Abstract
At any given moment, we experience a perceptual scene as a single whole and yet we may distinguish a variety of objects within it. This phenomenon instantiates two properties of conscious perception: integration and differentiation. Integration is the property of experiencing a collection of objects as a unitary percept and differentiation is the property of experiencing these objects as distinct from each other. Here, we evaluated the neural information dynamics underlying integration and differentiation of perceptual contents during bistable perception. Participants listened to a sequence of tones (auditory bistable stimuli) experienced either as a single stream (perceptual integration) or as two parallel streams (perceptual differentiation) of sounds. We computed neurophysiological indices of information integration and information differentiation with electroencephalographic and intracranial recordings. When perceptual alternations were endogenously driven, the integrated percept was associated with an increase in neural information integration and a decrease in neural differentiation across frontoparietal regions, whereas the opposite pattern was observed for the differentiated percept. However, when perception was exogenously driven by a change in the sound stream (no bistability), neural oscillatory power distinguished between percepts but information measures did not. We demonstrate that perceptual integration and differentiation can be mapped to theoretically motivated neural information signatures, suggesting a direct relationship between phenomenology and neurophysiology.
Keywords: auditory streaming, bistable perception, information differentiation, information integration, phenomenal consciousness
Introduction
Phenomenologically, conscious experience does not only depend on the external information we receive from the environment but also on internal information that is independent of sensory stimulation. This dissociation between external stimulation and conscious experience is observed in several visual and auditory perceptual illusions in which two or more internally driven percepts alternate under unchanging external stimulation (Sterzer et al. 2009). Moreover, conscious experience cannot be divided into discrete independent components (i.e., it is perceptually integrated), but it can contain an assortment of events and objects (i.e., it is perceptually differentiated) (Tononi et al. 2016). Thus, while integration is the property of experiencing a collection of objects as a unitary percept, differentiation is the property of experiencing these objects as distinct from each other. What are the neural markers of the integration and differentiation of internally driven perceptual contents? We propose that integration and differentiation of internally driven percepts can be neurophysiologically investigated during auditory bistability. During the particular form of auditory bistability employed here, an invariant sequence of tones is experienced as forming either an integrated percept (one stream) or a differentiated percept (two streams) (Snyder et al. 2012; Fig. 1).
Neurophysiologically, conscious experience is thought to require the joint presence of information integration and information differentiation (Fahrenfort et al., 2012, 2017; Oizumi et al. 2014; Tononi et al. 2016). In particular, the emergence of conscious percepts is believed to involve the integration of information coming from frontal and parietal brain areas to form a phenomenologically unified whole (Dehaene and Changeux 2011; Dehaene et al. 2014; Tononi et al. 2016). Therefore, a reasonable assumption is that the integrated percept (one stream, in the case of this experiment) should be associated with correspondingly higher neural information integration (NII). Recently, NII has been empirically measured in a direct manner by computing the amount of information shared between long-distance electroencephalography (EEG) signals, and it has been used to discriminate between vegetative and minimally conscious patients (King et al. 2013; Sitt et al. 2014). This NII measure can detect nonoscillatory coupling between signals as compared to classical measures of neural oscillatory integration (NOI) such as phase synchronization. In the case of auditory bistability, we expect higher NII for the perceptually integrated (one-stream) percept compared to the perceptually differentiated (two-stream) percept, as the former would require information about tones of two different frequencies to form a single, integrated percept.
Complementary to NII, empirical indices of neural information differentiation (NID) have been used to separate levels of consciousness by estimating the degree of compressibility of EEG signals (Casali et al. 2013; Sitt et al. 2014; Schartner et al. 2015, 2017). For instance, a decrease in NID has been observed in patients in vegetative states compared to minimally conscious states (Sitt et al. 2014), showing that differentiation of neural information is associated with a cognitively more advanced state of consciousness, as clinically defined. On the other hand, the only study providing a preliminary indication that neurophysiological differentiation might be related to perceptual processes is an functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study (Boly et al. 2015), showing that NID was highest when participants watched a coherent movie, intermediate when scenes were scrambled, and minimal for “TV noise.” However, it is unclear whether neurophysiological differentiation was specifically related to conscious awareness since factors such as low-level visual processing, expectations, and top–down attention might have influenced the differences observed between conditions. During auditory bistability, we can specifically evaluate perceptual differentiation directly, since what is changing is not the stimulus itself but how it is subjectively experienced. If the neural information associated with a conscious percept is highly differentiated, NID is expected to be high since information should be less compressible. In contrast, neural differentiation is expected to be low if EEG signals are processing information in a stereotypical way because information is highly redundant (easily compressed). Following this rationale, we expect that the differentiated (two-stream) percept should be associated with higher NID.
In addition, there is ample and rapidly growing evidence that endogenous or “ongoing” brain activity in the gamma band (30–70 Hz) is neither meaningless nor random but instead carries functional information largely determining the way incoming stimuli are interpreted (Engel et al. 2001, 2013; Varela et al. 2001; Freeman 2015). For instance, studies of the visual system have shown that NOI in the gamma band is involved in the alternation between visual conscious percepts (Doesburg et al. 2005; Hipp et al. 2012). Thus, drawing upon these results and a wealth of previous research that has identified gamma band activity as relevant for conscious perception (Melloni et al. 2007; Engel et al. 2013; Levy et al. 2015), here we analyze information and oscillatory dynamics of ongoing activity in the gamma band. However, we specifically evaluate the theoretical prediction (Koch et al. 2016) that information dynamics (NII and NID), but not oscillatory dynamics (NOI), of ongoing activity underpins phenomenological integration and differentiation.
By measuring high-density scalp EEG and intracranial EEG (iEEG) in humans, we tested the hypothesis that during the formation of internally driven percepts, conscious experience goes along with neurophysiological indices of information processing. Specifically, we predicted that the perceptually integrated content would correspond to high frontoparietal NII in the gamma band, and conversely, the perceptually differentiated content would be associated with high NID within frontal and parietal regions.
Materials and Methods
Healthy Participants and Patient
Twenty-nine right-handed healthy participants (14 males; mean ± standard deviation [SD] age = 21.30 ± 2.2 years) and one left-handed epileptic patient (female; 29 years) gave written informed consent to take part in the experiment. The study was approved by the Institutional Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Psychology of Universidad Diego Portales (Chile) and the Institutional Ethics Committee of the Hospital Italiano de Buenos Aires, Argentina, in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The patient suffered from drug-resistant epilepsy from the age of 8 years and was offered surgical intervention to alleviate her intractable condition. Drug treatment at the time of implantation included oxcarbazepine 600 mg/day, topiramate 200 mg/day, and levetiracetam 750 mg/day. Computed tomography (CT) and MRI scans were acquired after insertion of depth electrodes. The patient took part in the current study 1 day before the surgery. She was attentive and cooperative during testing, and her cognitive performance before and 1 week after the implantation was indistinguishable from healthy volunteers. The patient was specifically recruited for this study because she was implanted with electrodes covering frontal and parietal regions (Table 1). Healthy participants and the epileptic patient performed the task with eyes closed in a dimly lit room.
Table 1.
Electrode | MNI coordinates | Cortical region (gyrus) |
---|---|---|
1 | −26; −44; 62 | Left superior parietal lobe |
2 | −28; −44; 62 | Left superior parietal lobe |
3 | −30; −44; 62 | White matter |
4 | −14; 32; 40 | Left middle frontal gyrus |
5 | −17; 32; 40 | Left middle frontal gyrus |
6 | −20; 32; 40 | Left middle frontal gyrus |
7 | −12; 32; 40 | White matter |
8 | −8; 32; −10 | Left orbitofrontal cortex |
9 | −11; 32; −10 | Left orbitofrontal cortex |
10 | −14; 32; −10 | Left orbitofrontal cortex |
11 | −16; 32; 10 | White matter |
12 | −26; −28; 62 | Left primary motor cortex |
13 | −13; −28; 62 | White matter |
14 | −40; −8; 4 | Left posterior insular cortex |
15 | −40; −30; 4 | White matter |
Note: Electrodes are depicted in Figure 4 and supplementary Figures 8–13.
Stimuli and Experimental Conditions
There were two experimental conditions. In the endogenous condition (bistability), we used a bistable auditory tone sequence (Carlyon et al. 2001; Gutschalk et al. 2005; Pressnitzer and Hupé 2006). Participants listened to a repeating pattern of three tones (ABA), each group of which was followed by a silence (“-“); the pattern is experienced either as a one-stream percept or as a two-stream percept (Fig. 1A). The frequency of the A tone was 587 Hz and that of the B tone was 440 Hz (5 semitones difference). The duration of each tone was 120 ms. The silence (“-“) that completed the ABA… pattern was also 120 ms long, thus making both the set of A tones and the set of B tones isochronous (Pressnitzer and Hupé 2006). Participants were instructed to press a button with one hand when perceiving that the one-stream percept had fully changed into two streams and a second button with the other hand when perceiving that the two-stream percept had fully changed into one stream (Fig. 1A, middle panel).
In the exogenous condition (control), participants listened to repeating patterns of three (ABA) or two (AB) tones, each group of which was separated by one (“-”) or two periods of silence (“- -”), respectively. In both cases, the A and B tones had the same parameters (frequencies and duration) and the silence had the same duration as in the endogenous condition (ABA- pattern). In the exogenous condition, blocks of each pattern (ABA- and AB- -) were constructed to last 4–8 s before changing to the other pattern. This suppressed the effect of endogenous bistability, such that ABA- - was most often perceived as one stream, whereas AB- - was largely perceived as two streams. As in the endogenous conditions, participants were instructed to press a button with one hand when perceiving that ABA- had fully changed into AB- - and another button with the other hand when pattern AB- - had fully changed into ABA-. Importantly, the exogenous condition allowed us to characterize the dynamics of neural activity specifically related to external changes in the stimuli (the two alternating patterns) and to contrast them with the dynamics of internal neural activity elicited by the endogenous condition (bistability). The endogenous and exogenous conditions used physically similar stimuli, with the latter sometimes having one fewer A tone. Because the analysis windows were not time-locked to the stimuli, but rather to responses (see Analysis of Ongoing Neural Activity), differences in the evoked responses to specific tones are unlikely to account for the observed pattern of results.
In order to match both conditions as closely as possible, the exogenous condition was always preceded by the endogenous condition. By designing the experiment in this manner, we were able to quickly calculate the number of both types of alternations during the endogenous conditions on a participant-per-participant basis. Based on this individual calculation, we structured the subsequent exogenous condition based on the number of switches of the endogenous condition. Similarly, the pattern of alternation (i.e., stimuli sequence) in the exogenous conditions was defined based on the first switch observed during the endogenous condition (i.e., the sequence started either with a one-stream stimulus or with a two-stream stimulus). This procedure was meant to control for differences in the number of alternations and for differences in the sequence of alternations between conditions.
EEG Recordings, Preprocessing, and Analysis
EEG signals were recorded with 128-channel HydroCel Sensors using a GES300 Electrical Geodesic amplifier at a sampling rate of 500 Hz using the NetStation software. During recording and analyses, the electrodes’ average was used as the reference electrode. Two bipolar derivations were designed to monitor vertical and horizontal ocular movements. Following Chennu et al. (2014), data from 92 channels over the scalp surface were retained for further analysis. Channels on the neck, cheeks, and forehead, which reflected more movement-related noise than signal, were excluded. Eye movement contamination and cardiac and muscle artifacts were removed from data before further processing using an independent component analysis (Delorme and Makeig 2004). All conditions yielded at least 91% of artifact-free trials. Trials (−2500 to 0 ms relative to a button press) that contained voltage fluctuations exceeding ±200 μV or transients exceeding ±100 μV were also excluded. The EEGLAB MATLAB toolbox was used for data preprocessing and pruning (Delorme and Makeig 2004). MATLAB open source software FieldTrip (Oostenveld et al. 2011) and customized scripts were used for calculating NII, NID, NOI, and neural oscillatory power (NOP) measures. No filtering was performed during the preprocessing stage.
iEEG: Recordings and Preprocessing
Stereo-electroencephalography, an iEEG recording technique in which depth electrodes are inserted in the brain of epileptic patients, was obtained from semirigid, multilead electrodes implanted in a single patient. The electrodes had a diameter of 0.8 mm and consisted of 5, 10, or 15 contact leads that were 2 mm wide and 1.5 mm apart (DIXI Medical Instruments). The electrode shafts were implanted in different regions of the frontal, temporal, central, and parietal cortices and subcortical structures. For the purposes of the current study, iEEG recordings were analyzed from the left orbitofrontal cortex (OF), left middle frontal gyrus (MFG), left superior parietal lobe (SPL), left primary motor cortex (MC), and left posterior insular cortex (PIC) (Table 1). Each electrode located in gray matter was re-referenced to an electrode of the same electrode shaft located in white matter (Li et al. 2018). Following this procedure, we preserved the limited number of electrodes in SPL (2 electrodes), OF (3 electrodes), and MFG (3 electrodes) that would have been reduced in number using a bipolar reference (N-1). Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinates of the depth electrodes were obtained from MRI and CT images using SPM (Penny et al. 2011) and MRIcron (Rorden and Brett 2000) software. The recordings were sampled at 1024 Hz and downsampled to 500 Hz for further analysis. The exact MNI coordinates and cortical regions of the selected electrodes are reported in Table 1. Open-source BrainNet Viewer software was used for visualization of selected electrodes (Xia et al. 2013).
Analysis of Ongoing Neural Activity
A classical experimental approach for studying endogenous or “ongoing” activity in the EEG related to internal fluctuations during cognitive tasks is by analyzing the EEG window before the onset of motor responses when participants report internal changes. This approach has been used for studying neural signatures of conscious awareness, such as in bistable perception (Parkkonen et al. 2008), binocular rivalry (Doesburg et al. 2005; Frässle et al. 2014), and intrusions of consciousness (Noreika et al. 2015). Here, ongoing EEG and iEEG activities (not time-locked to stimuli) preceding the onset of each response (button press), and presumed to span the change in perception, were analyzed in terms of connectivity (NII and NOI) and complexity (NII).
Window size selection was based on the following procedure. First, we calculated the mean reaction time (RT) in the exogenous condition (M = 1342 ms; SD = 101 ms). Second, in the endogenous condition, we calculated the minimum inter-switch duration (ISD) such that windows would not overlap after EEG data epoching (the minimum window size was 2500 ms; epoching from −2500 to 0 ms relative to button press). Third, as the mean RT in the exogenous condition (1342 ms) was approximately half of the window size of the endogenous condition (2500 ms), we selected a window of analysis of 2500 ms for both conditions (from −2500 to 0 ms relative to button press) (Fig. 1, lower panel). Importantly, this window included the onset of the exogenous auditory patterns (“ABA-” and “AB—”). The same procedure was repeated for the intracranial patient (RTs in the exogenous condition: M = 1380 ms, SD = 79 ms; window size in the endogenous conditions: 2500 ms).
For statistical analyses (see below), two time windows of 500 ms were selected based on the exogenous condition. A window after the change between auditory percepts (after-change [AC] window) was defined based on the mean RT at the group level (from −1342 to −842 ms). A second time window was defined at the epoch onset (before-change [BC] window; from −2500 to −2000 ms). The rationale behind the latencies of both time windows was to select a priori the periods when both externally driven percepts remained perceptually stable. Windows were not selected using a data-driven approach (see supplementary Fig. 1). The same window lengths and latencies were used for the endogenous condition (Fig. 1).
NOP: Spectral Power
In order to derive our measure of NOP, we first band-pass filtered the raw signal using a sixth-order Butterworth filter. Spectral power was estimated by calculating the square of the envelope obtained from the absolute value of the Hilbert transform after filtering. This procedure provides the instantaneous power emission that reflects the strength of local synchronization. The frequency bands were defined as follows: theta (4–7 Hz), alpha (8–12 Hz), beta (15–25 Hz), gamma (30–60 Hz), following the canonical frequency band classification (e.g., Li et al. 2018).
Phase Synchronization
We quantified phase coherence between pairs of electrodes as a measure of dynamical linear coupling among signals oscillating in the same frequency band. Phase synchronization analysis proceeds in two steps: 1) estimation of the instantaneous phases and 2) quantification of the phase locking.
Estimation of the Instantaneous Phases
To obtain the instantaneous phase φ of the neural signals, we used the Hilbert transform approach (Foster et al. 2016). The analytic signal ξ(t) of the univariate measure x(t) is a complex function of continuous time t defined as
(1) |
where the function xh(t) is the Hilbert transform of x(t):
(2) |
P.V. indicates that the integral is taken in the sense of Cauchy principal value. Sequences of digitized values give a trajectory of the tip of a vector rotating counterclockwise in the complex plane with elapsed time.
The vector norm at each digitizing step t is the state variable for instantaneous amplitude aξ(t). This amplitude corresponds to the length of the vector specified by the real and imaginary part of the complex vector computed by Pythagoras’ law and is equivalent to the magnitude of the observed oscillation at a given time and frequency point.
(3) |
The arctangent of the angle of the vector with respect to the real axis is the state variable for instantaneous phase φx(t).
(4) |
The instantaneous phase φx(t) of x(t) is taken equal to φξ(t). Identically, the phase φy(t) is estimated from y(t). This phase is thus the angle of the vector specified by the real and imaginary components. For a given time and frequency point, it corresponds to a position inside the oscillation cycle (peak, valley, rising, or falling slope).
The instantaneous phase, although defined uniquely for any kind of signal to which the Hilbert transform can be applied, is difficult to interpret physiologically for broadband signals. For this reason, a standard procedure is to consider only narrow-band phase synchronization by estimating an instantaneous phase for successive frequency bands, which are defined by band-pass filtering the time series (Le Van Quyen et al. 2001). Thus, for each trial and electrode, the instantaneous phase of the signal was extracted at each frequency of the interval 1–60 Hz (in 1-Hz steps) by computing the Hilbert transform using a zero-phase shift noncausal finite impulse filter.
NOI: Weighted Phase Lag Index
Phase synchronization is often calculated from the phase or the imaginary component of the complex cross-spectrum between the signals measured at a pair of channels. For example, the well-known Phase Locking Value (Lachaux et al. 1999) is obtained by averaging the exponential magnitude of the imaginary component of the cross-spectrum. However, such phase coherence indices derived from EEG data are affected by the problem of volume conduction, and as such they can have a single dipolar source, rather than a pair of distinct interacting sources, producing spurious coherence between spatially disparate EEG channels. The Phase Lag Index (PLI), first proposed by Stam et al. (2007), attempts to minimize the impact of volume conduction and common sources inherent in EEG data, by averaging the signs of phase differences, thereby ignoring average phase differences of 0 or 180°. This is based on the rationale that such phase differences are likely to be generated by volume conduction of single dipolar sources. However, despite being insensitive to volume conduction, PLI has a strong discontinuity in the measure, which causes it to be maximally sensitive to noise.
The Weighted Phase Lag Index (wPLI) (Vinck et al. 2011) addresses this problem by weighting the signs of the imaginary components by their absolute magnitudes. Furthermore, as the calculation of wPLI also normalizes the weighted sum of signs of the imaginary components by the average of their absolute magnitudes, it represents a dimensionless measure of connectivity that is not directly influenced by differences in spectral or cross-spectral power. For these reasons, we employed the wPLI measure to estimate connectivity in our data. The wPLI index ranges from 0 to 1, with value 1 indicating perfect synchronization (phase difference is perfectly constant throughout the trials) and value 0 representing total absence of synchrony (phase differences are random). For each trial and pair of electrodes, wPLI was estimated using a 500-ms sliding window with 2-ms time step, that is, with a 96% overlap between two adjacent windows.
NII: Weighted Symbolic Mutual Information
In order to quantify the coupling of information flow between electrodes, we computed the weighted symbolic mutual information (wSMI) (King et al. 2013; Sitt et al. 2014). This measure assesses the extent to which two signals present joint nonrandom fluctuations, suggesting that they share information. wSMI has three main advantages: 1) it allows for a rapid and robust estimation of the signals’ entropies; 2) it provides an efficient way to detect nonlinear coupling; and 3) it discards spurious correlations between signals arising from common sources, favoring nontrivial pairs of symbols. For each trial, wSMI is calculated between each pair of electrodes after the transformation of the EEG and iEEG signals into a sequence of discrete symbols defined by the ordering of k time samples separated by a temporal separation τ (King et al. 2013). The symbolic transformation depends on a fixed symbol size (k = 3, i.e., 3 samples represent a symbol) and a variable τ between samples (temporal distance between samples), which determines the frequency range in which wSMI is estimated (Sitt et al. 2014). In our case, we chose τ = 32 and 6 ms to isolate wSMI in alpha (wSMIα) and gamma (wSMIγ) bands, respectively. The frequency specificity f of wSMI is related to k and τ as follows:
As per the above formula, with a kernel size k of 3, τ values of 32 and 6 ms produced a sensitivity to frequencies below and near to 55 Hz (gamma range) and 10 Hz (alpha range), respectively, with these two frequency values used as low-pass filters in order to avoid aliasing artifacts (King et al. 2013)
wSMI was estimated for each pair of transformed EEG and iEEG signals by calculating the joint probability of each pair of symbols. The joint probability matrix was multiplied by binary weights to reduce spurious correlations between signals, as confirmed by simulations and empirical data in the original wSMI publication (King et al. 2013). The weights were set to zero for pairs of identical symbols, which could be elicited by a unique common source, and for opposite symbols, which could reflect the two sides of a single electric dipole. wSMI is calculated using the following formula:
where x and y are all symbols present in signals X and Y, respectively, w(x,y) is the weight matrix and p(x,y) is the joint probability of co-occurrence of symbol x in signal X and symbol y in signal Y. Finally, p(x) and p(y) are the probabilities of those symbols in each signal and k is the number of symbols—used to normalize the mutual information (MI) by the signal’s maximal entropy. Temporal evolution of wSMI was calculated using a 500-ms sliding window with 2-ms time step, that is, with a 96% overlap between two adjacent windows.
In order to further address possible volume conduction issues in our scalp EEG analyses, we compared wSMI with the nonweighted versions of the metric (symbolic mutual information; SMI), computing both metrics as a function of Euclidean inter-electrode distance (supplementary Fig. 7). For distances below 5 cm, wSMI quickly dropped toward zero, as expected given that the weighted version of this measure was designed to eliminate common source artifacts and to provide higher spatial selectivity (King et al. 2013; supplementary Fig. 7, upper row). In contrast, the unweighted version of the metric (SMI) exhibited the highest values below 5 cm, typically produced by common source artifacts (supplementary Fig. 7, lower row). These analyses further confirm that the differences observed in our data are most likely due to a nonspurious spatial relationship between electrode pairs.
NID: Kolmogorov–Chaitin Complexity
Kolmogorov–Chaitin complexity (K complexity) quantifies the algorithmic complexity (Kolmogorov 1965; Chaitin 1974) of an EEG signal by measuring its degree of redundancy (Sitt et al. 2014; Schartner et al. 2015, 2017). Algorithmic complexity of a given EEG sequence can be described as the length of shortest computer program that can generate it. A short program corresponds to a less complex sequence. K complexity was estimated by quantifying the compression size of the EEG using the Lempel–Ziv zip algorithm (Lempel and Ziv 1976).
Algorithmic information theory was introduced by Andreï Kolmogorov and Gregory Chaitin as an area of interaction between computer science and information theory. The concept of algorithmic complexity or Kolmogorov–Chaitin complexity (K complexity) is defined as the shortest description of a string (or in our case a time series X). That is to say, K complexity is the size of the smallest algorithm (or computer program) that can produce that particular time series. However, it can be demonstrated by reductio ad absurdum that there is no possible algorithm that can measure K complexity (Chaitin 1995). To sidestep this issue, we can estimate an upper-bound value of K complexity(X). This can be concretely accomplished by applying a lossless compression of the time series and quantifying the compression size. Capitalizing on the vast signal compression literature, we heuristically used a classical open-source compressor gzip (Salomon 2004) to estimate K complexity (X). It is important to standardize the method of representation of the signal before compression in order to avoid nonrelevant differences in complexity. Specifically, to compute K complexity (X):
The time series were transformed into sequences of symbols. Each symbol represents, with identical complexity, the amplitude of the corresponding channel for each time point. The number of symbols was set to 32, and each one corresponds to dividing the amplitude range of that given channel into 32 equivalent bins. Similar results have been obtained with binning ranging from 8 to 128 bins (Sitt et al. 2014).
The time series were compressed using the compressLib library for Matlab; this library implements the gzip algorithm to compress Matlab variables.
K complexity(X) was calculated as the size of the compressed variable with time series divided by the size of the original variable before compression. Our premise is that the larger the size of the compressed string, the more complex the structure of the time series, thus potentially indexing the complexity of the electrical activity recorded at a sensor.
For each trial and channel, K complexity was estimated using a 500-ms sliding window with 2-ms time step, that is, with a 96% overlap between two adjacent windows.
EEG Electrode Cluster Analysis and Epoch Correction
For the hypothesis-driven analyses, canonical bilateral frontal (n = 26), parietal (n = 18), and temporal (n = 12) electrode clusters were selected for spectral power, complexity (K-complexity), and connectivity analysis (wPLI and wSMI) (supplementary Fig. 14). In the case of spectral power and complexity analysis, values within frontal and parietal electrode clusters were averaged per condition and participant. In the case of frontoparietal wPLI and wSMI, we calculated the mean connectivity that every electrode in the frontal cluster shared with every electrode in the parietal cluster. To evaluate long-distance interactions between frontal and parietal electrodes, values between pairs of frontal electrodes and between pairs of parietal electrodes were discarded. Similarly, in the case of temporotemporal connectivity analyses, we calculated the mean connectivity that every electrode in the right-temporal cluster shared with every electrode in the left-temporal cluster, discarding connectivity values within pairs of right-temporal electrodes and pairs of left-temporal electrodes. This procedure allowed us to specifically test the role of long-distance interactions (frontoparietal and temporotemporal) during the endogenous and exogenous conditions. Spectral power, K complexity, and wSMI and wPLI values of the corresponding regions of interest were averaged per condition and participant. For the exploratory analysis, connectivity between pairs of frontal and pairs of parietal electrodes were analyzed separately. In a similar manner as for the hypothesis-driven analysis, connectivity (wSMI) between frontotemporal and temporoparietal electrodes was analyzed, and within-cluster pairs were excluded.
Since during the endogenous condition, there was no true baseline period because we do not know precisely when the spontaneous changes were initiated; after transforming data into complexity (K complexity), connectivity (wPLI and wSMI), and spectral power time series and creating the corresponding electrode clusters, we subtracted the mean values between −2500 and −700 ms from each data point per epoch and condition. Motor-related activity in the gamma band has been reported approximately −200 ms before the button press during auditory bistable perception (Basirat et al. 2008). Thus, although it is common to analyze response-evoked activity by normalizing epochs using the mean of the entire window including the time of the motor response (Doesburg et al. 2005; Fesi and Mendola 2015; De Jong et al. 2016), we used a more conservative approach by baseline correcting each epoch from −2500 to −700 ms relative to the button press in order to avoid possible contamination due to motor-related artifacts. In order to make both conditions comparable, we performed the same procedure separately on the endogenous and exogenous conditions.
Statistical Analysis
For statistical analysis of wSMI (NII), K complexity (NID), wPLI (NOI), and spectral power (NOD) on the scalp EEG (29 participants) and iEEG data (1 patient), we performed repeated-measures analysis of variance (RANOVA) using 3 within-participant factors: condition (endogenous, exogenous), window (BC, AC), and direction (one- to two-stream, two- to one-stream). Bonferroni correction was computed for post hoc comparisons, and Bayes factors (Bfs) of the null and alternative hypothesis are reported (Masson 2011; Jarosz and Wiley 2014). Statistical analyses were performed using Statistical Product and Service Solutions (SPSS, version 20.0, IBM), MATLAB 2018b, and open-source statistical software JASP (JASP Team 2017, version 0.8.1.1).
Results
Twenty-nine healthy participants and one patient implanted with intracranial electrodes listened to a repeating pattern of three tones followed by a temporal gap. This flow of sounds is experienced either as a one-stream percept (perceptual integration) or as a two-stream percept (perceptual differentiation) (Fig. 1A, upper panel). Perception tends to alternate between these alternatives every few seconds (Pressnitzer and Hupé 2006). Participants were asked to press a button as they perceived that the one-stream percept had fully changed into the two-stream percept, and a second button when perceiving the two-stream percept had fully changed into the one-stream percept (Fig. 1A, middle panel). As an experimental control task, we used a condition where the stimuli were physically manipulated (varying the length of the silence between tones) in order to generate two externally driven alternating percepts (exogenous condition). Participants had to perform the same task as in endogenous condition. This control allowed us to establish the extent to which neural activity in the endogenous condition was specific to internally driven perceptual switches. It also allowed us to measure typical RTs to percept changes with known onset times (in the control task), in order to determine suitable time windows for analysis. Furthermore, the exogenous condition allowed us to control for top–down attention in the absence of bistability by creating externally driven switches between percepts as opposed to the internally driven ones (induced endogenously).
Behavioral Results
In the endogenous condition, ISD distributions were approximated well with a gamma distribution (P < 0.001; Fig. 1B), typically reported during bistable perception dynamics (e.g., Parkkonen et al. 2008). ISD histograms exhibited similar distributions for the one- to two-stream percept and the two- to one-stream percept, with an average dominance duration of 3.75 ± 0.0412 and 3.78 ± 0.0419 s, respectively (Fig. 1B). No significant differences were observed between distributions (t1,28 = 0.204, P = 0.910, Bf in favor of the null = 46.19), suggesting that both directions of perceptual change exhibit similar cognitive demand during internally driven perception.
Similarly, in the case of the exogenous condition, discrimination of the two auditory sequences showed behavioral equivalence, as no differences were found in RTs between the one- to two-stream percept and the two- to one-stream percept (t1,28 = 0.204, P = 0.840, Bf in favor of the null = 4.97).
NII during the Endogenous and Exogenous Conditions
We first investigated the dynamics of NII in the gamma range (NIIγ). We compared activity during a 500-ms window before a perceptual change with activity after the change (Fig. 1A, lower panel; see Materials and Methods). This approach contrasts with many previous studies of auditory bistability (Gutschalk et al. 2005; Hill et al. 2011; Szalardy et al. 2013; Billig et al. 2018; Sanders et al. 2018), where the analysis windows are time-locked to the stimulus and allowed us to concentrate on both stable and transition periods of bistable experience. Response-locked analyses focused on how “ongoing” neural activity relates to perception. A RANOVA (see Materials and Methods) revealed a significant triple interaction between condition (endogenous, exogenous), window (BC, AC), and direction (one- to two-stream, two- to one-stream) for NIIγ (F1,28 = 5.73, P = 0.024, = 0.16, Bf in favor of the alternative = 2.73). Bonferroni’s post hoc test revealed higher NIIγ in the one-stream compared to the two-stream percept in the BC window (P = 0.009, = 0.21, Bf in favor of the alternative = 6.42; Fig. 2A,C). Interestingly, ~1 s later in the AC window, NIIγ again showed higher values for the one-stream than the two-stream percept (P = 0.026, = 0.15, Bf in favor of the alternative = 2.49) (Fig. 2A,C). These findings suggest that the phenomenologically integrated percept consistently involved a higher level of gamma neurophysiological integration than the phenomenologically differentiated one.
Interestingly, while NIIγ discriminated between conscious percepts during the endogenous condition (Fig. 2A), it did not do so in the exogenous condition (BC: P = 0.561, Bf in favor of the null = 5.12 and AC: P = 0.349, Bf in favor of the null = 3.26; Bonferroni-corrected post hoc tests) (Fig. 2B), suggesting that frontoparietal NIIγ may be specifically indexing endogenously generated percepts. Furthermore, no differences were observed in the mean level of NIIγ between endogenous and exogenous conditions (main effect of condition: P = 0.248, Bf in favor of the null = 2.12) or between windows (main effect of window: P = 0.918, Bf in favor of the null = 3.59), indicating that the overall amount of information sharing within the frontoparietal network was similar across conditions. This index of information integration was hence sensitive to endogenously driven perceptual changes and may specifically underlie the formation of conscious auditory percepts.
In order to establish the specific role of frontoparietal signals, we computed inter-hemispheric NIIγ between temporal electrodes. RANOVA found no reliable triple interaction in temporotemporal NIIγ (F1,28 = 1.51, P = 0.228, Bf in favor of the null = 2.02) (Fig. 2D,E), implying relatively specific involvement of frontoparietal networks in the emergence of endogenous auditory percepts. Finally, and in addition to NIIγ, we investigated whether NII in the alpha band (NIIα) dissociates between auditory percepts, as alpha activity has been previously related to perceptual bistability (Flevaris et al. 2013; Handel and Jensen 2014). However, the ability of frontoparietal NII to track and distinguish between different endogenous percepts seems to be specifically related to the gamma range since RANOVA revealed no triple interaction between percepts in frontoparietal NIIα (F1,28 = 1.01, P = 0.321, Bf in favor of the null = 2.48) (Fig. 2F).
Finally, we performed post hoc exploratory analysis in order to further investigate NIIγ differences between other electrode regions (i.e., frontofrontal, parietoparietal, temporoparietal, and frontotemporal pairs of electrodes). RANOVA revealed no triple interaction (condition × window × direction) between these electrode clusters (supplementary Fig. 2). Similarly, we explored differences between these pairs of electrodes in other canonical frequency bands (theta: NIIθ, alpha: NIIα, and beta: NIIβ). RANOVA revealed no significant triple interaction (condition × window × direction) for the same groups of electrodes for theta (NIIθ: supplementary Fig. 3), alpha (NIIα: supplementary Fig. 4), and beta bands (NIIβ: supplementary Fig. 5). These exploratory results further support the specific role of NIIγ in discriminating between internally driven conscious percepts.
NID during the Endogenous and Exogenous Conditions
We next investigated the dynamics of NID within frontal and parietal electrodes during bistable perception (Fig. 3). The RANOVA revealed a significant triple interaction between condition (endogenous, exogenous), window (BC, AC), and direction (one- to two-stream, two- to one-stream) for NID (P = 0.013, = 0.19, Bf in favor of the alternative = 4.57). Bonferroni’s post hoc test revealed higher NID in the two- than in the one-stream percept in the BC window (P = 0.011, = 0.20, Bf in favor of the alternative = 5.41; Fig. 3A) and a similar pattern in the AC window, showing higher values for two streams than for one stream (P = 0.016, Bf in favor of the alternative = 3.88, = 0.19) (Fig. 3A). These results show that the differentiated percept exhibits higher neurophysiological differentiation than the integrated one.
As was the case for NIIγ, post hoc comparisons showed no NID dependence on percept in the exogenous condition (BC: P = 0.366, Bf in favor of the null = 2.66; AC: P = 0.174, Bf in favor of the null = 1.68) (Fig. 3B). Furthermore, no differences were observed in the mean level of NID between endogenous and exogenous conditions (main effect of condition: F1,28 = 1.39, P = 0.248, Bf in favor of the null = 2.12) or between windows (main effect of window: F1,28 = 0.15, P = 0.228, Bf in favor of the null = 3.53), indicating that the overall information differentiation within the frontoparietal network was similar across conditions. Finally, no triple interaction (condition × window × direction) was observed for NID between temporal electrodes (right and left hemispheres; F1,28 = 3.59, P = 0.069, Bf in favor of the null = 0.86) (Fig. 3C,D). In agreement with the NII results, this index of information complexity dissociated endogenous percepts. However, NID showed the opposite pattern compared to NII in terms of the direction of the effects between one and two streams, suggesting that NID is capturing a different but complementary aspect of neural information dynamics associated with conscious percepts.
Consistent with the analysis of NII, we explored NID differences between other electrode regions (i.e., frontofrontal, parietoparietal, temporoparietal, and frontotemporal pairs of electrodes). RANOVA revealed no triple interaction (condition × window × direction) between these electrode clusters (supplementary Fig. 6).
NII in Human Intracortical Recordings
In order to validate these findings in a similar manner as elsewhere (Canales-Johnson et al. 2015), we repeated the experiment in a patient implanted with intracranial electrodes for epilepsy surgery. We benefited from the high spatial resolution of intracranial recordings, allowing us to directly test the hypothesis that it is information sharing specifically between frontal and parietal areas that differentiates between the two auditory percepts. We computed NIIγ on iEEG recordings between the SPL and MFG and between SPL and the OF (Table 1) obtained from the intracranial patient performing the same task as above (Fig. 4A–C; supplementary Fig. 8). Taking the SPL-MFG and SPL-OF pairs of electrodes together, and as for the healthy participants, the RANOVA showed a triple interaction between condition (endogenous, exogenous), window (BC, AC), and direction (one- to two-stream, two- to one-stream) for NIIγ (F1,56 = 45.19, P < 0.001, = 0.42, Bf in favor of the alternative > 100). Simple effects within the BC window showed higher NIIγ for the one- than for the two-stream percept in the endogenous (F1,56 = 44.79, P < 0.001, = 0.25, Bf in favor of the alternative > 100; Fig. 4B) but not in the exogenous condition (F1,56 = 0.25, P = 0.517, Bf supporting the null = 4.34; Fig. 4C). In the case of the AC window, the one-stream percept again showed higher NIIγ than the two-stream percept in the endogenous (F1,56 = 42.83, P < 0.001, = 0.10, Bf supporting the alternative = 5.10; Fig. 4B) but not in the exogenous condition (F1,56 = 1.40, P = 0.731, Bf in favor of the null = 4.89; Fig. 4C).
In order to further test the frontoparietal specificity of the NIIγ modulation, we computed NIIγ between SPL and MFG with two unrelated—as per our hypothesis—cortical areas: MC and PIC (supplementary Fig. 9). No triple interaction was observed for NIIγ between SPL-MC, SPL-PIC, MFG-MC nor MFG-PIC, further confirming the spatial specificity of the intracortical NIIγ results.
Furthermore, no differences were found between these percepts in the same iEEG recordings within the alpha band (NIIα) (F1,56 = 0.03, P = 0.853, Bf in favor of the null = 7.24; Fig. 4F,G). Individual SPL-MFG and SPL-OF pairs for NIIγ and NIIα are depicted in supplementary Figs 8 and 10, respectively. Finally, and in agreement with the scalp EEG results, a further iEEG exploratory analysis revealed no triple interaction (RANOVA: condition × window × direction) between SPL-MFG and SPL-OF electrodes in the theta (NIIθ: supplementary Fig. 11) nor beta (NIIβ: supplementary Fig. 12) bands.
NID in Human Intracortical Recordings
Next, we investigated NID dynamics on iEEG signals in the intracranial patient within SPL, MFG, and OF (Fig. 4D, E). Again, RANOVA showed a triple interaction between (endogenous, exogenous), window (BC, AC), and direction (one- to two-stream, two- to one-stream) (F1,56 = 24.96, P < 0.001, = 0.25, Bf in favor of the alternative = 10.02). In agreement with the scalp EEG results, a simple effects analysis within the BC window showed higher NID for the phenomenologically differentiated percept than for the phenomenologically integrated percept in the endogenous (F1,56 = 19.08, P < 0.001, = 0.42, Bf in favor of the alternative > 100; Fig. 4D) but not in the exogenous condition (F1,56 = 1.92, P = 0.171, Bf in favor of the null = 1.94; Fig. 4E). In the case of the AC window, the two-stream percept again showed higher NID than the one-stream percept in the endogenous (F1,56 = 10.57, P = 0.003, = 0.28, Bf in favor of the alternative = 20.15; Fig. 4D) but not in the control (exogenous) condition (F1,56 = 0.25, P = 0.615, Bf in favor of the null = 4.82; Fig. 4E). These findings demonstrate strong convergent evidence between scalp EEG and direct cortical recordings, further supporting the hypothesis that phenomenology goes along with neurophysiology of conscious percepts, specifically indexed by frontoparietal ongoing activity. Individual SPL-MFG and SPL-OF pairs for NID are depicted in supplementary Fig. 13.
NOI in the Endogenous and Exogenous Conditions
We next evaluated the theoretical prediction that information dynamics, but not oscillatory dynamics, of brain activity underpins the emergence of internally driven percepts (Koch et al. 2016). Thus, we investigated whether NOI of ongoing activity might also capture the dynamics of auditory bistability. Specifically, we investigated whether frontoparietal gamma phase synchronization (Weighted Phase Lag Index [wPLIγ]) could differentiate between endogenous percepts. Unlike for NIIγ, RANOVA revealed no triple interaction between condition (endogenous, exogenous), window (BC, AC), and direction (one- to two-stream, two- to one-stream) for NOIγ (F(1,28) = 0.12, P = 0.726, Bf in favor of the null = 3.58) (Fig. 5A,B). However, a weak interaction between condition (endogenous, exogenous) and window (BC, AC) was found (F(1,28) = 4.22, P = 0.049, Cohen’s d = 0.77, Bf in favor of the alternative = 1.50; Fig. 5C,D). Bonferroni’s post hoc test showed that NOIγ significantly decreased in the AC window compared to the BC window in the endogenous (P = 0.016, = 0.17, Bf in favor of the alternative = 3.73; Fig. 5C) but probably not in the exogenous condition (P = 0.236, Bf in favor of the null = 2.04; Fig. 5D).
Furthermore, the same null result for NOIγ between directions was observed in the intracranial patient (RANOVA [condition × window × direction]: F(1,56) = 0.12; P = 0.726, Bf in favor of the null = 4.73) (Fig. 5E,F). As with the scalp EEG, the intracranial patient showed an interaction between condition and window (F(1,56) = 13.51, P = 0.001, = 0.19, Bf in favor of the alternative = 68.09) in NOIγ, showing a decrease in phase synchrony in the AC window compared to the BC window only in the endogenous condition (Bonferroni’s post hoc test in endogenous condition: P = 0.001, = 0.18, Bf in favor of the alternative = 53.48 and in exogenous condition: P = 0.147, Bf in favor of the null = 1.92; Fig. 5G,H) between SPL and MFG-OF electrodes. These findings suggest that oscillatory integration does not index the identity of auditory percepts but may relate to percept stability: phase synchrony maxima occur in the BC window (before the end of a stable percept) and phase synchrony minima in the AC window (just at the onset of percept stability).
NOP in the Endogenous and Exogenous Conditions
We finally performed an exploratory analysis aiming at investigating the neural differences during exogenously driven perception. In order to evaluate the neural response on electrodes sensitive to auditory perturbations, we analyzed the neural oscillatory power (NOP) in the same gamma band as previous analyses but focusing on the temporal and parietal electrodes. In the exogenous condition, we expected higher gamma spectral power during the integrated (ABA -) as compared to the differentiated (AB - -) pattern since the former contains one more tone than the latter, potentially eliciting a greater neural response. RANOVA revealed an interaction between condition (endogenous, exogenous), window (BC, AC), and direction (one- to two-stream, two-to one-stream) for NOP (F(1,28) = 4.30, P = 0.047, = 0.13, Bf in favor of the alternative = 4.67; Fig. 6). Bonferroni’s post hoc test revealed differences in the exogenous condition in line with our prediction. NOP was higher in the one- than in the two-stream percept in the BC window (F(1,28) = 6.37, P = 0.001, = 0.032, Bf in favor of the alternative = 6.45; Fig. 6) and a similar pattern held in the AC window, with larger values for one stream than for two streams (P = 0.001, Bf in favor of the alternative = 7.88, = 0.002; Fig. 6B). No differences in NOP were observed between conscious percepts in the endogenous condition (BC: P = 0.845, Bf in favor of the null = 8.13; AC: P = 0.453, Bf in favor of the null = 6.17) (Fig. 6A). These results suggest that spectral power within temporal and parietal electrodes is sufficient for indexing conscious percepts when these are externally driven, that is, when the two patterns (i.e., integrated and differentiated) were physically present in the auditory stimuli.
Discussion
Here, we demonstrate that frontoparietal information dynamics dissociate alternative conscious percepts during internally but not externally driven percepts. By studying the neural dynamics (EEG and direct cortical recordings) of theoretically motivated information metrics, we show that empirically tractable measures of NII and NID map auditory percepts experienced either as perceptually integrated (one-stream) or differentiated (two-stream), respectively. Furthermore, phase synchronization of oscillatory gamma activity in the frontoparietal network does not differentiate between auditory percepts nor does the information between temporal networks. Finally, when perception was driven externally (no bistability, control condition) by a change in the sound stream, NOP distinguished between percepts but information measures did not.
Neural Correlates of Consciousness and the Instantiation of Meaningful Signatures of Information Integration and Differentiation
Our results expand the understanding of the neural correlates of consciousness (Koch et al. 2016) in several ways. First, our experimental findings directly support information-based theories of consciousness (Dehaene and Changeux 2011; Dehaene et al. 2014; Tononi et al. 2016); in their current instantiation, the Integrated Information Theory (IIT) (Tononi et al. 2016) and the Global Neuronal Workspace Theory (GNWT) (Dehaene and Changeux 2011) of consciousness both emphasize the role of information exchange in generating conscious percepts. Although both theories conceptualize information differently, their proposed empirical indices are based on the classical Shannon-entropy information framework. Using these measures of information dynamics, our results show convergent evidence supporting both GNWT and IIT predictions by demonstrating a role of neural information in the emergence of contents of consciousness. However, the interpretation of these results in light of the IIT and GNWT is somewhat different.
The GNWT provides a framework for evaluating the process of becoming aware of a percept (i.e., the conscious access for a stimulus), specifically in our case the experience of perceiving one or two auditory streams. Of the proposed key features of neural processing that the GNWT supports is the modulation of frontoparietal networks when gaining conscious awareness. This is why we set out to test the frontoparietal network at the electrode level (EEG) and the intracranial cortical level (iEEG). In its original conception, the GNWT did not specifically proposed integration measures that would be neural signatures of gain of conscious awareness. Later on, however, in a landmark paper, Gaillard et al. (2009) showed connectivity and granger causality calculations that signaled the access to consciousness in a masking paradigm in intracranial patients. Echoing the need to better define what a putative measure of brain integration is, we searched for those measures that could be used to assess integration information and differentiation in the time dynamics of the stream of consciousness of the current task.
Although most of the IIT’s empirical work has been focused on finding proxy measures for distinguishing the levels of consciousness (e.g., wakefulness versus sleep), recent efforts have been put in investigating perceptual integration and differentiation at the first-person, phenomenological level using time-resolved experimental paradigms (Boly et al. 2015). We think that this current framework allows for testing the concepts of integration and differentiation in the context of the study of the first-person, content of consciousness (i.e., hearing one-stream vs. two-stream stimuli) rather than on the study of the third-person, state of consciousness (e.g., being awake vs. being asleep). We believe our study substantially improves this approach for three main reasons. First, the repertoire of conscious contents is controlled (i.e., one vs. two auditory streams). Second, the physical stimulation remains constant while perception does not. Finally, our EEG/iEEG information metrics possess higher temporal resolution than those used in fMRI studies, allowing the finely grained characterization of the phenomenology of conscious contents.
It is in this respect that we think that concepts taken from both theoretical frameworks (GNWT and IIT) are useful to explain some cognitive processes that we believe are at play in bistable perception. We do not claim that this unifies the theories, but that it is possible to make use of parts of them to frame results that can help understanding the neural signatures of contents of consciousness. An important caveat of this conceptualization is that the theoretical argumentations from GNWT and IIT do not make explicit predictions about the possible differences between internally and externally driven contents of consciousness, while we explicitly test this difference.
Our results suggest a differential role of information integration versus information differentiation in the emergence of conscious percepts from those proposed before. According to IIT, the neural activity associated with conscious percepts should reflect the joint presence of neurophysiological integration and neurophysiological differentiation. Under this theoretical framework, integration is expected—in principle—to be paralleled by differentiation of neural activity. Contrary to this prediction, our results show dissociation between neurophysiological integration and neurophysiological differentiation of frontoparietal ongoing activity. Interestingly, while the phenomenologically integrated percept (one-stream) showed a relative increase in NII and relative decrease in NID, the perceptually differentiated percept (two-stream) exhibited the opposite pattern, that is, a decrease in NII and increase in NID. Together, these dissociated patterns suggest that each measure is instead directly associated with the phenomenology of internally driven conscious percepts: whereas information integration of neural activity is capturing phenomenological integration (one-stream percept), information differentiation may be capturing phenomenological differentiation (two-stream percept).
Neural Signatures of the Emergence of Internally and Externally Driven Conscious Contents
We have demonstrated a potential mechanistic role of information integration and differentiation in the formation of endogenous percepts. Why do information metrics capture the neural dynamics of internally-driven conscious percepts? Coordination in the brain has been classically studied by computing phase synchronization between neural oscillations (Uhlhaas et al. 2009; Engel et al. 2013). Synchronization is a highly ordered form of neural coordination that primarily captures the linear (or proportional) phase relationship between signals at specific frequencies (phase-locking). Thus, a mechanism of coordination-by-synchrony captures only certain regimes of neural coordination that are periodic. However, brain dynamics exhibit both a tendency to integrate information (synchronization) and a tendency for the components to differentiate information (independent function) (Dehaene and Changeux 2011; Tognoli and Kelso 2014; Tononi et al. 2016). During auditory bistability, conscious percepts typically alternate continuously without becoming locked into any one percept for long periods. We propose that underlying this dynamical process are ensembles of neurons that are repeatedly assembled and disassembled and that this nontrivial dynamic might be instantiated by a mechanism of coding-by-information that captures complex, nonlinear patterns of neural activity and not merely simple proportional associations between neural signals. In fact, we have recently shown that wSMI (NII) performs better at capturing highly nonlinear interactions than wPLI (NOI) between long-distance neural signals by using realistic brain simulations (Imperatori et al. 2019).
Contrasting the emergence of internally driven conscious percepts, the two auditory sequences presented during the exogenous conditions were distinguished by a difference in spectral power in the gamma band. The difference was observed mainly in the temporoparietal electrodes, and the direction of the effect is consistent with the idea that an extra tone in the sequence elicited a higher neural response, that is, that the ABA- sequence showed higher gamma power than the AB- - sequence. Thus, contrary to the information theory metrics, we suggest that the spectral gamma power response is linearly (or proportionally) related to the physical characteristics of the stimulus itself (exogenous condition) and not to the internal interpretation of the bistable sequence, as no differences were observed in the endogenous condition. Hence for the exogenous condition, while the report is the same (one or two streams), the neural signatures point a different process in the brain when the nature of the contents differs.
Conscious Auditory Percepts and Ongoing Neural Activity
The frontoparietal patterns of neural information are a manifestation of the interaction between the external stimulation and endogenous, ongoing brain activity, as opposed to activation purely imposed by the auditory stimuli. Thus, NII and NID patterns do not merely reflect stimulus-driven neural activity but rather the intrinsic coordination of endogenous frontoparietal neural activity. Indeed, in the endogenous condition of our study, internally generated changes in neural activity were associated with changes in conscious percepts in the complete absence of any change in the auditory stimuli. In line with our results, recent studies in the visual system have shown that long-distance integration of ongoing oscillations reflects internally coordinated activity associated with conscious perception (Hipp et al. 2012; Engel et al. 2013; Helfrich et al. 2016). Here, by directly measuring the amount of information integration and information differentiation contained in the ongoing neural activity, we demonstrate a functional role of information dynamics in the emergence of auditory conscious percepts. Our approach provides a framework for examining whether this relationship between neural and perceptual integration and differentiation generalizes, for example, to the perception of ambiguous stimuli in other sensory modalities or across modalities.
Although the pioneering electrophysiological studies supporting the active role of ongoing activity in perception and cognition date from the 1970s (Freeman 1976, 2000), over the last decade ongoing brain activity has been mainly studied in the context of “resting state networks” (Fox and Raichle 2007). In these recent studies, fluctuations in ongoing activity between spatially segregated networks (brain regions) are correlated when a participant is not performing an explicitly defined task. In the present study, by taking advantage of the high temporal resolution of EEG and direct cortical recordings, we show that patterns of neural information are transiently coordinated during the active discrimination of internally generated auditory percepts. Furthermore, our results also allowed us to differentiate the contribution of information in frequency space, showing that gamma but not alpha NII differentiates auditory percepts during bistable perception.
Auditory Bistable Perception and Frontoparietal Activity
Our results also suggest a fruitful new approach to conceptualizing and investigating bistable auditory perception. We demonstrate that the dynamics of auditory endogenous percepts can be associated with long-distance coordination of neural activity. For the ABA- patterns presented here, while both one-stream and two-stream percepts depend on the integration of tones over time, the latter also requires integration across tone frequency. We argue that this additional integration may draw on, or be reflected in, an increase in information sharing in ongoing frontoparietal neural activity. Additionally, the maintenance of two distinct perceptual streams of sounds was associated with more differentiated neural patterns than when a single stream was perceived. How these findings relate to previous demonstrations of greater stimulus-locked activity in auditory cortex (Gutschalk et al. 2005; Hill et al. 2011; Szalardy et al. 2013; Billig et al. 2018; Sanders et al. 2018) and greater blood oxygen level–dependent (BOLD) response in intraparietal sulcus (Cusack 2005; Hill et al. 2011) for two streams is presently unclear. However, the involvement of neural circuits extending beyond sensory regions in parsing the auditory environment is not surprising, given that attention switching (Billig and Carlyon 2016), linguistic knowledge (Billig et al. 2013), and predictability (Winkler and Schröger 2015) have been shown to affect auditory streaming.
These findings provide convergent evidence about the role of frontoparietal networks in the dynamics of formation and maintenance of internally driven conscious contents. Research in visual bistability has focused on characterizing content-related activity predominantly in local brain areas or networks (Sterzer et al. 2009). Of those few studies that have expanded their scope to associative cortices and wider networks, one has recently proposed mechanistic accounts on visual percepts using multivariate pattern analysis of fMRI data (Wang et al. 2013). The results showed differential patterns of BOLD activity in high-order frontoparietal regions between visual percepts during bistable perception. The present study complements these results by showing a role for the frontoparietal network in indexing percepts in the auditory modality. Furthermore, the temporal resolution of our EEG and iEEG data enabled us to characterize the fine-grained temporal dynamics of NII associated with specific auditory percepts within the frontoparietal network. These results represent convergent evidence toward a possible general mechanism of information integration underlying the emergence of the contents of consciousness under invariant stimulation.
In conclusion, we have presented experimental evidence that conscious percepts may be supported by different neural mechanisms depending on whether they are internally or externally driven. We have highlighted the stark contrast between fleeting endogenous percepts, where neural integration and differentiation parallel the corresponding integrated and differentiated percepts, and those that are externally triggered, for which no differences between information theory measures were observed. However, when conscious percepts are driven by changes in the external stimulation, fluctuations in spectral power are sufficient for indexing perceptual integration and differentiation. Importantly, the conceptual mapping between phenomenology and the neurophysiology that we have highlighted here should be considered as a fruitful approach for measuring the different dimensions of phenomenology in an experimentally testable manner. In light of some of the main current theoretical frameworks of conscious perception—IIT and GNWT—we provide additional experimental scaffolding to further understand what we have in mind at any given time.
Author’s Contributions
A.C.-J. and T.A.B. conceived and designed the experiments. A.C.-J., F.O., A.G., and E.M. performed the experiments. A.C.-J. analyzed the data. A.J.B., D.H., and S.C. contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools. M.d.C.G., W.S., E.V., and C.C. gave access to clinical patient. A.C.-J., A.J.B., A.I., S.C., and T.A.B. wrote the paper.
Funding
Wellcome Trust Biomedical Research Fellowship (WT093811MA); Chilean National Fund for Scientific and Technological Development (FONDECYT 1171200).
Notes
We thank Robert Carlyon, Jacobo D. Sitt, Simon van Gaal, Jean-Phillippe Lachaux, Anat Arzi, William J. Harrison, Daniel Bor, and Eugenio Rodriguez for contributing to valuable discussion and insights. This work is dedicated to the memory of Prof. Walter J. Freeman (1927–2016) whose pioneering work on Neurodynamics has inspired and ignited countless meaningful insights during the execution of this project. Conflict of Interest: None declared.
Supplementary Material
References
- Basirat A, Sato M, Schwartz JL, Kahane P, Lachaux JP. 2008. Parieto-frontal gamma band activity during the perceptual emergence of speech forms. Neuroimage. 42:404–413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Billig AJ, Carlyon RP. 2016. Automaticity and primacy of auditory streaming: concurrent subjective and objective measures. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 42:339–353. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Billig AJ, Davis MH, Carlyon RP. 2018. Neural decoding of bistable sounds reveals an effect of intention on perceptual organization. J Neurosci. 38:2844–2853. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Billig AJ, Davis MH, Deeks JM, Monstrey J, Carlyon RP. 2013. Lexical influences on auditory streaming. Curr Biol. 23:1585–1589. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boly M, Sasai S, Gosseries O, Oizumi M, Casali A, Massimini M, Tononi G. 2015. Stimulus set meaningfulness and neurophysiological differentiation: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. PLoS One. 10e0125337. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Canales-Johnson A, Silva C, Huepe D, Alvaro R-R, Noreika V, Del Carmen GM, Silva W, Ciraolo C, Vaucheret E, Sedeno L, et al. . 2015. Auditory feedback differentially modulates behavioral and neural markers of objective and subjective performance when tapping to your heartbeat. Cereb Cortex. 25:4490–4503. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carlyon RP, Cusack R, Foxton JM, Robertson IH. 2001. Effects of attention and unilateral neglect on auditory stream segregation. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 27:115–127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Casali AG, Gosseries O, Rosanova M, Boly M, Sarasso S, Casali KR, Casarotto S, Bruno M-A, Laureys S, Tononi G, et al. . 2013. A theoretically based index of consciousness independent of sensory processing and behavior. Sci Transl Med. 5:198ra105–198ra105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chaitin GJ. 1974. Information-theoretic computation complexity. IEEE Trans Inf Theory. 20:10–15. [Google Scholar]
- Chaitin GJ. 1995. The berry paradox. Complex Syst Bin Networks. 461:23–31. [Google Scholar]
- Chennu S, Finoia P, Kamau E, Allanson J, Williams GB, Monti MM, Noreika V, Arnatkeviciute A, Canales-Johnson A, Olivares F, et al. . 2014. Spectral signatures of reorganised brain networks in disorders of consciousness. PLoS Comput Biol. 10:e1003887. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cusack R. 2005. The intraparietal sulcus and perceptual organization. J Cogn Neurosci. 17:641–651. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Jong MC, Hendriks RJM, Vansteense MJ, Raemaekers M, Verstraten FAJ, Ramsey NF, Erkelens CJ, Leijten FSS, Van Ee R. 2016. Intracranial recordings of occipital cortex responses to illusory visual events. J Neurosci. 36:6297–6311. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dehaene S, Changeux JP. 2011. Experimental and theoretical approaches to conscious processing. Neuron. 70:200–227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dehaene S, Charles L, King JR, Marti S. 2014. Toward a computational theory of conscious processing. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 25:76– 84. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delorme A, Makeig S. 2004. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J Neurosci Methods. 134:9–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doesburg SM, Kitajo K, Ward LM. 2005. Increased gamma-band synchrony precedes switching of conscious perceptual objects in binocular rivalry. Neuroreport. 16:1139–1142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Engel AK, Fries P, Singer W. 2001. Dynamic predictions: oscillations and synchrony in top-down processing. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2:704–716. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Engel AK, Gerloff C, Hilgetag CC, Nolte G. 2013. Intrinsic coupling modes: multiscale interactions in Ongoing brain activity. Neuron. 80:867–886 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fahrenfort JJ, Snijders TM, Heinen K, van Gaal S, Scholte HS, Lamme VA. 2012. Neuronal integration in visual cortex elevates face category tuning to conscious face perception. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 109:21504–21509. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fahrenfort JJ, van Leeuwen J, Olivers CNL, Hogendoorn H. 2017. Perceptual integration without conscious access. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 114:3744–3749. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fesi JD, Mendola JD. 2015. Individual peak gamma frequency predicts switch rate in perceptual rivalry. Hum Brain Mapp. 36:566–576. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flevaris AV, Martínez A, Hillyard SA. 2013. Neural substrates of perceptual integration during bistable object perception. J Vis. 13:17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foster BL, He BJ, Honey CJ, Jerbi K, Maier A, Saalmann YB. 2016. Spontaneous neural dynamics and multi-scale network organization. Front Syst Neurosci. 10:7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox MD, Raichle ME. 2007. Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nat Rev Neurosci. 8:700–711. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frässle S, Sommer J, Jansen A, Naber M, Einhäuser W. 2014. Binocular rivalry: frontal activity relates to introspection and action but not to perception. J Neurosci. 34:1738–1747. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freeman W. 2000. Neurodynamics: An Exploration in Mesoscopic Brain Dynamics, Perspectives in Neural Computing. London: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- Freeman WJ. 1976. Mass action in the nervous system. Neuroscience. 1:423. [Google Scholar]
- Freeman WJ. 2015. Mechanism and significance of global coherence in scalp EEG. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 31:199–205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaillard R, Dehaene S, Adam C, Clémenceau S, Hasboun D, Baulac M, Cohen L, Naccache L.. 2009. Converging intracranial markers of conscious access. PLoS Biol. 7:0472–0492. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gutschalk A, Micheyl C, Melcher JR, Rupp A, Scherg M, Oxenham AJ. 2005. Neuromagnetic correlates of streaming in human auditory cortex. J Neurosci. 25:5382–5388. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Handel BF, Jensen O. 2014. Spontaneous local alpha oscillations predict motion-induced blindness. Eur J Neurosci. 40:3371–3379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Helfrich RF, Knepper H, Nolte G, Sengelmann M, König P, Schneider TR, Engel AK. 2016. Spectral fingerprints of large-scale cortical dynamics during ambiguous motion perception. Hum Brain Mapp.37:4099–4111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hill KT, Bishop CW, Yadav D, Miller LM. 2011. Pattern of BOLD signal in auditory cortex relates acoustic response to perceptual streaming. BMC Neurosci. 12:85. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hipp JF, Hawellek DJ, Corbetta M, Siegel M, Engel AK. 2012. Large-scale cortical correlation structure of spontaneous oscillatory activity. Nat Neurosci. 15:884–890. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Imperatori LS, Betta M, Cecchetti L, Canales-Johnson A, Ricciardi E, Siclari F, Pietrini P, Chennu S, Bernardi G. 2019. EEG functional connectivity metrics wPLI and wSMI account for d distinct types of brain functional interactions. Sci Rep. 9:8894. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jarosz AF, Wiley J. 2014. What are the odds? A practical guide to computing and reporting Bayes factors. J Probl Solving. 7:2. [Google Scholar]
- King JR, Sitt JD, Faugeras F, Rohaut B, El Karoui I, Cohen L, Naccache L, Dehaene S. 2013. Information sharing in the brain indexes consciousness in noncommunicative patients. Curr Biol. 23:1914–1919. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koch C, Massimini M, Boly M, Tononi G. 2016. Neural correlates of consciousness: progress and problems. Nat Rev Neurosci. 17:307–321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kolmogorov AN. 1965. Three approaches to the definition of the concept of quantity of information. IEEE Trans Inf Theory. 14:662–669. [Google Scholar]
- Lachaux JP, Rodriguez E, Martinerie J, Varela FJ. 1999. Measuring phase synchrony in brain signals. Hum Brain Mapp. 8:194–208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Le Van Quyen M, Foucher J, Lachaux J, Rodriguez E, Lutz A, Martinerie J, Varela FJ. 2001. Comparison of Hilbert transform and wavelet methods for the analysis of neuronal synchrony. J Neurosci Methods. 111:83–98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li G, Jiang S, Paraskevopoulou SE, Wang M, Xu Y, Wu Z, Chen L, Zhang D, Schalk G. 2018. Optimal referencing for stereo-electroencephalographic (SEEG) recordings. Neuroimage. 183:327–335. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lempel A, Ziv J. 1976. On the complexity of finite sequences. IEEE Trans Inf Theory. 22:75–82. [Google Scholar]
- Levy J, Vidal JR, Fries P, Démonet J-F, Goldstein A, Demonet J-F, Goldstein A. 2015. Selective neural synchrony suppression as a forward gatekeeper to piecemeal conscious perception. Cereb Cortex. 1–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Masson MEJ. 2011. A tutorial on a practical Bayesian alternative to null-hypothesis significance testing. Behav Res Methods. 43:679–690. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Melloni L, Molina C, Pena M, Torres D, Singer W, Rodriguez E. 2007. Synchronization of neural activity across cortical areas correlates with conscious perception. J Neurosci. 27:2858–2865. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noreika V, Canales-Johnson A, Koh J, Taylor M, Massey I, Bekinschtein TA. 2015. Intrusions of a drowsy mind: neural markers of phenomenological unpredictability. Front Psychol. 6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oizumi M, Albantakis L, Tononi G. 2014. From the phenomenology to the mechanisms of consciousness: integrated information theory 3.0. PLoS Comput Biol. 10:e1003588. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oostenveld R, Fries P, Maris E, Schoffelen JM. 2011. FieldTrip: open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Comput Intell Neurosci. 2011:156869. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parkkonen L, Andersson J, Hämäläinen M, Hari R. 2008. Early visual brain areas reflect the percept of an ambiguous scene. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 105:20500–20504. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Penny W. D., Friston K. J., Ashburner J. T., Kiebel S. J., & Nichols T. E. (Eds.). (2011). Statistical parametric mapping: the analysis of functional brain images. Elsevier. [Google Scholar]
- Pressnitzer D, Hupé JM. 2006. Temporal dynamics of auditory and visual Bistability reveal common principles of perceptual organization. Curr Biol. 16:1351–1357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rorden C, Brett M. 2000. Stereotaxic display of brain lesions. Behav Neurol. 12:191–200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salomon D. 2004. Data compression: the complete reference. New York: Springer Science & Business Media. [Google Scholar]
- Sanders RD, Winston JS, Barnes GR, Reis G. 2018. Magnetoencephalographic correlates of perceptual state during auditory bistability. Sci Rep. 8:976. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schartner M, Seth A, Noirhomme Q, Boly M, Bruno MA, Laureys S, Barrett A. 2015. Complexity of multi-dimensional spontaneous EEG decreases during propofol induced general anaesthesia. PLoS One. 10:e0133532. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schartner MM, Carhart-Harris RL, Barrett AB, Seth AK, Muthukumaraswamy SD. 2017. Increased spontaneous MEG signal diversity for psychoactive doses of ketamine, LSD and psilocybin. Sci Rep. 7:46421. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sitt JD, King JR, El Karoui I, Rohaut B, Faugeras F, Gramfort A, Cohen L, Sigman M, Dehaene S, Naccache L. 2014. Large scale screening of neural signatures of consciousness in patients in a vegetative or minimally conscious state. Brain. 137:2258–2270. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snyder JS, Gregg MK, Weintraub DM, Alain C. 2012. Attention, awareness, and the perception of auditory scenes. Atten Conscious Differ senses. 95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stam CJ, Nolte G, Daffertshofer A. 2007. Phase lag index: assessment of functional connectivity from multichannel EEG and MEG with diminished bias from common sources. Hum Brain Mapp. 28:1178–1193. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sterzer P, Kleinschmidt A, Rees G. 2009. The neural bases of multistable perception. Trends Cogn Sci. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szalardy O, Bohm TM, Bendixen A, Winkler I. 2013. Event-related potential correlates of sound organization: early sensory and late cognitive effects. Biol Psychol. 93:97–104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tognoli E, Kelso JAS. 2014. The metastable brain. Neuron. 81:35–48 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tononi G, Boly M, Massimini M, Koch C. 2016. Integrated information theory: from consciousness to its physical substrate. Nat Rev Neurosci. 17:450–461. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uhlhaas PJ, Pipa G, Lima B, Melloni L, Neuenschwander S, Nikolic D, Singer W. 2009. Neural synchrony in cortical networks: history, concept and current status. Front Integr Neurosci. 3:17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Varela F, Lachaux JP, Rodriguez E, Martinerie J. 2001. The brainweb: phase synchronization and large-scale integration. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2:229–239. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vinck M, Oostenveld R, Van Wingerden M, Battaglia F, Pennartz CMA. 2011. An improved index of phase-synchronization for electrophysiological data in the presence of volume-conduction, noise and sample-size bias. Neuroimage. 55:1548–1565. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang M, Arteaga D, He BJ. 2013. Brain mechanisms for simple perception and bistable perception. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 110:E3350–E3359. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winkler I, Schröger E. 2015. Auditory perceptual objects as generative models: setting the stage for communication by sound. Brain Lang. 148:1–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xia M, Wang J, He Y. 2013. BrainNet viewer: a network visualization tool for human brain connectomics. PLoS One. 8:e68910. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.