Abstract
Unlike dogs and cats, people do not point their ears as they focus attention on novel, salient, or task-relevant stimuli. Our species may nevertheless have retained a vestigial pinna-orienting system that has persisted as a 'neural fossil’ within in the brain for about 25 million years. Consistent with this hypothesis, we demonstrate that the direction of auditory attention is reflected in sustained electrical activity of muscles within the vestigial auriculomotor system. Surface electromyograms (EMGs) were taken from muscles that either move the pinna or alter its shape. To assess reflexive, stimulus-driven attention we presented novel sounds from speakers at four different lateral locations while the participants silently read a boring text in front of them. To test voluntary, goal-directed attention we instructed participants to listen to a short story coming from one of these speakers, while ignoring a competing story from the corresponding speaker on the opposite side. In both experiments, EMG recordings showed larger activity at the ear on the side of the attended stimulus, but with slightly different patterns. Upward movement (perking) differed according to the lateral focus of attention only during voluntary orienting; rearward folding of the pinna’s upper-lateral edge exhibited such differences only during reflexive orienting. The existence of a pinna-orienting system in humans, one that is experimentally accessible, offers opportunities for basic as well as applied science.
Research organism: Human
eLife digest
Dogs, cats, monkeys and other animals perk their ears in the direction of sounds they are interested in. Humans and their closest ape relatives, however, appear to have lost this ability. Some humans are able to wiggle their ears, suggesting that some of the brain circuits and muscles that allow automatic ear movements towards sounds are still present. This may be a ‘vestigial feature’, an ability that is maintained even though it no longer serves its original purpose.
Now, Strauss et al. show that vestigial movements of muscles around the ear indicate the direction of sounds a person is paying attention to. In the experiments, human volunteers tried to read a boring text while surprising sounds like a traffic jam, a baby crying, or footsteps played. During this exercise, Strauss et al. recorded the electrical activity in the muscles of their ears to see if they moved in response to the direction the sound came from. In a second set of experiments, the same electrical recordings were made as participants listened to a podcast while a second podcast was playing from a different direction. The individuals’ ears were also recorded using high resolution video.
Both sets of experiments revealed tiny involuntary movements in muscles surrounding the ear closest to the direction of a sound the person is listening to. When the participants tried to listen to one podcast and tune out another, they also made ear ‘perking’ movements in the direction of their preferred podcast.
The results suggest that movements of the vestigial muscles in the human ear indicate the direction of sounds a person is paying attention to. These tiny movements could be used to develop better hearing aids that sense the electrical activity in the ear muscles and amplify sounds the person is trying to focus on, while minimizing other sounds.
Introduction
Watching the ears allows an equestrian to gauge their mount’s shifting attention. Ear movements are not a useful cue in humans or apes because higher primates have lost the ability to orient by adjusting pinna shape and focal direction. Instead we judge a person’s attention by their gaze direction. In thousands of research reports each year, though, casual observation of ocular orienting is replaced by sophisticated recording techniques. We show in the present paper that similar electrical and optical techniques allow us to extract muscular correlates of pinna-orienting in our species and even render subtle pinna-orienting movements visible. Activation of the ear muscles is directionally specific and it occurs during voluntary as well as reflexive attention.
A review of research in Hackley, 2015 on pinna-orienting in humans identified three relevant findings scattered across the preceding 100-or-so years. The first was Wilson’s oculo-auricular phenomenon (Wilson, 1908), in which shifting the gaze hard to one side elicits a 1 to 4 mm deflection of the lateral rim of both ears. The relevance to spatial attention is uncertain, though, with diverging results across studies, for example see Gerstle and Wilkinson, 1929; Urban et al., 1993; O'Beirne and Patuzzi, 1999. Additional evidence comes from a 1987 study (Hackley et al., 1987) of the bilateral postauricular muscle (PAM) reflex (onset latency = 10 ms) to acoustic onset transients. Increased amplitudes were observed when subjects directed their attention to a stream of tones on the same side as the recorded muscle while ignoring a competing, contralateral stream. Comparisons across left/right stimulus, attention, and PAM combinations localized modulation to the motor limb of the reflex arc. This pattern could indicate that the muscle behind an ear is primed when attention is directed toward that side. Finally, an experiment in Stekelenburg and van Boxtel, 2002 found that the automatic capture of attention by unexpected sounds coming from a speaker hidden to the left of the participant elicited greater activity in the left than right PAM.
Apart from the research just described, functional studies of the human auriculomotor system have been mainly limited to the PAM reflex, in the context of audiometry or affective psychophysiology. The auriculomotor system lies essentially untouched in the literature. Here we present evidence that our brains retain vestigial circuitry for orienting the pinnae during both exogenous, stimulus-driven attention to brief, novel sounds and endogenous, goal-directed attention to sustained speech. We also demonstrate a complex interplay of different auricular muscles which may be causally linked to subtle movements of the pinnae.
Results
Experiment 1 - Exogenous attention
To examine automatic, stimulus-driven attention we used novel sounds similar to those in the Stekelenburg and van Boxtel, 2002 study, for example traffic jam, baby crying, footsteps. However, we presented them randomly from four different speakers (at ± 30°, ± 120°; Figure 1) rather than just one, while the subject read a boring essay. As we were interested in the interactive role of distinct muscles in attempting to shape and point the pinnae, we recorded EMG from posterior, anterior, superior, and transverse auricular muscles (PAM, AAM, SAM, and TAM). Visual evidence had previously been limited to still photos of Wilson’s oculo-auricular phenomenon (Wilson, 1908), so we supplemented our EMG data with videos from four high-definition cameras, see Methods and Video 1, Video 2, and Video 3. To confirm that our findings would generalize to different age groups, older (62.7 ± 5.9 y) as well as younger (24.1 ± 3.1 y) adults were tested.
The signal-averaged EMG waveforms of Figure 2 show well-defined responses with an onset latency of about 70 ms, responses that vary in amplitude, duration, and morphology according to the relative direction of the sound source. The inter- and intra-subject variability for the analyzed auricular muscles is shown in Figure 3 and Figure 4, respectively. These plots portray the consistency of the PAM, AAM, and TAM responses across stimuli and subjects, especially for stimulation from the back. For the statistical analysis, mean amplitudes were subjected to a mixed, repeated-measures analysis of variance, with factors of age group, stimulus-muscle correspondence (ipsi-/contralateral), and anterior/posterior stimulus direction. EMG amplitudes were larger for stimulus sources on the same side as the recorded ear for PAM, AAM, and TAM [ = 47.44, 17.01, and 47.53, respectively; -values < 0.001; = 0.65, 0.40, and 0.65] but not SAM.
Figure 3. Experiment 1 – Responses of the PAM to stimuli from the back speakers, showing intersubject variability: Top panels: Every row corresponds to the averaged response of one participant.
Amplitude is encoded in color. The top rows (1-16) represent younger adult participants; the bottom rows (17-28), older adults. Bottom panels: Mean and standard deviation based on the above plots. The following figure supplements are available for Figure 3—figure supplement 1. The described intersubject variability analysis for the AAM, Figure 3—figure supplement 2. SAM, and Figure 3—figure supplement 3. TAM.
Figure 3—figure supplement 1. Experiment 1 – Responses of the AAM to stimuli from the back speakers, showing intersubject variability: Top panels: Every row corresponds to the averaged response of one participant.
Figure 3—figure supplement 2. Experiment 1 – Responses of the SAM to stimuli from the back speakers, showing intersubject variability: Top panels: Every row corresponds to the averaged response of one participant.
Figure 3—figure supplement 3. Experiment 1 – Responses of the TAM to stimuli from the back speakers, showing intersubject variability: Top panels: Every row corresponds to the averaged response of one participant.
Figure 4. Experiment 1 – Intrasubject variability of the PAM: Mean and standard deviations of the phasic responses 50 - 300 ms) of every participant.
Top panels: responses to the front speakers. Bottom panels: responses to the back speakers. Left panels: Responses of the younger adults. Right panels: responses of the older adults. Blue represents ipsilateral responses, red represents contralateral responses. The following figure supplements are available for Figure 4—figure supplement 1. The described intrasubject variability analysis for the AAM, Figure 4—figure supplement 2. SAM, and Figure 4—figure supplement 3. TAM.
Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Experiment 1 – Intrasubject variability of the AAM: Mean and standard deviations of the phasic responses ( 50 - 300 ms) of every participant.
Figure 4—figure supplement 2. Experiment 1 – Intrasubject variability of the SAM: Mean and standard deviations of the phasic responses (50 -300 ms) of every participant.
Figure 4—figure supplement 3. Experiment 1 – Intrasubject variability of the TAM: Mean and standard deviations of the phasic responses ( 50 - 300 ms) of every participant.
Responses were also larger to sounds emanating from the back than the front speakers for PAM, AAM, and TAM [ = 32.1, 12.0, and 19.9, respectively; -values < 0.003, = 0.55, 0.32, and 0.43]. Posterior, ipsilateral stimulation elicited the most vigorous responses from these three muscles. In particular, the interaction between the factors ipsi/contralateral and anterior/posterior for PAM, AAM, and TAM yields = 40.4, 14.9, and 23.6, respectively; -values < 0.002; = 0.61, 0.36, and 0.48. There were no interactions involving age group, but a main effect indicated that older participants had smaller AAM responses [ = 6.0, < 0.03, = 0.19].
These results support the hypothesis that the human brain retains circuits that attempt to point the ears in the direction of unexpected, potentially relevant sounds. The corresponding vestigial auriculomotor drive appears to be causally linked to very small ear displacements, see Figure 2—figure supplement 1, Video 1, and Video 2. Having documented the existence of directionally-appropriate responses of the ear muscles to brief novel sounds, we turn now to a qualitatively distinct type of attention.
Experiment 2 – Endogenous Attention
To examine voluntary, goal-directed attention we used the classic, dichotic-listening paradigm, see Hillyard et al., 1973; Cherry, 1953. Two competing short stories were played either over the two front speakers or the two back speakers. To increase motivation participants were allowed to choose, after a brief introduction, which of the two stories (podcasts) they would like to listen to. They were then told which speaker that story would be presented from. Our subjects were instructed to listen carefully while looking at a fixation cross and, as in the immediately preceding study, holding their head still on a chin rest. Upon completion, a new story was picked and the listening direction was switched to one of the other speakers. Recording methods were identical to those of the exogenous experiment. Muscle activity was quantified as the mean of the absolute EMG energy over the entire course of each 5 min listening trial in Figure 5 and for consecutive segments of 10 s duration in a temporal analysis shown in Figure 6.
Figure 5. Experiment 2.
Grand average of the PAM, AAM, and SAM activity when stories were played from the front (top) and back speakers (bottom). Shown is the normalized (total) energy of the left/right recording channels during attention to the left or right story (bars represent the standard error). The following figure supplements are available for Figure 5—figure supplement 1. Subband analysis of the described ipsi- vs. contralateral effect for PAM, AAM, and SAM; Figure 5—figure supplement 2. Reported results for a selected narrow frequency band.
Figure 5—figure supplement 1. Grand average of the PAM, AAM, and SAM activity when stories were played from front (± 30°) and back speakers (± 120°).
Figure 5—figure supplement 2. Analogous to Figure 5 and Figure 6, respectively, in the main text but for frequency band 5 (37.5 – 75 Hz): Grand average of the PAM, AAM, and SAM activity when stories were played from the back speakers (± 120°).
Figure 6. Experiment 2.
Time–resolved activity (each sampling point represents the energy induced in consecutive 10 s segments) after pooling the ipsi– and contralateral signals with a segment–wise normalization for the front (top) and back speakers (bottom).
As in the exogenous study, EMG energy at PAM and AAM was largest on the side to which attention was focused [analysis corresponding to Figure 5: F(1, 19) = 15.2 and 4.6, respectively; 0.001 and 0.04; = 0.44 and 0.20]; an effect that is particularly strong in narrow–band middle frequency components of the signal, see Figure 5—figure supplements 1 and 2 and the tables in Supplementary file 1.
A different pattern emerged for the other two muscles. Whereas TAM but not SAM activity had reflected lateralization of transient, exogenous attention, the reverse was true for sustained, goal-directed attention. That is to say, mean EMG energy at SAM was larger at the ipsi– than contralateral ear [F(1, 19) = 16.3; ; = 0.46] in Experiment 2, but there was no such difference for TAM.
Another main effect indicated that activation of all four muscles was generally enhanced when participants listened to one of the two speakers that were slightly behind as opposed to in front of them [PAM, AAM, TAM, SAM: F(1, 19) = 5.7, 3.1, 8.1, and 12.0, respectively; 0.03, 0.09, 0.01, and 0.003; = 0.23, 0.14, 0.30, and 0.39]. These effects did not interact with each other or with age. Although PAM activity declines over time, EMG energy of all three muscles is clearly sustained across the 5-min sessions, see Figure 6. A corresponding sustained deflection of the pinna is also noticeable in the co-registered Video 3.
Potential motor confounds
An alternative to the account we have been developing is that participants in Experiments 1 and 2 may have shifted their gaze toward the attended source. This would have then triggered Wilson, 1908 phenomenon, that is auriculomotor activity secondary to large gaze shifts. To test this hypothesis, we segmented the horizontal electrooculogram (EOG) in the same way as the auricular EMG. Voltages were converted to degrees of arc separately for each participant, based on findings from a cursor tracking protocol (± 35°). Figure 7 and Figure 8 document a complete absence of eye movements that were systematically related to attention direction. A limitation of these findings is that electro-oculographic recordings have a resolution of only 1 − 2°. However, gaze shifts less than 30° are rarely accompanied by auriculomotor activity, see Urban et al., 1993. Representative examples of macrosaccades during reading in Experiment one with co-registered auricular muscle activity can be found in Figure 7—figure supplement 1. There is no obvious linkage of saccades and PAM responses. Note that the mean visual angle range observed in this example generalizes across subjects, see Figure 7—figure supplement 2. Also when considering all the macrosaccades from all the subjects in Experiment 1, our data do not exhibit a regularity between auditory stimuli and macrosaccades, see Figure 7—figure supplement 3.
Figure 7. Experiment 1: Averaged horizontal EOG activity at around the time of stimulation from the back speakers, showing an apparent absence of systematic shifts in gaze direction: Top panels: Every row corresponds to the averaged response of one participant.
Gaze angle is encoded in color, such that positive values (yellow) indicate rightward eye movements/positive angles. The top rows (1-16) represent younger adult participants; the bottom rows (17-28), older adults. Bottom panels: Mean and standard deviation based on the above plots.The following figure supplements are available for Figure 7—figure supplement 1. Macrosaccades during reading for one subject as example; Figure 7—figure supplement 2. Boxplots of the EOG for all subjects; Figure 7—figure supplement 3. Density of all detected macrosaccades during Experiment 1; Figure 7—figure supplement 4. Responses of the M. sternocleidomastoideus to stimuli from the front speakers; Figure 7—figure supplement 5. Responses of the M. sternocleidomastoideus to stimuli from the back speakers; Figure 7—figure supplement 6. Responses of the M. frontalis to stimuli from the front speakers; Figure 7—figure supplement 7. Responses of the M. frontalis to stimuli from the back speakers; Figure 7—figure supplement 8. Responses of the M. zygomaticus to stimuli from the front speakers; Figure 7—figure supplement 9. Responses of the M. zygomaticus to stimuli from the back speakers.
Figure 7—figure supplement 1. Macrosaccades during reading in Experiment one as recorded by means of horizontal EOG (black line) along with time-synchronized EMG from left and right PAM (blue and orange lines, respectively).
Figure 7—figure supplement 2. Boxplots of EOG signals from all subjects in Experiment 1.
Figure 7—figure supplement 3. Density of detected macrosaccades during Experiment 1.
Figure 7—figure supplement 4. Experiment 1 – Responses of the M. sternocleidomastoideus (M.SCM) to stimuli from the front speakers, showing intersubject variability: Top panels: Every row corresponds to the averaged response of one participant.
Figure 7—figure supplement 5. Experiment 1 – Responses of the M. sternocleidomastoideus (M.SCM) to stimuli from the back speakers, showing intersubject variability: Top panels: Every row corresponds to the averaged response of one participant.
Figure 7—figure supplement 6. Experiment 1 – Responses of the frontalis muscle to stimuli from the front speakers, showing intersubject variability: Top panels: Every row corresponds to the averaged response of one participant.
Figure 7—figure supplement 7. Experiment 1 – Responses of the frontalis muscle to stimuli from the back speakers, showing intersubject variability: Top panels: Every row corresponds to the averaged response of one participant.
Figure 7—figure supplement 8. Experiment 1 – Responses of the zygomaticus muscle to stimuli from the front speakers, showing intersubject variability: Top panels: Every row corresponds to the averaged response of one participant.
Figure 7—figure supplement 9. Experiment 1 – Responses of the zygomaticus muscle to stimuli from the back speakers, showing intersubject variability: Top panels: Every row corresponds to the averaged response of one participant.
Figure 8. Experiment 2: Intrasubject variability of the horizontal EOG: Mean and standard deviations of the EOG during the complete trial of every participant.
Top panels: attending the front speakers. Bottom panels: attending the back speakers. Left panels: younger adults. Right panels: older adults. Blue represents the EOG when attending the left, red when attending the right speaker. A positive EOG indicates that the gaze is directed toward the side of the attended speaker. Note that the deviation of the mean from is well within one standard deviation and therefore indicates that participants did not systematically divert their gaze to the attended speaker. The following figure supplements are available for Figure 8—figure supplement 1. Time-resolved EOG analysis in Experiment 2; Figure 8—figure supplement 2. Activity of the frontalis and zygomaticus muscle in Experiment 2.
Figure 8—figure supplement 1. Time-resolved activity (sampling points represent the energy within consecutive 10 s segments) after pooling the ipsi- and contralateral EOG signals for the front (left) and back speakers (right) in Experiment 2.
Figure 8—figure supplement 2. Grand average of the zygomaticus and frontalis muscle activity when stories were played from the front (top row) and back speakers (bottom row) in Experiment 2.
Another line of evidence that the auricular responses observed in our study were not secondary to eye movements, concerns their pattern of lateralization. Activation of TAM during Wilson’s oculo-auricular phenomenon is more vigorous on the side opposite the direction of gaze, see Gerstle and Wilkinson, 1929; Urban et al., 1993. By contrast, we found in Experiment one that TAM activation was relatively enhanced at the ear on the same side as the attention-engaging sounds. The PAM component of Wilson's phenomenon does exhibit enhanced activity on the ipsilateral side, but this effect appears to be reliable only for gaze shifts greater than about 40 degrees (Patuzzi and O'Beirne, 1999, Figure 4).
Another alternative interpretation is that participants oriented not with their ears or eyes, but by lifting their chin from the chin rest and rotating their head toward the attended sound. If humans have a vestibulo-auricular response as do cats (Tollin et al., 2009), such head rotations could have indirectly triggered activity in the ear muscles. However, recent research has shown that azimuthal head rotations have little effect on auricular activity in humans, see Cook and Patuzzi, 2014. Moreover, analysis of sternocleidomastoid EMG in our data suggest that movements of the neck were rare, small, and unsystematic, see Figure 7—figure supplements 4 and 5. An additional statistical analysis in Supplementary file 1 also rejects an influence of the sternocleidomastoid EMG and horizontal EOG. Finally, head rotations would have been too slow to generate the rapid responses of around 70 ms onset latency observed in the ear muscles in Experiment 1. We note with interest, though, the possibility that subtle, covert activation of head turning muscles (Corneil et al., 2008) might be correlated with ocular and auricular orienting. Note that there was also no corresponding co–activation of the other measured (non–auricular) facial muscles, the zygomaticus and frontalis muscle, see Figure 7—figure supplements 6–9 for Experiment one and Figure 8—figure supplement 2 for Experiment 2.
Discussion
These data provide compelling evidence that our brains retain, in vestigial form, circuitry for orienting the pinnae during both exogenous and endogenous modes of attention. The neural drive to our ear muscles is so weak that the actual movements (see co-registered video data in the Supplementary Information) are at least one to two orders of magnitude smaller compared to those generated during biting, smiling, grimacing, or voluntary ear-wiggling. To understand what remains of the vestigial pinna-orienting system so as to exploit it for practical or scientific purposes it is helpful to take a comparative, phylogenetic approach (Hackley, 2015, Hackley et al., 2017).
Vestigial Pinna-Orienting
The ability to swivel and point the pinnae seems to have been lost during the transition from the primarily nocturnal lifestyles of prosimians to the diurnal ones of New World monkeys, and then, Old World monkeys (Coleman and Ross, 2004). Mobility continued to decline as the ears became shorter and more rigid, see Waller et al., 2008; Coleman and Ross, 2004. The musculature degenerated. For example, an inferior auricular muscle to oppose SAM still exists in lesser apes such as gibbons and siamangs (Burrows et al., 2011), but not in chimpanzees (Burrows et al., 2011) or humans (Cattaneo and Pavesi, 2014). Given that head rotation has little effect on PAM activity (Cook and Patuzzi, 2014), it seems likely that the vestibulo–auricular reflex as documented in cats (Tollin et al., 2009) has not been conserved in our species. Also presumably lost is the ability to use proprioceptive information to adjust auditory processing in accordance with pinna position, orientation, and shape as documented in cats, see Kanold and Young, 2001. Although the ear muscles of Old World monkeys have spindles (Lovell et al., 1977), those of humans do not (Cattaneo and Pavesi, 2014).
When pinna-orienting movements became too small to modify acoustic input substantially, possibly 25 million years ago when lesser apes branched off from Old World monkeys, see Gibbs et al., 2007 as discussed in Hackley, 2015, selective environmental pressure ceased. The neural system became more-or-less 'frozen’ in a form optimized for controlling taller, more flexible ears, mounted on a smaller, more spherical head. This evolutionary perspective helps us to understand the surprising finding that AAM, which pulls the base of the pinna forward, was activated in Experiment one by novel sounds coming from the rear. Co-activation of opposing muscles AAM and PAM in our remote ancestors would have reduced occlusion of the ear canal by the tragus. Note that this occlusion occurs in monkeys when contraction of the PAM homolog is unopposed, see Waller et al., 2008, supplementary video clip 17. In addition, PAM-AAM co-activation would have stabilized the base of the pinna and reduced myotendinous elasticity, thereby allowing quick changes in position or orientation. This perspective also illuminates our unexpected finding of ipsilateral SAM suppression in Experiment 1. A study of pinna orienting in cats, whose tall ears resemble those of prosimians, showed that they tend to tilt the ear downward slightly when orienting to a lateral target, see Populin and Yin, 1998.
Potential neural mechanisms
Neurobiologists have distinguished two types of pinna-orienting movements in cats, based on onset latency, see Siegmund and Santibáñez, 1982. The short-latency response is specific to auditory stimuli and is chronometrically uncorrelated with saccades toward the target. By contrast, the long-latency response can be elicited by visual as well as auditory stimuli and it is roughly synchronous with ocular orienting, see Populin and Yin, 1998. Using a 4-speaker set-up similar to that of the present Experiment 1, Siegmund and Santibáñez, 1982 found cats’ unconditioned pinna responses to have an EMG onset latency that averaged 78 ms, similar to our value of about 70 ms. The animals were then trained to make gaze shifts toward the sound sources. Onset latency of the auriculomotor responses dropped to a remarkable 29 ms and the responses were resistant to extinction over the course of 125 trials. Both findings were replicated by Populin and Yin, 1998 (mean = 26 ms; failure to extinguish across 10,000 unreinforced trials). The latter authors obtained an even more rapid response (mean = 21 ms) when the sound was preceded by a visual stimulus that served as warning signal and indicated that the cat should maintain gaze at a fixation point. They argued that the short-latency pinna response is too rapid to be mediated by the brain region most centrally involved in orienting, the superior colliculus (SC). This is because an earlier study, Populin and Yin, 1997, had found the average first-spike latency in the relevant portion of this structure to be 19 ms.
Comparisons with these cat studies suggest that our participants' auriculomotor responses may have been primarily also of the short-latency variety that is not mediated by the SC. Two of the conditions tested by Populin and Yin, 1998 involved brief, lateralized auditory stimuli that, as in the present Experiment 1, were not task-relevant. The stimuli elicited short-latency ipsilateral pinna movements that were temporally uncorrelated with gaze shifts (see their Figures 6 and 8). During the delayed-saccade condition of their study, laterally presented sounds were task-relevant and forward fixation was required, as in our Experiment 2. Ipsilateral pinna movements triggered by onset of these sounds were of the short-latency variety (21 ms, as noted above). Subsequent, smaller movements were then observed in synchrony with ocular orienting, roughly 400 ms after the fixation point was extinguished (Figure 7). It is long-latency pinna movements of this sort that Populin and Yin, 1998 suggested might be mediated by the SC.
Given the major role of the SC in controlling eye fixation (Krauzlis et al., 2017), this structure may also be responsible for sustained maintenance of pinna orientation, such as in Experiment 2. Pinna movements can be triggered by electrical stimulation of the deep and intermediate layers of the SC in accordance with a topographical pattern that is in register with that of eye movements (Stein and Clamann, 1981). Lesions of this structure reduce the likelihood of pinna orienting as well as its accuracy, see Czihak et al., 1983. Although monosynaptic connections from SC to auriculomotor neurons in the facial nucleus do exist (Vidal et al., 1988), pinna control is dominated by disynaptic pathways from the SC that include the paralemniscal, oculomotor, or pontine reticular zones, see Henkel and Edwards, 1978; Takeuchi et al., 1979; Vidal et al., 1988. Among these, the paralemniscal zone appears to be the most important, and its auditory input originates in the nearby nucleus sagulum, see Henkel, 1981.
Portions of neocortex also play a role in controlling pinna movements. Lesions of auditory cortex reduce the kinematic complexity of pinna orienting and slow its habituation, see Alvarado and Santibañez, 1971. Stimulation and recording studies in the macaque have identified a premotor ear-eye field (area 8B), which is connected with both auditory cortical areas and the SC, see Lanzilotto et al., 2013. These animal neuroanatomy and physiology studies, coupled with Wilson, 1908 seminal report, make it clear that the eyes and pinnae work together during endogenously cued attentional orienting.
Future work
A recent study in humans by Gruters et al., 2018, showed a close relationship between movements of the left and right eardrums and multiple parameters of task-related left- and right-directed eye movements. It will be important in future research to test whether muscular responses of the middle and outer ears are linked in a coordinated manner to ocular orienting. Furthermore, exploration of the relationship between human auriculomotor activity and subtle markers of covert attention (see the recent review given in van Ede et al., 2019) corticofugal modulation of ascending auditory pathways (Perrot et al., 2006) in endogenous attention, and the neural mechanisms of orienting discussed in the preceding section has scarcely begun.
Our results have implications for applied science, as well. They suggest that patterns of auricular muscle activity might serve as an easily accessible correlate of top-down processing in endogenous modes of attention. As such, the described effects might complement electroencephalographic indices of attentional focus (de Cheveigné et al., 2018; Schäfer et al., 2018) in that their sensitivity is exclusively spatial, rather than reflecting a context-specific mixture of modality, feature, location, and object representations. Registration of pinna-orienting might better support near real-time decoding of the attentional focus and, as compared to EEG-based stimulus reconstruction approaches, does not require the exogenous sound source, for example see the discussion in Schäfer et al., 2018. Thus, auricular muscle monitoring might support the decoding of auditory attention in technical applications such as attentionally controlled hearing aids that preferentially amplify sounds the user is attempting to listen to. We wish to underscore, though, that the development of such applications would benefit crucially from a better understanding of how the auditory and visual attention systems interact. We hope that the results presented here will stimulate research in this direction.
Materials and methods
Participants
Both older (N = 12, mean age = 62.7 ± 5.9 y, 8 F, all right-handed) and younger adult (N = 16, mean age = 24.1 ± 3.1 y, 8 F, 15 right-handed, one left-handed) volunteers in Experiment one had age-typical, pure tone audiometric thresholds (1, 2, 4, and 8 kHz; young < 20 dB; old < 40 dB). All served in both studies, but after the 8th participant, Experiment two was altered in several ways (e.g., four stimulus directions rather than two). Only data from the final 21 subjects were retained for Experiment 2. The two groups in this experiment comprised 11 older adults (mean age = 62.6 ± 6.2 y, 8 F, all right-handed) and 10 younger adult (mean age = 24.1 ± 3.6 y, 5 F, nine right-handed, one left-handed). After a detailed explanation of the procedure, all subjects signed a consent form. The study was approved by the responsible ethics committee (ethics commission at the Ärztekammer des Saarlandes, Saarbrücken, Germany; Identification Number: 79/16).
Stimuli and tasks
The four active loudspeakers (KH120A, Neumann, Germany) were positioned at head level, 115 cm. Sounds in Experiment 1 and 2 were reproduced with a soundcard (Scarlett 18i20, Focusrite, UK). The experimental paradigms were programmed using software for scientific computing (Matlab, Mathworks, USA) and Psychtoolbox 3. In Experiment 1, sounds lasted 1.7 – 10.0 s, were delivered every 15 – 40 s, and had an average intensity of 70 dBC, except for foot steps (65 dBC). Each of the nine stimuli (lemur howling, dog barking, helicopter flying, cell phone vibrating, birds singing, baby crying, mosquito buzzing, footsteps, and traffic jam) was repeated four times (i.e., once per speaker). In Experiment 2, the stories were 5 min long, with an average intensity of 50 dBA for younger and 60 dBA for older participants. Participants answered content questions at the conclusion of each condition in this experiment.
Electrophysiological recordings and signal processing
Surface EMGs were recorded with non-recessed, Ag/AgCl electrodes (BME4, BioMed Electrodes, USA), which were 4 mm in diameter for TAM and 6 mm (BME6) in all other cases, see Figure 1. The signals were AD-converted at 9600 Hz and 24 bit resolution per channel (4 × USBamp, g.tec GmbH, Austria). Skin temperature, skin resistance, electrocardiograms, and EOGs were also recorded. All signal processing algorithms were implemented using the scientific computing software Matlab (Mathworks, USA, Version: 2018a). Because surface electrodes had not previously been used to record from intrinsic ear muscles, we conducted preliminary tests with a participant who exhibited a large, reliable Wilson’s phenomenon and who could voluntarily contract her SAM and PAM. Isolation of the corresponding responses indicated that EMG from TAM electrodes was not an artifact of volume conduction from PAM or SAM. In other words, the TAM activity was not correlated with forced SAM/PAM innervation. Sternocleidomastoid EMG signals were zero-phase bandpass filtered from 60 to 1000 Hz (FIR, 2000th order), the auricular EMG signals from 10 to 1000 Hz (FIR, 2000th order) with a notch filter at 50 Hz (IIR, 2nd order). Horizontal EOG signals were zero-phase filtered from 0.01 to 20 Hz (IIR, 2nd order). All filter operations were performed using Matlab’s filtfilt-function for zero-phase filtering. The filtered signals were then downsampled to 2400 Hz for further processing. The statistical analysis was performed using repeated measures ANOVA (with IBM SPSS Statistics 26). Within-subjects factors were stimulus-muscle correspondence (ipsi- vs. contralateral responses) and anteriority (front vs. back speakers). The only between-subjects factor our statistical model accounted for was age and, in association with that, also stimulus level in the endogenous experiment. Other factors like head-size, audiogram shape or small electrode placement differences were not included in the model. All main and interaction effects were tested.
Exogeneous (transient) data
Root-mean-square (RMS) envelopes of the filtered and downsampled EMG signals were calculated with a sliding window (step size = 1 sample, window length = 150 samples/62.5 ms.) The data were then segmented into epochs extending from 3 s prior to stimulus onset until 3 s following termination of the auditory stimulus, which was of variable duration. Epochs were baseline corrected with respect to the mean RMS envelope amplitude of the pre-stimulus interval, that is this mean was subtracted from the epoch. For every participant, normalization was performed within every monitored auricular muscle (e.g., left PAM, right SAM) and for a specific stimulus type (e.g., traffic jam). Since every stimulus type was repeated four times, the largest voltage in the corresponding four epochs was used for normalization. The reference value in each case was the largest voltage at any time point within the four relevant epochs (e.g., the four directions/trials with a traffic jam stimulus) in the EMG recordings of that particular muscle. Each participant’s data were pooled according to whether the side of the stimulus and recorded muscle did or did not match, and then were averaged into contralateral and ipsilateral waveforms. Eighteen trials contributed to each of these per subject contralateral and ipsilateral waveforms, nine from the left speaker and nine from the right. Mean amplitudes were computed across a measurement window extending from 100 to 1500 ms following stimulus onset and were then subjected to statistical analysis.
Endogenous (sustained) data
Artifacts of the filtered and downsampled EMG data were reduced by averaging the signal energy of 1 s, non-overlapping segments and rejecting segments that deviated by more than two standard deviations from the mean. For each participant, the mean energy of a given channel during the four listening conditions (left/right × front/back) was calculated and then normalized to the largest value across the 5-min run. These normalized data were then averaged into ipsilateral/contralateral categories and subjected to statistical analysis.
EMG time-frequency decomposition (for Figure 5—figure supplements 1 and 2)
As the rather sustained muscle activity during endogenous attention might be reflected in low frequency components according to convolution models of the EMG (Farina et al., 2014), filtered and downsampled EMG signals in Experiment two were decomposed into eight frequency bands by a nonsubsampled octave-band filter bank (5th order Daubechies filter). Each frequency band was then further processed in the same fashion as the broadband signals reported in the main text.
Computer vision setup and motion analysis (for Videos 1, 2 and 3)
Videos were acquired using four Ximea MQ022CG–CM color sensors with a resolution of 1936 × 1216 at 120 frames per second and an exposure of 2 ms. Two cameras were positioned on each side of the head and focused on the ears to record pairwise stereo videos. We used hardware triggering for all four cameras and recorded each camera onto a separate m.2 solid-state-drive to reduce frame loss. We used a KOWA 35 mm macro lens with an aperture of F0.4 which gave us a close-up view of the ear with acceptable depth of field to allow slight movements towards the camera and enough distance such that the cameras did not cast shadows on to the scene. We illuminated the face uniformly with flicker-free LED studio illumination. The cameras were calibrated with the stereo camera calibrator app from the Mathworks Matlab Computer Vision System Toolbox. Calibration was performed whenever camera adjustment required re-alignment of relative stereo camera positions or a change of focus of one of the cameras.
For 3D reconstruction and motion visualization/quantification, we used functions from the Mathworks Matlab Computer Vision System Toolbox and custom written code. Our analysis system was able to reduce redundancies in optic flow and stereo depth estimation by exploiting the unilateral scene composition and limited degrees of freedom for ear and head movements. For 3D reconstructions, we initialized a sequence with one initial estimation of disparity and subsequently tracked points independently for the left and right image sequence.
We tracked points with respect to the first frame of the sequence as reference frame with dense optical flow initialized with a rigid motion estimation. Motion was visualized with a Lagrangian motion magnification approach that had a constant magnification factor with respect to the reference frame and prior removal of affine motion with respect to manually selected stable points. The results of the motion analysis with and without magnification can be seen in the videos.
Acknowledgements
This study was partially supported by the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research, Grant No. BMBF-FZ 03FH004IX5 (PI: DJS). We thank Larissa Arand for assistance with data collection and Becca Sullinger for the artwork in Figure 1. We acknowledge support by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation) and Saarland University within the funding programme Open Access Publishing.
Funding Statement
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Contributor Information
Daniel J Strauss, Email: daniel.strauss@uni-saarland.de.
Jennifer M Groh, Duke University, United States.
Barbara G Shinn-Cunningham, Carnegie Mellon University, United States.
Funding Information
This paper was supported by the following grant:
Bundesministerium für Bildung und Forschung BMBF-FZ No. 03FH004IX5 to Daniel J Strauss.
Additional information
Competing interests
No competing interests declared.
Author contributions
Conceptualization, Resources, Formal analysis, Supervision, Validation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing - original draft, Project administration, Writing - review and editing.
Conceptualization, Software, Formal analysis, Supervision, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology.
Software, Formal analysis, Validation, Investigation, Visualization.
Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Validation, Investigation, Visualization.
Conceptualization, Methodology.
Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing - original draft, Writing - review and editing.
Ethics
Human subjects: The study was approved by the responsible ethics committee (ethics commission at the Ärztekammer des Saarlandes, Saarbrücken, Germany; app. number 79/16) After a detailed explanation of the procedure, all subjects signed a consent form.
Additional files
Data availability
The code with and data are available from GitHub https://github.com/a-schroeer/ExogEndogEarProject (copy archived at https://github.com/elifesciences-publications/ExogEndogEarProject) and the Dryad Digital Repository https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.d4md86r, respectively.
The following dataset was generated:
Strauss DJ, Corona-Strauss FI, Schroeer A, Flotho P, Hannemann R, Hackley SA. 2020. Data from: Vestigial auriculomotor activity indicates the direction of auditory attention in humans. Dryad Digital Repository.
References
- Alvarado M, Santibañez G. Targeting reflex: some features and inhibition targeting reflex: some features and inhibition. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis. 1971;31:33–45. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burrows AM, Diogo R, Waller BM, Bonar CJ, Liebal K. Evolution of the muscles of facial expression in a monogamous ape: evaluating the relative influences of ecological and phylogenetic factors in hylobatids. The Anatomical Record: Advances in Integrative Anatomy and Evolutionary Biology. 2011;294:645–663. doi: 10.1002/ar.21355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cattaneo L, Pavesi G. The facial motor system the facial motor system. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2014;38:135–159. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cherry EC. Some experiments on the recognition of speech, with one and with two ears. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 1953;25:975–979. doi: 10.1121/1.1907229. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Coleman MN, Ross CF. Primate auditory diversity and its influence on hearing performance primate auditory diversity and its influence on hearing performance. Anatomical Record. 2004;281A:1123–1137. doi: 10.1002/ar.a.20118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cook A, Patuzzi R. Rotation of the eyes (not the head) potentiates the postauricular muscle response. Ear and Hearing. 2014;35:230–235. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0b013e3182a4efdf. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corneil BD, Munoz DP, Chapman BB, Admans T, Cushing SL. Neuromuscular consequences of reflexive covert orienting. Nature Neuroscience. 2008;11:13–15. doi: 10.1038/nn2023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Czihak E, Santibañez M, Klimann M, Santibañez G. Audio-visual reaction after unilateral lesions of the superior colliculus in cats. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis. 1983;43:15–25. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Cheveigné A, Wong DDE, Di Liberto GM, Hjortkjær J, Slaney M, Lalor E. Decoding the auditory brain with canonical component analysis. NeuroImage. 2018;172:206–216. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.01.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farina D, Merletti R, Enoka RM. The extraction of neural strategies from the surface EMG: an update. Journal of Applied Physiology. 2014;117:1215–1230. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00162.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gerstle M, Wilkinson P. The oculo-aural movement. Journal of Neurology and Psychopathology. 1929;9:228–230. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.s1-9.35.228. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gibbs RA, Rogers J, Katze MG, Bumgarner R, Weinstock GM, Mardis ER, Remington KA, Strausberg RL, Venter JC, Wilson RK, Batzer MA, Bustamante CD, Eichler EE, Hahn MW, Hardison RC, Makova KD, Miller W, Milosavljevic A, Palermo RE, Siepel A, Sikela JM, Attaway T, Bell S, Bernard KE, Buhay CJ, Chandrabose MN, Dao M, Davis C, Delehaunty KD, Ding Y, Dinh HH, Dugan-Rocha S, Fulton LA, Gabisi RA, Garner TT, Godfrey J, Hawes AC, Hernandez J, Hines S, Holder M, Hume J, Jhangiani SN, Joshi V, Khan ZM, Kirkness EF, Cree A, Fowler RG, Lee S, Lewis LR, Li Z, Liu YS, Moore SM, Muzny D, Nazareth LV, Ngo DN, Okwuonu GO, Pai G, Parker D, Paul HA, Pfannkoch C, Pohl CS, Rogers YH, Ruiz SJ, Sabo A, Santibanez J, Schneider BW, Smith SM, Sodergren E, Svatek AF, Utterback TR, Vattathil S, Warren W, White CS, Chinwalla AT, Feng Y, Halpern AL, Hillier LW, Huang X, Minx P, Nelson JO, Pepin KH, Qin X, Sutton GG, Venter E, Walenz BP, Wallis JW, Worley KC, Yang SP, Jones SM, Marra MA, Rocchi M, Schein JE, Baertsch R, Clarke L, Csürös M, Glasscock J, Harris RA, Havlak P, Jackson AR, Jiang H, Liu Y, Messina DN, Shen Y, Song HX, Wylie T, Zhang L, Birney E, Han K, Konkel MK, Lee J, Smit AF, Ullmer B, Wang H, Xing J, Burhans R, Cheng Z, Karro JE, Ma J, Raney B, She X, Cox MJ, Demuth JP, Dumas LJ, Han SG, Hopkins J, Karimpour-Fard A, Kim YH, Pollack JR, Vinar T, Addo-Quaye C, Degenhardt J, Denby A, Hubisz MJ, Indap A, Kosiol C, Lahn BT, Lawson HA, Marklein A, Nielsen R, Vallender EJ, Clark AG, Ferguson B, Hernandez RD, Hirani K, Kehrer-Sawatzki H, Kolb J, Patil S, Pu LL, Ren Y, Smith DG, Wheeler DA, Schenck I, Ball EV, Chen R, Cooper DN, Giardine B, Hsu F, Kent WJ, Lesk A, Nelson DL, O'brien WE, Prüfer K, Stenson PD, Wallace JC, Ke H, Liu XM, Wang P, Xiang AP, Yang F, Barber GP, Haussler D, Karolchik D, Kern AD, Kuhn RM, Smith KE, Zwieg AS, Rhesus Macaque Genome Sequencing and Analysis Consortium Evolutionary and biomedical insights from the rhesus macaque genome. Science. 2007;316:222–234. doi: 10.1126/science.1139247. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gruters KG, Murphy DLK, Jenson CD, Smith DW, Shera CA, Groh JM. The eardrums move when the eyes move: a multisensory effect on the mechanics of hearing. PNAS. 2018;115:E1309–E1318. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1717948115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hackley SA, Woldorff M, Hillyard SA. Combined use of microreflexes and event-related brain potentials as measures of auditory selective attention. Psychophysiology. 1987;24:632–647. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1987.tb00343.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hackley SA. Evidence for a vestigial pinna-orienting system in humans. Psychophysiology. 2015;52:1263–1270. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12501. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hackley SA, Ren X, Underwood A, Valle-Inclán F. Prepulse inhibition and facilitation of the postauricular reflex, a vestigial remnant of pinna startle. Psychophysiology. 2017;54:566–577. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12819. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henkel CK. Afferent sources of a lateral midbrain tegmental zone associated with the pinnae in the cat as mapped by retrograde transport of horseradish peroxidase. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1981;20:213–226. doi: 10.1002/cne.902030205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henkel CK, Edwards SB. The superior colliculus control of pinna movements in the cat: possible anatomical connections. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1978;182:763–776. doi: 10.1002/cne.901820502. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hillyard SA, Hink RF, Schwent VL, Picton TW. Electrical signs of selective attention in the human brain. Science. 1973;182:177–180. doi: 10.1126/science.182.4108.177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kanold PO, Young ED. Proprioceptive information from the Pinna provides somatosensory input to cat dorsal cochlear nucleus. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2001;21:7848–7858. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-19-07848.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krauzlis RJ, Goffart L, Hafed ZM. Neuronal control of fixation and fixational eye movements. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2017;372:20160205. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0205. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lanzilotto M, Perciavalle V, Lucchetti C. A new field in monkey's frontal cortex: premotor ear-eye field (PEEF) Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2013;37:1434–1444. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.05.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lovell M, Sutton D, Lindeman RC. Muscle spindles in nonhuman primate extrinsic auricular muscles. The Anatomical Record. 1977;189:519–523. doi: 10.1002/ar.1091890310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Beirne GA, Patuzzi RB. Basic properties of the sound-evoked post-auricular muscle response (PAMR) Hearing Research. 1999;138:115–132. doi: 10.1016/S0378-5955(99)00159-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patuzzi RB, O'Beirne GA. Effects of eye rotation on the sound-evoked post-auricular muscle response (PAMR) Hearing Research. 1999;138:133–146. doi: 10.1016/S0378-5955(99)00160-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perrot X, Ryvlin P, Isnard J, Guénot M, Catenoix H, Fischer C, Mauguière F, Collet L. Evidence for corticofugal modulation of peripheral auditory activity in humans. Cerebral Cortex. 2006;16:941–948. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Populin LC, Yin TCT. Sensitivity of auditory cells in the superior colliculus to eye position in the behaving cat. In: Palmer A. R, Rees A, Summerfield A, Meddis R, editors. Psychophysical and Physiological Advances in Hearing. London: Whurr; 1997. pp. 441–448. [Google Scholar]
- Populin LC, Yin TC. Pinna movements of the cat during sound localization. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1998;18:4233–4243. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-11-04233.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schäfer PJ, Corona-Strauss FI, Hannemann R, Hillyard SA, Strauss DJ. Testing the limits of the stimulus reconstruction approach: auditory attention decoding in a Four-Speaker free field environment. Trends in Hearing. 2018;22:233121651881660. doi: 10.1177/2331216518816600. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Siegmund H, Santibáñez G. Effector pattern of the audio-visual targeting reflex in cats. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis. 1982;42:311–326. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stein BE, Clamann HP. Control of pinna movements and sensorimotor register in cat superior colliculus. Brain, Behavior and Evolution. 1981;19:180–192. doi: 10.1159/000121641. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stekelenburg JJ, van Boxtel A. Pericranial muscular, respiratory, and heart rate components of the orienting response. Psychophysiology. 2002;39:707–722. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3960707. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takeuchi Y, Nakano K, Uemura M, Matsuda K, Matsushima R, Mizuno N. Mesencephalic and pontine afferent fiber system to the facial neucleus in the cat: a study using the horseradish peroxidase and silver impregnation techniques. Experimental Neurology. 1979;66:330–342. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(79)90084-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tollin DJ, Ruhland JL, Yin TC. The vestibulo-auricular reflex. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2009;101:1258–1266. doi: 10.1152/jn.90977.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Urban PP, Marczynski U, Hopf HC. The oculo-auricular phenomenon: findings in normals and patients with brainstem lesions. Brain : A Journal of Neurology. 1993;116 :727–738. doi: 10.1093/brain/116.3.727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Ede F, Chekroud SR, Nobre AC. Human gaze tracks attentional focusing in memorized visual space. Nature Human Behaviour. 2019;3:462–470. doi: 10.1038/s41562-019-0549-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vidal PP, May PJ, Baker R. Synaptic organization of the tectal-facial pathways in the cat. I. synaptic potentials following collicular stimulation. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1988;60:769–797. doi: 10.1152/jn.1988.60.2.769. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waller BM, Parr LA, Gothard KM, Burrows AM, Fuglevand AJ. Mapping the contribution of single muscles to facial movements in the rhesus macaque. Physiology & Behavior. 2008;95:93–100. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.05.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson SAK. A note on an associated movement of the eyes and ears in man. Review of Neurology and Psychiatry. 1908;6:331–336. [Google Scholar]