Hypertension is related to lower levels of cognitive performance and increased risk for mild cognitive impairment and dementia.1 Cut points for hypertension have been revised downward over time, with current diagnostic criteria of ≥140/90 mm Hg for hypertension and 120/80 to 139/89 mm Hg for prehypertension.1–3 This trend is consistent with well-established findings that: (i) blood pressure (BP) is inversely associated with cognitive performance across the full range of normal and hypertensive BP levels1–3 and; (ii) clinically notable reductions in cardiovascular events and death are observed following BP control to traditional (<140/90 mm Hg) and intensive (<120/90 mm Hg) targets.2–6
Due to significant improvement in physiological outcomes observed by the Systolic Blood Pressure Intervention Trial (SPRINT) research group,4,5 there is considerable interest in intensive management of BP to <120/90 mm Hg. In SPRINT, 9,361 treated and untreated hypertensive patients with systolic BP between 130 and 180 mm Hg, were randomized to receive standard (BP < 140 mm Hg) or intensive treatment (<120 mm Hg). The main exclusions were diabetes mellitus, prior stroke, 1-minute standing BP <110 mm Hg, heart failure, polycystic kidney disease, and age <50 years. The trial was terminated after 3.3 years due to major benefits of intensive over standard treatment, including a 38% reduction in risk of heart failure, a 43% reduction in risk of death from cardiovascular causes, and a 27% reduction in risk of death from any cause.5 Given that cardiovascular disease moderates and modifies associations between blood pressure and cognitive function, one would expect to see parallel benefits to cognitive functioning.
In this issue of the American Journal of Hypertension, Lamar et al.7 report that intensive treatment of hypertension is associated with improved cognitive function. Importantly, the Lamar et al. study deals specifically with Hispanic and Latino adults living in the United States, a population lagging behind other ethnic groups by 10 to 15 percent with respect to BP control using standard criteria. It seems likely that findings from this study will generalize to other populations, albeit empirical confirmation of this assumption is necessary.
The primary sample consisted of treated hypertensive individuals between 44 and 74 years of age (N = 1735) with verified antihypertensive medication histories. Three automated blood pressure measurements were obtained with an OMRON HEM-907 XL automatic sphygmomanometer after a 5-minute rest period. The conventional target of BP control (<140/90 mm Hg) was achieved in 63% of the participants and intensive control (<120/90 mm Hg) in 23%.
Multiple regression analyses were used to relate BP control status to each measure of cognitive functioning utilized (Table 1). Analyses were adjusted for age and various demographic, cardiovascular, and immigration-related variables. Participants achieving traditional BP control exhibited greater verbal fluency with adjustment for age and better information processing speed with full adjustment compared to those who were uncontrolled.
Table 1.
Associations between BP control and performance on each cognitive measure utilized with adjustment for demographic, cardiovascular, and immigration-related variables
Domain indexed | Cognitive measure | Range of scores | Improvement in scoresa | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Traditional vs. uncontrolled | Intensive vs. uncontrolled | Intensive vs. traditional | |||
Verbal fluency | Number of words beginning with a specified letter generated in 60 seconds | 0–50 | N.S. | 1.64 (0.54)b | 1.41 (0.58) |
Information processing speed | Digit Symbol Substitution Test | 0–80 | 1.43 (0.59) | N.S. | N.S. |
Learning | Brief Spanish English Verbal Learning Test: learning trials | 0–45 | N.S. | N.S. | N.S. |
Memory | Brief Spanish English Verbal Learning Test: free recall | 0–15 | N.S. | N.S. | N.S. |
Abbreviation: N.S., nonsignificant.
aResults reported are raw regression coefficients (with standard errors) obtained using model 2: treatment-related control status + age + sex + background + education + Six-Item Screener score + Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale score + current smoking status + presence of diabetes + presence of hypercholesterolemia + health insurance status + income + immigrant generational status + language-based acculturation.
bRemained significant in sensitivity analyses including untreated hypertensives and those with unverified medication histories; a direct comparison of BP criteria was not performed.
Achievement of intensive BP control was associated with better verbal fluency performance regardless of statistical adjustments. Direct comparison of control criteria revealed that participants with intensive BP control had significantly better verbal fluency performance than those with traditional BP control regardless of adjustment, albeit this relationship was only observed in women after sex stratification. Sensitivity analyses including hypertensive participants who were untreated and those with unverified medication histories yielded similar findings for many associations. Number of classes of medications used to achieve treatment targets was not associated with cognitive functioning.
A strength of the study was that patients with a history of psychotropic medication use were excluded from all analyses. This is important given reported interactions between psychotropic and hypertensive medications, which are often disregarded in studies of cognition and BP.8
Table 1 shows raw regression coefficients with the possible range of scores on each cognitive outcome. The improvement in cognitive performance with intensive management of BP was modest, with only a few points gained on each measure. This is true for many studies with traditional BP targets.1 Lamar et al. note the limited clinical significance of the findings, but propose that small improvements could have population-level significance. We agree; even small improvements in cognitive performance have major implications for treatment-related risk reduction at a population level. Seventy-five million adults in the United States have hypertension, many of which have characteristics similar to those in the SPRINT study.9 Thus, we may expect that intensive BP control would decreased population risk for lowered cognitive performance.
However, health care providers treat individuals, not populations.10 Here, the question is whether modest gains justify the risk of intensive treatment goals. Oparil and Lewis11 remind us that “the large benefits of intensive treatment in SPRINT come at some cost.” While there were no statistically significant differences in adverse events between conventional and intense treatment in SPRINT, adverse events such as orthostatic hypotension and falls leading to injury, hypotension, syncope, electrolyte abnormalities, and acute kidney injury or acute renal failure were more frequent in the intensive group as compared to the standard treatment group (4.7 and 2.5%, p 1308). This is a small differences in adverse events, albeit the population attributable risk issue applies here as well as it does to cognitive benefits. Small amounts of risk make a difference in large populations of individuals. But, forewarned is forearmed and SPRINT alerts us to which adverse effects are likely to be seen following similar protocols.
Findings by Lamar et al.7 encourage pushing the envelope of risk vs. benefit because their protocol was similar to the SPRINT protocol in important features (e.g., automated BP measurement), suggesting that improved cognition may be a collateral benefit to lowered cardiovascular events in carefully conducted trials. However, until there are further trials, the decision to treat intensively lies with the health care provider. This decision must be informed and take into account the specifics of SPRINT as they apply to subject selection (nondiabetics free from prior stroke, adults 50 years or older, persons free from prior stroke). Moreover, health care providers who wish to employ intensive treatment strategies would benefit by having full appreciation for individual differences in cognitive ability,12 variability of cognitive performance levels around the BP-cognition regression line13 and individual and racial/ethnic variability in positive response to treatment.14,15 There have been few studies of these variables in relation to successful response to treatment. More are needed.
While the use of automated BP measurement in SPRINT has been criticized as not reflecting office practice in measurement of BP, it is our hope that SPRINT and the Lamar et al. studies will encourage a movement toward accurate measurement of BP, an essential element in patient safety. Strongly supported by the literature,15 Oparil and Lewis11 argue that BP measurement in the office is often flawed. It is common practice for BP measurements to be taken manually with a sphygmomanometer, with little or no rest period prior to measurement, improper cuff size and placement, feet not flat on the floor, etc. These flawed procedures may result in overestimation of BP and consequent overtreatment of hypertension. Automated office BP measurement, as in SPRINT and in Lamar et al., is essential to avoid treating BP to dangerously low levels based on spuriously high BP values. One must avoid rise in BP associated with the alerting response to doctors and nurses (white coat rise in BP); patients should be instructed in systematic home BP measurement (with devices calibrated to the office device). Hopefully, research may tell us to what extent nocturnal monitoring of dipping and nondipping is a necessary adjunct to intensive BP treatment, albeit this is an expensive adjunct to safety monitoring.
The Lamar et al. study has identified important clinical issues with respect to intensive treatment to improve cognition, especially since negative findings have been reported in some trials, even with conventional BP lowering.1,16 Replication of the study in other ethnic populations and with more comprehensive cognitive test batteries is needed.1 Obviously longitudinal studies are absolutely critical, as we must determine if improved cognition in mid-life protects against mild-cognitive impairment and conversion to dementia later in life. Verdecchia et al.6 go to the heart of these issues with respect to pushing the envelope for better cognition: “At the end of 2016, the stage seems set for lower BP targets to reduce the risk for stroke and heart attack (we add cognitive deficit), but cum judicia.”
DISCLOSURE
The authors declared no conflict of interest.
REFERENCES
- 1. Elias MF, Goodell AL, Dore GA. Hypertension and cognitive functioning: a perspective in historical context. Hypertension 2012; 60:260–268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Lonn EM, Yusuf S. Should patients with cardiovascular risk factors receive intensive treatment of hypertension to <120/80 mmHg target? An antagonist view from the HOPE-3 trial (Heart Outcomes Evaluation-3). Circulation 2016; 134:1311–1313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Elias MF, Wolf PA, D’Agostino RB, Cobb J, White LR. Untreated blood pressure level is inversely related to cognitive functioning: the Framingham Study. Am J Epidemiol 1993; 138:353–364. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Williamson JD, Supiano MA, Applegate WB, Berlowitz DR, Campbell RC, Chertow GM, Fine LJ, Haley WE, Hawfield AT, Ix JH, Kitzman DW, Kostis JB, Krousel-Wood MA, Launer LJ, Oparil S, Rodriguez CJ, Roumie CL, Shorr RI, Sink KM, Wadley VG, Whelton PK, Whittle J, Woolard NF, Wright JT, Jr, Pajewski NM; SPRINT Research Group Intensive vs standard blood pressure control and cardiovascular disease outcomes in adults aged ≥75 years: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA 2016; 315:2673–2682. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Wright JT, Jr, Williamson JD, Whelton PK, Snyder JK, Sink KM, Rocco MV, Reboussin DM, Rahman M, Oparil S, Lewis CE, Kimmel PL, Johnson KC, Goff DC, Jr, Fine LJ, Cutler JA, Cushman WC, Cheung AK, Ambrosius WT; SPRINT Research Group A randomized trial of intensive versus standard blood-pressure control. N Engl J Med 2015; 373:2103–2116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Verdecchia P, Angeli F, Reboldi G. How low should we go with blood pressure? Circ Res 2017; 120:27–29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Lamar M, Wu D, Durazo-Arvizu RA, et al. Cognitive associates of current and more intensive control of hypertension: findings from the Hispanic Community Health Study/Study of Latinos. Am J Hyperten in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Lam YW, Shepherd AM. Drug interactions in hypertensive patients. Pharmacokinetic, pharmacodynamic and genetic considerations. Clin Pharmacokinet 1990; 18:295–317. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Merai R, Siegel C, Rakotz M, Basch P, Wright J, Wong B, Thorpe P; DHSc CDC grand rounds: a public health approach to detect and control hypertension. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep 2016; 65:1261–1264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Fuster V. No such thing as ideal blood pressure: a case for personalized medicine. J Am Coll Cardiol 2016; 67:3014–3015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Oparil S, Lewis CE. Should patients with cardiovascular risk factors receive intensive treatment of hypertension to <120/80 mmHg target? A protagonist view from the SPRINT trial (Systolic Blood Pressure Intervention Trial). Circulation 2016; 134:1308–1310. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Gruszka A, Matthews G, Szymura B. (eds), Handbook of Individual Differences in Cognition: Attention, Memory and Executive Control. Springer: New York, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- 13. Robbins MA, Elias MF, Elias PK, Budge MM. Blood pressure and cognitive function in an African-American and a Caucasian-American sample: the Maine-Syracuse Study. Psychosom Med 2005; 67:707–714. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Spence JD. Individualized therapy for hypertension. Hypertension 2006; 47:e11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Messerli FH, White WB, Staessen JA. If only cardiologists did properly measure blood pressure. Blood pressure recordings in daily practice and clinical trials. J Am Coll Cardiol 2002; 40:2201–2203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. American College of Cardiology. HOPE-3: Effect of BP Lowering and Statin Use on Cognitive Function American College of Cardiology; <http://www.acc.org/latest-in-cardiology/articles/2016/11/10/15/11/sun-445pmet-hope-3-the-effect-of-bp-and-cholesterol-lowering-on-cognition-aha-2016> 2016. Accessed 15 February 2017. [Google Scholar]