Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2020 Jun 25;9:e54978. doi: 10.7554/eLife.54978

Controlling motor neurons of every muscle for fly proboscis reaching

Claire E McKellar 1,2,†,, Igor Siwanowicz 1, Barry J Dickson 1,3, Julie H Simpson 1,4,‡,
Editors: Ronald L Calabrese5, Ronald L Calabrese6
PMCID: PMC7316511  PMID: 32584254

Abstract

We describe the anatomy of all the primary motor neurons in the fly proboscis and characterize their contributions to its diverse reaching movements. Pairing this behavior with the wealth of Drosophila’s genetic tools offers the possibility to study motor control at single-neuron resolution, and soon throughout entire circuits. As an entry to these circuits, we provide detailed anatomy of proboscis motor neurons, muscles, and joints. We create a collection of fly strains to individually manipulate every proboscis muscle through control of its motor neurons, the first such collection for an appendage. We generate a model of the action of each proboscis joint, and find that only a small number of motor neurons are needed to produce proboscis reaching. Comprehensive control of each motor element in this numerically simple system paves the way for future study of both reflexive and flexible movements of this appendage.

Research organism: D. melanogaster

Introduction

To interact with the world, many organisms rely on an ability to reach an appendage towards a target, yet much remains to be discovered about how neural circuits control this prototypical goal-directed behavior. Here we investigate the fly proboscis (mouthparts), a segmented appendage homologous to a limb (Bridges and Dobzhansky, 1933). The proboscis is capable of directed reaching towards a specifically placed food target, using gustatory information from taste receptors on the legs as they move and contact food (Dethier, 1976). Besides feeding, the proboscis also extends during courtship (Sturtevant, 1915), grooming (Szebenyi, 1969), flight-associated respiration (Lehmann and Heymann, 2005), and wing expansion after eclosion (Baker and Truman, 2002), suggesting that it is a multifunctional appendage that can be employed flexibly.

A good starting point for mapping circuits underlying motor control is to identify motor neurons, since they are the outputs upon which higher order circuits must act. In adult flies, most muscles are innervated by only one or a few motor neurons (see, for example, Baek and Mann, 2009; O'Sullivan et al., 2018; Rajashekhar and Singh, 1994; Trimarchi and Schneiderman, 1994). Identifying many or all of the motor neurons involved in proboscis extension should be feasible with available genetic tools. Groups of neurons can be activated and silenced to determine their behavioral phenotypes using collections of GAL4 lines (Brand and Perrimon, 1993), which reproducibly target different groups of neurons. Previous efforts using GAL4 lines with broader expression patterns, or stochastic techniques to refine GAL4 lines, identified some of the critical motor neurons (Gordon and Scott, 2009; Schwarz et al., 2017). Here we use the split GAL4 technique, which intersects two expression patterns to genetically target only the cells in common between the two (Luan et al., 2006), to provide more precise and reproducible targeting of particular neurons. We create a collection of split GAL4 fly strains to identify and separately target the primary motor neurons for every proboscis muscle.

To support this collection of genetic reagents, we provide detailed anatomy of the proboscis from a variety of imaging techniques. We show each proboscis muscle in new detail, propose a systematic nomenclature to resolve earlier discrepancies, and use muscle insertion sites to predict function. We predict that 8 of the 16 proboscis muscles should be involved in proboscis positioning (such as reaching), and the other half should be involved in the proboscis’ other main function, pumping food through the pharynx (investigated by others; Flood et al., 2013; Manzo et al., 2012; Tissot et al., 1998). Here we focus on the predicted positioning muscles and define their roles by genetic activation and silencing of their motor neurons. Altogether, we achieve genetic control over every muscle involved in this form of directed reaching, and provide detailed anatomy of this appendage.

Results

Anatomy of the proboscis and its joints

Our first goal was to use updated imaging techniques to display the overall anatomy of the proboscis (Figure 1).

Figure 1. Anatomy of the proboscis.

Figure 1.

(A–C) Thick sagittal sections of fly heads with proboscis in various states of extension. Scale bars: 100 µm. Arrows, D: dorsal, A: anterior. (A) Muscles and other internal tissues stained with phalloidin (red). Cuticle and sclerites stained with calcofluor white (cyan). (B–C) Soft tissues digested away to reveal rigid external and internal cuticle. Magenta: locations of proboscis joints. Green: pharynx. Cyan: esophagus. (D) Lateral view of a feeding fly, showing proboscis touching surface near forelegs.

Work that predated confocal microscopy (Miller, 1950) showed that the proboscis is extended during feeding. Food is sucked in through the lip-like labella at the tip, then pumped through the pharynx, traveling through two segments of the proboscis, first the haustellum then the rostrum. Inside the head, food then enters the esophagus, which passes through a foramen in the brain, and carries food towards the gut. Figure 1A shows these structures by confocal imaging of thick sections of fly heads with the proboscis in different degrees of extension.

In the retracted state, the rostrum rests near the brain inside the head, and the haustellum is folded up against the rostrum (Figure 1A). When the rostrum extends, it is protracted ventrally out of the head capsule. The outer cuticle of the rostrum is largely unsclerotized, giving it flexibility to fold like skin; support for several muscles therefore comes from an internal skeleton of rigid cuticular structures (Figure 1A–C). The haustellum extends and flexes from its joint with the rostrum. These movements were confirmed in live flies by filming proboscis extension in a synchrotron particle accelerator, using x-rays to render flies partly transparent and show the movement of the segments inside the head capsule (Video 1).

Video 1. Synchrotron x-ray video of proboscis extension.

Download video file (11.7MB, mp4)

Looping video of proboscis extension in a fly tethered with head glued, in a synchrotron x-ray beam, sagittal view, 1/100 speed. This example primarily shows movement of the rostrum (labeled magenta in the later portion of the video), pivoting around the joint marked by crosshairs, with little extension of the haustellum and labella (green).

To locate proboscis joints with standard light cameras, proboscis extension was triggered in tethered flies by stimulating leg gustatory receptors with a droplet of sucrose (approximate joint locations marked, Video 2). Gluing the head and one proboscis joint shows only the movement of the remaining joint: the rostrum pivots around a point on the anterior head that we designate the rostrum joint (Video 3, and also visible in Video 1), and the haustellum pivots around a point near the rostrum-haustellum junction (Video 4). We digested soft tissues from the thick slices of fly heads, leaving the rigid external and internal cuticle to visualize the locations of the articulation points (Figure 1B,C). Without implying genetic homology, a useful analogy is that the rostrum and haustellum move somewhat like the human upper arm and lower arm, the first segment swinging away from the body and the second unfolding from the first. (The movements of the rostrum are called protraction vs. retraction, whereas the movements of the haustellum are extension vs. flexion [Miller, 1950]). For this study, we focused on movements in only two dimensions, in the vertical plane - reaching up and down with respect to the body axis.

Video 2. Proboscis joint movements in response to sucrose.

Download video file (979.5KB, mp4)

Sagittal view of a tethered fly (anterior: up) reaching the proboscis towards a droplet of sucrose presented to the legs. 1/30 speed. Magenta dots: approximate locations of two proboscis joints.

Video 3. Rostrum joint movement.

Download video file (1.1MB, mp4)

Proboscis movement restricted to rostrum joint by gluing haustellum (in or out) and head. Sagittal view of tethered fly, anterior: up, 1/3 speed. Magenta dot: approximate location of rostrum joint.

Video 4. Haustellum joint movement.

Download video file (1.5MB, mp4)

Proboscis movement restricted mainly to haustellum joint by gluing rostrum (in or out) and head. (Rostrum glued along one surface, not completely immobilized). Sagittal view of tethered fly, anterior: up, 1/3 speed. Magenta dot: approximate location of haustellum joint.

A quantitative description of proboscis reaching

Armed with this demonstration of how the segments of the proboscis subserve proboscis extension, we sought quantitative proof that the Drosophila proboscis is capable of directed reaching. We had observed that, for a freely moving fly to feed, the proboscis must extend ventrally if the food is on the surface on which the fly is standing (Figure 2A, Video 5). In contrast, we find that a male courting a female on a flat surface typically extends his proboscis anteriorly towards the female for courtship licking (Figure 2B, Video 6). To quantify reaching in these particular situations, the angles of the head (Figure 2C), rostrum (Figure 2C), and haustellum (Figure 2D) were measured. Both the head and the rostrum were angled higher (more anteriorly) in the courtship experiments than in feeding, on average (Figure 2E–F). The haustellum was more extended for ventral feeding, and more flexed for anterior courtship licking (Figure 2G). The proboscis is thus capable of reaching to ethologically relevant targets in different locations, making use of the head, the rostrum and the haustellum.

Figure 2. The fly proboscis as a model system for directed reaching.

Figure 2.

(A) A fly extending the proboscis towards food on the surface of an experimental chamber (sagittal view). (B) A male fly (left) courting a female (right), extending his proboscis towards the female’s posterior. (C) Points used for measurement of angles of the head (connected by the longer lines) and rostrum (shorter lines). Scale bar: 200 µm. (D) Measurement of haustellum angle. (E–G) Angle of head (E), rostrum (F) and haustellum (G) in males feeding or courting on a flat surface. n = 15 flies per condition. T-test (unpaired) *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001, N.S.: not significant. (H–I) Proboscis extension in response to a low (H) or high (I) sucrose droplet presented to the legs of a tethered fly. Scale bar: 500 µm. (J–L) n = 28 males each presented with sucrose once in low position, once in high. T-test (paired). (J) Angle of target from the fly at frame of first leg contact, when target placed in low or high positions, where 90° would be directly ventral to the eye. (K) Mean reach angle: angle from the posterior-anterior axis of the fly to the proboscis tip, averaged over proboscis extension bout. (L) Aim deviation: reach angle minus target angle. (M–O) Joint angles scored 200 ms after beginning of PE: head (M), rostrum (N), haustellum (O).

Video 5. Proboscis extension during feeding.

Download video file (1,014.8KB, mp4)

A wildtype male feeding from sucrose painted onto wall of chamber (bright region) extends the proboscis ventrally towards the food. 1/1 speed.

Video 6. Proboscis extension during courtship.

Download video file (603.8KB, mp4)

A male (left) courting a female (right) on a flat surface extends the proboscis anteriorly towards the female. 1/10 speed, wildtype flies.

This differential reaching may not be inherently due to feeding or courtship, but to the location of the targets; perhaps the food was just located more ventrally than the female, prompting more ventral proboscis extension. We thus quantified proboscis reaching to food targets placed in different locations with respect to the head of a tethered fly. When the sucrose was placed low or high (more ventral or more anterior, Figure 2H–J), the proboscis was indeed capable of low vs. high reaching (Figure 2K, Video 7), with decent aim (Figure 2L). Although individual flies could angle joints differentially to different targets (gray lines, Figure 2M–O), only the haustellum showed a mean difference across target positions (black bars, Figure 2M–O), suggesting that the haustellum can be independently controlled to aid reaching. Each of the three body parts (rostrum, haustellum and head) is capable of at least some degree of independent movement, since if two of the three parts were glued, the remaining part could still move (Videos 3, 4 and 8). Reaching was guided not by a visual target (due to filming under invisible infrared light) but by contact between the waving legs and the sucrose target; the relevant sensory stimuli are therefore: 1) taste, and 2) proprioception about leg position at the time of sucrose contact.

Video 7. Proboscis reaching to targets in different locations.

Download video file (1.6MB, mp4)

Proboscis and head movements reaching towards a sucrose target in high or low (anterior or ventral) locations. Sagittal view of tethered fly, anterior: up, 1/1 speed.

Video 8. Head movement.

Download video file (323.7KB, mp4)

Head movement towards sucrose when both rostrum and haustellum joints are glued (not labella). Sagittal view of tethered fly, anterior: up, 1/3 speed.

An updated inventory of proboscis muscles

To explore how anatomy supports this reaching, we adapted a clearing technique (Ott, 2008) to stain and image whole-mount heads, and reveal intact muscles and their motor neurons with high clarity (Video 9). Using 3D segmentation, we traced every muscle in the proboscis of the mature adult (Figure 3A, Video 10). We find a total count of 16 proboscis muscles. (Other muscles are present at eclosion, possibly to help the fly push out of the puparium, but subsequently degenerate, Miller, 1950).

Figure 3. Proboscis muscles.

Figure 3.

(A) Frontal view of head with traced proboscis muscles, from clearing technique and segmentation software. Eyes removed at sides, and antennae removed, leaving two holes seen in upper center. Scale bar: 50 µm. (B) Schematics from different views as noted, showing proboscis muscles, brain, esophagus and pharynx. Pharynx superimposed for visibility (approximate outline: dotted line). D: dorsal, M: medial, A: anterior.

Video 9. Confocal stack showing an example of raw data from the head clearing technique, first in composite then each channel individually.

Download video file (1.8MB, mp4)

Red: muscles stained with phalloidin. Green: GFP stain of several motor neuron types genetically targeted in this example, projecting to the rostrum. Blue: synapses stained with nc82.

Video 10. A fly head imaged by cuticle autofluorescence (gray) with proboscis pointing downwards and eyes cropped out of image at sides.

Download video file (7.9MB, mp4)

All muscles were stained with phalloidin (stain not shown), then traced in segmentation software to display every proboscis muscle in a different color, first overlaid, then individually. Last: approximate path of pharynx. (Pharynx is a more complex shape than shown here but is only visible as negative space with this staining, difficult to trace). The brain can be seen in dark gray within the head, with hook-shaped sclerites underneath it. Fat body and air sacs not shown.

Different names are given to identical muscles in recent papers (Flood et al., 2013; Schwarz et al., 2017), therefore we propose a nomenclature with new suffixes to resolve confusion, consisting of informative letters instead of numbers (Figure 3B, Table 1). For example, the former 2–1 and 2–2 are now 2D and 2V, referring to more dorsal or ventral insertion sites. A prefix of ‘m’ refers to muscle, ‘mn’ refers to motor neuron(s) for that muscle. (We show below separable neuronal control of the newly found m3L and its neighbor m3M; however their adjacent insertions and origins lead us to define these as separate groups of fibers in a single muscle, m3. Aside from this exception, suffixes denote separate muscles).

Table 1. Muscle names and the best split GAL4 lines that target their motor neurons.

Muscle names in different papers, plus our split GAL4s targeting each muscle, motor neurons present in those lines, and location of motor neuron dendrites, soma and which proboscis nerve the axon uses. In split GAL4 names, D = dorsal, V = ventral, M = medial, L = lateral. In almost every case, when the motor neurons were present, they occupied 100% of the NMJs on the relevant muscle, with the exceptions of 3 and 10, described in the text. Former 12–1 is more similar to 11 than to 12–2; renamed as 11V. 12D and 12V are named for proximity, not implying related function. Yellow: positioning muscles that do not insert on the pharynx. Green: pharyngeal muscles.

Miller, 1950 Rajashekhar and Singh, 1994 Flood et al., 2013 Schwarz et al., 2017 Muscle location Split GAL4 AD split half DBD split half MNs in # proboscis sides Dend- rites Soma Nerve
1,lateral labial adductor m. retractor of rostrum 1 rostrum mn1 VT043075 VT019731 mn1 in 18/18 vent vent vent
2,maxillary retractor m. not investigated 2–1 rostrum mn2D GMR11F07 VT064563 mn2D in 16/16 vent vent vent
not known not known 2–2 rostrum mn2V VT064563 GMR13E04 mn2V in 12/12 vent vent vent
3 flexor of labrum 3 rostrum mn3M &1 VT025784 VT063630 mn3M in 4/4 mn1 in 4/4 vent dors ND
3 flexor of labrum 3 rostrum mn3M&7 VT063630 GMR75F02 mn3M in 12/12 mn7 in 6/12 vent dors ND
not known flexor of labrum not known rostrum mn3L VT031145 GMR89F06 mn3L in 10/10 vent dors dors
4,maxillary m. not investigated 4 rostrum mn4 GMR48H12 GMR45G01 mn4 in 10/10 vent vent vent
5,labral compressor m. not investigated 5 haustellum mn5 VT033616 VT043145 mn5 in 10/10 vent dors dors
6,palpal m. retractor of paraphysis 6 haustellum mn6 GMR18B07 GMR81B12 mn6 in 10/10 vent vent vent
7,palpal m. retractor of furca 7 haustellum mn7 VT014959 VT001484 mn7 in 9/10 vent vent vent
8,transverse labial m. transverse m. of haust. 8 haustellum mn8 and 7 VT027168 VT015822 mn8 in 15/16 mn7 in 7/16 vent vent vent
9,lateral pharyngeal m. protractor of fulcrum 9 rostrum mn9 VT061715 VT005008 mn9 in 12/12 vent dors dors
10,median pharyngeal m. median pharyngeal m. 10 rostrum mn10 GMR14H09 VT020713 mn10 in 6/12 dors dors dors
11,pharyngeal m. dorsal pharyng. dilator m.11 11–1,11-2 rostrum mn11D VT020737 GMR10B11 mn11D in 10/10 dors dors dors
12,cibarial m. ventral pharyng. dilator m.12–1 12–1 rostrum mn11V and 12D VT050240 GMR10E04 mn11V in 21/22 mn12D in 21/22 dors dors dors
12,cibarial m. not known m.12–2 11–3 rostrum mn11V and 12D VT050240 GMR10E04 mn11V in 21/22 mn12D in 21/22 dors dors dors
not known not known 12–2 rostrum mn12V GMR80D06 GMR75F02 mn12V in 14/14 dors dors dors
13,dorsal salivary m. not investigated 13 rostrum mn13 VT043700 VT034258 mn13 in 12/12 mn4 in 4/12 dors vent vent

Achieving genetic control of proboscis motor neurons

To understand the neural control of these muscles, we created genetic reagents to target every primary proboscis motor neuron type. By inspecting thousands of Rubin and VT-GAL4 lines (Jenett et al., 2012; Pfeiffer et al., 2010; Tirian and Dickson, 2017), we selected 100 lines with neurons resembling candidate proboscis motor neurons (characterized by large somata in the SEZ and axons in the SEZ nerves), to stain with the new head clearing technique. Of these, 77 indeed had motor neurons projecting to proboscis muscles (Table 2; see example in Video 9). Few contained only a single motor neuron type, and all contained many other cells in the brain.

Table 2. Head stain results from 100 GAL4 lines.

Lines from Rubin (GMR) and Dickson (VT) collections, in attP2 landing site, showing motor neurons found in proboscis. Note: this clearing technique showed more motor neuron types than were previously known in two lines, GMR18B07 and GMR81B12 (Schwarz et al., 2017).

GAL4 line Motor neurons in proboscis GAL4 line Motor neurons in proboscis GAL4 line Motor neurons in proboscis
GMR10B11 11 or 12D,12V GMR32D10 none VT037554 none
GMR10E04 2V,7,8,11,12 GMR41G04 1 or 2?,6,7, 8, 9, 11, 12? VT037583 1,6
GMR10E06 none GMR70E08 9 VT037859 1,4,7?
GMR11B04 1,2V,4,6,7,10,11,12,13 GMR72D06 2V VT038335 none
GMR11C05 9,11D GMR75F02 3M,7,12V VT039475 none
GMR11D03 11 or 12? (stochastic) GMR78E09 5,9,3? VT041887 none
GMR11D04 2(D?),7,8 GMR81B12 6,10,1 stochastic VT042739 none
GMR11D09 none GMR89F06 3,5 VT043075 1
GMR11F07 1,2D,4,9,11,12V VT000831 6,7,8 VT043088 1,7
GMR11F12 6 VT001484 3,7 VT043145 5,9,12V
GMR11G01 4,8 VT003229 1,4,11 VT043147 2V
GMR12E03 2V VT005008 9 VT043700 2D,6,13
GMR13B02 3L,4 VT011965 1,6,7 VT049356 none
GMR13E04 2V, 8? VT013506 11 VT049481 1,4,8,10
GMR13F07 none VT013605 1,2V,6,7,9,10, (12 or 3),13 VT049727 none
GMR14H09 10 VT014959 6,7,8 VT050217 11
GMR15E02 1,2D,2V,3L,4,5,6,7,12V VT015822 1,7,8 VT050240 3,11,12
GMR15E10 none VT017933 8 VT050663 4,6,7
GMR17D03 9,10 VT019731 1,7 VT055404 none
GMR18A08 none VT020713 10 VT056658 none
GMR18B07 7(stochastic),8(stoch.),9,10(dim),11,12,12V VT020737 11 VT057137 2D,2V
GMR18D09 none (in brains, one stochastic MN) VT022244 none VT057237 6,7,8
GMR18G02 1,2D,2V,4,5,6,7,8 VT023789 1,7,8,9 VT057379 6
GMR19A06 11 VT025784 1,3 VT058488 1,4,7,13
GMR19G04 1,2D,2V,3L,4,5,7,10,11,12V VT026026 8 VT059784 10,11,12
GMR20E07 6 VT027168 7,8 VT061715 9
GMR20E09 2D,2V VT031145 2V,3 VT062553 1,6,7,13
GMR20G03 none VT031157 11,12V VT063219 1,6,13
GMR21A10 none VT031562 4,12V VT063302 none
GMR23C08 4 VT032912 2D,2V,4,8 VT063630 1,2V(stochastic),3,4,7,8
GMR24A06 none VT033616 5 VT063635 2V
GMR27G06 9,10,11 VT034258 4,13 VT064563 2D,2V
GMR29F03 none VT037492 7,8 VT065306 none
GMR32A11 1,3,4,5,6,7,8,9,10,11,12,13,12V

To obtain more specific expression patterns, we intersected these lines using the split GAL4 genetic technique (Luan et al., 2006). A many-to-many approach was taken, staining several combinations of lines that each contained a motor neuron of interest (along with other cells). A final collection of 17 split GAL4 lines was selected, most of which target a single motor neuron type (with exceptions being one line targeting both mn11V and 12D, and three lines for mn3) (Table 1). Proboscis staining showed each line’s muscle targets (Figure 4), with good reproducibility of expression patterns across specimens (Table 1). Central nervous system (CNS) staining showed that besides expression in proboscis motor neurons, these 17 were otherwise quite sparse (Figure 4—figure supplement 1), having been selected from a total of 389 combinations by CNS staining.

Figure 4. A collection of fly strains to genetically control every proboscis muscle.

Confocal stacks of split GAL4 lines showing the proboscis muscles (magenta) targeted by the motor neurons of the collection (left images; green). (Note: cuticular structures can also autofluoresce green). Scale bar: 50 µm. Gain and contrast adjusted. Right images: location of those muscles in the head schematic from Figure 3, at a reduced scale.

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Sparse lines providing genetic access to specific proboscis motor neurons.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

Confocal maximum projections of brains (above) and VNCs (below) of split GAL4 lines targeting proboscis motor neurons. Targeted neurons: green (GFP). Counterstain: magenta (nc82). Most lines contain a single motor neuron type in SEZ; a few contain more than one (numbers in Table 1, along with split GAL4s used). Gain and contrast adjusted. Scale bar: 50 µm.
Figure 4—figure supplement 2. Completeness of motor neuron coverage in split GAL4 collection.

Figure 4—figure supplement 2.

(A) Example proboscis stained for muscles (phalloidin, red), genetically targeted motor neuron (mn9, GFP, green), and neuromuscular junctions (nc82, blue), to determine whether all synaptic sites on the appropriate muscle are occupied by the incoming green axon(s) in that line. (More examples of this staining are shown in Video 9 and in B). Frontal view, dorsal at top. (B) Partial stack projections cropped in the region of each muscle showing neuromuscular junction synapses (nc82, left panels within each group, magenta in overlay) and motor neuron terminals (GFP, middle panels, green in overlay). Gain and contrast adjusted. Scale bars: 50 µm.

Our goal in building this collection of lines was to target every primary excitatory motor neuron type of the proboscis. In the larva, the primary excitatory motor neurons are called Type Ib and Is; the motor neurons here most closely resemble the majority of larval Type Ib neurons in that they innervate only a single muscle, unlike the more promiscuous larval type Is (Budnik and Ruiz-Canada, 2006). (We did not seek modulatory motor neurons, analogous to type II and III in larva, which are characterized by small boutons on thin axons projecting to multiple muscles [Prokop, 2006], and are known to exist in the adult proboscis [Pauls et al., 2018].) To determine whether this collection was complete, we stained each line with nc82, considered a pan-synaptic marker (Wagh et al., 2006). All the boutons on the relevant muscle were indeed completely occupied by the labeled motor neurons (examples in Figure 4—figure supplement 2, N in Table 1), with two exceptions: muscle 3 is covered by separate lines (mn3M and mn3L) so was not expected to show complete occupancy, and mn10 was never labeled bilaterally in the split GAL4 line we selected (see Methods). All other synaptic sites were occupied, and since the synaptic marker does label primary motor neuron synapses, we conclude that this collection includes every primary motor neuron. The coverage of all a muscle’s synaptic sites by only one or a few motor neurons per hemisphere shows that single motor neurons must branch to many or all of the separate fibers that make up a muscle (Figure 4—figure supplements 1 and 2).

Organization of motor neuron dendrites in the brain

While the axons of motor neurons innervate muscles, their dendrites are located in the subesophageal zone of the brain. The organization of these dendrites was examined by segmenting them from split GAL4 images (or from stochastic labeling of split GAL4s, in cases where neighboring cells in the brain made identities uncertain), to give a clear view of every defined motor neuron type (Figure 5, Video 11).

Figure 5. Proboscis motor neuron collection: arbors in brain (subesophageal zone).

(A) Motor neurons from the split GAL4 lines in Table 1, segmented to show arbors in isolation. Colors match muscles in previous figures. Most motor neurons are segmented from split GAL4 combinations in which their arbors are clearly distinguishable, with the exception that mn3M, 8, 11V, 12D and 13 are segmented from stochastic staining in order to separate them from nearby cells. Single neurons from stochastic staining are superimposed upon their mirror images to show bilateral arbors, for comparison with the neurons segmented bilaterally. mn10 is shown unilaterally, since it was never found bilaterally in any split GAL4 combination. Scale bar: 50 µm. (B) Motor neurons colored according to whether their dendrites are primarily dorsal (magenta) or ventral (green). Magenta: 10, 11D, 11V, 12D, 12V (13: not shown). Rest: ventral.

Figure 5.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1. Location of motor neuron dendrites relative to other cell types in brain.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1.

Motor neuron confocal images (green) from brains computationally aligned with brains stained for other cell types in magenta: (A–H) sweet taste inputs (Gr64f-GAL4), (J) eye mechanosensory inputs (VT017251-LexA), (K) bitter taste inputs (Gr66a-GAL4), and (L–N) Hugin neurons (HUGS3-GAL4). Frontal view (‘front’) or sagittal view (‘side’) of confocal maximum projections, except single sections where noted. Each cell type stained individually for GFP then aligned and overlaid. Gain and contrast adjusted. Scale bar: 50 µm.

Video 11. Each proboscis motor neuron type, isolated using segmentation software.

Download video file (10.9MB, mp4)

Cropped to show only SEZ at the bottom of brain. Hole at top: esophageal foramen.

Each line has only one or a few bilateral motor neuron pairs (Figure 5) to cover its muscle. All dendritic arbors except salivary mn13 fall into one of two regions, dorsal or ventral, which partially overlap (Figure 5B). These divisions did not correlate with muscles found in the rostrum vs. haustellum (Table 1). Instead, with few exceptions, the dorsal region was occupied by motor neurons with pharyngeal insertion sites, whereas the ventral region was generally occupied by non-pharyngeal motor neurons (Table 1). Pharyngeal and non-pharyngeal motor neurons also tended to have dorsal and ventral somas, respectively, and axons leaving the brain through dorsal and ventral proboscis nerves (‘pharyngeal’ and ‘compound labial’ nerves, respectively, Hartenstein et al., 2018), with certain exceptions (Figure 5B, Table 1).

The segmented motor neuron dendrites permit computational registration with images of other feeding-related neuron classes, to determine proximity between them (although not synaptic connectivity). Sweet inputs avoid the ventral arbors of the rostrum protractor’s mn9, passing through two circular gaps to terminate more dorsally (Figure 5—figure supplement 1A,B), consistent with their known lack of contact with mn9 (Gordon and Scott, 2009). Sweet inputs likewise pass through gaps in the arbor of mn1 (Figure 5—figure supplement 1C,D), and also do not overlap with a pharyngeal neuron mn11D (Figure 5—figure supplement 1E,F). They do lie somewhat near the salivary mn13 (Figure 5—figure supplement 1G,H).

The circular gap in motor neuron arbors is particularly evident when several motor neurons (mn1, 9, 11D and 13) are overlaid (Figure 5—figure supplement 1I). We have previously shown that eye mechanosensory bristle neurons project to the ventral SEZ (Hampel et al., 2017), and now find that the glomerular-like terminals of these mechanosensory inputs occupy the circular gaps in motor neuron arbors (Figure 5—figure supplement 1J). Proboscis mechanosensory neurons also terminate in the same region (Zhou et al., 2019). Motor neurons are thus mainly excluded from the base of the compound labial nerve where certain mechanosensory and taste inputs enter the SEZ.

Although bitter taste can cause proboscis retraction (Dethier, 1976), we find that bitter inputs do not directly overlap the retractor motor neurons (one example: Figure 5—figure supplement 1K). The main retractor motor neuron, mn1, is near peptidergic Hugin neurons, thought to inhibit feeding initiation (Melcher and Pankratz, 2005; Figure 5—figure supplement 1L,M). Hugin neurons do not lie close to 9 or 11D (not shown), but do show proximity to the salivary mn13 (Figure 5—figure supplement 1N).

Fdg, a pair of command-like neurons that can activate several features of the feeding motor program (Flood et al., 2013), was segmented here from a broader GAL4 line. After computationally aligning images to a common template, Fdg shows little or no overlap with a retractor, protractor, or pharyngeal motor neuron (Video 12), suggesting that there are additional interneuron layers yet to be found.

Video 12. Translation through computational alignment of feeding command-like neuron ‘Fdg’ (magenta) with motor neurons 1, 9 and 11D (green) shown sequentially.

Download video file (2.2MB, mp4)

Motor neurons: confocal stacks. Fdg: manually segmented from a line with a broader expression pattern, NP883-GAL4. Central brain, with SEZ at bottom. Gain and contrast adjusted.

Muscles that control the pharynx

Although interneuronal circuits for proboscis motor control will be of future interest, here we aimed to establish the general functions of proboscis muscles. Predictions about function can be made by understanding the insertion sites of muscles. We stained cuticle, tendons and muscle to reveal every muscle insertion site (Figure 6 and Figure 6—figure supplement 1). Previously, three muscles were thought to control the pumping of food through the pharynx Flood et al., 2013; Manzo et al., 2012; Tissot et al., 1998; our staining now reveals a total of eight muscles associated with the pharynx. One (m13) inserts at the junction between the salivary duct and the pharynx (Figure 6A) and is predicted to open a valve for salivation. The seven others insert on the pharynx wall (Figure 6A), either directly or through tendons (Figure 6—figure supplement 2A,B). During feeding, furrows (pseudotrachea) on the labella may channel fluid to the opening of the pharynx (Figure 6—figure supplement 2C). Because the seven muscles attached to the pharynx are arrayed along the pharynx in series, we predict that sequential contraction would cause a traveling local dilation of the pharynx wall to move a bolus of fluid through the proboscis (Video 13), acting against the elasticity of the pharynx wall rather than antagonistic muscle counterparts. After food has been pumped through the pharynx to the esophagus, a ring of muscles surrounding the esophagus suggests that peristaltic action takes over (Figure 6—figure supplement 2D). This model, based on our analysis of insertion sites, provides testable predictions for future experiments.

Figure 6. Muscle insertion sites predict function.

Sagittal view of proboscis, showing phalloidin-stained muscles (red) and calcofluor white-stained cuticle (cyan). Single optical slices from the planes shown in insets, from 300 µm vibratome sections. Dotted line: path of pharynx. Arrowheads: insertion sites of muscles that do contact the pharynx (A), and that do not (B), predicted to be involved in pumping vs. proboscis positioning, respectively. The salivary muscle 13 can be seen inserting at the junction of the salivary duct (hollow tube – open arrowheads) with the pharynx. 12V is out of the plane of view in (A) but its tendon inserts on the pharynx. Rest of muscle insertions shown in Figure 6—figure supplement 1. Scale bar: 50 µm.

Figure 6.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1. Additional muscle insertion sites.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1.

Sagittal view of proboscis, showing phalloidin-stained muscles (red) and calcofluor white-stained cuticle (cyan). (A,B) Single optical slices from the planes shown in insets, from 300 µm vibratome sections. Arrowheads: insertion sites of additional muscles that do not contact the pharynx, predicted to be involved in proboscis positioning. Scale bar: 50 µm.
Figure 6—figure supplement 2. Features of the alimentary canal.

Figure 6—figure supplement 2.

(A,B) Vibratome horizontal sections of the head and proboscis cut at proximal (A) or distal (B) levels of the rostrum, stained for muscles (phalloidin, magenta) and connective tissue at muscle attachments (acetylated tubulin, green; also labels neurons). Some pump muscles (A, arrowhead) insert directly on the wall of the pharynx (dark vertical opening and tube at center, asterisk), and others use tendons (B, arrowhead) to insert onto the pharynx (B, asterisk). Note that the dark cavity of the pharynx is not simply a cylindrical tube but widens into complex cavities within the rostrum. Maximum projections of approximately horizontal ~200 µm slices in 7% agarose. (A) Is close to the head and shows parts of the compound eyes at the sides (grids of bright dots), and (B) Is close to the distal end of the rostrum, where the maxillary palps can be seen as two structures protruding from the anterior surface of the proboscis, filled with sensory neurons. (C) Frontal view of distal proboscis from whole cleared head prep, with labella in open position, showing how pseudotrachea (furrows; arrowhead) lead food to the opening of the pharynx (asterisk). Phalloidin imaged at higher gain to show muscles as well as cuticle. (D) Frontal view of the opening of the esophagus (asterisk) at the base of the rostrum inside the head. Muscles (phalloidin, magenta) and neuromuscular junctions (nc82, white). Arrowhead: circular muscles surround the esophagus, enabling peristalsis. Scale bars: 50 µm. Gain and contrast adjusted.

Video 13. Predicted mechanism of pumping food (blue) through the pharynx (white) by sequential activation of the seven muscles that insert on the pharynx wall (colored as in Figures 3 and 4).

Download video file (127.5KB, mp4)

Sagittal view of head.

Eight other muscles were of more interest to us as they do not insert on the pharynx (Figure 6B and Figure 6—figure supplement 1), and are therefore candidates for proboscis positioning. Having genetic access to each motor neuron type, we manipulated each of the eight putative positioning motor neuron types to identify the muscles controlling reaching. We used a comprehensive battery of tests involving genetic activation and silencing, from the resting proboscis position and also during feeding from an extended position, measuring the position of the proboscis and the angle of each joint.

Muscles that control rostrum movements

Examining the joints in order of proximal to distal, we first examined the control of the rostrum. Muscle 9 had previously been shown to be involved in rostrum protraction by stochastically targeting its motor neuron from a more complicated expression pattern (Gordon and Scott, 2009). Now with reproducible genetic access to the bilateral pair of mn9, we activated it with CsChrimson (Klapoetke et al., 2014) and confirmed that it elicited proboscis extension (Figure 7A,K,L, Figure 7—figure supplement 1). (Joint angle measurements are reported as change in angle from baseline wherever possible, as explained in the Materials and methods, but are also available as raw angles in Figure 7—figure supplement 2). We saw not only protraction of the rostrum but also extension of the haustellum (Figure 7B–C, Video 14). Activation during feeding, when the proboscis is already extended, did not elicit any further extension (Figure 7D–E). Silencing mn9 with tetanus toxin (Sweeney et al., 1995) did not impair proboscis position or joint angles at rest (Figure 7F–H), but prevented the movement of both the rostrum and haustellum during natural feeding (Figure 7I–J, Video 14), leaving only the movement of the labella (Figure 9H, Video 14). mn9 thus shows both activation and silencing phenotypes at the rostrum joint, where its muscle is physically located, but also at the more distant haustellum joint. This demonstrates an example of coupling, where one joint’s action affects others, often for biomechanical reasons (Lang and Schieber, 2004). In sum, muscle 9 is a protractor of the rostrum, but with wider effects.

Figure 7. Motor control of the rostrum.

(A–E) CsChrimson activation of split GAL4s for rostrum motor neurons, compared to controls (unfilled), quantifying change in proboscis position from rest (total extension of proboscis at maximum movement divided by at rest; therefore no movement = 1) (A), change in rostrum angle from rest (B), change in haustellum angle from rest (C), change in rostrum angle from a protracted position during feeding (D), and change in haustellum angle from an extended position during feeding (E). (F–J) TNT silencing of split GAL4s for rostrum motor neurons, compared to controls, quantifying proboscis position at rest (F), rostrum angle at rest (G), haustellum angle at rest (H), change in rostrum angle from rest to feeding position (I), and change in haustellum angle from rest to feeding position (J). Bar: mean. Biological replicates, n = 14–16 flies/genotype. Asterisks: unpaired t-tests, experimental (colored) vs. each control (showing least significant), with multiple testing correction. *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001, no asterisk, not signficant. See Methods for further explanations of metrics shown. (K–L) Sagittal schematics of proboscis movements controlled by muscle 9 (green): protraction of rostrum and extension of haustellum (arrows). Muscle 9 origin: ventral wall of head. Muscle 9 insertion: internal part of rostrum cuticle (asterisk). Proboscis segments (dark gray) pivot around joints (magenta dots). (M–O) Proboscis movements controlled by muscles 1, 2D and 2V (colored): retraction of rostrum and haustellum (arrows). Muscle origins: posterior wall of head. (P–Q) Frontal view of whole-mount heads (blue) with segmented muscles (green, numbered) and the apodeme within the rostrum (white) that swings outward during rostrum extension, shown with rostrum retracted (P) or extended (Q). Scale bar: 50 µm.

Figure 7.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1. Examples of rostrum muscle actions.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1.

(A–B) Example mn9 phenotype: extension of both the rostrum and haustellum in response to CsChrimson activation (B), compared to resting proboscis before the stimulus (A). (C–D) Example retractor phenotype: retraction of the rostrum in response to CsChrimson activation of mn1 (D), compared to feeding position before the stimulus (dotted line) (C).
Figure 7—figure supplement 2. Raw joint angles, not normalized, for all motor neurons from Figures 79.

Figure 7—figure supplement 2.

(A–B) CsChrimson activation of split GAL4s for the motor neurons shown, quantifying raw rostrum angle in a protracted position during feeding (A) and raw haustellum angle in an extended position during feeding (B). (C–D) TNT silencing of the same split GAL4s, quantifying maximum raw rostrum angle during feeding (C) and maximum raw haustellum angle during feeding (D). Bar: mean. Biological replicates, n = 14–16 flies/genotype. Asterisks: unpaired t-tests, experimental (colored) vs. each control (unfilled), showing least significant, with multiple testing correction. *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001, no asterisk, not significant.

Video 14. Behavioral phenotypes of rostrum protractor.

Download video file (499.4KB, mp4)

First part: activation of rostrum protractor (mn9) with CsChrimson, in frames noted, compared to CsChrimson control, beginning with the proboscis in the resting (retracted) position. Second part: silencing of mn9 with TNT, compared to TNT control, in a feeding assay where normal flies fully extend the proboscis towards a droplet of sucrose. Tethered males, sagittal view (dorsal up), 1/30 speed.

What muscles might counteract the protractor action of muscle 9, leading to rostrum retraction? Three muscles (1, 2D, 2V) could act as rostrum retractors based on their origins on the posterior wall of the head, and insertions near the distal end of the rostrum where they could potentially withdraw the rostrum into the head capsule (Figure 7M–O). Activation of the motor neurons for the largest of these, muscle 1, indeed retracted the proboscis into the head further than where it sits at rest (Figure 7A), by retraction of the rostrum (Figure 7B) but not the haustellum (Figure 7C). Activating mn1 while the proboscis was extended for feeding triggered both retraction of the rostrum and flexion of the haustellum (Figure 7D–E, Figure 7—figure supplement 1C–D, Video 15). Whereas activation triggered retraction, silencing mn1 showed the opposite phenotype, impairing the retraction of the proboscis at rest (Figure 7F, Video 15). Consistent with the effects of muscle 1 on more than one joint in the activation experiments (Figure 7D–E), the impaired retraction in silencing experiments could not be attributed to one specific joint (Figure 7G–H). In sum, muscle 1 is a retractor, located in the rostrum but with additional effects on the haustellum.

Video 15. Behavioral roles of rostrum retractor.

Download video file (125.1KB, mp4)

First part: activation of the main rostrum retractor (mn1) with CsChrimson, in frames noted, compared to CsChrimson control, beginning with the proboscis in the extended position during feeding. Second part: silencing of mn1 with TNT, compared to TNT control, in the resting proboscis position to demonstrate that mn1 silencing results in incomplete proboscis retraction. Tethered males, sagittal view (dorsal up), 1/30 speed.

The other two putative rostrum retractor muscles, m2D and m2V, also affected retraction, but more specifically for the rostrum. They showed distinct roles; mn2V activation caused rostrum retraction from rest, whereas mn2D activation caused retraction from the extended position (Figure 7B,D). Silencing mn2V caused a small impairment in how retracted the rostrum is at rest (Figure 7G). Other silencing phenotypes for mn2V and mn2D were not observed (Figure 7F–H), suggesting that they may be at least partly redundant. In sum, muscles 2D and 2V are accessory rostrum retractors. Silencing the retractor muscles, 1, 2D or 2V, did not impair the extension of the rostrum or the haustellum during feeding, thus we conclude that these muscles are not required for proboscis extension (Figure 7I–J), only for retraction.

Combining these behavioral results with anatomical results, we propose a model of the action of the rostrum. Imaging the rostrum in different positions shows that its extension correlates with the movement of an apodeme, a large part of cuticle within the rostrum (Figure 7P–Q, Video 16). Only one point of the apodeme attaches to the external cuticle; this point forms the joint around which the rostrum rotates, seen in Video 1. Muscle 9 causes rostrum protraction by anchoring on the head capsule and pulling on the free dorsal arm of the apodeme (via a tendon visible in Figure 6B), swinging it ventrally out of the head capsule. To counteract this protraction, retraction is controlled by muscles 1, 2D and 2V, whose dorsal ends anchor on the head capsule, and whose ventral ends insert not on the apodeme, but on cuticle near its ventral end, where their contraction can draw the rostrum back into the head (and potentially aid haustellum retraction, discussed below). Their role in rostrum retraction is supported by their decreased length in the retracted state, their decreased distance between sarcomeres (muscle striations) in the retracted state (consistent with contraction) (Figure 7P–Q), and by the behavioral results. In sum, for the rostrum, we find evidence that muscle 9 controls protraction, and that three muscles control retraction (1, 2D, 2V).

Video 16. Mechanism of rostrum movement.

Download video file (6.6MB, mp4)

Head (blue), muscles involved in rostrum movement (green), and apodeme within rostrum (white). First part: schematic of rostrum movement (pivot point: red crosshairs). Muscles: 1 (long, at left), 2V and 2D (short, at left), and 9 (right). Dorsal up, anterior at right. Second part: same structures segmented from confocal images (sagittal view, maximum projection) with rostrum more retracted (left) or extended (right), showing direction of muscle action (arrows). Third part: same segmented structures in rotating views with rostrum more retracted followed by more extended.

Muscles that control haustellum movements

With the above characterization of rostrum muscles, we then sought to describe the much less well understood haustellum. Although it was affected by rostrum muscles described above, the haustellum can also move independently of the rostrum (Figure 2M–O, Video 4). We determined that the muscle responsible for haustellum extension is the previously uncharacterized muscle 4; activation of mn4 elicited haustellum extension both from the rest position (Figure 8C,K,L, Figure 8—figure supplement 1A–B, Video 17) and from the extended position during feeding (Figure 8E), resulting in an overall change in proboscis position (Figure 8A). (Raw angles available in Figure 7—figure supplement 2). These activation phenotypes were specific to the haustellum, not affecting the rostrum (Figure 8B,D). Silencing instead of activating mn4 caused a defect specific to haustellum extension during feeding (Figure 8F–J, Video 17). Muscle 4 therefore shows both activation and silencing phenotypes for haustellum extension. Since haustellum extension during feeding was virtually abolished by silencing mn4, its muscle may be the only one required for haustellum extension. Although silencing mn9 also abolished haustellum extension, that may be because it also blocks rostrum extension, making haustellum extension more difficult mechanically.

Figure 8. Motor control of the haustellum.

(A–E) CsChrimson activation of haustellum split GAL4s, compared to controls (unfilled), quantifying change in proboscis position from rest (A), change in rostrum angle from rest (B), change in haustellum angle from rest (C), change in rostrum angle from a protracted position during feeding (D), and change in haustellum angle from an extended position during feeding (E). (F–J) TNT silencing of haustellum split GAL4, compared to controls, quantifying proboscis position at rest (F), rostrum angle at rest (G), haustellum angle at rest (H), change in rostrum angle from rest to feeding position (I), and change in haustellum angle from rest to feeding position (J). Bar: mean. Biological replicates, n = 14–16 flies/genotype. Asterisks: unpaired t-tests, experimental (colored) vs. each control (showing least significant), with multiple testing correction. *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001, no asterisk, not signficant. (K–L) Sagittal schematics of proboscis movement controlled by muscle 4 (blue): extension of haustellum (arrow). Muscle 4 location: near haustellum joint. Proboscis segments (dark gray) pivot around joints (magenta dots). (M–N) Proboscis movement controlled by muscle 3 (light blue): flexion of haustellum (arrow). Muscle 3 origin: anterior rostrum. Insertion: asterisk. (O–P) Haustellum mechanism. Lateral view of proboscis (blue) with segmented muscles (green, numbered) and a Y-shaped apodeme (red in composite, white below), from thick sections with haustellum partly flexed (O) or partly extended (P). Muscle 4 inserts on the free dorsal arm of the apodeme (asterisk). Muscle 2V inserts on the anterior apodeme arm (plus sign). Muscle 3 inserts in the haustellum (via tendons not stained here). Arrows: rotation of apodeme, controlling extension of haustellum. Scale bar: 50 µm.

Figure 8.

Figure 8—figure supplement 1. Examples of haustellum muscle actions.

Figure 8—figure supplement 1.

(A–B) Example mn4 phenotype: extension of the haustellum in response to CsChrimson activation (B), compared to resting proboscis before the stimulus (A). (C–D) Example mn3 phenotype: flexion of the haustellum in response to CsChrimson activation of mn3L (D), compared to proboscis in feeding position before the stimulus (C).

Video 17. Behavioral roles of haustellum extensor.

Download video file (585.4KB, mp4)

First part: activation of haustellum extensor (mn4) with CsChrimson, in frames noted, compared to CsChrimson control, beginning with the proboscis in the resting (retracted) position. Second part: silencing of mn4 with TNT, compared to TNT control, in a feeding assay where normal flies fully extend the proboscis towards a droplet of sucrose. Tethered males, sagittal view (dorsal up), 1/30 speed.

In opposition to extension, we find that haustellum flexion is controlled by muscle 3. Before behavioral testing, we stained 139 different combinations of split GAL4 lines to attempt to target mn3 (data not shown). None cleanly targeted motor neurons for all fibers of muscle 3, but its lateral fibers and medial fibers could be separately targeted (Table 1). (Muscles are often made up of multiple fibers; when only a single primary motor neuron controls that muscle, it branches to boutons on each muscle fiber. We describe these lateral and medial fibers of m3 as one muscle since all have adjacent insertions and origins). One split GAL4 line targeted the lateral fibers (mn3L) cleanly, with no other proboscis motor neurons present. No lines cleanly targeted motor neurons for the medial fibers, thus we selected two imperfect lines, each of which targeted one additional proboscis motor neuron type, ‘mn3M&1’ and ‘mn3M&7’. The function of muscle 3M could be inferred if a phenotype were found in both these lines, and not in both of the clean lines for the contaminating cell types (1 or 7).

Activating mn3 demonstrated that it acts as a haustellum flexor. mn3M&1, mn3M&7 and mn3L all triggered haustellum flexion during feeding (Figure 8E,M,N, Figure 8—figure supplement 1C–D, Video 18). Although the contaminating cell type mn1 does so as well (Figure 7E), the contaminating mn7 does not (Figure 9N), suggesting that muscle 3 is responsible. From the resting proboscis position, all mn3 lines could trigger additional proboscis retraction (Figure 8A), although that could not be attributed to a specific joint (Figure 8B–C). As opposed to the effects on the haustellum, activation did not produce consistent effects on the rostrum (Figure 8D).

Video 18. Behavioral roles of haustellum flexor.

Download video file (192.6KB, mp4)

First part: activation of one of the two m3 haustellum flexors (using line mn3M&7) with CsChrimson, in frames noted, compared to CsChrimson control, beginning with the proboscis in the extended position during feeding. Second part: silencing of mn3M&7 with TNT, compared to TNT control, in the resting proboscis position to demonstrate that mn3M&7 silencing results in incomplete proboscis retraction. Tethered males, sagittal view (dorsal up), 1/30 speed.

In silencing experiments, mn3M&1 and mn3M&7 both impaired proboscis retraction at rest (Figure 8F, Video 18). Although the contaminating cell type mn1 does so as well (Figure 7F), the contaminating mn7 does not (Figure 9O), suggesting that muscle 3 is responsible. Silencing mn3 lines otherwise did not produce consistent phenotypes (Figure 8F–J), possibly because mn3L and mn3M were not both targeted together. In sum, by examining three different lines for mn3, we conclude that muscle 3 is a haustellum flexor.

Figure 9. Motor control of the labella.

Figure 9.

(A–C) Sagittal schematics of labellar movements controlled by muscles 7 and 6 (colored): abduction (B) and extension (C) of labella (arrows). Muscle origins: dorsal haustellum. Insertions: at labella (asterisks). Proboscis segments (dark gray) pivot around joints (magenta dots). (D–E) Example phenotype: extension of labella in response to CsChrimson activation of mn6 (E), compared to resting proboscis before the stimulus (D). (F–I) % flies showing labellar extension (F,H) or abduction (G,I) in response to CsChrimson activation (F,G) or TNT silencing (H,I) of motor neurons listed, compared to controls. Fisher’s exact test. Biological replicates, n = 16 flies/genotype. (J–N) CsChrimson activation of split GAL4s listed compared to controls (unfilled), quantifying change in proboscis position from rest (J), change in rostrum angle from rest (K), change in haustellum angle from rest (L), change in rostrum angle from a protracted position during feeding (M), and change in haustellum angle from an extended position during feeding (N). (O–S) TNT silencing of split GAL4s listed compared to controls, quantifying proboscis position at rest (O), rostrum angle at rest (P), haustellum angle at rest (Q), change in rostrum angle from rest to feeding position (R), and change in haustellum angle from rest to feeding position (S). Bar: mean. Biological replicates, n = 14–16 flies/genotype. Asterisks: unpaired t-tests, experimental (colored) vs. each control (showing least significant), with multiple testing correction. *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001, no asterisk, not signficant.

Combining these behavioral results with anatomical results, we propose a model of the action of the haustellum. Imaging the haustellum in different positions (Figure 8O–P) shows that its extension correlates with the rotation of a Y-shaped apodeme found within the joint. Of the three arms of the Y, the ventroposterior arm was continuous with the external cuticle of the rostrum, the anterior arm was continuous with the external cuticle of the haustellum, and the dorsal arm was free. We propose that a muscle pulling against the free dorsal arm could rotate the haustellum anteriorly away from the rostrum. Indeed, we found that muscle 4 originates from a rigid part of the rostrum and inserts upon the free dorsal arm of the Y, where its contraction would cause rotation of the apodeme and thus haustellum extension. Behavioral experiments above did show that muscle 4 was a haustellum extensor.

Another muscle, 2V, inserts upon the anterior arm (Figure 8O–P) where it could rotate the apodeme back in the other direction, dorsally. This muscle showed no haustellum phenotype, but its insertion site is suggestive enough to bear further future investigation for a role in haustellum flexion, possibly masked here by muscle redundancy. (Not included in Figure 8O–P is muscle 1, because although it had an activation phenotype on the haustellum (Figure 7E), this was possibly an indirect effect such as coupling, since there were no silencing effects and the muscle is not located in a position to control haustellum flexion). Instead, another muscle, 3, was ideally located to act as a flexor (Figure 8O–P); coming from the anterior rostrum, it crosses the joint with the haustellum so that it could pull on the haustellum to fold it in towards the rostrum. In behavioral experiments (above), muscle 3 indeed triggered haustellum flexion. In sum, we find evidence that muscle 4 controls haustellum extension and that muscle 3 (possibly also 2V) controls flexion. Indeed, based on distances between sarcomeres (Figure 8O–P), muscle 4 appears more contracted when the haustellum is more extended, and 2V and 3 appear more contracted when the haustellum is more flexed.

Muscles that control labellar movements

The remaining two muscles (6 and 7) of the eight tested for roles in proboscis positioning were both found to insert upon on the lip-like labella (Figures 6B and 9A–C). Muscle 6 shows both activation and silencing phenotypes for labellar extension (Figure 9F,H, Video 19). Muscle 7 shows activation and silencing phenotypes for labellar abduction (the opening of the two lobes, Figure 9G,I, Video 20). Although mn7 activation also induced labellar extension (Figure 9F), that may be a side effect of abduction; muscle 7 showed no effect on labellar extension when silenced (Figure 9H). The line contaminated with mn7, mn3M&7, showed similar phenotypes to mn7 but at lower penetrance (Figure 9F–I), likely due to the fact that mn7 was not labeled in all flies of that line (Table 1). Muscle 6 and 7 are therefore the only muscles that showed labellar phenotypes. They showed certain effects on other proboscis movements (Figure 9J–S) which we interpret to be side effects, since they insert far from the rostrum or haustellum joints (Figure 9A). (Raw angles available in Figure 7—figure supplement 2).

Video 19. Behavioral roles of labellar extensor.

Download video file (477.5KB, mp4)

First part: activation of labellar extensor (mn6) with CsChrimson, in frames noted, compared to CsChrimson control, beginning with the proboscis in the resting (retracted) position. Second part: silencing of mn6 with TNT, compared to TNT control, in a feeding assay. Labella marked in blue in certain frames to show difference in labellar angle. Tethered males, sagittal view (dorsal: up), 1/30 speed.

Video 20. Behavioral roles of labellar abductor.

Download video file (892.1KB, mp4)

First part: activation of labellar abductor (mn7) with CsChrimson, in frames noted, compared to CsChrimson control, beginning from the resting position (proboscis retracted, labella closed). Labella marked in blue in certain frames. Second part: silencing of mn7 with TNT, compared to TNT control, in a feeding assay where normal flies open the labella towards a droplet of sucrose. Tethered males, anterior view (dorsal up), 1/30 speed.

To summarize the behavioral results, all eight of the muscles predicted to be involved in proboscis positioning (based on the location of their insertion sites) indeed showed positioning phenotypes; six control reaching movements and two control the labella. Most of these findings (Table 3) were not previously known or, in some cases, differed from previous work (Schwarz et al., 2017).

Table 3. Summary of motor neuron phenotypes from activation and silencing experiments (also informed by insertion site information). Asterisks: findings that were previously undescribed, or that differ from a previous study (Schwarz et al., 2017).

mn Activation phenotypes Silencing phenotypes
 1 proboscis retraction *impaired proboscis retraction
 2D *rostrum retraction from extended no phenotype (expected redundancy)
 2V *rostrum retraction from rest *impaired rostrum retraction
 3 *haustellum flexion *impaired proboscis retraction
 4 *haustellum extension *impaired haustellum extension
 6 labellar extension impaired labellar extension
 7 *labellar abduction *impaired labellar abduction
 9 rostrum protract. and *haust. exten. impaired rostrum protract. and *haust. exten.

Discussion

The present work describes the anatomy and function of motor neurons controlling each muscle of the fly proboscis. Building on previous studies in insects that have analyzed motor neurons in body parts such as the legs and proboscis (Baek and Mann, 2009; Brierley et al., 2012; Maniates-Selvin et al., 2020; Rajashekhar and Singh, 1994; Schwarz et al., 2017; Trimarchi and Schneiderman, 1994), we now provide the first comprehensive, genetically reproducible control of every primary motor neuron type for an appendage. We characterize the motor neurons’ outputs to musculature in the periphery and their dendrites in the brain.

Detailed imaging of muscle insertion sites is used to predict that eight muscles participate in proboscis positioning, confirmed in behavioral experiments. With these tools, we produce a model of the function of the musculature and control of each proboscis joint, where a small number of motor neurons can produce a goal-directed reaching behavior.

We consider the neurons of this collection to be primary excitatory motor neurons, as they do not resemble known modulatory motor neurons in adult flies, which are characterized by thin axons and small boutons on multiple muscles (Rivlin et al., 2004). Likewise, they do not resemble inhibitory motor neurons found in several arthropods, which mostly innervate multiple muscles (Wolf, 2014). Anatomical and behavioral evidence supports the identity of these neurons as primary motor neurons.

The split GAL4 combinations used here to target motor neurons were nicely sparse but not perfect, often containing some neurons in other brain regions. However, they are quite specific in the SEZ, where we think much of the motor circuitry of the proboscis will be found. Triple intersections with SEZ-specific reagents based on Hox expression (Simpson, 2016) could be used for further restricting expression. Future work must also examine the cooperative action of multiple motor neurons, rather than just their effects in isolation.

Computational brain alignment suggests that motor neuron dendrites are not found in close proximity to several known cell types involved in feeding. Unknown circuit layers between sensory and motor neurons likely aid with functions such as pattern generation or command-like roles. Although certain mechanosensory neurons have been investigated in the proboscis (Zhou et al., 2019), the role of sensory feedback in proboscis reaching remains to be explored; the approach used here could also be applied to the isolation of proprioceptive neurons.

With this motor neuron collection, we provide a description of the muscles that act at each proboscis joint. It extends previous efforts (Schwarz et al., 2017) by using a stronger neuronal silencer to find silencing phenotypes for more cell types (Table 3), tissue clearing techniques that show more neurons than were thought to be present in GAL4 lines (Table 2), and split GAL4 lines for more specificity. While the kinematics of proboscis reaching remains to be explored, the comprehensive genetic tools we describe provide the means to do so.

We discover unexpected complexity of phenotypes from rostrum motor neurons. Both mn9 and mn1 are located near the rostrum, not in a position to control the haustellum directly, yet showed phenotypic effects on the haustellum as well. These may represent examples of biomechanical coupling. We were struck by how common ‘side effects’ like this were – another example is that, although mn7 is responsible for labellar abduction (with both activation and silencing phenotypes), its activation also triggers labellar extension. We speculate that optogenetically forcing the movement of one body part may cause a fly to move another in response, even if the activated motor neuron is not directly involved in the latter at all, or that forced movement about a joint may cause afferent feedback that leads to activation of other motor neurons. Intrinsic biomechanical properties, such as passive joint forces, are also known to provide non-neural (and rapid) aid during motor responses to mechanical perturbations, for example in cockroach, locust and stick insect legs during locomotion (Ache and Matheson, 2013; Jindrich and Full, 2002).

These possible side effects of optogenetic activation highlight the utility of combining activation and silencing data to deduce muscle roles, since both methods can have pitfalls (Yoshihara and Yoshihara, 2018). By incorporating activation, silencing, and insertion site data, we derive interpretations of muscle roles (Table 3) that differ in several respects from those of a previous work based mainly on activation (Schwarz et al., 2017).

It will be of interest to determine how flexibility is achieved in the recruitment of proboscis muscles. Several muscles were innervated by only a single primary motor neuron, but in the case where two were present, it is possible that they control slow twitch and fast twitch fibers, as found in both the fly (Azevedo et al., 2019) and in mammalian muscle, to fine-tune the strength of a muscle response. Regardless, the dramatic numerical simplicity of only one or two primary motor neurons per muscle in flies presents an experimental advantage for future circuit studies. Given that the proboscis has a varied repertoire of movements in feeding and courtship (Manzo et al., 2012; McKellar et al., 2019), one exciting avenue would be to decipher the circuit logic of how different movements with multiple behavioral functions can arise from these few motor neurons. Activation and inactivation experiments of individual proboscis motor neuron types support a model of separable control, enabling articulated movements that allow flexibility.

Directed reaching occurs in different forms: humans show sophisticated, learned arm and hand movements (Parker, 1974), yet newborns show a more basic, innate form of intentional reaching (Bower et al., 1970). In primates, work on the neural coding of goal-directed behavior has often focused on how its features might be encoded by firing properties of single neurons or single brain areas, particularly within the cerebral cortex (Kalaska, 2019). Forms of reaching exist in smaller animals such as rodents and cephalopods (Gutnick et al., 2011; Peterson, 1934). We propose that proboscis reaching in flies could present a new model for understanding the neural circuit basis of goal-directed movement. Drosophila has rich behavior, a smaller nervous system, and powerful genetic tools. Additionally, a whole-brain synaptic connectome is underway (see flywire.ai, based on the images of Zheng et al., 2017) that can help map how diverse interneurons may converge upon this small set of motor neurons to produce a repertoire of flexible motor behaviors from a single appendage.

Materials and methods

Key resources table.

Reagent type
(species) or
resource
Designation Source or
reference
Identifiers Additional
information
Antibody mouse mAb anti-bruchpilot (nc82) Developmental Studies Hybridoma Bank Cat#nc82; RRID:AB_2314865 (1:50)
Antibody rabbit anti-GFP Thermo Fisher Scientific Cat#A11122; RRID:AB_221569 (1:500)
Antibody rabbit anti-GFP Thermo Fisher Scientific Cat#A10262; RRID:AB_2534023 (1:1000)
Antibody rat mAb anti-FLAG Novus Biologicals Cat# NBP1-06712; RRID:AB_1625981 (1:200)
Antibody rabbit anti-HA Cell Signaling Technology Cat# 3724S; RRID:AB_1549585 (1:300)
Antibody mouse anti-V5 AbD Serotec Cat# MCA1360; RRID:AB_322378 (1:300)
Antibody goat anti-rabbit AlexaFluor-488 Thermo Fisher Scientific Cat#A11034; RRID:AB_2576217 (1:500)
Antibody goat anti-chicken AlexaFluor-488 Thermo Fisher Scientific Cat#A11039; RRID:AB_142924 (1:500)
Antibody goat anti-mouse AlexaFluor-488 Thermo Fisher Scientific Cat #A11001; RRID:AB_2534069 (1:500)
Antibody goat anti-mouse AlexaFluor-568 Thermo Fisher Scientific Cat#A11031; RRID:AB_144696 (1:500)
Antibody goat anti-rabbit Alexa Fluor-488 Thermo Fisher Scientific Cat#A32731; RRID:AB_2633280 (1:1000)
Antibody goat anti-mouse Cy3 Jackson Immunoresearch Cat#115-166-003; RRID:AB_2338699 (1:250)
Antibody goat anti-rat AlexaFluor-568 Thermo Fisher Scientific Cat #A11077; RRID:AB_2534121 (1:500)
Antibody goat anti-rat AlexaFluor-633 Thermo Fisher Scientific Cat#A21094; RRID:AB_141553 (1:500)
Chemical compound, drug Texas Red-X Phalloidin Life Technologies Cat#T7471
Chemical compound, drug Calcofluor White Sigma-Aldrich Cat#F3543
Chemical compound, drug Congo Red Sigma-Aldrich Cat#C676
Chemical compound, drug Alexa Fluor 633 Phalloidin Life Technologies Cat#A22284
Chemical compound, drug All-trans retinal Toronto Research Chemical Cat# R240000
Genetic reagent Drosophila melanogaster Canton S Bloomington Stock Center RRID:BDSC_64349
Genetic reagent (D. melanogaster) Rubin and VT GAL4 lines listed in Table 2 Jenett et al., 2012; Pfeiffer et al., 2010; Tirian and Dickson, 2017 N/A
Genetic reagent (D. melanogaster) Split GAL4 lines targeting proboscis muscles, listed in Table 1 This paper N/A Split GAL halves from G. Rubin and B. Dickson (Jenett et al., 2012; Pfeiffer et al., 2010; Tirian and Dickson, 2017)
Genetic reagent (D. melanogaster) 10XUAS-IVS-mCD8::GFP in su(Hw)attP5 (pJFRC2) Gerald Rubin (Pfeiffer et al., 2010) N/A
Genetic reagent (D. melanogaster) 20XUAS-CsChrimson-mCherry in su(Hw)attP5 Insertion from Vivek Jayaraman, construct from Klapoetke et al., 2014 N/A
Genetic reagent (D. melanogaster) UAS-TeTxLC.TNT Sweeney et al., 1995 N/A
Genetic reagent (D. melanogaster) pBPhsFlp2::PEST in attP3;; pJFRC201-10XUAS-FRT > STOP > FRT-myr::smGFP-HA in VK00005 Nern et al., 2015 N/A
Genetic reagent (D. melanogaster) pJFRC240-10XUAS-FRT > STOP > FRT-myr::smGFP-V5-THS-10XUAS-FRT > STOP > FRT-myr::smGFP-FLAG in su(Hw)attP1 Nern et al., 2015 N/A
Genetic reagent '
(D. melanogaster)
VT017251-LexA Hampel et al., 2017 N/A
Genetic reagent (D. melanogaster) HUGS3-GAL4 Melcher and Pankratz, 2005 N/A
Genetic reagent (D. melanogaster) Gr64f-GAL4 [737-5];Gr64f-GAL4 [737-1] Dahanukar et al., 2007 N/A
Genetic reagent (D. melanogaster) Gr66a-GAL4 (II) Dunipace et al., 2001 N/A
Genetic reagent (D. melanogaster) NP883-GAL4 Flood et al., 2013 N/A
Genetic reagent (D. melanogaster) NP5137-GAL4 Flood et al., 2013 N/A
Software, algorithm Adobe Photoshop Adobe Systems (www.adobe.com/products/photoshop.html) RRID:SCR_014199
Software, algorithm Adobe Illustrator Adobe Systems (https://www.adobe.com/products/illustrator.html) RRID:SCR_010279
Software, algorithm Computational Morphometry Toolkit Jefferis et al., 2007 RRID:SCR_002234
Software, algorithm LabView National Instruments (www.labview.com) RRID:SCR_014325
Software, algorithm ImageJ https://imagej.nih.gov/ij/ RRID:SCR_003070
Software, algorithm Icy http://icy.bioimageanalysis.org/ RRID:SCR_010587
Software, algorithm gVision Gus Lott (http://gvision-hhmi.sourceforge.net/) N/A
Software, algorithm G*Power http://www.gpower.hhu.de/ RRID:SCR_013726
Software, algorithm VVDViewer https://github.com/takashi310/VVD_Viewer N/A

Terminology

The area formerly known as the sub(o)esophageal ganglion (SOG/SEG) is now called the gnathal ganglion (Ito et al., 2014). The gnathal ganglion, however, does not encompass the whole area ventral to the esophageal foramen, the subesophageal zone (SEZ). We use the term SEZ because the motor neurons described here are not confined to the gnathal ganglion, but are all within the larger area of the SEZ.

The axis system used is that of the body axis (Ito et al., 2014). During feeding the proboscis mostly extends ventrally toward the surface the fly is standing on, so the head is dorsal, the tip of the proboscis ventral, with the maxillary palps on the anterior surface.

Fly stocks and rearing

The GAL4-UAS system was used to direct gene expression to defined populations of cells (Brand and Perrimon, 1993). The split GAL4 system (Luan et al., 2006; Pfeiffer et al., 2010) was used to express the activation domain (AD) and the DNA-binding domain (DBD) of GAL4 under the separate control of two genomic enhancers, to obtain the intersection of their expression patterns, with the goal of obtaining a sparser pattern. GAL4, split GAL4 and LexA stocks were gifts of Gerald Rubin and Barry Dickson (Jenett et al., 2012; Pfeiffer et al., 2010; Tirian and Dickson, 2017). GAL4s and DBDs were inserted into the landing site attP2; ADs and LexAs were inserted into attP40. Other lines used were: wildtype: Canton S ‘A’ (a gift of Jeff Hall), GFP: pJFRC2-10x-UAS-IVS-mCD8::GFP in su(Hw)attP5 (Pfeiffer et al., 2010), CsChrimson: 20XUAS-CsChrimson-mCherry (Klapoetke et al., 2014) in su(Hw)attP5, TNT: UAS-TeTxLC.TNT (Sweeney et al., 1995), eye mechanosensory neurons: VT017251-LexA (Hampel et al., 2017), Hugin: HUGS3-GAL4 (Melcher and Pankratz, 2005), sweet taste neurons: Gr64f-GAL4 [737-5];Gr64f-GAL4 [737-1] (Dahanukar et al., 2007), bitter taste neurons: Gr66a-GAL4 (II) (Dunipace et al., 2001), Fdg neurons: NP883-GAL4 and NP5137-GAL4 (Flood et al., 2013), multicolor flipout stochastic labelling: pBPhsFlp2::PEST in attP3;; pJFRC201-10XUAS-FRT > STOP > FRT-myr::smGFP-HA in VK00005, pJFRC240-10XUAS-FRT > STOP > FRT-myr::smGFP-V5-THS-10XUAS-FRT > STOP > FRT-myr::smGFP-FLAG in su(Hw)attP1 (Nern et al., 2015). All transgenes were in a w1118 background. Behavior experiments used flies with a wildtype X chromosome to permit normal vision.

Experiments were carried out on 3–7 day old heterozygote males. Controls omitted either the GAL4 driver or the UAS effector; for example, for TNT experiments on mn9, the two controls were the mn9 split GAL4 combination with no UAS-TeTxLC.TNT (‘split control’) and UAS-TeTxLC.TNT with no split GAL4 (‘TNT control’). Flies were raised at 25°C and 50% relative humidity on standard cornmeal and molasses food in a 12 hr light/dark cycle, except for flies for CsChrimson experiments, which were dark-reared on food containing 0.2 mM all-trans retinal (Toronto Research Chemical, #R240000). Behavioral experiments were performed at 25°C, 40% humidity. When needed before experiments, anesthesia was performed on ice.

Synchrotron

Flies were tethered under cold anesthesia and filmed at 3000 fps with an x-ray beam in the Advanced Photon Source synchrotron at the Argonne National Laboratory. Proboscis extension was filmed during rare instances when it occurred spontaneously.

CNS immunohistochemistry and imaging

Brains and ventral nervous systems were dissected and stained using published methods (Nern et al., 2015). Antibodies: rabbit anti-GFP (1:500, Invitrogen, #A11122), mouse anti-Bruchpilot (1:50, Developmental Studies Hybridoma Bank, University of Iowa, mAb nc82), Alexa Fluor 488-goat anti-rabbit (1:500, ThermoFisher A11034), Alexa Fluor 568-goat anti-mouse (1:500, ThermoFisher A11031). Serial optical sections were obtained at 1 µm intervals on a Zeiss 700 confocal with a Plan-Apochromat 20x/0.8NA objective.

Preparation of whole mount heads for muscle and cuticle imaging

Flies were anesthetized with CO2 and briefly washed with 70% ethanol. Antennae, maxillary palps, eye cups (including retinas) and a small part of labellum were removed from isolated heads under 2% paraformaldehyde/PBS/0.1% triton X-100. Heads were fixed in this solution overnight at 4°C, then incubated in PBS with 1% triton X-100, 0.5% DMSO and Escin (0.05 mg/ml, Sigma-Aldrich, E1378) containing Texas Red-X Phalloidin (1:50, Life Technologies #T7471) and a chitin-binding dye Calcofluor White (0.1 mg/ml, Sigma-Aldrich #F3543-1G) at room temperature with agitation for up to 5 days. Long incubation times and the presence of surfactants assured better penetration of phalloidin into the muscles. The samples were then washed in PBS/1% triton (4 × 1 hr) and fixed for 4 hr in 2% paraformaldehyde to reduce leaching of bound phalloidin from muscles during the subsequent ethanol dehydration step. To avoid artefacts caused by osmotic shrinkage of soft tissue, samples were gradually dehydrated in glycerol (2–80%) and then ethanol (20% to 100%) (Ott, 2008) and mounted in methyl salicylate (Sigma-Aldrich #M6752) for imaging.

Preparation of head sections for muscle and cuticle imaging

Heads were prepared as described above, washed, embedded in 7% agarose (SeaKem, Lonza #50002) in PBS and sectioned sagittally on a vibratome (Leica VT1000s) at 0.3 mm. The slices were incubated with Texas Red-X Phalloidin and Calcofluor (solution composition as above) for 2 days at room temperature, washed and mounted in Tris-buffered (50 mM, pH 8.4) 50% glycerol. To image cuticle alone, soft tissues were digested away after imaging, with 0.25 mg/ml collagenase/dispase (Roche #10269638001) and 0.25 mg/ml hyaluronidase (Sigma Aldrich #H3884) in PBS for 5 hr at 37°C. After washing, exoskeleton was stained with Congo Red (0.5 mg/mL, Sigma-Aldrich #C676), washed and mounted in buffered glycerol. Serial optical sections were obtained at 1 µm intervals on a Zeiss 500 or 710 confocal microscopes with a LD-LCI 25x/0.8 NA objective. Congo Red was imaged using a 594 nm laser.

Whole mount head staining for synapse, muscle and GFP imaging

Type I, II and III boutons all express the presynaptic protein Bruchpilot (Wagh et al., 2006), so it was chosen here as a pan-synaptic marker for neuromuscular junctions. Heads were prepared as above, except fixation time was reduced to 2 hr due to the sensitivity of Bruchpilot epitopes to prolonged fixation. The heads were incubated in PBS with aforementioned surfactants and 3% goat serum (Life Technologies #776318), Alexa Fluor 633 Phalloidin (1:50, Life Technologies # A22284), rabbit anti-GFP polyclonal (1:1000, Thermo Fisher, #A10262) and mouse anti-Bruchpilot mouse monoclonal antibodies (1:50 [concentrate]; DSHB/Developmental Studies Hybridoma Bank, mAb nc82) at room temperature with agitation for 5 days. After a series of four ~ 1 hr washes in PBS containing surfactants, the sections were incubated for another 24 hr in the above buffer containing secondary antibodies (1:1000, Alexa Fluor 488 goat anti-rabbit, Thermo Fisher #A32731; 1:250 Cy3 goat anti-mouse, Jackson Immunoresearch #115-166-003). Samples were washed, post-fixed, dehydrated and cleared for imaging as described above. Serial optical sections were obtained at 1 µm intervals on a Zeiss 500 or 710 confocal microscopes with a LD-LCI 25x/0.8 NA objective. Calcofluor White, anti-GFP/anti-rabbit Alexa 488 antibodies, anti-nc82/anti-mouse Cy3 antibodies, Congo Red, Texas Red and Alexa 633 phalloidin-treated samples were imaged using 405, 488, 561, 594 and 633 nm lasers, respectively.

Stochastic labeling

For multicolor flipout stochastic labeling, flies received a 15 min heat shock at 37°C at 1–3 d old, and were dissected at 6–8 d. Dissections were performed to carefully remove the brain from the head but preserve both for staining, so that an individually labeled neuron in the brain could be correlated with its projections to the muscle. Staining was performed as described (Nern et al., 2015), with the following antibodies: rat anti-flag (Novus Biologicals, LLC, Littleton, CO, #NBP1-06712), rabbit anti-HA (Cell Signaling Technology, Danvers, MA, #3724S), mouse anti-V5 (AbD Serotec, Kidlington, England, #MCA1360), AlexaFluor-488 (1:500; goat anti-rabbit, goat anti-chicken, goat anti-mouse; Thermo Fisher Scientific), AlexaFluor-568 (1:500; goat anti-mouse, goat anti-rat; Thermo Fisher Scientific), AlexaFluor-633 (1:500; goat anti-rat; Thermo Fisher Scientific).

Image processing

Images were adjusted for gain and contrast without obscuring data. Images were processed in ImageJ (https://imagej.nih.gov/ij/), Icy (http://icy.bioimageanalysis.org/) and Photoshop (Adobe Systems Inc). Where noted, motor neurons and muscles were rendered and segmented from confocal stacks with VVDviewer software (https://github.com/takashi310/VVD_Viewer), to visualize them in isolation. For this rendering and for computational alignment of brain images used where noted, brain images were registered using the Computational Morphometry Toolkit (http://nitrc.org/projects/cmtk) (Jefferis et al., 2007), to a standard brain template (‘JFRC2014’) mounted and imaged with the same conditions, which had been corrected by a z scaling factor of 1.568 to match the true proportions of a fly brain (obtained from frontal vs. horizontal stacks of the brain [Ito et al., 2014]).

Occupancy experiments

Head staining (described above) was used to determine whether the collection included every primary motor neuron of the proboscis. To be sure that every axon terminal was accounted for, we stained each motor neuron line one by one, and inspected every bouton in the 3D stack. All the boutons on the relevant muscle were indeed completely occupied by the labeled motor neurons. The lack of unoccupied boutons suggests that modulatory motor neurons’ boutons in the proboscis may not be labeled by the synaptic marker used here, or may not be visible as separate punctae since modulatory boutons can lie directly on top of primary motor neuron terminals (Rivlin et al., 2004). Incomplete occupancy of the boutons of muscle 3 was to be expected, since no line could be found to cover all its motor neurons, so separate lines were used that targeted muscle 3’s medial and lateral fibers. For muscle 10, a bilateral motor neuron pair was never observed in 6 stained heads, only a single unilateral neuron that could be found in either hemisphere. Since it did not occupy all boutons on muscle 10, we infer that it is not an unpaired neuron, but stochastic expression in a pair of motor neurons.

Cameras and lighting

Behavior was filmed with Basler A622f cameras at 30 Hz, controlled by gVision software developed at Janelia (Gus Lott). Ambient lighting was provided by white light, or by two infrared security spotlights (Phenas) for CsChrimson experiments. For CsChrimson activation, a 656 nm LED spotlight (Mightex PLS-0656–101 C) was controlled through a NIDAQ board and Labview software (National Instruments, Austin, TX). An 880 nm infrared LED (Fairchild Semiconductor Corporation #QEE123) was connected to a fiber optic in the field of view, to indicate when the stimulus was on, since a long-pass 830 nm filter in front of the camera prevented the stimulus light from obscuring the behavior video. Each stimulus was 2 s of constant light at 100 µW/mm2.

Freely moving behavior

To compare proboscis extension during feeding vs. courtship, flies were placed in oval chambers, 3 mm high, 9 mm wide and 12 mm long, to provide a flat segment along the wall in which to film proboscis extension from the side. For feeding, wildtype males were starved with access to water for 24 hr, then loaded into chambers with 10% sucrose/2% agarose painted onto a segment of the wall. Proboscis angles were scored in the frame of greatest proboscis extension during one proboscis extension event (event = time from extension to retraction), in events where the fly was orthogonal to the camera, contacting the sucrose on the wall. Courtship was filmed in chambers of the same dimensions but with a virgin female present. Proboscis extension was likewise scored in the frame of maximum extension, in courtship licking events occurring when the male was orthogonal to the camera and directly behind the female on the flat segment of wall.

Tethered fly behavior

Flies were anesthetized using cold, and mounted by the thorax to a pin with UV-cured glue (LED-200 UV gun, Electro-Lite Corp.). The same glue was used in experiments in which proboscis segments were glued. Mounted flies were allowed to recover for a few minutes before CsChrimson stimulation experiments, or alternatively were deprived of food and water for 5 hr before sucrose presentation experiments.

For sucrose presentation, a small 100 mM sucrose droplet was presented to the legs from a syringe on a manipulator, holding the target at a distance that prevents proboscis contact. The proboscis is extended repeatedly in search of the sucrose, moving through a range of angles (Video 7). The legs are not in contact with the ground and wave almost continuously, therefore rapidly encounter the sucrose. The proboscis reaching is guided not by vision but by leg contact, since the assay is performed under infrared illumination not visible to the fly. Visually-guided proboscis reaching was never observed in these conditions; the proboscis never extended before the legs tasted the sucrose.

Proboscis, head and target angle measurements

Joint angles for the head, rostrum and haustellum were measured from individual video frames in ImageJ by marking the points shown in Figure 2C–D. The rostrum angle is closer to 180° than 0° at rest, so is expressed as subtracted from 180° so that values are smaller at rest than during proboscis extension. When the haustellum is drawn into the head, its angle cannot be directly measured, but can be extrapolated by trigonometry, using the law of cosines with the distance from the rostrum joint to the end of the haustellum, and assuming that the rostrum and haustellum lengths are fixed proportions of the measured size of the head.

Measures of joint angles were chosen to best suit each assay. Due to the challenge of gluing flies to pins manually, the camera view varied, leading to variability in the measured raw angles of the joints. To overcome this variability, within-fly normalization was performed wherever possible, subtracting food- or CsChrimson-stimulated extension angle from the angle immediately before stimulation. Note that normalization was not possible for resting state angles, since there is no change from baseline. Analysis of raw joint angles (Figure 7—figure supplement 2) shows similar relations between motor neurons and joint angles as the normalized data but with greater inter-fly variability, as expected from the range of starting positions, so the main figures use normalized values as follows. Wildtype joint angles (Figure 2M–O) were measured in the frame 200 ms after the beginning of proboscis movement elicited by sucrose. For CsChrimson stimulation from rest, angles were measured in the frame of maximal proboscis movement during the stimulus and subtracted from the frame before the stimulus to report change in angle. For CsChrimson stimulation during ongoing feeding, angles were measured in the frame 200 ms after the stimulus start and subtracted from the frame before the stimulus. For TNT silencing, angles were measured at rest (where noted in figures), or during feeding (as the difference in angle from the start of proboscis extension to 200 ms later). During CsChrimson activation from rest, proboscis change is total extension of proboscis (farthest point on back of head to tip of labellum) at maximum movement divided by at rest (frame before stimulus).

During TNT silencing, proboscis position provides a measure of how retracted the proboscis is at rest, and is measured as the distance from back of head to tip of labellum divided by this value averaged across controls, after first normalizing the size of the fly (using measured height of head). Labellar extension and abduction were quantified as binary values, occurring or not occurring.

The angle of the sucrose target was measured from a line along the posterior-anterior axis of the fly (drawn from a point on the thorax marked by the humeral bristles, to the center of the eye), in the first frame of leg contact with the sucrose. A target at 90°=directly ventral to the eye.

Mean reach angle was measured by creating a stack in ImageJ (https://imagej.nih.gov/ij/) of the trial video frames, and generating an average intensity projection, creating a cloud of pixels where the proboscis had moved. In the region where the proboscis tip had moved, the pixel of maximum brightness was taken as the mean location of the tip over the trial. Mean reach angle was calculated from this proboscis tip location, with respect to a line along the posterior-anterior axis of the fly (drawn from the humeral bristles to the center of the eye).

To determine the accuracy of reaching, aim deviation was calculated as mean reach angle minus target angle, so that a deviation above 0° would represent reaching above the target in the dorsoventral plane, and aim below 0° would be below. Physical constraints may affect aim; flies appear to reach too high to low targets, and too low to high targets (Figure 2L). For proboscis reaching in wildtype flies (Figure 2), occasional flies were excluded if they did not appear hungry after the deprivation period (no proboscis extension to sucrose).

Speed, velocity, length and distance measurements

Points were manually drawn on video frames in ImageJ to acquire coordinates, which were converted to lengths by basic trigonometry. Frame 1 was taken to be the frame of the first rapid acceleration of the proboscis after sucrose contact. Target location was taken to be site of leg contact with sucrose in most recent frame of contact before proboscis extension. Reach speed = distance proboscis tip traveled per ms (involving both proboscis and/or head motion, in any direction). Proboscis velocity = tip displacement towards target minus head displacement towards target, per ms. Proboscis length = distance from rostrum joint to tip, subtracting mean length at start to set starting length to zero. Proboscis distance from target = distance from tip to target.

Statistical information

Experiments were blinded for scoring by assigning random identifications and only unblinding after all scoring. T-tests were two-tailed. Kolmogorov–Smirnov tests were first used to confirm that the data were consistent with a normal distribution. Error bars are standard error of the mean (SEM). In jitter plots, horizontal bars represent the mean. Outliers were not excluded. Sample size and other statistical information is provided in each relevant figure legend or Methods section. Asterisks: *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001, N.S.: not significant. All replicates are biological, testing different flies, not retesting the same individuals as technical replicates. Benjamini-Hochberg multiple testing correction used a false discovery rate of 0.1, and was applied across every genotype tested (across all motor neuron testing figures). In comparing mean reach angle to high and low targets, an a priori power analysis was performed in G*Power software using an effect size of 0.7 (estimated from a pilot study), an alpha error probability of 0.05, one tail, and a power of 0.8, giving a required sample size of 28 per group.

Acknowledgements

For fly strains, we thank Barret Pfeiffer, Gerry Rubin, and Vivek Jayaraman. For technical support, we thank Phuong Chung and the Janelia Fly Core, Instrument Design & Fabrication, Project Technical Resources, and Scientific Computing groups (particularly Hideo Otsuna). For synchrotron movies, we thank John Tuthill and Nikolay Kladt. Use of the Advanced Photon Source was supported by the US Department of Energy, Office of Science, Office of Basic Energy Sciences, under Contract No. DE-AC02-06CH11357, with technical support from Wah-Keat Lee. This work was made possible in part by software funded by the NIH: Fluorender: An Imaging Tool for Visualization and Analysis of Confocal Data as Applied to Zebrafish Research, R01-GM098151-01.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Claire E McKellar, Email: claireem@princeton.edu.

Julie H Simpson, Email: jhsimpson@ucsb.edu.

Ronald L Calabrese, Emory University, United States.

Ronald L Calabrese, Emory University, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grant:

  • Howard Hughes Medical Institute Janelia Research Campus to Claire E McKellar, Igor Siwanowicz, Barry Dickson, Julie H Simpson.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Validation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing - original draft, Project administration.

Investigation, Methodology.

Resources, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Writing - review and editing.

Conceptualization, Resources, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Writing - review and editing.

Additional files

Transparent reporting form

Data availability

All data generated or analysed during this study are included in the manuscript and supporting files.

References

  1. Ache JM, Matheson T. Passive joint forces are tuned to limb use in insects and drive movements without motor activity. Current Biology. 2013;23:1418–1426. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.06.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Azevedo AW, Gurung P, Venkatasubramanian L, Mann R, Tuthill JC. A size principle for leg motor control in Drosophila. bioRxiv. 2019 doi: 10.1101/730218. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  3. Baek M, Mann RS. Lineage and birth date specify motor neuron targeting and dendritic architecture in adult Drosophila. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:6904–6916. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1585-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Baker JD, Truman JW. Mutations in the Drosophila glycoprotein hormone receptor, rickets, eliminate neuropeptide-induced tanning and selectively block a stereotyped behavioral program. The Journal of Experimental Biology. 2002;205:2555–2565. doi: 10.1242/jeb.205.17.2555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bower TG, Broughton JM, Moore MK. Demonstration of intention in the reaching behaviour of neonate humans. Nature. 1970;228:679–681. doi: 10.1038/228679a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Brand AH, Perrimon N. Targeted gene expression as a means of altering cell fates and generating dominant phenotypes. Development. 1993;118:401–415. doi: 10.1242/dev.118.2.401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bridges CB, Dobzhansky T. The mutant 'proboscipedia' in Drosophila melanogaster - A case of hereditary homoösis. Wilhelm Roux' Archiv fur Entwicklungsmechanik der Organismen. 1933;127:575–590. doi: 10.1007/BF01380474. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Brierley DJ, Rathore K, VijayRaghavan K, Williams DW. Developmental origins and architecture of Drosophila leg motoneurons. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2012;520:1629–1649. doi: 10.1002/cne.23003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Budnik V, Ruiz-Canada C. The Fly Neuromuscular Junction: Structure and Function. Second Edition. . San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  10. Dahanukar A, Lei YT, Kwon JY, Carlson JR. Two gr genes underlie sugar reception in Drosophila. Neuron. 2007;56:503–516. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Dethier VG. The Hungry Fly: A Physiological Study of the Behavior Associated with Feeding. Cambridge: Harvard University Press; 1976. [Google Scholar]
  12. Dunipace L, Meister S, McNealy C, Amrein H. Spatially restricted expression of candidate taste receptors in the Drosophila gustatory system. Current Biology. 2001;11:822–835. doi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(01)00258-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Flood TF, Iguchi S, Gorczyca M, White B, Ito K, Yoshihara M. A single pair of interneurons commands the Drosophila feeding motor program. Nature. 2013;499:83–87. doi: 10.1038/nature12208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Gordon MD, Scott K. Motor control in a Drosophila taste circuit. Neuron. 2009;61:373–384. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.12.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Gutnick T, Byrne RA, Hochner B, Kuba M. Octopus vulgaris uses visual information to determine the location of its arm. Current Biology. 2011;21:460–462. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.01.052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Hampel S, McKellar CE, Simpson JH, Seeds AM. Simultaneous activation of parallel sensory pathways promotes a grooming sequence in Drosophila. eLife. 2017;6:e28804. doi: 10.7554/eLife.28804. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Hartenstein V, Omoto JJ, Ngo KT, Wong D, Kuert PA, Reichert H, Lovick JK, Younossi-Hartenstein A. Structure and development of the subesophageal zone of the Drosophila brain. I. segmental architecture, compartmentalization, and lineage anatomy. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2018;526:6–32. doi: 10.1002/cne.24287. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Ito K, Shinomiya K, Ito M, Armstrong JD, Boyan G, Hartenstein V, Harzsch S, Heisenberg M, Homberg U, Jenett A, Keshishian H, Restifo LL, Rössler W, Simpson JH, Strausfeld NJ, Strauss R, Vosshall LB, Insect Brain Name Working Group A systematic nomenclature for the insect brain. Neuron. 2014;81:755–765. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.12.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Jefferis GS, Potter CJ, Chan AM, Marin EC, Rohlfing T, Maurer CR, Luo L. Comprehensive maps of Drosophila higher olfactory centers: spatially segregated fruit and pheromone representation. Cell. 2007;128:1187–1203. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2007.01.040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Jenett A, Rubin GM, Ngo TT, Shepherd D, Murphy C, Dionne H, Pfeiffer BD, Cavallaro A, Hall D, Jeter J, Iyer N, Fetter D, Hausenfluck JH, Peng H, Trautman ET, Svirskas RR, Myers EW, Iwinski ZR, Aso Y, DePasquale GM, Enos A, Hulamm P, Lam SC, Li HH, Laverty TR, Long F, Qu L, Murphy SD, Rokicki K, Safford T, Shaw K, Simpson JH, Sowell A, Tae S, Yu Y, Zugates CT. A GAL4-driver line resource for Drosophila neurobiology. Cell Reports. 2012;2:991–1001. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2012.09.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Jindrich DL, Full RJ. Dynamic stabilization of rapid hexapedal locomotion. The Journal of Experimental Biology. 2002;205:2803–2823. doi: 10.1242/jeb.205.18.2803. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Kalaska JF. Emerging ideas and tools to study the emergent properties of the cortical neural circuits for voluntary motor control in non-human primates. F1000Research. 2019;8:749. doi: 10.12688/f1000research.17161.1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Klapoetke NC, Murata Y, Kim SS, Pulver SR, Birdsey-Benson A, Cho YK, Morimoto TK, Chuong AS, Carpenter EJ, Tian Z, Wang J, Xie Y, Yan Z, Zhang Y, Chow BY, Surek B, Melkonian M, Jayaraman V, Constantine-Paton M, Wong GK, Boyden ES. Independent optical excitation of distinct neural populations. Nature Methods. 2014;11:338–346. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2836. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Lang CE, Schieber MH. Human finger independence: limitations due to passive mechanical coupling versus active neuromuscular control. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2004;92:2802–2810. doi: 10.1152/jn.00480.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Lehmann FO, Heymann N. Unconventional mechanisms control cyclic respiratory gas release in flying Drosophila. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2005;208:3645–3654. doi: 10.1242/jeb.01788. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Luan H, Peabody NC, Vinson CR, White BH. Refined spatial manipulation of neuronal function by combinatorial restriction of transgene expression. Neuron. 2006;52:425–436. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.08.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Maniates-Selvin JT, Hildebrand DGC, Graham BJ, Kuan AT, Thomas LA, Nguyen T, Buhmann J, Azevedo AW, Shanny BL, Funke J, Tuthill JC, W-Ca L. Reconstruction of motor control circuits in adult Drosophila using automated transmission electron microscopy. bioRxiv. 2020 doi: 10.1101/2020.01.10.902478. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  28. Manzo A, Silies M, Gohl DM, Scott K. Motor neurons controlling fluid ingestion in Drosophila. PNAS. 2012;109:6307–6312. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1120305109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. McKellar CE, Lillvis JL, Bath DE, Fitzgerald JE, Cannon JG, Simpson JH, Dickson BJ. Threshold-Based ordering of sequential actions during Drosophila Courtship. Current Biology. 2019;29:426–434. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.12.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Melcher C, Pankratz MJ. Candidate gustatory interneurons modulating feeding behavior in the Drosophila brain. PLOS Biology. 2005;3:e305. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030305. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Miller A. The internal anatomy and histology of the imago of Drosophila melanogaster. In: Demerec M, editor. Biology of Drosophila. New York: Wiley; 1950. pp. 502–503. [Google Scholar]
  32. Nern A, Pfeiffer BD, Rubin GM. Optimized tools for multicolor stochastic labeling reveal diverse stereotyped cell arrangements in the fly visual system. PNAS. 2015;112:E2967–E2976. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1506763112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. O'Sullivan A, Lindsay T, Prudnikova A, Erdi B, Dickinson M, von Philipsborn AC. Multifunctional wing motor control of song and flight. Current Biology. 2018;28:2705–2717. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.06.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Ott SR. Confocal microscopy in large insect brains: zinc-formaldehyde fixation improves synapsin immunostaining and preservation of morphology in whole-mounts. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 2008;172:220–230. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2008.04.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Parker CE. The antecedents of man the manipulator. Journal of Human Evolution. 1974;3:493–500. doi: 10.1016/0047-2484(74)90009-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  36. Pauls D, Blechschmidt C, Frantzmann F, El Jundi B, Selcho M. A comprehensive anatomical map of the peripheral octopaminergic/tyraminergic system of Drosophila melanogaster. Scientific Reports. 2018;8:15314. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-33686-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Peterson GM. Mechanisms of Handedness in the Rat Comp. Psychol. Monogr. Johns Hopkins Press; 1934. [Google Scholar]
  38. Pfeiffer BD, Ngo TT, Hibbard KL, Murphy C, Jenett A, Truman JW, Rubin GM. Refinement of tools for targeted gene expression in Drosophila. Genetics. 2010;186:735–755. doi: 10.1534/genetics.110.119917. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Prokop A. Organization of the efferent system and structure of neuromuscular junctions in Drosophila. In: Ruiz-Canada C, editor. The Fly Neuromuscular Junction: Structure and Function. Second Edition. Academic Press; 2006. pp. 71–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Rajashekhar KP, Singh RN. Organization of motor neurons innervating the Proboscis musculature in Drosophila melanogaster meigen (diptera : Drosophilidae) International Journal of Insect Morphology and Embryology. 1994;23:225–242. doi: 10.1016/0020-7322(94)90020-5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  41. Rivlin PK, St Clair RM, Vilinsky I, Deitcher DL. Morphology and molecular organization of the adult neuromuscular junction of Drosophila. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2004;468:596–613. doi: 10.1002/cne.10977. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Schwarz O, Bohra AA, Liu X, Reichert H, VijayRaghavan K, Pielage J. Motor control of Drosophila feeding behavior. eLife. 2017;6:e19892. doi: 10.7554/eLife.19892. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Simpson JH. Rationally subdividing the fly nervous system with versatile expression reagents. Journal of Neurogenetics. 2016;30:185–194. doi: 10.1080/01677063.2016.1248761. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Sturtevant AH. Experiments on sex recognition and the problem of sexual selection in drosoophilia. Journal of Animal Behavior. 1915;5:351–366. doi: 10.1037/h0074109. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  45. Sweeney ST, Broadie K, Keane J, Niemann H, O'Kane CJ. Targeted expression of tetanus toxin light chain in Drosophila specifically eliminates synaptic transmission and causes behavioral defects. Neuron. 1995;14:341–351. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(95)90290-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Szebenyi AL. Cleaning behaviour in Drosophila melanogaster. Animal Behaviour. 1969;17:641–651. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(69)80006-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  47. Tirian L, Dickson B. The VT GAL4, LexA and split-GAL4 driver line collections for targeted expression in the Drosophila nervous system. bioRxiv. 2017 doi: 10.1101/198648. [DOI]
  48. Tissot M, Gendre N, Stocker RF. Drosophila P[Gal4] lines reveal that motor neurons involved in feeding persist through metamorphosis. Journal of Neurobiology. 1998;37:237–250. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-4695(19981105)37:2&#x0003c;237::AID-NEU4&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-F. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Trimarchi JR, Schneiderman AM. The motor neurons innervating the direct flight muscles of Drosophila melanogaster are morphologically specialized. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1994;340:427–443. doi: 10.1002/cne.903400311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Wagh DA, Rasse TM, Asan E, Hofbauer A, Schwenkert I, Dürrbeck H, Buchner S, Dabauvalle MC, Schmidt M, Qin G, Wichmann C, Kittel R, Sigrist SJ, Buchner E. Bruchpilot, a protein with homology to ELKS/CAST, is required for structural integrity and function of synaptic active zones in Drosophila. Neuron. 2006;49:833–844. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.02.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Wolf H. Inhibitory motoneurons in arthropod motor control: organisation, function, evolution. Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 2014;200:693–710. doi: 10.1007/s00359-014-0922-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Yoshihara M, Yoshihara M. ‘Necessary and sufficient’ in biology is not necessarily necessary – confusions and erroneous conclusions resulting from misapplied logic in the field of biology, especially neuroscience. Journal of Neurogenetics. 2018;32:53–64. doi: 10.1080/01677063.2018.1468443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Zheng Z, Lauritzen JS, Perlman E, Robinson CG, Nichols M, Milkie D, Torrens O, Price J, Fisher CB, Sharifi N, Calle-Schuler SA, Kmecova L, Ali IJ, Karsh B, Trautman ET, Bogovic J, Hanslovsky P, Jefferis G, Kazhdan M, Khairy K, Saalfeld S, Fetter RD, Bock DD. A complete electron microscopy volume of the brain of adult Drosophila melanogaster. bioRxiv. 2017 doi: 10.1101/140905. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  54. Zhou Y, Cao LH, Sui XW, Guo XQ, Luo DG. Mechanosensory circuits coordinate two opposing motor actions in Drosophila feeding. Science Advances. 2019;5:eaaw5141. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.aaw5141. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Ronald L Calabrese1
Reviewed by: Ronald L Calabrese2

In the interests of transparency, eLife publishes the most substantive revision requests and the accompanying author responses.

Thank you for submitting your article "Comprehensive genetic tools to target motor neurons for each muscle of the fly proboscis" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, including Ronald Calabrese as the Reviewing Editor, Senior Editor and Reviewer #2.

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

Summary:

This is a co-submission of two papers. The goal of the first work is achieving genetic control over motor neurons for every muscle of the Drosophila proboscis, and it is accomplished convincingly. The degree of success constructing split Gal4 lines that label only a single bilateral pair of motor neuron corresponding to each muscle of the proboscis is really remarkable, and there is no doubt that these tools will be of tremendous use in defining the neuronal networks responsible for reaching movements of the proboscis and indeed the sucking movements also. The anatomical data presented is beautiful. After achieving genetic control over motor neurons for every muscle of the Drosophila proboscis, the authors in the second work systematically test every motor neuron that inserts on cuticle (including apodemes) and is thus likely to move the proboscis (rather than inserting on the pharynx and likely involved in sucking) with activation and inactivation experiments. By combining kinematics and some biomechanics, the authors arrive at a functional role for each of the proboscis inserting muscle and their motor neurons. The data presented is comprehensive and convincing. When combined these two papers, very clearly supersede a previous attempt (Schwarz et al., 2017) at such tool development and analysis with several important corrections and many major refinements.

Essential revisions:

Upon careful review of these two co-submitted papers (henceforth referred to a Paper 1 and Paper 2) it was the clear consensus of the reviewers that neither paper alone is strong enough for eLife and we would understand if you chose to take the paper(s) elsewhere. If on the other hand, you chose to submit the two papers to eLife as one joint paper, then we would ensure that they were sent to the same reviewers, and we think that there is a high chance of publication with a short turn-around. No new experiments or analyses are needed.

If you take the eLife route the papers should be combined and focused. One paper will be easier for the authors to present and for the reader to comprehend. One could not read the Paper 2 without constantly referring to Paper 1 and the Paper 2 contains much repetition of the findings of Paper 1.

There are several ways that the papers can be fitted together as detailed in the individual expert reviews.

One potential path:

1) The theme presented for Paper 1 of rhythmic vs. discrete behaviors is not effectively exploited nor can it be given the data presented. Likewise, the comparison to human reaching in Paper 2 seems premature and focusing the combined single paper on the tools developed and the wonderful anatomy and first-pass functional analysis seems more fruitful. The data Figure 9 Paper 1 seems premature and underdeveloped for publication as does the data of Figure 8 Paper 2. They are presented is support of the rhythmic vs. discrete behaviors and human reaching themes and are not compelling. We recommend removing these figures to focus the paper.

2) Efficiency with the figures would help. Figure 1 of Paper 2 could benefit from comparison directly to Figure 1 of Paper 1. Figure 2 of Paper 1 should be supplementary and fits thematically with Paper 2 material. The expert reviews suggest other figure fusions and changes.

3) The data of Figure 7 Paper 1 is reminiscent of data in Schwarz et al., 2017, albeit with corrections but need not be elaborated and could be perhaps combined with Figure 8 Paper 1, which is somewhat underdeveloped.

4) Discussions can be shortened and in Paper 2 shifted away from detailed comparison to in Schwarz et al., 2017.

5) All Essential revisions in the expert reviews should all be addressed.

Reviewer #1:

In two related papers, McKellar and colleagues present the results of a screen to genetically label and optogenetically activate each motor neuron that innervates muscles in the proboscis of Drosophila melanogaster.

In the first manuscript, entitled "Comprehensive genetic tools to target motor neurons for each muscle of the fly proboscis", they present genetic driver lines for each motor neuron together with careful anatomy of cuticular structures and musculature. Ten proboscis muscles and their associated motor neurons control the position of proboscis parts and are involved in the proboscis extension response (PER), while the remaining eight muscles control the constriction of the pharynx. In the course of this work, the authors identify several new muscles.

Once the authors develop split-Gal4 genetic labels for each motor neuron, they can drive the expression of CsChrimson to activate, or Tetanus toxin to silence, each motor neuron. They present the results of these experiments in a second paper entitled "Motor neuron control of Drosophila proboscis reaching". The main advances of this work are the resulting genetic tools and a more comprehensive knowledge of how particular muscles insert onto cuticular structures to extend or flex proboscis parts about joints. These advances clarify the results of past investigations similar into motor control of the proboscis (in particular, Schwarz, Motor control of Drosophila feeding behavior).

Across both papers, the images of motor neuron and musculature anatomy are illustrative and quite beautiful. Optogenetic activation of individual motor neurons confirms the role each plays in controlling the proboscis segment. Together, these data allow the authors to articulate a model of how motor neuron activation and muscle contraction enables the fly to extend its proboscis.

Although the tools and data in these papers are detailed and valuable, the papers do not effectively show what new insight has been gained relative to previous studies. There is also a large experimental gap: while the authors create the necessary genetic tools, they do not investigate the neural control and natural dynamics of proboscis movement beyond artificial optogenetic stimulation. In the absence of such data, the main contribution of these papers is the clear and thorough anatomy. However, the convoluted organization of the two manuscripts require the reader to sift through disparate data to reach a clear understanding of how the proboscis motor system is organized. These manuscripts would need to be significantly reorganized and streamlined to be understood by a general neuroscience audience.

Essential revisions:

1) It is not clear to why these manuscripts are separate. The basis for separating them seems to be to avoid one behemoth paper but the division between tools and function obscures the take-home message of the work. For example, many of the descriptions of musculature in "Comprehensive genetic tools to target motor neurons for each muscle of the fly proboscis" (called paper 1 below) are critical for understanding the optogenetic activation of motor neurons in "Motor neuron control of Drosophila proboscis reaching" (called paper 2 below), resulting in an unnecessary duplication of effort between the manuscripts to understand how the proboscis is moved. Take mn9, for example: it is hard to know what to take from Figure 1, Figure 4 and Figure 9 in paper 1, if the critical proof to show that mn9 extends the proboscis is in paper 2. (Past work has shown m9 to be involved in proboscis protraction, so you could argue that a knowledgeable reader may know this, but that undercuts the purpose of Figure 2 in paper 2). There are also examples of data in primary figures that do not advance the author's case. For example, in paper 1, Figure 2, Figure 3, Figure 6 and Figure 8 are supplementary figures. Figure 2: The images of FlyLight Gen 1 Gal4 lines are unhelpful, fly researchers can find these and they are not helpful to a non-Drosophilist. Figure 3: I commend the authors for including images of the entire nervous system, not often shown, but again, these are supporting information. Figure 6 can support Figure 5. Figure 8 does suggest that MNs likely do not receive direct information from sensory neurons or other previously described interneurons, but these hypotheses are not tested further, so Figure 8 can support Figure 7.

By the same token, paper 2 would benefit from schematics of muscles and an overview of reaching related vs. pharyngeal motor neurons. This distinction is helpful, and since it is consistent with past work. Because the function of pharyngeal muscles is not explored further, it would not detract from the message of paper 2. paper 2 contains functional data that can be combined: e.g. protraction (Figure 2) and retraction (Figure 3) of the rostrum could be one figure, together with anatomical data from paper 1. Figure 2, Figure 3, Figure 4 and Figure 5 also contain many panels of different metrics, normalized or baseline subtracted. It is not necessary to show all these data in this form, and it detracts from the message. My suggestion would be to combine Figure 2 Figure 3, Figure 4 and Figure 5 haustellum and rostrum images. Figure 7 is beautiful! But it should be split up to accompany the data in Figure 2 and Figure 3 and Figure 4 and Figure 5.

In addition, much of the introductions of both manuscripts are either unnecessary or misleading (see below), so combining the data into a single manuscript will also help to eliminate those problems. In summary, I do not see a clear reason to split data across papers.

2) The optogenetic activation data that the authors present do not provide insight into how reaching kinematics are produced: 2 second stimulation of motor neurons is not a naturalistic manipulation, and resulting proboscis trajectories are collapsed into single numbers in Figure 2, Figure 3, Figure 4, Figure 5 and Figure 6 of paper 2. Schwarz et al., also concluded that reaching is the result of precise temporal patterns of activation of proboscis motor neurons. The authors have all the tools to explore this further with calcium imaging or electrophysiology, though it is understandably beyond the scope of this paper. I anticipate another paper that begins with the observations in Figure 8 of paper 2 and then uses the tools shown here to explore how these searching movements are created.

3) A related aspect of reaching is the treatment of what the authors call "coupling" of distinct parts of the proboscis, as when mn9 is activated and extends the haustellum in addition to the rostrum. Is this coupling mechanical, neural, or driven by proprioceptive feedback? The activation of additional motor neurons could be ruled out by silencing haustellum extending mns while activating mn9. The authors do not discuss sensory feedback, or the role of proprioception in determining the location of the target. This would be an interesting direction to pursue to draw closer connections to reaching in other organisms.

4) The authors state at various times that these genetic tools could help to make neural control of the proboscis a model for (1) rhythmic vs. discrete movements, and (2) a model of reaching in humans. These statements appear in the abstract and introductions of both papers, with little scholarship or links to literature that may prime the reader as to functional features that are similar in both models. The data to support these assertions are that proboscis speed is a bell shaped curve as a function of time and that proboscis reaching pauses to enter a second phase of slow approach. These observations seem rather trivial; what is the alternative? As the authors point out, proboscis reaching is already thought to be more complex than simply a reflex.

Reviewer #2:

Achieving genetic control over motor neurons for every muscle of the Drosophila proboscis is the laudable goal of this work, and it is accomplished convincingly. Surely there are nits to pick over the failure the achieve split Gal4 lines that label only a single bilateral pair of motor neuron corresponding to each muscle of the proboscis, BUT the degree of success is really remarkable, and there is no doubt that these tools will be of tremendous use in defining the neuronal networks responsible for reaching movements of the proboscis and indeed the sucking movements also. The anatomical data presented is beautiful. When combined with the companion paper (Motor neuron control of Drosophila proboscis reaching) it is very clear that this work supersedes a previous attempt (Schwarz et al., 2017) at such tool development and analysis with several important corrections and many major refinements

Essential revisions:

Upon carefully reviewing this paper (henceforth referred to a Paper 1) and its companion by the exact same authors (Motor neuron control of Drosophila proboscis reaching – henceforth referred to as Paper 2), I do not recommend that they be published independently. One paper will be easier for the authors to present and for the reader to comprehend. I could not read the Paper 2 without constantly referring to Paper 1 and the Paper 2 contains much repetition of the findings of Paper 1. The theme presented for Paper 1 of rhythmic vs. discrete behaviors is not effectively exploited nor can it be given the data presented so much of the rationale for an independent paper is lost. There are several ways that the papers can be fitted together. Efficiency with the figures would help. Figure 1 of Paper 2 could benefit from comparison directly to Figure 1 of Paper 1. Figure 2 of Paper 1 should be supplementary and fits thematically with Paper 2. The VNC confocal projection of Figure 3 Paper 1 are distracting, convey little information useful to the general reader, and can certainly be supplementary. The data of Figure 7 Paper 1 is reminiscent of data in Schwarz et al., 2017, albeit with corrections but need not be elaborated and could be perhaps combined with Figure 8 Paper 1 which is somewhat underdeveloped. The data Figure 9 Paper 1 seems premature for publication.

Reviewer #3:

This work provides tools that target every type of motor neurons in the proboscis, as evidenced by the fact that all muscle types are covered by the domains of neuromuscular junctions. Proximity of motor neuron dendrites to other neurons provide hints as to the interaction of sensory and central circuits with the motor neurons. These tools can now be used for functional studies, an example of which is provided in the accompanying manuscript. I found this to be really nice manuscript that will certainly be very useful for the Drosophila neural circuit community, particularly those working on feeding behavior. It really fills a missing dataset, complementing the numerous sensory neuron lines that are available, as well as the increasing number of interneuronal lines that have been used for feeding behavior analysis. My comments are not of the major kind, focusing more on presentation style.

[Editors' note: further revisions were suggested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

Thank you for resubmitting your article "Controlling every muscle for directed reaching of the fly proboscis" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, including Ronald Calabrese as the Reviewing Editor, Senior Editor and Reviewer #2.

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

We would like to draw your attention to changes in our revision policy that we have made in response to COVID-19 (https://elifesciences.org/articles/57162). Specifically, we are asking editors to accept without delay manuscripts, like yours, that they judge can stand as eLife papers without additional data, even if they feel that they would make the manuscript stronger. Thus the revisions requested below only address clarity and presentation.

Summary:

This paper presents remarkable anatomy and structure-function analysis of the muscles controlling the proboscis of Drosophila and firmly establishing this as model for the study of directed movements. They also perform thorough motor neuron activation (CsChrimson) /silencing (TNT) experiments using highly specific splitGAL4 lines and the lines provide a great set of tools for deeper functional analysis going forward.

Title: Please consider the changes to the title suggested by reviewer #1.

Essential revisions:

The authors have made a very strong revision of the manuscript, responding appropriately to all the reviewer comments. In the spirit of make a strong paper even stronger, the reviewers present further revisions, that can be accomplished expeditiously and can be reviewed by the RE.

Reviewer #1:

Summary:

This revised manuscript, in which the authors combined their two previous manuscripts, is much improved. I have one point that should be addressed to help improve the clarity and organization of the paper.

Essential revisions:

Figure 7 and Figure 8 contain many separate panels that describe the effects of activation and deficits of silencing proboscis motor neurons. Even though the y-axis labels contain the information they report, each panel uses different units, which makes it confusing for readers to understand how these values relate to proboscis kinematics.

In particular, interpreting changes in angle during feeding requires extra mental gymnastics. The change in angle from rest to feeding appears to be 80 deg (Figure 7I and Figure 7J). Activating mn9 does not further increase the rostrum feeding angle in Figure 7D. One might then attempt to compare angles in Figure 7I and Figure 7D, but the authors are thinking about this differently. Their logic is that activation (Figure7D) happens after the proboscis is extended, so the change should be about 0, whereas the positive 80 deg change during feeding does not occur when mn9 is silenced.

I suggest that instead of displaying the change in rostrum and haustellum angle during feeding, the authors should plot rostrum and haustellum angle during feeding. The authors should also explain the rationale behind the units in the legend. It is possible that chrimson expression in mn9 actually causes a different feeding angle to begin with, which would be interesting. If the proboscis was more extended, this would suggest an increase in baseline excitation. If it was more flexed and retracted, it could imply compensatory feedback to combat overexcitation. Same for mns 1 and 2V.

Reviewer #3:

What a gorgeous paper. I think it's a wonderful piece of work. I only have two very minor comments on the revision:

1) Subsection “An updated inventory of proboscis muscles” (second paragraph). Either a semi-colon or full stop after m3M. Also, in the figure legend, define L at the bottom.

2) In the Abstract, they write "…and throughout entire circuits, as the fly connectome approaches." However, there is no mention of this theme in the Discussion section. Maybe a sentence or two somewhere in the Discussion section on how their identification of the muscles and the motor neurons might help decipher the meaning of the interneuronal connections that will be revealed in the connectome. I personally believe it will be enormously useful, because, as the authors write (Introduction), "…they are the outputs upon which higher order circuits must act."

eLife. 2020 Jun 25;9:e54978. doi: 10.7554/eLife.54978.sa2

Author response


Essential revisions:

Upon careful review of these two co-submitted papers (henceforth referred to a Paper 1 and Paper 2) it was the clear consensus of the reviewers that neither paper alone is strong enough for eLife and we would understand if you chose to take the paper(s) elsewhere. If on the other hand, you chose to submit the two papers to eLife as one joint paper, then we would ensure that they were sent to the same reviewers, and we think that there is a high chance of publication with a short turn-around. No new experiments or analyses are needed.

If you take the eLife route the papers should be combined and focused. One paper will be easier for the authors to present and for the reader to comprehend. One could not read the Paper 2 without constantly referring to Paper 1 and the Paper 2 contains much repetition of the findings of Paper 1.

There are several ways that the papers can be fitted together as detailed in the individual expert reviews.

One potential path:

1) The theme presented for Paper 1 of rhythmic vs. discrete behaviors is not effectively exploited nor can it be given the data presented.

Now removed.

Likewise, the comparison to human reaching in Paper 2 seems premature and focusing the combined single paper on the tools developed and the wonderful anatomy and first-pass functional analysis seems more fruitful.

We understand the reservations about the analogy to directed reaching in vertebrates, and have removed the more speculative mention of this idea. However, since we do believe this to be an exciting future application of this work, we include it briefly in the Discussion section.

The data Figure 9 Paper 1 seems premature and underdeveloped for publication as does the data of Figure 8 Paper 2. They are presented is support of the rhythmic vs. discrete behaviors and human reaching themes and are not compelling. We recommend removing these figures to focus the paper.

Done.

2) Efficiency with the figures would help. Figure 1 of Paper 2 could benefit from comparison directly to Figure 1 of Paper 1.

Now reorganised.

Figure 2 of Paper 1 should be supplementary and fits thematically with Paper 2 material.

now removed for the sake of streamlining, since it made only a minor point.

The expert reviews suggest other figure fusions and changes.

Done.

3) The data of Figure 7 Paper 1 is reminiscent of data in Schwarz et al., 2017, albeit with corrections but need not be elaborated and could be perhaps combined with Figure 8 Paper 1, which is somewhat underdeveloped.

We have handled this according to Reviewer 1’s request that the former Figure 8 be a supplement to the former Figure 7 – now Figure 5.

4) Discussions can be shortened and in Paper 2 shifted away from detailed comparison to in Schwarz et al., 2017.

We have removed the detailed comparison from the text. There are differences in the findings that significantly alter interpretation of muscle roles, so wenow provide a supplementary table (newFigure 9 —figure supplement 1) summarizing our findings, and showing with asterisks which are new or different from published work.]

5) All Essential revisions in the expert reviews should all be addressed.

Done.

Reviewer #1:

Although the tools and data in these papers are detailed and valuable, the papers do not effectively show what new insight has been gained relative to previous studies.

Please see newFigure 9 —figure supplement 1 summarizing that 12 of the 16 behavioral phenotypes we describe were previously unknown.

There is also a large experimental gap: while the authors create the necessary genetic tools, they do not investigate the neural control and natural dynamics of proboscis movement beyond artificial optogenetic stimulation.

While optogenetic activation is artificial, silencing experiments are carried out in flies performing natural behaviors (for example, in Figure 8F-J and Figure 9H-I). Controls in these experiments provide measures of natural behavior. Besides these experiments, natural behavior was also tested in wildtype flies using five other measures (Figure 2E-G, K-L), plus an additional four now removed by reviewer request (previous paper 2, Figure 8). Certainly, more characterization of natural behavior will be needed in the future to describe precise kinematics.

In the absence of such data, the main contribution of these papers is the clear and thorough anatomy. However, the convoluted organization of the two manuscripts require the reader to sift through disparate data to reach a clear understanding of how the proboscis motor system is organized. These manuscripts would need to be significantly reorganized and streamlined to be understood by a general neuroscience audience.

Now combined into one paper.

Essential revisions:

1) It is not clear to why these manuscripts are separate. The basis for separating them seems to be to avoid one behemoth paper but the division between tools and function obscures the take-home message of the work. For example, many of the descriptions of musculature in "Comprehensive genetic tools to target motor neurons for each muscle of the fly proboscis" (called paper 1 below) are critical for understanding the optogenetic activation of motor neurons in "Motor neuron control of Drosophila proboscis reaching" (called paper 2 below), resulting in an unnecessary duplication of effort between the manuscripts to understand how the proboscis is moved. Take mn9, for example: it is hard to know what to take from Figure 1, Figure 4 and Figure 9 in paper 1, if the critical proof to show that mn9 extends the proboscis is in paper 2. (Past work has shown m9 to be involved in proboscis protraction, so you could argue that a knowledgeable reader may know this, but that undercuts the purpose of Figure 2 in paper 2). There are also examples of data in primary figures that do not advance the author's case. For example, in paper 1, Figure 2, Figure 3, Figure 6 and Figure 8 are supplementary figures.

Now reorganized.

Figure 2: The images of FlyLight Gen 1 Gal4 lines are unhelpful, fly researchers can find these and they are not helpful to a non-Drosophilist.

now removed.

Figure 3: I commend the authors for including images of the entire nervous system, not often shown, but again, these are supporting information.

Done.

Figure 6 can support Figure 5.

We have made room in other ways so that this can remain a main figure, since it is a key theme of the paper that insertion sites predict muscle function.

Figure 8 does suggest that MNs likely do not receive direct information from sensory neurons or other previously described interneurons, but these hypotheses are not tested further, so Figure 8 can support Figure 7.

Done.

By the same token, paper 2 would benefit from schematics of muscles and an overview of reaching related vs. pharyngeal motor neurons. This distinction is helpful, and since it is consistent with past work. Because the function of pharyngeal muscles is not explored further, it would not detract from the message of paper 2. paper 2 contains functional data that can be combined: e.g. protraction (Figure 2) and retraction (Figure 3) of the rostrum could be one figure,

now combined

together with anatomical data from paper 1.

Done.

Figure 2, Figure 3, Figure 4 and Figure 5 also contain many panels of different metrics, normalized or baseline subtracted. It is not necessary to show all these data in this form, and it detracts from the message.

Each panel shows a different behavior without redundancy; all are necessary for a full description of each motor neuron’s phenotype. We now streamline by showing extensors and retractors in the same plots.

My suggestion would be to combine Figure 2 Figure 3, Figure 4 and Figure 5 haustellum and rostrum images.

Previous 2-4 and 2-5 now combined.

Figure 7 is beautiful! But it should be split up to accompany the data in Figure 2 and Figure 3 and Figure 4 and Figure 5.

Great suggestion – done.

In addition, much of the introductions of both manuscripts are either unnecessary or misleading (see below), so combining the data into a single manuscript will also help to eliminate those problems. In summary, I do not see a clear reason to split data across papers.

Due to another request, we have removed the former Paper 2, Figure 8, but this had shown an un-collapsed quantification of how proboscis speed, velocity, length and distance varied over time.

2) The optogenetic activation data that the authors present do not provide insight into how reaching kinematics are produced: 2 second stimulation of motor neurons is not a naturalistic manipulation, and resulting proboscis trajectories are collapsed into single numbers in Figure 2, Figure 3, Figure 4, Figure 5 and Figure 6 of paper 2.

We agree.

Schwarz et al., also concluded that reaching is the result of precise temporal patterns of activation of proboscis motor neurons. The authors have all the tools to explore this further with calcium imaging or electrophysiology, though it is understandably beyond the scope of this paper. I anticipate another paper that begins with the observations in Figure 8 of paper 2 and then uses the tools shown here to explore how these searching movements are created.

We agree with this assessment as a future direction, and mention this need in the Discussion.

3) A related aspect of reaching is the treatment of what the authors call "coupling" of distinct parts of the proboscis, as when mn9 is activated and extends the haustellum in addition to the rostrum. Is this coupling mechanical, neural, or driven by proprioceptive feedback? The activation of additional motor neurons could be ruled out by silencing haustellum extending mns while activating mn9. The authors do not discuss sensory feedback, or the role of proprioception in determining the location of the target. This would be an interesting direction to pursue to draw closer connections to reaching in other organisms.

4) The authors state at various times that these genetic tools could help to make neural control of the proboscis a model for (1) rhythmic vs. discrete movements, and (2) a model of reaching in humans. These statements appear in the abstract and introductions of both papers, with little scholarship or links to literature that may prime the reader as to functional features that are similar in both models. The data to support these assertions are that proboscis speed is a bell shaped curve as a function of time and that proboscis reaching pauses to enter a second phase of slow approach. These observations seem rather trivial; what is the alternative?

Figure now removed, as per editor’s suggestion, and topics now moved to Discussion as future areas of research.

As the authors point out, proboscis reaching is already thought to be more complex than simply a reflex.

Reviewer #2:

Essential revisions:

Upon carefully reviewing this paper (henceforth referred to a Paper 1) and its companion by the exact same authors (Motor neuron control of Drosophila proboscis reaching – henceforth referred to as Paper 2), I do not recommend that they be published independently. One paper will be easier for the authors to present and for the reader to comprehend. I could not read the Paper 2 without constantly referring to Paper 1 and the Paper 2 contains much repetition of the findings of Paper 1. The theme presented for Paper 1 of rhythmic vs. discrete behaviors is not effectively exploited nor can it be given the data presented so much of the rationale for an independent paper is lost.

Theme now removed.

There are several ways that the papers can be fitted together. Efficiency with the figures would help. Figure 1 of Paper 2 could benefit from comparison directly to Figure 1 of Paper 1.

Now reorganised.

Figure 2 of Paper 1 should be supplementary and fits thematically with Paper 2.

The VNC confocal projection of Figure 3 Paper 1 are distracting, convey little information useful to the general reader, and can certainly be supplementary.

Done.

The data of Figure 7 Paper 1 is reminiscent of data in Schwarz et al., 2017, albeit with corrections but need not be elaborated and could be perhaps combined with Figure 8 Paper 1 which is somewhat underdeveloped.

We have handled this according to Reviewer 1’s request that the former Figure 8 be a supplement to the former 7, now Figure 5.

The data Figure 9 Paper 1 seems premature for publication.

Now removed.[Editors' note: further revisions were suggested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

Title: Please consider the changes to the title suggested by reviewer #1.

Essential revisions:

The authors have made a very strong revision of the manuscript, responding appropriately to all the reviewer comments. In the spirit of make a strong paper even stronger, the reviewers present further revisions, that can be accomplished expeditiously and can be reviewed by the RE.

Reviewer #1:

Essential revisions:

Figure 7 and Figure 8 contain many separate panels that describe the effects of activation and deficits of silencing proboscis motor neurons. Even though the y-axis labels contain the information they report, each panel uses different units, which makes it confusing for readers to understand how these values relate to proboscis kinematics.

In particular, interpreting changes in angle during feeding requires extra mental gymnastics. The change in angle from rest to feeding appears to be 80 deg (Figure 7I and Figure 7J). Activating mn9 does not further increase the rostrum feeding angle in Figure 7D. One might then attempt to compare angles in Figure 7I and Figure 7D, but the authors are thinking about this differently. Their logic is that activation (Figure7D) happens after the proboscis is extended, so the change should be about 0, whereas the positive 80 deg change during feeding does not occur when mn9 is silenced.

I suggest that instead of displaying the change in rostrum and haustellum angle during feeding, the authors should plot rostrum and haustellum angle during feeding. The authors should also explain the rationale behind the units in the legend.

The units for Figure 7 are briefly explained in the legend, which also now directs readers to more information in the Materials and methods section.

It is possible that chrimson expression in mn9 actually causes a different feeding angle to begin with, which would be interesting. If the proboscis was more extended, this would suggest an increase in baseline excitation. If it was more flexed and retracted, it could imply compensatory feedback to combat overexcitation. Same for mns 1 and 2V.

Figure 7 shows how motor neuron activations change proboscis angle. We chose normalized angles because the way we mount flies results in different starting positions, which makes raw (absolute) angles more variable. We expand the explanation for using normalization in Materials and methods section, and direct the reader there from both the text and the legend of Figure 7. We also include a new Figure 7 – supplement with an analysis of the raw angles, as the reviewer requested.

The expanded explanation reads, “Measures of joint angles were chosen to best suit each assay. Due to the challenge of gluing flies to pins manually, the camera view varied, leading to variability in the measured raw angles of the joints. To overcome this variability, within-fly normalization was performed wherever possible, subtracting food- or CsChrimson-stimulated extension angle from the angle immediately before stimulation. Note that normalization was not possible for resting state angles, since there is no change from baseline. Analysis of raw joint angles (Figure 7—figure supplement 2) shows similar relations between motor neurons and joint angles as the normalized data but with greater inter-fly variability, as expected from the range of starting positions, so the main figures use normalized values as follows…”

The relevant data panels for comparison of normalized vs. raw are:

Normalized: Raw:

Figure 7D, Figure 8D, Figure 9M Figure 7—figure supplement 2A

Figure 7E, Figure 8E, Figure 9N Figure 7—figure supplement 2B

Figure 7I, Figure 8I, Figure 9R Figure 7—figure supplement 2C

Figure 7J, Figure 8J, Figure 9S Figure 7—figure supplement 2D

Reviewer #3:

1) Subsection “An updated inventory of proboscis muscles” (second paragraph). Either a semi-colon or full stop after m3M.

Done.

Also, in the figure legend, define L at the bottom.

Added to legend of Table 1.

2) In the Abstract, they write "…and throughout entire circuits, as the fly connectome approaches."

Now removed, per reviewer 1.

However, there is no mention of this theme in the Discussion section. Maybe a sentence or two somewhere in the Discussion section on how their identification of the muscles and the motor neurons might help decipher the meaning of the interneuronal connections that will be revealed in the connectome. I personally believe it will be enormously useful, because, as the authors write (Introduction), "…they are the outputs upon which higher order circuits must act."

Now a brief mention in the Discussion section.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Transparent reporting form

    Data Availability Statement

    All data generated or analysed during this study are included in the manuscript and supporting files.


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES