Significance
Resting-state functional connectivity provides novel insight into variations in neural networks associated with addiction to stimulant drugs in individuals with and without a family history of addiction, and both with and without personal drug use. An increased risk for addiction, either because of drug use or genetic/psychosocial vulnerability, is associated with hypoconnectivity in frontostriatal networks, which may weaken goal-directed decision-making. Resilience against addiction development, by contrast, is characterized by hyperconnectivity in two corticostriatal pathways, possibly reflecting compensatory responses in networks associated with regulatory control over habitual behaviors. It is thus conceivable that defying the risk of developing stimulant drug addiction requires increased efforts to control behavior—a hypothesis that may open up new pathways for therapeutic and preventative strategies.
Keywords: functional connectivity, cocaine, vulnerability, resilience, fMRI
Abstract
Regular drug use can lead to addiction, but not everyone who takes drugs makes this transition. How exactly drugs of abuse interact with individual vulnerability is not fully understood, nor is it clear how individuals defy the risks associated with drugs or addiction vulnerability. We used resting-state functional MRI (fMRI) in 162 participants to characterize risk- and resilience-related changes in corticostriatal functional circuits in individuals exposed to stimulant drugs both with and without clinically diagnosed drug addiction, siblings of addicted individuals, and control volunteers. The likelihood of developing addiction, whether due to familial vulnerability or drug use, was associated with significant hypoconnectivity in orbitofrontal and ventromedial prefrontal cortical-striatal circuits—pathways critically implicated in goal-directed decision-making. By contrast, resilience against a diagnosis of substance use disorder was associated with hyperconnectivity in two networks involving 1) the lateral prefrontal cortex and medial caudate nucleus and 2) the supplementary motor area, superior medial frontal cortex, and putamen—brain circuits respectively implicated in top-down inhibitory control and the regulation of habits. These findings point toward a predisposing vulnerability in the causation of addiction, related to impaired goal-directed actions, as well as countervailing resilience systems implicated in behavioral regulation, and may inform novel strategies for therapeutic and preventative interventions.
The use of addictive drugs is a growing global public health problem. Drug addiction develops with persistent drug use, and individuals who are vulnerable to developing addiction may do so as quickly as within their first year of use (1). Improving our understanding of the brain systems that are implicated in resilience to developing addiction, or “substance use disorder” according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual DSM-5 (2), would offer new opportunities for its prevention and treatment, particularly in young people at risk for addiction.
Over the past decade, a large body of work has elucidated how addictive drugs affect brain function, specifically that of different striatal subsystems that have been critically implicated in the development of addictive behavior (3). Importantly, not everyone who uses drugs, even highly addictive drugs such as amphetamines or cocaine, makes the transition to addiction (1). A family history of drug addiction has been shown to confer an eightfold increased risk of developing addiction (4), and this likely relates to a combination of genetic (5) and environmental (6) risk factors. However, relatively little is known about factors that protect people from developing addiction: i.e., confer resilience. The term “resilience” refers to protective factors that help individuals to successfully cope with or overcome exposure to significant risk, adversity, or potentially harmful environments (7). These factors may include personality traits and attitudes, supportive environments (8), and neural systems that are more robust to, or able to compensate for, adverse exposures (9). Much of the neuroscientific research on resilience has been examined in adolescence (10), which is a period of heightened vulnerability and neural plasticity (11). While studies of adolescents are important to inform the development and implementation of preventative strategies, we also need to understand how resilience operates in vulnerable adults as the compensatory mechanisms that have been characterized in adolescents do not necessarily translate into adulthood. For example, the cognitive deficits frequently measured in adolescents with a family history of addiction (12, 13) are not seen in adults with familial risk (14, 15), which may suggest that adults manage to counteract their vulnerabilities more effectively than adolescents by recruiting compensatory brain systems (16–18). Understanding how potentially vulnerable adults defy the risk of addiction may inform the development of more effective treatment strategies for individuals affected by the disorder.
A more compelling illustration of resilience to drug addiction is demonstrated by individuals who regularly use highly addictive drugs like cocaine but do not succumb to addiction and maintain successful lives (19). These individuals have been described as highly intelligent (20), well-educated, affluent, and socially well-integrated (21). They use drugs in a manner that avoids conflict with their private and professional commitments (21). These recreational users are less visible and thus less well-studied than their addicted counterparts, who already have a diagnosis of substance use disorder and are generally recruited through referrals from treatment services. The available evidence, however, indicates that the recreational users function at a comparable level to control participants by recruiting compensatory brain systems, which appears to buffer the impact of their drug use (22, 23). It is thus conceivable that this ability to recruit compensatory brain systems represents a resilience factor to developing drug addiction. However, to date, very little is known about the mechanisms by which drugs of abuse interact with addiction vulnerability.
Alteration of dopamine signaling in the basal ganglia is thought to be involved in the development of certain substance-related addictions (24). There is considerable evidence that addiction develops through the progressive engagement of different striatal subsystems along a ventral-to-dorsal gradient (25, 26) via associative learning processes implicated in habit formation. Habits typically arise when goal-directed behaviors become autonomous of the goal through repetition. Drugs of abuse may foster this transition, precipitating the development of compulsive behavior possibly by nonmutually exclusive mechanisms: 1) Chronic exposure to drugs of abuse (contrary to natural rewards) compromises the neural system for goal-directed behavior (orbitofrontal cortex [OFC] and ventromedial prefrontal cortex [vmPFC]), thereby biasing decisions toward drug-related rewards and shifting behavioral control toward the habit system; 2) regular drug use alters the dopamine system and facilitates the ventral-to-dorsal striatal transition from goal-directed to habitual instrumental performance, and 3) chronic drug use weakens cognitive control—the flexible regulation of behavior between goal-directed and habitual actions, which intervenes whenever behavior becomes maladaptive. Drug-induced changes affect interactions between the inferior lateral prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, and striatum (27).
We posited that addiction risk and resilience would be associated with critical changes in the functioning of these corticostriatal systems, and we therefore investigated resting-state corticostriatal connectivity in individuals with familial risk for addiction, or those engaging in regular drug use, and the interaction of these factors. As addiction develops in combination of drug use and predisposing factors, we hypothesized that familial characteristics (as defined by risk factors shared by family members) in interaction with drug-related effects are critical for the transition to addiction in at-risk individuals. We deliberately focused on stimulant drugs because of their direct effect on the midbrain dopamine system that modulates corticostriatal function, resulting in their high addiction liability. We collected resting-state brain activity in individuals with familial risk of addiction, of whom half were addicted to stimulant drugs, and the other half were their unaffected biological siblings. We compared these participants with individuals with no family history of addiction, of whom half had a personal history of regular stimulant drug use (but not a formal diagnosis of addiction), while the other half did not. Our specific hypotheses were that an increased vulnerability for addiction would be reflected in functional dysconnectivity in frontostriatal pathways implicated in goal-directed behavior and prefrontal control (28) and that resilience to developing addiction would be associated with compensatory responses within the goal-directed system or dorsal-striatal subsystems implicated in habit regulation (29).
Results
Group characteristics are shown in Table 1. Three of the six striatal seed regions, the ventromedial caudate (vmCAU), ventrolateral putamen (vlPUT), and posterior/dorsolateral putamen (pdlPUT) revealed significant group differences in resting-state functional connectivity (RSFC) within distinct anatomical pathways (Table 2). The topographies of the identified networks are consistent with prior work by DiMartino et al. (30) and the widely described corticostriatal circuitries (31) (Fig. 1).
Table 1.
No familial risk | Familial risk | Familial risk vs. no fam. risk | Drug use vs. no drug use | |||||
No stimulant use | Stimulant use | No stimulant use | Stimulant use | |||||
Demographics | Mean (±SD) | Mean (±SD) | Mean (±SD) | Mean (±SD) | F, t, or χ2 P | F, t, or χ2 P | ||
Number | 48 | 25 | 47 | 42 | 162 | 162 | ||
Gender, % male | 63% | 48% | 51% | 95% | 3.7 | 0.056 | 7.5 | 0.006 |
Handedness, % right | 85% | 84% | 89% | 81% | 0.01 | 0.934 | 0.9 | 0.352 |
Ethnicity, white: black: Asian: mixed | 48: 0: 0: 0 | 25: 0: 0 | 36: 4: 2: 5 | 32: 3: 2: 5 | 20.7 | 0.001 | 0.8 | 0.560 |
Age, y | 32.4 (±8.8) | 28.4 (±6.7) | 32.2 (±8.2) | 34.7 (±7.7) | 2.7 | 0.103 | 0.1 | 0.797 |
Body mass index | 25.2 (±3.3) | 24.8 (±4.2) | 26.3 (±5.6) | 24.7 (±3.9) | 0.6 | 0.451 | 2.3 | 0.127 |
Duration of formal education, y | 12.7 (±1.9) | 13.4 (±1.8) | 12.2 (±2.0) | 11.6 (±1.6) | 12.2 | 0.001 | 0.3 | 0.586 |
Verbal intelligence, NART score | 112.6 (±7.9) | 116.2 (±5.2) | 108.6 (±8.9) | 110.4 (±7.1) | 14.2 | <0.001 | 1.5 | 0.216 |
Childhood adversity, CTQ abuse | 17.5 (±5.4) | 18.5 (±3.9) | 24.3 (±10.8) | 27.0 (±11.9) | 28.5 | <0.001 | 32 | 0.077 |
Community Integration, CIQ-II score | 17.9 (±3.2) | 18.3 (±2.9) | 17.6 (±3.4) | 14.4 (±3.9) | 9.8 | 0.002 | 7.1 | 0.009 |
Disposable income, £ per month | 663.8 (±938.3) | 710.4 (±1149.7) | 403.8 (±4.13.5) | 415.2 (±667.1) | 5.0 | 0.026 | 0.02 | 0.883 |
Alcohol consumption, AUDIT score | 3.2 (±2.3) | 5.7 (±1.6) | 4.0 (±4.6) | 11.3 (±10.9) | 7.0 | 0.009 | 23.7 | <0.001 |
Tobacco, % never/current/past | 43/15/42 | 12/72/16 | 4/58/38 | 2/93/5 | 36.8 | <0.001 | 36.9 | <0.001 |
Cannabis, % never/current/past | 79/0/21 | 0/44/56 | 26/17/57 | 0/69/31 | 30.3 | <0.001 | 70.1 | <0.001 |
Age of onset tobacco use, y | 16.2 (±2.8) | 16.1 (±4.0) | 14.5 (±2.1) | 13.1 (±3.0) | 19.6 | <0.001 | 2.8 | 0.094 |
Age of onset cannabis use, y | 17.9 (±4.2) | 16.6 (±2.4) | 17.8 (±4.3) | 15.0 (±2.8) | 0.8 | 0.367 | 10.7 | <0.001 |
Age of onset stimulant drug use, y | – | 19.0 (±2.7) | – | 16.4 (±2.5) | 4.1 | <0.001 |
The term “stimulant use” does not reflect severity and includes both recreational and addictive use. Verbal intelligence estimated by the National Adult Reading Test (NART), childhood adversity assessed by the abuse score of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ), community integration was evaluated from the Community Integration Questionnaire Version 2 (CIQ-II), and alcohol consumption assessed by the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT).
Table 2.
Seed | Effect | Significant cluster | No. of voxels | Peak MNI coordinates | Brodmann areas |
Ventromedial caudate | Familial Risk | OFC, mPFC, rostral ACC, | 143 | −9, 39, −9 | 11 |
mPFC | 21 | 9, 36, −15 | 11 | ||
mPFC | 19 | −6, 66, 6 | 10 | ||
Stimulant Use | PCC | 10 | −3, −24, 42 | 23, 31 | |
Interaction | IFG, middle frontal gyrus | 78 | 42, 36, 6 | 45, 46 | |
Ventrolateral putamen | Stimulant Use | Insula | 126 | −39, 6, −3 | 48 |
Supramarginal gyrus | 79 | 57, −30, 42 | 40 | ||
Supramarginal gyrus | 57 | −57, −42, 36 | 39, 40 | ||
Interaction | ACC | 2 | 9, 30, 18 | 32, 24 | |
Dorsolateral putamen (posterior) | Interaction | Central opercular cortex | 35 | 57, 3, 9 | 48, 6 |
Superior medial frontal cortex, SMA, cingulate gyrus (middle), insula | 10 | 0, 12, 45 | 6, 32 | ||
6 | 15, 9, 39 | 32, 24 | |||
3 | 39, 6, 6 | 13 |
ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; IFG, inferior frontal gyrus; mPFC, medial prefrontal cortex; OFC, medial and lateral parts of the orbitofrontal cortex; PCC, posterior cingulate cortex; SMA, supplementary motor area.
Vulnerability-Related Functional Dysconnectivity.
Familial risk of addiction was associated with significantly reduced functional connectivity in frontostriatal pathways between the vmCAU seed with the rostral anterior cingulate cortex (rACC), medial, though mainly lateral, OFC, and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC). These connections survived cluster-level correction (within the whole brain) and (for extracted scores) post hoc statistical correction using a mixed-effect model (β = −0.189, P = 0.045). As shown in Fig. 2A, the group differences were mainly left-lateralized, and participants with both familial risk and stimulant use (F+S+) showed the greatest reduction in RSFC compared with all of the other groups; participants with familial risk but without stimulant drug use (F+S−) fell midway between F+S+ and F−S− groups. Thus, stimulant use interacts with familial risk further to reduce functional connectivity of cortical (rACC, mPFC, OFC) regions with the vmCAU.
Stimulant use (i.e., whether in recreational users or diagnosed stimulant use disorder) was related to hypoconnectivity in two left-lateralized networks associated with the vmCAU or vlPUT seeds (Fig. 2B). Compared with nonusers (S−), stimulant users (S+) showed reduced RSFC between 1) vmCAU and the posterior cingulate gyrus (β = −0.231, P = 0.194) and 2) the vlPUT, the insula and the supramarginal gyrus, but only the latter survived post hoc correction (β = −0.299, P = 0.045).
The sibling pairs differed significantly from the other two groups in terms of education, income, and age of tobacco use onset (Table 1)—variables that have been considered as preexisting individual and family characteristics which contribute to chronic drug use in adulthood (32, 33). When we added these three variables in our mixed-effect model, the effect of familiality was no longer significant (β = −0.114, P = 0.274) while the effect of stimulant use survived (β = −0.366, P = 0.020), supporting the notion that socioeconomic factors play a critical part in the addiction vulnerability that family members share.
Interaction of Familial and Stimulant-Related Risks.
Significant interaction effects were observed for three of the six seeds: vmCAU vlPUT, and pdlPUT (Table 2). Individuals who had not developed addiction despite familial risk (F+S−) or regular stimulant use (F−S+), showed hyperconnectivity in key networks compared with those having both (F+S+) or none (F−S−). Both of these groups (F+S−, F−S+) can be described as “resilient.” As shown in Fig. 3, both groups showed significant functional hyperconnectivity between 1) vmCAU and the right inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and middle frontal gyrus (Brodmann areas [BA] 32 and 6) (β = −0.740, P < 0.001), as well as between 2) pdlPUT and several structures, including the central opercular cortex, supplementary motor area (SMA), medial superior frontal cortex, middle cingulate gyrus, and insula cortex (β = −0.482, P = 0.020). The significant interaction effects related to the vmCAU and the posterior dlPUT pathways, respectively, were right-lateralized and significantly correlated with one another in the entire sample (r = 0.31, P < 0.001). However, this correlation was only evident in individuals who did not use stimulant drugs (F−S−: r = 0.31, P = 0.030; F+S−: r = 0.37, P = 0.010), and not in stimulant-using participants (F−S+, F+S+). The lack of this relationship in stimulant-using participants (t = 1.43, P = 0.157) was partly mediated by their pattern of compulsive stimulant drug use, as measured by the obsessive-compulsive drug use scale (OCDUS) (34) (R2 = 0.38, F1,65 = 10.97, P = 0.002), suggesting that 1) compulsive stimulant use disrupted the interplay between these two regulatory networks and 2) that the corticostriatal hyperconnectivity in the “resilient” group contributed to their protection from compulsive drug use. The inclusion of education, income, and age of tobacco onset as covariates in the mixed-effect model did not change the significance of the results (vmCAU-IFG β = −0.725, P < 0001 and pdlPUT-SMA β = −0.457, P = 0.033).
Discussion
Changes in corticostriatal circuits have been suggested to underlie both the development and the phenotype of drug addiction, possibly driven by distinct mechanisms. Using seed-based correlational analysis in corticostriatal systems in four groups (individuals exposed to stimulant drugs, both with and without clinically diagnosed drug addiction, the siblings of addicted individuals, and healthy control volunteers), we identified distinct, lateralized abnormalities in functional connectivity strength associated with both increased risk of, and resilience to, developing stimulant addiction. The risk of developing addiction (whether due to familial vulnerability or a diagnosis of stimulant use disorder) was associated with significant reduction in functional connectivity (hypoconnectivity) between vmCAU, left OFC, and vmPFC, regions that have been critically implicated in flexible, goal-directed, value-based decision-making (35). Resilience against the risk of developing addiction (either because drug use was not initiated despite increased genetic or social/environmental risk [unaffected siblings] or regular drug use had not transitioned into addiction [recreational stimulant users]) was associated with hyperconnectivity in two regulatory control networks: One network involved the right lateral prefrontal cortex (lPFC) (including the right IFG) and its connections to the vmCAU. This network is implicated in regulating selected actions (36) by down-regulating habitual control over behavior (37). The lPFC should be distinguished from the vmPFC, which is more associated with the representation of goals (28) and exhibits hypoconnectivity in participants with familial risk. The other network involved is the dorsal putamen-SMA, which is critically implicated in sensorimotor function and the transition from goal-directed to habitual control over behavior (38, 39). Resilience thus appears to be associated with hyperconnectivity between different sectors of the prefrontal cortex and the striatum, presumably maintaining regulation over goal-directed behavior and its transition to habitual control.
Risk of Addiction Is Associated with Hypoconnectivity in vmPFC and Striatal Pathways.
As shown in Fig. 2A, familial risk of developing addiction was associated with a significant reduction in functional connectivity within a vmPFC/OFC-striatal network encompassing the vmCAU, mPFC, and adjacent medial and lateral OFC and rACC regions. Hypoconnectivity has generally been associated with functional decline and cognitive impairment, which, in light of the present findings, points toward a weakness in the vmPFC, goal-directed system, possibly biasing behavior toward habitual control. Preclinical studies have shown that lesions and/or pharmacological manipulations of this pathway reduce sensitivity to outcome value and facilitate habit formation (40–42). Indeed, reduced task-related activation in this pathway has repeatedly been reported in drug-addicted adults during maladaptive decision-making (43–45), which may exemplify choices that were not controlled by their consequences. As hypoconnectivity in the goal-directed system is also seen in unaffected siblings, it is possible that this functional weakness may have predated drug-taking, rendering individuals vulnerable to developing drug addiction upon initiation of stimulant drug use. Regular stimulant drug use has been shown to correlate with a reduction of gray matter in the vmPFC (46), suggesting that the likely preexisting underactivity of this system has been exacerbated by chronic stimulant drug use.
The amygdala is also important for modulating both goal-directed and habitual behavior in addiction, based on several animal-based studies (47). However, examining the connectivity of the vmPFC with the amygdala was outside the scope of our seed-based analysis that was focused solely on the striatum.
Importantly, structures implicated in motivational control over behavior, such as the nucleus accumbens, did not reveal abnormalities in functional connectivity. This is consistent with previous research showing normal activation in the ventral striatum during reward anticipation in stimulant-addicted adults (48, 49) and their unaffected siblings (17). Our data thus further confirm the notion that addiction vulnerability is reflected more by dysconnectivity in the network that underlies deliberate action selection rather than the motivational priming of actions.
Stimulant Use Is Associated with Hypoconnectivity between Ventral Putamen and Insula.
We also found that regular use of stimulant drugs was associated with reduced functional connectivity between the vlPUT, insula (both anterior and posterior parts), and the supramarginal gyrus (Fig. 2B), a network associated with emotional awareness (50) and habituation to either pleasant (51) or painful stimuli (52). Hypoconnectivity in this network has previously been reported in cocaine-addicted adults with a high risk for relapse (53). In light of cocaine’s ability to acutely increase activity in this network (54, 55), the reduced functional coupling in regular stimulant drug users might reflect tolerance to the effects of the drug. Reduced functional connectivity between the vlPUT and the insula further supports the notion of compromised interoception in stimulant addiction (56, 57), which may impair intuitive decision-making (58). As bodily feedback has been shown to influence cognitive–affective processes (59) and guide individuals’ choices (60, 61), it could be speculated that reduced functional connectivity in this pathway may in part account for the lack of avoidance responses to aversive events (62, 63) frequently observed in chronic stimulant users and may contribute to the development of compulsive drug-seeking.
Resilience Against Addiction Is Associated with Hyperconnectivity in Corticostriatal Pathways.
Hyperconnectivity in two regulatory control pathways in resilient individuals may seem paradoxical as these individuals do not meet the criteria for addiction and typically perform within the normal range. However, hyperconnectivity at rest is not thought to be associated with dysfunction but instead to reflect compensatory efforts to meet increased functional demands (64–66). This may explain why unaffected first-degree relatives of addicted individuals show significant overactivation in the lPFC network during tasks of response inhibition (16, 67) and interference control (22, 68) for which hyperconnectivity at rest may be enabling. In this study, addicted individuals (F+S+), whose reduced activation in this network has previously been associated with widespread impairments in a range of executive control tasks (69), show equally strong resting-state connectivity in these networks as their nondrug using peers (F−S−). This may suggest that their regulatory networks are already at maximal capacity compensating for the executive control deficits typically seen in addiction (27). Likewise, the connectivity strength of the putamen-sensorimotor-cortical circuit was not measurably different between addicted individuals and control volunteers. This may seem surprising in light of prior evidence suggesting that stimulant-addicted individuals rely on bilateral recruitment of the putamen-sensorimotor-cortical pathway during simple sensorimotor task performance while, for nondrug using controls, contralateral recruitment suffices (70). Given that the putamen-sensorimotor-cortical network is critically implicated in habit learning, it is tempting to speculate whether bilateral recruitment facilitates the development of pathological habits.
Our data suggest that addiction vulnerability is associated with increased demands on regulatory control systems, to which resilient individuals respond with hyperconnectivity. Compromised control is a hallmark of addiction, and this may not be solely reflected in the connectivity strength of the goal-directed and habit system, but by their interactions. Our data suggest that regular stimulant drug use disrupts the coupling between these two regulatory networks, and the degree of this disruption is partly determined by the severity of compulsive drug use, as measured by the OCDUS scale. Conceivably, hyperconnectivity of these two circuitries in the nonaddicted stimulant drug users might prevent their drug use from developing into a compulsive habit.
A second “top-down” system exerting inhibitory control over goal-directed actions between the dorsal anterior cingulate and the vlPUT (68) was also shown to be hyperconnected in resilient individuals (Table 2), but this effect did not survive post hoc correction. This is noteworthy because the dorsal anterior cingulate has been termed an automatic alarm system (71), in support of habit regulation (72, 73), which would support the notion of resilience being associated with increased strength of regulation over both goal-directed and habitual behaviors.
Strengths, Weaknesses, and Implications.
Examining functional connectivity within corticostriatal networks across individuals with and without familial risk and stimulant drug use offers a unique opportunity for delineating differences in network function that may underlie vulnerability and resilience to addiction. By including groups in the study that are understudied in addiction, such as individuals with a family history of addiction and recreational drug users, we provide a different perspective on the current addiction literature (74). Most striking is the observation that we did not find differences between the groups in networks associated with the nucleus accumbens, which modulates the reinforcing effects of stimulant drugs. Instead, differences were only seen in networks involved in decision-making (goal-directed and interoceptive decision-making) and behavioral control (anterior cingulate and prefrontal inhibitory control over striatal outflow).
There are, however, several other factors that we have not examined, such as genetic influences, drug use severity, or the concomitant use of other drugs. We therefore want to emphasize that our main aim was not to ascertain the effects of specific drugs on brain connectivity, which has already been the subject of prior work, but to identify the pattern of abnormality associated with drug addiction and addiction vulnerability more generally. We therefore employed a study design in which the regular use of several drugs was restricted to the group that met criteria for addiction and less severe drug use was characteristic of the nonaddicted drug user group. However, all drug-using participants regularly used stimulant drugs, which directly affect the function of dopaminergic striatal pathways.
We acknowledge that a major limitation is that relevant behavioral data are not available to endorse our conclusions made about the functioning of the relevant neural circuitries based on our citation of the extensive functional neuroimaging literature that already exists for both healthy individuals and stimulant drug users. Of course, caution should also be used when interpreting these data because task-induced blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) activation and spontaneous BOLD activation during resting-state functional MRI (fMRI) represent different neuronal processes and are therefore not directly comparable (75).
Although our sample size of 162 was reasonable, the group sizes were unbalanced, and there was relatedness between the familiality (F+) but not the control groups (F−). These limitations are largely due to practical reasons of recruitment, but we have statistically controlled for them and did not include these covariates in the main model to avoid overfitting. We also acknowledge the limitation that participants with familial risk were related whereas participants who were using stimulants were not. We statistically accounted for their relationship by using a mixed-effect model in which we considered clustering of siblings within family as a random effect.
Overall exposure to drugs differs between the addicted and nonaddicted drug users, as would be expected. It also raises the important question whether nonaddicted users may only be at an earlier stage in the addiction cycle or represent an entirely different phenotype. We argue for the latter, suggesting that these nonaddicted drug users show a number of characteristics that render them less vulnerable to addiction, including less compulsive drug use, later onset of drug use (including stimulant drug use), higher socioeconomic status, and the absence of both familial risk and childhood adversity (46). More research is therefore warranted to test the hypothesis that the characteristics seen in this nonaddictive phenotype offer protection against the development of addiction in people who use drugs.
The networks we identified using the striatal seeds are strikingly consistent with previously published research using the same seeds in healthy individuals (30), and, while the absence of a significant role of networks involving the nucleus accumbens was surprising, they otherwise concur with certain contemporary neural models of addiction (26, 76). Our findings are also consistent with previous resting-state studies in addicted individuals reporting reduced functional connectivity in prefrontal networks (77–79), but direct comparisons with prior work are difficult because of differences in the analyses [i.e., the analysis method (80, 81) or location of the seeds (82, 83)] and the factorial design of this study.
Conclusion
This study of functional resting-state connectivity in 162 participants examined two key contributors to the neural changes associated with familial risk, assessed using a group of sibling pairs of whom one was stimulant-addicted while the other was unaffected, and stimulant drug use, assessed not only in Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edition, Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR)–diagnosed stimulant-addicted individuals (84), but also in control participants regularly using stimulants recreationally. This innovative design not only yielded markers of familial risk and resilience but also revealed their interactions. Familial vulnerability for drug addiction is associated with reduced functional connectivity in networks implicated in instrumental learning and goal-directed decision-making. Stimulant drugs weaken goal-directed control even further via hypoconnectivity in pathways implicated in interoception and negative feedback processing, which may increase the risk of behavior becoming compulsive, even in the face of adverse consequences. Resilient individuals counteract the drive to addiction through hyperconnectivity in two distinct corticostriatal pathways that have been associated with top-down goal-directed inhibitory and automatic–habitual control of behavior, respectively. The present study therefore provides valuable insight into possible interactions between familial risk and stimulant drug use for the regulation of behavioral control and potential disruptions that may occur in the development of addiction, but more research is clearly warranted into the neural basis of the changes in functional connectivity to inform the development of more effective strategies for preventative and therapeutic interventions of addiction.
Materials and Methods
Participants and Procedures.
A total of 179 participants were recruited for this study (full details described elsewhere) (46). Briefly, the sample included 50 sibling pairs and 79 healthy control volunteers. The sibling pairs comprised full biological siblings of whom one met the DSM-IV-TR (84) criteria for dependence on cocaine or amphetamines, who have been using stimulant drugs for an average of 16.3 y (±6.7 SD), while the other had no personal history of addiction. Of the 79 healthy control volunteers, none had a family history of addiction, but 27 had been using stimulant drugs regularly for at least 2 y (on average, 8.0 y [±6.1 SD]) whereas the remaining 52 reported no prior experience with drugs (including stimulants). The level of stimulant drug use differed significantly between the two drug-using groups both in terms of the duration of stimulant drug use as well as the compulsive pattern of use. Addicted individuals had used stimulant drugs for a longer duration (t = −5.1, P < 0.001) and in a highly compulsive manner (t = −16.0, p < 0.001), as reflected by the OCDUS score (mean 24.4 [±9.2]) whereas the nonaddicted users without a family history of addiction did not (mean 1.2 [±1.7]). Exclusion criteria were a lifetime history of a psychiatric, neurological, or neurodevelopmental disorder, traumatic head injury, and stimulant use for medical reasons.
All participants followed the same protocol, as described elsewhere (46). They consented in writing before undergoing medical review and psychiatric screening, which involved a psychiatric assessment using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR (85), ascertainment of family history of addiction by a semistructured interview, evaluation of childhood adversity (common in families affected by addiction) (86) using the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ) (87), and social integration (which may play a role in resilience) (88, 89) using the Community Integration Questionnaire Version 2 (CIQ-II) (90). Drug and alcohol use was quantified using the Drug Abuse Screening Test (DAST-20) (91) and the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT) (92), both of which have demonstrated sensitivity in nonclinical populations. All stimulant drug-using participants completed the OCDUS (34) to assess the degree of how compulsively they used the drug. All addicted individuals (except two) provided a stimulant-positive urine sample; urine samples provided by all other participants were negative. The study protocol was approved by the National Research Ethics Committee (NREC08/H0308/310) and written informed consent was obtained from all participants before study enrollment.
Acquisition and Preprocessing of Neuroimaging Data.
All participants underwent magnetic resonance brain scans at the Wolfson Brain Imaging Centre, University of Cambridge, United Kingdom, using a Siemens TIM-Trio 3T system with an eight-channel radio frequency head coil array. Participants were instructed to keep their eyes closed, relax, think of nothing particular, and lie still in the scanner during the resting-state fMRI scan. The fMRI data including 261 volumes were acquired with a T2*-weighted echo planar imaging sequence (echo time [TE] = 30 ms, repetition time [TR] = 2,000 ms, flip angle = 78°, within-plane matrix = 64 × 64, field of view [FOV] = 192 × 192 mm, 32 axial-slices with 3-mm thickness and 0.75-mm interslice gap). Whole brain T1-weighted structural images were acquired with a magnetization-prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo sequence (TE = 2.98 ms, TR = 2,300 ms, inversion time [TI] = 900 ms, flip angle = 9°, FOV = 240 × 256 mm, 176 sagittal-slices of 1-mm thickness, isotropic 1-mm3 voxels). All scans were screened for abnormal radiological appearance by a specialist in neuroradiology. Neuroimaging data of five participants were unavailable and had to be excluded for 12 owing to poor scanning quality, resulting in a final sample of 162 participants.
Resting-State fMRI Preprocessing.
The resting-state fMRI data were preprocessed using the FMRIB Software Library (FSL) (version 5.0.9, https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/). The first six volumes were discarded to ensure longitudinal magnetization reaches a steady state, resulting in 255 volumes for each participant. We applied motion correction, removed nonbrain structures and signals related to 10% background noise, performed intensity normalization with the grand mean = 1,000 and spatial smoothing with a 6-mm full width at half maximum Gaussian Kernel (using FSL’s SUSAN). Head motion was evaluated by framewise displacement (FD) (93, 94). Four participants were excluded due to excessive head motion (mean FD >0.5 and over 40% volumes were contaminated with FD >0.5). For the remaining participants, signal-based motion noise removal was further applied to reduce the temporal impact of head motion on resting-state fMRI measures (95). The independent component analysis automatic removal of motion artifacts (ICA-AROMA) is an automated approach combining independent component analysis and machine learning for removing motion artifacts from fMRI signals, which has been shown effective for reducing confounding effects on resting-state functional connectivity and increasing reproducibility of resting-state network identification (96). In this study, we used FSL’s ICA tool (MELODIC) to estimate 100 spatial components for each participant’s fMRI data, and then ICA-AROMA for motion noise classification and removal. Voxel-wise regression was then conducted for nuisance variables, including linear and quadratic trends, average white matter, and cerebrospinal signals of their eroded masks based on individual structural image segmentation by FSL’s FAST. Subsequently, low frequency nonneuronal signals were removed from voxel-wise time series by high-pass filtering (>0.01 Hz) (96). Finally, we denoised and normalized the data from native to standard space using three-step registration. We first aligned the mean functional image to participants’ T1 structural image using boundary-based registration. We then registered the T1 image to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) 152 structural template using linear and nonlinear registration. Finally, we wrapped the whole fMRI data into standard MNI space by combining the above registrations and resampled to isotropic 3-mm voxels with trilinear interpolation.
Functional Connectivity Analysis and Group Comparisons.
The RSFC estimates were computed using a seed-based approach in this study (97). In light of our prior work, which identified abnormalities in striatal structure associated with stimulant drug addiction (46, 98), we selected six bilateral striatal seed regions that relate to well-defined frontostriatal and corticostriatal networks (30), shown in Fig. 1: ventral striatum (nucleus accumbens: ±9,9,−8), vmCAU (±10,15,0), dorsomedial caudate (dmCAU) (±13,15,9), pdlPUT (±28,1,3), dlPUT (±25,8,6), and vlPUT (±20,12,−3). We generated the seeds with a 3.5-mm-radius spherical regions of interest (MNI coordinates described by ref. 30). The average preprocessed fMRI time series were extracted for each seed and then regressed against other voxels’ time series over the whole brain (using FSL’s FSL_GLM with time series normalized to the unit SD beforehand), which produced a statistical parametric map in terms of Pearson’s correlation—RSFC-r map. The Fisher’s r-to-z transformation was conducted, resulting in an RSFC-z map for each seed and each participant, as the final RSFC outcome measure.
Second-Level Statistical Analysis.
To test the effects of stimulant use and familiality as well as their interaction on RSFC, RSFC-z maps were entered into the two factorial analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) model as previously described (17) to assess main effects of familiality and stimulant use as well as their interaction on the functional connectivity network of striatal subregions. As stimulant drugs have been shown to affect men and women differentially (99), we controlled for gender as the covariate of no interest. As differential effects have to the best of our knowledge not been found with respect to ethnicity, we did not control for it. The design matrix was generated by FSL’s GLM tool, with four group label columns and one column of demeaned scores for the gender. Nonparametric testing was employed to identify significant clusters related to effects of interest by FSL’s RANDOMIZE with 5,000 permutations and multicomparison-corrected threshold (i.e., threshold-free cluster enhancement corrected P < 0.05) (100).
The significant mean cluster RSFC-z values (mean corticostriatal connectivity) identified in the main and interaction effects from the neuroimaging data were imported into Mplus software (101) and analyzed separately using mixed-effect models with the following variables as covariates to control for differences in drug-taking experiences: severity of stimulant use (OCDUS score) (34), duration of stimulant use (years), alcohol consumption (AUDIT score) (92), tobacco use (past/present), and cannabis use (past/present). As the family environment presents a risk factor for addiction (102), we controlled for the shared upbringing of the sibling pairs by assigning siblings pairs the same family identification number and including it as a random effect in the mixed-effect model. This two-step approach to the inclusion of covariates was taken to avoid overfitting in the neuroimaging model.
Demographic and questionnaire data were examined with SPSS (v22, IBM) using a two-way analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) model with the two between-subject factors: “familial risk” (familiality, nonfamiliality) and “stimulant use” (present, absent) (17). Both were included in the same statistical model to concurrently investigate their distinct and interaction effects, while mitigating Type I error. In light of the gender-specific responses to cocaine (103), and that stimulant-using participants are predominantly male, gender was included as a covariate in all analyses. We also applied mediation analysis using the process software tool (v2.13; afhayes.com/index.html) implemented in SPSS to examine the impact of stimulant-related compulsivity (OCDUS) on the regulatory balance between the identified inhibitory and habitual control RSFC networks. The χ2 or Fisher’s exact tests were used for the analysis of categorical data. All statistical tests were two-tailed, and a significance level was set at 0.05.
Acknowledgments
We thank all our volunteers, without whom this study would not have been possible, and also staff at the Mental Health Research Network for their dedicated support with participant recruitment. We thank the staff at the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Cambridge Clinical Research Facility at Addenbrooke’s Hospital and the Wolfson Brain Imaging Centre for their help and support throughout the study. A special word of thanks to Abigail Turton for her invaluable help with data collection. This research was funded by Medical Research Council Grant G0701497, financially supported by the NIHR Cambridge Biomedical Research Centre, and conducted within the Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute. C.M. was supported by a Wellcome Trust grant to K.D.E. (Grant 105602/Z/14/Z) and the NIHR Cambridge Biomedical Research Centre. J.S. was partly supported by the NIHR Collaboration for Leadership in Applied Health Research and Care (CLAHRC) East of England and by the Charles University Research Development Schemes Programme P38. T.W.R. is a recipient of Wellcome Trust Senior Investigator Award 104631/z/14/z and is supported by grants from Shionogi and GlaxoSmithKline. E.T.B. is an NIHR Senior Investigator.
Footnotes
Competing interest statement: T.W.R. discloses consultancy with Cambridge Cognition, Greenfield Bioventures, and Unilever; he receives royalties for the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery from Cambridge Cognition and editorial honoraria from Springer Verlag and Elsevier.
This article is a PNAS Direct Submission.
Data deposition: All data described in this paper (demographics, fMRI), including any accession codes, unique identifiers, or web links have been deposited in the University of Cambridge’s institutional repository, Apollo (https://doi.org/10.17863/CAM.53188).
References
- 1.Wagner F. A., Anthony J. C., From first drug use to drug dependence; developmental periods of risk for dependence upon marijuana, cocaine, and alcohol. Neuropsychopharmacology 26, 479–488 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.American Psychiatric Association , Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5, (American Psychiatric Association, Washington, D.C., 2013). [Google Scholar]
- 3.Koob G. F., Volkow N. D., Neurocircuitry of addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 217–238 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Merikangas K. R. et al., Familial transmission of substance use disorders. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 55, 973–979 (1998). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Hartz S. M., Bierut L. J., Genetics of addictions. Clin. Lab. Med. 30, 847–864 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Zimić J. I., Jukić V., Familial risk factors favoring drug addiction onset. J. Psychoactive Drugs 44, 173–185 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Fletcher D., Sarkar M., Psychological resilience a review and critique of definitions, concepts, and theory. Eur. Psychol. 18, 12–23 (2013). [Google Scholar]
- 8.Rudzinski K., McDonough P., Gartner R., Strike C., Is there room for resilience? A scoping review and critique of substance use literature and its utilization of the concept of resilience. Subst. Abuse Treat. Prev. Policy 12, 41 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Russo S. J., Murrough J. W., Han M.-H., Charney D. S., Nestler E. J., Neurobiology of resilience. Nat. Neurosci. 15, 1475–1484 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Burt K. B. et al.; IMAGEN Consortium , Structural brain correlates of adolescent resilience. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 57, 1287–1296 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Cousijn J., Luijten M., Feldstein Ewing S. W., Adolescent resilience to addiction: A social plasticity hypothesis. Lancet Child Adolesc. Health 2, 69–78 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Tapert S. F., Brown S. A., Substance dependence, family history of alcohol dependence and neuropsychological functioning in adolescence. Addiction 95, 1043–1053 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Kumar R., Kumar K. J., Benegal V., Cognitive and behavioural dispositions in offspring at high risk for alcoholism. Asian J. Psychiatr. 35, 38–44 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Ersche K. D. et al., Cognitive dysfunction and anxious-impulsive personality traits are endophenotypes for drug dependence. Am. J. Psychiatry 169, 926–936 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Gillen R., Hesselbrock V., Cognitive functioning, ASP, and family history of alcoholism in young men at risk for alcoholism. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 16, 206–214 (1992). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Morein-Zamir S., Simon Jones P., Bullmore E. T., Robbins T. W., Ersche K. D., Prefrontal hypoactivity associated with impaired inhibition in stimulant-dependent individuals but evidence for hyperactivation in their unaffected siblings. Neuropsychopharmacology 38, 1945–1953 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Just A. L. et al., Effects of familial risk and stimulant drug use on the anticipation of monetary reward: An fMRI study. Transl. Psychiatry 9, 65 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.DeVito E. E. et al., Neural correlates of impulsivity in healthy males and females with family histories of alcoholism. Neuropsychopharmacology 38, 1854–1863 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Parker H., Williams L., Aldridge J., The normalization of “sensible” recreational drug use: Further evidence from the north west England longitudinal study. Sociol. J. Br. Sociol. Assoc. 36, 941–964 (2002). [Google Scholar]
- 20.White J., Batty G. D., Intelligence across childhood in relation to illegal drug use in adulthood: 1970 British cohort study. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 66, 767–774 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Harrison L. D., Cocaine using careers in perspective. Addict. Res. 2, 1–20 (1994). [Google Scholar]
- 22.Smith D. G., Simon Jones P., Bullmore E. T., Robbins T. W., Ersche K. D., Enhanced orbitofrontal cortex function and lack of attentional bias to cocaine cues in recreational stimulant users. Biol. Psychiatry 75, 124–131 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Morein-Zamir S., Simon Jones P., Bullmore E. T., Robbins T. W., Ersche K. D., Take it or leave it: Prefrontal control in recreational cocaine users. Transl. Psychiatry 5, e582 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Berke J. D., Hyman S. E., Addiction, dopamine, and the molecular mechanisms of memory. Neuron 25, 515–532 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Burton A. C., Nakamura K., Roesch M. R., From ventral-medial to dorsal-lateral striatum: Neural correlates of reward-guided decision-making. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 117, 51–59 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Everitt B. J., Robbins T. W., Neural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: From actions to habits to compulsion. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 1481–1489 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Jentsch J. D., Taylor J. R., Impulsivity resulting from frontostriatal dysfunction in drug abuse: Implications for the control of behavior by reward-related stimuli. Pschopharmacology (Berl.) 146, 373–390 (1999). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Haber S. N., Knutson B., The reward circuit: Linking primate anatomy and human imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 4–26 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.de Wit S. et al., Corticostriatal connectivity underlies individual differences in the balance between habitual and goal-directed action control. J. Neurosci. 32, 12066–12075 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Di Martino A. et al., Functional connectivity of human striatum: A resting state FMRI study. Cereb. Cortex 18, 2735–2747 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Haber S. N., The place of dopamine in the cortico-basal ganglia circuit. Neuroscience 282, 248–257 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Redonnet B., Chollet A., Fombonne E., Bowes L., Melchior M., Tobacco, alcohol, cannabis and other illegal drug use among young adults: The socioeconomic context. Drug Alcohol Depend. 121, 231–239 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Swendsen J. et al., Socio-demographic risk factors for alcohol and drug dependence: The 10-year follow-up of the national comorbidity survey. Addiction 104, 1346–1355 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Franken I. H. A., Hendriksa V. M., van den Brink W., Initial validation of two opiate craving questionnaires the obsessive compulsive drug use scale and the desires for drug questionnaire. Addict. Behav. 27, 675–685 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.McNamee D., Liljeholm M., Zika O., O’Doherty J. P., Characterizing the associative content of brain structures involved in habitual and goal-directed actions in humans: A multivariate FMRI study. J. Neurosci. 35, 3764–3771 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Aron A. R., Robbins T. W., Poldrack R. A., Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 8, 170–177 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Bogdanov M., Timmermann J. E., Gläscher J., Hummel F. C., Schwabe L., Causal role of the inferolateral prefrontal cortex in balancing goal-directed and habitual control of behavior. Sci. Rep. 8, 9382 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Horga G. et al., Changes in corticostriatal connectivity during reinforcement learning in humans. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 793–803 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Rushworth M. F. S., Walton M. E., Kennerley S. W., Bannerman D. M., Action sets and decisions in the medial frontal cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 8, 410–417 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Balleine B. W., Killcross A. S., Dickinson A., The effect of lesions of the basolateral amygdala on instrumental conditioning. J. Neurosci. 23, 666–675 (2003). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Corbit L. H., Balleine B. W., The role of prelimbic cortex in instrumental conditioning. Behav. Brain Res. 146, 145–157 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Ostlund S. B., Balleine B. W., Lesions of medial prefrontal cortex disrupt the acquisition but not the expression of goal-directed learning. J. Neurosci. 25, 7763–7770 (2005). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Tanabe J. et al., Reduced neural tracking of prediction error in substance-dependent individuals. Am. J. Psychiatry 170, 1356–1363 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Fishbein D. H. et al., Risky decision making and the anterior cingulate cortex in abstinent drug abusers and nonusers. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 23, 119–136 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Ersche K. D. et al., Abnormal frontal activations related to decision-making in current and former amphetamine and opiate dependent individuals. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 180, 612–623 (2005). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Ersche K. D. et al., Distinctive personality traits and neural correlates associated with stimulant drug use versus familial risk of stimulant dependence. Biol. Psychiatry 74, 137–144 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Murray J. E. et al., Basolateral and central amygdala differentially recruit and maintain dorsolateral striatum-dependent cocaine-seeking habits. Nat. Commun. 6, 10088 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Jia Z. et al., An initial study of neural responses to monetary incentives as related to treatment outcome in cocaine dependence. Biol. Psychiatry 70, 553–560 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Yip S. W. et al., Anticipatory reward processing among cocaine-dependent individuals with and without concurrent methadone-maintenance treatment: Relationship to treatment response. Drug Alcohol Depend. 166, 134–142 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Craig A. D., How do you feel—now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 59–70 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Feinstein J. S., Goldin P. R., Stein M. B., Brown G. G., Paulus M. P., Habituation of attentional networks during emotion processing. Neuroreport 13, 1255–1258 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Mobascher A. et al., Brain activation patterns underlying fast habituation to painful laser stimuli. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 75, 16–24 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.McHugh M. J. et al., Striatal-insula circuits in cocaine addiction: Implications for impulsivity and relapse risk. Am. J. Drug Alcohol Abuse 39, 424–432 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Breiter H. C. et al., Acute effects of cocaine on human brain activity and emotion. Neuron 19, 591–611 (1997). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Risinger R. C. et al., Neural correlates of high and craving during cocaine self-administration using BOLD fMRI. Neuroimage 26, 1097–1108 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Naqvi N. H., Bechara A., The insula and drug addiction: An interoceptive view of pleasure, urges, and decision-making. Brain Struct. Funct. 214, 435–450 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Paulus M. P., Tapert S. F., Schulteis G., The role of interoception and alliesthesia in addiction. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 94, 1–7 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Bechara A., Damasio H., Decision-making and addiction (part I): Impaired activation of somatic states in substance dependent individuals when pondering decisions with negative future consequences. Neuropsychologia 40, 1675–1689 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Dunn B. D. et al., Listening to your heart. How interoception shapes emotion experience and intuitive decision making. Psychol. Sci. 21, 1835–1844 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Damasio A. R., Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain, (Grosset/Pullman, New York, 1994). [Google Scholar]
- 61.Hinson J. M., Jameson T. L., Whitney P., Somatic markers, working memory, and decision making. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2, 341–353 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Ersche K. D. et al., Carrots and sticks fail to change behavior in cocaine addiction. Science 352, 1468–1471 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Thompson L. L. et al., Negative reinforcement learning is affected in substance dependence. Drug Alcohol Depend. 123, 84–90 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Hillary F. G., Grafman J. H., Injured brains and adaptive networks: The benefits and costs of hyperconnectivity. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 21, 385–401 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Xiong J. et al., Long-term motor training induced changes in regional cerebral blood flow in both task and resting states. Neuroimage 45, 75–82 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Pravatà E. et al., Hyperconnectivity of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex following mental effort in multiple sclerosis patients with cognitive fatigue. Mult. Scler. 22, 1665–1675 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Martz M. E., Zucker R. A., Schulenberg J. E., Heitzeg M. M., Psychosocial and neural indicators of resilience among youth with a family history of substance use disorder. Drug Alcohol Depend. 185, 198–206 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Smith D. G., Jones P. S., Bullmore E. T., Robbins T. W., Ersche K. D., Cognitive control dysfunction and abnormal frontal cortex activation in stimulant drug users and their biological siblings. Transl. Psychiatry 3, e257 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Luijten M. et al., Systematic review of ERP and fMRI studies investigating inhibitory control and error processing in people with substance dependence and behavioural addictions. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 39, 149–169 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Hanlon C. A., Wesley M. J., Roth A. J., Miller M. D., Porrino L. J., Loss of laterality in chronic cocaine users: An fMRI investigation of sensorimotor control. Psychiatry Res. 181, 15–23 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Satpute A. B., Lieberman M. D., Integrating automatic and controlled processes into neurocognitive models of social cognition. Brain Res. 1079, 86–97 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Cohen J. D., Botvinick M., Carter C. S., Anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex: who’s in control? Nat. Neurosci. 3, 421–423 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Kerns J. G. et al., Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science 303, 1023–1026 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Pariyadath V., Gowin J. L., Stein E. A., “Chapter 8 - Resting state functional connectivity analysis for addiction medicine: From individual loci to complex networks” in Progress in Brain Research Neuroscience for Addiction Medicine: From Prevention to Rehabilitation - Methods and Interventions, Ekhtiari H., Paulus M. P., Eds. (Elsevier, ed. 224, 2016), pp. 155–173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Fox M. D., Raichle M. E., Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 700–711 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Goldstein R. Z., Volkow N. D., Drug addiction and its underlying neurobiological basis: Neuroimaging evidence for the involvement of the frontal cortex. Am. J. Psychiatry 159, 1642–1652 (2002). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Motzkin J. C., Baskin-Sommers A., Newman J. P., Kiehl K. A., Koenigs M., Neural correlates of substance abuse: Reduced functional connectivity between areas underlying reward and cognitive control. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 4282–4292 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Hu Y., Salmeron B. J., Gu H., Stein E. A., Yang Y., Impaired functional connectivity within and between frontostriatal circuits and its association with compulsive drug use and trait impulsivity in cocaine addiction. JAMA Psychiatry 72, 584–592 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Wisner K. M., Patzelt E. H., Lim K. O., MacDonald A. W. 3rd, An intrinsic connectivity network approach to insula-derived dysfunctions among cocaine users. Am. J. Drug Alcohol Abuse 39, 403–413 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Regner M. F. et al., Top-down network effective connectivity in abstinent substance dependent individuals. PLoS One 11, e0164818 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Ipser J. C. et al., Distinct intrinsic functional brain network abnormalities in methamphetamine-dependent patients with and without a history of psychosis. Addict. Biol. 23, 347–358 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Gu H. et al., Mesocorticolimbic circuits are impaired in chronic cocaine users as demonstrated by resting-state functional connectivity. Neuroimage 53, 593–601 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Dean A. C., Kohno M., Hellemann G., London E. D., Childhood maltreatment and amygdala connectivity in methamphetamine dependence: A pilot study. Brain Behav. 4, 867–876 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.American Psychiatric Association , Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, (American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC, ed. 4, text revision, 2000). [Google Scholar]
- 85.First M. B., Spitzer R. L., Gibbon M., Williams J. B. W., Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Non-patient Edition. (SCID-I/NP), (Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York, 2002). [Google Scholar]
- 86.Wallace B. C., Crack cocaine smokers as adult children of alcoholics: The dysfunctional family link. J. Subst. Abuse Treat. 7, 89–100 (1990). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Bernstein D. P. et al., Development and validation of a brief screening version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire. Child Abuse Negl. 27, 169–190 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Ashida S., Robinson E. L., Gay J., Slagel L. E., Ramirez M. R., Personal disaster and emergency support networks of older adults in a rural community: Changes after participation in a preparedness program. Disaster Med. Public Health Prep. 11, 110–119 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Dumont M., Provost M. A., Resilience in adolescents: Protective role of social support, coping strategies, self-esteem, and social activities on experience of stress and depression. J. Youth Adolesc. 28, 343–363 (1999). [Google Scholar]
- 90.Sander A. M. et al., The community integration questionnaire revisited: An assessment of factor structure and validity. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 80, 1303–1308 (1999). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Skinner H. A., The drug abuse screening test. Addict. Behav. 7, 363–371 (1982). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Saunders J. B., Aasland O. G., Babor T. F., de la Fuente J. R., Grant M., Development of the alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT): WHO collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol consumption—II. Addiction 88, 791–804 (1993). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Power J. D., Barnes K. A., Snyder A. Z., Schlaggar B. L., Petersen S. E., Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. Neuroimage 59, 2142–2154 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Van Dijk K. R. A., Sabuncu M. R., Buckner R. L., The influence of head motion on intrinsic functional connectivity MRI. Neuroimage 59, 431–438 (2052). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Power J. D., Schlaggar B. L., Petersen S. E., Recent progress and outstanding issues in motion correction in resting state fMRI. Neuroimage 105, 536–551 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Pruim R. H. R. et al., ICA-AROMA: A robust ICA-based strategy for removing motion artifacts from fMRI data. Neuroimage 112, 267–277 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Biswal B., Yetkin F. Z., Haughton V. M., Hyde J. S., Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magn. Reson. Med. 34, 537–541 (1995). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Ersche K. D. et al., Abnormal brain structure implicated in stimulant drug addiction. Science 335, 601–604 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Regner M. F. et al., Sex differences in gray matter changes and brain-behavior relationships in patients with stimulant dependence. Radiology 277, 801–812 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Smith S. M., Nichols T. E., Threshold-free cluster enhancement: Addressing problems of smoothing, threshold dependence and localisation in cluster inference. Neuroimage 44, 83–98 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Muthén B., Muthén L., Mplus User’s Guide, (Muthén & Muthén, Los Angeles, CA, ed. 8, 2018). [Google Scholar]
- 102.Baker J. H., Maes H. H., Kendler K. S., Shared environmental contributions to substance use. Behav. Genet. 42, 345–353 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Kuhn C., Francis R., Gender difference in cocaine-induced HPA axis activation. Neuropsychopharmacology 16, 399–407 (1997). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Haber S. N., Fudge J. L., McFarland N. R., Striatonigrostriatal pathways in primates form an ascending spiral from the shell to the dorsolateral striatum. J. Neurosci. 20, 2369–2382 (2000). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]