studies, which typically require multiday training (Schoups et al., 2001). It remains unclear whether visual cortical plasticity can occur under the same timescale as VDAC.
Value signals outside the cortex
Alternatively to the visual cortical plasticity account, VDAC may be explained by plasticity in subcortical regions (Anderson, 2016; Hikosaka et al., 2019), to which MEG is relatively insensitive (Hillebrand and Barnes, 2002). One route may be a recently identified circuit involved in rapid orienting to reward-associated stimuli, which includes the tail of the caudate nucleus, thesubstantia nigra pars reticulata, and the superior colliculus (Yamamoto et al., 2012, 2013; Yasuda and Hikosaka, 2015; Griggs et al., 2018; Hikosaka et al., 2019). Learned value modulates neural activity in each of these regions, which can ultimately affect visual cortical processing via the thalamus (Hikosaka et al., 2019).
Value information can affect selective attention via multiple parallel pathways (Hikosaka et al., 2019). Models of corticobasal ganglia interactions suggest that value may first be encoded in the basal ganglia, which gradually establishes stimulus–value associations in the cortex (Houk and Wise, 1995; Hélie et al., 2015). Consistent with this, value coding in LIP and extrastriate cortical neurons shifts earlier (e.g., earlier than 200 ms) after days of training (Peck et al., 2009; Frankó et al., 2010). It is therefore possible that with longer training we would have observed earlier cortical value signals in the current study. Other candidate regions for reward-related plasticity include the amygdala and hippocampus, both of which show learning-driven value coding (Paton et al., 2006; Peck and Salzman, 2014; Chau et al., 2015; Wimmer et al., 2018).
Conclusion
Overall, our results suggest that VDAC is underpinned by learned value signals that modulate spatial selection throughout posterior visual and parietal cortex. However, despite previous studies suggesting an important role for early cortical plasticity, we suggest that VDAC can occur in the absence of changes in early processing in cortex.
Footnotes
This research was funded by Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council Grant BB/M010732/1 and James S. McDonnell Foundation Scholar Award 220020405 to M.G.S.; and by the NIHR Oxford Health Biomedical Research Center. The Wellcome Center for Integrative Neuroimaging is supported by core funding from the Wellcome Trust (Grant 203139/Z/16/Z), and The Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO Veni Grant 016.Veni.198.065 awarded to E.S.).
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
References
- Ahlfors SP, Han J, Belliveau JW, Hämäläinen MS (2010) Sensitivity of MEG and EEG to source orientation. Brain Topogr 23:227–232. 10.1007/s10548-010-0154-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anderson BA. (2013) A value-driven mechanism of attentional selection. J Vis 13(3):7, 1–16. 10.1167/13.3.7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anderson BA. (2016) The attention habit: how reward learning shapes attentional selection. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1369:24–39. 10.1111/nyas.12957 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anderson BA, Laurent PA, Yantis S (2011) Value-driven attentional capture. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 108:10367–10371. 10.1073/pnas.1104047108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anderson BA, Laurent PA, Yantis S (2014) Value-driven attentional priority signals in human basal ganglia and visual cortex. Brain Res 1587:88–96. 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.08.062 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Awh E, Belopolsky AV, Theeuwes J (2012) Top-down versus bottom-up attentional control: a failed theoretical dichotomy. Trends Cogn Sci 16:437–443. 10.1016/j.tics.2012.06.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barbaro L, Peelen MV, Hickey C (2017) Valence, not utility, underlies reward-driven prioritization in human vision. J Neurosci 37:10438–10450. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1128-17.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beck DM, Kastner S (2009) Top-down and bottom-up mechanisms in biasing competition in the human brain. Vision Res 49:1154–1165. 10.1016/j.visres.2008.07.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bisley JW, Goldberg ME (2010) Attention, intention, and priority in the parietal lobe. Annu Rev Neurosci 33:1–21. 10.1146/annurev-neuro-060909-152823 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buffalo EA, Fries P, Landman R, Liang H, Desimone R (2010) A backward progression of attentional effects in the ventral stream. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 107:361–365. 10.1073/pnas.0907658106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chau BKH, Sallet J, Papageorgiou GK, Noonan MP, Bell AH, Walton ME, Rushworth MFS (2015) Contrasting roles for orbitofrontal cortex and amygdala in credit assignment and learning in macaques. Neuron 87:1106–1118. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.08.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chelazzi L, Perlato A, Santandrea E, Della Libera C (2013) Rewards teach visual selective attention. Vision Res 85:58–72. 10.1016/j.visres.2012.12.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chelazzi L, Eštočinová J, Calletti R, Lo Gerfo E, Sani I, Della Libera C, Santandrea E (2014) Altering spatial priority maps via reward-based learning. J Neurosci 34:8594–8604. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0277-14.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corbetta M, Shulman GL (2002) Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 3:201–215. 10.1038/nrn755 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Desimone R, Duncan J (1995) Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annu Rev Neurosci 18:193–222. 10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.001205 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Donohue SE, Hopf J-M, Bartsch MV, Schoenfeld MA, Heinze H-J, Woldorff MG (2016) The rapid capture of attention by rewarded objects. J Cogn Neurosci 28:529–541. 10.1162/jocn_a_00917 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Egeth HE, Yantis S (1997) Visual attention: control, representation, and time course. Annu Rev Psychol 48:269–297. 10.1146/annurev.psych.48.1.269 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Failing M, Theeuwes J (2018) Selection history: how reward modulates selectivity of visual attention. Psychon Bull Rev 25:514–538. 10.3758/s13423-017-1380-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Failing MF, Theeuwes J (2014) Exogenous visual orienting by reward. J Vis 14(5):6, 1–9. 10.1167/14.5.6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Failing MF, Theeuwes J (2015) Nonspatial attentional capture by previously rewarded scene semantics. Vis Cogn 23:82–104. 10.1080/13506285.2014.990546 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Faul F, Erdfelder E, Buchner A, Lang A-G (2009) Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav Res Methods 41:1149–1160. 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foxe JJ, Simpson GV (2002) Flow of activation from V1 to frontal cortex in humans. Exp Brain Res 142:139–150. 10.1007/s00221-001-0906-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frank DW, Sabatinelli D (2012) Stimulus-driven reorienting in the ventral frontoparietal attention network: the role of emotional content. Front Hum Neurosci 6:116. 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00116 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frankó E, Seitz AR, Vogels R (2010) Dissociable neural effects of long-term stimulus-reward pairing in macaque visual cortex. J Cogn Neurosci 22:1425–1439. 10.1162/jocn.2009.21288 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ghazizadeh A, Griggs W, Leopold DA, Hikosaka O (2018) Temporal-prefrontal cortical network for discrimination of valuable objects in long-term memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 115:E2135–E2144. 10.1073/pnas.1707695115 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gottlieb J, Snyder LH (2010) Spatial and non-spatial functions of the parietal cortex. Curr Opin Neurobiol 20:731–740. 10.1016/j.conb.2010.09.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Griggs WS, Amita H, Gopal A, Hikosaka O (2018) Visual neurons in the superior colliculus discriminate many objects by their historical values. Front Neurosci 12:396. 10.3389/fnins.2018.00396 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hélie S, Ell SW, Ashby FG (2015) Learning robust cortico-cortical associations with the basal ganglia: an integrative review. Cortex 64:123–135. 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.10.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hickey C, Peelen MV (2015) Neural mechanisms of incentive salience in naturalistic human vision. Neuron 85:512–518. 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.12.049 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hickey C, Peelen MV (2017) Reward selectively modulates the lingering neural representation of recently attended objects in natural scenes. J Neurosci 37:7297–7304. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0684-17.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hickey C, Chelazzi L, Theeuwes J (2010) Reward changes salience in human vision via the anterior cingulate. J Neurosci 30:11096–11103. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1026-10.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hikosaka O, Kim HF, Amita H, Yasuda M, Isoda M, Tachibana Y, Yoshida A (2019) Direct and indirect pathways for choosing objects and actions. Eur J Neurosci 49:637–645. 10.1111/ejn.13876 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hillebrand A, Barnes GR (2002) A quantitative assessment of the sensitivity of whole-head MEG to activity in the adult human cortex. Neuroimage 16:638–650. 10.1006/nimg.2002.1102 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Houk JC, Wise SP (1995) Distributed modular architectures linking basal ganglia, cerebellum, and cerebral cortex: their role in planning and controlling action. Cereb Cortex 5:95–110. 10.1093/cercor/5.2.95 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Husain M, Nachev P (2007) Space and the parietal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci 11:30–36. 10.1016/j.tics.2006.10.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Itthipuripat S, Cha K, Rangsipat N, Serences JT (2015) Value-based attentional capture influences context-dependent decision-making. J Neurophysiol 114:560–569. 10.1152/jn.00343.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Itthipuripat S, Vo VA, Sprague TC, Serences JT (2019) Value-driven attentional capture enhances distractor representations in early visual cortex. PLoS Biol 17:e3000186. 10.1371/journal.pbio.3000186 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jahfari S, Theeuwes J (2017) Sensitivity to value-driven attention is predicted by how we learn from value. Psychon Bull Rev 24:408–415. 10.3758/s13423-016-1106-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kahnt T, Heinzle J, Park SQ, Haynes J-D (2010) The neural code of reward anticipation in human orbitofrontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 107:6010–6015. 10.1073/pnas.0912838107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kleiner M, Brainard D, Pelli D, Ingling A, Murray R, Broussard C (2007) What's new in psychtoolbox-3. Perception 36:1–16. [Google Scholar]
- Kriegeskorte N, Goebel R, Bandettini P (2006) Information-based functional brain mapping. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 103:3863–3868. 10.1073/pnas.0600244103 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lawrence MA. (2016) ez: easy analysis and visualization of factorial experiments. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation. [Google Scholar]
- Le Pelley ME, Vadillo M, Luque D (2013) Learned predictiveness influences rapid attentional capture: evidence from the dot probe task. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 39:1888–1900. 10.1037/a0033700 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Le Pelley ME, Mitchell CJ, Beesley T, George DN, Wills AJ (2016) Attention and associative learning in humans: an integrative review. Psychol Bull 142:1111–1140. 10.1037/bul0000064 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ledoit O, Wolf M (2004) A well-conditioned estimator for large-dimensional covariance matrices. J Multivar Anal 88:365–411. 10.1016/S0047-259X(03)00096-4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- LeMessurier AM, Feldman DE (2018) Plasticity of population coding in primary sensory cortex. Curr Opin Neurobiol 53:50–56. 10.1016/j.conb.2018.04.029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luck SJ, Woodman GF, Vogel EK (2000) Event-related potential studies of attention. Trends Cogn Sci 4:432–440. 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01545-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luque D, Beesley T, Morris RW, Jack BN, Griffiths O, Whitford TJ, Le Pelley ME (2017) Goal-directed and habit-like modulations of stimulus processing during reinforcement learning. J Neurosci 37:3009–3017. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3205-16.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacLean MH, Giesbrecht B (2015a) Neural evidence reveals the rapid effects of reward history on selective attention. Brain Res 1606:86–94. 10.1016/j.brainres.2015.02.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacLean MH, Giesbrecht B (2015b) Irrelevant reward and selection histories have different influences on task-relevant attentional selection. Atten Percept Psychophys 77:1515–1528. 10.3758/s13414-015-0851-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacLean MH, Diaz GK, Giesbrecht B (2016) Irrelevant learned reward associations disrupt voluntary spatial attention. Atten Percept Psychophys 78:2241–2252. 10.3758/s13414-016-1103-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marcus D, Harwell J, Olsen T, Hodge M, Glasser M, Prior F, Jenkinson M, Laumann T, Curtiss S, Van Essen D (2011) Informatics and data mining tools and strategies for the human connectome project. Front Neuroinform 5:4. 10.3389/fninf.2011.00004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miyashita Y. (1988) Neuronal correlate of visual associative long-term memory in the primate temporal cortex. Nature 335:817–820. 10.1038/335817a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moore T, Zirnsak M (2017) Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annu Rev Psychol 68:47–72. 10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033400 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nolte G. (2003) The magnetic lead field theorem in the quasi-static approximation and its use for magnetoencephalography forward calculation in realistic volume conductors. Phys Med Biol 48:3637–3652. 10.1088/0031-9155/48/22/002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oostenveld R, Fries P, Maris E, Schoffelen J-M (2011) FieldTrip: open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Comput Intell Neurosci 2011:156869. 10.1155/2011/156869 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Op de Beeck HP, Baker CI (2010) The neural basis of visual object learning. Trends Cogn Sci 14:22–30. 10.1016/j.tics.2009.11.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paton JJ, Belova MA, Morrison SE, Salzman CD (2006) The primate amygdala represents the positive and negative value of visual stimuli during learning. Nature 439:865–870. 10.1038/nature04490 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peck CJ, Salzman CD (2014) Amygdala neural activity reflects spatial attention towards stimuli promising reward or threatening punishment. Elife 3:e04478 10.7554/eLife.04478 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peck CJ, Jangraw DC, Suzuki M, Efem R, Gottlieb J (2009) Reward modulates attention independently of action value in posterior parietal cortex. J Neurosci 29:11182–11191. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1929-09.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pernet CR, Wilcox R, Rousselet GA (2012) Robust correlation analyses: false positive and power validation using a new open source matlab toolbox. Front Psychol 3:606. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poort J, Khan AG, Pachitariu M, Nemri A, Orsolic I, Krupic J, Bauza M, Sahani M, Keller GB, Mrsic-Flogel TD, Hofer SB (2015) Learning enhances sensory and multiple non-sensory representations in primary visual cortex. Neuron 86:1478–1490. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.05.037 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Posner MI. (1980) Orienting of attention. Q J Exp Psychol 32:3–25. 10.1080/00335558008248231 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Qi S, Zeng Q, Ding C, Li H (2013) Neural correlates of reward-driven attentional capture in visual search. Brain Res 1532:32–43. 10.1016/j.brainres.2013.07.044 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raymond JE, O'Brien JL (2009) Selective visual attention and motivation: the consequences of value learning in an attentional blink task. Psychol Sci 20:981–988. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02391.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Richards JE, Sanchez C, Phillips-Meek M, Xie W (2016) A database of age-appropriate average MRI templates. Neuroimage 124:1254–1259. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.04.055 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoups A, Vogels R, Qian N, Orban G (2001) Practising orientation identification improves orientation coding in V1 neurons. Nature 412:549–553. 10.1038/35087601 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith SM, Nichols TE (2009) Threshold-free cluster enhancement: addressing problems of smoothing, threshold dependence and localisation in cluster inference. Neuroimage 44:83–98. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.061 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Srihasam K, Vincent JL, Livingstone MS (2014) Novel domain formation reveals proto-architecture in inferotemporal cortex. Nat Neurosci 17:1776–1783. 10.1038/nn.3855 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Theeuwes J, Belopolsky AV (2012) Reward grabs the eye: oculomotor capture by rewarding stimuli. Vision Res 74:80–85. 10.1016/j.visres.2012.07.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Todd TP, Vurbic D, Bouton ME (2014) Behavioral and neurobiological mechanisms of extinction in Pavlovian and instrumental learning. Neurobiol Learn Mem 108:52–64. 10.1016/j.nlm.2013.08.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Koningsbruggen MG, Ficarella SC, Battelli L, Hickey C (2016) Transcranial random-noise stimulation of visual cortex potentiates value-driven attentional capture. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 11:1481–1488. 10.1093/scan/nsw056 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Veen BD, van Drongelen W, Yuchtman M, Suzuki A (1997) Localization of brain electrical activity via linearly constrained minimum variance spatial filtering. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng 44:867–880. 10.1109/10.623056 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walther A, Nili H, Ejaz N, Alink A, Kriegeskorte N, Diedrichsen J (2016) Reliability of dissimilarity measures for multi-voxel pattern analysis. Neuroimage 137:188–200. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.12.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wimmer GE, Li JK, Gorgolewski KJ, Poldrack RA (2018) Reward learning over weeks versus minutes increases the neural representation of value in the human brain. J Neurosci 38:7649–7618. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0075-18.2018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winkler AM, Ridgway GR, Webster MA, Smith SM, Nichols TE (2014) Permutation inference for the general linear model. Neuroimage 92:381–397. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.060 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolff MJ, Ding J, Myers NE, Stokes MG (2015) Revealing hidden states in visual working memory using electroencephalography. Front Syst Neurosci 9:123. 10.3389/fnsys.2015.00123 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolff MJ, Jochim J, Akyürek EG, Stokes MG (2017) Dynamic hidden states underlying working-memory-guided behavior. Nat Neurosci 20:864–871. 10.1038/nn.4546 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woodman GF, Luck SJ (1999) Electrophysiological measurement of rapid shifts of attention during visual search. Nature 400:867–869. 10.1038/23698 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woolrich M, Hunt L, Groves A, Barnes G (2011) MEG beamforming using Bayesian PCA for adaptive data covariance matrix regularization. Neuroimage 57:1466–1479. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.04.041 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yamamoto S, Monosov IE, Yasuda M, Hikosaka O (2012) What and where information in the caudate tail guides saccades to visual objects. J Neurosci 32:11005–11016. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0828-12.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yamamoto S, Kim HF, Hikosaka O (2013) Reward value-contingent changes of visual responses in the primate caudate tail associated with a visuomotor skill. J Neurosci 33:11227–11238. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0318-13.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yarkoni T. (2009) Big correlations in little studies: inflated fMRI correlations reflect low statistical power—commentary on Vul et al. (2009). Perspect Psychol Sci 4:294–298. 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01127.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yasuda M, Hikosaka O (2015) Functional territories in primate substantia nigra pars reticulata separately signaling stable and flexible values. J Neurophysiol 113:1681–1696. 10.1152/jn.00674.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zelinsky GJ, Bisley JW (2015) The what, where, and why of priority maps and their interactions with visual working memory. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1339:154–164. 10.1111/nyas.12606 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
Analysis scripts and data are available at https://osf.io/6ky8b/.