Abstract
Introduction
This study examines COVID-19–associated discrimination regardless of infection status. It evaluates the contribution of various risk factors (e.g., race/ethnicity and wearing a face mask) and the relationship with mental distress among U.S. adults in March and April 2020, when the pandemic escalated across the country.
Methods
Participants consisted of a probability-based, nationally representative sample of U.S. residents aged ≥18 years who completed COVID-19–related surveys online in March and April (n=3,665). Multivariable logistic regression was used to predict the probability of a person perceiving COVID-19–associated discrimination. Linear regression was used to analyze the association between discrimination and mental distress. Analyses were conducted in May 2020.
Results
Perception of COVID-19–associated discrimination increased from March (4%) to April (10%). Non-Hispanic Black (absolute risk from 0.09 to 0.15 across months) and Asians (absolute risk from 0.11 to 0.17) were more likely to perceive discrimination than other racial/ethnic groups (absolute risk from 0.03 to 0.11). Individuals who wore face masks (absolute risk from 0.11 to 0.14) also perceived more discrimination than those who did not (absolute risk from 0.04 to 0.11). Perceiving discrimination was subsequently associated with increased mental distress (from 0.77 to 1.01 points on the 4-item Patient Health Questionnaire score).
Conclusions
Perception of COVID-19–associated discrimination was relatively low but increased with time. Perceived discrimination was associated with race/ethnicity and wearing face masks and may contribute to greater mental distress during early stages of the pandemic. The long-term implications of this novel form of discrimination should be monitored.
INTRODUCTION
The coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic threatens global health and national economies. Both the UN and the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention have called for increased attention to prevent stigma associated with COVID-19,1 , 2 which could undermine disease control efforts, worsen mental health outcomes, and exacerbate disparities.3, 4, 5, 6 This study examines perceived discrimination among U.S. residents and its relationship with mental distress.
Disease-associated stigma toward people, regardless of infection status, has been seen in previous outbreaks of novel viruses.7, 8, 9, 10 For example, Mexican and Latinx individuals were s`hunned during the 2009 H1N1 pandemic owing to the virus’ link to hog farms where migrants worked.11 Family members of patients during the 2014 Ebola epidemic also faced stigmatization.12 Such stigmatization could be attributable to implicit cognition associating disease avoidance with these individuals.13 , 14
Discrimination toward people who share social or behavioral characteristics with COVID-19 patients but may not carry the novel virus, termed COVID-19–associated discrimination (CAD), was first seen in heightened anti-Chinese rhetoric online15 , 16 and a rapid accumulation of reports on in-person racist acts against Asians.17 Social media analyses showed a nearly 10-fold increase in the use of offensive language,18 , 19 which could be linkable to in-person hate incidents.20
Soon after, CAD expanded to a broader subset of Americans regardless of race/ethnicity. For instance, people wearing face masks were perceived as more likely to carry the virus,21 , 22 although mask wearing was recommended for healthy individuals.23 Essential workers in healthcare and service industries were considered high risk,24 , 25 when others were instructed to stay home.26 CAD was also seen in other countries.27
This study uses national survey data to examine CAD among U.S. residents when the pandemic escalated across the country. It explores the contribution of risk factors including (1) social and behavioral identifiers that might tie individuals to COVID-19 (e.g., race/ethnicity and mask wearing) and (2) in-person and social media encounters during which discriminatory acts might occur. Additionally, the study examines the association between perceived CAD and mental distress.
METHODS
Study Sample
The survey cohort came from the Understanding America Study (UAS), a nationally representative panel maintained by the Center for Economic and Social Research at the University of Southern California. The panel consists of a random sample of residents aged ≥18 years across the U.S., recruited using address-based sampling since 2014. Respondents were invited by mail and provided a tablet and broadband Internet if they did not have Internet access. They then answered survey questions on a computer, tablet, or smartphone. The design of the UAS is documented extensively elsewhere,28 and studies29 show that the UAS is comparable to other national panels including the Current Population Survey30 and Health and Retirement Study.31
Participants were surveyed on perceptions and responses to the COVID-19 pandemic,32 with a first survey fielded between March 10 and March 31, 2020 (March survey) and a second from April 1 to April 28 (April survey). Among the 8,502 invited to the March survey, 6,884 (81%) completed it. The April survey was part of a tracking series33 requiring a separate consent. Although all panelists were invited on March 29, only 5,891 consented by April 1, with 5,450 (93%) completing the April survey. Individuals who completed both surveys were included for analysis.
Because prior discrimination experience could be a potential confounder in this study, participants without such information were excluded from the analysis. Roughly 1 year before COVID-19 surveys, respondents were asked about discrimination experience in day-to-day life. Panelists at the time (6,708) were invited to this survey, with 5,569 (83%) completing it. The analytic sample was then restricted to the 3,665 respondents who completed all 3 surveys. The parent study protocol was approved by the IRB of University of Southern California.
Measures
This study assessed CAD using a 4-item scale adapted from the Everyday Discrimination Scale Short Version,34 abbreviated from a well-studied 9-item scale.35, 36, 37 Respondents answered yes, no, or unsure to the question of whether they had the following experiences due to people thinking they might have the coronavirus: treated with less courtesy and respect than others; receiving poorer service at restaurants or stores; people act as if they were afraid of them; threatened or harassed. Perceived CAD was identified if one reported yes to any of the experiences. Sensitivity analysis showed similar results when unsure was coded as no or missing.
Mental distress was assessed using the Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-4) validated in the general population.38 , 39 Respondents answered how often in the past 14 days they felt bothered by the following: feeling anxious; not being able to stop or control worrying; feeling depressed; having little interest in doing things. Response options included: not at all (0), several days (1), more than half the days (2), and nearly every day (3). Although the first 2 items assessed anxiety and the latter 2 assessed depression, the sum score measured overall mental distress.
The study also included social and behavioral risk factors of CAD. Both COVID-19 surveys asked respondents whether they had worn a face mask or covering in the past 7 days. Social media exposure was only assessed in March, as number of minutes a respondent spent on social media in a day on average, which was then categorized as: none (0), 1–30 minutes (1), 31–60 minutes (2), 61–120 minutes (3), and more than 120 minutes (4). Assuming that respondents’ social media usage was similar across months, responses were carried over to analyze the April data.
Working status was assessed in April as number of days in the past week a respondent worked at a job and days worked from home. The ratio between the 2 was then derived as: not working (0), working outside (1), working partially from home (2), and working completely from home (3). The April survey also asked about in-person interaction in the past 7 days (e.g., going to the grocery store or pharmacy or visiting others).
Discrimination experience before the pandemic was assessed between December 2018 and February 2019 by asking respondents if they had been treated unfairly or harassed because of their parents’ education or possessions. Prior mental distress was evaluated using the 8-item Center for Epidemiologic Studies–Depression Scale40 , 41 on a rolling basis—for this sample, 53% in 2018 and 39% in 2019. As prior data are often strong predictors of later observation on a similar construct, controlling for these prior measures was useful to parse out the impact of the pandemic.
Additional covariates were used in this study. The COVID-19 surveys asked about respondents’ symptoms experienced in the past 7 days (e.g., cough and fever). Although COVID-19 cannot be diagnosed based on symptoms alone, a combination of self-reported shortness of breath and fever, the 2 cardinal symptoms of the disease,42 was considered indicative of a respondent potentially having COVID-19. Sociodemographic data were collected periodically in the UAS—for this sample, between December 2019 and March 2020.
Two contextual measures were constructed from publicly available data, quantifying the progression of and policy response to the pandemic in the state in which the respondent resided on the date they responded. Disease prevalence was constructed by dividing the total number of confirmed cases documented by the New York Times43 over a 1-year estimate of the state population based on the 2018 American Community Survey.44 The number of days under states’ Shelter-in-Place (SIP) orders was calculated using the effective dates archived by the New York Times.26
Statistical Analysis
Analyses were performed using Stata, version 16 in May 2020 and restricted to respondents who completed both COVID-19 surveys and a prior survey on perceived discrimination (n=3,665). Differences in sample characteristics by whether one perceived CAD were tested using Pearson chi-square test for categorical variables and independent sample t-test for continuous variables.
Two distinct modeling strategies were used. The cross-sectional approach analyzed each month's data separately because people's exposure to CAD could take different forms before (March) and during (April) the implementation of SIP orders requiring most people to stay home. The longitudinal approach then compared across months by predicting the April dependent variable while controlling for its March counterpart.
Multivariable logistic regression was used to predict the probability of a person perceiving CAD as a function of risk factors (e.g., race/ethnicity and wearing face masks) and prior discrimination experience, with AOR quantifying association and predicted probability assessing absolute risk. Multivariable linear regression was then used to model mental distress (PHQ-4 score), using CAD as a predictor rather than an outcome as in the logistic regression, while controlling for prior mental distress. All models adjusted for COVID-19 prevalence in each state, number of days under the SIP order, self-reported symptoms, and demographic characteristics. SEs were clustered at the state level using the Eicker–Huber–White estimator,45 accounting for nonindependence of respondents who resided in the same state. Less than 5% of data were missing for each variable and list-wise deletion was used in analyses.
RESULTS
The study sample consisted of 3,665 U.S. adults aged ≥18 years. Appendix Table 1, available online, shows that three quarters (75%) were non-Hispanic White; more than half (55%) were female; three quarters (73%) were aged 18–64 years; and less than half (41%) reported having a bachelor's degree or higher. More than half (59%) had a household income <$75,000.
Compared with the excluded sample who completed COVID-19 surveys but not the prior discrimination survey, the analytic sample was more likely to be non-Hispanic White; male; older; and with higher income. Because the UAS oversampled Los Angeles County residents in the past year,46 46% of the excluded sample came from Los Angeles, driving the differences. Appendix Table 2, available online, shows a similar composition of included and excluded non–Los Angeles samples.
The overall percentage perceiving CAD doubled, from 4% in March to 10% in April. Table 1 contrasts data by perceived CAD. In both months, relative to respondents who did not experience CAD, those who did were more likely to be racial/ethnic minorities, immigrants, and younger, and to have disease-related symptoms, face mask usage, and prior discrimination experience. Additionally, those who perceived CAD in April tended to have a lower household income and reside in states with higher COVID-19 prevalence. Mental distress, during and before the pandemic, was higher for those perceiving CAD than for those who did not.
Table 1.
Comparison of Respondents Perceiving and Not Perceiving COVID-19–Associated Discrimination (n=3,665)
March survey |
April survey |
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Characteristics | No perceived discrimination (n=3,460) | Perceived discrimination (n=157) | No perceived discrimination (n=3,285) | Perceived discrimination (n=371) | ||
Race, n (%) | ||||||
White | 2,627 (76) | 86 (55) | *** | 2,499 (76) | 234 (63) | *** |
Black | 229 (7) | 23 (15) | 226 (7) | 36 (10) | ||
Hispanic | 317 (9) | 16 (10) | 289 (9) | 54 (15) | ||
Asian | 96 (3) | 21 (13) | 92 (3) | 26 (7) | ||
Other race/ethnicity | 185 (5) | 11 (7) | 176 (5) | 21 (6) | ||
Age, years, n (%) | ||||||
18‒34 | 411 (12) | 33 (21) | *** | 382 (12) | 63 (17) | *** |
35‒44 | 1,253 (36) | 74 (47) | 1,177 (36) | 166 (45) | ||
55‒64 | 825 (24) | 32 (20) | 782 (24) | 85 (23) | ||
≥65 | 971 (28) | 18 (11) | 944 (29) | 57 (15) | ||
Education, n (%) | ||||||
High school or less | 739 (21) | 36 (23) | 703 (21) | 93 (25) | ||
Some college | 1,301 (38) | 58 (37) | 1,236 (38) | 135 (36) | ||
Bachelor or higher | 1,420 (41) | 63 (40) | 1,346 (41) | 143 (39) | ||
Household income, n (%) | ||||||
<$25,000 | 607 (18) | 33 (21) | 571 (17) | 92 (25) | ** | |
$25,000–$49,999 | 722 (21) | 36 (23) | 692 (21) | 76 (21) | ||
$50,000–$74,999 | 694 (20) | 32 (20) | 643 (20) | 72 (19) | ||
>$75,000 | 1,430 (41) | 56 (36) | 1,374 (42) | 130 (35) | ||
Sex, n (%) | ||||||
Female | 1,914 (55) | 89 (57) | 1,832 (56) | 194 (52) | ||
Fever/chills and shortness of breath, n (%) | ||||||
Yes | 36 (1) | <10 (<6) | ** | 18 (1) | <10 (<3) | * |
Prior perceived discrimination, n (%) | ||||||
Yes | 954 (28) | 66 (42) | *** | 854 (26) | 169 (46) | *** |
Immigrant status, n (%) | ||||||
Nonimmigrant | 2,053 (61) | 85 (54) | *** | 1,976 (62) | 190 (52) | *** |
First-generation immigrant | 252 (7) | 25 (16) | 249 (8) | 31 (8) | ||
Second-generation immigrant | 356 (11) | 27 (17) | 319 (10) | 66 (18) | ||
Third-generation immigrant | 714 (21) | 20 (13) | 661 (21) | 78 (21) | ||
Wear a face mask, n (%) | ||||||
Yes | 229 (7) | 41 (26) | *** | 1,584 (48) | 215 (58) | *** |
Time spent on social media in a day on average, n (%) | ||||||
None | 727 (21) | 21 (14) | 701 (22) | 60 (16) | ||
1‒30 minutes | 1,192 (35) | 52 (34) | 1,126 (35) | 130 (35) | ||
31‒60 minutes | 722 (21) | 41 (27) | 688 (21) | 83 (22) | ||
61‒120 minutes | 535 (16) | 23 (15) | 494 (15) | 63 (17) | ||
>120 minutes | 259 (8) | 17 (11) | 242 (7) | 34 (9) | ||
Mental distress (PHQ-4), mean (IQR) | 1.70 (0‒2) | 3.05 (0‒5) | *** | 2.33 (0‒4) | 3.60 (1‒5) | *** |
Prior mental distress (CES-D 8), mean (IQR) | 1.50 (0‒2) | 2.39 (0‒4) | *** | 1.48 (0‒2) | 2.23 (0‒4) | *** |
Number of COVID-19 cases per 100,000 population in the state, mean (IQR) | 8.21 (0‒4) | 12.16 (1‒6) | 921.76 (286‒787) | 875.38 (298‒872) | ||
Number of days under state SIP order, mean (IQR) | 0.16 (0‒0) | 0.27 (0‒0) | 13.13 (7‒19) | 14.33 (9‒20) | ** | |
Work from home, n (%) | ||||||
Do not work | 1,705 (53) | 158 (45) | ** | |||
Work but no day WFH | 655 (20) | 97 (28) | ||||
Some days WFH | 156 (5) | 20 (6) | ||||
All days WFH | 681 (21) | 75 (21) | ||||
Social activities, n (%) | ||||||
Go out to a bar, club, or other places where people gather | 30 (1) | <10 (<3) | ||||
Go to grocery store or pharmacy | 2,535 (77) | 302 (81) | ||||
Go to a friend, neighbor, or relatives’ residence (that's not your own) | 691 (21) | 84 (23) | ||||
Have visitor such as friends, neighbors, or relatives at your residence | 694 (21) | 96 (26) | * | |||
Attend a gathering with more than 10 people such as a reunion, wedding, or funeral | 49 (1) | 10 (3) | ||||
Go outside to walk, hike, or exercise | 2,455 (75) | 275 (74) |
Note: Boldface indicates statistical significance (*p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001) using chi-square test for categorical variables and independent sample t-test for continuous variables.
CES-D 8, 8-item Center for Epidemiologic Studies–Depression Scale; PHQ-4, 4-item Patient Health Questionnaire; SIP, Shelter-in-Place; WFH, working from home.
Cross-sectional analyses (Table 2 ) show the AOR of perceiving CAD in March. In addition to race/ethnicity, risk factors, including prior discrimination experience, immigrant status, mask wearing, and social media usage, entered the model one at a time to illustrate their incremental contribution and potential mediation of the racial/ethnic effects. Compared with non-Hispanic Whites, more non-Hispanic Blacks perceived CAD (AOR=2.69, p<0.001 in Model 4). Asians also had a higher likelihood, but the magnitude reduced when immigration status and face mask usage entered the model (AOR=5.79, p<0.001 in Model 1; AOR=3.71, p<0.001 in Model 2; AOR=3.04, p<0.001 in Model 3), suggesting their potential to mediate the association between being Asian and perceiving CAD. Those who experienced discrimination before also experienced more CAD than those who did not (AOR=1.74, p<0.01), as did respondents who wore face masks in March relative to those who did not (AOR=3.51, p<0.001).
Table 2.
AORs of Perceiving COVID-19–Associated Discrimination in March (n=3,665)
Variable | Model 0 | Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 3 | Model 4 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Racea | |||||
Black |
2.83 (1.82, 4.39)*** |
2.87 (1.86, 4.42)*** |
2.84 (1.81, 4.44)b |
2.51 (1.59, 3.95)*** |
2.69 (1.69, 4.28)*** |
Hispanic | 1.08 (0.57, 2.03) |
1.02 (0.53, 1.96) |
0.71 (0.27, 1.84) |
0.68 (0.28, 1.68) |
0.70 (0.28, 1.75) |
Asian |
5.62 (3.38, 9.32)*** |
5.79 (3.41, 9.85)*** |
3.71 (2.54, 5.44)b |
3.04 (2.03, 4.55)*** |
3.23 (2.12, 4.92)*** |
Other race/ethnicity | 1.56 (0.79, 3.09) |
1.58 (0.79, 3.14) |
1.46 (0.78, 2.73) |
1.39 (0.74, 2.62) |
1.50 (0.80, 2.83) |
Prior perceived discrimination | |||||
Yes | — |
1.76 (1.23, 2.51)** |
1.74 (1.21, 2.52)c |
1.70 (1.17, 2.48)** |
1.74 (1.17, 2.57)** |
Immigrant statusb | |||||
First-generation immigrant | — | — | 1.76 (0.97, 3.21) |
1.64 (1.02, 2.64)* |
1.64 (1.00, 2.70) |
Second-generation immigrant | — | — | 1.76 (0.99, 3.10) |
1.75 (0.99, 3.09) |
1.76 (1.00, 3.17) |
Third-generation immigrant | — | — | 0.97 (0.60, 1.59) |
0.97 (0.59, 1.60) |
1.03 (0.63, 1.69) |
Wear a face mask | |||||
Yes | — | — | — |
3.63 (2.56, 5.15)*** |
3.51 (2.46, 5.02)*** |
Time spent on social media in a day on averagec | |||||
1‒30 minutes | — | — | — | — | 1.28 (0.79, 2.07) |
31‒60 minutes | — | — | — | — | 1.67 (0.98, 2.85) |
61‒120 minutes | — | — | — | — | 1.09 (0.70, 1.69) |
>120 minutes | — | — | — | — | 1.75 (0.91, 3.34) |
Note: Boldface indicates statistical significance (*p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001) using multivariable logistic regression. Values in parentheses are 95% CI. All models adjusted for age, education, sex, household income, self-reported symptom, COVID-19 prevalence in the state a respondent resided, and the number of days under the state's Shelter-in-Place order on the day of the response, with Eicker–Huber–White robust SEs clustered at the state level.
Non-Hispanic White as reference category.
Nonimmigrant as reference category.
Spent no time on social media in a day on average as reference category.
Table 3 presents the AOR of perceiving CAD in April including risk factors presented in the March analysis (Model 4), whereas Appendix Table 3, available online, includes the summary for Models 0–3. Because the April survey collected data on respondents’ working status and social activities, these measures were added to Models 5 and 6. Compared with non-Hispanic Whites, non-Hispanic Blacks (AOR=1.80, p<0.001 in Model 6) and Asians (AOR=2.02, p<0.05) were more likely to experience CAD, but the racial/ethnic gaps were smaller than those observed in March. Prior discrimination experience remained a strong predictor of CAD (AOR=2.18, p<0.001).
Table 3.
AORs of Perceiving COVID-19–Associated Discrimination in April (n=3,665)
Variable | Model 4 | Model 5 | Model 6 | Longitudinal analysisa |
---|---|---|---|---|
Raceb | ||||
Black |
1.70 (1.22, 2.36)** |
1.80 (1.30, 2.48)*** |
1.80 (1.30, 2.51)*** |
1.53 (1.05, 2.23)* |
Hispanic | 1.07 (0.71, 1.60) |
0.98 (0.67, 1.44) |
0.97 (0.66, 1.43) |
1.09 (0.71, 1.67) |
Asian |
2.34 (1.19, 4.61)* |
2.07 (1.08, 3.94)* |
2.02 (1.07, 3.80)* |
1.35 (0.77, 2.38) |
Other race/ethnicity | 1.11 (0.60, 2.05) |
1.20 (0.65, 2.22) |
1.20 (0.66, 2.19) |
1.14 (0.57, 2.26) |
Prior perceived discrimination | ||||
Yes |
2.19 (1.74, 2.76)*** |
2.15 (1.67, 2.75)*** |
2.18 (1.69, 2.80)*** |
2.14 (1.60, 2.87)*** |
Immigrant statusc | ||||
First-generation immigrant | 0.91 (0.47, 1.78) |
0.98 (0.51, 1.88) |
0.95 (0.48, 1.87) |
0.90 (0.42, 1.96) |
Second-generation immigrant |
1.73 (1.20, 2.48)** |
1.85 (1.24, 2.77)** |
1.86 (1.27, 2.73)** |
1.80 (1.26, 2.58)** |
Third-generation immigrant |
1.59 (1.21, 2.10)** |
1.64 (1.24, 2.18)** |
1.65 (1.24, 2.19)** |
1.67 (1.26, 2.23)*** |
Wear a face mask | ||||
Yes |
1.35 (1.04, 1.75)* |
1.34 (1.02, 1.75)* |
1.30 (0.99, 1.71) |
1.21 (0.90, 1.63) |
Time spent on social media in a day on averaged | ||||
1‒30 minutes | 1.30 (0.99, 1.70) |
1.33 (1.03, 1.73)* |
1.30 (1.00, 1.69) |
1.27 (0.96, 1.70) |
31‒60 minutes | 1.31 (0.90, 1.89) |
1.33 (0.89, 2.00) |
1.31 (0.86, 1.99) |
1.22 (0.75, 2.00) |
61‒120 minutes | 1.27 (0.93, 1.74) |
1.33 (0.97, 1.81) |
1.27 (0.93, 1.75) |
1.28 (0.92, 1.79) |
>120 minutes |
1.47 (1.03, 2.10)* |
1.59 (1.09, 2.32)* |
1.58 (1.09, 2.29)* |
1.50 (1.02, 2.21)* |
Work from home by wearing a face maske | ||||
Work but no day WFH and no mask | — | 1.29 (0.76, 2.20) 0.35 |
1.26 (0.75, 2.11) |
1.32 (0.74, 2.33) |
Some days WFH and no mask | — | 1.61 (0.84, 3.09) 0.15 |
1.55 (0.79, 3.00) |
1.51 (0.74, 3.07) |
All days WFH and no mask | — | 1.12 (0.85, 1.48) 0.42 |
1.11 (0.83, 1.48) |
1.16 (0.83, 1.61) |
Work but no day WFH and wear mask | — | 1.52 (1.13, 2.05) 0.01 |
1.58 (1.17, 2.14)** |
1.73 (1.25, 2.41)** |
Some days WFH and wear mask | — | 1.10 (0.58, 2.12) 0.76 |
1.12 (0.59, 2.11) |
1.08 (0.57, 2.07) |
All days WFH and wear mask | — | 1.20 (0.88, 1.63) 0.26 |
1.23 (0.89, 1.69) |
1.25 (0.87, 1.78) |
Social activities | ||||
Go out to a bar, club, or other places where people gather | — | — | 1.17 (0.47, 2.89) |
0.88 (0.25, 3.05) |
Go to grocery store or pharmacy | — | — | 1.10 (0.82, 1.47) |
1.10 (0.80, 1.50) |
Go to a friend, neighbor, or relatives’ residence (that's not your own) | — | — | 0.94 (0.73, 1.22) |
0.94 (0.73, 1.23) |
Have visitor such as friends, neighbors, or relatives at your residence | — | — |
1.32 (1.08, 1.60)** |
1.29 (1.04, 1.61)* |
Attend a gathering with more than 10 people such as a reunion, wedding, or funeral | — | — | 1.79 (0.89, 3.60) |
2.09 (1.06, 4.11)* |
Go outside to walk, hike, or exercise | — | — | 1.18 (0.89, 1.57) |
1.22 (0.86, 1.73) |
Note: Boldface indicates statistical significance (*p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001) using multivariable logistic regression. Values in parentheses are 95% CI. All models adjusted for age, education, sex, household income, self-reported symptom, COVID-19 prevalence in the state a respondent resided, and the number of days under the state's Shelter-in-Place order on the day of the response, with Eicker–Huber–White robust SEs clustered at the state level.
Also controlled for March perception in COVID-19 associated discrimination in the longitudinal analysis.
Non-Hispanic White as reference category.
Nonimmigrant as reference category.
Spent no time on social media in a day on average as reference category.
Did not work as reference category.
WFH, working from home.
In April, respondents who were second- (AOR=1.86, p<0.01) and third- (AOR=1.65, p<0.001) generation immigrants experienced more CAD relative to nonimmigrants. Frequent social media users, who spent >2 hours in a day on average, were also at a higher risk than those who did not use social media (AOR=1.58, p<0.05). To understand the association with use of face masks, reports of mask wearing were interacted with respondents’ working status, given that most states implemented SIP orders in April requiring everyone except essential workers to stay home. Compared with people who did not work, only those who worked outside and wore face masks perceived more CAD (AOR=1.58, p<0.01). No difference was found for those who worked outside but did not wear face masks (AOR=1.26, p>0.05) nor those who worked partially or fully from home regardless of face mask usage. Engagement in social activities in which in-person discrimination might occur (e.g., going to the grocery store or pharmacy) was not associated with more discrimination except for those with visitors at their own residence (AOR=1.32, p<0.01).
Figure 1 illustrates the predicted absolute risk by race/ethnicity, adjusted for risk factors and covariates in the cross-sectional analyses. In both months, non-Hispanic Blacks (absolute risk [AR]=0.09, 95% CI=0.059, 0.122 for March, Model 4; AR=0.151, 95% CI=0.115, 0.187 for April, Model 6) and Asians (AR=0.105, 95% CI=0.070, 0.140 for March, Model 4; AR=0.165, 95% CI=0.086, 0.244 for April, Model 6) had higher risk than other racial/ethnic groups (AR ranged from 0.027 to 0.054 in March and 0.090 to 0.108 in April). From March to April, the risk increased for all racial/ethnic groups including non-Hispanic Whites, although it was less evident for Asians. Mask wearing also increased the likelihood of perceiving CAD, persistent in both March (AR=0.112, 95% CI=0.082, 0.142 for wearing; AR=0.037, 95% CI=0.031, 0.042 for not wearing) and April (AR=0.144, 95% CI=0.110, 0.178 for wearing and working completely outside home; otherwise, ARs ranged from 0.079 to 0.111).
Figure 1.
Predicted probability for racial/ethnic groups experiencing COVID-19–associated discrimination (n=3,665). All models adjusted for age, education, sex, household income, self-reported symptom, COVID-19 prevalence in the state a respondent resided, and the number of days under the state's Shelter-in-Place order on the day of the response, with Eicker–Huber–White robust SEs clustered at the state level. NH, non-Hispanic.
Longitudinal analysis (Table 3, last column) associated the increase in perceiving CAD over time with several aforementioned risk factors, including being non-Hispanic Black; a second- or third-generation immigrant; and a frequent social media user (i.e., spending >2 hours in a day on average), as well as having prior discrimination experiences and a combination of working outside and using a face mask. Different from the cross-sectional analysis (Table 3, Model 6), attending a gathering with >10 people in April significantly predicted the increased CAD perception over time. Relative to non-Hispanic Whites, Asians did not experience more CAD in April after accounting for their experience in March (AOR=1.35, p>0.05).
In addition to assessing factors associated with perceived CAD, subsequent analyses examined the relationship between CAD and mental health factors to determine potential impact (Appendix Table 4, available online). Cross-sectional analysis revealed that perceived CAD predicted a higher PHQ-4 score in March (b=0.77, p<0.001) and April (b=1.01, p<0.001) after adjusting for the prior score on 8-item Center for Epidemiologic Studies–Depression Scale and person- and state-level covariates (e.g., demographics and COVID-19 prevalence). The longitudinal approach associated perceived CAD with the PHQ-4 score in April after controlling for PHQ-4 score in March (b=0.50, p<0.001) and the same set of covariates.
DISCUSSION
Anecdotal discriminatory acts amid the COVID-19 pandemic have been widely documented in media reports. To the authors’ knowledge, this study provides the first systematic assessment on how perceived CAD is associated with potential risk factors (e.g., race/ethnicity and social media usage) and mental distress. Given the representativeness of this sample, the authors believe that these results are generalizable to U.S. residents.
The findings suggest that perceptions of CAD are increasing although the overall prevalence remains relatively low. Although all racial/ethnic groups have experienced CAD, disparities are observed. Elevated risk among Asians in March confirms the media reports on hate crimes,47 and it was in part mediated by their immigration status and use of face masks. Although their risk also appeared higher than non-Hispanic Whites in April, it was mostly carried over from March. Non-Hispanic Blacks are another group who persistently experience more CAD, and their risk increased from March to April. This could reflect the longstanding stereotypes associating non-Hispanic Blacks with the spread of infectious diseases,48 , 49 whereas the increased risk in April could be attributable to media coverage on their disproportionately higher mortality rate because of COVID-19.50, 51, 52 Smaller racial/ethnic gaps in April than March could be due to the increase in CAD among non-Hispanic Whites.
Besides race/ethnicity, wearing face masks is another risk factor consistently associated with CAD. This confirms media reports on the stigmatization of mask wearing during the pandemic,21 , 22 reflecting longstanding bias in the West53 and contradicting public messaging across different disease control phases.54 , 55 In addition to mask-related cultural factors studied in previous outbreaks,56 perceived CAD because of mask wearing could be attributable to the spotlight effect, in which individuals overestimate others’ attention directed toward them when their appearances are different from people around.57 , 58 As many states transit to lifting SIP orders and reopen businesses and public activities, CAD associated with mask wearing, a highly recommended disease control metric,59 calls for public awareness. This may ease as more authorities start to mandate face coverings in public60 and wearing face masks becomes commonplace.
The results also point to CAD's potential association with other risk factors such as heavy social media usage (i.e., spending >2 hours on a day on average), being an immigrant, and engagement in some social activities. However, such relationships seem less robust and need further confirmation.
As for potential health outcomes of CAD, this study showcases substantial evidence linking CAD to increased mental distress, consistent with literature associating general discrimination with poorer mental health, especially among racial/ethnic minorities.61 , 62 The relationship is particularly pertinent during this pandemic as it compounds mental health distress63 , 64 attributable to concerns of disease spread, austere social restrictions, and financial stress.
Limitations
Findings from this study should be interpreted in light of several limitations. First, the analysis was based on observational data. The reported correlational relationships need further confirmation in stronger designs such as a field experiment. Additionally, CAD was operationalized as people's self-reported perception instead of direct observation. Other data types detailing occasions or types of discrimination might provide an in-depth view. Moreover, risk factors examined here are limited. For example, coughing in public was not covered in the data although anecdotal evidence associates it with disease avoidance. Finally, mental health was assessed by perceived mental distress instead of mental illness as in other studies.61
CONCLUSIONS
The impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on public health, including mental health, will continue to reverberate for years to come. This study's initial findings of perceived CAD and its association with poorer mental health underscore the existing racial/ethnic inequalities experienced by many non-Hispanic Blacks and Asians. The findings also suggested discrimination toward those who wear face masks, signaling the need to raise public awareness, especially as economies reopen and wearing a face mask is highly recommended to prevent transmission.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The project described in this paper relies on data from surveys administered by the Understanding America Study (UAS), which is maintained by the Center for Economic and Social Research (CESR) at the University of Southern California (USC). The content of this paper is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of USC or UAS. For questions or additional information about the UAS, contact Tania Gutsche, Project and Panel Manager, CESR, USC, at tgutsche@usc.edu. UAS is funded from several sources, including the Social Security Administration and the National Institute on Aging under grant 5U01AG054580. The UAS coronavirus surveys referred to in this paper were initiated and financed by USC and funded in part by the Bill & Melinda Gates Foundation. The parent study protocol was approved by the USC IRB, and data access was granted by UAS’ data user agreement.
Ms. Brenneke's work was supported by the National Institute on Drug Abuse (T32DA007292) and Dr. Le's work was supported by a training grant from the National Institute of Mental Health (T32MH103210). The research presented in this paper is that of the authors and does not reflect the official policy of NIH. No financial disclosures were reported by authors of this paper.
YL and BKF conceived the study and analyzed and interpreted the data; all authors contributed to drafting and revising the article. The contents of the article have not been presented or published elsewhere.
Footnotes
Supplemental materials associated with this article can be found in the online version at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2020.06.007.
Appendix. SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL
REFERENCES
- 1.Reducing stigma. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. https://www.cdc.gov/coronavirus/2019-ncov/daily-life-coping/reducing-stigma.html. Updated June 11, 2020. Accessed June 22, 2020.
- 2.WHO. Social stigma associated with COVID-19. Geneva, Switzerland: WHO.https://www.unicef.org/media/65931/file/Social%20stigma%20associated%20with%20the%20coronavirus%20disease%202019%20(COVID-19).pdf. Published February 24, 2020. Accessed June 22, 2020.
- 3.Fischer LS, Mansergh G, Lynch J, Santibanez S. Addressing disease-related stigma during infectious disease outbreaks. Disaster Med Public Health Prep. 2019;13(5–6):989–994. doi: 10.1017/dmp.2018.157. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Person B, Sy F, Holton K, Govert B, Liang A, et al. National Center for Infectious Diseases/SARS Community Outreach Team. Fear and stigma: the epidemic within the SARS outbreak. Emerg Infect Dis. 2004;10(2):358–363. doi: 10.3201/eid1002.030750. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Hatzenbuehler ML, Phelan JC, Link BG. Stigma as a fundamental cause of population health inequalities. Am J Public Health. 2013;103(5):813–821. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2012.301069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Earnshaw VA, Quinn DM. Influenza stigma during the 2009 H1N1 pandemic. J Appl Soc Psychol. 2013;43(suppl 1):E109–E114. doi: 10.1111/jasp.12049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Holzemer WL, Makoae LN, Greeff M, et al. Measuring HIV stigma for PLHAs and nurses over time in five African countries. SAHARA J. 2009;6(2):76–82. doi: 10.1080/17290376.2009.9724933. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Verhaeghe M, Bracke P. Associative stigma among mental health professionals: implications for professional and service user well-being. J Health Soc Behav. 2012;53(1):17–32. doi: 10.1177/0022146512439453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Stangl AL, Earnshaw VA, Logie CH, et al. The Health Stigma and Discrimination Framework: a global, crosscutting framework to inform research, intervention development, and policy on health-related stigmas. BMC Med. 2019;17:31. doi: 10.1186/s12916-019-1271-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Eichelberger L. SARS and New York's Chinatown: the politics of risk and blame during an epidemic of fear. Soc Sci Med. 2007;65(6):1284–1295. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.04.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.McCauley M, Minsky S, Viswanath K. The H1N1 pandemic: media frames, stigmatization and coping. BMC Public Health. 2013;13:1116. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-13-1116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Yadav S, Rawal G. The current mental health status of Ebola survivors in western Africa. J Clin Diagn Res. 2015;9(10):LA01–LA02. doi: 10.7860/JCDR/2015/15127.6559. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Faulkner J, Schaller M, Park JH, Duncan LA. Evolved disease-avoidance mechanisms and contemporary xenophobic attitudes. Group Process Intergroup Relat. 2004;7(4):333–353. doi: 10.1177/1368430204046142. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Schaller M, Neuberg SL. Danger, disease, and the nature of prejudice(s) Adv Exp Soc Psychol. 2012;46:1–54. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-394281-4.00001-5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Kelly L.Trump. GOP ‘dog whistle’ over coronavirus inflames anti-Chinese rhetoric online: analysis. The Hill. March 18, 2020 https://thehill.com/policy/technology/488236-trump-gop-dog-whistle-on-coronavirus-inflames-anti-chinese-rhetoric-online [Google Scholar]
- 16.Timberg C, Chiu A. As the coronavirus spreads, so does online racism targeting Asians, new research shows. Washington Post. April 9, 2020 https://www.washingtonpost.com/technology/2020/04/08/coronavirus-spreads-so-does-online-racism-targeting-asians-new-research-shows/ [Google Scholar]
- 17.Yoshiko Kandil C. Asian Americans report over 650 racist acts over last week, new data says. NBC News. March 27, 2020 https://www.nbcnews.com/news/asian-america/asian-americans-report-nearly-500-racist-acts-over-last-week-n1169821 [Google Scholar]
- 18.Budhwani H, Sun R. Creating COVID-19 stigma by referencing the novel coronavirus as the “Chinese virus” on Twitter: quantitative analysis of social media data. J Med Internet Res. 2020;22(5):e19301. doi: 10.2196/19301. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Stechemesser A, Wenz L, Levermann A. Corona crisis fuels racially profiled hate in social media networks. EClinicalMedicine. 2020;23 doi: 10.1016/j.eclinm.2020.100372. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Williams ML, Burnap P, Javed A, Liu H, Ozalp S. Hate in the machine: anti-black and anti-Muslim social media posts as predictors of offline racially and religiously aggravated crime. Br J Criminol. 2020;60(1):93–117. doi: 10.1093/bjc/azz049. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Taylor DB. For black men, fear that masks will invite racial profiling. The New York Times. April 14, 2020 https://www.nytimes.com/2020/04/14/us/coronavirus-masks-racism-african-americans.html. Updated May 26, 2020. Accessed June 22, 2020. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Hay A, Caspani M. Fake flyers and face-mask fear: California fights coronavirus discrimination. Reuters. February 14, 2020 https://www.reuters.com/article/us-china-health-usa-discrimination/fake-flyers-and-face-mask-fear-california-fights-coronavirus-discrimination-idUSKBN208063 [Google Scholar]
- 23.Feng S, Shen C, Xia N, Song W, Fan M, Cowling BJ. Rational use of face masks in the COVID-19 pandemic. Lancet Respir Med. 2020;8(5):434–436. doi: 10.1016/S2213-2600(20)30134-X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.White D. Slashed tires and violence: health care workers face new dangers amid COVID-19 battle. The Miami Herald. April 13, 2020 https://www.miamiherald.com/news/coronavirus/article241967281.html [Google Scholar]
- 25.Stone JR. Coronavirus pandemic: Bay Area grocery store workers fear for their safety. ABC7 News. April 22, 2020 https://abc7news.com/bay-area-coronavirus-update-california-shelter-in-place-lockdown/6122342/ [Google Scholar]
- 26.Mervosh S, Lu D, Swales V. See which states and cities have told residents to stay at home. The New York Times. Updated April 20, 2020.https://www.nytimes.com/interactive/2020/us/coronavirus-stay-at-home-order.html. Accessed June 22, 2020.
- 27.He J, He L, Zhou W, Nie X, He M. Discrimination and social exclusion in the outbreak of COVID-19. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(8):2933. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17082933. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Understanding America Study methodology. University of Southern California. https://uasdata.usc.edu/page/Methodology. Updated 2017. Accessed June 22, 2020.
- 29.Angrisani M, Finley B, Kapteyn A. Can Internet match high-quality traditional surveys? Comparing the Health and Retirement Study and its online version. In: The Econometrics of Complex Survey Data (Advances in Econometrics, Vol. 39). United Kingdom: Emerald Publishing Limited; 2019:3–33. 10.1108/S0731-905320190000039001. [DOI]
- 30.Current Population Survey (CPS). U.S. Census Bureau. https://www.census.gov/programs-surveys/cps.html. Accessed June 22, 2020.
- 31.The Health and Retirement Study. University of Michigan. https://hrs.isr.umich.edu/. Accessed June 22, 2020.
- 32.The Understanding America Study: coronavirus in America (COVID) survey. University of Southern California. https://uasdata.usc.edu/page/Covid-19+Home. Updated 2020. Accessed May 1, 2020.
- 33.Kapteyn A, Marco A, Bennett D, et al. Tracking the effect of the COVID-19 pandemic on the lives of American households. Survey Research Methods. 2020;14(2):179–186. doi: 10.18148/srm/2020.v14i2.7737. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Sternthal MJ, Slopen N, Williams DR. Racial disparities in health: how much does stress really matter? Du Bois Rev. 2011;8(1):95–113. doi: 10.1017/S1742058X11000087. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Williams DR, Yu Y, Jackson JS, Anderson NB. Racial differences in physical and mental health: socio-economic status, stress and discrimination. J Health Psychol. 1997;2(3):335–351. doi: 10.1177/135910539700200305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Krieger N, Smith K, Naishadham D, Hartman C, Barbeau EM. Experiences of discrimination: validity and reliability of a self-report measure for population health research on racism and health. Soc Sci Med. 2005;61(7):1576–1596. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.03.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Taylor TR, Kamarck TW, Shiffman S. Validation of the Detroit area study discrimination scale in a community sample of older African American adults: the Pittsburgh Healthy Heart Project. Int J Behav Med. 2004;11(2):88–94. doi: 10.1207/s15327558ijbm1102_4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JB, Löwe B. An ultra-brief screening scale for anxiety and depression: the PHQ–4. Psychosomatics. 2009;50(6):613–621. doi: 10.1176/appi.psy.50.6.613. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Löwe B, Wahl I, Rose M, et al. A 4-item measure of depression and anxiety: validation and standardization of the Patient Health Questionnaire-4 (PHQ-4) in the general population. J Affect Disord. 2010;122(1–2):86–95. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2009.06.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Radloff LS. The CES-D Scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psychol Meas. 1977;1(3):385–401. doi: 10.1177/014662167700100306. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Bracke P, Levecque K, Van de Velde S. The psychometric properties of the CES-D 8 depression inventory and the estimation of cross-national differences in the true prevalence of depression. Ghent, Belgium: Ghent University; 2008.https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Piet_Bracke/publication/237379690_The_psychometric_properties_of_the_CES-D_8_depression_inventory_and_the_estimation_of_cross-national_differences_in_the_true_prevalence_of_depression/links/00b7d52e119c9bc4b3000000.pdf.
- 42.Symptoms of coronavirus. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention.https://www.cdc.gov/coronavirus/2019-ncov/symptoms-testing/symptoms.html. Updated May 13, 2020. Accessed June 22, 2020.
- 43.Coronavirus (COVID-19) data in the United States. The New York Times.https://github.com/nytimes/covid-19-data. Updated May 14, 2020. Accessed June 22, 2020.
- 44.American Community Study. 2018 data release new and notable. U.S. Census Bureau. https://www.census.gov/programs-surveys/acs/news/data-releases/2018/release.html#par_textimage_copy. Updated February 13, 2020. Accessed June 22, 2020.
- 45.Abadie A, Athey S, Imbens GW, Wooldridge J. When should you adjust standard errors for clustering? Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research. 10.3386/w24003. Published November 2017. [DOI]
- 46.Understanding America Study: Los Angeles barometer. University of Southern California. https://uasdata.usc.edu/page/Los+Angeles+Barometer. Updated 2020.
- 47.Tavernise S, Oppel RA., Jr Spit on, yelled at, attacked: Chinese-Americans fear for their safety. The New York Times. March 23, 2020 https://www.nytimes.com/2020/03/23/us/chinese-coronavirus-racist-attacks.html [Google Scholar]
- 48.O'Shea BA, Watson DG, Brown GDA, Fincher CL. Infectious disease prevalence, not race exposure, predicts both implicit and explicit racial prejudice across the United States. Soc Psychol Personal Sci. 2020;11(3):345–355. doi: 10.1177/1948550619862319. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Devine PG, Elliot AJ. Are racial stereotypes really fading? The Princeton Trilogy revisited. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 1995;21(11):1139–1150. doi: 10.1177/01461672952111002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Thebault R, Ba Tran A, Williams V. The coronavirus is infecting and killing black Americans at an alarmingly high rate. Washington Post. April 7, 2020 https://www.washingtonpost.com/nation/2020/04/07/coronavirus-is-infecting-killing-black-americans-an-alarmingly-high-rate-post-analysis-shows/?arc404=true [Google Scholar]
- 51.Maqbool A. Coronavirus: why has the virus hit African Americans so hard? BBC News. April 11, 2020 https://www.bbc.com/news/world-us-canada-52245690 [Google Scholar]
- 52.Erdman SL. Black communities account for disproportionate number of Covid-19 deaths in the U.S., study finds. CNN. May 6, 2020 https://www.cnn.com/2020/05/05/health/coronavirus-african-americans-study/index.html [Google Scholar]
- 53.Friedman U. Face masks are in. Atlantic. April 2, 2020 https://www.theatlantic.com/politics/archive/2020/04/america-asia-face-mask-coronavirus/609283/ [Google Scholar]
- 54.Leung H. Why wearing a face mask is encouraged in Asia, but shunned in the U.S. Time. March 12, 2020 https://time.com/5799964/coronavirus-face-mask-asia-us/ [Google Scholar]
- 55.Parker M, Fabian J. Trump won't wear a face mask – ‘It's only a recommendation’ - even though it can stop COVID-19 spread. National Post. April 4, 2020 https://nationalpost.com/news/world/its-only-a-recommendation-trump-wont-wear-mask-even-though-cdc-says-it-can-stop-covid-19-spread [Google Scholar]
- 56.Lau JT, Yang X, Tsui HY, Pang E. SARS related preventive and risk behaviours practised by Hong Kong-mainland China cross border travellers during the outbreak of the SARS epidemic in Hong Kong. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2004;58(12):988–996. doi: 10.1136/jech.2003.017483. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Gilovich T, Medvec VH, Savitsky K. The spotlight effect in social judgment: an egocentric bias in estimates of the salience of one's own actions and appearance. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2000;78(2):211–222. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.78.2.211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Crosby JR, King M, Savitsky K. The minority spotlight effect. Soc Psychol Personal Sci. 2014;5(7):743–750. doi: 10.1177/1948550614527625. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Use of cloth face coverings to help slow the spread of COVID-19. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. https://www.cdc.gov/coronavirus/2019-ncov/prevent-getting-sick/diy-cloth-face-coverings.html. Updated June 28, 2020. Accessed August 18, 2020.
- 60.Sharp S, Fry H. L.A. now requires face coverings even when outside. Here are the rules. Los Angeles Times. May 14, 2020 https://www.latimes.com/california/story/2020-05-14/l-a-now-requires-face-coverings-even-when-outside-here-are-the-rules [Google Scholar]
- 61.Brown TN, Williams DR, Jackson JS, et al. “Being black and feeling blue”: the mental health consequences of racial discrimination. Race Soc. 2000;2(2):117–131. doi: 10.1016/S1090-9524(00)00010-3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Wei M, Ku TY, Russell DW, Mallinckrodt B, Liao KY. Moderating effects of three coping strategies and self-esteem on perceived discrimination and depressive symptoms: a minority stress model for Asian international students. J Couns Psychol. 2008;55(4):451–462. doi: 10.1037/a0012511. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Rajkumar RP. COVID-19 and mental health: a review of the existing literature. Asian J Psychiatr. 2020;52 doi: 10.1016/j.ajp.2020.102066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.McGinty EE, Presskreischer R, Han H, Barry CL. Psychological distress and loneliness reported by U.S. adults in 2018 and April 2020. JAMA. 2020;324(1):93–94. doi: 10.1001/jama.2020.9740. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.