Abstract
Skin conductance monitoring is one of the promising methods for objectively evaluating pain. However, skin conductance might possibly increase in response to sympathetic stimulation other than pain. In this study, we aimed to test whether skin conductance monitoring can distinguish physical pain stimulation (heat, mechanical and cold stimulation) from other sympathetic stimuli (stimulation by noise and painful images). Twenty-three healthy volunteers participated in this prospective observational study. The number of fluctuations in skin conductance (NFSC) and normalized skin conductance level (nSCL) were measured and compared with pain scores on a self-reported pain scale (numerical pain scale [NPS]). Both NFSC and nSCL increased during mechanical stimulation. Further, nSCL, but not NFSC, well reflected heat stimulus intensity, suggesting its ability to quantitatively evaluate pain. nSCLs during physical pain stimulation were greater than those during other sympathetic stimulations. However, NFSC was not able to completely distinguish between the stimuli. These results suggest that nSCL could better differentiate physical pain stimuli from other sympathetic stimuli than NFSC. In comparisons between subjective and objective pain assessment in the same individual, nSCL correlated better with NPS score, indicating the possibility of being able to monitor the transition of pain. Monitoring changes in skin conductance using nSCL might be useful for objectively detecting physical pain.
Subject terms: Neurophysiology, Preclinical research
Introduction
Psychometric response scales, such as the visual analogue scale, have been widely used for assessment of pain1. This seems to be reasonable because the sensation of pain is usually affected by psychogenic factors, including mood, attention, anxiety, expectation, hypnosis and empathy2. This fact is reflected in the following definition of pain by the International Association for the Study of Pain: An unpleasant sensory and emotional experience with actual or potential tissue damage, or described in terms of such damage3. However, an objective pain assessment tool is still required, because verbal expression of pain is difficult for certain individuals, such as children, mentally-handicapped persons and intubated and sedated patients.
Numerous studies regarding objective pain assessment have been conducted, and several physiological markers of pain, such as heart rate variability and pupillary reflexes, were proposed. However, none of them has been established as a validated marker for pain assessment so far4,5. Among the different markers proposed, changes in skin conductance might be promising. Activation of sympathetic nerves in the skin following the experience of pain sensation and/or certain emotions, including fear and excitement, results in sweating in the palmar and plantar areas, which in turn increases skin conductance in these areas. Hence, the rationale for development of the skin conductance monitor was to detect changes in skin conductance over time6.
The principle of the skin conductance monitor raised the question regarding whether it could be used as a pain monitor. In other words, whether the skin conductance monitor can distinguish a stimulus that stimulates a sympathetic nerve but does not cause pain from a stimulus that causes pain. Few studies have, however, aimed to clarify this issue.
In most studies using the skin conductance monitor, the number of fluctuations in skin conductance (NFSC) was evaluated. Indeed, NFSC reportedly increased in response to painful stimuli7,8 and well reflected pain intensity9–14, but not in other studies15–18. These results suggested that NFSC is not necessarily useful for the assessment of pain. On the other hand, the normalized skin conductance level (nSCL) has been used to discriminate pain intensity in several studies. In one study, nSCL was reportedly even better than NFSC19.
Based on this evidence, we hypothesized that it might be possible for the skin conductance monitor to discriminate between physical pain stimuli and other sympathetic stimuli by using nSCL rather than NFSC as the assessment parameter. To investigate this hypothesis, we conducted the present study on healthy volunteers who were exposed to various experimental stimuli. Further, as already shown by previous studies19,20, we examined whether the skin conductance monitor can differentiate various thermal pain intensities.
Material and methods
Participants
This study was approved by the institutional review board of Gunma University Graduate School of Medicine (Approval No. 1031) and was registered with the University Hospital Medical Information Network Clinical Trials Registry (UMIN-CTR ID: 000,014,614). The study was conducted between February 2014 and October 2015 in accordance with institutional ethics provisions and the Declaration of Helsinki. The participants in the present study were 23 healthy volunteers, including 14 women and 9 men, ranging in age from 20 to 48 years old. Written informed consent was obtained from each participant. Inclusion criteria were: (1) age between 20 and 60 years, and (2) ability to understand the purpose and instructions of the study. Exclusion criteria were: (1) suffering from any type of chronic pain; (2) inability to tolerate a heat stimulus; (3) use of medication(s) that can potentially affect the activity of the autonomic nervous system; (4) not suitable for receiving a physical pain stimulus because of severe pre-existing disease; and (5) pregnancy.
The sample size of this study was determined by the results of our pilot study, in which a total of six subjects participated. In the pilot study, we applied stimuli at 32 °C and 47 °C in random order. As a result, the mean difference in nSCL values and averaged SD values of nSCL after application of the 32 °C and 47 °C stimuli was determined to be 351 and 460, respectively. The standardized mean difference was 0.75. From this value, a significance level of 0.05 and a power of 80%, the required sample size was calculated as 16 subjects. Anticipating a drop-out rate of up to 20% because the intervention in this study might have been painful, 23 individuals were recruited to the study.
State-Trait Anxiety Assessment
We measured the degree of anxiety in participants before the training session by using the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory-Form JYZ (STAI-JYZ) questionnaire, because we considered the possibility that anxiety might affect the test results9,21. The questionnaire we used was the Japanese version of STAI (Form Y) for measurement of emotional states (STAI Y-1) and personality traits (STAI Y-2). The original version of the questionnaire was developed by Gaudry et al.22, following which the Japanese version was published23. Since each questionnaire consists of 20 questions and each question is allotted 1–4 points, the total scores of each questionnaire range from 20 to 80 points. Although assessment of the degree of anxiety in participants during/after painful events is suggested to be important 9, such assessments were not performed in this study.
Study procedure
All tests were performed by a single tester in the same room of Gunma University Hospital. We monitored the temperature of the room used for the experiments and confirmed that it was tightly controlled (25.3 ± 1.1 °C).
Training session
All the participants were trained to evaluate pain intensity before the main tests. The participants were asked to assess pain on a numerical pain scale (NPS: from 0 = no pain to 100 = the worst pain imaginable), which they expressed verbally at the end of each stimulus period. In the training session, a thermode of 30 × 30 mm (Pathway Model ATS, Medoc Ltd., Ramat Yishai, Israel) was attached to the volar forearm on the dominant hand side, and heat stimuli were applied through the thermode. The thermode temperature was gradually raised from 32 °C (8 °C/s) to reach the target temperature (45, 46, or 47 °C) within 2 s, and was maintained at this temperature for 7 s. Each target temperature was chosen once in random order. This training session was repeated three times at 1 min intervals.
Main tests
All the participants were exposed to physical pain stimuli (heat, mechanical and cold) and other sympathetic stimuli (noisy auditory stimulus and visual stimulus that evoked the thought of pain). The participants drew lots to determine the order of presentation of these five categorized stimuli, so that the order of stimulation was random, and the participants were blinded to the order in which the stimuli would be presented. Each stimulus session involved waiting (5–10 min), pre-stimulus (20 s), stimulus (60 s) and post-stimulus periods (30 s) (Fig. 1a).
To obtain stabilized values of skin conductance before each stimulus, participants were asked to wait for 5–10 min while wearing noise-cancelling headphones (BOSE, QuietComfort 15) and closing their eyes (waiting period). Participants were asked to open their eyes 20 s before presentation of each stimulus (pre-stimulus period). All stimulations lasted for 60 s (stimulus period). After exposure to the stimulus, the participants were asked to remain still for 30 s (post-stimulus period). During the physical pain stimulus period (i.e., heat, mechanical and cold stimuli), participants were asked to verbally rate pain intensity using the NPS every 10 s. For all stimuli, participants were also asked to verbally rate unpleasantness on a numerical scale (from 0 = not unpleasant at all to 100 = extremely unpleasant) after the post-stimulus period, to evaluate the emotional impact of each stimulus20. Pain intensity and unpleasantness refer to the sensory-discriminate component and emotional-motivational component of pain, respectively.
Stimuli
For heat stimulation, three temperatures were adopted: 46 °C, 47 °C and 48 °C. We adopted multiple stimuli of different temperatures because a secondary aim of this study was evaluation of the ability of the skin conductance monitor to assess quantitative pain. The same equipment was used as in the training session. Although the stimulus period was 60 s, as with the other stimuli, the stimulus period for 48 °C was shortened to 30 s to avoid burn injury, in accordance with the users’ manual. The order in which the target temperature was applied was determined by additional lottery draws by the participants. For convenience, the control stimulus of 32 °C was included in the heat category. Hence, the lottery was used to determine the order of stimulation by four targeted temperatures.
For mechanical stimulation, the tester applied pressure on the inter-digital web between the second and third fingers of the non-dominant hand, using an algometer to which a 1 cm2 rubber tip was attached (Somedic AB, Stockholm, Sweden)24. The pressure was increased from 0 to 250–300 kPa at a rate of 30 kPa/s.
For cold stimulation, we applied frozen ice packs (size, 12 × 6 cm) on the medial aspect of each participant’s non-dominant forearm25–27.
For auditory stimulation, the participants continued wearing headphones that were connected to an audiometer (AA-76, Rion Co., Ltd., Tokyo), and were forced to listen to masking sounds (band noise, 1,000 Hz, 85 dB) generated by the audiometer.
For visual stimulation, an image of an 18 G needle penetrating the forearm was shown on a display placed in front of the participant28. The participants were asked to imagine that the arm on the display was their arm.
The volume of noise and the picture used for visual stimulation were determined so that the unpleasantness score was comparable between stimuli. In our pilot study, we confirmed that the unpleasantness score of all types of stimuli was comparable.
Measurement and processing of skin conductance
Skin conductance was recorded using the Pain Monitor (Med-Storm, Oslo, Norway) with three circular electrodes (1-cm diameter). A sample recording of skin conductance is shown in Fig. 1a. The sampling rate was 65 Hz. Measuring electrodes, a counter current electrode, and a reference voltage electrode were positioned on the hypothenar and thenar eminences and below the middle finger of the dominant hand, respectively6,29. To measure and analyze skin conductance values, we developed our own software program for converting the graphic data acquired by the Pain Monitor to numerical values. SCL and NFSC were calculated as previously reported19. SCL was defined as the average skin conductance value (μS) for 10 s. SCL was normalized by calculating the percentage change from the pre-stimulus average: normalized SCL (nSCL) = 100 × (SCL − average SCL in the pre-stimulus period)/average SCL in the pre-stimulus period 19. Average SCL in the pre-stimulus period was defined as the averaged SCL of the 60 s just before asking the participant to open their eyes for preparation of the stimulus period. Skin conductance fluctuations were defined as peaks with minimum amplitudes of 0.02 μS and a slope rate < 2 μS/s29. The NFSC value was defined as the number of skin conductance fluctuations during the 10 s period.
Analysis
Analysis was performed using Sigmaplot version 12.5 (Systat Software Inc., CA, USA). One-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with post-hoc Student–Newman–Keuls test was used for comparison of unpleasantness scores, NPS, nSCL and NFSC among all types of stimuli. Pearson’s correlation coefficients between total STAI score and unpleasantness score were calculated to investigate the relationship between the degree of anxiety and subjective evaluations. Similarly, the relationship between total STAI score and NPS was also examined.
Receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve analysis was performed to investigate the cut off value, sensitivity and specificity of skin conductance variables in distinguishing each physical pain stimulus from other sympathetic stimuli. Moreover, the area under the ROC curve of nSCL and NFSC was compared. For the comparison between NPS and skin conductance values (i.e. nSCL and NFSC), non-parametric Spearman rank order correlation analysis was performed because some of these data were not normally distributed.
Results
Participants
Although one participant declined exposure to the 48 °C heat stimulus because of unendurable pain, the remaining 22 participants were able to complete the study. There were no severe complications, including heat injury with blisters, throughout the study in any of the participants. The participants’ characteristics are shown in Table 1.
Table 1.
Number | Age (year) | STAI score | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
State anxiety | Trait anxiety | |||
Male | 9 | 34.8 ± 10.3 | 29.2 ± 5.3 | 32 ± 4.8 |
Female | 14 | 23.5 ± 4.3 | 31.3 ± 5.5 | 34.2 ± 5.5 |
Total | 23 | 28.0 ± 9.3 | 30.5 ± 5.4 | 33.3 ± 5.3 |
All values are expressed as the mean ± SD.
Subjective evaluation of the stimuli
Unpleasantness scores during each stimulus are shown in Fig. 1b. The only difference in unpleasantness scores was that between control (32 °C) stimulation and all other stimuli (P < 0.05, one-way ANOVA with post-hoc Student–Newman–Keuls test). These results suggest that the emotional impact of these stimuli, except for the control stimulus, was comparable.
Verbal responses (i.e. NPS scores) during physical pain stimulation are shown in Fig. 1c and d. Since stimulation at 48 °C was applied for only 30 s, it was excluded from statistical analysis. The only difference in NPS scores was that between control (32 °C) stimulation and all other stimuli (Fig. 1d, P < 0.05, one-way ANOVA with post-hoc Student–Newman–Keuls test). These results demonstrated that there was no difference in subjective evaluation of physical pain stimuli (heat, mechanical, and cold stimuli).
Effect of anxiety on subjective evaluation
We next investigated the relationship between anxiety scores and subjective evaluation of stimuli. The degree of anxiety in all participants was assessed by the STAI before the experiment (Table 1). STAI scores (anxiety scores) did not correlate with unpleasantness scores (47 °C heat; r = 0.03, mechanical; r = − 0.03, cold; r = − 0.05, audio; r = − 0.07, visual; r = 0.35). Moreover, STAI scores did not correlate with NPS scores (47 °C heat; r = 0.008, mechanical; r = 0.01, cold; r = 0.15). These results suggest that anxiety status did not appear to affect the subjective evaluation of unpleasantness or subjective pain evaluation.
Objective evaluation of stimuli
Several past studies suggested the superiority of nSCL to NFSC for differentiating heat intensities19,20. In order to validate the results of these past studies, heat stimuli at various temperatures (32, 45, 46 and 47 °C, 48 °C) were applied in this study, and NFSC and nSCL were compared. NFSC did not reflect the temperature of heat stimuli (Fig. 2a). There were no significant differences in the area under the curve between the different heat stimulus temperatures (Fig. 2b, P > 0.05, one-way ANOVA post hoc Student–Newman–Keuls test). nSCL on the hand well reflected heat intensity (Fig. 2c), with significant differences in nSCL between control stimulation and each heat stimulus temperature (P < 0.05, one-way ANOVA post hoc Student–Newman–Keuls test, Fig. 2d).
Next, we examined whether skin monitoring can distinguish the physical pain stimulus from other sympathetic stimuli. Changes in NFSC during all types of stimulation are shown in Fig. 3a. NFSC was not able to distinguish the physical pain stimulus (i.e. 47 °C heat, mechanical and cold stimuli) from other sympathetic stimuli (i.e. noise and visual stimuli), as shown in Fig. 3a. The only exception was NFSC after mechanical stimulation. NFSC during mechanical stimulation was greater than that during other stimuli (Fig. 3b, P < 0.05, one-way ANOVA post hoc Student–Newman–Keuls test). Changes in nSCL during all types of stimulation are shown in Fig. 3c. nSCL during control (32 °C) stimulation was smaller than that during application of any other stimuli (Fig. 3c, d, P < 0.05, one-way ANOVA post hoc Student–Newman–Keuls test). Moreover, nSCLs during physical pain stimulation (heat and mechanical stimuli) were greater than those during other sympathetic stimulations (Fig. 3c, d, P < 0.05, one-way ANOVA post hoc Student–Newman–Keuls test). These results suggest that nSCL could better differentiate the common physical pain stimuli (mechanical and heat stimuli) from other sympathetic stimuli than NFSC.
Receiver operating curve analysis
We performed ROC curve analysis to test whether NFSC and nSCL could discriminate between physical pain stimuli and other sympathetic stimuli. Sample ROC curves are shown in Fig. 4. Figure 4a shows ROC curves of NFSC and nSCL when mechanical and visual stimuli were compared, and Fig. 4b shows ROC curves of NFSC and nSCL when cold and auditory stimuli were compared. Table 2 presents a summary of ROC curve analysis. The accuracy for differentiating physical pain stimuli from other sympathetic stimuli was evaluated using area under the ROC curve analysis. The area under the ROC curve of NFSC was 0.6 or more for both heat (47 °C) and mechanical stimuli as compared to control (32 °C), auditory and visual stimuli. This was also true for nSCL. However, the area under the ROC curve of NFSC was smaller than 0.6 with cold stimuli as compared to control (32 °C), auditory and visual stimuli. Again, this was also true for nSCL. These results indicate that both NFSC and nSCL could discriminate common physical pain (heat and mechanical stimulation) from other stimuli (including control). However, neither NFSC nor nSCL could distinguish cold stimulation from other stimuli. Next, we compared NFSC and nSCL to determine their discriminatory ability. The gray cells in Table 2 show that the area under the ROC curve of nSCL was greater than that of NFSC (P < 0.05). This suggests that nSCL can distinguish between mechanical stimulation and stimulation without a painful sensation (control, auditory and visual stimuli) with high accuracy compared to NFSC. For heat stimulation (47 °C), the discriminatory superiority of nSCL was shown only in comparison with the control stimulus (Table 2).
Table 2.
Control (32 °C) | Auditory | Visual | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Cut off | Sensitivity Specificity |
Area | Cut off | Sensitivity Specificity |
Area | Cut off | Sensitivity Specificity |
Area | ||
47 °C | nSCL | 21.84 |
0.70 0.70 |
0.76 | 68.97 |
0.56 0.58 |
0.60 | 39.69 |
0.64 0.63 |
0.66 |
NFSC | 0.50 |
0.59 0.57 |
0.62 | 0.50 |
0.59 0.54 |
0.60 | 1.5 |
0.42 0.78 |
0.60 | |
Mech | nSCL | 93.14 |
0.76 0.87 |
0.89 | 152.00 |
0.64 0.78 |
0.77 | 102.80 |
0.75 0.75 |
0.80 |
NFSC | 0.50 |
0.75 0.57 |
0.70 | 0.50 |
0.75 0.54 |
0.68 | 0.50 |
0.75 0.54 |
0.68 | |
Cold | nSCL | 22.21 |
0.48 0.70 |
0.59 | – | – | 0.41 | – | – | 0.48 |
NFSC | 0.5 |
0.55 0.57 |
0.56 | 0.50 |
0.55 0.54 |
0.54 | 0.5 |
0.55 0.54 |
0.54 |
nSCL could discriminate between common physical pain (heat and mechanical stimulation) and other types of stimuli (including control) significantly better than NFSC. In the stimulus combination where the values of the area under the curve are shown in bold, the area under the curve of nSCL is greater than that of NFSC (P < 0.05). Since the area under the curve of nSCL during cold stimulation was less than 0.5 when it was compared to that during both auditory and visual stimulation, we did not calculate cut off values, sensitivity and specificity for these comparisons. Mech mechanical.
Correlation between subjective and objective evaluations
Correlation coefficients between subjective evaluations (unpleasantness score or NPS score) and changes in skin conductance values (NFSC or nSCL) were calculated to compare subjective and objective evaluations of pain. We found no significant correlation between unpleasantness score and skin conductance value (data not shown). There was a weak correlation (r = 0.28) between NPS and nSCL scores only for the mechanical stimulus (Table S1).
In addition to the analyses using all population data, correlation coefficients between NPS and skin conductance (NFSC or nSCL) for each individual were calculated. This was done because although the correlation between NPS and skin conductance (NFSC or nSCL) was poor in the analysis using the entire population data (Table S1), we speculated that this poor correlation might be due to a large inter-participant variability in skin conductance responses.
Intra-individual analysis showed that nSCL correlated with NPS in 16 out of 23 participants (70%), whereas NFSC correlated with NPS in only eight out of 23 participants (35%), as shown in Fig. 4C. The correlation coefficients between NPS and nSCL were greater than those between NPS and NFSC (Fig. 4c, P < 0.001, Spearman rank order correlation analysis).
Discussion
We showed here that both the skin conductance parameters, NFSC and SCL, can differentiate between physical pain stimuli (except the cold stimulus) and other sympathetic stimuli, although the accuracy was higher with nSCL than NFSC. Moreover, nSCL showed particularly good agreement with subjective pain assessment in individual analysis.
The purpose of this study was to test whether skin conductance monitoring can distinguish between stimuli that stimulate the sympathetic nerves but do not cause pain and those that cause pain as well as stimulate these nerves. We measured unpleasantness scores and confirmed that their values during pain stimulation were comparable to those with other sympathetic stimulation (Fig. 1b), suggesting similar emotional impacts of each stimulus.
Skin conductance, especially nSCL, was found to be able to distinguish between physical pain stimuli and other sympathetic stimuli. Painful stimuli seem to naturally accompany various other stimuli, including tactile and mental stimuli. Although the skin conductance monitor indicated a response even to stimuli that did not cause pain, the degree of the reaction was weaker than the response to painful stimuli. We believe that the skin conductance monitor can be used as a pain-specific monitor. The mild increase in nSCL after stimulation with auditory and painful visual images support this idea (Fig. 3).
Elevation of nSCL during cold stimulation was smaller than that with other physical pain stimuli (Fig. 3d), although subjective evaluation showed that pain due to cold stimulation was equivalent to that of other pain stimuli (i.e., heat and mechanical). Although there was no difference in subjective pain assessment when comparing heat and cold stimulation, muscle sympathetic nerve activity was higher during heat stimulation as compared to cold stimulation in a previous study30. Although this result is consistent with our research results, it is necessary to investigate methods of objective pain assessment during cold stimulation. From another point of view, the area in contact with the skin when the ice pack for cold stimulation was pressed against the skin was greater than that for heat and mechanical stimulation. These differences in the size of the area over which the stimulus was applied might have affected changes in skin conductance. In addition, since there is no definite view on the transmission pathway of skin sensations that eventually stimulate the sympathetic nerves and produce the resultant biological responses, including sweating, further research on this is necessary.
Most research on the evaluation of pain using skin conductance monitors first categorized the degree of pain and then determined the threshold of NFSC in each category10,15,16,19. However, since we observed changes in skin conductance with various experimental stimuli, we determined thresholds according to the type of stimulation, and not the degree of pain. In order to directly compare our results with those of previous studies that first categorized the degree of pain, we examined the degree of pain experienced by the participants with each stimulus. In particular, we referred to the paper by Treister et al. for comparison19. They adjusted the stimulation temperature for each individual so that NPS score would fall into three categories (“low pain”: ~ 30, “medium pain”: ~ 60 and “high pain”: ~ 90). As a result, “medium pain”, with an NPS score of 53.5 ± 18.9, was generated by a heat stimulus of 47.43 ± 0.63 °C, and “high pain”, with an NPS score of 70.2 ± 18.9, was generated by heat stimulation at a temperature of 48.34 ± 0.33°C19. In the present study, pain with NPS scores of 48.13 ± 24.18 and 67.62 ± 21.04 were generated by heat stimulation at temperatures of 46 °C and 47 °C, respectively. These results suggest that heat stimulation at 47 °C in this study is considered to have caused pain corresponding to “high pain”. It is unknown why the participants of this study expressed NPS scores equivalent to “high pain” at lower temperatures (i.e. 47 °C) than in the previous study. Treister et al. reported that both NFSC and SCL could distinguish between “high pain” and “no pain” to the same extent (P < 0.001), but SCL rather than NFSC could more accurately distinguish pain intensity on the whole19. These results are consistent with our results. However, since they did not evaluate pain intensity by determining the pain threshold, we further compared our results with other studies that analyzed the responses in a similar manner as our method. Since our participants did not include children, our results were compared to those in adults. In a previous study on postoperative pain, when no pain to mild pain (score 0–3) and moderate to severe pain (score 4–10) were distinguished by setting the threshold of NFSC to 0.1, the sensitivity and specificity was reportedly 88.5% and 67.7%, respectively10. However, in the case of awake patients as compared to immediate post-anesthesia patients, other stresses might be included in skin responses judged to be less severe than moderate pain (score < 4–5)31. In the present study, absence of sedation in the participants might have affected the assessment of pain using NFSC.
Use of NFSC assessment is advantageous over nSCL evaluation, since devices for evaluating pain using NFSC are commercially available. Further, many studies have shown that NFSC assessment can be used to evaluate pain in patients of all ages and in various clinical settings, including postoperative and intensive care units9–11,15–18,32,33. In this study, however, nSCL had a better correlation with subjective pain assessment compared to NFSC within the same individual (Fig. 4c), indicating that nSCL might be useful for monitoring the transition of pain in each individual. This is consistent with the results of a previous study 16, in which the authors argued that nSCL did not significantly correlate with each participant’s pain rating at the group level despite the strong within-participant correlations. Although this is not a new way of standardization19,20, obtaining a “stimulation mean” and “prestimulation mean” for calculating changes in skin conductance might help to minimize inter-individual differences. Further investigations to test whether nSCL can evaluate pain in clinical practice will be needed in the future, because studies using nSCL, including the present study, targeted experimental pain19.
Self-reported pain is influenced by several factors, including previous pain experiences, gender, cultural background, sociodemographic factors, anxiety and depression12. For example, preoperative anxiety has been reported to affect postoperative pain21. Further, in a study on experimental pain, the anxiety level reportedly correlated with the numerical rating scale of pain9. In this study, however, anxiety status did not correlate with subjective pain evaluation. This discrepancy might have resulted from the fact that only a few of the participants in our study were anxious. Alternatively, the difference in the evaluation method of anxiety level might have influenced the results. For example, anxiety status in our participants was measured only at the beginning of the experiment, although it could have changed during the course of the study. Lack of examination of anxiety status when the stimuli were applied is a limitation of this study.
Since skin temperature reportedly correlates with the skin conductance value33, lack of skin temperature monitoring might be another limitation of this study. However, since we expect that changes in the participants’ skin temperatures during the experiments were not significant, we believe that the influence of skin temperature on the skin conductance value in this study was minimal.
Several researchers have shown the efficacy of objective pain evaluation using multiple physiological parameters, including “nociception level index”34. Indeed, devices using four sensors, namely a photoplethysmograph, galvanic skin response, temperature and accelerometer, are already commercially available for use in a clinical setting5,34–36. The results of the present study suggest that nSCL should also be considered as one of the many parameters that can be used in such monitors. In future, since skin conductance monitoring is minimally invasive, it might become a common tool for monitoring pain in the clinical setting.
Conclusion
Although both NFSC and nSCL increase during mechanical stimulation, only nSCL increases during heat stimulation. This study showed that nSCL is more useful for the detection of physical pain than NFSC. Skin conductance monitors appear to be useful for detecting physical pain in the clinical setting.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgement
We thank Amy MacDermott for her helpful comments on the manuscript. This study was supported by JSPS KAKENHI Grant Numbers 15K10533 and 24592326 for T.T., and 16H02678 for S.S. This work was partially conducted by the MEXT-Supported Program for the Strategic Research Foundation at Private Universities, “Creation of 3D nano-micro structures and its application to biomimetics and medicine”, 2015–2019.
Author contributions
S.S. and T.T. conceived the project. S.S. and T.T. designed the experiments. S.S. performed the experiments. S.S. gave advice on the experiments. S.S. and T.T. wrote the manuscript. All authors read and edited the manuscript prior to publication.
Data availability
The datasets of the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Competing interests
We have no conflict of interest to declare.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
is available for this paper at 10.1038/s41598-020-67936-0.
References
- 1.Joyce CR, Zutshi DW, Hrubes V, Mason RM. Comparison of fixed interval and visual analogue scales for rating chronic pain. Eur. J. Clin. Pharmacol. 1975;8:415–420. doi: 10.1007/bf00562315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Garcia-Larrea L, Peyron R. Pain matrices and neuropathic pain matrices: a review. Pain. 2013;154(Suppl 1):S29–S43. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2013.09.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Bonica JJ. The need of a taxonomy. Pain. 1979;6:247–248. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(79)90046-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Cowen R, Stasiowska MK, Laycock H, Bantel C. Assessing pain objectively: the use of physiological markers. Anaesthesia. 2015;70:828–847. doi: 10.1111/anae.13018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Ledowski T. Objective monitoring of nociception: a review of current commercial solutions. Br. J. Anaesth. 2019 doi: 10.1016/j.bja.2019.03.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Storm H. Changes in skin conductance as a tool to monitor nociceptive stimulation and pain. Curr. Opin. Anaesthesiol. 2008;21:796–804. doi: 10.1097/ACO.0b013e3283183fe4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Aslanidis T, Grosomanidis V, Karakoulas K, Chatzisotiriou A. Electrodermal activity monitoring during painful stimulation in sedated adult intensive care unit patients: a pilot study. Acta Med. (Hradec Kralove) 2018;61:47–52. doi: 10.14712/18059694.2018.50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Aslanidis T, Grosomanidis V, Karakoulas K, Chatzisotiriou A. Electrodermal activity monitoring during endotracheal suction in sedated adult intensive care unit patients. Folia Med. (Plovdiv) 2018;60:92–101. doi: 10.1515/folmed-2017-0063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Storm H, Gunther A, Sackey PV, Bernhardsson J, Bjarta A. Measuring pain-physiological and self-rated measurements in relation to pain stimulation and anxiety. Acta Anaesthesiol. Scand. 2019;63:668–675. doi: 10.1111/aas.13323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Ledowski T, et al. Monitoring of skin conductance to assess postoperative pain intensity. Br. J. Anaesth. 2006;97:862–865. doi: 10.1093/bja/ael280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Gunther AC, et al. Palmar skin conductance variability and the relation to stimulation, pain and the motor activity assessment scale in intensive care unit patients. Crit. Care. 2013;17:R51. doi: 10.1186/cc12571. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Hansen JO, et al. Skin conductance as a pain assessment tool during chest tube removal: an observational study. Eur. J. Pain. 2017;21:987–996. doi: 10.1002/ejp.999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Hullett B, et al. Monitoring electrical skin conductance: a tool for the assessment of postoperative pain in children? Anesthesiology. 2009;111:513–517. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0b013e3181b27c18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Ledowski T, et al. The assessment of postoperative pain by monitoring skin conductance: results of a prospective study. Anaesthesia. 2007;62:989–993. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2044.2007.05191.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Ledowski T, Ang B, Schmarbeck T, Rhodes J. Monitoring of sympathetic tone to assess postoperative pain: skin conductance vs surgical stress index. Anaesthesia. 2009;64:727–731. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2044.2008.05834.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Choo EK, et al. Skin conductance fluctuations correlate poorly with postoperative self-report pain measures in school-aged children. Anesthesiology. 2010;113:175–182. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0b013e3181de6ce9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Harrison D, et al. Skin conductance as a measure of pain and stress in hospitalised infants. Early Hum. Dev. 2006;82:603–608. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2005.12.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Czaplik M, et al. Acute pain therapy in postanesthesia care unit directed by skin conductance: a randomized controlled trial. PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e41758. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0041758. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Treister R, Kliger M, Zuckerman G, Goor Aryeh I, Eisenberg E. Differentiating between heat pain intensities: the combined effect of multiple autonomic parameters. Pain. 2012;153:1807–1814. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2012.04.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Loggia ML, Juneau M, Bushnell MC. Autonomic responses to heat pain: heart rate, skin conductance, and their relation to verbal ratings and stimulus intensity. Pain. 2011;152:592–598. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2010.11.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Kain ZN, Sevarino F, Alexander GM, Pincus S, Mayes LC. Preoperative anxiety and postoperative pain in women undergoing hysterectomy: a repeated-measures design. J. Psychosom. Res. 2000;49:417–422. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(00)00189-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Gaudry E, Vagg P, Spielberger CD. Validation of the state-trait distinction in anxiety research. Multivar. Behav. Res. 1975;10:331–341. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr1003_6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Hidano, T., Iwasaki, M., Soga, S., & Spielberger, C. D. State Trait Anxiety Inventory (Form JYZ) Test Manual (Japanese Adaptation of STAI). (2000).
- 24.Neziri AY, et al. Factor analysis of responses to thermal, electrical, and mechanical painful stimuli supports the importance of multi-modal pain assessment. Pain. 2011;152:1146–1155. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2011.01.047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Posner J, Telekes A, Crowley D, Phillipson R, Peck AW. Effects of an opiate on cold-induced pain and the CNS in healthy volunteers. Pain. 1985;23:73–82. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(85)90232-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Chen AC, Dworkin SF, Haug J, Gehrig J. Human pain responsivity in a tonic pain model: psychological determinants. Pain. 1989;37:143–160. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(89)90126-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Ogino Y, Kakeda T, Nakamura K, Saito S. Dehydration enhances pain-evoked activation in the human brain compared with rehydration. Anesth. Analg. 2014;118:1317–1325. doi: 10.1213/ANE.0b013e3182a9b028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Ogino Y, et al. Inner experience of pain: imagination of pain while viewing images showing painful events forms subjective pain representation in human brain. Cereb. Cortex. 2007;17:1139–1146. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Storm H, Fremming A, Odegaard S, Martinsen OG, Morkrid L. The development of a software program for analyzing spontaneous and externally elicited skin conductance changes in infants and adults. Clin. Neurophysiol. Off. J. Int. Fed. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2000;111:1889–1898. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(00)00421-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Lautenschlager G, et al. The impact of baroreflex function on endogenous pain control: a microneurography study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2015;42:2996–3003. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13096. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Storm H. Why do similar studies conclude differently when they are performed with nearly the same protocol and the same skin conductance technology and on the same population of patients? Anesthesiology. 2011;114:464–465. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0b013e31820704f8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Ledowski T, Albus S, Stein J, Macdonald B. Skin conductance for monitoring of acute pain in adult postoperative patients: influence of electrode surface area and sampling time. J. Clin. Monit. Comput. 2011;25:371–376. doi: 10.1007/s10877-011-9314-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Valkenburg AJ, Niehof SP, van Dijk M, Verhaar EJ, Tibboel D. Skin conductance peaks could result from changes in vital parameters unrelated to pain. Pediatr. Res. 2012;71:375–379. doi: 10.1038/pr.2011.72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Ben-Israel N, Kliger M, Zuckerman G, Katz Y, Edry R. Monitoring the nociception level: a multi-parameter approach. J. Clin. Monit. Comput. 2013;27:659–668. doi: 10.1007/s10877-013-9487-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Edry R, Recea V, Dikust Y, Sessler DI. Preliminary intraoperative validation of the nociception level index: a noninvasive nociception monitor. Anesthesiology. 2016;125:193–203. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0000000000001130. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Martini CH, et al. Ability of the nociception level, a multiparameter composite of autonomic signals, to detect noxious stimuli during propofol-remifentanil anesthesia. Anesthesiology. 2015;123:524–534. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0000000000000757. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The datasets of the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.