Highlights
-
•
Visual metacognition of controls was dependent on spatial frequency.
-
•
Visual metacognition of schizophrenia patients was independent of spatial frequency.
-
•
Patients and controls differently rely on the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.
-
•
Sensory inputs may reach metacognitive circuits in an atypical manner among patients.
Keywords: Schizophrenia, Metacognition, Spatial frequency, fMRI, Multivoxel decoding, Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
Abstract
Although altered early stages of visual processing have been reported among schizophrenia patients, how such atypical visual processing may affect higher-level cognition remains largely unknown. Here we tested the hypothesis that metacognitive performance may be atypically modulated by spatial frequency (SF) of visual stimuli among individuals with schizophrenia, given their altered magnocellular function. To study the effect of SF on metacognitive performance, we asked patients and controls to perform a visual detection task on gratings with different SFs and report confidence, and analyzed the data using the signal detection theoretic measure meta-d′. Control subjects showed better metacognitive performance after yes- (stimulus presence) than after no- (stimulus absence) responses (‘yes-response advantage’) for high SF (HSF) stimuli but not for low SF (LSF) stimuli. The patients, to the contrary, showed a ‘yes-response advantage’ not only for HSF but also for LSF stimuli, indicating atypical SF dependency of metacognition. An fMRI experiment using the same task revealed that the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), known to be crucial for metacognition, shows activity mirroring the behavioral results: decoding accuracy of perceptual confidence in DLPFC was significantly higher for HSF than for LSF stimuli in controls, whereas this decoding accuracy was independent of SF in patients. Additionally, the functional connectivity of DLPFC with parietal and visual areas was modulated by SF and response type (yes/no) in a different manner between controls and patients. While individuals without schizophrenia may flexibly adapt metacognitive computations across SF ranges, patients may employ a different mechanism that is independent of SF. Because visual stimuli of low SF have been linked to predictive top-down processing, this may reflect atypical functioning in these processes in schizophrenia.
1. Introduction
Schizophrenia is a neurodevelopmental psychopathology which is found in about 1% of the population (Regier et al., 1993, van Os and Kapur, 2009). A comprehensive understanding of how neural functions go awry with schizophrenia is still underway. Among schizophrenia patients, studies have demonstrated atypicality not only in their cognitive functions (Callicott et al., 2003, Goldman-Rakic and Selemon, 1997, Weinberger and Gallhofer, 1997) but also in their early sensory processing (Butler and Javitt, 2005, Butler et al., 2007, Javitt and Freedman, 2015, Saccuzzo et al., 1974). Although there has been speculation regarding the relationship between the two functions (Butler et al., 2007, Javitt and Freedman, 2015), how atypical sensory functions contribute to altered higher cognitive functions remains largely unknown. Here, we examine the potential interaction between early stage visual processing of spatial frequency (SF) and higher-order metacognitive function among schizophrenia patients.
One widely reported atypicality with schizophrenia in low-level visual function is the response profile across SF. SF is typically critical for gating sensory inputs for distinct visual computations. For example, while coarse visual inputs with lower SF rapidly frame the visual processing in a predictive top-down manner (Bar et al., 2006, Javitt and Freedman, 2015), fine visual inputs with higher SF contribute to more elaborated perceptual decisions e.g., object identification (Bar et al., 2006, Javitt and Freedman, 2015). Among schizophrenia patients, studies have shown reduced neural response to lower SF in visual cortices and some other areas, which is likely due to their magnocellular dysfunction (Butler and Javitt, 2005, Martinez et al., 2012, Martinez et al., 2008). Patients also showed hindered perceptual sensitivity to lower SF inputs in some (Butler and Javitt, 2005, Martinez et al., 2008, Slaghuis and Curran, 1999) but not all tasks (Keri et al., 2002).
Some studies have shown that at later stages of perceptual decision-making such as object recognition, patients put more weight on low SF input, while control subjects generally rely more on high SF input (Laprevote et al., 2010, Laprevote et al., 2013). One possibility is that, among schizophrenia patients, sensory inputs across SF ranges may get atypically wired to later stage visual processing, including metacognition.
We here examined whether low level visual properties of SF modulate higher-order metacognitive function in an atypical manner among schizophrenia patients. Metacognition involves the monitoring of one’s own cognition (Frith, 2012, Nelson and Narens, 1990). One critical metacognitive function is to evaluate the accuracy of one’s perceptual judgments (Fleming et al., 2012a, Lau and Rosenthal, 2011), which is carried out in higher cortical areas, especially the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) (Cortese et al., 2016, Fleming et al., 2012b, Koizumi et al., 2016, Shekhar and Rahnev, 2018). This function enables one to discern between true and false percepts by creating an elevated sense of confidence in the former relative to the latter (Fleming et al., 2012a, Lau and Rosenthal, 2011).
Although recent studies have examined perceptual metacognitive performance among people with schizophrenia, without manipulation of SF, the results appear mixed and non-conclusive (Charles et al., 2017, Powers et al., 2017, Rouault et al., 2018). For example, patients showed lowered sensitivity for discriminating between correct versus incorrect percepts (i.e., metacognitive sensitivity, meta-d′) relative to controls (Charles et al., 2017) in conditions where patients also showed lowered perceptual sensitivity (d′). Thus, there remains a possibility that lowered metacognitive sensitivity was merely due to lowered perceptual sensitivity since perceptual sensitivity and metacognitive sensitivity generally covary (Fleming and Lau, 2014, Maniscalco and Lau, 2014). In line with this, Powers et al. (2017) reported that metacognitive sensitivity was unrelated with psychosis and/or hallucination when perceptual sensitivity (d′) was taken into account. Similarly, Rouault et al. (2018) showed that self-reported schizotypy had little contribution to metacognitive performance. One reason for these mixed results could be that schizophrenia patients are atypical not in their general metacognitive performance, but rather in how their metacognitive performance is modulated by features of the perceptual decision-making context, such as SF as investigated here.
We here examined metacognitive performance during a visual detection task with perceptual confidence rating using high and low SF stimuli among schizophrenia patients. During a detection task, metacognitive sensitivity for discriminating between correct versus incorrect percepts is typically better for stimulus-present responses (yes-responses) than for stimulus-absent responses (no-responses) among healthy individuals (Fleming and Dolan, 2010, Kanai et al., 2010) (see also our replication in Supplementary Fig. 1). That is, it is generally easier to discriminate one's correct versus incorrect yes-responses (i.e. by giving higher confidence ratings to hits vs false alarms) than to discriminate correct versus incorrect no-responses (i.e., correct rejections vs misses) when detecting a visual target.
An intuitive explanation for this phenomenon is that confidence is typically evaluated by reference to the amount of sensory evidence under consideration, and the amount of available sensory evidence differs for yes- and no-responses. In a detection task, a “yes” response indicates that the observer saw the target and therefore has access to sensory evidence for the target, and such sensory evidence can then be evaluated to determine confidence. By contrast, a “no” response indicates that the observer did not see the target and therefore likely has little-to-no sensory evidence pertaining to the target, which entails that the typical strategy of rating confidence by reference to the available sensory evidence is not tenable and an alternative strategy must be used. Given this known asymmetry in metacognitive performance between yes- and no-response (i.e., yes-response advantage) (Fleming and Dolan, 2010, Kanai et al., 2010) (Supplementary Fig. 1), we here separately estimated the response-specific metacognitive sensitivity with a previously developed signal detection theoretic measurement (rs-meta-d′) (Maniscalco and Lau, 2014). rs-meta-d′ provides a bias-free measure of one’s ability to distinguish correct vs incorrect instances of a particular response type with confidence ratings. For instance, rs-meta-d′ for yes-responses measures an observer’s sensitivity for discriminating between correct yes-responses (hits) vs incorrect yes-responses (false alarms) by reporting higher confidence for the former than the latter.
We first conducted Experiment 1 where we behaviorally examined the metacognitive performance of schizophrenia patients and healthy controls in a visual detection task where the target could have either high or low SF (HSF or LSF). We then conducted Experiment 2 to examine the neural underpinnings of the group difference in metacognition with fMRI. Specifically, we examined whether decoding accuracy of perceptual confidence may differ between the groups in the DLPFC, a region where metacognitive computations take place (Fleming et al., 2012a, Lau and Rosenthal, 2011). We also examined whether the DLPFC may show differential functional connectivity with the rest of the brain as a function of group, SF, and the response type (yes/no).
2. Experiment 1
In Experiment 1, schizophrenia patients and age matched controls performed a grating detection task and rated confidence in their detection responses. Gratings had either high or low SF (HSF and LSF, respectively). We measured metacognitive sensitivity separately for yes- and no-responses as a function of SF condition, and compared the effect of SF on metacognitive performance between the patient and control groups. We chose to use simple grating stimuli rather than more complex stimuli, such as faces, in order to avoid potential confounding variables due to other schizophrenia-related visual dysfunctions in processing complex stimuli (Revheim et al., 2014, Soria Bauser et al., 2012).
Perceptual sensitivity (d′) was prepared to be similar across groups and conditions by calibrating the grating contrast level prior to the detection task, which allowed us to selectively examine the potential differences in metacognitive performance.
2.1. Methods and materials
2.1.1. Participants
Twenty schizophrenia patients diagnosed with DSM-IV Axis I Disorders - Patients Edition, Version 2.0 (SCID-P) (9 males, mean age = 42.3 ±SD 11.9) by psychiatrists at Kyoto University Graduate School of Medicine and eighteen healthy controls (12 males, mean age = 40.2 ± 11.6) were enrolled in this study. The data from three female patients were excluded from the analyses because of difficulty in comprehending the task, leaving data from the remaining seventeen patients for analysis (9 males, mean age = 40.5 ± 11.9). See supplementary Table 1 for their demographic information. The protocol was approved by the ethical committee of Kyoto University as well as by that of NICT. All participants gave written consent forms prior to participation.
2.1.2. Procedure
Participants performed a pair of tasks to detect HSF (2.6 cycle per degree, cpd) and LSF (0.4 cpd) gratings (Fig. 1) in blocks, in a counterbalanced order. Each task consisted of a detection task block (360 trials, performed in six subblocks of 60 trials each), which followed two practice blocks (10 trials each) and one calibration block (80 trials). These two levels of SF were expected to evoke differential neural activity between schizophrenia patients and controls (Martinez et al., 2008). On every trial, a dynamic random noise pattern was displayed for 1.5 s. In half of these trials, after the first 1 s of random noise, a horizontal grating was superimposed on the noise pattern for 250 ms. Subsequently, participants were asked to indicate whether a grating had been presented or not (i.e., yes- or no-response) and then to rate their perceptual confidence on a four-point scale by using a keyboard. After an inter-trial-interval of 3.75 s, a next trial begun.
Prior to the task, the Michelson contrasts of the gratings were titrated with a QUEST threshold estimation procedure (Watson and Pelli, 1983) to achieve two slightly different levels of difficulty (hit rates of 55% and 65%). Calibration was conducted separately for each task with HSF or LSF gratings. Within each task, the level of difficulty was fixed within a given subblock and two levels of difficulty were alternated between subblocks. This alternation was intended to avoid over-habituation during the task by including slight variability of the stimuli. The data from all subblocks were used to estimate metacognitive performance for each task with HSF or LSF gratings in subsequent main analyses. The results are separately shown for each difficulty level in Supplementary Fig. 4. See Additional information on Experiment 1 in Supplementary material for further details.
2.1.3. Analyses
Detection sensitivity was calculated as d′ for each SF level with standard signal detection theory (SDT) methods (Macmillan and Creelman, 1991). Metacognitive sensitivity for yes- and no-responses was separately calculated as response-specific meta-d′ (rs-meta-d′; http://www.columbia.edu/~bsm2105/type2sdt/fit_rs_meta_d_MLE.m) (Maniscalco and Lau, 2014) for each SF level. High values of rs-meta-d′ for yes-responses indicate that the participant’s confidence ratings discriminate well between correct and incorrect yes-responses, i.e. mean confidence is higher for hits than for false alarms. Similarly, high values of rs-meta-d′ for no-responses indicate that the participant’s confidence ratings discriminate well between correct and incorrect no-responses, i.e. mean confidence is higher for correct rejections than for misses. Here, the ratio of meta-d′ (metacognitive sensitivity) to d′ (perceptual sensitivity) (meta-d′/d′) was then calculated for each SF level to quantify metacognitive efficiency (Fleming and Lau, 2014, Maniscalco and Lau, 2014). Meta-d′ corresponds to the degree of metacognitive sensitivity (to discriminate correct versus incorrect perceptual response), which is expected to equal to the value of perceptual sensitivity d′ (to discriminate stimulus presence or absence) when the signal strength in SDT framework is equally contributing to d′ and meta-d′. Thus, for a subject whose behavior matches SDT expectation, meta-d′ = d′, meaning that the subject’s measured metacognitive sensitivity (meta-d′) corresponds to the level of metacognitive sensitivity that would be expected to arise from their performance on the primary perceptual task (d′). For subjects with meta-d′ < d′, metacognitive sensitivity is lower than would be expected based on primary task performance. For such subjects, metacognitive sensitivity can be considered to be “inefficient” or “suboptimal,” relative to SDT expectation.
Across-participant means for d′, meta-d′, and meta-d′/d′ were analyzed with Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) in SPSS version 25 (IBM). For subsequent analyses, an ANOVA with two within-participant factors of SF (high/low) and response type (yes/no) and one between-participant factor of a group was conducted.
2.2. Results and discussion
First, the analysis of Meta-d′/d′ revealed a main effect of response type (F(1, 33) = 25.99, p < .001) (Fig. 2). This main effect was due to higher Meta-d′/d′ following yes- than no-responses, which is consistent with previous studies (Fleming and Dolan, 2010, Kanai et al., 2010) and our pilot experiment (Supplementary Fig. 1). Interestingly, such yes-response advantage was modulated by group and SF (a three-way interaction: F(1, 33) = 4.60, p = .039). Post-hoc t-tests revealed that controls showed significantly higher Meta-d′/d′ following yes- than no-responses (i.e., yes-response advantage) only when detecting an HSF grating target (p < .001) but not LSF grating target (p = .169). By comparison, patients showed significant yes-response advantage irrespective of target SF (HSF: p = .013; LSF: p = .001). There was no significant main effect of group (F(1, 33) = 0.77, p = .386), suggesting that patients did not show any generic reduction of metacognitive performance relative to controls. The magnitude of yes-response advantage (i.e., the difference in meta-d′/d′ between yes- and no-responses, Fig. 2B) was significantly larger with HSF than LSF targets among controls (t(17) = 2.22, p = .040) but not among patients (t(16) = −0.63, p = .540). Confidence rating difference between correct and incorrect responses mirrored the results of Meta-d′/d′ (Supplementary Fig. 2).
For the detection sensitivity d′ (Supplementary Fig. 3B), there was neither a significant main effect of group (F(1, 33) = 0.16, p = .694) nor a significant interaction between group and SF (F(1,33) = 3.80, p = .060), suggesting that patients did not statistically differ from controls in their perceptual performance. There was a main effect of SF (F(1, 33) = 4.33, p = .045), which was due to lower d′ with LSF relative to HSF stimuli. A potential relationship between hallucination severity (measured with Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) (Kay et al., 1987, Kay et al., 1991)) and metacognitive performance among patients, although tentative, can be found in Supplementary Material (Relationship between hallucination severity and metacognitive performance).
Taken together, the results suggest that insensitivity to SF modulation may characterize metacognitive performance among patients (Fig. 2).
3. Experiment 2
Experiment 1 indicated that only controls show SF dependency in their metacognitive performance, while patients do not. We predicted that controls, but not patients, may rely less on the neural circuit typically supporting metacognition when perceiving LSF stimuli. A large body of literature has shown that DLPFC typically plays a central role in metacognition (Cortese et al., 2016, Fleming et al., 2012a, Fleming et al., 2012b, Koizumi et al., 2016, Lau and Passingham, 2006, Rounis et al., 2010). However, whether DLPFC atypically contributes to metacognition among schizophrenia patients remains unknown. We here particularly predicted that DLPFC may be a potential source of the group difference in metacognitive function. To test this, we next conducted the same experimental task as Experiment 1 in an fMRI scanner.
3.1. Methods and materials
3.1.1. Participants
Seventeen schizophrenia patients diagnosed with DSM-IV Axis I Disorders - Patients Edition, Version 2.0 (SCID-P) (6 males, mean age = 42.8 ± 9.5) and seventeen healthy controls (7 males, mean age = 40.2 ± 10.1) were enrolled in this study. The data from two male patients were excluded from the analyses because of their inability to respond within the limited windows (<2.4 s). The data from the remaining fifteen patients (4 males, mean age = 44.7 ± 8.4) were analyzed (see Supplementary Table 1). The protocol was approved by the ethical committee of Kyoto University as well as by that of NICT. All participants gave written consent forms prior to participation.
3.1.2. Procedure
The task procedure was similar to that of Experiment 1. The runs were blocked for HSF and LSF stimuli, and the order of SF level was counterbalanced across participants. Participants viewed the stimuli presented on a monitor screen via a mirror, and performed the task with an MRI compatible response keypad (Current Designs, Inc.). See Additional information on Experiment 2 in Supplementary Material for more details.
3.1.3. ROI definition
The DLPFC ROI was functionally defined. Specifically, the clusters of voxels that were significantly activated for the confidence rating relative to the fixation period from all the trials (p < .01, Bonferroni-corrected) in a group GLM were selected, which were located within Brodmann area (BA) 46 (MRIcron, Brodmann 48 Area Atlas Template, https://people.cas.sc.edu/rorden/mricron/index.html) (Fig. 4). For the DLPFC ROI, the center of gravity in Talairach space was [X = −34.28, Y = 35.33, Z = 32.43] for the left hemisphere cluster and [X = 37.67, Y = 30.44, Z = 31.61 ] for the right hemisphere cluster (see ROI definition in Supplementary Material).
As a control ROI which is related to visual processing but not directly to metacognitive processing (Cortese et al., 2016), the clusters of voxels that were significantly activated for the stimulus period relative to fixation from all the trials (p < .01, Bonferroni-corrected) were selected within visual cortex (Supplementary Fig. 6A). For the control ROI within visual cortex, the center of gravity in Talairach space was [X = −22.21, Y = −81.09, Z = −10.29] for the left hemisphere and [X = 22.40, Y = −81.35, Z = −8.27] for the right hemisphere.
3.1.4. Analyses
The fMRI data were preprocessed and analyzed with BrainVoyager 21.0 (Brain Innovation, the Netherlands). To decode the trial-by-trial confidence level in a binary manner, high or low confidence, from the multivoxel activation pattern in DLPFC during the confidence rating period (i.e., predicting the confidence level on each trial from the activation pattern), we built a decoder with sparse logistic regression (SLR) which automatically selects relevant features (i.e., voxels) (Yamashita et al., 2008). For the decoding analysis, the data from one control participant were excluded for having too few trials with confidence ratings of 1 and 4 (low and high, respectively) (see Decoding Analysis in Supplementary Material).
In addition, we examined whether the functional connectivity between DLPFC and other brain areas was modulated as a function of SF, response type, and group. Specifically, we conducted a whole-brain Generalized form of context-dependent psychophysiological interaction (gPPI) analysis (McLaren et al., 2012) with DLPFC as a seed ROI.
3.2. Results and discussion
3.2.1. Behavioral results
The behavioral data were not of main interest in Experiment 2, given that there were too small trial numbers in Experiment 2 (96 trials) relative to Experiment 1 (360 trials) to properly fit the data to estimate Meta-d′. The overall results were qualitatively similar to that of Experiment 1 (Supplementary Fig. 5).
3.2.2. fMRI results
The results of the multivoxel decoding analysis supported the possibility that patients and controls may differ in DLPFC recruitment during metacognitive judgement (Fig. 3). That is, the decoding analysis with the DLPFC ROI revealed a significant interaction between group and SF (F(1, 27) = 5.08, p = .032). This interaction was due to higher decoding accuracy for HSF relative to LSF stimuli among controls (p = .043), while there was no such SF dependence of decoding accuracy among patients (p = .289). There was no significant main effect of group (F(1, 27) = 0.07, p = .799) or SF (F(1, 27) = 2.81, p = .105). Unlike with DLPFC, there was no significant difference in decoding accuracy as a function of group and SF within the control ROI in a visual cortex (Supplementary Fig. 6B). There was no significant main effect of group (F(1, 27) = 2.33, p = .138) or SF (F(1, 27) = 0.10, p = .751), and their interaction was also not significant (F(1, 27) = 2.15, p = .154). Note that the factor of response type was not considered in the decoding analysis due to the limits in trial number (see Decoding analysis in Supplementary Material).
Consistent with Experiment 1, patients and controls did not differ in their overall behavioral performance in metacognition and perception (i.e., unless considering response type), regardless of the SF level (Supplementary Fig. 5). Thus, it is unlikely that the decoding accuracy difference merely reflected the metacognitive performance difference. Instead, such results more likely reflect the group difference in the involvement of DLPFC in metacognition, as a function of SF.
The whole-brain analysis of gPPI effect revealed that the DLPFC ROI showed altered functional connectivity as a function of SF, response type, and group with clusters in parietal and visual cortices (p < .01, corrected with cluster-size thresholding (Forman et al., 1995, Goebel et al., 2006)) (Fig. 4). Note that here, the parietal and visual cortices were not predetermined as ROIs but were shown significant in the whole brain analysis. The bilateral clusters in the parietal cortex and visual cortex were located posterior of BA7 and ventral of BA18 (MRIcron, Brodmann 48 Area Atlas Template), respectively. The centers of gravity for the left and right clusters in parietal cortex were [X = −21.34, Y = −68.37, Z = 38.32 ] and [X = 10.50, Y = −61.86, Z = 36.32], respectively. The centers of gravity for the left and right clusters in visual cortex were [X = −24.0, Y = −77.51, Z = −19.52] and [X = 17.13, Y = −72.81, Z = −16.73], respectively.
For illustrative purposes only, the parameter estimates (Beta) for gPPI terms in the parietal and visual clusters (averaged between the hemispheres) are visualized separately for each level of SF, response type, and group in Supplementary Fig. 7. This result, although illustrative, shows that the functional connectivity between DLPFC and parietal or visual cortex was more enhanced following no- relative to yes-responses with the HSF stimuli among controls. Considering that controls behaviorally showed yes-response advantage in metacognitive performance only with the HSF stimuli (Experiment 1), one interpretation of this functional connectivity effect may be that there was more effortful (thus enhanced) integration of sensory and decision information by the DLPFC following no-responses to the HSF stimuli.
4. General discussion
While a potential relationship between schizophrenia and atypical metacognitive function has been speculated for decades (Frith and Done, 1988), whether schizophrenia patients have altered metacognitive ability to introspect perception has remained inconclusive (Charles et al., 2017, Powers et al., 2017, Rouault et al., 2018). We here show that patients and controls generally perform equally well in a visual metacognitive task. However, the groups differed in both behavioral performance and neural activity in terms of their dependency on SF.
Overall, metacognitive efficiency for introspecting perceptual accuracy was higher for yes-responses (perceived stimulus presence) than for no-responses (perceived stimulus absence), consistent with earlier studies (Fleming and Dolan, 2010, Kanai et al., 2010). A novel finding of this study was that patients and controls differ in the SF range where they display the yes-response advantage in metacognition. Specifically, controls showed a yes-response advantage only when detecting a high SF visual target but not when detecting a low SF target. Meanwhile, patients showed a yes-response advantage irrespective of the SF level.
An fMRI study (Experiment 2) further supported such SF independence of metacognitive function among patients. That is, among controls, trial-by-trial confidence ratings could be more accurately decoded from the multivoxel patterns in DLPFC with high than low SF stimuli. Meanwhile, decoding accuracy was similar between the SF levels among patients. Moreover, the functional connectivity between DLPFC and parietal or visual cortex during the metacognitive judgement was differently modulated as a function of the group, SF, and response type.
To provide explanations for the group difference in SF dependence, we first turn to why there is generally a yes-response advantage in metacognition. It has been repeatedly demonstrated that metacognitive judgements are mainly based on perceptual evidence that contributed to finalize perceptual decisions (Koizumi et al., 2015, Maniscalco et al., 2016, Peters et al., 2017, Zylberberg et al., 2012). While these findings are mainly based on discrimination tasks, a similar heuristic mechanism may be at play with a detection task. In a detection task, there is perceptual evidence to support yes-responses (stimulus presence) but little-to-no evidence to support no-responses (stimulus absence). Thus, perceptual confidence for only the yes-responses likely reflects the amount of supporting evidence, which is typically larger for correct than incorrect responses. However, confidence for no-responses could be more difficult to estimate, because supporting evidence for no-responses is absence of evidence, which might provide a poorer basis for discerning correct versus incorrect responses. The yes-response advantage in a given perceptual condition may be considered as an index for the degree to which supporting perceptual evidence is directly translated to metacognitive judgements (i.e., confidence rating).
Considering this, one possibility is that the group difference in yes-response advantage reflects the degree to which metacognitive judgement is based on perceptual evidence in a given SF range. Individuals without schizophrenia may base their metacognitive judgements more on higher SF evidence because it typically contributes more to finalized perceptual decisions (Bar et al., 2006). Meanwhile, schizophrenia patients may rely on evidence not only in the higher but also in the lower SF range, potentially because inputs in the lower SF range tend to contribute more to finalized perceptual decisions relative to controls (Laprevote et al., 2010, Laprevote et al., 2013).
Previous studies have shown reduced neural responses and lowered perceptual sensitivity to low SF stimuli among schizophrenia patients (Butler and Javitt, 2005, Martinez et al., 2012, Martinez et al., 2008, Slaghuis and Curran, 1999) although it may depend on a task (Keri et al., 2002). The current results, together with the findings at the early visual stages, suggest the possibility that the early stage sensory inputs across SF ranges may get wired to higher-order metacognitive function differently among schizophrenia patients compared with controls.
The fMRI results (Experiment 2) suggested that the group difference in metacognition may be at least partly related to the function in the DLPFC. First, the decoding results showed that, among controls, metacognitive judgements (i.e., trial-wise confidence ratings) are more accurately decoded from the activation patterns in DLPFC during the detection of high SF stimuli than that of low SF stimuli (Fig. 3). Meanwhile, patients did not show such SF dependency. While controls may rely on the DLPFC when estimating perceptual confidence in a SF-specific manner, schizophrenia patients may rely on the DLPFC in a more uniform manner across the SF range.
The result of gPPI analysis further supported the possibility that DLPFC may at least partly contribute to the group difference in metacognition. The gPPI result showed that controls and patients differ in how the functional connectivity between the DLPFC and parietal or visual cortex is modulated during metacognitive judgement, as a function of the stimulus SF and the response type (yes/no) (Fig. 4). Given that parietal cortex and visual cortex are each involved in perceptual decisions (Kiani and Shadlen, 2009) and sensory evidence processing (Cortese et al., 2016), the result suggests that the degree to which the DLPFC engages to integrate the perceptual-level information in parietal and visual cortices differs between the groups, depending on whether they perceived stimulus presence (yes- or no-responses) and on whether the stimulus was high or low in its SF. Whether the degree of such functional connectivity reflects more effortful metacognitive processing (Supplementary Fig. 7) or other computational differences remain to be further examined in future studies with larger sample size.
What remains to be explained is the result that metacognitive efficiency for no-responses is generally lower than yes-responses but is not completely abolished (i.e., above-zero Meta-d′). This suggests that participants (both controls and patients) may rely on some compensatory strategies to evaluate their perceptual confidence for no-responses, which are alternative to, but not mutually exclusive with, the aforementioned strategy to rely on sensory evidence that contributed to perceptual decisions (Koizumi et al., 2015, Maniscalco et al., 2016, Peters et al., 2017, Zylberberg et al., 2012). One such alternative strategy could be to rely on the level of attention allocated on given trials (Kanai et al., 2010). As attentional level is expected to be generally higher for correct than for incorrect responses, such a difference in attentional level can be mapped onto higher confidence for correct responses, resulting in some degree of metacognitive sensitivity. This strategy based on attentional level difference may also partly contribute to metacognitive performance with LSF stimuli among controls, where metacognitive efficiency was similar between yes- and no-responses. If participants were to evaluate their perceptual confidence for correct versus incorrect responses based on the attentional level difference between those responses, then metacognitive sensitivity is expected to be similar between yes- and no-responses. This is because attentional level allocated to the task stimuli is expected to be similar between stimulus present and absent trials (which, other things being equal, would map onto correct yes- and no-responses when attentional level is sufficiently higher), given that participants had no prior information as regards to which trial (i.e., target present or absent) will be presented. Schizophrenia patients, on the other hand, may have less flexibility to switch between different metacognitive strategies across SF ranges.
The result showed that metacognitive efficiency for no-responses differs between the tasks with HSF and LSF stimuli among controls, despite the fact that stimulus absent trials (which constitute the ‘correct’ no-responses) were identical between the tasks. This result likely owes to a general limitation in perceptual decision making strategy which results in a failure to alternate between different perceptual strategies across trials when the trials are performed in a single block (Rahnev et al., 2011, Gorea and Sagi, 2000). That is, controls are likely to evaluate perceptual confidence with one strategy uniformly within a given task, depending on SF of the target stimuli. And the use of different strategies across the tasks may explain the difference in metacognitive efficiency for no-responses between the tasks with different SF levels. Future studies may further examine this possibility with a non-blocked design where both HSF and LSF stimuli are presented as targets within a single block.
Lastly, whether patients show atypicality in metacognitive performance with other sensory modalities such as audition remains one important question to be examined, as hallucinations among schizophrenia patients are even more prevalent in the auditory domain than the visual domain (59% and 27% prevalence, respectively) (Waters et al., 2014). As a previous study demonstrated that one’s metacognitive performance correlates across the sensory modalities (Faivre et al., 2018), future studies may examine whether metacognitive function among patients also show atypical dependence on some features of auditory stimuli.
5. Conclusions
Taken together, our results demonstrate that schizophrenia patients do not show SF dependence in their metacognitive performance, while controls show differential metacognitive performance as well as differential reliance on DLPFC across the SF levels. The finding among controls is itself novel and may suggest that they can flexibly shift their metacognitive strategy depending on stimulus features. Inability of patients to adaptively shift their metacognitive strategy across visual conditions might contribute to their altered subjective experience of the sensory world.
CRediT authorship contribution statement
AK, BM, HL, HiT and KA contributed to conceptualization, AK, BM, and HL designed the tasks, AK, HoT, MH, RM, TK, JM, and TA curated data. AK and BM conducted formal analyses. AK, BM, HL, HiT, and KA wrote the original draft, and all authors reviewed and edited the manuscript.
Declaration of Competing Interest
The authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper.
Acknowledgements
We thank Aurelio Cortese for his helpful comments on the analysis and Raihyung Lee, Ben Smith, Matthias Michel, Kiyofumi Miyoshi for their helpful comments on the manuscript. This work was supported by JSPS KAKENHI Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research B [19H03583 to HT, 18H02714 to AK, 17H04248 and 18H02749 to JM] and for Scientifice Research A [17H00740 to JM], MEXT Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research on Innovative Areas [16H06572 to HT, 18H05130 to JM], and the Strategic Research Program for Brain Sciences by the Japan Agency for Medical Research and Development (AMED) [19dm0307008h0002 and 19dm0107151s0104 to HT], JST presto awarded each to AK and KA [JPMJPR18J3, JPMJPR17J1]. The funders played no scientific role in the project.
Footnotes
Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2020.102296.
Contributor Information
Ai Koizumi, Email: bellkoizumi@gmail.com.
Hidehiko Takahashi, Email: hidepsyc@tmd.ac.jp.
Appendix A. Supplementary data
The following are the Supplementary data to this article:
References
- Bar M., Kassam K.S., Ghuman A.S., Boshyan J., Schmid A.M., Dale A.M., Hamalainen M.S., Marinkovic K., Schacter D.L., Rosen B.R., Halgren E. Top-down facilitation of visual recognition. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A. 2006;103:449–454. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0507062103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Butler P.D., Javitt D.C. Early-stage visual processing deficits in schizophrenia. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry. 2005;18:151–157. doi: 10.1097/00001504-200503000-00008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Butler P.D., Martinez A., Foxe J.J., Kim D., Zemon V., Silipo G., Mahoney J., Shpaner M., Jalbrzikowski M., Javitt D.C. Subcortical visual dysfunction in schizophrenia drives secondary cortical impairments. Brain. 2007;130:417–430. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl233. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Callicott J.H., Mattay V.S., Verchinski B.A., Marenco S., Egan M.F., Weinberger D.R. Complexity of prefrontal cortical dysfunction in schizophrenia: more than up or down. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2003;160:2209–2215. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.12.2209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Charles L., Gaillard R., Amado I., Krebs M.O., Bendjemaa N., Dehaene S. Conscious and unconscious performance monitoring: Evidence from patients with schizophrenia. Neuroimage. 2017;144:153–163. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.09.056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cortese A., Amano K., Koizumi A., Kawato M., Lau H. Multivoxel neurofeedback selectively modulates confidence without changing perceptual performance. Nat. Commun. 2016;7:13669. doi: 10.1038/ncomms13669. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faivre N., Filevich E., Solovey G., Kuhn S., Blanke O. Behavioral, modeling, and electrophysiological evidence for supramodality in human metacognition. J. Neurosci. 2018;38:263–277. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0322-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fleming S.M., Dolan R.J. Effects of loss aversion on post-decision wagering: implications for measures of awareness. Conscious. Cogn. 2010;19:352–363. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2009.11.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fleming S.M., Dolan R.J., Frith C.D. Metacognition: computation, biology and function. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2012;367:1280–1286. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fleming S.M., Huijgen J., Dolan R.J. Prefrontal contributions to metacognition in perceptual decision making. J. Neurosci. 2012;32:6117–6125. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6489-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fleming S.M., Lau H.C. How to measure metacognition. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2014;8:443. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Forman S.D., Cohen J.D., Fitzgerald M., Eddy W.F., Mintun M.A., Noll D.C. Improved assessment of significant activation in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI): use of a cluster-size threshold. Magn. Reson. Med. 1995;33:636–647. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910330508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frith C.D. The role of metacognition in human social interactions. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2012;367:2213–2223. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frith C.D., Done D.J. Towards a neuropsychology of schizophrenia. Br. J. Psychiatry. 1988;153:437–443. doi: 10.1192/bjp.153.4.437. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goebel R., Esposito F., Formisano E. Analysis of functional image analysis contest (FIAC) data with brainvoyager QX: from single-subject to cortically aligned group general linear model analysis and self-organizing group independent component analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2006;27:392–401. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20249. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldman-Rakic P.S., Selemon L.D. Functional and anatomical aspects of prefrontal pathology in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 1997;23:437–458. doi: 10.1093/schbul/23.3.437. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gorea A., Sagi D. Failure to handle more than one internal representation in visual detection tasks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A. 2000;97:12380–12384. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.22.12380. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Javitt D.C., Freedman R. Sensory processing dysfunction in the personal experience and neuronal machinery of schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2015;172:17–31. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.13121691. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kanai R., Walsh V., Tseng C.H. Subjective discriminability of invisibility: a framework for distinguishing perceptual and attentional failures of awareness. Conscious. Cogn. 2010;19:1045–1057. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2010.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kay S.R., Fiszbein A., Opler L.A. The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 1987;13:261–276. doi: 10.1093/schbul/13.2.261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kay, S.R., Opler, L.A., Fiszbein, A., (translated by Yamada, H., Matsui, K., Kikumoto, K.), 1991. Positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) rating manual. Seiwa Shoten Publishers, Tokyo.
- Keri S., Antal A., Szekeres G., Benedek G., Janka Z. Spatiotemporal visual processing in schizophrenia. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2002;14:190–196. doi: 10.1176/jnp.14.2.190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiani R., Shadlen M.N. Representation of confidence associated with a decision by neurons in the parietal cortex. Science. 2009;324:759–764. doi: 10.1126/science.1169405. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koizumi A., Maniscalco B., Lau H. Does perceptual confidence facilitate cognitive control? Atten. Percept Psychophys. 2015;77:1295–1306. doi: 10.3758/s13414-015-0843-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koizumi A., Mobbs D., Lau H. Is fear perception special? Evidence at the level of decision-making and subjective confidence. Soc. Cogn. Affect Neurosci. 2016;11:1772–1782. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw084. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laprevote V., Oliva A., Delerue C., Thomas P., Boucart M. Patients with schizophrenia are biased toward low spatial frequency to decode facial expression at a glance. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48:4164–4168. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.10.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laprevote V., Oliva A., Ternois A.S., Schwan R., Thomas P., Boucart M. Low spatial frequency bias in schizophrenia is not face specific: when the integration of coarse and fine information fails. Front. Psychol. 2013;4:248. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00248. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lau H., Rosenthal D. Empirical support for higher-order theories of conscious awareness. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2011;15:365–373. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.05.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lau H.C., Passingham R.E. Relative blindsight in normal observers and the neural correlate of visual consciousness. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A. 2006;103:18763–18768. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0607716103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Macmillan N.A., Creelman C.D. 2nd ed. Lawrence Erlbaum; 1991. Detection Theory: a User’s Guide. [Google Scholar]
- Maniscalco B., Lau H. Signal Detection Theory Analysis of Type 1 and Type 2 Data: Meta-d', ResponseSpecific Meta-d', and the Unequal Variance SDT Model. In: Fleming S.M., Frith C.D., editors. The Cognitive Neuroscience of Metacognition. Springer; New York: 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Maniscalco B., Peters M.A., Lau H. Heuristic use of perceptual evidence leads to dissociation between performance and metacognitive sensitivity. Atten. Percept Psychophys. 2016;78:923–937. doi: 10.3758/s13414-016-1059-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martinez A., Hillyard S.A., Bickel S., Dias E.C., Butler P.D., Javitt D.C. Consequences of magnocellular dysfunction on processing attended information in schizophrenia. Cereb. Cortex. 2012;22:1282–1293. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr195. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martinez A., Hillyard S.A., Dias E.C., Hagler D.J., Jr., Butler P.D., Guilfoyle D.N., Jalbrzikowski M., Silipo G., Javitt D.C. Magnocellular pathway impairment in schizophrenia: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging. J. Neurosci. 2008;28:7492–7500. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1852-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLaren D.G., Ries M.L., Xu G., Johnson S.C. A generalized form of context-dependent psychophysiological interactions (gPPI): a comparison to standard approaches. Neuroimage. 2012;61:1277–1286. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nelson T.O., Narens L. Metamemory: A theoretical framework and some new findings. In: Bower G.H., editor. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation. Academic Press; New York: 1990. pp. 125–173. [Google Scholar]
- Peters M.A.K., Thesen T., Ko Y.D., Maniscalco B., Carlson C., Davidson M., Doyle W., Kuzniecky R., Devinsky O., Halgren E., Lau H. Perceptual confidence neglects decision-incongruent evidence in the brain. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2017;1 doi: 10.1038/s41562-017-0139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Powers A.R., Mathys C., Corlett P.R. Pavlovian conditioning-induced hallucinations result from overweighting of perceptual priors. Science. 2017;357:596–600. doi: 10.1126/science.aan3458. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rahnev D., Maniscalco B., Graves T., Huang E., de Lange F.P., Lau H. Attention induces conservative subjective biases in visual perception. Nat. Neurosci. 2011;14:1513–1515. doi: 10.1038/nn.2948. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Regier D.A., Narrow W.E., Rae D.S., Manderscheid R.W., Locke B.Z., Goodwin F.K. The de facto US mental and addictive disorders service system. Epidemiologic catchment area prospective 1-year prevalence rates of disorders and services. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 1993;50:85–94. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1993.01820140007001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Revheim N., Corcoran C.M., Dias E., Hellmann E., Martinez A., Butler P.D., Lehrfeld J.M., DiCostanzo J., Albert J., Javitt D.C. Reading deficits in schizophrenia and individuals at high clinical risk: relationship to sensory function, course of illness, and psychosocial outcome. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2014;171:949–959. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.13091196. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rouault M., Seow T., Gillan C.M., Fleming S.M. Psychiatric symptom dimensions are associated with dissociable shifts in metacognition but not task performance. Biol. Psychiatry. 2018;84:443–451. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.12.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rounis E., Maniscalco B., Rothwell J.C., Passingham R.E., Lau H. Theta-burst transcranial magnetic stimulation to the prefrontal cortex impairs metacognitive visual awareness. Cogn. Neurosci. 2010;1:165–175. doi: 10.1080/17588921003632529. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saccuzzo D.P., Hirt M., Spencer T.J. Backward masking as a measure of attention in schizophrenia. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 1974;83:512–522. doi: 10.1037/h0037072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shekhar M., Rahnev D. Distinguishing the roles of dorsolateral and anterior PFC in visual metacognition. J. Neurosci. 2018;38:5078–5087. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3484-17.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Slaghuis W.L., Curran C.E. Spatial frequency masking in positive- and negative-symptom schizophrenia. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 1999;108:42–50. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.108.1.42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Soria Bauser D., Thoma P., Aizenberg V., Brune M., Juckel G., Daum I. Face and body perception in schizophrenia: a configural processing deficit? Psychiatry Res. 2012;195:9–17. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.07.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Os J., Kapur S. Schizophrenia. Lancet. 2009;374:635–645. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(09)60995-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waters F., Collerton D., Ffytche D.H., Jardri R., Pins D., Dudley R., Blom J.D., Mosimann U.P., Eperjesi F., Ford S., Laroi F. Visual hallucinations in the psychosis spectrum and comparative information from neurodegenerative disorders and eye disease. Schizophr. Bull. 2014;40(Suppl 4):S233–245. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbu036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watson A.B., Pelli D.G. QUEST: a Bayesian adaptive psychometric method. Percept Psychophys. 1983;33:113–120. doi: 10.3758/bf03202828. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weinberger D.R., Gallhofer B. Cognitive function in schizophrenia. Int. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 1997;12(Suppl 4):S29–36. doi: 10.1097/00004850-199709004-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yamashita O., Sato M.A., Yoshioka T., Tong F., Kamitani Y. Sparse estimation automatically selects voxels relevant for the decoding of fMRI activity patterns. Neuroimage. 2008;42:1414–1429. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.05.050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zylberberg A., Barttfeld P., Sigman M. The construction of confidence in a perceptual decision. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 2012;6:79. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2012.00079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.