Abstract
Objective
To test whether polygenic risk score for schizophrenia (PRS‐S) interacts with childhood adversity and daily‐life stressors to influence momentary mental state domains (negative affect, positive affect, and subtle psychosis expression) and stress‐sensitivity measures.
Methods
The data were retrieved from a general population twin cohort including 593 adolescents and young adults. Childhood adversity was assessed using the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire. Daily‐life stressors and momentary mental state domains were measured using ecological momentary assessment. PRS‐S was trained on the latest Psychiatric Genetics Consortium schizophrenia meta‐analysis. The analyses were conducted using multilevel mixed‐effects tobit regression models.
Results
Both childhood adversity and daily‐life stressors were associated with increased negative affect, decreased positive affect, and increased subtle psychosis expression, while PRS‐S was only associated with increased positive affect. No gene–environment correlation was detected. There is novel evidence for interaction effects between PRS‐S and childhood adversity to influence momentary mental states [negative affect (b = 0.07, P = 0.013), positive affect (b = −0.05, P = 0.043), and subtle psychosis expression (b = 0.11, P = 0.007)] and stress‐sensitivity measures.
Conclusion
Exposure to childhood adversities, particularly in individuals with high PRS‐S, is pleiotropically associated with emotion dysregulation and psychosis proneness.
Keywords: gene–environment interaction, psychosis, childhood trauma, daily‐life stressors, experience sampling method, emotion regulation
Significant outcomes.
Early‐life stressors and daily‐life stressors influence momentary mental state domains (negative affect, positive affect, and subtle psychosis expression).
Polygenic risk for schizophrenia moderates the association between early‐life stressors and momentary mental state domains as well as stress‐sensitivity measures.
It appears that the exposure to childhood adversities, especially in individuals with high molecular genetic risk for schizophrenia, is linked to emotion dysregulation and psychosis proneness.
Limitations.
Daily‐life stressors may not only influence momentary mental states but also be influenced by them.
Measuring psychosis proneness in the general population may be more challenging than measuring emotional reactivity.
Introduction
Converging evidence suggests that the genetic and nongenetic vulnerability contributing to the development of schizophrenia and related psychotic disorders is shared across a broad range of psychotic and non‐psychotic clinical syndromes and expressed non‐specifically in the affective, psychotic, and cognitive domains in the general population 1, 2, 3, 4, 5. Understanding the pleiotropic effects of risk factors associated with schizophrenia on the earliest stages of pluripotent psychopathology may therefore pave the way for gaining insight into the shared biological and mental processes underlying psychosis spectrum disorder (PSD). Contemporary concepts of mental disorders acknowledge psychopathology as a highly dynamic and time‐varying complex system that can only be understood from its interconnected constituent parts. These concepts provide a useful theoretical framework to investigate how alterations of micro‐level transdiagnostic mental states, varying from moment‐to‐moment, precede the transition to the more discrete clinical syndrome of PSD 6.
Studies of ecological momentary assessment (EMA), designed to collect micro‐level mental state variation, have consistently shown that disturbed emotional (affective dysregulation) and psychotic reactivity to daily‐life stressors (aberrant salience attribution) are associated with psychosis expression in different populations at varying severity stages, the general population, clinical high‐risk samples, siblings of patients with PSD, and cases 7, 8, 9, 10, 11, 12, 13. Further, in agreement with the diathesis–stress theory 14, EMA studies have provided evidence that genetic and environmental vulnerabilities are associated with alterations in emotional reactivity. Individuals who experienced childhood adversity (CA) showed heightened emotional and psychotic reactivity to daily‐life stressors 15, 16, 17, 18, 19, and increased persistence of momentary mental states 20; the influence of CA on the reactivity to daily‐life stressors was stronger in populations with increased proxy genetic risk (i.e., service users, clinical high risk, or first‐episode psychosis compared to healthy controls) 15, 16.
Early hypothesis‐driven candidate gene studies also provided some evidence for the role of gene–environment interaction (G × E) in affective and psychotic reactivity to daily‐life stressors 21, 22, 23. While these studies can be considered the first steps in understanding the genetic correlates of daily stress reactivity, they were undersized and by design too simplistic to capture the complex genetic architecture. The use of cumulative risk scores—polygenic risk score (PRS)—as a single molecular metric has significantly enhanced the power to detect G × E without compromising the validity of the results 24. We previously showed that the likelihood of schizophrenia is increased as a function of the interaction between PRS for schizophrenia (PRS‐S) and childhood adversities as well as cannabis use 25.
A recent perspective article discusses how real‐time measurement of cognitive and emotional processes via EMA, which eliminates retrospective recall bias, combined with modern polygenic approach may greatly advance our understanding of the role of G × E in psychopathology and mental wellbeing 26. PRS‐based approaches for testing G × E represent a novel approach, and to the best of our knowledge, no EMA study has utilized PRS‐S yet.
In this study, guided by the transdiagnostic Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) framework prioritizing shared dimensional psychological constructs cutting across diagnostic categories 27, we outlined a step‐by‐step analytical plan to test the contribution of G × E to altered emotional processes, previously associated with the earliest stages of PSD. Bringing together a unique sampling frame of a general population twin cohort of young adults and adolescents with rich EMA data, we aimed to investigate for the first time whether molecular genetic risk score for schizophrenia (PRS‐S) interacts with early‐life stressors (CA) and daily‐life stressors (social, event, activity, and overall stress) to influence momentary mental state domains (negative affect, positive affect, and subtle psychosis expression) and whether PRS‐S moderates the association between CA and stress‐sensitivity measures.
Methods
Sample
The data were derived from the first wave of the TwinssCan, a general population twin cohort that started including adolescent and young adult (age range = 15–35 years) twins (n = 796), their siblings (n = 43), and parents (n = 363) from April 2010 to April 2014 28. The TwinssCan cohort comprises individuals fulfilling the inclusion criteria from the East Flanders Prospective Twin Survey 29, a prospective population‐based, multi‐birth registry positioned in Flanders, Belgium. Participants were excluded whether they had a pervasive mental disorder as indicated by caregivers. All participants gave written informed consent and parent(s) signed an informed consent for participants below the age of 18 years. The local ethics committee approved the study (Commissie Medische Ethiek van de Universitaire ziekenhuizen KU Leuven, Nr. B32220107766). Sequential analysis based on sex, fetal membranes, umbilical cord blood groups, placental alkaline phosphatase, and DNA fingerprints was used to determine zygosity 29.
Measures
Ecological momentary assessment (EMA)
EMA is a well‐validated structured diary technique that assesses individual and contextual measures in the current moment, throughout the day 30, 31, 32. During the assessment period (six consecutive days), participants used a digital device (PsyMate)33 to electronically fill out a brief questionnaire assessing their emotions, thoughts, context, and their appraisal of that context 10 times/day at an unpredictable moment (semi‐random) in each of ten 90‐min time blocks between 7:30 and 22:30 33.
Conforming to previously described methods, the negative affect (NA) 15 and the positive affect (PA) 34 domains were the mean scores of items assessing emotional states. Subtle psychosis expression (PE) was the mean score of items concerning psychotic‐like experiences 35. Daily‐life stress domains were constructed as event 36, social 15, activity 15, and overall stress (average of event, social, and activity stress). For detailed description of EMA items, see Table 1.
Table 1.
Negative affect | Mean score of five items (I feel anxious, lonely, down, insecure, and irritated). Each item was rated on a 7‐point Likert scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 7 (very). |
Positive affect | Mean score of four items (I feel cheerful, satisfied, relaxed, and globally feeling well). Each item was rated on a 7‐point Likert scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 7 (very). |
Subtle psychosis expression | Mean score of five items (suspiciousness, being afraid of losing control, racing thoughts, pervasive thoughts, and difficulties to express thoughts). Each item was rated on a 7‐point Likert scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 7 (very). |
Event stress | Participants were asked to rate the most important event since last entry on its pleasantness on a bipolar Likert scale from −3 (very unpleasant) to 3 (very pleasant). Consistent with previous studies 36, ratings from −3 to −1 were considered stressful and scores from 1 to 3 were recoded to 0. For simplification, the score was reversed and very unpleasant events therefore represented the highest score on a scale from 0 to 3. |
Social stress | Participants were asked with whom they currently were (e.g., nobody or family). When participants reported to be alone, they were asked to answer the following items: I like to be alone (reversed); I would prefer to have company; and I feel safe alone (reversed). When participants reported to be in company, they were asked the following items: I would prefer to be alone; I find the people I am with pleasant (reversed); I feel safe (reversed); I feel I belong (reversed); and I feel judged. All items were scored on a Likert scale from 1 (not at all) to 7 (very), and the mean score of all items was used to estimate the social stress. |
Activity stress | Participants were asked about the activity they participated in just before the beep (e.g., resting, watching TV, and smoking). The mean score of the following items [rated on a 7‐point Likert scale from 1 (not at all) to 7 (very)] was used to calculate an activity stress score: I would prefer doing something else; This activity is difficult for me; and I can do this well (reversed). |
Overall stress | Average of event stress, social stress, and activity stress. |
EMA, ecological momentary assessment.
Childhood adversity
CA was assessed using the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ) 37 that consists of 28 items rated on a 5‐point Likert scale assessing five domains of maltreatment (emotional and physical neglect along with emotional, physical, and sexual abuse). Table S3 reports the frequencies of childhood adversity domains. Consistent with previous work 38 using this dataset, CA was defined as the mean score of all five domains.
Genotyping, imputation, and PRS
Genotypes of the twins and their siblings were generated on two platforms: the Infinium CoreExome‐24 and Infinium PsychArray‐24 kits. Quality control (QC) procedures were performed using PLINK v1.9 39 in both datasets separately. Single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) and participants with call rates below 95% and 98%, respectively, were removed. A strict SNP QC only for subsequent sample QC steps was then conducted. This involved a minor allele frequency (MAF) threshold >10% and a Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium (HWE) P‐value >10−5, followed by linkage disequilibrium (LD)‐based SNP pruning (R 2 < 0.5). This resulted in ~58K SNPs to assess sex errors (n = 8), heterozygosity [F < 5× the standard deviation (SD), n = 3], homozygosity (F > 5× SD), and relatedness by pairwise identity by descent (IBD) values (monozygotic: , dizygotic and full siblings: or , n = 5). The ancestry‐informed principal component (PC) analyses were conducted by EIGENSTRAT 40. The ethnic outliers of which the first 4 PCs diverged >10× SD from Utah residents with Northern and Western European ancestry from the CEPH collection (CEU) and Toscani in Italia (TSI) samples (n = 5), and >3× SD of the TwinssCan samples (n = 7) were excluded. After removing these subjects, a regular SNP QC was performed (SNP call rate >98%, HWE P > 1e‐06, MAF > 1%, and strand ambiguous SNPs and duplicate SNPs were removed).
The two QCed datasets were imputed on the Michigan server 41 using the HRC r1.1 2016 reference panel with European samples after phasing with Eagle v2.3. Postimputation QC involved removing SNPs with imputation quality (R 2) < 0.8, with a MAF < 0.01, SNPs that had a discordant MAF compared to the reference panel (MAF difference with HRC reference > 0.15), as well as strand ambiguous AT/CG SNPs and multi‐allelic SNPs. The two chips were merged, and an additional check for MAF > 0.01, HWE P > 1e‐06 was executed, which resulted in 3,407,392 SNPs for 688 individuals.
PRS‐S was calculated based on the meta‐analysis results from the Psychiatric Genetics Consortium (PGC)‐2 SZ and the CLOZUK sample (schizophrenia cases on clozapine from the UK) 42. Then, insertions and deletions, ambiguous SNPs, SNPs with a MAF < 0.01, imputation quality R 2 < 0.9, SNPs located in complex‐LD regions, and long‐range LD regions 43 were excluded. Overlapping SNPs between the schizophrenia GWAS (training), 1000 genomes (reference), and our dataset (target) were selected. These SNPs were clumped in two rounds using PLINK's clump function (round 1: ‐‐clump‐kb 250 ‐‐clump‐r2 0.5; round 2: ‐‐clump‐kb 5000 ‐‐clump‐r2 0.2), resulting in 88 736 SNPs for PRS‐S calculation. Odds ratios for autosomal SNPs reported in the schizophrenia summary statistics were log‐converted into beta values. PRS‐S was calculated using PLINK’s score function. Informed by the PGC analyses, PRS‐S with cutoff P < 0.05 (including 21 901 SNPs) was used in the following analyses to achieve a balance between the number of false‐positive and true‐positive risk alleles 44. For details, see supplementary information.
Statistical analyses
For the purpose of this analysis, parents were excluded. Only participants with complete data on the CTQ, age, sex, and PRS‐S were included in the analyses. Conforming to previous studies 32, participants who completed less than 1/3 of the EMA questionnaires were excluded from the analysis (n = 52). One individual with a visibly extreme value of CA (>7 SD from the mean) was excluded from analyses. The final sample included 593 participants: monozygotic (n = 180), dizygotic (n = 380) twin pairs, and their siblings (n = 33).
The data have a hierarchical structure. Multiple EMA observations (level 1) were clustered within subjects (level 2), who were part of twin pairs (level 3). Multilevel mixed‐effects model is the recommended method to handle data including observations at more than one level in terms of unit of analysis by taking into account of the variability associated with each level of nesting 45, 46, 47. To handle this nested structure including familial relatedness, multilevel mixed‐effects models were applied. In the current study, as typically observed in EMA studies, left censoring (NA or PE) and right censoring (PA) were present due to a greater amount of observations with a score of one (NA or PE) or seven (PA) on the outcome variables. In consideration of the skewness, multilevel mixed tobit regression 48 (censored regression) with an unstructured covariance matrix was performed using the Stata version 15.0 49 ‘METOBIT’ command. The independent variables [PRS‐S, CA, and daily‐life stressors (overall, event, social, and activity stress)] were standardized and centered (min = 0, SD = 1).
First, we analyzed the associations between CA and PRS‐S, and their interaction, with EMA outcomes. Second, we tested the associations of daily‐life stressors, and their interaction with PRS‐S, and EMA outcomes. Third, for sensitivity analyses, we constructed stress‐sensitivity measures for use in G × E analyses. Consistent with previous work 50, we applied a two‐step approach to analyze stress sensitivity. First, separate multilevel tobit regression models including the daily‐life stressors as independent variables and EMA outcomes as dependent variables were estimated. The models were fitted via maximum likelihood and included a random slope for the daily‐life stressor. From these models, fitted (predicted) values (substituting maximum‐likelihood estimates for fixed effects and empirical Bayes predictions for random effects) were stored as stress sensitivity (e.g., NA—event stress sensitivity; the association between event stress and NA) scores. Eventually, we tested the associations between CA and PRS‐S, and their interaction, with normally distributed stress‐sensitivity scores as dependent variables in multilevel linear regression models using the ‘MIXED’ command.
All models were controlled for a priori covariates (age and sex), while models including PRS‐S were additionally adjusted for ancestry, using the first 2 genomic principal components (PCs). To adequately control for confounding 51, interaction models included these covariates not only as main effects but also as covariate × environment and covariate × PRS‐S interaction terms.
Results
Sample characteristics are reported in Table 2. A correlation matrix of the three momentary mental state domains is provided in Table S4.
Table 2.
Total sample (N = 593) Mean (SD), EMA observations |
|
---|---|
Sex | |
Female | 362 (61%) |
Male | 231 (39%) |
Age | 17.60 (3.81) |
Childhood adversity | 1.35 (0.31) |
Negative affect | 1.78 (0.84), n = 23293 † , † |
Positive affect | 5.06 (1.06), n = 23265 † , † |
Subtle psychosis expression | 1.89 (0.99), n = 23272 † , † |
Overall stress | 1.89 (0.78), n = 23225 † , † |
Event stress | 0.34 (0.84), n = 22812 † , † |
Social stress | 2.33 (1.08), n = 23172 † , † |
Activity stress | 2.95 (1.43), n = 23192 † , † |
EMA, ecological momentary assessment; PRS‐S, polygenic risk score for schizophrenia; SD, standard deviation.
Number of observations.
Main associations and interactions of CA and PRS‐S on momentary mental state domains
CA was associated with increased NA, decreased PA, and increased PE, while PRS‐S was only associated with increased PA (Table 3). These results remained significant after controlling for daily‐life stressors (Table S5). No gene–environment correlation was present as PRS‐S was not associated strongly or significantly with CA (b = −0.01, 95% CI −0.03 to 0.02, P = 0.676).
Table 3.
Association with CA | Association with PRS‐S † , † | Interaction between PRS‐S and CA † , † | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
b | P‐value | 95% CI | b | P‐value | 95% CI | b | P‐value | 95% CI | |
Negative affect | 0.13 | <0.001 † , † | 0.07–0.19 | −0.02 | 0.502 | −0.08 to 0.04 | 0.07 | 0.013 † , † | 0.01–0.13 |
Positive affect | −0.12 | <0.001 † , † | −0.17 to −0.06 | 0.08 | 0.003 † , † | 0.03–0.14 | −0.05 | 0.043 | −0.10 to −0.00 |
Subtle psychosis expression | 0.18 | <0.001 † , † | 0.10–0.26 | −0.03 | 0.547 | −0.11 to 0.06 | 0.11 | 0.007 † , † | 0.03–0.19 |
CA, childhood adversity; CI, confidence interval; PRS‐S, polygenic risk score for schizophrenia.
All analyses were adjusted for age and sex.
Also adjusted for two principal components.
Significant after controlling for family‐wise type I error using the Bonferroni method (0.05/3 = 0.0167).
PRS‐S moderated the association of CA with all three momentary mental state domains, while only NA and PE reached the Bonferroni‐adjusted statistical significance level (Table 3). The interaction effects remained significant after controlling for daily‐life stressors (Table S5). As shown in Fig. 1, visualizing the fitted interaction effects between PRS‐S and CA on momentary mental state domains, the association between CA and mental state domains increased as a function of increased PRS‐S (for scatter plots of raw data, see Figure S3).
Main associations and interactions of daily‐life stressors and PRS‐S on momentary mental state domains
The overall mean and each of the daily‐life stressors were associated with increased NA, decreased PA, and increased PE (Table S6). No gene–environment correlation was present as PRS‐S was not associated with any of the daily‐life stressors (overall stress: b = −0.02, 95% CI −0.06 to 0.03, P = 0.491; event stress: b = 0.01, 95% CI −0.04 to 0.06, P = 0.672; social stress: b = −0.03, 95% CI −0.07 to 0.02, P = 0.267; activity stress: b = −0.01, 95% CI −0.05 to 0.03, P = 0.571). No evidence for significant interaction effects between daily‐life stressors and PRS‐S was found (Table S6).
Main associations and interactions of CA and PRS‐S on stress‐sensitivity measures
CA was associated with increased stress‐sensitivity measures, while PRS‐S was only associated with increased PA stress sensitivity. Evidence was found for significant gene–environment interaction. The associations between CA and stress‐sensitivity measures were greater if individuals had higher PRS‐S (Table 4).
Table 4.
Association with CA | Association with PRS‐S † , † | Interaction between PRS‐S and CA † , † | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
b | P‐value | 95% CI | b | P‐value | 95% CI | b | P‐value | 95% CI | |
NA—Overall | 0.12 | <0.001* | 0.07–0.18 | −0.02 | 0.487 | −0.08 to 0.04 | 0.07 | 0.012* | 0.01–0.12 |
NA—Event | 0.12 | <0.001 | 0.07–0.18 | −0.02 | 0.493 | −0.08 to 0.04 | 0.07 | 0.013 | 0.01–0.12 |
NA—Social | 0.12 | <0.001 | 0.07–0.18 | −0.02 | 0.473 | −0.08 to 0.04 | 0.07 | 0.012 | 0.01–0.12 |
NA—Activity | 0.12 | <0.001 | 0.07–0.18 | −0.02 | 0.471 | −0.08–0.04 | 0.07 | 0.011 | 0.02–0.12 |
PA—Overall | −0.11 | <0.001* | −0.16 to −0.06 | 0.08 | 0.003* | 0.03–0.13 | −0.05 | 0.042 | −0.10 to −0.00 |
PA—Event | −0.11 | <0.001 | −0.16 to −0.06 | 0.08 | 0.003 | 0.03–0.13 | −0.05 | 0.042 | −0.10 to −0.00 |
PA—Social | −0.11 | <0.001 | −0.16 to −0.06 | 0.08 | 0.003 | 0.03–0.13 | −0.05 | 0.043 | −0.10 to −0.00 |
PA—Activity | −0.11 | <0.001 | −0.16 to −0.06 | 0.08 | 0.003 | 0.03–0.13 | −0.05 | 0.042 | −0.10 to −0.00 |
PE—Overall | 0.18 | <0.001* | 0.10–0.25 | −0.03 | 0.546 | −0.11 to 0.06 | 0.11 | 0.007* | 0.03–0.19 |
PE—Event | 0.17 | <0.001 | 0.10–0.25 | −0.03 | 0.546 | −0.11 to 0.06 | 0.11 | 0.006 | 0.03–0.19 |
PE—Social | 0.18 | <0.001 | 0.10–0.26 | −0.03 | 0.545 | −0.11 to 0.06 | 0.11 | 0.006 | 0.03–0.19 |
PE—Activity | 0.18 | <0.001 | 0.10–0.26 | −0.03 | 0.520 | −0.11 to 0.06 | 0.11 | 0.006 | 0.03–0.19 |
CA, childhood adversity; CI, confidence interval; NA, negative affect; PE, subtle psychosis expression; PA, positive affect; PRS‐S, polygenic risk score for schizophrenia.
All analyses were adjusted for age and sex.
Additionally adjusted for two principal components,
Significant after controalling for family‐wise type I error using the Bonferroni method (0.05/3 = 0.0167).
Discussion
Principal findings
In this first study testing PRS‐S for an interaction with early and late stressors (childhood adversity and minor daily‐life stressors) in association with dynamic pluripotent mental processes in the largest EMA dataset to date, evidence emerged for an interaction between PRS‐S and childhood adversity to influence momentary mental states (negative affect, positive affect, and subtle psychosis expression) and stress‐sensitivity measures.
Stress exposure and emotional processes
In line with long‐established findings from population‐based datasets and samples of help‐seeking adolescents and young adults 9, 15, we showed that minor daily‐life stressors, regardless of the type of the stressor, were associated with all three domains of momentary mental states. More importantly, we provided further support for the shared vulnerability theory of mental disorders by demonstrating that CA was associated with NA, PA, and PE. These results echo recent findings from our line of research showing that CA is not exclusively associated with a specific mental disorder category, but rather with multidimensional psychopathology (cutting across diagnostic categories) in the general population, such as psychotic experiences, affective dysregulation, and negative symptoms 52, 53, 54, 55, 56, 57. Therefore, it is plausible to conceptualize that the sensitivity to daily‐life stressors is molded by previous exposure to significant life stressors as discussed in the models of diathesis–stress 14 and sensory processing sensitivity 58. Furthermore, the exposome, a dense network of environmental exposures 59, 60, may contribute to a person’s sensitivity to stress.
Genetic vulnerability for schizophrenia moderates sensitivity to childhood adversity
PRS‐S, as anticipated, had no significant predictive power for the EMA outcomes with the exception of positive affect‐related items that were positively associated with PRS‐S. The positive association between PRS‐S and positive affect might seem counterintuitive at first glance given that EMA studies have shown a decreased positive affect in patients with schizophrenia and PRS‐S is associated with schizophrenia. However, emerging evidence suggests that the relation between PRS‐S and symptom dimensions at the population level appears to be not following a simple logic. As this is the very first and the only EMA study investigating PRS‐S, we could not make an exact comparison of our findings. Unfortunately, because most studies focus on psychopathology, it is also difficult to draw a parallel between our current results on positive affect and findings from studies investigating the association between PRS‐S and symptom dimensions in healthy participants. These studies in healthy participants have shown inconsistent results 61, 62, 63, 64, 65, 66. Some reported no association between PRS‐S and several symptom dimensions 63, 67, while others reported negative associations between polygenic risk and schizotypy 64, 67. In addition, in the rare instances where it has been examined, PRS‐S appears to contribute to abilities required for a creative profession 68. Taken together, these findings in fact suggest a substantial fraction of pathoetiology may be explained by the influence of environment and the GxE. In agreement, our interaction analyses showed a consistent pattern. PRS‐S moderated the influence of CA, but not the impact of minor daily‐life stressors, for all momentary mental states, with negative affect and subtle psychosis expression reaching the Bonferroni‐adjusted statistical significance level. This interaction effect was similarly present in sensitivity to overall stress and consistently observed for sensitivity to event, activity, and social stress.
The current results showing a difference between the degree of genetic moderation of two stressors (CA and daily‐life stressors) underscore the importance of the type, timing, and extent of stressor in mental health impact. This is consistent with the neurodevelopmental hypothesis 69, 70 that postulates that exposure to early‐life stressors in neurodevelopmentally sensitive periods is more likely to disturb the balance of important stress systems and leads to enduring emotional and behavioural problems in later life. These findings, combined with a recent meta‐analysis showing that patients with PSD experience more negative emotion and less positive emotion in daily life 71, suggest that genetic and early adversities may have a permanent impact on mental wellbeing, resulting in a trait‐like feature of person‐specific alterations in emotional expression and psychosis proneness. An interesting question for future research spanning an extended period would be whether persistent low‐threshold daily‐life stressors may influence emotional reactivity toward mental ill health in the long‐term or whether more serious life events are required to reach the threshold for clinical syndrome. Early candidate gene studies investigating the genetic moderation of mental health outcomes have generated mostly inconclusive findings because of methodological issues. COMTVal158Met Val/Val carriers displayed increased paranoia in response to stress 22, while, in another study, Met/Met genotype was associated with increased PA in response to experiencing positive events 72. Momentary stress interacted with genetic variation in the brain‐derived neurotrophic factor gene, Met carriers reporting higher paranoia scores than Val carriers 22. However, a recent study failed to replicate these findings, but showed an interaction between childhood trauma and RGS4, FKBP5, and OXTR respectively 73. As PRS‐based approaches for testing G × E have recently emerged, no comparable EMA study was available. However, in line with our study, research showed that higher PRS (consisting of 13 genes previously associated with vulnerability to environmental exposure) increased the influence of CA on stress sensitization 74. Further, we recently showed evidence that the interaction between PRS‐S and childhood adversity increases the likelihood of schizophrenia 25.
Given the influence of psychosocial stressors on immune processes and hypothalamic–pituitary axis modulation underlying the etiopathogenesis of PSD 75, future studies embracing biologically informative target approaches may exploit the unique ability of EMA to capture dynamic fluctuation of mental states and combine the granular information with multi‐omics data (e.g., genome, proteome, and epigenome) to study candidate molecular mechanisms such as FKBP5 76 and extend previous EMA work investigating cortisol reactivity to daily‐life stressors in relation to PSD 77.
Pleiotropic influence of exposures and genetic vulnerability on psychopathology
Our findings agree with the literature showing that the influence of CA 78 and schizophrenia genetic liability 2, 63, 65 on mental health in the general population is pleiotropic and converge on shared psychological constructs and multidimensional psychopathology in the causal path to PSD. Considering the fact that mental health phenotypes (EMA outcomes in our study) are associated with each other at both dimensional and diagnostic levels and thereby violating the assumption of independence of pleiotropy, it is also plausible to argue that these disorders defined at the symptom level might be different expressions (phenotypic presentations) of a substantially shared pathoetiology with varying outcomes because of disease modifiers rather than distinct entities 79, 80. Furthermore, in accordance with the idea of an affective pathway to psychosis, it is also plausible that the EMA outcomes may be temporally associated with each other, such that the interaction between PRS‐S and childhood adversity may influence psychosis expression through negative affect.
A growing investment into transdiagnostic research of mental health will hopefully shed more light on this matter. Abundant evidence shows that the earliest psychopathological processes expressed before the prodrome of PSD are non‐specific and include affective dysregulation, aberrant salience, and subtle cognitive disturbances 79. In this regard, EMA outcomes capturing subtle and transitory mental states, such as emotional reactivity and stress sensitivity, are arguably more useful transdiagnostic phenotypes than static questionnaire‐based interval assessments to examine the contributions of environmental and genetic factors to variation in mental health at the community level 26. As recently proposed 81, multi‐layered digital phenotyping via mobile devices may advance the RDoC work in the era of ‘Big Data’ boosted by historic efforts of personalized medicine such as the National Institutes of Health initiative, the All of Us research programme.
Limitations
The current study provided the first insights into the influence of genetic regulation of exposure to stressors on dynamic mental states by taking advantage of a unique population dataset with fine‐grained phenotyping. However, several methodological considerations should be noted. First, although one of the strengths is that the sample comprises individuals at an age range when mental disorders often emerge, it is also possible that the association between PRS‐S and stress may change as a function of aging and cumulative stressor load. Second, the retrospective collection of CA might be subject to recall and response biases; however, it is not intuitive how these would be differential with regard to EMA outcomes or PRS‐S, or their interaction. Third, daily‐life stressors might not only influence momentary mental states but also be influenced by them. Fourth, EMA provides a unique opportunity to focus on moment‐to‐moment fluctuations of mental states; nevertheless, it may be more difficult to detect psychosis proneness than emotional reactivity in the general population.
To conclude, this observational study suggests that the exposure to childhood adversities, especially in individuals with high molecular genetic risk for schizophrenia, is associated with emotion dysregulation and psychosis proneness. Further preregistered confirmatory research is required to validate these findings.
Supporting information
Acknowledgements
The authors thank Jill Ielegems, Katrien Lyssens, Davinia Verhoeven, and Debora op’t Eijnde for data collection. The authors would like to acknowledge that the East Flanders Prospective Twin Survey (EFPTS) is partly supported by the Association for Scientific Research in Multiple Births and that the TwinssCan project is part of the European Community's Seventh Framework Program under grant agreement no. HEALTH‐F2‐2009‐241909 (Project EU‐GEI).
Declaration of interest
The authors declare no conflicts of interest.
Pries L‐K, Klingenberg B, Menne‐Lothmann C, Decoster J, van Winkel R, Collip D, Delespaul P, De Hert M, Derom C, Thiery E, Jacobs N, Wichers M, Cinar O, Lin BD, Luykx JJ, Rutten BPF, van Os J, Guloksuz S. Polygenic liability for schizophrenia and childhood adversity influences daily‐life emotion dysregulation and psychosis proneness.
Data availability statement
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.
References
- 1. van Os J, Kenis G, Rutten BP. The environment and schizophrenia. Nature 2010;468:203–212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. van Os J, van der Steen Y, Islam MA et al. Evidence that polygenic risk for psychotic disorder is expressed in the domain of neurodevelopment, emotion regulation and attribution of salience. Psychol Med 2017;47:2421–2437. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Nivard MG, Gage SH, Hottenga JJ et al. Genetic overlap between schizophrenia and developmental psychopathology: longitudinal and multivariate polygenic risk prediction of common psychiatric traits during development. Schizophr Bull. 2017;43:1197–1207. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Misiak B, Krefft M, Bielawski T, Moustafa AA, Sasiadek MM, Frydecka D. Toward a unified theory of childhood trauma and psychosis: A comprehensive review of epidemiological, clinical, neuropsychological and biological findings. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2017;75:393–406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Anttila V, Bulik‐Sullivan B, Finucane HK et al. Analysis of shared heritability in common disorders of the brain. Science 2018;360:eaap8757. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Nelson B, McGorry PD, Wichers M, Wigman JTW, Hartmann JA. Moving from static to dynamic models of the onset of mental disorder: a review. JAMA Psychiatry 2017;74:528–534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Myin‐Germeys I, van Os J. Stress‐reactivity in psychosis: evidence for an affective pathway to psychosis. Clin Psychol Rev 2007;27:409–424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Collip D, Nicolson N, Lardinois M, Lataster T, Van Os J, Myin‐Germeys I. Daily cortisol, stress reactivity and psychotic experiences in individuals at above average genetic risk for psychosis. Psychol Med 2011;41:2305–2315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Lataster T, Wichers M, Jacobs N et al. Does reactivity to stress cosegregate with subclinical psychosis? A general population twin study. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2009;119:45–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Myin‐Germeys I, van Os J, Schwartz JE, Stone AA, Delespaul PA. Emotional reactivity to daily life stress in psychosis. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2001;58:1137–1144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Myin‐Germeys I, Peeters F, Havermans R et al. Emotional reactivity to daily life stress in psychosis and affective disorder: an experience sampling study. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2003;107:124–131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Myin‐Germeys I, Delespaul P, Van Os J. Behavioural sensitization to daily life stress in psychosis. Psychol Med 2005;35:733–741. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Palmier‐Claus JE, Dunn G, Lewis SW. Emotional and symptomatic reactivity to stress in individuals at ultra‐high risk of developing psychosis. Psychol Med 2011;42:1003–1012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Pruessner M, Cullen AE, Aas M, Walker EF. The neural diathesis‐stress model of schizophrenia revisited: An update on recent findings considering illness stage and neurobiological and methodological complexities. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2017;73:191–218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Rauschenberg C, van Os J, Cremers D, Goedhart M, Schieveld JNM, Reininghaus U. Stress sensitivity as a putative mechanism linking childhood trauma and psychopathology in youth's daily life. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2017;136:373–388. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Reininghaus U, Gayer‐Anderson C, Valmaggia L et al. Psychological processes underlying the association between childhood trauma and psychosis in daily life: an experience sampling study. Psychol Med 2016;46:2799–2813. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Glaser JP, van Os J, Portegijs PJ, Myin‐Germeys I. Childhood trauma and emotional reactivity to daily life stress in adult frequent attenders of general practitioners. J Psychosom Res 2006;61:229–236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Wichers M, Schrijvers D, Geschwind N et al. Mechanisms of gene‐environment interactions in depression: evidence that genes potentiate multiple sources of adversity. Psychol Med 2009;39:1077–1086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Lardinois M, Lataster T, Mengelers R, Van Os J, Myin‐Germeys I. Childhood trauma and increased stress sensitivity in psychosis. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2011;123:28–35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Wigman JT, Collip D, Wichers M et al. Altered transfer of momentary mental states (ATOMS) as the basic unit of psychosis liability in interaction with environment and emotions. PLoS ONE 2013;8:e54653. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. van Winkel R, Henquet C, Rosa A et al. Evidence that the COMT(Val158Met) polymorphism moderates sensitivity to stress in psychosis: an experience‐sampling study. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 2008;147B:10–17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Simons CJ, Wichers M, Derom C et al. Subtle gene‐environment interactions driving paranoia in daily life. Genes Brain Behav 2009;8:5–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Henquet C, Rosa A, Delespaul P et al. COMT Val158Met moderation of cannabis‐induced psychosis: a momentary assessment study of ‘switching on’ hallucinations in the flow of daily life. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2009;119:156–160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Iyegbe C, Campbell D, Butler A, Ajnakina O, Sham P. The emerging molecular architecture of schizophrenia, polygenic risk scores and the clinical implications for GxE research. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2014;49:169–182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Guloksuz S, Pries LK, Delespaul P et al. Examining the independent and joint effects of molecular genetic liability and environmental exposures in schizophrenia: results from the EUGEI study. World Psychiatry 2019;18:173–182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Fox E, Beevers CG. Differential sensitivity to the environment: contribution of cognitive biases and genes to psychological wellbeing. Mol Psychiatry 2016;21:1657–1662. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Insel TR. The NIMH Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) Project: precision medicine for psychiatry. Am J Psychiatry 2014;171:395–397. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Pries LK, Guloksuz S, Menne‐Lothmann C et al. White noise speech illusion and psychosis expression: An experimental investigation of psychosis liability. PLoS ONE 2017;12:e0183695. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Derom C, Thiery E, Peeters H, Vlietinck R, Defoort P, Frijns JP. The East Flanders Prospective Twin Survey (EFPTS): an actual perception. Twin Res Hum Genet 2013;16:58–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Myin‐Germeys I, Oorschot M, Collip D, Lataster J, Delespaul P, van Os J. Experience sampling research in psychopathology: opening the black box of daily life. Psychol Med 2009;39:1533–1547. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Shiffman S, Stone AA, Hufford MR. Ecological momentary assessment. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2008;4:1–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Palmier‐Claus JE, Myin‐Germeys I, Barkus E et al. Experience sampling research in individuals with mental illness: reflections and guidance. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2011;123:12–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Verhagen SJ, Hasmi L, Drukker M, van Os J, Delespaul PA. Use of the experience sampling method in the context of clinical trials. Evid Based Ment Health 2016;19:86–89. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Verhagen SJ, Berben JA, Leue C et al. Demonstrating the reliability of transdiagnostic mHealth Routine Outcome Monitoring in mental health services using experience sampling technology. PLoS ONE 2017;12:e0186294. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Henquet C, van Os J, Kuepper R et al. Psychosis reactivity to cannabis use in daily life: an experience sampling study. Br J Psychiatry 2010;196:447–453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Vaessen T, van Nierop M, Decoster J et al. Is sensitivity to daily stress predictive of onset or persistence of psychopathology? Eur Psychiatry 2017;45:167–173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Bernstein DP, Stein JA, Newcomb MD et al. Development and validation of a brief screening version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire. Child Abuse Negl 2003;27:169–190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Lecei A, Decoster J, De Hert M et al. Evidence that the association of childhood trauma with psychosis and related psychopathology is not explained by gene‐environment correlation: A monozygotic twin differences approach. Schizophr Res 2019; 205: 58–62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Purcell S, Neale B, Todd‐Brown K et al. PLINK: a tool set for whole‐genome association and population‐based linkage analyses. Am J Hum Genet. 2007;81:559–575. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Price AL, Patterson NJ, Plenge RM, Weinblatt ME, Shadick NA, Reich D. Principal components analysis corrects for stratification in genome‐wide association studies. Nat Genet 2006;38:904–909. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Das S, Forer L, Schonherr S et al. Next‐generation genotype imputation service and methods. Nat Genet 2016;48:1284–1287. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Pardinas AF, Holmans P, Pocklington AJ et al. Common schizophrenia alleles are enriched in mutation‐intolerant genes and in regions under strong background selection. Nat Genet 2018;50:381–389. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Price AL, Weale ME, Patterson N et al. Long‐range LD can confound genome scans in admixed populations. Am J Human Genet 2008;83:132–135; author reply 5–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Wray NR, Lee SH, Mehta D, Vinkhuyzen AA, Dudbridge F, Middeldorp CM. Research review: Polygenic methods and their application to psychiatric traits. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2014;55:1068–1087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Simons CJ, Wichers M, Derom C et al. Subtle gene–environment interactions driving paranoia in daily life. Genes, Brain Behavior 2009;8:5–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Snijders TAB, Bosker RJ. Multilevel analysis: an introduction to basic and advanced multilevel modeling. London: SAGE Publications Ltd; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- 47. Carlin JB, Gurrin LC, Sterne JA, Morley R, Dwyer T. Regression models for twin studies: a critical review. Int J Epidemiol 2005;34:1089–1099. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Tobin J. Estimation of relationships for limited dependent variables. Econometrica. 1958;26:24–36. [Google Scholar]
- 49. StataCorp . Stata Statistical Software. College Station, TX: StataCorp LLC; 2017. [Google Scholar]
- 50. Reininghaus U, Kempton MJ, Valmaggia L et al. Stress sensitivity, aberrant salience, and threat anticipation in early psychosis: an experience sampling study. Schizophr Bull 2016;42:712–722. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Keller MC. Gene× environment interaction studies have not properly controlled for potential confounders: the problem and the (simple) solution. Biol Psychiatry 2014;75:18–24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Guloksuz S, van Nierop M, Lieb R, van Winkel R, Wittchen H‐U, van Os J. Evidence that the presence of psychosis in non‐psychotic disorder is environment‐dependent and mediated by severity of non‐psychotic psychopathology. Psychol Med 2015;45:2389–2401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Pries LK, Guloksuz S, Ten Have M et al. Evidence that environmental and familial risks for psychosis additively impact a multidimensional subthreshold psychosis syndrome. Schizophr Bull 2018;44:710–719. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Guloksuz S, van Nierop M, Bak M et al. Exposure to environmental factors increases connectivity between symptom domains in the psychopathology network. BMC Psychiatry 2016;16:223. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Radhakrishnan R, Guloksuz S, Ten Have M et al. Interaction between environmental and familial affective risk impacts psychosis admixture in states of affective dysregulation. Psychol Med 2019;49:1879–1889. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. van Nierop M, Bak M, De Graaf R et al. The functional and clinical relevance of childhood trauma‐related admixture of affective, anxious and psychosis symptoms. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2016;133:91–101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. van Nierop M, Viechtbauer W, Gunther N et al. Childhood trauma is associated with a specific admixture of affective, anxiety, and psychosis symptoms cutting across traditional diagnostic boundaries. Psychol Med 2015;45:1277–1288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Aron EN, Aron A, Jagiellowicz J. Sensory processing sensitivity: a review in the light of the evolution of biological responsivity. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 2012;16:262–282. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Pries L‐K, Lage‐Castellanos A, Delespaul P et al. Estimating exposome score for schizophrenia using predictive modeling approach in two independent samples: the results from the EUGEI study. Schizophr Bulletin 2019;45:960–965. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Guloksuz S, Rutten BPF, Pries LK et al. The complexities of evaluating the exposome in psychiatry: a data‐driven illustration of challenges and some propositions for amendments. Schizophrenia Bull 2018;44:1175–1179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. Mistry S, Harrison JR, Smith DJ, Escott‐Price V, Zammit S. The use of polygenic risk scores to identify phenotypes associated with genetic risk of schizophrenia: systematic review. Schizophr Res 2018;197:2–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62. Hatzimanolis A, Bhatnagar P, Moes A et al. Common genetic variation and schizophrenia polygenic risk influence neurocognitive performance in young adulthood. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 2015;168:392–401. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63. Jones HJ, Stergiakouli E, Tansey KE et al. Phenotypic manifestation of genetic risk for schizophrenia during adolescence in the general population. JAMA Psychiatry 2016;73:221–228. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64. Hatzimanolis A, Avramopoulos D, Arking DE et al. Stress‐dependent association between polygenic risk for schizophrenia and schizotypal traits in young army recruits. Schizophrenia Bull 2017;44:338–347. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65. Riglin L, Collishaw S, Richards A et al. Schizophrenia risk alleles and neurodevelopmental outcomes in childhood: a population‐based cohort study. Lancet Psychiatry 2017;4:57–62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66. Nivard MG, Gage SH, Hottenga JJ et al. Genetic overlap between schizophrenia and developmental psychopathology: longitudinal and multivariate polygenic risk prediction of common psychiatric traits during development. Schizophr Bull 2017;43:1197–1207. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67. van Os J, Pries L‐K, Delespaul P et al. Replicated evidence that endophenotypic expression of schizophrenia polygenic risk is greater in healthy siblings of patients compared to controls, suggesting gene–environment interaction. The EUGEI study. Psychol Med 2019;1–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68. Power RA, Steinberg S, Bjornsdottir G et al. Polygenic risk scores for schizophrenia and bipolar disorder predict creativity. Nat Neurosci 2015;18:953. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69. Murray RM, Lewis SW. Is schizophrenia a neurodevelopmental disorder? Br Med J (Clin Res Ed) 1987;295:681. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70. Weinberger DR. The neurodevelopmental origins of schizophrenia in the penumbra of genomic medicine. World Psychiatry 2017;16:225–226. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71. Cho H, Gonzalez R, Lavaysse LM, Pence S, Fulford D, Gard DE. Do people with schizophrenia experience more negative emotion and less positive emotion in their daily lives? A meta‐analysis of experience sampling studies. Schizophr Res 2017;183:49–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72. Wichers M, Aguilera M, Kenis G et al. The catechol‐O‐methyl transferase Val158Met polymorphism and experience of reward in the flow of daily life. Neuropsychopharmacology 2008;33:3030–3036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73. Cristobal‐Narvaez P, Sheinbaum T, Myin‐Germeys I et al. The role of stress‐regulation genes in moderating the association of stress and daily‐life psychotic experiences. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2017;136:389–399. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74. Keers R, Pluess M. Childhood quality influences genetic sensitivity to environmental influences across adulthood: a life‐course gene× environment interaction study. Dev Psychopathol 2017;29:1921–1933. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75. Radhakrishnan R, Kaser M, Guloksuz S. The link between the immune system, environment, and psychosis. Schizophr Bull 2017;43:693–697. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76. Zannas A, Binder E. Gene–environment interactions at the FKBP5 locus: Sensitive periods, mechanisms and pleiotropism. Genes Brain Behav 2014;13:25–37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77. Vaessen T, Kasanova Z, Hernaus D et al. Overall cortisol, diurnal slope, and stress reactivity in psychosis: an experience sampling approach. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2018;96:61–68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78. Isvoranu AM, Borsboom D, van Os J, Guloksuz S. A Network approach to environmental impact in psychotic disorder: brief theoretical framework. Schizophr Bull 2016;42:870–873. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79. Guloksuz S, Van Os J. The slow death of the concept of schizophrenia and the painful birth of the psychosis spectrum. Psychol Med 2018;48:229–244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80. Guloksuz S, Pries L, Van Os J. Application of network methods for understanding mental disorders: pitfalls and promise. Psychol Med 2017;47:2743–2752. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81. Torous J, Onnela JP, Keshavan M. New dimensions and new tools to realize the potential of RDoC: digital phenotyping via smartphones and connected devices. Transl Psychiatry 2017;7:e1053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.