Abstract
Posterior parietal cortex (PPC) activity correlates with monkeys’ decisions during visual discrimination and categorization tasks. Yet recent work questioned whether decision-correlated PPC activity plays a causal role in such decisions. That study focused on PPC’s contribution to motor aspects of decisions (deciding where to move), but not sensory evaluation aspects (deciding what you are looking at). We employed reversible inactivation to compare PPC’s contributions to motor and sensory aspects of decisions. Inactivation affected both aspects of behavior, but preferentially impaired decisions when visual stimuli, rather than motor response targets, were in the inactivated visual field (IVF). This demonstrates a causal role for PPC in decision making, with preferential involvement in evaluating attended task-relevant sensory stimuli compared to motor planning.
One Sentence Summary:
Using reversible inactivation and electrophysiological recordings, we demonstrate that the primate PPC plays a causal role in perceptual and categorical decisions, with a preferential role in evaluating sensory stimuli compared to motor planning.
Decision-making requires evaluating task-relevant sensory stimuli to select appropriate motor responses. The primate PPC is well suited to mediate decision making because of its anatomical position at a midpoint in the sensory-cognitive-motor cortical hierarchy(1–3). Indeed, there is a correlation between PPC activity and monkeys’ decisions during visual discrimination and categorization tasks(4–12). Much of this work has focused on the lateral intraparietal area (LIP) (13–15). Yet, PPC’s role in sensory stimulus evaluation and motor planning aspects of decisions has been debated. Recent work showed that reversible inactivation of LIP does not affect motor aspects of decisions during a visual motion discrimination task(16). This leaves open the possibility that LIP is instead more engaged in processes related to evaluating sensory stimuli (e.g. stimulus feature processing, perceptual or abstract categorization, and selective attention), as opposed to selecting motor responses, during decision making. Furthermore, neuronal recordings have demonstrated correlates of more flexible categorical or rule-based tasks in PPC(13), raising the possibility that the primate PPC may be more engaged in decisions requiring greater cognitive flexibility or abstraction than in the classic perceptual decision tasks.
We directly compared LIP’s role in sensory stimulus evaluation and motor planning functions during flexible visual perceptual and categorical decisions. We reversibly inactivated LIP in one hemisphere with intracortical injections of the muscimol during several variants of motion-direction categorization (MDC) and motion-direction discrimination (MDD) tasks. Within each task, monkeys reported their decisions about the category membership (MDC task) or motion direction (MDD task) of a sample stimulus with a saccade to a target of the associated color (Fig. 1A). The mappings between motion categories/directions and target color were arbitrarily assigned at the start of training (and fixed across the study), with the two categories (MDC) or directions (MDD) assigned to red and green targets, respectively. Because the locations of the red and green targets were randomly interleaved across trials, there was no fixed mapping between motion category/direction and saccade direction. In the MDC task, 10 motion directions were shown at three angular distances from the category boundary (Fig. 1B). In the MDD task, two directions of motion were shown each at three levels of coherence (i.e. signal to noise). To assess LIP’s contribution to stimulus evaluation and motor planning, each task was tested in blocks of trials using three spatial configurations of motion stimuli and saccade targets with respect to the location of the IVF contralateral to muscimol injection (Fig. 1C). In the “Stimulus IN” (SIN) condition, the motion stimulus was shown within the IVF, with both saccade targets outside the IVF. In the “Target IN” (TIN) condition, the motion stimulus was outside the IVF and one of the colored saccade targets was shown in the IVF. In the “Both OUT” (BOUT) condition, neither the motion stimuli nor saccade targets were in the IVF.
We examined the impact of LIP inactivation on evaluating visual motion in the SIN condition (Fig. 2). LIP inactivation produced a significant behavioral impairment in both the MDC and MDD tasks, shown by decrease in overall accuracy (Fig. 2, A,E,G and K; MDC: p(accuracy) =2.2e-08, t(37) = -7.1; MDD: p(accuracy) = 5.8e-11, t(37) = -9.1, inactivation vs control, unpaired t-test) and increase in reaction time (RT) (Fig. 2, B,F,H and L) (MDC: p(RT) = 0.0062, t(37) = 2.9; MDD: p(RT) = 0.0011, t(37) = 3.5, unpaired t-test). In both tasks, a greater behavioral impairment was observed for one of the motion categories/directions (fig. S1, MDC: p = 6.8e-04, t(16) = 4.2; MDD: p = 0.0048, t(16) = 3.3, paired t-test). Psychometric curve fitting showed a significant change in bias and an increase in psychophysical threshold during both tasks (Fig. 2, M to P and fig. S2, MDC: R2(inactivation) = 0.90±0.034, R2(control) = 0.94±0.044, p(bias) = 2.7e-04, t(37) = 4.0, p(threshold) = 1.9e-08, t(37) = 7.1; MDD: R2(inactivation) = 0.91± 0.069, R2(control) = 0.98± 0.025, p(bias) = 9.5e-4, t(37) = 3.6, p(threshold) = 1.2e-7, t(37) = 6.5, unpaired t-test). Behavioral impairments in the SIN condition of both tasks were significantly greater than in BOUT (Fig. 2, E,F,K and L, MDC: p(accuracy) =7.5e-06, t(16) = -6.5, p(RT) = 0.0011, t(16) = 4.0; MDD: p(accuracy) = 1.1e-09, t(16) = -12.6, p(RT) = 1.9e-04, t(16) = 4.8, SIN vs. BOUT, paired t-test), in which we did not observe a global impact on accuracy (Fig. 2, C,E,I and K; MDC: p(accuracy) = 0.17, t(37) = -1.4; MDD: p(accuracy) = 0.12, t(37) = -1.6, inactivation vs. control, unpaired t-test) or RT (Fig. 2, D,F,I and L; MDC: p(RT) = 0.60, t(37) = 0.53; MDD: p(RT) = 0.71, t(37) = 0.37, unpaired t-test).
LIP’s role in motor aspects of decisions was assessed in the TIN condition (Fig. 3), which is the spatial configuration nearly always tested in past studies of LIP with the MDD task(7, 8, 12, 14, 15, 17) (but see (18)). Although the motion stimulus was outside the IVF, note that the TIN condition did require evaluating the color of the in-RF saccade target. After LIP inactivation, the monkeys’ saccadic choices were significantly biased away from the IVF in both tasks (Fig. 3, A,C,G and I; MDC: p = 1.3e-07, t(16) =8.9, MDD: p = 3.0e-04, t(16) = -5.4, paired t-test), and a greater RT increase was observed on trials when the saccade was directed toward vs away from the IVF (Fig. 3, B,D,H and J; MDC: p =7.0e-06, t(16) = -6.5, MDD: p = 6.0e-05, t(16) = 4.6, paired t-test). Although LIP inactivation produced an ipsilateral saccade bias, there was no significant difference in mean accuracy between inactivation and control sessions for either task (Fig. 3, A,E,G and K, MDC: p = 0.13, t(37) = -1.5; MDD: p = 0.56, t(37) = -0.59, unpaired t-test), and accuracy on both tasks was less influenced by inactivation in TIN than SIN (Fig. 3, E and K; MDC: p = 1.6e-08, t(16) = -10.4; MDD: p =1.2e-06, t(16) = -7.5, paired t-test). The magnitudes of RT effects between TIN and SIN were statistically indistinguishable (Fig. 3, F and L; MDC: p = 0.40, t(16) = 0.86; MDD: p = 0.093, t(16) = 1.8, paired t-test). Psychometric curve fitting showed a significant bias in saccades away from the IVF, but no increase in threshold in TIN in both tasks (Fig. 3, M to P and fig. S3, MDC: R2(inactivation) = 0.92±0.070, R2(control) = 0.94±0.047, p(bias) =4.0e-07, t(37) = 6.1, p(threshold) = 0.15, t(37) = 1.5; MDD: R2(inactivation) = 0.98±0.010, R2(control) = 0.98±0.010, p(bias) =0.0013, t(37) = 3.5, p(threshold) = 0.33, t(37) =0.99, unpaired t-test). These effects were consistent across monkeys (figs. S4 and S5).
We examined how distinct components of the decision process were affected by inactivation. We fitted our results with fixed-boundary drift diffusion models, which captured monkeys’ decision behavior across different task conditions in both MDD and MDC tasks, including both accuracy and the RT distributions (figs. S6 and S7). This revealed that LIP inactivation significantly slowed the evidence accumulation process (drift rate) only in SIN, while significantly changing the saccade choice bias (starting point of the diffusion process) and decision boundary only in TIN, across monkeys and tasks (tables S1 to S4). Inactivation also did not produce substantial impairments of either gaze position, microsaccades, or peak saccade velocity (figs. S8, S9 and S10).
We hypothesized that the inactivation-related behavioral deficits in the SIN condition of the MDD task are due to disrupting neuronal activity related to evaluating stimulus motion within the IVF. Thus, we recorded from 194 LIP neurons (Monkey M: N=78; Monkey B: N=116) during the SIN condition, targeting the same cortical locations within the same hemispheres as in the inactivation experiments. More than half of LIP neurons (monkey M: 50/78, monkey B: 54/116) showed significant motion direction selectivity (DS) in the MDD task (one-way ANOVA, P < 0.01). DS emerged prior to the monkeys’ mean RTs (fig. S11A), and the magnitude of DS was positively correlated with motion coherence at the single neuron and population levels, but did not converge to a fixed threshold near the decision time (Fig. 4, A to D and fig. S11B). Neuronal activity on zero-coherence motion trials (in which monkeys could only guess about motion direction) reflected the monkeys’ trial-by-trial decisions, consistent with a role in the decision process. Decision-correlated activity across all motion-coherence levels was evident in single-neuron activity (Fig. 4, A and B and fig. S12), the LIP population (Fig. 4C; fig. S13 for results reported separately for each monkey), and was confirmed using a receiver operating characteristic (ROC) analysis (Fig. 4D).
To further test whether DS in LIP was decision related, we compared activity on correct vs. error low-coherence trials (see Methods). LIP population activity was more similar on trials in which monkeys decided that different motion directions were the same vs. different (Fig. 4E, p = 1.9e-04, t(100) = 3.9, paired t-test). Accordingly, LIP activity co-varied more closely with the monkeys’ trial-by-trial decisions about motion direction than it did with the physical motion direction (Fig. 4F; p = 6.8e-04, t(103) = -3.4, paired t-test, comparing ROC values on chosen vs physical direction of motion). Furthermore, LIP activity co-varied with the monkeys’ RTs, with most neurons showing a greater response to their preferred motion direction on shorter vs longer RT trials for all motion coherence levels (and weaker response to their non-preferred direction; fig. S14, S15). Even on low-coherence (Fig. 4G) and zero-coherence (Fig. 4H) trials, DS evolved more rapidly on shorter vs longer RT trials (comparing slope: p(low) < 0.01, p(zero) < 0.01, bootstrap). At the population level, the slope of DS negatively correlated with monkeys’ RT (fig. S15, r = -0.9455, p = 0.0044). Elevated DS was even observed before stimulus onset on low-coherence trials (Fig. 4H), suggesting that the state of LIP activity prior to stimulus onset was predictive of the monkeys’ upcoming decision on that trial. Furthermore, a partial correlation analysis examined choice- and stimulus-related components of DS (i.e., r-choice and r-stimulus; Fig. 4I and fig. S16)(19). This revealed that the choice and stimulus components of LIP DS were positively correlated (Fig. 4J, r = 0.6276, p < 0.0001).
Taken together, our results suggest that LIP plays a significant role in visually-based perceptual and categorical decisions, with preferential involvement in stimulus evaluation compared to motor planning aspects of such decisions. This is supported by behavioral impairments on discrimination and categorization tasks caused by reversibly inactivating LIP. This is also supported by neurophysiological recordings revealing decision-correlated LIP activity during the MDD task, as well as work from multiple groups showing sensory-decision-related LIP activity in several tasks(11, 13, 20), including categorization(6, 9). Both tasks tested in this study had a flexible mapping between the decisions about motion stimuli and the direction of the saccade used to report those decisions. Thus, it will be interesting to examine whether LIP plays a similar role during tasks with fixed sensory-motor mappings.
It is well established that LIP helps mediate covert spatial attention(21–23) and saccades(1, 24, 25). However, the current study highlights LIP’s role in evaluating the abstract behavioral significance of visual stimuli during decision making. Although attention contributes toward the selection and representation of task-relevant stimuli, and disruption of attention by LIP inactivation may contribute to the current results, our inactivation results in the SIN condition are unlikely to arise primarily from disrupting attention or saccade related functions for several reasons. First, the LIP neurons targeted for inactivation showed activity which correlated with MDD task decisions, indicating that LIP’s role extends beyond the comparatively fixed representation of motion direction in the middle temporal area(4, 6, 11, 14, 16). The partial correlation analysis suggests LIP DS may be more closely choice correlated compared to other parietal areas (19, 26), although a direct comparison across multiple areas during the same tasks will be needed. Second, inactivation produced greater impairments for specific directions and categories, rather than uniformly impacting performance, and we found similar results when we analyzed inactivation behavioral data with respect to neurons’ stimulus preferences at the targeted LIP sites (figs. S17 and S18). This may relate to biased representations of categories and directions observed in LIP during MDD and MDC tasks(27), or could reflect anatomical clustering of such representations in LIP. Third, LIP inactivation did not produce an obvious impairment of eye movements or profound spatial neglect, although we did observe a mild ipsilateral bias during the free choice saccade task (fig. S19), consistent with previous studies(16, 23, 28). Nevertheless, future work using more precise causal approaches—such as stimulating or silencing pools of neurons with specific direction/category preferences—can more precisely dissect LIP’s contributions toward decision making tasks.
A recent study came to a different conclusion regarding LIP’s role in perceptual decisions—finding no significant impact of inactivation on MDD task performance(16). However, that study only considered LIP’s contributions to motor planning (TIN) aspects of the MDD task, but not sensory stimulus evaluation (SIN). Another study found that LIP activity was decision-correlated in both SIN and TIN conditions of a MDD task, but concluded that LIP activity correlated more closely with motor aspects (but did not directly test LIP’s causal contributions)(18). Consistent with the earlier inactivation study, we found that LIP inactivation did not impair average MDD-task accuracy in the TIN condition. However, we did observe a saccade bias away from the IVF in the TIN condition, suggesting a deficit in mapping decisions to actions. Several factors may account for this discrepancy. First, we used an RT rather than fixed-interval version of the MDD task, perhaps prompting different strategies or speed-accuracy tradeoffs. Second, our tasks employed a flexible mapping (based on saccade target color) between motion stimuli and saccades. Thus, LIP may be more engaged by decisions requiring greater cognitive flexibility or abstraction. Furthermore, the TIN condition in our tasks required discriminating the color of the in-RF saccade target to plan the correct saccade, so that condition had greater sensory evaluation demands than the traditional MDD task(13). Compared to the previous study, our animals learned both the MDD and MDC tasks, and the strategy used to solve the MDD task could have been affected by DMC training. The greater behavioral deficit observed in the MDD task (which employed low-coherence motion stimuli) than MDC task (which used only 100% coherent motion) is consistent with LIP integrating noisy sensory evidence(12, 14, 17), and the saccadic choice bias in the TIN condition supports LIP’s involvement in mediating the saccades used for reporting decisions(14, 29). Thus, decision-related motion encoding might be transformed into saccadic signals via coordination between sensory and motor pools of LIP neurons, in contrast to LIP reading out sensory inputs exclusively from upstream visual areas such as MT(30).
Perceptual and categorical decisions rely on contributions from and coordination among a network of cortical and subcortical areas spanning the sensory, cognitive, and motor hierarchy(13–15, 31, 32). Although the current study establishes primate PPC as an important node in that network, along with recent work in rodents(33–35), a more complete understanding necessitates the characterization of local and long-range circuits, within different animal models and humans, which flexibly transform sensory encoding into decisions and actions, and the process by which task-related neuronal encoding emerges during learning.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments:
We thank John Assad, Nicolas Masse, Kenneth Latimer, Krithika Mohan and William Johnston for their comments on an earlier version of this manuscript. We also thank the veterinary staff at The University of Chicago Animal Resources Center for expert assistance.
Grants:
NIH R01EY019041
Footnotes
Competing interests:
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
Data and materials availability:
All data and analyses necessary to understand and assess the conclusions of the manuscript are presented in the main text and in the supplementary materials. Data and analysis code from this study is available at crcns.org.
References and Notes:
- 1.Andersen RA, Visual and eye movement functions of the posterior parietal cortex. Annual review of neuroscience 12, 377–403 (1989). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Blatt GJ, Andersen RA, Stoner GR, Visual receptive field organization and cortico-cortical connections of the lateral intraparietal area (area LIP) in the macaque. The Journal of comparative neurology 299, 421–445 (1990). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Lewis JW, Van Essen DC, Corticocortical connections of visual, sensorimotor, and multimodal processing areas in the parietal lobe of the macaque monkey. The Journal of comparative neurology 428, 112–137 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Cook EP, Maunsell JH, Dynamics of neuronal responses in macaque MT and VIP during motion detection. Nature neuroscience 5, 985–994 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.de Lafuente V, Jazayeri M, Shadlen MN, Representation of accumulating evidence for a decision in two parietal areas. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 35, 4306–4318 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Freedman DJ, Assad JA, Experience-dependent representation of visual categories in parietal cortex. Nature 443, 85–88 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Roitman JD, Shadlen MN, Response of neurons in the lateral intraparietal area during a combined visual discrimination reaction time task. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 22, 9475–9489 (2002). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Shadlen MN, Newsome WT, Motion perception: seeing and deciding. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 93, 628–633 (1996). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Swaminathan SK, Freedman DJ, Preferential encoding of visual categories in parietal cortex compared with prefrontal cortex. Nature neuroscience 15, 315–320 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Swaminathan SK, Masse NY, Freedman DJ, A comparison of lateral and medial intraparietal areas during a visual categorization task. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 33, 13157–13170 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Williams ZM, Elfar JC, Eskandar EN, Toth LJ, Assad JA, Parietal activity and the perceived direction of ambiguous apparent motion. Nature neuroscience 6, 616–623 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Shadlen MN, Newsome WT, Neural basis of a perceptual decision in the parietal cortex (area LIP) of the rhesus monkey. Journal of neurophysiology 86, 1916–1936 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Freedman DJ, Assad JA, Neuronal Mechanisms of Visual Categorization: An Abstract View on Decision Making. Annual review of neuroscience 39, 129–147 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Gold JI, Shadlen MN, The neural basis of decision making. Annual review of neuroscience 30, 535–574 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Huk AC, Katz LN, Yates JL, The Role of the Lateral Intraparietal Area in (the Study of) Decision Making. Annual review of neuroscience 40, 349–372 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Katz LN, Yates JL, Pillow JW, Huk AC, Dissociated functional significance of decision-related activity in the primate dorsal stream. Nature 535, 285–288 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Hanks TD, Ditterich J, Shadlen MN, Microstimulation of macaque area LIP affects decision-making in a motion discrimination task. Nature neuroscience 9, 682–689 (2006). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Shushruth S, Mazurek M, Shadlen MN, Comparison of Decision-Related Signals in Sensory and Motor Preparatory Responses of Neurons in Area LIP. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 38, 6350–6365 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Zaidel A, DeAngelis GC, Angelaki DE, Decoupled choice-driven and stimulus-related activity in parietal neurons may be misrepresented by choice probabilities. Nat Commun 8, 715 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Ibos G, Freedman DJ, Sequential sensory and decision processing in posterior parietal cortex. eLife 6, (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Bisley JW, Goldberg ME, Attention, intention, and priority in the parietal lobe. Annual review of neuroscience 33, 1–21 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Colby CL, Goldberg ME, Space and attention in parietal cortex. Annual review of neuroscience 22, 319–349 (1999). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Wardak C, Olivier E, Duhamel JR, A deficit in covert attention after parietal cortex inactivation in the monkey. Neuron 42, 501–508 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Andersen RA, Buneo CA, Intentional maps in posterior parietal cortex. Annual review of neuroscience 25, 189–220 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Liu Y, Yttri EA, Snyder LH, Intention and attention: different functional roles for LIPd and LIPv. Nature neuroscience 13, 495–500 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Yu X, Gu Y, Probing Sensory Readout via Combined Choice-Correlation Measures and Microstimulation Perturbation. Neuron 100, 715–727 e715 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Fitzgerald JK et al. , Biased associative representations in parietal cortex. Neuron 77, 180–191 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Balan PF, Gottlieb J, Functional significance of nonspatial information in monkey lateral intraparietal area. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 29, 8166–8176 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Shadlen MN, Kiani R, Hanks TD, Churchland AK, Neurobiology of decision making: an intentional framework., in: Engel C, Singer W (Eds.), Better than conscious? : decision making, the human mind, and implications for institutions. Strüngmann Forum reports (MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass., 2008), pp. xiv, 449 p. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Yates JL, Park IM, Katz LN, Pillow JW, Huk AC, Functional dissection of signal and noise in MT and LIP during decision-making. Nature neuroscience 20, 1285–1292 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Crapse TB, Lau H, Basso MA, A Role for the Superior Colliculus in Decision Criteria. Neuron 97, 181–194 e186 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Seger CA, Miller EK, Category learning in the brain. Annual review of neuroscience 33, 203–219 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Licata AM et al. , Posterior Parietal Cortex Guides Visual Decisions in Rats. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 37, 4954–4966 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Hanks TD et al. , Distinct relationships of parietal and prefrontal cortices to evidence accumulation. Nature 520, 220–223 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Zhong L et al. , Causal contributions of parietal cortex to perceptual decision-making during stimulus categorization. Nature neuroscience 22, 963–973 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Asaad WF, Santhanam N, McClellan S, Freedman DJ, High-performance execution of psychophysical tasks with complex visual stimuli in MATLAB. Journal of neurophysiology 109, 249–260 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Bennur S, Gold JI, Distinct representations of a perceptual decision and the associated oculomotor plan in the monkey lateral intraparietal area. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 31, 913–921 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Ratcliff R, McKoon G, The diffusion decision model: Theory and data for two-choice decision tasks. Neural computation 20, 873–922 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Ratcliff R, Smith PL, A comparison of sequential sampling models for two-choice reaction time. Psychological review 111, 333–367 (2004). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Ratcliff R, Rouder JN, Modeling response times for two-choice decisions. Psychological science 9, 347–356 (1998). [Google Scholar]
- 41.Vandekerckhove J, Tuerlinckx F, Diffusion model analysis with MATLAB: a DMAT primer. Behav Res Methods 40, 61–72 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Zhou Y, Liu Y, Lu H, Wu S, Zhang M, Neuronal representation of saccadic error in macaque posterior parietal cortex (PPC). eLife 5, (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Zhou Y, Liu Y, Wu S, Zhang M, Neuronal Representation of the Saccadic Timing Signals in Macaque Lateral Intraparietal Area. Cereb Cortex 28, 2887–2900 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Herrington TM et al. , The Effect of Microsaccades on the Correlation between Neural Activity and Behavior in Middle Temporal, Ventral Intraparietal, and Lateral Intraparietal Areas. Journal of Neuroscience 29, 5793–5805 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Martinez-Conde S, Macknik SL, Hubel DH, Microsaccadic eye movements and firing of single cells in the striate cortex of macaque monkeys. Nature neuroscience 3, 251–258 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.