Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2020 Jun 23;9:e53908. doi: 10.7554/eLife.53908

Tuning of feedforward control enables stable muscle force-length dynamics after loss of autogenic proprioceptive feedback

Joanne C Gordon 1, Natalie C Holt 2, Andrew Biewener 3, Monica A Daley 1,4,
Editors: K VijayRaghavan5, K VijayRaghavan6
PMCID: PMC7334023  PMID: 32573432

Abstract

Animals must integrate feedforward, feedback and intrinsic mechanical control mechanisms to maintain stable locomotion. Recent studies of guinea fowl (Numida meleagris) revealed that the distal leg muscles rapidly modulate force and work output to minimize perturbations in uneven terrain. Here we probe the role of reflexes in the rapid perturbation responses of muscle by studying the effects of proprioceptive loss. We induced bilateral loss of autogenic proprioception in the lateral gastrocnemius muscle (LG) using self-reinnervation. We compared in vivo muscle dynamics and ankle kinematics in birds with reinnervated and intact LG. Reinnervated and intact LG exhibit similar steady state mechanical function and similar work modulation in response to obstacle encounters. Reinnervated LG exhibits 23ms earlier steady-state activation, consistent with feedforward tuning of activation phase to compensate for lost proprioception. Modulation of activity duration is impaired in rLG, confirming the role of reflex feedback in regulating force duration in intact muscle.

Research organism: Other

Introduction

Sensory feedback is widely accepted as an integral component of vertebrate locomotor control (Cohen, 1992; Donelan and Pearson, 2004; Grillner, 2011; Prochazka and Ellaway, 2012; Rossignol et al., 2006). Proprioception from muscle mechanoreceptors contributes to 1) short-latency reflexes via spinal mono- and polysynaptic pathways to regulate the ongoing activity and muscle mechanical output (force, stiffness, impedance and work) and 2) longer-latency responses to coordinate and maintain task-level goals for balance and movement (Grillner, 2011; Lam and Pearson, 2002; Frigon and Rossignol, 2006; Prochazka and Ellaway, 2012; Proske and Gandevia, 2012; Rossignol et al., 2006; Safavynia and Ting, 2013; Sherrington and Laslett, 1903; Sherrington, 1910). Proprioceptive reflexes can occur through autogenic (self-generated) pathways arising from the same muscle, and through heterogenic pathways arising from other muscles via spinal interneurons (Abelew et al., 2000; Frigon and Rossignol, 2006; Lam and Pearson, 2002; Nichols, 1989; Ross and Nichols, 2009). Thus, the relationship between a specific sensory signal and its resulting effects is complex and dynamic.

Despite recognized functions of proprioception, the relative contribution of feedback control in high-speed locomotion remains unclear. Sensorimotor delay constrains how quickly an animal can sense and respond to a stimulus using feedback control (More and Donelan, 2018; More et al., 2010). The fastest possible feedback loop occurs through mono-synaptic reflexes, which involve a delay that increases in proportion to nerve transmission distance. This reflex delay becomes a larger fraction of the stride cycle with increasing speed, limiting time available for reflex-mediated correction.

The challenges of long delays relative to stride cycle times likely necessitates greater reliance on feedforward control strategies at higher speeds. Here we use feedforward to refer to the contributions to motor output arising from the motor cortex, descending pathways and rhythmic spinal networks (Frigon and Rossignol, 2006; Pearson, 2000; Yakovenko et al., 2004). Rhythmic spinal networks can generate the basic flexion and extension motor pattern for gait, even when proprioceptive feedback is removed (e.g., Pearson et al., 2003; Sharp and Bekoff, 2015). Normally, however, descending networks act in concert with spinal networks and feedback, using multimodal and distributed sensory inputs to update state estimates, regulate rhythm and control foot placement (Cohen, 1992; Drew and Marigold, 2015; Marigold and Drew, 2017; Pearson, 2000; Roth et al., 2014; Todorov, 2004; Potocanac et al., 2014; Wolpert et al., 2011). Consequently, there is no ‘pure’ feedfoward control within vertebrate systems. We use the term here as a pragmatic distinction, where feedforward refers to anticipatory ‘look-ahead’ control over one or more stride cycles, and feedback refers to reflex-mediated reactive responses to perturbations.

Although feedforward networks normally act in concert with feedback, feedforward motor activation coupled to intrinsic muscle properties can be sufficient to produce stable gait (Yakovenko et al., 2004). Consistent with this, the lateral gastrocnemius (LG) of guinea fowl (Numida meleagris) rapidly absorbs energy in response to unexpected drop perturbations (Daley et al., 2009), stabilizing high speed running without a reflex response. The rapid perturbation response arises from the intrinsic mechanical properties of the muscle-tendon tissues and musculoskeletal system (Brown and Loeb, 2000; Loeb et al., 1999; Jindrich and Full, 2002Azizi et al., 2008). Intrinsic mechanical responses can be actively tuned by the specific feedforward pattern of muscle activation. For example, humans hopping on surfaces with randomized, sudden increases in ground stiffness show a feedforward increase in muscle co-activation and knee flexion, increasing mechanical stability (Moritz and Farley, 2004). However, many perturbation responses involve multiple control mechanisms that overlap in time. Guinea fowl running over obstacles use a combination of feedforward, intrinsic mechanical and reflex-mediated mechanisms, with a delay of ~40 ms for reflex-mediated increases in muscle force (6ms reflex latency + 34ms force development delay: Daley et al., 2009; Daley and Biewener, 2011). Considering that the feedforward and intrinsic mechanical contributions alter ongoing muscle dynamics before the reflex-mediated response, it is difficult to disentangle the specific contributions of proprioceptive reflexes to the observed perturbation responses (Daley and Biewener, 2011; Gordon et al., 2015).

Investigating the role of proprioception through self-reinnervation

Here we probe the integration of feedforward, feedback and intrinsic mechanical control by eliciting a proprioceptive deficit in the lateral gastrocnemius muscle (LG) of the guinea fowl (Numida meleagris) using bilateral self-reinnervation (Figure 1). Self-reinnervation involves peripheral nerve branch transection and immediate repair, resulting in recovery of motor output with long-term, local loss of autogenic muscle proprioception (Cope et al., 1994; Bullinger et al., 2011). Self-reinnervation occurs through axonal regrowth and reconnection with denervated tissues over a recovery period of 4-8 weeks (Carr et al., 2010; Cope et al., 1994; Gordon and Stein, 1982; Vannucci et al., 2019). Reinnervated muscles retain a deficit in the monosynaptic stretch reflex due to synaptic retraction of primary muscle spindle afferents and disconnection from parent motoneuron populations (Alvarez et al., 2011; Brandt et al., 2015). However, intermuscular force and length feedback networks may remain partially intact (Lyle et al., 2016). Cats and rats with reinnervated muscles maintain whole-limb function by adjusting inter-joint coordination and muscle co-activation to compensate for loss of reflex-mediated ankle stiffness (Abelew et al., 2000; Maas et al., 2007; Chang et al., 2009; Boeltz et al., 2013). These findings highlight the ability of animals to flexibly exploit musculoskeletal plasticity to maintain function and suggest self-reinnervation as a promising tool to investigate sensorimotor control mechanisms.

Figure 1. Reinnervation protocol.

Figure 1.

Procedure for bilateral self-reinnervation of the lateral gastrocnemius (LG), followed by transducer implantation for in vivo recordings of muscle force (tendon buckle), fascicle length (sonomicrometry crystals) and electromyographic activity (EMG).

Studying neuromuscular control in the guinea fowl, a bipedal animal model, provides insight into similarities and differences among vertebrates that may relate to locomotor modality, evolutionary history, or both. Birds share features of sensorimotor structure and function with mammals, including muscle tissue properties (Nelson et al., 2004; Poore et al., 1997) and muscle proprioception through muscle spindle and Golgi tendon organs (Dorward, 1970; Haiden and Awad, 1981; Maier, 1992). Ground birds use bipedal walking and running gaits with mechanics and energetics similar to human locomotion (Heglund et al., 1982; Taylor et al., 1982; Gatesy and Biewener, 1991; Roberts et al., 1997; Daley and Birn-Jeffery, 2018). Bipedal gaits involve substantial periods of single-limb contact, which limits the redundancy of balance mechanisms and poses a challenge for stability (Daley et al., 2009; Clark and Higham, 2011; Daley and Biewener, 2011). Whereas a quadrupedal cat or rat might be able to compensate for deficits by shifting weight bearing among legs, a biped with a bilateral proprioceptive deficit cannot. Accordingly, one goal of the current study is to explore whether or not guinea fowl exhibit a similar response to proprioceptive deficit as observed in quadrupedal vertebrates (Abelew et al., 2000; Maas et al., 2007; Chang et al., 2009; Boeltz et al., 2013).

We hypothesize that autogenic proprioceptive deficit will lead to increased reliance on feedforward control mechanisms and intrinsic muscle mechanics to maintain stable locomotion. To test for shifts in stability and control mechanisms, we measured ankle kinematics and in vivo LG muscle dynamics (length, force and activation) during treadmill running on level and obstacle terrain. There are several potential mechanisms to compensate for autogenic proprioceptive deficit: 1) Birds might compensate for proprioceptive deficit by increasing feedforward muscle activation before obstacle contact, as observed in birds negotiating high-contrast obstacles (Gordon et al., 2015). 2) Alternatively, if feedback regulation of LG is essential for stability in fast locomotion, loss of autogenic proprioception may necessitate increased reliance on heterogenic reflex pathways from synergists, with a slight delay compared to intact animals, as suggested by work in cats and rats (Boeltz et al., 2013; Lyle et al., 2016). 3) Finally, if intrinsic mechanics are mainly responsible for the modulation of muscle force and work, we might expect minimal change in muscle activity patterns (EMG), as observed in birds subjected to unexpected drop perturbations (Daley et al., 2009). We expect birds to compensate for proprioceptive deficit by tuning gait and feedforward muscle activity to maintain a stable response to obstacle perturbations, as observed in reinnervated rats and cats walking on slopes (Abelew et al., 2000; Maas et al., 2007; Chang et al., 2009; Boeltz et al., 2013). If stability is impaired following reinnervation, this should be evident from increased variance and longer time to recover from obstacles. By investigating the shifts in guinea fowl LG muscle force, length and activation dynamics following reinnervation, we hope to gain insight into the mechanisms of sensorimotor integration and plasticity that enable robustly stable and agile bipedal locomotion.

Results

Mechanical function of intact versus reinnervated LG

We find that many features of the steady-state in vivo mechanical function of the guinea fowl reinnervated lateral gastrocnemius (rLG) are similar to that previously measured in the intact LG (iLG) (Table 1). In Figure 2, average trajectories (mean±95% confidence interval) are shown for muscle strain, force and electromyographic activity (iLG at top, rLG below), with the average for steady level running in grey. During the swing phase of the stride cycle, both iLG and rLG exhibit a period of passive stretch, followed by rapid shortening. Activation and force development begin in late swing around the time of the transition from stretch to shortening, initiating rapid active shortening until foot contact (Figure 2, triangles). At the time of foot contact, force increases rapidly to a peak before midstance, then declines more slowly. Typically, in level running both iLG and rLG show a near-isometric phase in early stance, followed by shortening in late stance, which produces net positive work, as indicated by counter-clockwise force-length work loops (Figure 3). The average magnitude of work output (Wnet) during steady level running is similar between the iLG and rLG, with similar spread of the distribution around the mean (Figure 4). However, rLG shows faster shortening velocity at peak force (VpkF) compared to the iLG across both level and obstacle terrains (Figure 4), indicating a difference in steady state contraction dynamics.

Table 1. Statistical results.

F-statistics for linear mixed effect model ANOVA with fixed effects of treatment cohort (treatment: intact, reinnervated) and stride category (stride ID) and the interaction treatment x stride ID on measures of muscle contraction mechanics and activation. Bolding indicates statistical significance using FDR corrected threshold (p<=0.0263, see Methods). Degrees of freedom for fixed effects were treatment = 1, stride ID = 4, interaction = 4, and error = 2529. See Table 1—source data 1 for p-values.

Table 1—source data 1. P-values linear mixed effect model ANOVA with fixed effects of treatment cohort (treatment: intact, reinnervated) and stride category (stride ID) and the interaction treatment x stride ID.
F-statistic
Variable treatment stride ID: interaction
Wnet 2.44 172.04 7.44
Fpk 0.52 30.12 61.73
LpkF 1.52 238.03 39.52
VpkF 11.43 28.09 8.73
Tforce 0.27 99.09 18.32
Tstride 0.10 12.17 17.82
Etot 0.01 49.05 3.51
Efreq 3.93 8.71 1.85
Ephase 5.72 2.34 7.64
Edur 10.02 8.86 10.69

Figure 2. Muscle trajectories during obstacle negotiation for intact and reinnervated lateral gastrocnemius (iLG: blue, top, rLG: orange, bottom).

Stride cycle averages are shown, from mid-swing to mid-swing (mean ± 95% ci) for a 4-stride sequence in obstacle terrain, with steady level terrain means as a reference, in grey. The shaded box is an obstacle encounter (S 0). Obstacle terrain strides are coded as in Daley and Biewener, 2011, for strides preceding (S −1), on (S 0) and following obstacle contact (S +1), with S +2 including all other strides between obstacles. Trajectories are fractional muscle fascicle length (top), muscle-tendon force (middle) and rectified myoelectric activity (EMG). Triangles indicate the timing of foot-ground contact (grey: level terrain, black: obstacle terrain). Example data is shown from one individual in each treatment cohort. See Figure 2—figure supplement 1 for details on stride-cycle cutting and categorization in an example stride sequence in obstacle terrain.

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Example 6-stride sequence of in vivo muscle recordings of the reinnervated lateral gastrocnemius (rLG) in the right leg, running at 1.7 ms−1 on the obstacle treadmill.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

Muscle length (top, orange), force (bottom, black) and activation (rectified EMG, bottom, orange) are shown, with triangles indicating the time of foot-ground contact, a shaded box indicating an obstacle encounter stride (S 0), and vertical lines indicating the mid-swing cut points between stride cycles. Stride categories were identified from video. Grey silhouettes at the top illustrate the leg posture at the time of foot contact. Strides cycles were cut based on a minimum in muscle-tendon force after it was low-pass filtered with a 6th order Butterworth filter with a cutoff frequency of 3.4 Hz. This resulted in a sinusoidal trajectory with a mid-swing minimum, which was confirmed against video to correspond to when the swing leg crossed vertical. Note that between the last two strides, the contralateral leg stepped on the obstacle, leading to a downward step of the instrumented leg in the final stride. For simplicity, these strides are group with the ‘mid-flat’ strides S+2 (as in Daley and Biewener, 2011) because the focus of the current analysis is the direct response to the obstacle encounter (S 0).

Figure 3. Force-length work loops for iLG (top) and rLG (bottom), for a single individual from each treatment cohort (intact/reinnervated, mean ±95% ci).

Figure 3.

Level mean in grey and obstacle strides in colored lines (iLG: blue, rLG: orange). Stride categories as in Figure 2, where the shaded box is an obstacle encounter (S 0). Triangles indicate the timing of foot-ground contact and arrows indicate the direction of the work loop, with a counter-clockwise loop corresponding to net positive muscle work.

Figure 4. LG muscle mechanical output during obstacle negotiation.

Figure 4.

(A) Distributions of muscle total work output (Wnet), peak force (Fpk), velocity and length at peak force (VpkF, LpkF) across stride categories for iLG (blue) and rLG (orange). Circles indicate group means. Lines connect means between stride categories, to highlight the shifts in relation to obstacle encounters (S 0). (B) Pairwise mean differences (mean ±95% ci) for fixed effect categories, between intact and reinnervated treatment cohorts (grey bar), and between obstacle stride categories compared to level means, within treatment cohorts (colored bars). See Tables 1 and 2 for full statistics results and summary data.

Force-length dynamics and work output during obstacle negotiation

In obstacle encounters (Figure 2, S 0), foot contact with the obstacle occurs earlier in the stride cycle compared to level terrain, altering force-length dynamics during the obstacle stance period. During obstacle contact, both iLG and rLG remain at longer lengths, force increases rapidly to reach a higher peak force, and the muscle shortens throughout force development, producing positive work (Figure 2, S 0). Both iLG and rLG exhibit increased force and work output in obstacle strides compared to level strides (Figure 3, S 0). The magnitude of the shift in work output (Wnet) in obstacle strides (S 0) is similar between intact and reinnervated cohorts, increasing by 3.60±0.57 Jkg−1 in iLG and 3.88±0.60 Jkg−1 in rLG (mean±95% ci, Figure 4BTable 2 ).

Table 2. Pairwise posthoc comparisons. .

Pairwise mean differences (mean ± 95% ci) between intact and reinnervated treatment cohorts (left), and between obstacle stride categories compared to level stride means, within treatment cohorts (intact/reinnervated). Bolding indicates statistical significance using FDR corrected threshold (p <= 0.0263). See Table 2—source data 1 for p-values.

Table 2—source data 1. P-values for posthoc pairwise mean differences between intact and reinnervated treatment cohorts (left column) and between obstacle stride categories compared to the level terrain means, within treatment cohorts (intact/reinnervated).
Variable Treatment cohort Intact Reinnervated
S −1 S 0 Str +1 S +2 S −1 S 0 Str +1 S +2
Wnet −0.47 ± 0.59 −0.91 ± 0.60 3.60 ± 0.57 0.00 ± 0.59 −0.86 ± 0.34 0.34 ± 0.55 3.88 ± 0.60 0.49 ± 0.70 −0.19 ± 0.46
Fpk −0.02 ± 0.06 −0.04 ± 0.07 0.17 ± 0.06 0.04 ± 0.07 −0.04 ± 0.04 0.09 ± 0.06 0.62 ± 0.07 0.10 ± 0.08 0.05 ± 0.05
LpkF −0.02 ± 0.04 −0.02 ± 0.01 0.10 ± 0.01 −0.01 ± 0.01 −0.02 ± 0.01 −0.05 ± 0.01 0.14 ± 0.01 −0.06 ± 0.02 −0.04 ± 0.01
VpkF −1.89 ± 1.10 0.07 ± 0.45 −1.26 ± 0.43 0.00 ± 0.45 0.11 ± 0.26 0.60 ± 0.42 −0.39 ± 0.46 0.63 ± 0.53 0.81 ± 0.35
Tforce 0.02 ± 0.07 −0.01 ± 0.02 0.11 ± 0.02 −0.01 ± 0.02 −0.01 ± 0.01 −0.07 ± 0.02 0.07 ± 0.02 −0.06 ± 0.02 −0.06 ± 0.02
Tstride 0.00 ± 0.02 −0.01 ± 0.02 0.03 ± 0.02 −0.02 ± 0.02 0.01 ± 0.01 −0.07 ± 0.02 0.04 ± 0.02 −0.04 ± 0.03 −0.03 ± 0.02
Etot 0.01 ± 0.22 0.12 ± 0.18 0.76 ± 0.18 0.11 ± 0.18 0.12 ± 0.11 0.16 ± 0.17 0.74 ± 0.19 0.39 ± 0.22 0.09 ± 0.14
Efreq 55.70 ± 55.12 3.05 ± 9.44 −15.05 ± 9.04 −1.46 ± 9.34 −0.46 ± 5.43 −2.86 ± 8.75 −22.06 ± 9.52 −6.81 ± 11.04 −7.68 ± 7.28
Ephase −0.06 ± 0.05 −0.01 ± 0.02 0.00 ± 0.02 0.00 ± 0.02 −0.01 ± 0.01 0.01 ± 0.02 0.04 ± 0.02 −0.01 ± 0.02 0.00 ± 0.01
Edur 0.08 ± 0.05 0.00 ± 0.03 0.04 ± 0.02 −0.01 ± 0.02 0.00 ± 0.01 −0.04 ± 0.02 −0.01 ± 0.03 0.00 ± 0.03 −0.03 ± 0.02

Although the magnitude of the shifts in work output are similar between iLG and rLG, the mechanisms underlying the shift in work output differ between them (Figure 4). In iLG, increased work upon obstacle contact occurs through modest increases in both peak force (Fpk) and shortening velocity (VpkF), compared to level strides. In contrast, rLG exhibits a substantially larger increase in Fpk on obstacle strides and maintains similar VpkF between level and obstacle terrain strides (S 0, Figure 4B, Table 2). Reinnervated LG also exhibits small but significant increases in Fpk in the strides preceding and following obstacle contact (S-1 and S+1, respectively), compared to level terrain.

Shifts in activation patterns between intact and reinnervated LG

Despite deficits in LG monosynaptic reflex following reinnervation, rLG and iLG show similar increases in total muscle activation intensity (Etot, integral of EMG) in obstacle strides compared to steady level strides (S 0, Figure 5). Intact LG exhibits a 4% increase in duration in obstacle strides (S 0) compared to level; however, there is no significant increase in EMG duration for rLG in S 0 (Figure 5B, Table 2). This suggest that the observed increase in Etot in rLG obstacle strides occurs through increased activation amplitude, not increased duration.

Figure 5. LG muscle activation during obstacle negotiation.

(A) Distributions of total intensity EMG activity (Etot), duration of activity (Edur) and mean frequency of activity (Efreq) across stride categories for iLG (blue) and rLG (orange). Circles indicate group means. Lines connect means between stride categories, to highlight the shifts in relation to obstacle encounters (S 0). (B) Pairwise mean differences (mean ±95% ci) for fixed effects, as presented as in Figure 4. See Tables 1 and 2 for full statistics results and summary data.

Figure 5.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1. LG frequency distribution.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1.

Distribution of EMG activation frequency (mean ±95% ci) for intact LG (top, blue) and reinnervated LG (bottom, orange), during level running (dark grey lines) and obstacle encounters (S 0, colored lines). Note the shift in peak frequency of EMG activity in the reinnervated LG for both level terrain and obstacle strides, suggesting recruitment of faster motor units.

Several results suggest a shift in central drive and feedforward activation pattern in rLG compared to iLG. Reinnervated LG exhibits longer steady-state duration of activity (Edur) compared to iLG across all level and obstacle terrain strides, averaging 37% of the stride cycle in rLG compared to 29% in iLG (Figure 5B, Table 2). Additionally, rLG exhibits higher average frequency of EMG activity across all strides compared to the intact cohort (Figure 5, Table 2, Figure 5—figure supplement 1). Finally, the steady-state timing of rLG activation is phase-shifted to 6% (23ms) earlier in the stride cycle relative to the length trajectory, quantified by the variable ‘Ephase’ (Figure 6, Tables 12). Earlier activation onset may help explain the higher rate of shortening in rLG compared to iLG, reported above.

Figure 6. Phase relationship (Ephase) between length and EMG activation.

Figure 6.

(A) Average steady-state length and activation trajectories for iLG and rLG in level terrain, aligned in time based on peak length during the swing phase, before foot-substrate contact. Black dot and vertical dashed line indicate the time of peak fascicle length. Triangles indicate timing of foot contact. (B) Pairwise mean differences in Ephase (mean ±95% ci) between intact and reinnervated treatment cohorts (grey), and obstacle stride categories compared to level terrain within each cohort (colored bars). Ephase is reported in the ANOVA tables as a fraction of the stride cycle but is reported in milliseconds here.

Timing of obstacle-induced changes in EMG activity in iLG and rLG

To explore the timing of obstacle-induced shifts in EMG activity relative to perturbations in force and length, we calculated a difference trajectory between the steady-state level and obstacle perturbed stride cycles (S 0 – Lev) for each individual, and then calculated the mean and 95% confidence interval across individuals (Figure 7). Increased EMG activity begins ~30-40 ms before obstacle-induced increases in length and force, for both iLG and rLG, suggesting an anticipatory (feedforward) contribution to increases in Etot (Figure 7, arrows indicating ‘anticipatory increase’). In iLG, the anticipatory increase in EMG starts ~25% of stride period (asterisk in EMG trace and vertical dashed line in Figure 7A). Starting around 58% of stride period, there is another distinct burst of increased activity, suggesting reflex-mediated contribution to increased EMG in obstacle strides (Figure 7, arrow indicating ‘reflex’). In rLG, the ‘anticipatory increase’ in EMG starts around 21% of stride (asterisk and vertical dashed line in 7B); however activity in the latter half of stance is highly variable and idiosyncratic among individuals (Figure 7—figure supplement 1), as indicated by the wide 95% confidence intervals spanning the region of time where the iLG shows a distinct reflex response (compare Figure 7B versus 7A lower panels). Cross-correlation between the obstacle perturbation trajectories for iLG reveals a correlation of 0.82 between length and EMG deviations, and a correlation of 0.85 between force and EMG deviations. For rLG, the cross-correlations are reduced to 0.53 between length and EMG deviations, and 0.57 between force and EMG deviations, respectively. The reduced correlations suggest a disrupted reflex-mediated response to muscle load and strain in the latter half of stance in rLG, but which is present in iLG (Figure 7). Considering that the changes in muscle length and force are strongly correlated with each other during obstacle encounters in both intact and reinnervated conditions (Figure 7), it is difficult to distinguish the specific sensory signal eliciting reflex responses.

Figure 7. Deviations from steady state in the stride cycle trajectories of muscle length, force and activation, between obstacle strides (S 0) and level strides (grand mean ±95% ci across individuals).

The horizontal zero line indicates no difference from steady state in S 0. The stride cycle is from mid-swing to mid-swing, as in Figure 2. A black asterisk (*) indicates the first timepoint in each trajectory that differs significantly from the level mean. The dashed vertical line and arrow indicating ‘anticipatory increase’ highlights a significant increase in EMG that starts before deviations length and force in S 0. In (A) (iLG), solid vertical lines and yellow fill indicates a 2nd period of significantly increased EMG in late stance that correlates with increased fascicle length and force, suggesting a reflex response. In (B) (rLG), the anticipatory increase in EMG is present; however, wide confidence intervals for EMG in late stance indicates inconsistent patterns of activity across individuals, despite similar increases in length and force as iLG. This suggests disrupted autogenic feedback and idiosyncratic heterogenic feedback patterns across individuals (Figure 7—figure supplement 1).

Figure 7.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1. Muscle trajectories during obstacle negotiation for all individuals in the reinnervated cohort.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1.

Average stride cycle trajectories (mean ±95% ci) for fascicle length, muscle-tendon force and myoelectric activity (EMG) during obstacle encounter strides (S 0, orange) compared to the level terrain mean (grey), for all birds with reinnervated LG. Although the deviations in length and force during obstacle encounters are similar across individuals, the shifts in EMG activity in the latter half of stance vary substantially across individuals, with some individuals showing reflex inhibition (Ind 13, Ind 25) and others showing reflex excitation.

Stability and kinematic changes during obstacle negotiation in intact vs reinnervated birds

Compared to the intact cohort, birds with rLG exhibit more pronounced shifts in gait dynamics in obstacle terrain relative to level terrain. Obstacle-induced increases in peak force (Fpk) are larger for rLG compared to iLG (S 0 Figure 4, Table 2), reflecting larger deviations from steady state in response to the same obstacle. Additionally, rLG shows small but significant increases in peak force (Fpk) in the strides preceding and following obstacle contact (S -1, S +1) compared to steady level strides (Figure 4B, Table 2). Multiple significant differences from level strides occur for rLG in S +1, including a 39±22% increase in Etot, a 6±2% decrease in force duration and a 4±3% decrease in stride duration (mean±95% ci, Table 2). In contrast, most variables for iLG have recovered to steady state in S +1 (Table 2). Both iLG and rLG rapidly increase work output during obstacle encounters and face increased activation costs for locomotion in obstacle terrain. However, rLG shows larger deviations from steady state, and a slower recovery to steady state mechanics and activation level compared to iLG, indicating reduced stability.

Reinnervated birds also show differences in running kinematics in obstacle terrain compared to the intact cohort, undergoing a more pronounced increase in ankle flexion in obstacle encounters (Figure 8). Reinnervated birds also use a shorter stride duration immediately preceding the obstacle encounter (S -1), suggesting anticipatory preparation that is not observed in intact birds (Figure 8). These observations suggest reduced ankle stiffness and increased anticipatory preparation for obstacle encounters in the reinnervated birds.

Figure 8. Ankle kinematics in guinea fowl with intact and reinnervated lateral gastrocnemius (LG).

Figure 8.

(A) Example ankle joint angle trajectories for a bird with intact LG (blue, top) and a bird with reinnervated LG (orange, below), running in obstacle terrain (solid lines) with level terrain (grey dashed lines). (B) Pairwise mean differences (mean ±95% ci) between intact and reinnervated treatment cohorts (grey), and obstacle stride categories compared to level terrain within each treatment cohort (intact: blue, reinnervated: orange). In obstacle strides (S 0, shaded box), the ankle is more flexed at foot contact in reinnervated compared to intact birds. Reinnervated birds show a shorter stride period in S −1, preceding the obstacle encounter, suggesting increased anticipatory preparation. (See Figure 8—source data 1 for statistical results on ankle angle at the time of foot contact).

Figure 8—source data 1. ANOVA results for ankle angle at time of foot contact.
F-statistics, p-values and posthoc pairwise comparisons for linear mixed effect model ANOVA with fixed effects of treatment cohort (treatment: intact, reinnervated) and stride category (stride ID) and the interaction treatment x stride ID. Posthoc pairwise mean differences (mean ±95% ci) are shown between intact and reinnervated treatment cohorts (left), and between obstacle stride categories compared to level stride means, within treatment cohorts (intact/reinnervated).

Discussion

What is the role of proprioception in the control of high-speed locomotion?

We investigated the role of reflexes in the sensorimotor control of running by examining the effects of proprioceptive deficit on the mechanical function of the lateral gastrocnemius muscle (LG) of guinea fowl. Long sensorimotor delays relative to limb cycling times necessitate that animals use a combination of feedforward, feedback and intrinsic mechanical control mechanisms to achieve stable locomotion at high speeds (Brown and Loeb, 2000; Jindrich and Full, 2002Birn-Jeffery et al., 2014; Daley and Biewener, 2011; Daley et al., 2009; Frigon and Rossignol, 2006; Grillner, 2011; Lam and Pearson, 2002; More and Donelan, 2018; Pearson and Gramlich, 2010; Prochazka and Ellaway, 2012). We hypothesized that an autogenic proprioceptive deficit will lead to increased reliance on feedforward tuning of muscle activity to achieve stable muscle dynamics in obstacle terrain. In birds with intact LG proprioception, the timing of muscle activity in obstacle-perturbed strides is consistent with combined feedforward and feedback control (Daley and Biewener, 2011; Gordon et al., 2015). Birds with reinnervated LG (rLG) exhibit a consistent phase shift in EMG onset relative to muscle length, with activation starting 6% earlier (23 ms) in the steady state contraction cycle, in both level and obstacle terrain (Figure 6, Table 2). This is consistent with a feedforward tuning of rLG activation timing to enable rapid force development and high muscle stiffness during stance, in the absence of monosynaptic reflexes. Regulation of EMG duration in obstacle strides (S 0) is absent in rLG (Figure 5), suggesting that proprioceptive feedback in late stance normally regulates force duration, which is disrupted following reinnervation.

A stable intrinsic mechanical response with neither feedforward- nor feedback-mediated changes in neural drive can occur when a perturbation is encountered at high running speeds (Daley et al., 2009). Rapid changes in muscle length and velocity in response to perturbations can decouple activation and force development (Daley et al., 2009; Daley and Biewener, 2011). In our previous work on intact in vivo muscle dynamics, variation in LG muscle strain during initial foot contact and limb loading explained 60% of the variation in force developed in obstacle encounters, while variation in LG muscle activation explained only 9%. This clearly demonstrates the decoupling between activation and force development that can occur in vivo (Daley and Biewener, 2011). These intrinsic mechanical effects minimize the disturbances in body dynamics that arise from terrain height perturbations, enabling rapid recovery to steady gait. Similar intrinsic mechanical stabilizing responses have been demonstrated in the distal hindlimb joints of hopping and running humans subjected to unexpected changes in terrain height and stiffness (Dick et al., 2019; Ferris et al., 1999; Moritz and Farley, 2004). In concert with the stabilizing contributions of the intrinsic muscle-tendon dynamics, guinea fowl with intact proprioception also use feedforward and feedback regulation of muscle activity to maintain stability in obstacle terrain, with greater feedforward contributions when obstacles are visible and high contrast (Daley and Biewener, 2011; Gordon et al., 2015).

We find that guinea fowl with LG proprioceptive deficit achieve similar increases in total EMG activity during obstacle strides; however, the increases in activity occur early in the stride, before obstacle-induced changes in muscle force and length (Figure 7). This is consistent with anticipatory, feedforward increases in neural drive to the muscle, as observed in birds running over high-contrast visible obstacles (Gordon et al., 2015), and humans hopping on randomized but expected increases in surface stiffness (Moritz and Farley, 2004). These findings are consistent with a hybrid feedforward/feedback control model as conceptualized by Kuo, 2002 in which feedforward and feedback gains are balanced to enable accurate state estimation and robust cyclical dynamics in the presence of both disturbances and sensory error. Although the reinnervated LG contributes to an effective obstacle negotiation response, it requires a longer recovery time and increased muscle activity following obstacle contact (Figure 5B). This suggests that the integrated response of the intact neuromuscular system enables robust stability with lower muscle activation costs.

Several features of the kinematics and muscle dynamics suggests coordinated plasticity and tuning of feedfoward control to compensate for reflex deficit following recovery from nerve injury (Figure 9). We observe similar increases in work output in response to obstacle encounters in rLG and iLG (Figure 4B). This finding is consistent with the idea that muscle work modulation is an important feature of task-level control for stability in uneven terrain (Daley et al., 2009; Daley and Biewener, 2011). However, work modulation is achieved through different underlying mechanisms in rLG and iLG. Earlier steady state activation in rLG (lacking autogenic proprioceptive feedback) enables higher muscle force development in early stance to resist the external load applied at foot contact, which likely contributes to the higher rate of shortening throughout stance. Additionally, rLG exhibits larger increases in peak force in obstacle encounters compared to iLG (Figure 4B). This increase in peak force likely involves both active and passive components: an active contribution from increased feedforward drive and EMG amplitude (Figure 5B), and a passive contribution from increased stretch of connective tissues associated with a more flexed ankle posture at foot contact (Figure 8). Finally, Birds with rLG also show an anticipatory shift in stride duration before obstacle encounters, which is not observed in the intact cohort (Figure 8B) and may help control landing conditions for obstacle encounters (Gordon et al., 2015). These findings suggest that reinnervated birds achieve effective muscle work modulation and stable obstacle negotiation through feedforward tuning of muscle activation and gait to compensate for loss of autogenic proprioception.

Figure 9. Schematic of the neuromechanical control mechanisms regulating function of the reinnervated lateral gastrocnemius (rLG) of the guinea fowl.

Figure 9.

Green text indicates the in vivo experimental measures used to infer sensorimotor control mechanisms. Differences in muscle dynamics between intact and reinnervated cohorts suggest that guinea fowl use a combination of feedforward and intrinsic mechanical mechanisms to compensate for disrupted proprioceptive reflexes, suggesting interconnected plasticity of neural and musculoskeletal mechanisms in the recovery from nerve injury.

A recent study by Sawicki et al., 2015 found that earlier onset of activation was associated with a shift to energy absorption in cyclical muscle contractions with a sinusoidal MTU length trajectory. We find here that earlier onset is associated with greater shortening and work production. The specific response of a muscle to a shift in activation phase is likely to be highly sensitive to the specific steady-state length trajectory of the muscle. Muscle force capacity and the activation and deactivation kinetics are substantially influenced by velocity and recent strain history, as demonstrated in controlled studies of in vitro muscle force-length work loops (Askew and Marsh, 1998; Josephson, 1999). Further work is needed to understand how in vivo muscle fascicle length dynamics interact with neural activation patterns and MTU compliance to enable tuning of muscle contraction dynamics to the mechanical demands of cyclical locomotor tasks.

We do observe shifts in muscle activity in late stance in some reinnervated individuals in response to obstacle encounters, which suggests heterogenic reflex responses in reinnervated LG (Figure 2). However, these responses are variable and idiosyncratic, with some birds showing increased EMG in late stance in obstacle strides, and others showing a decrease (Figure 7—figure supplement 1). The variable and idiosyncratic reflex responses result in wide confidence intervals for the obstacle-induced EMG response in the latter half of stance, despite consistent force-length trajectories over the same time-period (Figure 7). Idiosyncratic use of heterogenic reflex modulation across individuals following nerve injury recovery is consistent with findings in cats (Lyle et al., 2016; Lyle and Nichols, 2018). Guinea fowl have several agonist muscles to the LG that could contribute to heterogenic feedback modulation, including the medial gastrocnemius and digital flexors (Daley and Biewener, 2011; Gordon et al., 2015). However, no muscle is an exact synergist of the LG, because each has a unique combination of moment arms, fiber length, pennation angle and connective tissue compliance (Daley and Biewener, 2003; Cox et al., 2019). Consequently, it is unlikely that proprioception from agonists can completely restore accurate sensing to regulate LG force and work output. Additionally, in the presence of increased sensory error and noise, birds may learn over time to compensate through sensory integration in higher CNS pathways, leading to updated central coordination and feedforward drive to rhythm generating networks.

It was previously unknown whether guinea fowl would respond to reinnervation and proprioceptive deficit in a manner similar to quadrupedal mammals. We find that our results are consistent with the findings on rats and cats. Reinnervated cats and rats exhibit shifts in feedforward muscle activity and inter-joint coordination during slope walking, to compensate for loss of reflex-mediated ankle stiffness (Abelew et al., 2000; Maas et al., 2007; Chang et al., 2009; Boeltz et al., 2013). Cats and rats also preserve task level features of gait, such as leg length and body motions, despite variance in muscle and joint dynamics (Chang et al., 2009; Boeltz et al., 2013). These findings suggest performance of task-level goals as a target of sensorimotor optimization. Also similar to cats, guinea fowl exhibit variation among individuals in heterogenic compensation for loss of the autogenic stretch reflex, as suggested by the variable tendon tap responses and variation in late-stance EMG activity in obstacle perturbed steps (Figure 7; Figure 7—figure supplement 1). Work on cats suggests complex intermuscular feedback connectivity, which can recovery to varying degrees following reinnervation (Cope et al., 1994; Pearson, 2000; Lyle et al., 2016; Lyle and Nichols, 2018). This complexity and variability among individuals following recovery from nerve injury reflects the complexity and plasticity of proprioceptive feedback networks. Nonetheless, the recovery of consistent task-level mechanical function supports the idea that sensorimotor control is optimized to maintain task-level performance goals such as stable body dynamics (Chang et al., 2009; Safavynia and Ting, 2013).

Limitations and future directions

One of the major limitations in the current study is the potential for multiple differences between the intact and reinnervated cohorts that were not controlled, because the experiments on the two cohorts were conducted over different periods of time. The self-reinnervation procedure requires a long-term recovery period and results in a chronic sensory deficit, which is likely to lead to a complex array of changes in the musculoskeletal tissues and the sensorimotor networks. The current study did not include a sham-surgery experimental control, and we did not strictly monitor the ages of the original intact cohort at the time of the in vivo muscle-tendon surgeries. Nonetheless, it is reassuring that our findings are consistent with similar studies in cats and rats, suggesting feedforward tuning of muscle activation and ankle kinematics to maintain stability following loss of proprioception. In future experiments, it will be important to control the timing and amount of exercise training in intact and reinnervated experimental groups, considering the potential for exercise to influence the recovery process (Boeltz et al., 2013; Brandt et al., 2015).

The recovery process almost certainly involves coupled changes across multiple systems, including connective tissue compliance, muscle activation kinetics, fiber type distribution, motor unit size and distribution, spinal intraneuronal connectivity, and sensory integration in higher CNS centers for state estimation and movement planning. Due to the complex nature of these adaptations, it is challenging to fully tease apart individual contributions and mechanisms from in vivo experimental measures alone. In future studies, the coordinated mechanisms of sensorimotor adaptation and plasticity could be systematically explored through a combination of integrative experimental and computational approaches. These approaches could include 1) closed loop neuromechanical simulations to enable predictive hypothesis testing (Ijspeert, 2014; Roth et al., 2014), 2) combined use of in vivo measures of muscle dynamics with in vitro testing of muscle contractile dynamics, to replicate biologically realistic force-length contraction dynamics, 3) histological studies to examine changes in muscle fiber type distribution and connective tissue characteristics following reinnervation, and 4) perturbation approaches that probe both short and long term adaptation processes.

It remains unclear how the specific length trajectory and velocity features of in vivo muscle dynamics contribute to the intrinsic stability and control of movement. Dynamic measurement techniques are needed to address this challenge and to develop realistic models for in vivo muscle-tendon function. In addition to widely recognized force-length and force-velocity ‘Hill-type’ properties, muscle exhibits short and long-term history-dependent changes in force capacity in response to stretch and shortening (Edman, 1975; Edman et al., 1978; Edman, 1980; Josephson, 1999; Herzog, 2004; Edman, 2012; Herzog, 2014; Rode et al., 2009; Nishikawa et al., 2012; Yeo et al., 2013; Nishikawa et al., 2018). Recent developments in biorobotic platforms that enable controlled muscle experiments with realistic loading and length trajectories are promising tools for advancing our understanding of the role of intrinsic muscle dynamics in the control of movement (Clemente and Richards, 2012; Richards, 2011; Robertson and Sawicki, 2015). Integrative neuromechanical studies using multiple techniques will be essential for unravelling mechanisms of muscle function, sensorimotor integration and plasticity. Findings from these studies have important implications for many human health conditions, including acute nerve injury, diabetic neuropathy, neurodegenerative disorders, cerebral palsy, and muscular dystrophies.

Materials and methods

Animals and treadmill training

We obtained and reared six hatchling guinea fowl keets (Numida meleagris) from a breeder (Hidden Hollow Acres, Whitehouse Station, NJ), to allow re-innervation surgeries in juveniles with at least 12 weeks for recovery before in vivo muscle procedures (see below). At the time of the in vivo muscle measurements, the guinea fowl had reached adult size, averaging 1.81±0.28 kg body mass (mean±S.D.). Birds had primary feathers clipped and were trained to run on a level motorized treadmill (Woodway, Waukesha, WI, USA). Training sessions were 15-20 minutes in duration, with breaks for 2 minutes as needed. All experiments were undertaken at the Concord Field Station of Harvard University, in Boston (MA, USA), and all procedures were licensed and approved by the Harvard Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (AEP #20-09) in accordance with the guidelines of the National Institutes of Health and the regulations of the United States Department of Agriculture.

We also include data previously reported in Daley and Biewener, 2011 from intact individuals (n=6, 1.77±0.63 kg body mass) to serve as a control group for statistical comparison to the new dataset (n=6 reinnervated individuals). We re-analyzed the intact cohort dataset alongside the reinnervated cohort, to ensure consistency in data processing and statistics. The intact data includes a larger sample than reported in Daley and Biewener, 2011, because the analysis here includes all strides in the level and obstacle terrain collected for running speeds between 1.3-2.0 ms−1. Trials were recorded only for speeds that each bird could comfortably maintain on the treadmill belt for at least 30 seconds, allowing for 10-minute rest periods between trials with access to food and water. We focus on running speeds (>1.3ms−1), to avoid the confounding effect of different sensorimotor control strategies in walking versus running (Gordon et al., 2015). Due to variation among individuals in the successful trials recorded, the intact cohort dataset includes a wider speed range (1.3-2.0 ms−1) than the reinnervated cohort dataset (1.7-2.0 ms−1). However, the analysis is focused on obstacle perturbations compared to steady gait at the same speed, and the datasets include comparable samples of obstacle encounters between the two cohorts: 128 for intact and 133 for reinnervated birds, respectively. In total, the dataset includes 1027 strides for reinnervated and 1512 strides for intact individuals and excludes 81 strides as outliers that were non-obstacle encounter strides with Z-score > 4. No obstacle encounter strides were excluded as outliers. The complete datasets for reinnervated and intact cohorts are available through DataDryad.org, including metadata and Matlab processing scripts (Daley et al., 2020, https://doi.org/10.7280/D11H49).

Anesthesia and post-operative care

Birds were induced and maintained on a mid-plane of anesthesia using isoflurane (2 - 3%, mask/intubation delivery). We administered perioperative enrofloxacin and flunixin intramuscularly for analgesia after induction and continued for three days after each surgery. Birds recovered to bilateral weight bearing within 20 minutes following completion of surgical procedures.

Reinnervation surgery

The timing of surgeries was planned based on a pilot study, which found full recovery of LG motor activity by 6 weeks following reinnervation surgeries, and continued absence of calcaneal tendon reflex one year later, indicating continued absence of autogenic stretch reflexes (Carr et al., 2010). We bilaterally transected and immediately repaired the peripheral nerve branch supplying the LG muscle in maturing guinea fowl between 7-12 weeks of age. We allowed time for full reinnervation recovery of motor output and growth to adult size before a subsequent surgery to implant muscle transducers (Figure 1). In the reinnervation surgery, a lateral incision was made posterior-distal to the knee to expose the underlying muscle. Blunt dissection enabled exposure and identification of relevant nerve branches, and the identity of the correct nerve branch was confirmed using an isolated nerve stimulator (SD48, Grass Instruments, Warwick, RI) to visualize contraction in the LG. After pre-placement of single longitudinal throw of 6-0 braided non-absorbable silk (Silk, Ethicon, Somerville, NJ, USA) through a 3 mm nerve section, we transected the nerve branch and sutured to appose the cut nerve endings. Fibrin glue (bovine thrombin in CaCl2, fibrinogen, fibronectin from bovine plasma) was applied over the apposed nerve endings as an additional repair scaffold (Carr et al., 2010; Spotnitz, 2010). We closed the fascia and skin with 3-0 braided absorbable polyglactin (Vicryl, Ethicon,Somerville, NJ, USA).

In the immediate post-operative period, bilateral limb posture was visibly more crouched compared to ‘intact’ birds and Achilles tendon tap revealed no stretch reflex response. LG atrophy was qualitatively observed during the first 2 weeks of the recovery period. Within 1 week of surgery, bird activity levels appeared comparable to intact conspecifics, with limb posture partially recovered. From 2-3 weeks onwards, differentiating reinnervated from ‘intact’ birds was not possible from grossly observable limb morphology, posture and gait. We conducted regular treadmill training from 7 weeks after reinnervation surgery. Exercise was started at 7 weeks to ensure synaptic withdrawal of primary afferents before recovery, eliciting a proprioceptive deficit in the self-reinnervated LG. In rats, synaptic withdrawal and resulting proprioceptive deficit is minimized if training is initiated on the 3rd day after nerve injury (Boeltz et al., 2013; Brandt et al., 2015). During training and experiments, birds did not stumble or fall with noticeably greater frequency than observed in intact birds and were able to maintain treadmill position over a similar speed range. At the time of muscle recordings, Achilles tendon tap revealed variable latencies of 49±43ms (mean±S.D., range 10.3-91.5ms). This may reflect variable recovery of intermuscular reflex connectivity, consistent with observations in cats and rats (Boeltz et al., 2013; Brandt et al., 2015; Lyle et al., 2016). In comparison, the Achilles tendon tap reflex latency in intact birds was 6.1±1.2ms (mean±S.D), consistent with the mono-synaptic stretch reflex (Nishikawa et al., 2007; Daley and Biewener, 2011).

Transducer implantation surgery

When the birds were 23-28 weeks old (13-16 weeks following bilateral reinnervation surgeries), we performed a 2nd surgery for transducer placement, following similar procedures as Daley and Biewener, 2003. The surgical field was plucked of feathers and gently cleaned with antiseptic solution (Prepodyne, West Argo, Kansas City, MO, USA). We tunneled transducer leads subcutaneously from a 1–2 cm incision over the synsacrum to a second 4–5 cm incision over the lateral left shank. Sonomicrometry crystals (2.0 mm; Sonometrics Inc, London, Canada) were implanted into the lateral head of the gastrocnemius (LG) along the fascicle axis in the middle 1/3rd of the muscle belly. Crystals were placed in small openings using fine forceps, approximately 3–4 mm deep and 15 mm apart. We verified signal quality using an oscilloscope and secured the crystals by closing the overlying muscle fascia and lead wires with separate 4-0 silk sutures (Silk, Ethicon, Somerville, NJ, USA). Next to the crystal pair, we implanted bipolar EMG electrodes constructed from two strands of 38-gauge Teflon-coated stainless steel (AS 632, Cooner Wire Co., California, USA) with staggered 1 mm exposed regions spaced 1.5 mm apart. Electrodes were placed using sew-through methods and surface silicon anchors (3 x 3 x 2 mm) positioned with a single square knot at the muscle surface-electrode interface (Deban and Carrier, 2002). An “E”-type stainless-steel tendon buckle force transducer insulated with a polyurethane coating (Micro-Measurements, Raleigh NC) was implanted on the common gastrocnemius tendon, equipped with a metal foil strain gauge (type FLA-1, Tokyo Sokki Kenkyujo). We connected transducers to a micro-connector plug (15-way Micro-D, Farnell Ltd, Leeds, UK) sutured to the bird’s dorsal synsacrum.

Transducer recordings

A lightweight shielded cable was used to connect the microconnector to data acquisition systems. Sonomicrometry data were collected via a Sonometrics TRX analog data-acquisition device and PC interface (TRX Series 8, Sonometrics, Ontario, Canada). Crystals were tested before surgery in a saline bath to confirm distances measured by digital caliper matched those measured by the software. Occasional drop-outs and level-shift artifacts in the sonomicrometry length signal (arising from variation in signal-to-noise characteristics) were corrected within the Sonometrics software where possible and smoothed using cubic smoothing spline with a tolerance of 0.1 in MATLAB (‘spaps’ function, Mathworks, Inc; Natick, MA, USA). Tendon buckle signals were fed through a bridge amplifier (Vishay 2120, Micro-Measurements, Raleigh, NC), and EMG signals were amplified and bandpass filtered (10Hz and 3kHz) using GRASS pre-amplifiers (P511, Grass Instruments, Warwick, RI). Signals were recorded at 10kHz using a 16-channel, 16-bit Biopac A/D acquisition device (MP150, BIOPAC systems, Gotleta, CA, USA). Following experiments, birds were euthanized using an intravenous injection of sodium pentobarbital (100 mg kg−1) while under deep isoflurane anesthesia (4%, mask delivery).

Muscle morphology

Postmortem, we recorded the morphology of the muscle and the location of transducers to confirm muscle fascicle and tendon alignment. In the reinnervated cohort (this study) LG mass was 10.7±2.7 g and total gastrocnemius mass was 26.5±5.1 g. In the intact cohort (Daley and Biewener, 2011), LG mass was 10.2±4.3 g, and total gastrocnemius mass was 23.5±7.4 g. These muscle masses are a comparable to those measured from intact guinea fowl in previous studies, representing approximately 0.5% body mass for LG and 1.3% body mass for total gastrocnemius mass (Daley and Biewener, 2003; Higham and Biewener, 2008). This suggests full recovery from denervation-induced muscle atrophy in the reinnervated cohort. Fascicle lengths for were 17 ±1 mm and 18±2 mm and pennation angles were 25±5° and 24±5° for reinnervated and intact LG, respectively. Crystal alignment relative to the fascicle axis (α) was within 2°, indicating that errors due to misalignment were <0.1%. We calibrated the tendon force buckle in situ post mortem by applying a series of known cyclical loads using a force transducer (model 9203, Kistler, Amherst, MA), which yielded linear least-squares calibration slopes with R2 > 0.97.

Level and obstacle terrain conditions

We recorded trials on i) uniform level terrain and ii) terrain with repeating 5 cm obstacles, at the same treadmill speeds, as in Daley and Biewener, 2011. The treadmill belt (Woodway, Waukesha WI) was slatted black rubber-coated steel with running surface 55.8 cm x 172.7 cm with clearance for obstacles beneath. Obstacles were constructed from styrofoam reinforced with cardboard covered with black neoprene to form a light, stiff surface. Waterproof glue (Shoe Goo, Eugene, OR, USA) secured heavy-duty fabric hook and loop fastener (Velcro, Cheshire, UK) to the obstacle and treadmill surface. Four sequential slats of obstacles produced a 20 cm2 continuous obstacle surface. Obstacles were encountered approximately every 4-5 strides, with some variation due to varied stride length and treadmill station keeping. We recorded high-speed video at 250 Hz (Photron, San Diego, CA, USA) for analysis of ankle kinematics, detection of stride timing and statistical coding of strides in relation to obstacle encounters.

Data processing

We assigned strides categories in relation to the obstacle encounters, using the approach in Daley and Biewener, 2011, based on the stride sequence of the instrumented leg: the stride prior to an obstacle contact (S −1), obstacle contact strides (S 0), the stride following obstacle contact (S +1), and strides in flat terrain between obstacles (S +2). All level terrain strides were assigned the same stride category (L). Note, this is simpler stride coding than presented in Gordon et al., 2015, which also considered the timing of obstacle encounters by the contralateral leg. Strides in which the contralateral limb had made contact with the obstacle in the previous step are grouped here into the (S +2) category, for simplicity. This does not substantially alter the findings, because the coding was similar between intact and reinnervated birds, and the current analysis is focused on the shifts in LG force-length dynamics related to a direct obstacle encounter by the instrumented leg.

Features of LG activation, force-length dynamics and work output were measured, similar to Daley and Biewener, 2011. Raw EMG signals were used to calculate myoelectric intensity in time and frequency domains using wavelet decomposition (Daley et al., 2009; Gordon et al., 2015). This was used to calculate total myoelectric intensity per stride (Etot) and mean frequency of muscle activation (Efreq). We calculated fractional fascicle length (L) from sonomicrometry data using mean length in level terrain as a reference length (Lo). Note, however, that Lo is not directly related to sarcomere length or optimal length for the isometric force-length curve, which were not measured. Fractional fascicle length was differentiated to obtain fascicle velocity (V, in lengths per second, Ls−1). Shortening strains are negative. We multiplied fascicle velocity (in ms−1) by tendon force (in Newtons, N) to calculate muscle power (Watts), which was integrated through time to calculate total work per stride (Joules, J; with shortening work being positive), and then normalized by muscle mass to obtain mass-specific muscle work (Jkg−1). We also recorded muscle (fascicle) length, velocity and force at specified times to evaluate how strain and activation factors influence muscle force and work output. All data processing was completed using MATLAB (Mathworks, Inc; Natick, MA, USA).

Statistics

The statistical analysis approach is similar to that used in Gordon et al., 2015 to investigate obstacle perturbation responses relative to steady state level terrain strides. We used a linear mixed-effects model ANOVA to test for significant effects of treatment (intact/reinnervated cohorts) and stride category (stride ID: Level, S -1, S 0, S +1, S +2) as fixed categorical factors, with individual (ind) included as a random effect. Statistical analysis was completed in Matlab using ‘fitlme’ and associated functions in the Statistics and Machine Learning Toolbox (Mathworks, Inc; Natick, MA, USA). Several linear mixed effects models were evaluated:

  1. 'Y ~ 1 + (1|ind)'

  2. 'Y ~ 1 + treatment + (1|ind)'

  3. 'Y ~ 1 + stride_ID + (1|ind)'

  4. 'Y ~ 1 + stride_ID + treatment + (1|ind)'

  5. 'Y ~ 1 + stride_ID *treatment + (1|ind)'

Model 5 (with the interaction term between fixed effects) was used as the final model, because it had the lowest AIC for 13 of 14 of the variables analyzed (AIC, Akaike, 1976). Model 4 had the lowest AIC for mean EMG frequency (Efreq), but the difference between Models 4 and 5 was not significant according to a likelihood ratio test. Therefore, for consistency, Model 5 was used for all variables. Posthoc pairwise comparisons were calculated for the mean difference ± 95% confidence interval between intact and reinnervated treatment cohorts and for the mean differences between level and obstacle strides categories within each treatment cohort. Pairwise comparisons were calculated after removing the random effect of individual, because the intact and reinnervated datasets came from different cohorts of individuals. We used False Discovery Rate to calculate an adjusted p-value threshold to maintain a 5% false positive rate across all statistical tests, including fixed effects tests and post-hoc pairwise comparisons (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995).

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by NIH grant NIAMS 5R01AR055648 to AAB, grant BB/H005838/1 to MAD from the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council (BBSRC), and a doctoral training studentship from the BBSRC to JCG supervised by MAD. Thanks to Jennifer A Carr for assistance in the experiments. Thanks to Reviewers N Cowan and L Ting and Reviewing Editor K VijayRaghavan for thoughtful and constructive feedback.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Monica A Daley, Email: madaley@uci.edu.

K VijayRaghavan, National Centre for Biological Sciences, Tata Institute of Fundamental Research, India.

K VijayRaghavan, National Centre for Biological Sciences, Tata Institute of Fundamental Research, India.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • National Institutes of Health NIAMS 5R01AR055648 to Andrew Biewener.

  • Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council BB/H005838/1 to Monica A Daley.

  • Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council Doctoral training studentship to Joanne C Gordon, Monica A Daley.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing - original draft.

Investigation, Writing - review and editing.

Conceptualization, Resources, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Writing - review and editing.

Conceptualization, Resources, Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Visualization, Methodology, Writing - original draft, Writing - review and editing.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: All experiments were undertaken at the Concord Field Station of Harvard University, in Boston (MA, USA), and all procedures were licensed and approved by the Harvard Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (AEP #20-09) in accordance with the guidelines of the National Institutes of Health and the regulations of the United States Department of Agriculture. Surgery was performed under isoflurane anesthesia, and every effort was made to minimize suffering.

Additional files

Transparent reporting form

Data availability

The full dataset including raw data, metadata files and processing code have been deposited to Dryad (https://doi.org/10.7280/D11H49).

The following dataset was generated:

Daley MA, Gordon JC, Holt NC, Biewener A. 2020. Dataset for 'Tuning of feedforward control enables stable muscle force-length dynamics after loss of autogenic proprioceptive feedback'. Dryad Digital Repository.

References

  1. Abelew TA, Miller MD, Cope TC, Nichols TR. Local loss of proprioception results in disruption of interjoint coordination during locomotion in the cat. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2000;84:2709–2714. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.84.5.2709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Alvarez FJ, Titus-Mitchell HE, Bullinger KL, Kraszpulski M, Nardelli P, Cope TC. Permanent central synaptic disconnection of proprioceptors after nerve injury and regeneration. I. loss of VGLUT1/IA synapses on motoneurons. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2011;106:2450–2470. doi: 10.1152/jn.01095.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Askew GN, Marsh RL. Optimal shortening velocity (V/Vmax) of skeletal muscle during cyclical contractions: length-force effects and velocity-dependent activation and deactivation. The Journal of Experimental Biology. 1998;201:1527–1540. doi: 10.1242/jeb.201.10.1527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Azizi E, Brainerd EL, Roberts TJ. Variable gearing in pennate muscles. PNAS. 2008;105:1745–1750. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0709212105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B. 1995;57:289–300. doi: 10.2307/2346101. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  6. Birn-Jeffery AV, Hubicki CM, Blum Y, Renjewski D, Hurst JW, Daley MA. Don't break a leg: running birds from quail to ostrich prioritise leg safety and economy on uneven terrain. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2014;217:3786–3796. doi: 10.1242/jeb.102640. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Boeltz T, Ireland M, Mathis K, Nicolini J, Poplavski K, Rose SJ, Wilson E, English AW. Effects of treadmill training on functional recovery following peripheral nerve injury in rats. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2013;109:2645–2657. doi: 10.1152/jn.00946.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Brandt J, Evans JT, Mildenhall T, Mulligan A, Konieczny A, Rose SJ, English AW. Delaying the onset of treadmill exercise following peripheral nerve injury has different effects on axon regeneration and motoneuron synaptic plasticity. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2015;113:2390–2399. doi: 10.1152/jn.00892.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Brown IE, Loeb GE. A reductionist approach to creating and using neuromusculoskeletal models. In: Winters J. M, Crago P. E, editors. Biomechanics and Neural Control of Posture and Movement. New York: Springer; 2000. pp. 148–163. [Google Scholar]
  10. Bullinger KL, Nardelli P, Pinter MJ, Alvarez FJ, Cope TC. Permanent central synaptic disconnection of proprioceptors after nerve injury and regeneration. II. loss of functional connectivity with motoneurons. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2011;106:2471–2485. doi: 10.1152/jn.01097.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Carr JA, Chao L, Biewener AA. The effects of denervation and self-reinnervation in the guinea fowl lateral gastrocnemius . American Society of Biomechanics 34th Annual Meeting; Providence, RI. 2010. [Google Scholar]
  12. Chang YH, Auyang AG, Scholz JP, Nichols TR. Whole limb kinematics are preferentially conserved over individual joint kinematics after peripheral nerve injury. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2009;212:3511–3521. doi: 10.1242/jeb.033886. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Clark AJ, Higham TE. Slipping, sliding and stability: locomotor strategies for overcoming low-friction surfaces. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2011;214:1369–1378. doi: 10.1242/jeb.051136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Clemente CJ, Richards C. Determining the influence of muscle operating length on muscle performance during frog swimming using a bio-robotic model. Bioinspiration & Biomimetics. 2012;7:036018. doi: 10.1088/1748-3182/7/3/036018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Cohen AH. The role of heterarchical control in the evolution of central pattern generators. Brain, Behavior and Evolution. 1992;40:112–124. doi: 10.1159/000113907. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Cope TC, Bonasera SJ, Nichols TR. Reinnervated muscles fail to produce stretch reflexes. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1994;71:817–820. doi: 10.1152/jn.1994.71.2.817. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Cox SM, Easton KL, Lear MC, Marsh RL, Delp SL, Rubenson J. The interaction of compliance and activation on the Force-Length operating range and force generating capacity of skeletal muscle: a computational study using a guinea fowl musculoskeletal model. Integrative Organismal Biology. 2019;1:obz022. doi: 10.1093/iob/obz022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Daley MA, Voloshina A, Biewener AA. The role of intrinsic muscle mechanics in the neuromuscular control of stable running in the guinea fowl. The Journal of Physiology. 2009;587:2693–2707. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2009.171017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Daley MA, Gordon JC, Biewener AA. Tuning of feedforward control enables stable muscle force-length dynamics after loss of autogenic proprioceptive feedback. bioRxiv. 2020 doi: 10.1101/851626. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  20. Daley MA, Biewener AA. Muscle force-length dynamics during level versus incline locomotion: a comparison of in vivo performance of two guinea fowl ankle extensors. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2003;206:2941–2958. doi: 10.1242/jeb.00503. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Daley MA, Biewener AA. Leg muscles that mediate stability: mechanics and control of two distal extensor muscles during obstacle negotiation in the guinea fowl. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2011;366:1580–1591. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0338. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Daley MA, Birn-Jeffery A. Scaling of avian bipedal locomotion reveals independent effects of body mass and leg posture on gait. The Journal of Experimental Biology. 2018;221:jeb152538. doi: 10.1242/jeb.152538. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Deban SM, Carrier DR. Hypaxial muscle activity during running and breathing in dogs. The Journal of Experimental Biology. 2002;205:1953–1967. doi: 10.1242/jeb.205.13.1953. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Dick TJM, Punith LK, Sawicki GS. Humans falling in holes: adaptations in lower-limb joint mechanics in response to a rapid change in substrate height during human hopping. Journal of the Royal Society Interface. 2019;16:20190292. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2019.0292. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Donelan JM, Pearson KG. Contribution of force feedback to ankle extensor activity in decerebrate walking cats. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2004;92:2093–2104. doi: 10.1152/jn.00325.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Dorward PK. Response characteristics of muscle afferents in the domestic duck. The Journal of Physiology. 1970;211:1–17. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1970.sp009262. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Drew T, Marigold DS. Taking the next step: cortical contributions to the control of locomotion. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2015;33:25–33. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2015.01.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Edman KA. Mechanical deactivation induced by active shortening in isolated muscle fibres of the frog. The Journal of Physiology. 1975;246:255–275. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1975.sp010889. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Edman KA, Elzinga G, Noble MI. Enhancement of mechanical performance by stretch during tetanic contractions of vertebrate skeletal muscle fibres. The Journal of Physiology. 1978;281:139–155. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1978.sp012413. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Edman KA. Depression of mechanical performance by active shortening during twitch and tetanus of vertebrate muscle fibres. Acta Physiologica Scandinavica. 1980;109:15–26. doi: 10.1111/j.1748-1716.1980.tb06559.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Edman KA. Residual force enhancement after stretch in striated muscle. A consequence of increased myofilament overlap? The Journal of Physiology. 2012;590:1339–1345. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2011.222729. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Ferris DP, Liang K, Farley CT. Runners adjust leg stiffness for their first step on a new running surface. Journal of Biomechanics. 1999;32:787–794. doi: 10.1016/S0021-9290(99)00078-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Frigon A, Rossignol S. Experiments and models of sensorimotor interactions during locomotion. Biological Cybernetics. 2006;95:607–627. doi: 10.1007/s00422-006-0129-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Gatesy SM, Biewener AA. Bipedal locomotion: effects of speed, size and limb posture in birds and humans. Journal of Zoology. 1991;224:127–147. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7998.1991.tb04794.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  35. Gordon JC, Rankin JW, Daley MA. How do treadmill speed and terrain visibility influence neuromuscular control of guinea fowl locomotion? Journal of Experimental Biology. 2015;218:3010–3022. doi: 10.1242/jeb.104646. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Gordon T, Stein RB. Time course and extent of recovery in reinnervated motor units of cat triceps surae muscles. The Journal of Physiology. 1982;323:307–323. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1982.sp014074. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Grillner S. Handbook of Physiology the Nervous System, Motor Control. Wiley; 2011. Control of locomotion in bipeds, tetrapods, and fish. Comprehensive Physiology 2011; pp. 1179–1236. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  38. Haiden GJ, Awad EA. The ultrastructure of the avian golgi tendon organ. The Anatomical Record. 1981;200:153–161. doi: 10.1002/ar.1092000205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Heglund NC, Cavagna GA, Taylor CR. Energetics and mechanics of terrestrial locomotion III. Energy changes of the centre of mass as a function of speed and body size in birds and mammals. The Journal of Experimental Biology. 1982;97:41–56. doi: 10.1242/jeb.97.1.41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Herzog W. History dependence of skeletal muscle force production: implications for movement control. Human Movement Science. 2004;23:591–604. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2004.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Herzog W. Mechanisms of enhanced force production in lengthening (eccentric) muscle contractions. Journal of Applied Physiology. 2014;116:1407–1417. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00069.2013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Higham TE, Biewener AA. Integration within and between muscles during terrestrial locomotion: effects of incline and speed. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2008;211:2303–2316. doi: 10.1242/jeb.016139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Ijspeert AJ. Biorobotics: using robots to emulate and investigate agile locomotion. Science. 2014;346:196–203. doi: 10.1126/science.1254486. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Jindrich DL, Full RJ.  Dynamic stabilization of rapid hexapedal locomotion. The Journal of Experimental Biology. 2002;205:2803–2823. doi: 10.1242/jeb.205.18.2803. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Josephson RK. Dissecting muscle power output. The Journal of Experimental Biology. 1999;202:3369–3375. doi: 10.1242/jeb.202.23.3369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Kuo AD. The relative roles of feedforward and feedback in the control of rhythmic movements. Motor Control. 2002;6:129–145. doi: 10.1123/mcj.6.2.129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Lam T, Pearson KG. The role of proprioceptive feedback in the regulation and adaptation of locomotor activity. In: Gandevia S. C, Proske U, Stuart D. G, editors. Sensorimotor Control of Movement and Posture. Boston: Springer; 2002. pp. 343–355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Loeb GE, Brown IE, Cheng EJ. A hierarchical foundation for models of sensorimotor control. Experimental Brain Research. 1999;126:1–18. doi: 10.1007/s002210050712. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Lyle MA, Prilutsky BI, Gregor RJ, Abelew TA, Nichols TR. Self-reinnervated muscles lose autogenic length feedback, but intermuscular feedback can recover functional connectivity. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2016;116:1055–1067. doi: 10.1152/jn.00335.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Lyle MA, Nichols TR. Patterns of intermuscular inhibitory force feedback across cat hindlimbs suggest a flexible system for regulating whole limb mechanics. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2018;119:668–678. doi: 10.1152/jn.00617.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Maas H, Prilutsky BI, Nichols TR, Gregor RJ. The effects of self-reinnervation of cat medial and lateral gastrocnemius muscles on hindlimb kinematics in slope walking. Experimental Brain Research. 2007;181:377–393. doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-0938-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Maier A. The avian muscle spindle. Anatomy and Embryology. 1992;186:1–25. doi: 10.1007/BF00710398. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Marigold DS, Drew T. Posterior parietal cortex estimates the relationship between object and body location during locomotion. eLife. 2017;6:e28143. doi: 10.7554/eLife.28143. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. More HL, Hutchinson JR, Collins DF, Weber DJ, Aung SKH, Donelan JM. Scaling of sensorimotor control in terrestrial mammals. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2010;277:3563–3568. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.0898. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. More HL, Donelan JM. Scaling of sensorimotor delays in terrestrial mammals. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2018;285:20180613. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2018.0613. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Moritz CT, Farley CT. Passive dynamics change leg mechanics for an unexpected surface during human hopping. Journal of Applied Physiology. 2004;97:1313–1322. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00393.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Nelson FE, Gabaldón AM, Roberts TJ. Force–velocity properties of two avian hindlimb muscles. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology Part A: Molecular & Integrative Physiology. 2004;137:711–721. doi: 10.1016/j.cbpb.2004.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Nichols TR. The organization of heterogenic reflexes among muscles crossing the ankle joint in the decerebrate cat. The Journal of Physiology. 1989;410:463–477. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1989.sp017544. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Nishikawa K, Biewener AA, Aerts P, Ahn AN, Chiel HJ, Daley MA, Daniel TL, Full RJ, Hale ME, Hedrick TL, Lappin AK, Nichols TR, Quinn RD, Satterlie RA, Szymik B. Neuromechanics: an integrative approach for understanding motor control. Integrative and Comparative Biology. 2007;47:16–54. doi: 10.1093/icb/icm024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Nishikawa KC, Monroy JA, Uyeno TE, Yeo SH, Pai DK, Lindstedt SL. Is titin a ‘winding filament’? A new twist on muscle contraction. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2012;279:981–990. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2011.1304. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Nishikawa KC, Monroy JA, Tahir U. Muscle function from organisms to molecules. Integrative and Comparative Biology. 2018;58:194–206. doi: 10.1093/icb/icy023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Pearson K. Motor systems. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2000;10:649–654. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(00)00130-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Pearson KG, Misiaszek JE, Hulliger M. Chemical ablation of sensory afferents in the walking system of the cat abolishes the capacity for functional recovery after peripheral nerve lesions. Experimental Brain Research. 2003;150:50–60. doi: 10.1007/s00221-003-1445-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Pearson K, Gramlich R. Updating neural representations of objects during walking. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2010;1198:1–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.05422.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Poore SO, Ashcroft A, Sanchez-Haiman A, Goslow G. The contractile properties of the M. supracoracoideus in the pigeon and starling: A case for long-axis rotation of the humerus. Journal of Experimental Biology. 1997;200:2987–3002. doi: 10.1242/jeb.200.23.2987. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Potocanac Z, de Bruin J, van der Veen S, Verschueren S, van Dieën J, Duysens J, Pijnappels M. Fast online corrections of tripping responses. Experimental Brain Research. 2014;232:3579–3590. doi: 10.1007/s00221-014-4038-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Prochazka A, Ellaway P. Sensory systems in the control of movement. Comprehensive Physiology. 2012;2:2615–2627. doi: 10.1002/cphy.c100086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Proske U, Gandevia SC. The proprioceptive senses: their roles in signaling body shape, body position and movement, and muscle force. Physiological Reviews. 2012;92:1651–1697. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00048.2011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Richards CT. Building a robotic link between muscle dynamics and hydrodynamics. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2011;214:2381–2389. doi: 10.1242/jeb.056671. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Roberts TJ, Marsh RL, Weyand PG, Taylor CR. Muscular force in running turkeys: the economy of minimizing work. Science. 1997;275:1113–1115. doi: 10.1126/science.275.5303.1113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Robertson BD, Sawicki GS. Unconstrained muscle-tendon workloops indicate resonance tuning as a mechanism for elastic limb behavior during terrestrial locomotion. PNAS. 2015;112:E5891–E5898. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1500702112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Rode C, Siebert T, Blickhan R. Titin-induced force enhancement and force depression: a 'sticky-spring' mechanism in muscle contractions? Journal of Theoretical Biology. 2009;259:350–360. doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2009.03.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Ross KT, Nichols TR. Heterogenic feedback between hindlimb extensors in the spontaneously locomoting premammillary cat. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2009;101:184–197. doi: 10.1152/jn.90338.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Rossignol S, Dubuc R, Gossard JP. Dynamic sensorimotor interactions in locomotion. Physiological Reviews. 2006;86:89–154. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00028.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Roth E, Sponberg S, Cowan NJ. A comparative approach to closed-loop computation. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2014;25:54–62. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2013.11.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Safavynia SA, Ting LH. Long-latency muscle activity reflects continuous, delayed sensorimotor feedback of task-level and not joint-level error. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2013;110:1278–1290. doi: 10.1152/jn.00609.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Sawicki GS, Robertson BD, Azizi E, Roberts TJ. Timing matters: tuning the mechanics of a muscle-tendon unit by adjusting stimulation phase during cyclic contractions. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2015;218:3150–3159. doi: 10.1242/jeb.121673. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Sharp AA, Bekoff A. Pyridoxine treatment alters embryonic motility in chicks: implications for the role of proprioception. Developmental Psychobiology. 2015;57:271–277. doi: 10.1002/dev.21282. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Sherrington CS. Remarks on the reflex mechanism of the step. Brain. 1910;33:1–25. doi: 10.1093/brain/33.1.1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  80. Sherrington CS, Laslett EE. Observations on some spinal reflexes and the interconnection of spinal segments. The Journal of Physiology. 1903;29:58–96. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1903.sp000946. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Spotnitz WD. Fibrin sealant: past, present, and future: a brief review. World Journal of Surgery. 2010;34:632–634. doi: 10.1007/s00268-009-0252-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Taylor CR, Heglund NC, Maloiy GM. Energetics and mechanics of terrestrial locomotion I. Metabolic energy consumption as a function of speed and body size in birds and mammals. The Journal of Experimental Biology. 1982;97:1–21. doi: 10.1242/jeb.97.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Todorov E. Optimality principles in sensorimotor control. Nature Neuroscience. 2004;7:907–915. doi: 10.1038/nn1309. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Vannucci B, Santosa KB, Keane AM, Jablonka-Shariff A, Lu CY, Yan Y, MacEwan M, Snyder-Warwick AK. What is normal? neuromuscular junction reinnervation after nerve injury. Muscle & Nerve. 2019;60:604–612. doi: 10.1002/mus.26654. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Wolpert DM, Diedrichsen J, Flanagan JR. Principles of sensorimotor learning. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2011;12:739–751. doi: 10.1038/nrn3112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Yakovenko S, Gritsenko V, Prochazka A. Contribution of stretch reflexes to locomotor control: a modeling study. Biological Cybernetics. 2004;90:146–155. doi: 10.1007/s00422-003-0449-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Yeo SH, Monroy JA, Lappin AK, Nishikawa KC, Pai DK. Phenomenological models of the dynamics of muscle during isotonic shortening. Journal of Biomechanics. 2013;46:2419–2425. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2013.07.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: K VijayRaghavan1
Reviewed by: Noah J Cowan2, Lena H Ting3

In the interests of transparency, eLife publishes the most substantive revision requests and the accompanying author responses.

Acceptance summary:

Gordon et al. use bilateral reinnervation of a leg muscle to deduce reflex contributions to muscle activity, kinematics, and muscle force-length trajectories during walking and obstacle crossing in bipedal birds, consistent with prior work in quadrupedal locomotion. Not only does the work advance our understanding, but the paper is very well-written with an extensive analysis that is of broad interest to those interested in complex sensorimotor integration. The work described in this paper is novel and its impact would be considerable in understanding the complexities of sensorimotor reflexes in bipedal locomotion.

Decision letter after peer review:

Thank you for submitting your article "Tuning of feedforward control enables stable muscle dynamics after loss of autogenic proprioceptive feedback" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by two peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by K VijayRaghavan as the Senior Editor and Reviewing Editor. The following individuals involved in review of your submission have agreed to reveal their identity: Noah J Cowan (Reviewer #1); Lena H Ting (Reviewer #2).

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

Summary:

This paper studies the mechanism of sensorimotor integration of guinea fowl as an example of bipedal locomotion. Sensorimotor integration consists of neuronal feedforward, neuronal feedback and intrinsic muscle mechanics. Authors have studied how lateral gastrocnemius muscle (LG) and nervous system respond to the rapid perturbation when the proprioceptive sense as the fast feedback response is lost. Statistical comparison has been conducted between two groups (reinnervated and intact birds) through the recorded running trials at the same speed for both level and obstacle terrain. From the collected data of muscle force and length (intrinsic mechanics) and EMG (feedforward) signals, the authors have found that despite deficits in LG monosynaptic reflex following reinnervation, the general pattern of muscle dynamics of the reinnervated LG (rLG) is qualitatively similar to that of previously measured intact birds (iLG). However, the steady-state timing of EMG activation of rLG, as the feedforward, is phase-shifted earlier in the stride cycle. This earlier onset enables rapid force development and higher muscle stiffness at the time of foot contact and likely helps compensate for the loss of proprioceptive feedback. Additionally, regulation of force duration in obstacle strides is disrupted following reinnervation, suggesting that proprioceptive feedback in late stance normally regulates force duration. The authors have concluded that the autogenic proprioceptive deficit will lead to increased reliance on feedforward tuning of muscle activity to achieve stable muscle dynamics in obstacle terrain.

In general, this is a very well-written paper, with an extensive analysis that is of broad interest to those interested in complex sensorimotor integration. Also, in general, the figures and methods were very clear. The work described in this paper is novel and its impact would be considerable in understanding complexities of sensorimotor in bipedal locomotion.

There are some important concerns that, however, need to be addressed. In the main, these require:

A) Presenting the data more clearly.

B) A clear qualitative description of the differences between cohorts.

C) Redo the statistical analysis in a more interpretable manner.

D) Improve clarity of figures.

E) Reproducing data from the previous manuscript in order to make side-by-side comparisons will be convenient.

Essential revisions:

1) Suggestions:

Figure 2: To avoid confusion, it's better to use different colors since blue and orange are used extensively in this paper for iLG and rLG.

Figure 5: Figure is really busy and it's hard to visually see the differences between iLG and rLG. The difference between iLG and rLG looks negligible. It might be useful to show the mean and standard deviation of the difference between rLG and iLG.

2) A major weakness is the lack of discussion about potentially profound differences across the two groups, and inconsistent comparison across different cohorts of bird. While the authors do acknowledge that different birds were sampled in the intact and reinnervated groups in the Materials and methods, this point may easily be missed by the reader and should be re-stated as a limitation in the Discussion (perhaps starting at the seventh paragraph of the subsection “What is the role of proprioception in the control of high-speed locomotion?”). Considering that the nerves were injured while the birds were juveniles and testing was performed in adults means the normal maturation process may have impacted adaptation to proprioceptive loss and may limit applicability to understanding the roles of feedback and feedforward control, or effects of nerve injury, in adults. In general, similarities among groups are stated to be "comparable" without supporting quantification from the intact group (e.g. in the Materials and methods, body and muscle masses, muscle lengths, gait speed etc.; in Results, Figure 2. While is it also reported that tendon tap reflexes were absent, no quantification was provided. How are potentially different levels of recovery accounted for? Was exercise begun 7 weeks after injury to avoid regeneration associated with immediate training? (Brandt et al., 2015).

3) Despite the assertion in the Results (first paragraph), there are clearly differences in the level- and obstacle-terrain gait kinematics and kinetics between groups (c.f. Figures 2, 3, 6, 8).

4) Differences across S-1, S-0, and S+1 are interesting but not discussed. Why is it that S+1 has a greater advance in EMG than S-0 and S-1? This could be compared to downslope walking?

5) In general, figures were difficult to digest without careful parsing of the legends and could use better in-figure descriptors. For example, Figures 3 and 4 are not readable in black and white, the upper and lower rows could be labeled as "intact" and "reinnervated", while a dark line indicating "level terrain" with the colors indicating "obstacle". Foot contact lines could also be labeled, as the legends are hard to follow. Figure 3 legend "for a single individual from each condition" it is not clear whether "condition" refers to level vs. obstacle or intact vs. reinnervated.

6) The mixed model ANOVA results were difficult to follow, and it may be better to present specific individual findings based on the hypothesis, e.g. earlier LG activation, evidence of later reflex, There is also concern over the number of variables in the model and the potential for false positives (Tables 1, 2 and Figure 5). A select number of tests should be done in alignment with specific hypotheses posed. Mean effects should be reported using ANOVA and post hoc tests. The coefficients reported are not interpretable in terms of differences across conditions/steps (e.g. Figure 6B). Bar charts with the main effect would be easier to understand.

7) The implication for broader audiences could be enhanced, and the differences between bipedal and quadrupedal locomotor impairments are not clearly discussed. It is not clear that the shift to bipedal gait added significant insight into the role of proprioception in movement, as similar deficits occur in both.

eLife. 2020 Jun 23;9:e53908. doi: 10.7554/eLife.53908.sa2

Author response


In general, this is a very well-written paper, with an extensive analysis that is of broad interest to those interested in complex sensorimotor integration. Also, in general, the figures and methods were very clear. The work described in this paper is novel and its impact would be considerable in understanding complexities of sensorimotor in bipedal locomotion.

There are some important concerns that, however, need to be addressed. In the main, these require:

A) Presenting the data more clearly.

We have reformatted the figures to present data distributions and pairwise mean differences for the fixed effect categories. The figures, tables and text have been updated to allow direct comparison between the intact and reinnervated cohorts for all results.

B) A clear qualitative description of the differences between cohorts.

We have added text to the Materials and methods, Results and Discussion to provide clear descriptions of how the two cohorts compare in terms of experimental conditions (subsection “Animals and treadmill training”), reflex latency (subsection “Reinnervation surgery”), muscle morphology (subsection “Muscle morphology”), muscle function (subsections “Mechanical function of intact versus reinnervated LG”, “Force-length dynamics and work output during obstacle negotiation” and “Shifts in activation patterns between intact and reinnervated LG”) and gait kinematics (subsection “Stability and kinematic changes during obstacle negotiation in intact vs. reinnervated birds”). We revisit the differences between the cohorts in the limitations section of the Discussion (subsection “Limitations and future directions”). Throughout the text, we avoid stating that the two cohorts were ‘qualitatively similar’ or ‘comparable’ and instead state the specific similarities and differences between them. All figures provide direct comparison between the iLG and rLG cohorts, and we also provide the full datasets for both cohorts on DataDryad (https://doi.org/10.7280/D11H49).

C) Redo the statistical analysis in a more interpretable manner.

We revised the statistics as suggested to present pairwise mean differences between fixed effect categories in the figures and tables, with correction for multiple tests to maintain a 5% false discovery rate. We have also added a more detailed description of the statistical methods (subsection “Statistics”).

D) Improve clarity of figures.

We have modified the formatting of the figures as suggested by the reviewers, to consistently show the intact and reinnervated cohort datasets in direct comparison, with half-violin plots to show distributions, and bar plots with the mean and 95% confidence interval for pairwise comparison between fixed effect categories. We have added annotations to the figures to aid interpretation, as suggested by the reviewers.

E) Reproducing data from the previous manuscript in order to make side-by-side comparisons will be convenient.

All figures and analyses include the data from Daley and Biewener, 2011, alongside the new experimental data. We have provided the complete datasets (and processing code) for both cohorts through DataDryad as noted above.

Essential revisions:

1) Suggestions:

Figure 2: To avoid confusion, it's better to use different colors since blue and orange are used extensively in this paper for iLG and rLG.

Figure 2 has been revised to provide a direct comparison of average stride cycles for the intact and reinnervated birds. Annotations have been added to make the figure easier to read, and the color scheme is consistent with all other figures in the paper, with blue for intact gastrocnemius (iLG) and orange for reinnervated lateral gastrocnemius (rLG). (The current Figure 2 is a revised version of former Figure 4. The original Figure 2 has been updated and now provided as Figure 2—figure supplement 1).

Figure 5: Figure is really busy and it's hard to visually see the differences between iLG and rLG. The difference between iLG and rLG looks negligible. It might be useful to show the mean and standard deviation of the difference between rLG and iLG.

The original Figure 5 has been replaced with two separate figures (Figures 4 and 5) to allow clearer visual interpretation of the differences between iLG and rLG and to aid interpretation with respect to the hypotheses stated in the Introduction. Figure 4 contains half-violin distributions and bar plots for mean differences for variables relating to muscle mechanical function. Figure 5 contains half-violin distributions and bar plots for variables relating to muscle activation. Annotations have been added to aid the reader. The bar plots provide the mean and 95% CI for differences between the iLG and rLG treatment cohorts, and between level and obstacle terrain stride categories within treatment cohorts.

2) A major weakness is the lack of discussion about potentially profound differences across the two groups, and inconsistent comparison across different cohorts of bird. While the authors do acknowledge that different birds were sampled in the intact and reinnervated groups in the Materials and methods, this point may easily be missed by the reader and should be re-stated as a limitation in the Discussion (perhaps starting at the seventh paragraph of the subsection “What is the role of proprioception in the control of high-speed locomotion?”). Considering that the nerves were injured while the birds were juveniles and testing was performed in adults means the normal maturation process may have impacted adaptation to proprioceptive loss and may limit applicability to understanding the roles of feedback and feedforward control, or effects of nerve injury, in adults. In general, similarities among groups are stated to be "comparable" without supporting quantification from the intact group (e.g. in Materials and methods, body and muscle masses, muscle lengths, gait speed etc.; in Results, Figure 2. While is it also reported that tendon tap reflexes were absent, no quantification was provided. How are potentially different levels of recovery accounted for? Was exercise begun 7 weeks after injury to avoid regeneration associated with immediate training? (Brandt et al., 2015).

We have added text to the Materials and methods, Results and Discussion to provide clear descriptions of how the two cohorts compare in terms of experimental conditions (subsection “Animals and treadmill training”), reflex latency (subsection “Reinnervation surgery”), muscle morphology (subsection “Muscle morphology”), muscle function (subsections “Mechanical function of intact versus reinnervated LG”, “Force-length dynamics and work output during obstacle negotiation” and “Shifts in activation patterns between intact and reinnervated LG”) and gait kinematics (subsection “Stability and kinematic changes during obstacle negotiation in intact vs. reinnervated birds”). We revisit the differences between the cohorts in the limitations section of the Discussion (subsection “Limitations and future directions”. Throughout the text, we avoid stating that the two cohorts were ‘qualitatively similar’ or ‘comparable’ and instead state the specific similarities and differences between them.

We have also added additional text on the methods rationale and relevant citations to the literature on cats and rats.

All figures provide direct comparison between the iLG and rLG cohorts, and we also provide the full datasets for both cohorts on DataDryad (https://doi.org/10.7280/D11H49).

3) Despite the assertion in the Results (first paragraph), there are clearly differences in the level- and obstacle-terrain gait kinematics and kinetics between groups (c.f. Figures 2, 3, 6, 8).

The Results and Discussion have been updated to more clearly state the differences in gait kinematics between the intact and reinnervated cohorts (subsections “Stability and kinematic changes during obstacle negotiation in intact vs. reinnervated birds”, and “What is the role of proprioception in the control of high-speed locomotion?”).

4) Differences across S-1, S-0, and S+1 are interesting but not discussed. Why is it that S+1 has a greater advance in EMG than S-0 and S-1? This could be compared to downslope walking?

We summarize these differences between stride categories in the Results (subsection “Stability and kinematic changes during obstacle negotiation in intact vs. reinnervated birds”) and Discussion in the context of interpreting stability and neuromuscular control strategies (subsection “What is the role of proprioception in the control of high-speed locomotion?”). Although S +1 has slightly greater phase advance in activation, the difference is not statistically significant (Figure 6, Table 2). The main difference in S +1 for rLG is a 39% increase in total EMG activity, at 6% decrease in force duration and 4% decrease in stride duration, compared to steady state level strides (reported in the subsection “Stability and kinematic changes during obstacle negotiation in intact vs. reinnervated birds”).

We have added a paragraph to the Discussion to address how our findings relate to similar work on cats and rats (subsection “What is the role of proprioception in the control of high-speed locomotion?”).

5) In general, figures were difficult to digest without careful parsing of the legends and could use better in-figure descriptors. For example, Figures 3 and 4 are not readable in black and white, the upper and lower rows could be labeled as "intact" and "reinnervated", while a dark line indicating "level terrain" with the colors indicating "obstacle". Foot contact lines could also be labeled, as the legends are hard to follow. Figure 3 legend "for a single individual from each condition" it is not clear whether "condition" refers to level vs. obstacle or intact vs. reinnervated.

We have revised the figures as suggested, including added annotations to indicate ‘intact’ and ‘reinnervated’ cohorts, arrows indicating level and obstacle terrain strides, triangles for foot contact with an annotation arrow. We have revised the phrasing to refer to intact and reinnervated ‘cohorts’ and level and obstacle terrain ‘conditions’.

6) The mixed model ANOVA results were difficult to follow, and it may be better to present specific individual findings based on the hypothesis, e.g. earlier LG activation, evidence of later reflex, There is also concern over the number of variables in the model and the potential for false positives (Tables 1, 2 and Figure 5). A select number of tests should be done in alignment with specific hypotheses posed. Mean effects should be reported using ANOVA and post hoc tests. The coefficients reported are not interpretable in terms of differences across conditions/steps (e.g. Figure 6B). Bar charts with the main effect would be easier to understand.

The figures have been revised to focus on specific variables relating to muscle mechanical function in Figure 4 and muscle activation in Figure 5, to make the link to the specific hypotheses clearer. (The hypotheses are stated in Introduction in the subsection “Investigating the role of proprioception through self-reinnervation”). The results have been re-sequenced to follow the revised figure sequence.

The statistics have been updated to provide post hoc pairwise mean differences between fixed effect categories from the ANOVA, with correction for multiple tests to maintain a 5% false positive rate. The figures include bar charts of the pairwise mean differences, with 95% confidence intervals. Comparisons are made between the intact and reinnervated cohorts (in grey) and between level and obstacle stride categories within treatment cohorts (in blue for iLG and orange for rLG).

7) The implication for broader audiences could be enhanced, and the differences between bipedal and quadrupedal locomotor impairments are not clearly discussed. It is not clear that the shift to bipedal gait added significant insight into the role of proprioception in movement, as similar deficits occur in both.

We have added a paragraph discussing the findings in the context of previous work in cats and rats (subsection “What is the role of proprioception in the control of high-speed locomotion?”). In general, our findings are consistent with the studies of quadrupeds. However, the studies of rats and cats have focused more on muscle activity patterns in relation to kinematics, without detailed analysis of in vivo muscle mechanical function (direct measures of muscle force and work). The current study provides insight into how shifts in muscle activation relate to shifts in muscle mechanical function to compensate for loss of reflex-mediated ankle stiffness.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Data Citations

    1. Daley MA, Gordon JC, Holt NC, Biewener A. 2020. Dataset for 'Tuning of feedforward control enables stable muscle force-length dynamics after loss of autogenic proprioceptive feedback'. Dryad Digital Repository. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]

    Supplementary Materials

    Table 1—source data 1. P-values linear mixed effect model ANOVA with fixed effects of treatment cohort (treatment: intact, reinnervated) and stride category (stride ID) and the interaction treatment x stride ID.
    Table 2—source data 1. P-values for posthoc pairwise mean differences between intact and reinnervated treatment cohorts (left column) and between obstacle stride categories compared to the level terrain means, within treatment cohorts (intact/reinnervated).
    Figure 8—source data 1. ANOVA results for ankle angle at time of foot contact.

    F-statistics, p-values and posthoc pairwise comparisons for linear mixed effect model ANOVA with fixed effects of treatment cohort (treatment: intact, reinnervated) and stride category (stride ID) and the interaction treatment x stride ID. Posthoc pairwise mean differences (mean ±95% ci) are shown between intact and reinnervated treatment cohorts (left), and between obstacle stride categories compared to level stride means, within treatment cohorts (intact/reinnervated).

    Transparent reporting form

    Data Availability Statement

    The full dataset including raw data, metadata files and processing code have been deposited to Dryad (https://doi.org/10.7280/D11H49).

    The following dataset was generated:

    Daley MA, Gordon JC, Holt NC, Biewener A. 2020. Dataset for 'Tuning of feedforward control enables stable muscle force-length dynamics after loss of autogenic proprioceptive feedback'. Dryad Digital Repository.


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES