Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2020 May 26;9:e52658. doi: 10.7554/eLife.52658

Fast and reversible neural inactivation in macaque cortex by optogenetic stimulation of GABAergic neurons

Abhishek De 1,2, Yasmine El-Shamayleh 3, Gregory D Horwitz 2,
Editors: Joshua I Gold4, Michael Schmid5
PMCID: PMC7329331  PMID: 32452766

Abstract

Optogenetic techniques for neural inactivation are valuable for linking neural activity to behavior but they have serious limitations in macaques. To achieve powerful and temporally precise neural inactivation, we used an adeno-associated viral (AAV) vector carrying the channelrhodopsin-2 gene under the control of a Dlx5/6 enhancer, which restricts expression to GABAergic neurons. We tested this approach in the primary visual cortex, an area where neural inactivation leads to interpretable behavioral deficits. Optical stimulation modulated spiking activity and reduced visual sensitivity profoundly in the region of space represented by the stimulated neurons. Rebound firing, which can have unwanted effects on neural circuits following inactivation, was not observed, and the efficacy of the optogenetic manipulation on behavior was maintained across >1000 trials. We conclude that this inhibitory cell-type-specific optogenetic approach is a powerful and spatiotemporally precise neural inactivation tool with broad utility for probing the functional contributions of cortical activity in macaques.

Research organism: Rhesus macaque

Introduction

A major goal of systems neuroscience is to understand how neural activity mediates behavior. Neural inactivation techniques are central to this endeavor (Wurtz, 2015). However, these techniques can have unintended consequences that complicate data interpretation (Abraham, 2008; Goold and Nicoll, 2010; Goshen et al., 2011; Sokolova and Mody, 2008; Stemmler and Koch, 1999; Turrigiano et al., 1998). For example, by impairing task performance, neural inactivation can cause animals to explore new task strategies for acquiring reward. This change in strategy may change the information flow through neural circuits. To avoid these complications, inactivation methods are needed that can be reversed more quickly than these circuit-level changes can occur.

Optogenetics is the fastest method for reversible neural inactivation currently available. In rodents, optogenetic inactivation has revealed links between neural activity and behavior that would have been difficult to discover with traditional, slower inactivation methods based on injection of pharmacological agents, cortical cooling, or lesioning (Goshen et al., 2011; Hanks et al., 2015; Yartsev et al., 2018). Optogenetic inactivation has already been used in a few pioneering studies to perturb the behavior of macaque monkeys (Acker et al., 2016; Afraz et al., 2015; Cavanaugh et al., 2012; Fetsch et al., 2018). The approach taken in these studies was to reduce neuronal spiking by activating hyperpolarizing opsins (eNpHR, Arch, or Jaws). The directness of this approach facilitates the interpretation of behavioral effects. However, the behavioral effects produced this way have been small, perhaps because most promoters used in viral vectors drive expression in many neuronal types, and suppression of inhibitory neurons may counteract suppression of excitatory neurons.

An alternative approach, which has been successful in rodents, is to selectively activate inhibitory neurons with channelrhodopsin-2 (ChR2) (Cone et al., 2019; Glickfeld et al., 2013; Guo et al., 2014; Khan et al., 2018; McBride et al., 2019). This approach has two advantages. First, it is based on the opening of ion channels, which conduct more ions per photon absorbed than ion pumps. Second, it leverages the dense local connectivity and low synaptic failure rates of GABAergic neurons to suppress long-range excitatory signaling locally and robustly (Isaacson and Scanziani, 2011; Kubota et al., 2015; Packer and Yuste, 2011; Wiegert et al., 2017).

To test the efficacy of this approach for cortical inactivation in macaques, we injected area V1 of three rhesus monkeys with a viral vector containing a cell-type-specific promoter (AAV–mDlx5/6–ChR2) (Dimidschstein et al., 2016). In this study, we confirm the specificity of GABAergic neuronal transduction in macaque cortex and demonstrate that illumination of the injection site modulates spiking activity. We also show that illumination impairs visual sensitivity profoundly, reversibly, and reliably at the receptive fields of the illuminated neurons but not outside. We conclude that optogenetic stimulation of inhibitory neurons is a powerful method for inactivating regions of the macaque monkey brain with high spatial and temporal precision.

Results

Selectivity of opsin expression

A previous study showed that an AAV vector carrying the gene for the fluorescent reporter, GFP, under the control of the mDlx5/6 enhancer, transduced V1 GABAergic neurons in a marmoset with 93% selectivity (Dimidschstein et al., 2016). To determine whether AAV–mDlx5/6–ChR2–mCherry has similar selectivity in macaque, we injected V1 of one animal (monkey 1) and examined the tissue histologically (Figure 1 and Figure 1—figure supplement 1). mCherry-positive cells had non-pyramidal morphologies, consistent with them being GABAergic. Similar histological results with this viral vector have been described in macaques previously (Scerra et al., 2019).

Figure 1. Immunohistochemical analysis of transduction by AAV1-mDlx5/6-ChR2-mCherry.

(A) A histological section of V1 from monkey 1 stained with DAPI (blue) and antibodies against parvalbumin (green) and mCherry (red). Scale bar is 250 μm. The pial surface is indicated by the dashed gray curve and the border between layers 1 and 2/3 is indicated by the solid gray curve. The laminar specificity is an idiosyncrasy of this particular injection; see Figure 1—figure supplement 1 for a histological section of the V1/V2 border. (B) Locations of cell bodies in (A) expressing mCherry (red), parvalbumin (green), or both (‘+').

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Immunohistochemical analysis of transduction by AAV1-mDlx5/6-ChR2-mCherry.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

(A) A histological section of V1/V2 from monkey 1 processed with antibodies against parvalbumin (green) and mCherry (red), imaged at 10X. Scale bar is 1 mm. (B) Locations of cell bodies in (A) expressing mCherry (red), parvalbumin (green) or both (‘+'). Sensitivity analysis of AAV–mDlx5/6–ChR2–mCherry transduction to PV+ neurons in two regions of efficient transduction.

Most mCherry-positive neurons co-expressed parvalbumin (468/543), a marker for 75% of GABAergic neurons in macaque V1 (Van Brederode et al., 1990). This high level of co-expression is consistent with selective transduction of GABAergic neurons and is sufficiently high to suggest that parvalbumin-positive neurons were transduced with particularly high efficiency (p<0.005; binomial test).

Optogenetic control of neural activity

To test whether ChR2 expression was sufficiently strong to perturb neural activity, we recorded extracellular spiking responses from single- and multi-units near the injection sites in two other monkeys (monkeys 2 and 3) while they performed a contrast detection task. Most sites were visually driven (46/56, response to a low-contrast Gabor stimulus greater than baseline firing rate; 19/56, p<0.05; Mann-Whitney U test, Figure 2—figure supplement 1). Given our selection criteria, all sites were significantly modulated by optical stimulation (p<0.06; Mann-Whitney U test; see Methods). Some units were excited by optical stimulation (Figure 2A) whereas others were suppressed (Figure 2B). At 38 of the 56 sites, optical stimulation increased spiking. Excitation was prevalent in our dataset because we searched for sites at which optical stimulation produced an audible change in the baseline firing rate (Figure 2C). The mean latency to response was 14±26 (SD) ms and was <5 ms at 11 sites (Figure 2—figure supplement 2A–B). Neurons excited at short latency (<5 ms) presumably expressed ChR2 and suppressed other neurons via synaptic inhibition. The latency of suppression was longer than the latency of excitation, but this comparison is challenging because baseline firing rates were low (Figure 2—figure supplement 2C, Figure 2—figure supplement 3).

Figure 2. Optogenetic activation and suppression of single- and multi-units.

(A,B) Responses (in impulses per second, ips) of two example single units, aligned to the onset of optical stimulation, which lasted 300 ms (blue rectangle). Rasters (tick marks) and peristimulus time histograms (blue traces) are shown for an activated single unit (A) and a suppressed single unit (B). Insets: Mean spike waveform (thick black curve) and noise waveform (thick gray curve) ± 1 standard deviation (thin curves). (C) Scatter plot of firing rate on laser trials against baseline firing rate of units from monkey 2 (squares) and monkey 3 (circles). Data from example activated and suppressed units are circled in red. Firing rates were computed during optical stimulation or the equivalent epoch on control trials.

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Analysis of visually driven responses at activated and suppressed sites.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

Visually driven firing rate was computed during the Gabor stimulus presentation period (200 ms) and plotted against the baseline firing rate. A total of 46 sites were driven by visual stimuli. 19 of those were significantly visually driven.
Figure 2—figure supplement 2. Analysis of latency at activated and suppressed sites.

Figure 2—figure supplement 2.

(A) Analysis of latency to first spike at activated sites. Latency was defined as the time to first spike following optical stimulation on each trial. Black points represent medians across trials within a site, and the lower and the upper end of vertical black lines represent the 25th and 75th percentiles. (B) Histogram of average latencies to first spike following optogenetic activation. (C) Histogram of latencies at activated (black) and suppressed (gray) sites. For each site, firing rate was computed in a sliding 50-ms window from −50–150 ms after the laser was turned off. This firing rate was compared against the pre-laser firing rate (computed in a 50-ms window before optical stimulation). The time at which the firing rates in the two windows differed significantly was defined as the latency (p<0.05, Wilcoxon rank sum test).
Figure 2—figure supplement 3. Rasters from all of the 18 suppressed sites.

Figure 2—figure supplement 3.

Responses are aligned to the onset of optical stimulation.

Neural activity suppression using halorhodopsins in monkeys is typically followed by a rebound of activity at the termination of optical stimulation (Acker et al., 2016; Fetsch et al., 2018). We did not observe such rebounds with AAV–mDlx5/6–ChR2. We compared average firing rates at 18 suppressed sites in a 50-ms window before and after optical stimulation. At one example site, the pre-laser firing rate exceeded the post-laser firing rate (22 vs. 0 impulses/sec, p<0.001, Wilcoxon signed rank test, Figure 3A), consistent with sustained suppression. At a different example site, the pre-laser firing rate was lower than the post-laser firing rate, consistent with a small rebound (10 vs. 25 impulses/sec, p=0.02, Wilcoxon signed rank test, Figure 3B). Such rebounds were rare; post-laser firing rates exceeded pre-laser firing rates at only 2 of 18 sites (Figure 3C).

Figure 3. Analysis of activity rebound and recovery at suppressed sites.

Figure 3.

(A) Responses of a single unit with a pre-laser firing rate that exceeded the post-laser firing rate. Recovery time to the baseline firing rate was 195 ms (vertical gray line). (B) Responses of another single unit with a post-laser firing rate that exceeded the pre-laser firing rate. Recovery time was 15 ms. (C) Scatter plot of pre-laser firing rates against post-laser firing rates. For each site, post-laser firing rate was computed in a sliding 50-ms window from 0 to 200 ms after the laser was switched off. The ranges of post-laser firing rates are plotted as black lines and averages are plotted as black points. Data from neurons in (A) and (B) are circled in red. (D) Histogram of recovery times following optogenetic suppression. Recovery times of example units are marked with red tick marks, and the median is marked with a triangle.

Activity at most suppressed sites recovered to baseline levels gradually after laser termination. We measured this recovery time by computing the first time at which the average spike count in a 50-ms sliding window returned to 90% of the pre-laser firing rate. Recovery times ranged from 0 to 215 ms (Figure 3D) with roughly half of the sites recovering within 100 ms (median = 97.5 ms). These data demonstrate that suppression persists several tens of milliseconds after laser termination.

Optogenetic control of behavior

To evaluate the behavioral efficacy of optogenetic stimulation, we trained monkeys 2 and 3 to perform two visually demanding tasks. Reward contingencies were independent of laser stimulation in both tasks.

In the visually guided saccade task, a target appeared inside the receptive fields (RFs) of the stimulated V1 neurons on a random subset of trials and outside on other trials (Figure 4A–B). Data from an example block of trials from each monkey show the main results (Figure 4C–D). On control trials, both monkeys made accurate saccades to most target locations. On laser trials, the monkeys failed to make saccades into the RFs of the optically stimulated neurons. Saccades were unaffected when the target appeared at other locations, indicating that the optogenetic effect was retinotopically specific. On laser trials when the target appeared inside the RFs, monkey 2 typically maintained fixation, and monkey 3 typically made leftward ~10° saccades. Similar behaviors were observed on catch trials in which no target was shown (Figure 4E–F, see Materials and methods). The inaccuracy of saccades made by monkey 3 into the left visual field was likely due to repeated electrode penetrations in the midbrain of this animal that were unrelated to the current experiments (Figure 4—figure supplement 1).

Figure 4. Effect of optogenetic inactivation of V1 on visually guided saccades.

(A) Task design. (B) Timing of events. The small overshoot in the laser trace accurately reflects the temporal profile of the light. (C,D) Eye position traces on control (gray) and laser (blue) trials are shown from 0 to 300 ms after the fixation point was extinguished for one block of trials. The RF location of the illuminated neurons (gray filled circle) and the target locations outside of the RF (gray open circles) are highlighted. (E,F) Eye positions on catch trials from the same blocks as (C,D).

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Effect of optogenetic inactivation on visually guided saccades from monkey 3.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

Data are shown from each individual block of trials. Gray traces show saccades on control trials, and the blue traces show saccades on laser trials. The position of the target inside the RF of the illuminated neurons (gray filled circle) and outside (gray unfilled circle) are shown.
Figure 4—figure supplement 2. Effects of optogenetic inactivation of V1 on visually guided saccade accuracy and latency.

Figure 4—figure supplement 2.

(A,B) Saccade accuracy (the average distance between saccade end points and target location) on control and laser trials. Data from trials in which the target appeared inside the RFs of the stimulated neurons (filled symbols) were analyzed separately from interleaved trials in which the target appeared in other locations (open symbols). (C) Histograms of saccade latency on control (gray) and laser (blue) trials when the target was presented inside the RF. (E) Histograms of saccade latency on control (gray) and laser (blue) trials when the target was presented outside the RF. (B,D,F) Data from monkey 3 in the same format as (A,C,E).

We collected data in 10 sessions from monkey 2 (16 blocks of trials) and 7 sessions from monkey 3 (20 blocks of trials). Within each session, we calculated the distance between saccade end points and target locations. When the target appeared inside the RFs of stimulated neurons, the saccade end points tended to be closer to the target on control trials than on laser trials (p<0.002 for monkey 2, p=0.03 for monkey 3; Wilcoxon signed rank tests). When the target appeared in other locations, the saccade endpoints were similarly close to the target on control and laser trials (p=0.92 for monkey 2, p=0.38 for monkey 3; Wilcoxon signed rank tests; Figure 4—figure supplement 2A–B). Saccade latencies were greater on laser trials than on control trials when targets were inside the RFs (p<0.0001 for monkey 2 and 3; Mann-Whitney U tests; Figure 4—figure supplement 2C–D) but not when targets were elsewhere (p=0.90 for monkey 2 and p=0.41 for monkey 3; Mann-Whitney U tests; Figure 4—figure supplement 2E–F).

To confirm that the deficit in task performance was not purely oculomotor, we trained monkeys 2 and 3 to perform a contrast detection task that required saccades to targets outside of the RFs of the stimulated neurons (Figure 5A–B: see Materials and methods). An example block of trials from each monkey demonstrates the main results. Both monkeys detected the visual stimulus more frequently on control than on laser trials (proportion of hits on control vs. laser trials; p<0.001 for monkey 2 and monkey 3; binomial tests for equality of proportions; Figure 5C–D). This performance deficit was also reflected in psychometric functions (Figure 5C–D inset) and in the contrast values selected by the staircase procedure (Figure 5E–F). Neither monkey was able to detect the visual stimulus with above-chance accuracy on laser trials even at the maximum stimulus contrast achievable. Saccades to the stimulus location were never required, and thus the behavioral effects produced by optical stimulation in this task cannot be explained by an oculomotor deficit.

Figure 5. Effect of V1 inactivation on visual contrast detection.

Figure 5.

(A) Task design. (B) Timing of events. (C) Performance of monkey 2 over one block of trials. Hits (H) and misses (M) are proportions of Gabor-present trials that were answered correctly and incorrectly, respectively. Correct rejections (CR) and false alarms (FA) are proportions of Gabor-absent trials that were answered correctly and incorrectly, respectively. Insets show psychometric functions on control (gray) and laser (blue) trials. Symbol size in insets reflects the number of trials that contributed to each data point. (E) Contrasts selected by the staircase procedure on control (gray) and laser (blue) trials. (D,F) Performance of monkey 3 in the same format as (C,E). Luminance contrast could not exceed 0.66 because the gray background was close to the upper limit of the display range.

In one session, the Gabor stimulus location was randomized across trials, confirming the retinotopic specificity of the effect (Figure 6). Additional control experiments confirmed that the monkeys were able to make saccades to both target locations irrespective of optical stimulation (data not shown) and showed that performance on control trials was unaffected by the interleaved laser trials (Figure 6—figure supplement 1).

Figure 6. Retinotopic specificity of optogenetic effects on contrast detection.

Data are from a single session consisting of 4 blocks of trials from monkey 2. (A) On each trial, the Gabor stimulus appeared at one of three randomly interleaved locations (X, Y, or Z), all of which were 9.6° away from the fixation point (central black dot). Locations Y and Z were on the vertical and horizontal meridians, respectively. (B) The proportions of hits (H), misses (M), correct rejections (CR) and false alarms (FA) are plotted in the same format as in Figure 5C. The laser reduced the monkey’s contrast sensitivity when the Gabor stimulus appeared at the receptive fields of the transduced neurons (X, gray circle). No significant effect was observed at locations Y and Z (C,D).

Figure 6.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1. Contrast detection thresholds were stable across seven blocks collected during a single session from monkey 3.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1.

In the first block, the laser power was low (0.8 mW) and performance was statistically indistinguishable on laser and control trials. For this block only, data from laser and control trials were pooled. In the subsequent blocks, the laser power was >60 mW, and performance was significantly impaired on laser trials (p<0.05 in all cases, binomial test of proportions). For these blocks, data from control trials are only presented. (A) Proportion of hits in each block is plotted in chronological order. (B) d’ in each block is plotted in chronological order. (C) Psychometric functions in the first and the pooled subsequent blocks did not differ significantly (p=0.11, Likelihood ratio test of separate Weibull fits to the first and the subsequent blocks data versus the best single fit to the pooled data). (D) Luminance contrast values selected by the staircase procedure as a function of trial number in each block.

We collected data in 11 sessions from monkey 2 (69 blocks of trials) and 12 sessions from monkey 3 (81 blocks of trials). In almost every session (10/11 in monkey 2, 11/12 in monkey 3), the proportion of hits on control trials was significantly greater than on laser trials (binomial tests for equality of proportions, p<0.05, Figure 7A–B). An analysis of sensitivity indices (d’) confirmed that this change in performance was consistent with a reduction in sensitivity and inconsistent with a pure change in criterion (Figure 7C–D, Figure 7—figure supplement 1). In most blocks of trials (52/69 in monkey 2 and 63/81 in monkey 3), optical stimulation increased detection thresholds beyond the limits of the display, an event that occurred rarely on control trials (0/63 blocks in monkey 2, 8/81 blocks in monkey 3).

Figure 7. Effect of V1 inactivation on visual contrast detection across multiple sessions.

(A) Scatter plot showing proportion of hits on control trials against laser trials from each session in monkey 2. Sessions with significantly fewer hits on laser trials than control trials are shown in black (p<0.05, binomial test for equal proportions). Error bars represent the standard error of mean. (B) Data from monkey 3 in the same format as (A). (C) Scatterplot of d’ from control trials plotted against d’ from laser trials from each session performed by monkey 2. (D) Data from monkey 3 in the same format as (C).

Figure 7.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1. Analysis of the relationship between d’ and c-criterion.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1.

(A) Noise (gray, 𝒩 (0,1)) and signal (blue) distributions of decision variables with same variance are plotted. The effect of the laser is assumed to reduce the mean of the signal distribution. Shown are the monkey’s criterion (vertical dashed line) and the optimal criterion (blue triangle) for each signal distribution. The optimal criterion is the point of intersection between the signal and noise distributions. Even if the monkey’s criterion does not depend on laser power, c-criterion changes. This is because c-criterion is the difference between the optimal and the monkey’s criterion. (B) d’ is plotted against the c-criterion for different signal distributions. Under this model, changes in d’ are conflated with changes in c-criterion.

As laser power increased, errors became more common, which caused the staircase procedure to increase the stimulus contrast rapidly (Figure 8A). The magnitude of the behavioral effect increased steeply with laser intensity between 12.8 and 22.3 mW, and it saturated by 30.0 mW (Figure 8B). Behavior on control trials was not significantly affected by changes in laser power (r=−0.15, p=0.78; Spearman’s correlation between d’ on control trials from each block and laser power).

Figure 8. Effect of laser power and repeated optical stimulation on contrast detection.

(A) Contrast values selected by the staircase procedure on laser trials (solid lines) and interleaved control trials (dashed lines) across seven blocks. (B) The difference in d’ between control and laser trials as a function of laser power calculated from the data in (A). A Naka-Rushton fit to the data is shown in black. (C) Differences in d’ between control and laser trials as a function of trial number in each session. Each session consisted of at least five blocks of 120 trials. The duration of an individual trial was 2.80 ± 0.51 s (mean ± SD), and the number of trials per session was 813 ± 253. Points are means and error bars are standard error of the mean (SEM). SEM was not plotted for the final two points, each of which represent data from a single session. (D) Scatter plot of the differences in d’ for early trials (1–480) vs. late trials (480–beyond) within each session.

Figure 8.

Figure 8—figure supplement 1. Correlation between optogenetic effects on neural activity and behavior.

Figure 8—figure supplement 1.

(A) Effect of laser power on firing rate across seven blocks from a single session. The difference in laser-evoked firing rate and baseline firing rate is plotted as a function of laser power. Behavioral effects for these blocks of trials are shown in Figure 8B. (B) Relationship between neurophysiological and behavioral effects at the activated (black) and suppressed (gray) sites during the Gabor contrast detection task. Neurophysiological effects were computed as the absolute value of the difference between laser-evoked and baseline firing rate, divided by the sum of two.
Figure 8—figure supplement 2. Analysis of visual sensitivity in monkey 2.

Figure 8—figure supplement 2.

The data in panels (B,C and D) were collected 840 days after the vector injections, and 663 days after the termination of optogenetic silencing experiments that contributed to the manuscript. (A) Electrophysiologically mapped receptive fields (RFs) before (unfilled circles) and after (filled circles) AAV injections into area V1. The polygon (black outline) enclosing all the RFs represents the region of interest where monkey’s visual sensitivity could be affected. (B) Saccade accuracy data from a visually guided saccade task. On each trial, a target appeared, the fixation point disappeared, and the monkey was rewarded for making a saccade to the target within ~300 ms. Targets were randomly drawn from two 7 × 7° grids (98 locations), one in the upper visual field and one in the lower visual field. (10 repetitions at each location). The size of each disk represents the proportion of saccades made to the corresponding target (landing within a 5 × 5° window). Each target location tested is plotted in a unique color which is preserved across panels. The monkey’s performance was ≥60% at all the tested locations. (C) Average saccade latencies are plotted as a function of target location. (D) Saccade end points are plotted as a function of target location in the unique color assigned to each location. Relative to saccades up and left, saccades down and right were less likely to be correct, had longer latencies, and were less accurate. The ‘shearing’ of the saccade end point distributions relative to the target positions is due to a small tilt in the infrared camera (SMI Inc, Hi-Speed Primate) relative to the eye.

Optogenetic modulations of neural activity were linked to effects on behavior across these trials (r=0.36, p=0.43; Spearman’s correlation between neural laser modulation index and difference in d’ between control and laser trials; Figure 8—figure supplement 1). Pooling data across all blocks of trials reduced the correlation (r=0.16, p=0.23). Pooling the data reduced statistical power due to covariates across blocks of trials that exerted different effects on neurophysiological and behavioral outcomes (e.g. fiber position, stimulus location in the visual field, and quality of neural recordings). These covariates were held fixed in the data shown in Figure 8A–B.

In a previous study, the behavioral effects produced by optogenetic silencing of neurons in area MT using the suppressive opsin, Jaws (red-shifted cruxhalorhospsin), decreased over tens of minutes (Fetsch et al., 2018). To determine whether a similar phenomenon occurred with ChR2-mediated inactivation, we analyzed 4 experimental sessions (28 blocks of trials) from monkey 2 and 5 sessions (33 blocks of trials) from monkey 3. From these sessions, we considered only the subset of blocks with identical laser power.

The behavioral effects we observed were consistently large over the course of ~1000 trials (or ~50 mins). We quantified the behavioral effect as the difference in d’ between laser and control trials within each block. The behavioral effect varied little as a function of block number, (r=0.18, p=0.59; Spearman’s correlation between block number and d’ averaged across sessions; Figure 8C). It was also consistent within individual sessions; linear regression slopes of the behavioral effect as a function of block number in each session did not differ significantly from zero (p=0.57, Student’s t-test). For comparison with previous work (Fetsch et al., 2018), we calculated the behavioral effect in early and late trials within each session. Unlike the previous work, the behavioral effect did not differ between the first 480 trials (4 blocks) and the subsequent trials, suggesting the absence of compensatory changes under the conditions of the current study (p=0.79, Student’s t-test, Figure 8D).

Discussion

The fast activation and inactivation of neurons afforded by optogenetics has revolutionized our understanding of the nervous systems of rodents and invertebrates. Understanding the primate brain at a similar level of detail is facilitated by optogenetics in the macaque monkey—a model organism with a brain structure similar to humans that can be trained to perform complex behavioral tasks. Rapid activation was already feasible in primates using microsimulation or optogenetics, and now rapid inactivation is too.

We achieved inactivation by stimulating GABAergic neurons in macaque V1 and measured electrophysiological and behavioral consequences. First, we showed that the AAV–mDlx5/6–ChR2 vector targeted ChR2 expression to GABAergic neurons in area V1. Second, we showed that optical stimulation modulated the activity of neurons near the injection site. Third, we showed that optical stimulation impaired visual sensitivity in two behavioral tasks. The reduction in sensitivity was specific to trials in which optical stimulation was delivered and to the RF location of the stimulated neurons, demonstrating the temporal and spatial precision of the inactivation. Laser-induced modulations of neural responses were rapid, rebound activity following light pulses was negligible, and behavioral effects were consistent across ~1000 trials.

Below, we compare the results of our study with those of previous studies that used optogenetic neural inactivation to perturb macaque behavior. We then discuss the effect of optogenetic stimulation of V1 on eye movements and ways in which the method could be improved. Finally, we discuss potential applications of AAV–mDlx5/6–ChR2 for understanding primate brain function.

Comparison with optogenetic inactivation studies

Four previous studies used optogenetic inactivation to perturb monkey behavior (Acker et al., 2016; Afraz et al., 2015; Cavanaugh et al., 2012; Fetsch et al., 2018). The two studies most similar to ours quantified the effect of neural inactivation on behavior as changes in visual discrimination thresholds on 2AFC tasks (Afraz et al., 2015; Fetsch et al., 2018). In one study, inactivation of inferotemporal cortical neurons raised thresholds for classifying face stimuli on the basis of gender (Afraz et al., 2015). In the other, inactivation of MT cortical neurons biased judgements of visual motion direction (Fetsch et al., 2018). In both cases, threshold changes were smaller than those we observed (~5% vs. >100%, Figure 7).

The threshold changes we observed were large for potentially several reasons. First, we excited ChR2-expressing inhibitory neurons to reduce the spiking of excitatory neurons. Stimulation of a small number of inhibitory neurons can suppress the activity of a large number of excitatory neurons (Wiegert et al., 2017). Second, we used ChR2, which conducts more ions per absorbed photon than ion pumps (Jaws and ArchT). Third, we used higher laser power (4–160 mW vs. ~2 mW and ~12 mW; Figures 48). Fourth, we inactivated area V1, an area that is indispensable for the behaviors we studied (Koerner and Teuber, 1973; Merigan et al., 1993; Radoeva et al., 2008). Higher-order visual cortical areas may be sufficiently interconnected to allow one or more areas to compensate for others with regard to the behaviors tested. An intriguing, and now-testable, hypothesis is that the spared visual sensitivity following visual cortical lesions is due to the engagement of slow compensatory mechanisms, not the unmasking of normally functioning pathways (Leopold, 2012).

We interpreted the stimulation-induced change in the monkeys’ performance as a change in sensitivity, and it is inconsistent with a change in criterion alone. Additionally, the brevity and unpredictability of the optical stimulation makes large, consistent, selective changes in criterion on laser trials unlikely. Nevertheless, we cannot rule out the possibility that the optical stimulation affected sensitivity and criterion together (Figure 7—figure supplement 1).

Comparison with non-selective optogenetic stimulation of V1

Illumination of ChR2-expressing neurons in area V1 causes monkeys to make saccades into the RFs of the stimulated neurons under some conditions (Jazayeri et al., 2012). This behavior is consistent with the perception of a phosphene (Tehovnik et al., 2003). In our study, however, monkeys rarely made saccades into the RFs of the stimulated neurons, suggesting that they did not experience a phosphene. This result held on trials requiring a saccade to a visual target inside the RFs of the stimulated neurons and on trials in which no target was shown, a condition similar to the key condition in the study of Jazayeri et al., 2012. In principle, detection of the optical stimulation could have provided a cue for acquiring reward in the visually guided saccade task. Having sensed the optical stimulation, and seen no target, the monkey could have increased its reward rate by making a saccade into the RFs of the stimulated neurons. The fact that the monkeys did not behave this way suggests that they were unable to detect the stimulation, or at least were unable to use it to direct saccades. Sensing the optical stimulation would not have been useful for increasing reward rate in the contrast detection task.

We attribute the difference between studies to the population of V1 neurons stimulated. We used a Dlx5/6 enhancer to express ChR2 selectively in inhibitory neurons whereas Jazayeri et al., 2012 used the human synapsin I promoter, which drives expression in both excitatory and inhibitory neurons (Nathanson et al., 2009). One hypothesis is that phosphenes are caused by spikes in a subset of excitatory projection neurons. In this case, pan-GABAergic stimulation would not be expected to produce phosphenes, but stimulation of specific GABAergic subtypes might. For example, stimulation of VIP-expressing neurons might produce a phosphene through disinhibition of excitatory neurons (Cone et al., 2019).

Effect of laser power and optical fiber insertion on cortex

Over the course of this study, monkeys 2 and 3 acquired visual deficits in areas of the visual field corresponding to the regions of V1 inactivated. To ask whether the optogenetic manipulations produced long-lasting visual deficits, we conducted behavioral experiments in monkey 2 twenty-two months after the final optogenetic experiment was conducted (Figure 8—figure supplement 2). Visual sensitivity, assessed by the probability, latency, and accuracy of visually guided saccades, was reduced in the optogenetically manipulated lower-right visual field relative to the unmanipulated upper-left visual field, but the deficit was subtle. We presume that this deficit reflects cortical damage, which could be due to tissue heating by the light, repeated penetrations by optical fibers, or single-unit recordings that were made in this animal for three years prior to commencing the current study. While a comparable behavioral dataset could not be obtained from monkey 3, histological analysis of the calcarine sulcus, where most of the optogenetic manipulations were made in this animal, revealed nothing unusual (e.g. areas of necrosis, burn marks, etc.) (data not shown). They did reveal electrode/optical fiber tracks, the expected gliosis associated with these tracks, and healthy-looking mCherry+ neurons that were similar in morphology and density to those in monkey 1.

The laser power used in the current study spanned a broad range (4–160 mW for 200–300 ms) and, on average, was higher than that used in other studies (Afraz et al., 2015; Cavanaugh et al., 2012; Dai et al., 2014; El-Shamayleh et al., 2017; Fetsch et al., 2018; Gerits et al., 2012; Inoue et al., 2015; Ohayon et al., 2013; Stauffer et al., 2016; Tamura et al., 2017). Given the stimulation parameters we used, (450 nm light conducted through 300 µm-diameter optical fibers), we likely heated tissue near the fiber tip by several °C in many of our experiments (Arias-Gil et al., 2016). However, the consistency of the behavioral effect within individual sessions after repeated optical stimulation argues against acute damage (Figure 8C–D).

The tissue damage produced by optogenetic manipulations can be mitigated by using artificial dura (Nandy et al., 2019; Ruiz et al., 2013) and red-shifted or step-function opsins (Berndt et al., 2009). Artificial dura allows non-invasive optical stimulation of the superficial cortical layers, reducing mechanical damage. Red-shifted opsins are activated by long-wavelength light, which heats tissue less and thus causes less thermal damage than short-wavelength lights do. The neural activity produced by step-function opsins outlast the light pulses required to trigger them, allowing brief, safe light pulses to produce longer lasting stimulation events (Gong et al., 2020).

Potential applications of AAV–mDlx5/6–ChR2

Optogenetic stimulation of inhibitory neurons using AAV–mDlx5/6–ChR2 facilitates at least three broad categories of experiments. The first category includes experiments in which slow neural inactivation precludes data collection, for example, experiments probing the neural substrate of life-sustaining processes (e.g. breathing) (Baertsch et al., 2018; Simonyan, 2014). Less extreme examples include the inactivation of oculomotor structures that are necessary for stable visual fixation, a simple oculomotor behavior without which more complicated behaviors are difficult to study (Goffart et al., 2012; Krauzlis et al., 2017). Experiments in which inactivation induces compensatory changes in task strategy (Paolini and McKenzie, 1997) or the routing of neural signals also fall in this category (Cowey, 2010; Kinoshita et al., 2019; Leopold, 2012; Mori et al., 2006).

The second class of experiments are those that address questions about the functional significance of spike timing. Monkeys can learn to use signals in sensory cortices at particular times relative to external and internal events to mediate their behavior (Poort et al., 2012; Roelfsema et al., 1998; Seidemann et al., 1998). Just as electrical microstimulation can be used to reveal the contribution of spikes added to sensory representations at specific times, optogenetic inactivation can be used, complementarily, to eliminate spikes. Indeed, optogenetic inactivation was used recently to show that spiking activity in the frontal eye fields of macaques contributes to memory-guided saccades before, during, and after target presentation (Acker et al., 2016). Future studies may reveal differences between the transient and sustained phases of sensory-, decision- and movement-related signals for guiding behavior (Freedman et al., 2001; Hegdé, 2008; Ibos and Freedman, 2017; Roelfsema et al., 2007; Shushruth et al., 2018).

A third class of experiments probes the electrophysiological response properties of inhibitory neurons in vivo (Adesnik et al., 2012; Atallah et al., 2012; Cardin et al., 2009; Scholl et al., 2015; Sohal et al., 2009; Wilson et al., 2017; Wilson et al., 2012). Excitatory and inhibitory neurons within a cortical area have different response properties in mice, cats, and ferrets, a fact that is presumably related to differences in their respective functional contributions (Huang and Paul, 2018). Identification of inhibitory and excitatory neurons in vivo has been challenging in monkeys. The discovery of fast-spiking excitatory neurons in primates undermines the use of extracellularly recorded spike waveforms (Kelly et al., 2019). Optogenetic phototagging of inhibitory neurons, using AAV–mDlx5/6–ChR2 permits electrophysiological identification of neuronal subtypes more decisively (Figure 9).

Figure 9. Responses of a putative GABAergic, direction-selective single unit to optical (450 nm laser) and visual (drifting achromatic 3 Hz sinusoidal grating) stimulation (A).

Figure 9.

Peristimulus time histograms (black) in response to sinusoidal laser modulation from 2 Hz to 254 Hz (blue). Inset: Mean spike waveforms (thick black curve) and noise waveforms (thick gray curve) ± 1 standard deviation (thin curves). (B) Direction tuning curve showing individual (black points) and mean responses (black line) across repeated presentations of a drifting sinusoidal grating. (C) Spatial frequency tuning curve with symbols identical to (B).

In summary, the optogenetic approach used in this study holds promise for a finer level of neural circuit interrogation than previously achievable in monkeys. This union of neural inactivation technique and animal model has broad utility for addressing many outstanding questions in systems neuroscience that span the domains of sensation, action and cognition.

Materials and methods

Key resources table.

Reagent type
(species) or
resource
Designation Source or
reference
Identifiers Additional
information
Antibody mCherry
monoclonal antibody
Clontech Cat. No. 632543 RRID:AB_2307319 (1:250)
Antibody Rabbit anti-parvalbumin Swant Code: 27 RRID:AB_2631173 (1:5000)
Antibody Donkey anti-
Mouse IgG (H+L) highly cross-adsorbed secondary antibody, Alexa
Fluor 568
Molecular Probes Cat. No. A10037 RRID:AB_2534013 (1:200)
Antibody Donkey anti-Rabbit IgG (H+L) highly cross-adsorbed secondary antibody, Alexa Fluor 488 Molecular Probes Cat. No. A21206 RRID:AB_2535792 (1:200)
Antibody Donkey anti-Rabbit IgG (H+L) highly cross-adsorbed secondary antibody, Alexa Fluor 350 Molecular Probes Cat. No. A10039 RRID:AB_2534015 (1:200)
Other DAPI Invitrogen Cat. No. D21490 (1:5000)
Recombinant DNA reagent pAAV-mDlx-ChR2-mCherry-Fishell-3 Addgene Cat. No. 83898 RRID:Addgene_83898
Cell line (Homo-sapiens) HEK293T American Type Culture Collection CRL-3216 RRID:CVCL_0063
Biological sample (Macaca Mulatta) Rhesus monkey Washington National Primate Research Center NA
Software/Algorithm MATLAB Mathworks https://www.mathworks.com/products/matlab.html RRID:SCR_001622
Software/Algorithm Fiji NIH (ImageJ) https://imagej.net/Fiji RRID:SCR_002285
Software/Algorithm Plexon Sort Client Plexon http://www.plexon.com RRID:SCR_003170
Software/Algorithm Plexon Offline Sorter Plexon http://www.plexon.com RRID:SCR_000012

Contact for resource sharing

Further information and requests for resources should be directed to and will be fulfilled by the Lead Contact, Gregory D Horwitz (ghorwitz@u.washington.edu).

Experimental model and subject details

Three rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) participated in this study (males; 7–14 kg). Two monkeys were surgically implanted with a head-holding device and a recording chamber that provided access to the primary visual cortex (V1). Surgical procedures, experimental protocols, and animal care conformed to the NIH Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at the University of Washington.

Animal husbandry and housing were overseen by the Washington National Primate Research Center. All monkeys had ad-libitum access to biscuits (Fiber Plus Monkey Diet 5049, Lab Diet). Monkeys 2 and 3 had controlled daily access to fresh produce and water. When possible, animals were pair-housed and allowed grooming contact. Cages were washed every other week, bedding was changed every day, and animals were examined by a veterinarian at least twice per year.

During each experiment, monkeys viewed a CRT monitor binocularly with their heads fixed. The viewing distance was 100 cm for monkey 2 and 50 cm for monkey 3. Eye position signals were measured with an optical eye tracker for monkey 2 and a scleral search coil for monkey 3. Behavioral and stimulation timing events and eye position signals were digitized and stored for offline analysis.

Method details

AAV vector production

Recombinant AAV vectors were produced using a conventional three-plasmid transient transfection of human embryonic kidney cells (HEK293T, female, unauthenticated) with polyethylenimine (25 kDa, Polysciences). The transfer plasmid was pAAV-mDlx-ChR2-mCherry-Fishell-3 (Addgene #83898). Vectors were harvested and purified by ultracentrifugation through an iodixanol gradient column, exchanged into phosphate buffered saline (PBS), and titered using qPCR.

AAV vector injections

After mapping a track through V1 gray matter using standard extracellular recording techniques in awake fixating monkeys, we advanced an electrode and cannula to the deepest point of the track and began a series of injections. Using a Hamilton syringe attached to a manual pump, we injected 1.0–1.5 µl of AAV vector at each of several locations spaced 500 µm apart along a track (normal to the opercular surface). Each injection was followed by a 2 min wait period after which the electrode and cannula were slowly retracted to the next site. This process was repeated at 9–14 sites, and a total of 14–17 µl was injected along each track. In monkey 2, we injected 14 μL of AAV9–mDlx5/6–ChR2–mCherry (1.5 × 1013 genomes/ml) at each of two opercular sites that were ~2 mm apart. The AAV vector was injected along 4 mm tracks throughout the thickness of the cortex at both sites, in the left hemisphere. In monkey 3, we injected ~17 μL of AAV1–mDlx5/6–ChR2–mCherry (1.0 × 1013 genomes/ml) along a 5 mm track in the first site and 14 μL along a 6.5 mm track in the second site, in the right hemisphere, to target both opercular and calcarine regions of area V1. The two injection tracks were ~1.5 mm apart.

Histology

We injected area V1 of monkey 1 with AAV1–mDlx5/6–ChR2-mCherry to examine the specificity of vector transduction. These injections were performed during a surgical procedure while the monkey was anesthetized, and electrophysiological recordings were not made. The monkey recovered from the surgery and was euthanized 45 days later with an overdose of pentobarbital and perfused transcardially with 4% paraformaldehyde (wt/vol). The brain was removed, cryoprotected in 30% sucrose (wt/vol) and 50 μm-thick sections were cut on a sliding microtome. Fluorescence signals from mCherry (primary antibody: 1:250, Clontech 632543, mouse anti-mCherry; secondary antibody: 1:200, Invitrogen Molecular Probe) and parvalbumin (primary antibody: 1:5000, rabbit anti-PV, Swant 27; secondary antibody: 1:200, Invitrogen Molecular Probes) were detected immunocytochemically. Sections were counterstained with DAPI (1:5000, Molecular Probes D-21490) and cover-slipped using a DABCO-based mounting medium.

Neurophysiology

Three to four weeks after AAV injections in monkeys 2 and 3, we searched for neuronal responses to blue light (450 nm; 33–161 mW) delivered to area V1 via an optical fiber (300 μm outer diameter; Thor Labs) with a beveled tip that eased entry through the dura. A fiber and a glass-coated tungsten electrode (1–3 MΩ FHC) were placed in a common guide tube and lowered independently into the brain by microdrive (Narashige or Alpha-Omega). Extracellular spikes were amplified (1x head-stage), high-pass filtered (250 Hz cutoff), digitized (sampling rate of 40 kHz) and sorted (Plexon MAP system).

Site selection criteria

Stimulation sites were selected by inserting an electrode into V1 and finding a region with vigorous visual activity and a clearly defined receptive field (RF). The optical fiber was then lowered while repeatedly delivering brief laser pulses. The optical fiber typically lagged the electrode by 100–500 µm. Only sites at which optical stimulation produced an audible change in firing rate were tested.

Laser setup

The laser was developed in-house by the Bioengineering Core at the Washington National Primate Research Center. Light was generated by a laser diode (part # PL TB450B). Light delivery was modulated by modulating the current to the laser diode (digital to analog converter part # AD5683) not by shutter.

Visually guided saccade task

Monkeys were trained to make saccades to visual targets 4–17° from the fixation point. Each trial began when the monkey acquired a central fixation point (0.2–0.3° sided square) within a 1.6 × 1.6° electronic window. Then, 13 ms after the central target disappeared, a saccade target (square with sides 0.3–0.4°) was presented. Two to ten target locations, equiangularly spaced at fixed radius, were interleaved within each block of trials. Monkeys were rewarded for making a saccade to the target. On half of the target-present trials at each location, a 300-ms laser pulse was delivered simultaneously with the target presentation (Figure 4B). We interleaved 10–30 catch trials in which no target was presented, and the monkey was rewarded unconditionally. Optical stimulation was delivered on half of the catch trials, immediately after the fixation point disappeared.

Two-alternative forced choice (2AFC) Gabor contrast detection task

Monkeys were trained to detect a Gabor stimulus positioned 4–17° from the fixation point. Each trial began when the monkey acquired the fixation point. Then, after a 520-ms delay, a drifting Gabor stimulus appeared on half of the trials (spatial frequency = 1 cycle/°, temporal frequency = 8 Hz, standard deviation = 0.2°, duration = 200 ms). Immediately after the Gabor stimulus disappeared, a pair of targets appeared along the horizontal meridian, 2° from the fixation point. A saccade to the target on the same side of the screen as the Gabor stimulus was rewarded on Gabor-present trials, and a saccade to the target on the opposite side was rewarded on Gabor-absent trials.

The Gabor stimulus appeared inside the RF of neurons at the recording site in all trials except a few in which retinotopic specificity was tested (Figure 6). Optical stimulation began at the stimulus onset, lasted 300 ms (Figure 5B), and was delivered on half of the Gabor-present and half of the Gabor-absent trials. The monkey typically performed several blocks of trials per session, each consisting of 120 trials. Stimulus strength was adjusted by independent contrast staircase procedures on laser and control trials. Contrast, defined as the difference between the highest and lowest luminance values, divided by the sum of the two, increased by a factor of 1.18–1.33 following an incorrect response and decreased by a factor of 0.75–0.85 following three consecutive correct responses.

Quantification and statistical analyses

All statistical analyses were performed in MATLAB.

Sensitivity index (d’)

Sensitivity (d’) was measured using a standard formula from signal detection theory (Green and Swets, 1966; Macmillan and Creelman, 2004).

d=Φ1(proportionofhits)Φ1(proportionoffalsealarms)

In this equation, Φ-1 is the inverse normal cumulative distribution function. Proportions of 0 were replaced with 0.5/n, and proportions of 1 were replaced by 1-0.5/n, where n is the number of Gabor-present (for hits) or Gabor-absent trials (for false alarms) (Stanislaw and Todorov, 1999).

Fit to behavioral data

Proportions of correct responses were fit with a cumulative Weibull distribution function by maximizing likelihood assuming binomial error. Fitting was performed using the inbuilt MATLAB fmincon function. Detection threshold was defined as the luminance contrast corresponding to 82% correct.

Acknowledgements

We thank Michael N Shadlen for helpful comments on the manuscript, Elizabeth Buffalo for generous microscope access, and Albert Ng for help with MRI-related software. This work was funded by NIH EY018849 to Gregory D Horwitz, NIH/ORIP grant P51OD010425 to Washington National Primate Research Center, NEI Center Core Grant for Vision Research P30 EY01730 to the University of Washington and R90 DA033461 (Training Program in Neural Computation and Engineering) to Abhishek De.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Gregory D Horwitz, Email: ghorwitz@uw.edu.

Joshua I Gold, University of Pennsylvania, United States.

Michael Schmid, Newcastle University, United Kingdom.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • National Eye Institute EY030441 to Gregory D Horwitz.

  • NIH Office of Research Infrastructure Programs P51OD010425 to Abhishek De, Yasmine El-Shamayleh, Gregory D Horwitz.

  • National Institutes of Health R90 DA033461 to Abhishek De.

  • National Eye Institute P30 EY01730 to Abhishek De, Yasmine El-Shamayleh, Gregory D Horwitz.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation.

Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation.

Conceptualization, Supervision, Project administration.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: Surgical procedures, experimental protocols and animal care conformed to the NIH Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at the University of Washington (IACUC protocol #4167-01).

Additional files

Transparent reporting form

Data availability

All data have been uploaded to https://github.com/horwitzlab/fast-and-reversible-neural-inactivation (copy archived at https://github.com/elifesciences-publications/Fast-and-reversible-neural-inactivation).

References

  1. Abraham WC. Metaplasticity: tuning synapses and networks for plasticity. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2008;9:387. doi: 10.1038/nrn2356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Acker L, Pino EN, Boyden ES, Desimone R. FEF inactivation with improved optogenetic methods. PNAS. 2016;113:E7297–E7306. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1610784113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Adesnik H, Bruns W, Taniguchi H, Huang ZJ, Scanziani M. A neural circuit for spatial summation in visual cortex. Nature. 2012;490:226–231. doi: 10.1038/nature11526. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Afraz A, Boyden ES, DiCarlo JJ. Optogenetic and pharmacological suppression of spatial clusters of face neurons reveal their causal role in face gender discrimination. PNAS. 2015;112:6730–6735. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1423328112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Arias-Gil G, Ohl FW, Takagaki K, Lippert MT. Measurement, modeling, and prediction of temperature rise due to optogenetic brain stimulation. Neurophotonics. 2016;3:045007. doi: 10.1117/1.NPh.3.4.045007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Atallah BV, Bruns W, Carandini M, Scanziani M. Parvalbumin-expressing interneurons linearly transform cortical responses to visual stimuli. Neuron. 2012;73:159–170. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.12.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Baertsch NA, Baertsch HC, Ramirez JM. The interdependence of excitation and inhibition for the control of dynamic breathing rhythms. Nature Communications. 2018;9:843. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-03223-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Berndt A, Yizhar O, Gunaydin LA, Hegemann P, Deisseroth K. Bi-stable neural state switches. Nature Neuroscience. 2009;12:229–234. doi: 10.1038/nn.2247. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cardin JA, Carlén M, Meletis K, Knoblich U, Zhang F, Deisseroth K, Tsai LH, Moore CI. Driving fast-spiking cells induces gamma rhythm and controls sensory responses. Nature. 2009;459:663–667. doi: 10.1038/nature08002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cavanaugh J, Monosov IE, McAlonan K, Berman R, Smith MK, Cao V, Wang KH, Boyden ES, Wurtz RH. Optogenetic inactivation modifies monkey visuomotor behavior. Neuron. 2012;76:901–907. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cone JJ, Scantlen MD, Histed MH, Maunsell JHR. Different inhibitory interneuron cell classes make distinct contributions to visual contrast perception. Eneuro. 2019;6:ENEURO.0337-18.2019. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0337-18.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cowey A. The blindsight saga. Experimental Brain Research. 2010;200:3–24. doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-1914-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Dai J, Brooks DI, Sheinberg DL. Optogenetic and electrical microstimulation systematically bias visuospatial choice in primates. Current Biology. 2014;24:63–69. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.11.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Dimidschstein J, Chen Q, Tremblay R, Rogers SL, Saldi GA, Guo L, Xu Q, Liu R, Lu C, Chu J, Grimley JS, Krostag AR, Kaykas A, Avery MC, Rashid MS, Baek M, Jacob AL, Smith GB, Wilson DE, Kosche G, Kruglikov I, Rusielewicz T, Kotak VC, Mowery TM, Anderson SA, Callaway EM, Dasen JS, Fitzpatrick D, Fossati V, Long MA, Noggle S, Reynolds JH, Sanes DH, Rudy B, Feng G, Fishell G. A viral strategy for targeting and manipulating interneurons across vertebrate species. Nature Neuroscience. 2016;19:1743–1749. doi: 10.1038/nn.4430. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. El-Shamayleh Y, Kojima Y, Soetedjo R, Horwitz GD. Selective optogenetic control of Purkinje cells in monkey cerebellum. Neuron. 2017;95:51–62. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.06.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Fetsch CR, Odean NN, Jeurissen D, El-Shamayleh Y, Horwitz GD, Shadlen MN. Focal optogenetic suppression in macaque area MT biases direction discrimination and decision confidence, but only transiently. eLife. 2018;7:e36523. doi: 10.7554/eLife.36523. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Freedman DJ, Riesenhuber M, Poggio T, Miller EK. Categorical representation of visual stimuli in the primate prefrontal cortex. Science. 2001;291:312–316. doi: 10.1126/science.291.5502.312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Gerits A, Farivar R, Rosen BR, Wald LL, Boyden ES, Vanduffel W. Optogenetically induced behavioral and functional network changes in primates. Current Biology. 2012;22:1722–1726. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.07.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Glickfeld LL, Histed MH, Maunsell JH. Mouse primary visual cortex is used to detect both orientation and contrast changes. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:19416–19422. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3560-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Goffart L, Hafed ZM, Krauzlis RJ. Visual fixation as equilibrium: evidence from superior colliculus inactivation. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:10627–10636. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0696-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Gong X, Mendoza-Halliday D, Ting JT, Kaiser T, Sun X, Bastos AM, Wimmer RD, Guo B, Chen Q, Zhou Y, Pruner M, Wu CW-H, Park D, Deisseroth K, Barak B, Boyden ES, Miller EK, Halassa MM, Fu Z, Bi G, Desimone R, Feng G. An ultra-sensitive step-function opsin for minimally invasive optogenetic stimulation in mice and macaques. Neuron. 2020;398:30239-7. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2020.03.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Goold CP, Nicoll RA. Single-cell optogenetic excitation drives homeostatic synaptic depression. Neuron. 2010;68:512–528. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.09.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Goshen I, Brodsky M, Prakash R, Wallace J, Gradinaru V, Ramakrishnan C, Deisseroth K. Dynamics of retrieval strategies for remote memories. Cell. 2011;147:678–689. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2011.09.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Green DM, Swets JA. Signal Detection Theory and Psychophysics. New York: Wiley; 1966. [Google Scholar]
  25. Guo ZV, Li N, Huber D, Ophir E, Gutnisky D, Ting JT, Feng G, Svoboda K. Flow of cortical activity underlying a tactile decision in mice. Neuron. 2014;81:179–194. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.10.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hanks TD, Kopec CD, Brunton BW, Duan CA, Erlich JC, Brody CD. Distinct relationships of parietal and prefrontal cortices to evidence accumulation. Nature. 2015;520:220–223. doi: 10.1038/nature14066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hegdé J. Time course of visual perception: coarse-to-fine processing and beyond. Progress in Neurobiology. 2008;84:405–439. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2007.09.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Huang ZJ, Paul A. Diversity of GABAergic interneurons and diversification of communication modules in cortical networks. bioRxiv. 2018 doi: 10.1101/490797. [DOI]
  29. Ibos G, Freedman DJ. Sequential sensory and decision processing in posterior parietal cortex. eLife. 2017;6:e23743. doi: 10.7554/eLife.23743. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Inoue KI, Takada M, Matsumoto M. Neuronal and behavioural modulations by pathway-selective optogenetic stimulation of the primate oculomotor system. Nature Communications. 2015;6:8378. doi: 10.1038/ncomms9378. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Isaacson JS, Scanziani M. How inhibition shapes cortical activity. Neuron. 2011;72:231–243. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Jazayeri M, Lindbloom-Brown Z, Horwitz GD. Saccadic eye movements evoked by optogenetic activation of primate V1. Nature Neuroscience. 2012;15:1368–1370. doi: 10.1038/nn.3210. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Kelly JG, García-Marín V, Rudy B, Hawken MJ. Densities and laminar distributions of Kv3.1b-, PV-, GABA-, and SMI-32-immunoreactive neurons in macaque area V1. Cerebral Cortex. 2019;29:1921–1937. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy072. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Khan AG, Poort J, Chadwick A, Blot A, Sahani M, Mrsic-Flogel TD, Hofer SB. Distinct learning-induced changes in stimulus selectivity and interactions of GABAergic interneuron classes in visual cortex. Nature Neuroscience. 2018;21:851–859. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0143-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Kinoshita M, Kato R, Isa K, Kobayashi K, Kobayashi K, Onoe H, Isa T. Dissecting the circuit for blindsight to reveal the critical role of pulvinar and superior colliculus. Nature Communications. 2019;10:135. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-08058-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Koerner F, Teuber HL. Visual field defects after missile injuries to the geniculo-striate pathway in man. Experimental Brain Research. 1973;18:88–113. doi: 10.1007/BF00236558. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Krauzlis RJ, Goffart L, Hafed ZM. Neuronal control of fixation and fixational eye movements. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2017;372:20160205. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0205. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Kubota Y, Kondo S, Nomura M, Hatada S, Yamaguchi N, Mohamed AA, Karube F, Lübke J, Kawaguchi Y. Functional effects of distinct innervation styles of pyramidal cells by fast spiking cortical interneurons. eLife. 2015;4:e07919. doi: 10.7554/eLife.07919. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Leopold DA. Primary visual cortex: awareness and blindsight. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2012;35:91–109. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150356. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Macmillan NA, Creelman CD. Detection Theory: A User's Guide. Psychology press; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  41. McBride EG, Lee SJ, Callaway EM. Local and global influences of visual spatial selection and locomotion in mouse primary visual cortex. Current Biology. 2019;29:1592–1605. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.03.065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Merigan WH, Nealey TA, Maunsell JH. Visual effects of lesions of cortical area V2 in macaques. Journal of Neuroscience. 1993;13:3180–3191. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-07-03180.1993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Mori F, Nakajima K, Tachibana A, Mori S. Obstacle clearance and prevention from falling in the bipedally walking japanese monkey, Macaca fuscata. Age and Ageing. 2006;35:ii19–ii23. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afl079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Nandy A, Nassi JJ, Jadi MP, Reynolds J. Optogenetically induced low-frequency correlations impair perception. eLife. 2019;8:e35123. doi: 10.7554/eLife.35123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Nathanson JL, Yanagawa Y, Obata K, Callaway EM. Preferential labeling of inhibitory and excitatory cortical neurons by endogenous tropism of adeno-associated virus and lentivirus vectors. Neuroscience. 2009;161:441–450. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.03.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Ohayon S, Grimaldi P, Schweers N, Tsao DY. Saccade modulation by optical and electrical stimulation in the macaque frontal eye field. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:16684–16697. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2675-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Packer AM, Yuste R. Dense, unspecific connectivity of neocortical parvalbumin-positive interneurons: a canonical microcircuit for inhibition? Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:13260–13271. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3131-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Paolini AG, McKenzie JS. Effects of inactivation of the magnocellular preoptic nucleus of olfactory bulb processing. NeuroReport. 1997;8:929–935. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199703030-00023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Poort J, Raudies F, Wannig A, Lamme VA, Neumann H, Roelfsema PR. The role of attention in figure-ground segregation in areas V1 and V4 of the visual cortex. Neuron. 2012;75:143–156. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.04.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Radoeva PD, Prasad S, Brainard DH, Aguirre GK. Neural activity within area V1 reflects unconscious visual performance in a case of blindsight. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2008;20:1927–1939. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Roelfsema PR, Lamme VA, Spekreijse H. Object-based attention in the primary visual cortex of the macaque monkey. Nature. 1998;395:376–381. doi: 10.1038/26475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Roelfsema PR, Tolboom M, Khayat PS. Different processing phases for features, figures, and selective attention in the primary visual cortex. Neuron. 2007;56:785–792. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Ruiz O, Lustig BR, Nassi JJ, Cetin A, Reynolds JH, Albright TD, Callaway EM, Stoner GR, Roe AW. Optogenetics through windows on the brain in the nonhuman primate. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2013;110:1455–1467. doi: 10.1152/jn.00153.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Scerra VE, Avery M, Dimidschstein J, Fishell GJ, Reynolds JH. Optogenetic activation of GABAergic neurons in primate V1 impairs detection performance through indirect effects on excitatory neurons. Program No 30710 2019 Neuroscience Meeting Planner Chicago, IL: Society for Neuroscience, 2019 Online.2019. [Google Scholar]
  55. Scholl B, Pattadkal JJ, Dilly GA, Priebe NJ, Zemelman BV. Local integration accounts for weak selectivity of mouse neocortical parvalbumin interneurons. Neuron. 2015;87:424–436. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.06.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Seidemann E, Zohary E, Newsome WT. Temporal gating of neural signals during performance of a visual discrimination task. Nature. 1998;394:72–75. doi: 10.1038/27906. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Shushruth S, Mazurek M, Shadlen MN. Comparison of decision-related signals in sensory and motor preparatory responses of neurons in area LIP. Journal of Neuroscience. 2018;38:6350–6365. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0668-18.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Simonyan K. The laryngeal motor cortex: its organization and connectivity. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2014;28:15–21. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2014.05.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Sohal VS, Zhang F, Yizhar O, Deisseroth K. Parvalbumin neurons and gamma rhythms enhance cortical circuit performance. Nature. 2009;459:698–702. doi: 10.1038/nature07991. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Sokolova IV, Mody I. Silencing-induced metaplasticity in hippocampal cultured neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2008;100:690–697. doi: 10.1152/jn.90378.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Stanislaw H, Todorov N. Calculation of signal detection theory measures. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers. 1999;31:137–149. doi: 10.3758/BF03207704. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Stauffer WR, Lak A, Yang A, Borel M, Paulsen O, Boyden ES, Schultz W. Dopamine neuron-specific optogenetic stimulation in rhesus macaques. Cell. 2016;166:1564–1571. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2016.08.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Stemmler M, Koch C. How voltage-dependent conductances can adapt to maximize the information encoded by neuronal firing rate. Nature Neuroscience. 1999;2:521–527. doi: 10.1038/9173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Tamura K, Takeda M, Setsuie R, Tsubota T, Hirabayashi T, Miyamoto K, Miyashita Y. Conversion of object identity to object-general semantic value in the primate temporal cortex. Science. 2017;357:687–692. doi: 10.1126/science.aan4800. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Tehovnik EJ, Slocum WM, Schiller PH. Saccadic eye movements evoked by microstimulation of striate cortex. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;17:870–878. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02489.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Turrigiano GG, Leslie KR, Desai NS, Rutherford LC, Nelson SB. Activity-dependent scaling of quantal amplitude in neocortical neurons. Nature. 1998;391:892–896. doi: 10.1038/36103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Van Brederode JF, Mulligan KA, Hendrickson AE. Calcium-binding proteins as markers for subpopulations of GABAergic neurons in monkey striate cortex. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1990;298:1–22. doi: 10.1002/cne.902980102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Wiegert JS, Mahn M, Prigge M, Printz Y, Yizhar O. Silencing neurons: tools, applications, and experimental constraints. Neuron. 2017;95:504–529. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.06.050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Wilson NR, Runyan CA, Wang FL, Sur M. Division and subtraction by distinct cortical inhibitory networks in vivo. Nature. 2012;488:343–348. doi: 10.1038/nature11347. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Wilson DE, Smith GB, Jacob AL, Walker T, Dimidschstein J, Fishell G, Fitzpatrick D. GABAergic neurons in ferret visual cortex participate in functionally specific networks. Neuron. 2017;93:1058–1065. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.02.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Wurtz RH. Using perturbations to identify the brain circuits underlying active vision. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2015;370:20140205. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2014.0205. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Yartsev MM, Hanks TD, Yoon AM, Brody CD. Causal contribution and dynamical encoding in the striatum during evidence accumulation. eLife. 2018;7:e34929. doi: 10.7554/eLife.34929. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Michael Schmid1
Reviewed by: Michael Schmid2, Wim Vanduffel3, David Sheinberg

In the interests of transparency, eLife publishes the most substantive revision requests and the accompanying author responses.

Acceptance summary:

Targeted optogenetic inactivation of neural circuits in non-human primates is essential to clarify specific links between neuronal activity and behaviour. Here the authors capitalise on the recent development of Dlx5/6 enhancer-guided targeting of GABAergic neurons (Dimidschstein et al., 2016) for optogenetic manipulation of macaque primary visual cortex (V1). The authors show how optogenetic targeting of V1 GABAergic neurons modulates V1 neural activity and leads to a substantial, specific impairment in vision guided behaviour.

Decision letter after peer review:

Thank you for submitting your article "Fast and reversible neural inactivation in macaque cortex by optogenetic stimulation of GABAergic neurons" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, including Michael Schmid as the Reviewing Editor and Reviewer #1, and the evaluation has been overseen by Joshua Gold as the Senior Editor. The following individuals involved in review of your submission have agreed to reveal their identity: Wim Vanduffel (Reviewer #2); David Sheinberg (Reviewer #3).

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

Summary:

De, El-Shamayleh, and Horwitz describe anatomical, electrophysiological and behavioral results of optogenetic deactivation experiments targeting primary visual cortex (V1) in macaques. The authors capitalise on the recent development of Dlx5/6 enhancer-guided targeting of GABAergic neurons (Dimidschstein et al., 2016). Here De et al. used the Dlx5/6 enhancer in combination with a depolarizing opsin (ChR2) to activate inhibitory neurons, with the aim to inactivate the local downstream excitatory neurons. A key advance of this study is the demonstration that a AAV approach for targeting inhibitory neurons that has been shown to work in the marmoset translates to the rhesus monkey. The authors show histological evidence for transduced neurons near the injection site, as evidenced by mCherry expression. Moreover, most of the transduced neurons were PV+, indicating high specificity for inhibitory neurons. At neuron level, they observed both increased (2/3 of the stimulated sites) and suppressed (1/3 of the sites) light-induced activity. Moreover, the monkeys failed to make reliable saccades to targets represented by the stimulated neurons. Finally, the monkeys had severely reduced contrast detection thresholds at these sites. The authors provide compelling results from a combination of histological, electrophysiological and behavioural tests. Particularly the strong behavioural effects advance the field and will be of great interest to a wide audience. Unwanted rebound effects, which are typically present when using alternative hyperpolarizing opsins (e.g. ArchT or Jaws), are largely absent. Overall, the evidence presented is solid, the analyses are sound, the writing is very straightforward and the message is clear. The new research is important, timely and provides an important step forward for the field. However, the reviewers have expressed important concerns that need to be addressed before the manuscript can be accepted for publication.

Essential revisions:

1) The expression pattern needs to be more fully characterised. The selectivity of the vector seems to be high -i.e. mainly restricted to PV+ interneurons. Yet, the sensitivity is surprisingly low (only 41 neurons are transduced). Would this be a vast underestimation of the real number of the neurons expressing ChR2? It would be good for readers to know the percentage of Parvalbumin neurons that show fluorescence to get a better estimate on the expression sensitivity. There are some recent reports indicating that the threshold for detecting FP expression might be higher than the threshold for the functional gene (Kinoshita et al., 2019). Or do the authors think that the number of neurons expressing ChR2 can be as low as ~40 in order to evoke a clear behavioral effect? Figure 1 suggests a very laminar-specific expression pattern, but the authors explain that this is not typical. Was this slice the only one analyzed? Ideally the reader would like to see an assessment across cortical laminae, but perhaps the authors could show further sections that give a more representative view of the expression pattern. Although seemingly annoying, this may be useful for layer-specific optogenetic deactivations.

2) Characterise more fully electrophysiological responses. Given the relatively long latencies of the optogenetic effect (see Figure 2—figure supplement 2), it is unlikely that these are only first order neurons expressing the opsin which are directly activated by the blue light. How do the authors explain the long latency effects? It would be also interesting to plot the latencies of the cells showing a suppression effect (i.e. the time after stimulation onset that the activity drops significantly below the pre-stimulation firing rate). These latencies should be longer than those of neurons showing an excitatory effect. Estimating the onset of suppression is not trivial, but this could be informative regarding potential direct and indirect effects. Figure 3B also shows suppression for a site with some very bursty responses which seem to drastically inflate the Y-axis (Response). Was this high variability and bursty activity common for suppressed sites? The overall spontaneous rates of many GABAergic cells is fairly high, but it's not clear if that is the case for the population explored here. That spiking increased in >60% of recorded sites is in line with successful targeting of GABAergic interneurons. But what do we learn about these neurons? The authors discuss the potential of photo-tagging in the Discussion and provide one exemplary direction selective unit in Figure 9. But one is left with the question what happened at the other sites? Are they visually responsive?

3) Further aspects should be considered that might have influenced behavioural performance. For the behavioral tasks the authors should probably emphasize that reward contingencies were not dependent on laser delivery. It was also unclear on why the measure used quantifying the effect for the oculomotor task was not simply distance from the target? For this task, the data for Monkey 3 shown in Figure 4 even for the control trials looks like it's not right on the center of the RF location. Does this figure show exactly where the target was presented and how were the eye positions calibrated? In both paradigms opto stimulation occurred at the same time as visual stimulation. Given a visual response latency of 40 ms or more in V1 neurons, at least in theory, the opto stimulus could serve as a cue telling the monkey how to act in order to get reward. It is indicated that the change in contrast detection performance is due to the reduction in sensitivity and not a change in criterion. One cannot conclude that from d-primes only. The c-criterion should also be listed as there can be a change in sensitivity and criterion.

4) Electrophysiological and behavioural measures should be more directly related to each other. There's no obvious reason why these couldn't be done simultaneously. If possible, it would be good to see opto elicted spiking activity from the trials during behavioural testing and to probe whether there is a direct relationship between the strength of spiking and the behavioural effect.

5) Clarify for the detection conditions, how the authors move from the example sessions (Figure 5) to the population data (Figure 7). Some rewording here to make it clear that the comments in the paragraph below are referring to the examples in Figure 5 and not the whole population (which follows in a couple of paragraphs). For the population, the authors should revisit Figure 7 to not include all the blocks, as this conflates the independent sessions (11 and 12) from the blocks, which are clearly not independent. To include all the blocks in Figure 7 is a clear case for pseudo-replication. The population analysis needs to be by session, not block. The authors should also revisit the psychometric fits (examples in Figure 5, e.g.). The laser fits don't look very good – was there some estimate of goodness of fit for these?

6) Clarify details about injection and stimulation procedures (see minor points), including heating induced damage considerations. A concern is in understanding and justifying the need for the large increase in power used to activate the neurons under study. The absolute power levels are on a direct concern if they cause lasting damage to the tissue. On one hand the prolonged efficacy across the session is evidence that effects of greater power did not present an acute problem, but there could be concern that prolonged use in a single site, for example, could lead to irreversible damage. More discussion on the power would be useful.

eLife. 2020 May 26;9:e52658. doi: 10.7554/eLife.52658.sa2

Author response


Essential revisions:

1) The expression pattern needs to be more fully characterised. The selectivity of the vector seems to be high -i.e. mainly restricted to PV+ interneurons. Yet, the sensitivity is surprisingly low (only 41 neurons are transduced). Would this be a vast underestimation of the real number of the neurons expressing ChR2? It would be good for readers to know the percentage of Parvalbumin neurons that show fluorescence to get a better estimate on the expression sensitivity.

The reviewers are correct that the number of transduced neurons in Figure 1 is a vast underestimate. Indeed, many more neurons expressed ChR2 in monkey 1 than are shown in the Figure 1. Unfortunately, a nearby injection of an entirely different viral vector that expressed ChR2-eYFP under the control of a different promoter complicated our analyses of selectivity and sensitivity. Transduction by the two vectors is easily distinguished on the basis of their distinct fluorescent protein genes. However, the spectral overlap between the eYFP signal and the green secondary antibody we used to label PV neurons required us to look at sections where the two injections did not overlap. In Figure 1, we show a section of V1 near the edge of the AAV-mDlx5/6-ChR2mCherry transduction zone that lacks ChR2-eYFP expression; this region allows us to estimate selectivity easily but provides an underestimate of sensitivity due to the sparse mCherry label.

To address reviewers’ comments, we have analyzed a substantially larger region in monkey 1 that spans the V1–V2 border (Figure 1—figure supplement 1). We have amplified the AAV-mDlx5/6-ChR2-mCherry signal and PV signal, the latter using a short wavelength secondary antibody that avoids confusion with the ChR2-eYFP signal from the second viral vector. This section had many more ChR2-mCherry+ cells (N=543). In regions of efficient transduction, we estimate that ~50% of parvalbumin+ neurons were transduced.

There are some recent reports indicating that the threshold for detecting FP expression might be higher than the threshold for the functional gene (Kinoshita et al., 2019). Or do the authors think that the number of neurons expressing ChR2 can be as low as ~40 in order to evoke a clear behavioral effect?

The threshold for detecting FP expression may indeed exceed the threshold for functional ChR2 expression. We were able to detect native FP signal in histological sections from monkey 1, suggesting that ChR2 expression was likely sufficient to manipulate spiking activity (monkey 1 was not used in the electrophysiological/ behavioral experiments). To facilitate cell counting, all sections shown in the manuscript were amplified immunohistochemically.

The minimum number of V1 neurons that must be manipulated to cause a behavioral effect is an important issue that our data do not speak to. The number of neurons affected by the light stimulation may depend on the efficiency of AAV transduction, the shape of the optical fiber tip, spread of the laser light, tissue transmissibility, laser power, sensitivity of the behavioral assay, and other factors.

Figure 1 suggests a very laminar-specific expression pattern, but the authors explain that this is not typical. Was this slice the only one analyzed? Ideally the reader would like to see an assessment across cortical laminae, but perhaps the authors could show further sections that give a more representative view of the expression pattern. Although seemingly annoying, this may be useful for layer-specific optogenetic deactivations.

The efficiency of transduction across cortical laminae is determined by in part by where and how the vector injection is made. The AAV injections into monkey 1 were made during a surgical procedure, without electrophysiological guidance, which may explain the concentration of expression in superficial cortical layers. Injections into monkeys 2 and 3 were based on electrophysiological depth measurements and are therefore more likely to have spanned all V1 layers. To provide the reviewers with evidence for expression in deeper layers, we have recently made another injection of AAV-mDlx5/6ChR2-mCherry into area V4 of a monkey that was not used in this study. In that experiment, transduction spanned all of the layers (except for layer 4 which, in our hands, is difficult to transduce efficiently irrespective of the vector injected). Please see Figure 1B of http://www.pnas.org/content/116/52/26195 for the results of that experiment.

2) Characterise more fully electrophysiological responses. Given the relatively long latencies of the optogenetic effect (see Suppl Figure 1), it is unlikely that these are only first order neurons expressing the opsin which are directly activated by the blue light. How do the authors explain the long latency effects?

The long latency effects are likely due to complex network activity within and beyond V1. The existence of these complex interactions means that selective optical stimulation of inhibitory neurons need not necessarily exert a net-inhibitory effect on the circuit.

It would be also interesting to plot the latencies of the cells showing a suppression effect (i.e. the time after stimulation onset that the activity drops significantly below the pre-stimulation firing rate). These latencies should be longer than those of neurons showing an excitatory effect. Estimating the onset of suppression is not trivial, but this could be informative regarding potential direct and indirect effects.

We agree with the reviewers that estimating the onset of suppression is not trivial. Decreases in firing rate are more difficult to detect than increases in firing rate, especially when the baseline firing rate is low, as is often the case in V1. We have done our best to estimate the latency of the optogenetic effect for both activated and suppressed sites. As anticipated by the reviewer, the latencies at suppressed sites were longer than those at activated sites (Figure 2—figure supplement 2C). However, the interpretation of this result is complicated; the expected delay from synaptic transmission is brief relative to the bias produced by estimating a reduction in an already-low firing rate (relative to an increase). We have provided raster plots that i l lust rate laser responses at ever y suppressed site we studied (Figure 2—figure supplement 3).

Figure 3B also shows suppression for a site with some very bursty responses which seem to drastically inflate the Y-axis (Response). Was this high variability and bursty activity common for suppressed sites? The overall spontaneous rates of many GABAergic cells is fairly high, but it's not clear if that is the case for the population explored here.

Suppressed sites were not unusually variable and bursty. The example neuron shown in Figure 3B was selected specifically because its baseline firing rate was high (it also happened to be bursty), which made the suppression effect particularly clear. Most suppressed sites had low baseline firing rates, making suppression less obvious (Figure 2—figure supplement 3). The baseline firing rates of suppressed and activated sites were similar (p=0.87; unpaired t test).

That spiking increased in >60% of recorded sites is in line with successful targeting of GABAergic interneurons. But what do we learn about these neurons? The authors discuss the potential of photo-tagging in the Discussion and provide one exemplary direction selective unit in Figure 9. But one is left with the question what happened at the other sites? Are they visually responsive?

All neurophysiological and behavioral data were collected concurrently except for the data in Figure 9, which was collected during a block of fixation trials. Once we found a site that was modulated by the laser, we focused on documenting the behavioral deficit. The non-stationarity of firing rate apparent in a few of the plots in Figure 2—figure supplement 3 is due to changes in isolation quality.

At most sites—both activated and suppressed by the laser—presentation of the Gabor stimulus evoked a response (Figure 2—figure supplement 1). Of the 56 sites, 46 had elevated responses during visual stimulation, and of those, 19 attained statistical significance (p<0.05, Mann Whitney U test). We report these numbers in the revised manuscript. The weakness of the visual response is expected. We did not tailor the visual stimulus (an achromatic, 1 cycle/° upward-drifting, hor i zontal Gabor pat tern) to the preferences of the neurons at the stimulation site, and the contrast of the Gabor stimulus was usually low because it was adjusted by a staircase procedure to be near psychophysical detection threshold.

3) Further aspects should be considered that might have influenced behavioural performance. For the behavioral tasks the authors should probably emphasize that reward contingencies were not dependent on laser delivery.

We emphasize in the revised manuscript that the reward contingencies were not dependent on laser delivery.

It was also unclear on why the measure used quantifying the effect for the oculomotor task was not simply distance from the target?

Thank you for the suggestion. We have repeated the analysis of saccade-task performance using distance from the end point to the target as suggested by the reviewer (Figure 4—figure supplement 1).

For this task, the data for Monkey 3 shown in Figure 4 even for the control trials looks like it's not right on the center of the RF location. Does this figure show exactly where the target was presented and how were the eye positions calibrated?

We have represented the target locations outside RFs in the revised figure (Figure 4). The figures show the nominal locations of the targets on the screen and calibrated estimates of eye position relative to these locations. Our eye position calibration is imperfect but is reasonably accurate (< 1° error).

Monkey 3 made inaccurate saccades into the left visual field even on some control trials (Figure 4—figure supplement 1, Figure 4E–F). This was likely due to repeated electrode penetrations into the midbrain of this animal, unrelated to the current experiments, that resulted in oculomotor deficits. This animal exhibited a leftward nystagmus that precluded accurate fixation behavior several months before the collection of data presented in this manuscript. During data collection for the current study, this animal developed several blind spots presumably due to cortical damage. The nystagmus is unrelated to the optogenetic manipulations made in this study and therefore unlikely to be of interest to the readers of eLife, but the blind spots are relevant and now discussed in the revised Discussion.

In both paradigms opto stimulation occurred at the same time as visual stimulation. Given a visual response latency of 40 ms or more in V1 neurons, at least in theory, the opto stimulus could serve as a cue telling the monkey how to act in order to get reward.

We agree with the reviewers, and have elaborated on this point in the revised manuscript. Indeed, if the monkey had been able to detect the optical stimulation, he might have been able to use this information in the saccade task to get reward on optical stimulation trials (Figure 4, Figure 4—figure supplement 1A–B). On trials in which optical stimulation was delivered and no target was visible, a saccade into the receptive fields of the stimulated neurons would often have been rewarded. The fact that the monkey did not routinely make saccades to the target in the RFs of the illuminated neurons suggests that he was unable to detect the stimulation, or at least was unable to use it to direct his saccades. In the contrast detection task, the optical stimulation does not provide a cue that is useful for getting a reward. The two possible choices are equally likely to be rewarded on both control and laser stimulation trials.

It is indicated that the change in contrast detection performance is due to the reduction in sensitivity and not a change in criterion. One cannot conclude that from d-primes only. The c-criterion should also be listed as there can be a change in sensitivity and criterion.

We now address this point in the revised manuscript. We can explain the changes in c-criterion and d’ using a model in which the effect of the laser is to make the signal distribution more similar to the noise distribution, and we include a figure for the reviewers illustrating this point (Figure 7—figure supplement 1). We are unable, however, to explain the changes in d’ on the basis of a change in subjective criterion alone; a pure change in criterion does not affect d’. We cannot rule out the possibility that the laser changes the monkeys' subjective criterion and sensitivity, but the brevity and unpredictability of the optical stimulation argues against a large change in criterion.

4) Electrophysiological and behavioural measures should be more directly related to each other. There's no obvious reason why these couldn't be done simultaneously. If possible, it would be good to see opto elicted spiking activity from the trials during behavioural testing and to probe whether there is a direct relationship between the strength of spiking and the behavioural effect.

All of the electrophysiological recordings, with the exception of those in Figure 9, were made while the monkeys were performing the contrast detection task. We clarify this point in the revised manuscript.

We looked for a relationship between the strength of optical stimulation on spiking responses and behavioral effects, and we observed a weak, positive correlation that failed to reach statistical significance (Figure 8—figure supplement 1B).

In interpreting this result, it is important to consider uninteresting reasons for finding such a relationship and also for not finding one. The laser power changed across blocks of trials. Low laser power affects neural responses and behavior weakly, and high laser power affects both strongly, which would be expected to produce a positive correlation. A reason for not detecting such a correlation is that the electrodes recorded only a fraction of the neurons affected by the optical stimulation, and the quality of the neural signal varied from day to day. These two sources of variability (laser power and recording quality), prevent us from accurately estimating the relationship between the electrophysiologically generated response (across all neurons) and the resultant behavioral effect.

Nevertheless, in one session, we manipulated the laser power on seven blocks of trials, keeping the fiber position, electrode position and the spatial location of stimulus fixed. Under these conditions, we were able to observe a clear, positive correlation between neural and behavioral modulation (Figure 8—figure supplement 1A).

5) Clarify for the detection conditions, how the authors move from the example sessions (Figure 5) to the population data (Figure 7). Some rewording here to make it clear that the comments in the paragraph below are referring to the examples in Figure 5 and not the whole population (which follows in a couple of paragraphs).

We have rewritten the confusing passages to clarify the division between the example data in Figure 5 and the population data in Figure 7.

For the population, the authors should revisit Figure 7 to not include all the blocks, as this conflates the independent sessions (11 and 12) from the blocks, which are clearly not independent. To include all the blocks in Figure 7 is a clear case for pseudo-replication. The population analysis needs to be by session, not block.

We have repeated the analysis in Figure 7, treating each session as an independent observation.

The authors should also revisit the psychometric fits (examples in Figure 5, e.g.). The laser fits don't look very good – was there some estimate of goodness of fit for these?

We agree that the fits to the psychometric function data on laser trials are not very good. There are two reasons for this. First, in many blocks, performance increased shallowly with stimulus contrast because of the strong inactivation. Performance on laser trials even at the highest contrast was therefore poor, forcing the psychometric fit to have a shallow slope within the range of contrasts tested. Second, not all stimulus contrasts were probed equally often because of the staircase procedure. This fact can give the appearance of a poor model fit. The fit takes into account the number of stimulus presentations at each contrast. As a result, the model more accurately fits the points that were probed more often. We have replotted the psychometric functions, representing the number of stimulus presentations at each contrast as the size of the corresponding data points (Figures 5–6, Figure 6—figure supplement 1).

The deviance, the measure of fitting error that is minimized in generalized linear models, was actually lower on control trials (median = -20.07) than on laser trials (median = -17.76) because of the flatness of the psychometric function over the range of contrasts we were able to test.

6) Clarify details about injection and stimulation procedures (see minor points), including heating induced damage considerations. A concern is in understanding and justifying the need for the large increase in power used to activate the neurons under study. The absolute power levels are on a direct concern if they cause lasting damage to the tissue. On one hand the prolonged efficacy across the session is evidence that effects of greater power did not present an acute problem, but there could be concern that prolonged use in a single site, for example, could lead to irreversible damage. More discussion on the power would be useful.

We have clarified the details of the injection and stimulation procedures in the revised manuscript.

During some of our initial experiments, we used high laser power because we did not know a priori the laser power needed to induce a behavioral effect. However, we show that a laser power as low as 30 mW is sufficient to achieve a strong behavioral effect (Figure 8B).

Both monkeys currently have scotomas in areas of the visual field corresponding to some of the regions of V1 inactivated. The laser power used in some experiments was unnecessarily high and likely caused thermal damage in the stimulated regions. As pointed out the reviewer, we did not observe any acute change in the behavioral effect over repeated stimulation (Figure 8C–D) but we cannot rule out that heating did not lead to permanent damage. Another likely cause of permanent damage in these experiments is mechanical due to repeated insertions of the 300 µm optical fibers. We have added discussion on these points and potential remedies for future experiments in the revised manuscript.


Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

RESOURCES