Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2021 Jul 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2020 Jan 5;61(7):818–825. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13182

Less imitation of arbitrary actions is a specific developmental precursor to callous-unemotional traits in early childhood

Nicholas J Wagner 1,*, Rebecca Waller 2,*, Megan Flom 1, Samuel Ronfard 3, Susan Fenstermacher 4, Kimberly Saudino 1
PMCID: PMC7335314  NIHMSID: NIHMS1062675  PMID: 31903558

Abstract

Objective:

Callous-unemotional (CU) traits in early childhood explain heterogeneity within conduct problems and are associated with higher risk for later diagnoses of childhood disruptive behavior disorders and antisocial behavior in adulthood. Emerging research implicates impairments in affiliative processes in the etiology of CU traits. The current study tests whether the imitation of intentional actions with no functional significance –a behavior that supports the acquisition of social conventions and affiliative bonds, is a specific developmental precursor to CU traits in early childhood.

Methods:

Data came from a longitudinal twin study of 628 children (Age 2: 47% females; Age 3: 44.9% females) with observations of arbitrary (i.e., non-functional actions) and instrumental (i.e., functional actions) imitation and parent reports of CU traits and oppositional defiant (ODD) behaviors at ages 2 and 3.

Results:

Lower arbitrary imitation at age 2, but not instrumental imitation, was related to increases in CU traits from ages 2 to 3 (β=−0.10, p=0.02).

Conclusions:

These findings establish early social and affiliative processes in the etiology of CU traits, highlighting that novel personalized treatment and intervention strategies for CU traits may benefit from targeting these processes to help reduce CU traits and risk for persistent conduct problems in children.

Keywords: Behaviour problems, callous unemotional traits, developmental psychopathology, social behaviour

Introduction

Conduct problems (CP), including aggressive, oppositional, and rule-breaking behaviors, are a highly prevalent form of childhood psychopathology that confer risk for poor socioemotional, educational, and mental and physical health outcomes across the lifespan.1 However, CP are highly heterogeneous, which undermines our ability to effectively identify and treat children at risk for persistent CP across development. To better parse this heterogeneity, research has focused on the presence or absence of callous-unemotional (CU) traits among children with CP. CU traits refer to the presence of callous, uncaring, and remorseless behavior, a lack of guilt and empathy, and reduced sensitivity to the emotions of others.2 CU traits differentiate a distinct group of children with CP at increased risk for severe and chronic aggression, violence, and psychopathy, beyond risk associated with early oppositional defiant or conduct-disordered behaviors.3,4

Research has begun to establish that CU traits arise from a distinct set of developmental processes, particularly impaired behavioral, physiological, and neural sensitivity to cues of social affiliation, emotion, and threat.5 In early childhood, there is emerging evidence that impaired sensitivity to affiliative bonding and low social motivation undermines attachment formation and the development of more complex interpersonal processes, increasing risk for CU traits.58 For example, lower infant preferential face tracking at 5 weeks9 and lower parent-directed affection and eye contact at 18 months7 and 4 years old10 have been linked to CU traits in early childhood. This research contributes to our understanding of potential links between deficits in affiliation and CU traits.8 However, extant research has primarily focused on narrow aspects of affiliation (e.g., attention) or single social relationships assessed in brief laboratory paradigms (e.g., the parent-child relationship).7 Moreover, studies on the origins of CU traits have largely not been grounded in what we know more broadly about the emergence of social learning and connection, including the imitation of others’ actions or behaviors, a core indicator of early preference for and seeking out of affiliative interactions.11

The early imitation of others is a behavioral phenomenon that forms a vital foundation for social learning processes and social connections. Human imitation enriches our understanding of social conventions, the formation of relationships, attachments, and affiliative bonds, and the development of fundamental social beliefs and attitudes, differentiating humans from other species.11 Throughout development, and particularly in infancy and early childhood when children are pre-verbal, two forms of imitation facilitate children’s rapid acquisition of knowledge and skills through repeating observed behaviors of peers and adults.12,13 Instrumental imitation refers to children copying the intentional or goal-directed actions of others and ignoring mistakes or failed attempts, thus allowing them to rapidly acquire new skills while avoiding the pitfalls of trial-and-error learning.14 Arbitrary imitation (also known as “overimitation”) refers to children deliberately copying the behaviors of others even when the model’s actions have no apparent purpose or causal function.1416

Arbitrary imitation is central to the promotion of social affiliation, and unlike instrumental imitation, is specific to humans and considered a foundational element of complex social cultures.13,15 Specifically, arbitrary imitation signals shared intentions, conformity to normative conventions, and a desire to affiliate.11,17 Unsurprisingly, arbitrary imitation occurs more often when the individual who demonstrated an action is present at the time of the imitation,13 highlighting the social nature of the action (i.e., centered on who is imitated rather than what is imitated). In addition, multiple studies have demonstrated that priming social exclusion increases arbitrary imitation in young children,16,18 suggesting that arbitrary imitation serves an affiliative function. That is, children use arbitrary imitation as a strategy to promote social affiliation and reinclusion. However, no prior studies have examined whether fewer displays of arbitrary imitation represent a developmental precursor to CU traits during early childhood.

The current study sought to advance our knowledge of social-affiliative processes as a developmental precursor to CU traits in early childhood using a longitudinal design. Specifically, we explored longitudinal associations between instrumental and arbitrary imitation at age 2 and CU traits at age 3. We also examine the associations between instrumental and arbitrary imitation and children’s oppositional defiant (ODD) behaviors, a broad set of externalizing problems including defiant, uncooperative, or disobedient behaviors, angry moods, and stubbornness. To establish whether imitation was a distinct and specific precursor of CU traits, we included ODD at age 3 as a covarying outcome in all predictive models. In the context of the literature on arbitrary imitation suggesting that it serves a specific social-affiliative purpose,11 we hypothesized that low levels of arbitrary imitation, but not instrumental imitation, would be specifically related to higher CU traits but not ODD behaviors. We tested associations during a critical developmental window of early childhood (i.e., from 2–3 years old) when instances of arbitrary imitation serve as a means for pre-verbal children to bolster their social affiliation and communication.19

Method

Participants

The participants were drawn from Boston University Twin Project and were recruited from birth records supplied by the Massachusetts Registry of Vital Records. Twins were selected preferentially for higher birth weight and gestational age. No twins with birth weights below 1750 grams or with gestational ages less than 34 weeks were included in the study. Twins were also excluded if they had a known developmental disorder (e.g., chromosomal abnormalities) that might affect their task performance. Six hundred and twenty-eight twins (314 pairs) participated in the age 2 assessments, and 608 twins returned for the age 3 assessments (96.8% retention rate). There was approximately equal numbers of males and females at each age (Age 2: 47% females; Age 3: 44.9% females). Race and socioeconomic status were generally representative of the Massachusetts population (85.4% Caucasian, 3.2% Black, 2% Asian, 7.3% mixed, 2.2% other). Written informed consent was obtained from all subjects. The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. All procedures involving human subjects/patients were approved by the Boston University Institutional Review Board.

Measures

ODD and CU Traits.

Measures of ODD and CU traits were derived from the Achenbach system of Empirically Based Assessment, Preschool Forms (ASEBA,21 also known as the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL). Primary caregivers (94% mothers) completed the ASEBA for each twin at both the 2- and 3-year assessments. The ASEBA includes a scoring profile drawn from DSM-referenced scales for ODD comprised of six items (“defiant”, “disobedient”, “angry moods”, “stubborn”, “temper tantrum”, and “uncooperative”). Further, Willoughby and colleagues (2011) demonstrated that five items drawn from the ASEBA (“no guilt after misbehave”, “punish does not change behavior”, “unresponsive to affection”, “shows little affection”, and “too little fear”) could be used to measure individual differences in CU traits at these early ages. Factor analytic studies suggest that CU traits can be assessed as a distinct construct in 2- and 3-year-olds, demonstrating that parents are able to discriminate between CU traits and other behavior problems at young ages.22 The approach to measuring CU traits used in the current study has been validated in multiple longitudinal samples,4,7,23 including in the current sample.24,25 Internal consistency for the ODD (age 2, α=.79; age 3 α=.81) and CU traits (age 2, α=0.55; age 3, α=0.61) are consistent with other studies using this measurement approach at these ages.6,7,23,26

Imitation.

Observations of imitated instrumental and arbitrary actions were coded based on the Birdhouse Task at both ages 2 and 3 and the Trap Tube Task at age 3.27 The Birdhouse Task apparatus consisted of a wooden birdhouse modified with a wooden pin that slid out of the left side to release the front door. As part of the procedure, an examiner placed the birdhouse on the table facing the child and directed the child’s attention to it, saying, “Look, (child’s name)” and “Watch this.” The examiner then completed both instrumental behaviors (i.e., necessary for retrieving the bird) and arbitrary behaviors and vocalizations (i.e., “Look! It’s a birdy! Cheep cheep cheep”) (see Figure 1). The Trap Tube Task consisted of a tube in which a research assistant placed a cracker, which was retrieved with a stick. As before, the examiner completed instrumental behaviors (i.e., retrieval of cracker with the stick) and arbitrary behaviors and vocalizations (i.e., saying, “one two three,” or tapping the stick on the table) (see Figure 2). Two trials were completed for all tasks at each time point. For trial 1, the child was given 60 seconds from the first contact with the birdhouse or trap tube to imitate the actions they had seen. Trial 2, which occurred immediately after trial 1, afforded children the same opportunities as trial 1. Experimenters did not replicate any behaviors or vocalizations prior to trial 2. To assess instrumental and arbitrary imitation, we generated two composite scores across the two trials for each task at each age: the proportion of instrumental actions imitated and the proportion of arbitrary actions imitated. These tasks have been widely-used to assess children’s tendencies to imitate instrumental and arbitrary actions at these ages.27 Interrater reliability was high (all ICCs≥.80). Additional details regarding procedures and coding are provided in Appendix S1 and Table S1 in the Supporting Information.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Demonstration of the birdhouse task. The task began when the tester (a) twisted the pin and pulled it from the left side of the birdhouse (instrumental). The tester then (b) opened the door (instrumental) and (c) retrieved the toy bird, making vocalizations and hopping movements with the bird before replacing it in the birdhouse (arbitrary).

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Demonstration of the Trap Tube Task. The task began with the tester (a) placing a cracker in the tube. The tester (b) retrieved the cracker with the stick (instrumental) and said “one, two, three” or tapped the stick on the table (arbitrary).

Additional covariates.

Twin’s sex, race (European-American vs. Other), and a semi-continuous measure of parent education were reported by parents and included as covariates in each predictive model. Fewer than 1% of the sample reported only finishing middle school or some high school, about 4% reported completing high school, 2% reported completing trade school, 10.5% had completed some college or university study, 41.6% received a degree from a four-year college, 6.7% had completed some graduate training, and about 32% had earned a graduate degree.

Analytic plan

We estimated a linear path model in which children’s ODD and CU traits at age 3 were regressed onto instrumental imitation, arbitrary imitation, and relevant covariates. Specifically, we examined whether observed instrumental and arbitrary imitation at age 2 predicted ODD and CU traits at age 3, controlling for relevant covariates including ODD and CU traits at age 2. All participants with complete or partial data were included in the predictive analyses using full‐information maximum likelihood (FIML),28 and both models were saturated. FIML is well recognized as an effective method for analyzing data with moderate to large amounts of missing data and has been demonstrated to provide less biased parameter estimates than other commonly used techniques, such as listwise deletion.28 Total missingness for each measure is reported in Table 1. All analyses were conducted in Mplus 7.1.29 Corrections to the standard errors in each predictive model to account for non-independence of observations due to the nested structure of twin data (i.e., twins are nested in families) were implemented using the TYPE=COMPLEX and CLUSTER procedures in Mplus.30

Table 1.

Zero-order Bivariate Correlations Between Model Outcomes

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
1. Race -
2. Sex 0.012 -
3. Parent Education 0.032 0.014 -
4. Instr. Imitation Age 2 −0.04 0.006 0.123* -
5. Instr. Imitation Age 3 0.051 0.019 0.051 0.043 -
6. Arbit. Imitation Age 2 −0.092 −0.028 0.078 0.075 0.053 -
7. Arbit. Imitation Age 3 0.038 −0.014 0.059 −0.016 0.133** 0.110* -
8. CU Traits Age 2 −0.112^ −0.055 −0.078 0.026 −0.011 0.017 −0.074 -
9. CU Traits Age 3 −0.071 −0.097* −0.083 −0.015 −0.04 −0.087^ −0.116** 0.475** -
10. ODD Age 2 −0.087 −0.094* −0.107 −0.034 −0.009 −0.026 −0.065 0.569** 0.371** -
11. ODD Age 3 −0.096 −0.062 −0.026 0.008 −0.074^ −0.049 −0.085* 0.437** 0.552** 0.595** -
Number 618 627 618 604 592 479 590 620 596 623 598
Mean 4.07 1.47 7.40 0.87 0.91 0.43 0.31 1.54 1.26 2.94 3.19
Standard Deviation 0.65 0.50 1.42 0.19 0.15 0.39 0.19 1.48 1.32 2.35 2.53

Note: p ≤ .10^, p ≤ .05*, p ≤ .01**; Instr. = Instrumental; Arbit. = Arbitrary

Results

Descriptive statistics

Table 1 provides the bivariate correlations, means and standard deviations for the model covariates and variables of interest. Twin’s sex was negatively associated with both ODD and CU traits such that male twins demonstrated higher ODD behaviors at age 2 and higher CU traits at age 3 than female twins. Parent’s educational attainment was positively correlated with children enacting more instances of instrumental imitation at age 2 but not age 3. Arbitrary imitation at age 3 was negatively correlated with both ODD and CU traits at age 3. Scores for each of the ODD and CU traits measures at ages 2 and 3 were significantly positively correlated.

Longitudinal associations across ages 2 and 3

A saturated path model was estimated to examine the longitudinal predictive associations between instrumental and arbitrary imitation at age 2 and ODD and CU traits at age 3, controlling for the autoregressive effects of ODD and CU traits at age 2 (Figure 3; Table 2). Lower arbitrary imitation at age 2, but not instrumental imitation, was related to higher CU traits at age 3, β=−0.10, b=−0.33, p=0.02, accounting for autoregressive associations between ODD and CU traits across time and the significant covariance of ODD and CU traits at age 3 cov(odd,cu) β=0.42, b=0.96, p<0.001 (see Table 2). Follow-up analyses show that the unique relation between lower arbitrary imitation at age 2 and CU traits at age 3 persists, β=−0.08, b=−0.29, p=0.03, when a measure of children’s Attention Problems (ADHD) at age 3, β=−0.04, b=−0.30, p=0.27 is included a covarying outcome along with ODD, β=−0.05, b=−0.29, p=0.24 (see Table S2).

Figure 3. Path model showing significant longitudinal association between imitation at age 2 and CU traits at age 3.

Figure 3.

Note: Model covariates (race, sex, parent education, ODD age 2, CU traits age 2) have been excluded from the figure; cov(ζODDζCU), b = 0.963, β = 0.423, p = 0.001; cov(ζODDζCU), b = 0.01, β = 0.12, p = 0.12; ODD = Oppositional Defiant Behaviors; CU = callous-unemotional.

Table 2.

Longitudinal path models showing that fewer observations of arbitrary imitation, but not instrumental imitation, were uniquely related to CU traits at age 3

ODD Age 3 CU Traits Age 3
Parameter B (β) 95% CI B (β) 95% CI
Race −0.154 (−0.040) [−0.517, 0.209] −0.041 (−0.021) [−0.267, 0.185]
Sex −0.066 (−0.013) [−0.437, 0.305] −0.191 (−0.072)^ [−0.401, 0.019]
Parent Education 0.085 (0.047) [−0.073, 0.242] −0.024 (−0.025) [−0.116, 0.069]
CU Traits Age 2 0.258 (0.151)** [0.11, 0.406] 0.358 (0.400)** [0.262, 0.453]
ODD Age 2 0.549 (0.509)** [0.446, 0.653] 0.073 (0.130)* [0.009, 0.138]
Instrumental Imitation Age 2 0.232 (0.018) [−0.665, 1.128] −0.093 (−0.014) [−0.546, 0.36]
Arbitrary Imitation Age 2 −0.316 (−0.048) [−0.81, 0.179] −0.328 (−0.096)* [−0.601, −0.055]
cov(ζODDζCU) = 0.963 (0.423)**

Note: CI: confidence interval; p ≤ .10^, p ≤ .05*, p ≤ .01**; Magnitude and direction of estimates and patterns of significance remain unchanged after controlling for birth weight, gestational age, Mental Development Index from the Bayley Scales (IQ), and the Pervasive Developmental Problems subscale from the Child Behavior Checklist (autism-like traits). Results available upon request.

Discussion

Social imitation is a pervasive and early emerging phenomenon in development that supports individual learning and skill acquisition, promotes social communication and affiliation, and perpetuates human culture.13,31 Unlike instrumental imitation, one of the primary functions of arbitrary imitation is to increase affiliation and social inclusion. The current study is the first to identify fewer displays of arbitrary imitation as a distinct developmental precursor to CU traits. Specifically, we found that less observed imitation of arbitrary actions, but not imitation of instrumental actions, was related to increases in CU traits at age 3. Moreover, these pathways were distinct for CU traits, as we did not find a significant association between arbitrary imitation and ODD. Findings lend further support for investigations into whether and how early etiological processes differentiate risk for eventual ODD and CU traits early in life, particularly given their, at least partial, conceptual overlap and clinical comorbidity.3

Affiliative inputs, such as facial expressions, vocalizations, and touch, are salient aspects of human experience from birth. The rewarding and repetitive nature of these affiliative experiences, most often with caregivers, are critical for early survival and promote the emergence of more complex social bonds and adaptive future social relationships.32 The imitation of arbitrary actions in pre-verbal children is thought to play a critical role in the foundation of these affiliative and social bonds, setting the stage for adaptive and cooperative interpersonal behaviors across the lifespan.11 Emerging evidence suggests that impairments to normative social-affiliative processes may contribute to the development of CU traits in children and the broader psychopathy phenotype in adults.5,8 The current study contributes to this line of inquiry by demonstrating that reduced arbitrary imitation is predictive of increases in CU traits, but not ODD. This finding suggests that CU traits, and its interpersonal and social correlates, may occur downstream of a lack of motivation for affiliative bonding, indexed via deficits in arbitrary imitation.

Limitations and future directions

The current study is characterized by a number of strengths, including a prospective longitudinal design and use of observational methods for assessing imitation. However, the findings must also be interpreted in the context of several important limitations. First, although the current study was conducted using a twin sample, because of modest magnitude of the bivariate association, our sample does not afford the power to decompose the genetic and environmental sources of covariance between CU traits and observed arbitrary imitation. Nonetheless, in exploratory analyses, we found that only genetic factors contributed to the covariance between arbitrary imitation and CU traits at age 3 thereby supporting future research testing whether the origins of CU traits lie in genetically mediated individual differences in social affiliation (see Table S3).

Second, while the links between arbitrary imitation at age 2 and ODD at age 3 were nonsignificant, unstandardized parameter estimates were similar to those for the association between arbitrary imitation at age 2 and CU traits at age 3. Future research should examine the stability of the pattern of significance of the current findings over time. Ideally this work would be done using a more comprehensive measure of CU traits than the 6-item measure used in the current study, which exhibits only moderate internal consistency across a number of samples.

Third, because we only had one follow-up assessment (i.e., at age 3), we cannot speak to whether the association between arbitrary imitation and CU traits extends across later childhood, nor how CU traits and arbitrary imitation might be dynamically and reciprocally related over time. Of note, there is significant heterotypic continuity in how arbitrary imitation manifests across the lifespan, especially once children are verbal, meaning that future studies of arbitrary imitation and CU traits in older individuals would necessarily have to employ different methods to assess arbitrary imitation. For example, studies of older children and adults have explored both joint actions and movement33 and phonetic imitation during social conversation,34 which, like arbitrary imitation, might function to increase social connection and promote affiliation with others. Such methods have yet to be applied in relation to risk for, or amelioration of, CU traits but represent an intriguing and innovative target for future studies.

Finally, the observed relationship we report between arbitrary imitation and CU traits may have arisen because of parent-child dyadic processes that began earlier in infancy, which we did not assess. For example, parental modeling and attentional synchrony in infancy could have shaped tendencies to enact arbitrary imitation with potentially accumulating and reciprocal effects on the emergence of CU traits across development.35 Moreover, research demonstrates increases in faithful imitation of arbitrary actions just after the first year of life, and that this form of imitation is associated with increased sociability, further providing support for examining these processes earlier in development.36

In sum, we applied a novel paradigm assessing imitation to provide compelling evidence linking lower arbitrary imitation, a core skill linked to social cooperation and affiliation across the lifespan, to the early development of CU traits. Our findings provide initial evidence for impairments in socio-affiliative mechanisms being critical for understanding risk for CU traits, which are pathways that have been hypothesized but rarely tested. The results suggest that socio-affiliative processes could be targeted in future personalized treatment and intervention strategies for children with CU traits and at risk for persistent and severe conduct problems across development.8 More specifically, treatment strategies for decreasing CU traits beginning early in childhood could focus on social skills and empathy training37 and, drawing on treatment research for autism spectrum disorders, reciprocal imitation training.38

Supplementary Material

supp info

Key points.

  • Callous-unemotional traits differentiate patterns of homogeneity within child conduct problems and are associated with greater risk for later disruptive behavior disorders and antisocial behavior in adulthood.

  • CU traits arise from a distinct set of developmental processes, and there is emerging evidence that impaired sensitivity to affiliative bonding and low social motivation may increase risk for CU traits by undermining the development of interpersonal processes.

  • Imitation of arbitrary behaviors which have no apparent causal function promote social affiliation by signaling shared intentions and conformity to normative conventions. Despite the importance of imitation for the development maintenance of social relationships in human cultures, no study has tested whether lower arbitrary imitation is a specific precursor to CU traits in early childhood.

  • This study shows that lower arbitrary imitation at age 2, but not imitation of goal directed actions, was related to increases in CU traits from ages 2 to 3. In exploratory analyses which leveraged the study’s twin design, we find that only genetic factors contributed to the covariance between arbitrary imitation and CU traits at age 3.

  • The results suggest that socio-affiliative processes could be targeted in future personalized treatment and intervention strategies for children with CU traits and at risk for persistent and severe conduct problems across development.

Acknowledgements

The Boston University Twin Project (BUTP) is supported by grants from the National Institute of Mental Health (MH062375) and the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (HD068435) to K.S.. M.F. is supported by F31MH114590. The authors have declared that they have no competing or potential conflicts of interest

Footnotes

Conflict of interest statement: No conflicts declared.

Supporting information

Additional supporting information may be found online in the Supporting Information section at the end of the article:

Appendix S1. Description of imitation tasks.

Table S1. Behaviors coded in imitation tasks.

Table S2. Longitudinal path models including ADHD outcomes.

Table S3. Genetic and environmental correlations explaining covariance between CU and Arbitrary Imitation.

References

  • 1.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edition (DSM-5). Diagn Stat Man Ment Disord 4th Ed TR. 2013:280. doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596.744053 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Waller R, Hyde LW. Callous-unemotional behaviors in early childhood: the development of empathy and prosociality gone awry. Curr Opin Psychol. 2018;20:11–16. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.07.037 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Frick PJ, Ray JV, Thornton LC, Kahn RE. Annual research review: A developmental psychopathology approach to understanding callous-unemotional traits in children and adolescents with serious conduct problems. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2014;55:532–548. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12152 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Waller R, Hyde LW. Callous-Unemotional Behaviors in Early Childhood: Measurement, Meaning, and the Influence of Parenting. Child Dev Perspect. 2017;0:1–7. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12222 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Viding E, McCrory E. Towards understanding atypical social affiliation in psychopathy. Lancet Psychiatry. 2019;6(5):437–444. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(19)30049-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Bedford R, Wagner NJ, Rehder PD, Propper C, Willoughby MT, Mills-Koonce RW. The role of infants’ mother-directed gaze, maternal sensitivity, and emotion recognition in childhood callous unemotional behaviours. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2017. doi: 10.1007/s00787-017-0967-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Wagner NJ, Mills-Koonce WR, Propper CB, et al. Associations between Infant Behaviors during the Face-To-Face Still-Face Paradigm and Oppositional Defiant and Callous-Unemotional Behaviors in Early Childhood. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2016. doi: 10.1007/s10802-016-0141-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Waller R, Wagner N. The Sensitivity to Threat and Affiliative Reward (STAR) model and the development of callous-unemotional traits. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2019;107:656–671. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.10.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Bedford R, Pickles A, Sharp H, Wright N, Hill J. Archival Report Reduced Face Preference in Infancy : A Developmental Precursor to Callous- Unemotional Traits ? Biol Psychiatry. 2014:1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.09.022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Dadds MR, Allen JL, Oliver BR, et al. Love, eye contact and the developmental origins of empathy v. psychopathy. Br J Psychiatry J Ment Sci. 2012;200:191–196. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.110.085720 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Over H, Carpenter M. The Social Side of Imitation. Child Dev Perspect. 2013;7(1):6–11. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Meltzoff AN, Kuhl PK, Movellan J, Sejnowski TJ. Foundations for a New Science of Learning. Science. 2009;325(5938):284–288. doi: 10.1126/science.1175626 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Nielsen M, Blank C. Imitation in young children: When who gets copied is more important than what gets copied. Dev Psychol. 2011;47(4):1050–1053. doi: 10.1037/a0023866 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Lyons DE, Young AG, Keil FC. The hidden structure of overimitation. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2007;104(50):19751–19756. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0704452104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Nielsen M, Simcock G, Jenkins L. The effect of social engagement on 24-month-olds’ imitation from live and televised models. Dev Sci. 2008;11(5):722–731. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00722.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Watson-Jones RE, Whitehouse H, Legare CH. In-Group Ostracism Increases High-Fidelity Imitation in Early Childhood. Psychol Sci. 2016;27(1):34–42. doi: 10.1177/0956797615607205 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Keupp S, Behne T, Rakoczy H. Why do children overimitate? Normativity is crucial. J Exp Child Psychol. 2013;116(2):392–406. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.07.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Over H, Carpenter M. Priming third-party ostracism increases affiliative imitation in children. Dev Sci. 2009;12(3):F1–F8. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00820.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Eckerman CO, Didow SM. Toddlers’ social coordinations: Changing responses to another’s invitation to play. Dev Psychol. 1989;25(5):794. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Price TS, Freeman B, Craig I, Petrill SA, Ebersole L, Plomin R. Infant zygosity can be assigned by parental report questionnaire data. Twin Res 2000. 2000;3(3):129–133. doi: 10.1375/136905200320565391 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Achenbach TM. Manual for the child behavior checklist/ 4–18 and 1991 profile. Burlingt VT: 1991. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Willoughby MT, Waschbusch D a, Moore G a, Propper CB. Using the ASEBA to Screen for Callous Unemotional Traits in Early Childhood: Factor Structure, Temporal Stability, and Utility. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2011;33:19–30. doi: 10.1007/s10862-010-9195-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Willoughby MT, Mills-Koonce WR, Gottfredson NC, Wagner NJ. Measuring callous unemotional behaviors in early childhood: Factor structure and the prediction of stable aggression in middle childhood. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2014;36:30–42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Flom M, Saudino KJ. Callous–unemotional behaviors in early childhood: Genetic and environmental contributions to stability and change. Dev Psychopathol. 2016;29:1–8. doi: 10.1017/S0954579416001267 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Flom M, White D, Ganiban J, Saudino KJ. Longitudinal Links Between Callous-Unemotional Behaviors and Parenting in Early Childhood: A Genetically-Informed Design. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. March 2019. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2019.03.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Waller R, Gardner F, Shaw DS, Dishion TJ, Wilson MN, Hyde LW. Callous-Unemotional Behavior and Early-Childhood Onset of Behavior Problems: The Role of Parental Harshness and Warmth. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol Off J Soc Clin Child Adolesc Psychol Am Psychol Assoc Div 53 2014:37–41. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2014.886252 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Carpenter M, Call J, Tomasello M. Understanding “Prior Intentions” Enables Two-Year-Olds to Imitatively Learn a Complex Task. Child Dev. 2002;73(5):1431–1441. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00481 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Enders CK, Bandalos DL. The Relative Performance of Full Information Maximum Likelihood Estimation for Missing Data in Structural Equation Models. Struct Equ Model. 2001;8:430–457. doi: 10.1207/S15328007sem0803_5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Muthén LK, Muthen B. Mplus User’s Guide: Statistical Analysis with Latent Variables, User’s Guide. Muthén & Muthén; 2017. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Asparouhov T, Muthén B. Resampling Methods in Mplus for Complex Survey Data.; 2010. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Tomasello M, Call J. Primate Cognition. Oxford University Press; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Panksepp J, Nelson E, Siviy S. Brain opioids and mother—infant social motivation. Acta Paediatr. 1994;83(s397):40–46. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.1994.tb13264.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Marsh KL, Richardson MJ, Schmidt RC. Social connection through joint action and interpersonal coordination. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2009;(2), 320–339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Alan CL, Abrego-Collier C, Sonderegger M. Phonetic imitation from an individual-difference perspective: Subjective attitude, personality and “autistic” traits. PloS one, 2013; 8(9), e74746. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.D’Onofrio BM, Lahey BB. Biosocial Influences on the Family: A Decade Review. J Marriage Fam. 2010;72:762–782. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2010.00729.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Hilbrink Elma E., et al. “Selective and faithful imitation at 12 and 15 months.” Developmental Science 166 (2013): 828–840. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Wilkinson S, Waller R, & Viding E. Practitioner review: involving young people with callous unemotional traits in treatment–does it work? A systematic review. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry; 2016, 57(5), 552–565. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Ingersoll Brooke, Berger Natalie, Carlsen Danielle & Hamlin Theresa (2017) Improving social functioning and challenging behaviors in adolescents with ASD and significant ID: A randomized pilot feasibility trial of reciprocal imitation training in a residential setting, Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 20:4, 236–246, DOI: 10.1080/17518423.2016.1211187 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

supp info

RESOURCES