Skip to main content
PLOS One logoLink to PLOS One
. 2020 Jul 9;15(7):e0235552. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0235552

Exploring the spatial working memory and visual perception in children with autism spectrum disorder and general population with high autism-like traits

Manxue Zhang 1, Jian Jiao 1, Xiao Hu 1, Pingyuan Yang 1, Yan Huang 1, Mingjing Situ 1, Kuifang Guo 1, Jia Cai 1, Yi Huang 1,2,3,*
Editor: Inmaculada Riquelme4
PMCID: PMC7347168  PMID: 32645114

Abstract

The aim of the study is to compare the spatial working memory and visual perception between children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and typically developing control (TDC). Furthermore, this study validated whether this impairment was a feature of autism in general population with different autism-like traits (ALTs). This study contains two parts: case-control study and community population study. The ASD group and the control group were enlisted voluntarily (ASD group, n = 52; control group, n = 32). In the population study, we recruited 2994 children. Based on the scores of Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ), children were divided into two groups (higher ALTs n = 122, lower ALTs n = 122). The participants completed the cognition tasks focusing on spatial working memory, visual-motor integration, and Intelligence. Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was conducted, with potential confounders IQ, age, and gender were controlled. Pearson correlations were computed by controlling the IQ and age as covariate to better understand the relations between visual perception, spatial working memory, and autism-like traits. In the case-control study, the results of cognition tasks focusing on the spatial working memory and visual perception indicated underperformance in children with ASD. In the community population study, we found that individuals with higher ALTs performed worse than children with lower ALTs in spatial working memory. Pearson correlation analysis suggested that a correlation between SWM total errors and visual perception was identified both in the children with ASD and in community population (ASD group, r = -0.592, p<0.001; general population, r = -0.201, p = 0.003). It suggested that spatial working memory deficit was a characteristic of autism, and may be distributed across the general population. Furthermore, we speculated a correlation between spatial working memory and visual perception in children with ASD and in general population.

Introduction

Autism spectrum disorders (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder, characterized by deficits in social interaction, social communication, and restricted, repetitive patterns of behavior, interests including unusual sensory perceptions [1]. Perceptual symptoms such as visual reception, auditory and motor are prominent in the early life of ASD [2, 3] and show a persistent relationship to clinical measures of cognition and behavior [4]. Mental processes are the way people perceive the world, and sensation, perception, and cognition probably affect one another to different degrees.

Visual perception was identified as the ability to receive, recognize, analyze, and elaborate the visual stimulus from objects and events [5]. As early as 28 weeks, most infants can learn to distinguish between a circle, square, and triangle [6]. Part-whole integration has been of special significance in visual perceptual development, while many children with ASD seem to be unable to synthesize parts into the whole [7, 8]. Visual problems and spatial visual processing dysfunction in ASD might vary in onset, severity and behavior patterns. They can be generally grouped into hypersensitivity and hyposensitivity. Previous studies have shown that visual perceptual dysfunctions in early age would be predictive of future ASD symptoms over time [912]. Visual processing is a complex process, including visual recognition, visual memory, visual spatial orientation, and the perception of graphics. Several investigations showed that the results of visual perception are inconsistent, as evidenced by impairment of eye movements and contrast sensitivity [13, 14]; enhanced visual searching [15, 16] and hyper-local orientation [17] in individuals with autism; normal in orientation processing, crowding, and flicker detection [1820]. The inconsistences may be explained by different tools used to exploring various aspects of visual processing. The present study focuses on overall visual perception. In addition, the inconsistency of visual perception studies results might be attributed to the heterogeneity of ASD, it is necessary to study from the perspective of the continuous distribution characteristics.

Previous researches had provided inconclusive evidence about whether children with ASD are impaired in working memory. Investigations of school aged children and adolescents suggested working memory impairment in ASD [21, 22]. Working memory deficit is associated with symptoms of children and adolescents with ASD such as communication and socialization impairments [23, 24], restrictive and repetitive interests and behaviors [25]. While some researches indicated normal working memory in preschoolers and adults with ASD [2628]. The mixed results indicated that adolescents with ASD showed remarkable working memory dysfunction, and indicated that age had important influences in the field. A Meta-Analysis [29] of working memory in individuals ranged from school-age children to adults suggested that spatial working memory was more severely impaired than verbal working memory. Several studies investigated the spatial working memory to confirm the dysfunction. However, whether children with ASD are impaired in the spatial working memory was not confirmed because of different measures used [21, 30, 31]. Furthermore, the visual perception requires executive functions such as planning and working memory, visual perception also affects the test results of spatial working memory.

We believe cognitions should be seen as complementary rather than mutually exclusive. There have been many theories about the psychopathological mechanisms of ASD, such as ‘executive function deficit’, and ‘weak central coherence’ [32, 33], but none of these theories can explain the whole picture of the symptoms of ASD. Neurobiological accounts of how and why these lower and higher order symptoms might be related in autism are largely divided into two camps: Sensory first accounts [34] and Top down accounts [3537]. There are many studies on spatial working memory and visual perception, but there are few studies on the relationship between them, and the results are inconsistent: Mottron and his group [35] put forward a general tendency toward a local bias of autism that may be related to impairments in the maintenance of several representations in spatial working memory. However, the investigation did not explore the interaction between them. One study found abnormalities in executive function and visual motor integration in premature infants, and the two cognitions were moderately correlated [38]. Another study investigated in young healthy children have shown similar correlation between working memory and visual motor integration [39]. But the results in preschool children suggest that working memory does not affect visual perception [40]. Karisa and his team, using a sophisticated measure that tested visual processing and executive function, found that visual perception is dissociable with executive function in individuals with various degree of ALTs [41]. Further experiments will be necessary to discover the direct relationship between visual perception and spatial working memory in the processing of visual information.

Autistic-like traits (ALTs) were defined as subclinical traits in the broader population and were continuously distributed throughout the normal range to the clinical extreme population [42]. Individuals with high ALTs show similar symptoms [43, 44] and cognition deficits such as, visual-spatial process [4547], working memory [48], shared-attention [49], and cognitive flexibility [50]. Research indicated that the majority of genetic liability for ASD is attributed to common inherited variances [51]. Therefore, the ALTs exist continuously in the general population. There are several methodological advantages that general population samples brought to the research on the etiology of autism, such as substantially more power to conduct model-fitting analyses (see review [52]).

The aim of the study is to investigate the visual perception and spatial working memory function deficits in children with ASD compared with typically developing controls. Meanwhile, we are interested in whether the results would be repeated in population study when comparing higher trait individuals with lower trait individuals. Furthermore, we detected the correlations between visual perception and spatial working memory.

Materials and methods

Participants

This study contains two parts: case-control study and community population study. We recruited ASD without intellectual disability comorbidity(n = 52, mean age = 9.23 years, SD = 3.35 years, Min–Max = 4–17 years; mean IQ = 95.27,SD = 19.18, Min–Max = 70–132; Male/Female = 37/7) and typically developing control children (n = 32, mean age = 10.63 years, SD = 3.15, Min–Max = 6-16years; IQ = 108.22, SD = 13.94, Min–Max = 81–136; Male/Female = 16/16). (see in Table 1) during the period from September 2016 to June 2018. Both off-line and on-line recruitment methods were adopted in the recruitment process. Off-line method were conducted in the West China Hospital outpatient. Our online recruitment advertises were on the main social platforms including WeChat as well as the official accounts of the schools and hospitals involved. Participants were included after evaluation based on Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R), Autism Diagnostic Observation Scale-General (ADOS-G), and clinical judgment by experienced child psychiatrists.

Table 1. Participants demographics characteristics.

Measures ASD group(n = 52) TDC group (n = 32) t/χ2 p
Gender ratio (Male/Female) 37/7 16/16 10.2 0.001**
Age (year) Mean 9.23 10.63 1.896 0.062
SD 3.35 3.15
Min 4 6
Max 17 16
IQ Mean 95.27 108.22 3.191 0.002**
SD 19.18 13.94
Min 70 81
Max 132 136

*p<0.05

**p<0.01, ***p<0.001; ASD: autism spectrum disorders; TDC: typically developing controls; IQ: Intelligence Quotient

We also investigated in the sites of three primary schools in Pi Xian county Xi Pu District, in the city of Chengdu, Sichuan Province, China. Combination of the following factors was considered to select the district: 1) population from 5,000 to 15,000; 2) the lowest rate of the migrant population in the past years, 3) cooperation from the local government. The eligible children were identified in each selected street through the local public security bureau household registration system. Pi Xian is a county with 750,000 population consists of 13 districts. The population study was conducted in Pi Xian county, Xi Pu district from October 2016 to January 2017. Two thousand nine hundred ninety-four school aged students in three public primary schools -Si Yuan experimental primary school, Xi Pu experimental primary school, and Xi Pu foreign language primary school- were included in our study which can stand for the characteristics of the children population of the district. According to the sample size calculation formula N = (UαS/δ) 2, (Uα is the U value corresponding to the inspection level α, set as Uα = 1.96; S is the standard deviation, δ is the allowable error; assume S/δ = 5), the estimated sample size is N = 99. Considering the loss of interview and quality control, we chose all the students in the schools. All the 2994 students studying in the schools were recruited in this investigation. The caregivers of these children were asked to fill in the questionnaire: The Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ). After quality control of the questionnaires filled by parents, the ultimate response rate was 70.33%. According to the AQ score, 15% of the high score was set as the higher trait group, including a total of 122 individuals. Then, using stratified random sampling method take samples of 122 children from the remaining people as the lower trait group.

All the participants were asked to complete cognition tasks including the SWM tasks, the Berry VMI tasks, and the short-form Chinese Version of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale. The exclusion criteria of our study included: a history of psychotic disorder, severe head injury, syndromic genetic disorder associated with autism (e.g. fragile X syndrome), intellectual disability (i.e. IQ <70), Tourette’s disorder, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, any other medical condition significantly affecting brain function; unable to cooperate to complete the test. Finally, the demographic features of the population study participants were as follows, higher trait group(n = 115, mean age = 8.35 years, SD = 1.42 years, Min–Max = 6–11 years; mean IQ = 117.37, SD = 12.25, Min–Max = 86–139; Male/Female = 44/71), lower trait group(n = 105, mean age = 8.03 years, SD = 1.50 years, Min–Max = 6–11 years; mean IQ = 116.16,SD = 10.74, Min–Max = 82–137; Male/Female = 42/63).(see in Table 3) Parents of all the participants provided the written informed consent for study participation, which was approved by the Medical Ethical Committee of West China Hospital of Sichuan University.

Table 3. Participants demographics characteristics.

Measures H-trait group (n = 115) L-trait group (n = 105) T/χ2 p
Gender ratio (Male/Female) 44/71 42/63 0.069 0.79
Age (year) Mean 8.35 8.03 1.619 0.107
SD 1.42 1.42
Min 6 6
Max 11 11
IQ Mean 117.37 116.16 0.562 0.575
SD 12.25 10.74
Min 86 82
Max 139 137

*p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001; H-trait group was abbreviation for higher trait group; L-trait group was abbreviation for lower trait group; IQ: Intelligence Quotient.

Measures

Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ)

The original AQ is a 50-item self-administered questionnaire for the explicit purpose of measuring tendency towards autistic traits [53]. Cultural study had been investigated in several countries and showed good property. Winnie Yu-Pow Lau et al [54] adapted abridged AQ version in Chinese samples, consisting of 35-item. AQ-35 Chinese version consisted of five tightly semantically coherent subscale constructs, named as Socialness, Mindreading, Patterns, Attention to Details and Attention Switching. The items clustered in a way that depicted distinctive dimensions of ASD symptomatology.

Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R) and Autism Diagnostic Observation Scale-General (ADOS-G)

ADI-R is used for children aged from approximately 18 months to adulthood [55]. The Chinese ADI-R was approved by the World Psychological Association in 2007 [56]. The ratings were based on an assessment under current conditions and under the most severe state at 4–5 years, as recalled by the caregivers. It covers most developmental and behavioral aspects of ASD, including reciprocal social interaction, communication, and repetitive behaviors and stereotyped patterns. The ADOS-G is a semi-structured, standardized assessment of social interaction, communication, play, and imaginative use of materials [57]. It has four different units for different levels of development and verbal abilities. ADOS-G provides scores that are distinct in three domains: language and communication score, reciprocal social interaction scores and total scores. ADI-R and ADOS-G are the golden standards for ASD diagnosis.

Chinese version of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale (short-version)

Wechsler Intelligence Scales for Children-III (WISC-III) [58] was used to measure the intelligence of children. In this study, we used the Chinese short-version of the Wechsler Children Intelligence Scale-III, which consists of Vocabulary, Similarity, Picture Completion, and Block Design. For the present investigation, FSIQ was measured using the WISC–III.

Spatial Working Memory (SWM)

The Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery CANTAB eclipse Test (Administration Guide Manual version 5.0.0 http://www.cambridgecognition.com/) is a set of computerized paradigms run on a Lenovo-compatible computer with a high-resolution color monitor and the touch-sensitive screen. In our study, children were requested to complete Spatial Working Memory (SWM). Kids were asked to search through a number of colored boxes presented on the screen to find blue tokens hidden inside, which is set to be shown only once a trail in each box. Touching any box in which a blue token has already been found is an error. The number of boxes is gradually increased from three to eight boxes. The color and position of the boxes used are changed from trial to trial to discourage the use of stereotyped search strategies. The outcome measures for the SWM test may be regarded as “Total Errors” in previous studies. The lower the total errors of SWM, the better the cognitive function of Spatial Working Memory. The SWM, based on a self-ordered search test [59], assesses spatial working memory. Also, the SWM had been used to assess the spatial working memory among ASD children [21, 30, 31].

The Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration (Beery VMI)

The Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration (Beery VMI) [5, 60] is a developmental sequence of geometric forms to be reproduced with paper and pencil and scored according to objective scoring criteria outlined in the test manuals based on accuracy of copying when compared to the original. Considering the longitudinal nature of the parent project, the 3-5th editions of the Beery VMI were used during the various stages of data collection. The stimuli in the various editions did not change. In the subtest of visual perception, children need to choose the figure which was same as the one in the box above, one point is awarded for each correct item until three consecutive incorrect item scores or the 3-minute time limit expires. The Beery VMI has been demonstrated to have good reliability and validity as reported in the manual. Beery VMI is one of the most commonly used standardized measurements of visual-motor integration, visual perception and motion coordination in several developmental disorders. The Beery VMI has a rich tradition in assessing children and adults with various neurodevelopmental disorders, including ASD [6163].

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were carried out with the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences, 22.0 (SPSS 22.0). The statistical significance level was set to p ≤0.05 (two-tailed) for all tests. Independent samples t-tests and Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) were conducted to compare groups of demographic information and cognitive factors, with potential confounders age, gender, and IQ was controlled. Pearson correlations were computed under age, IQ control to better understand the relations between Beery VMI, SWM performance and AQ scores.

Results

Case-control study

Participants demographics characteristics

We chose children with ASD whose IQ scores were higher than 70, to avoid the intelligence influences on cognition tasks. The average IQ of ASD group was normal, IQ = 95.76, although there was a significant IQ difference between groups (t = 3.191, df = 83, p = 0.002). The t-test of independent sample results indicated that there is no significant difference in age between the ASD group and the TD control group (t = 1.896, df = 83, p = 0.062). And the rate of genders was especially unbalanced in the ASD group (χ2 = 10.2, df = 83, p = 0.001). (see in Table 1)

Group differences regarding neuropsychological test performance

The t-test results showed significant differences between ASD group and TDC controls in all cognitive functions tested in our study except for visual perception. However, to minimize the effect of unmatched IQ, age, and gender between groups, the ANCOVA was conducted with IQ, gender, and age as covariant. Results of cognition tasks focused on the spatial working memory showed significant group differences (F = 2.986, df = 83, p = 0.004), which indicated a spatial working memory dysfunction in ASD individuals. We also found children with ASD performed worse in visual perception (F = 2.006, df = 83, p = 0.048), motion coordination (F = 3.416, df = 83, p = 0.001) and visual-motor integration (t = 2.150, df = 83, p = 0.035), which may indicate visual perception and motor dysfunction in ASD individuals. (see Table 2)

Table 2. Performance differences on Spatial Working Memory (SWM) and Berry-VMI between ASD and control.
ASD(n = 52) TDC (n = 32) t p Fc pc
AQ 19.65±3.71 13.97±4.80 6.083 <0.001*** 6.083 <0.001***
SWM total errors 57.81±16.78 45.88±19.32 2.086 0.002** 2.986 0.004**
VMI-Integration 96.65±19.28 104.88±12.43 2.150 0.039* 2.150 0.035*
VMI-Visual 102.00±19.04 109.44±11.13 1.549 0.123 2.006 0.048*
VMI-Motion 95.46±15.79 106.97±13.58 3.504 <0.001*** 3.416 0.001**

*p<0.05

**p<0.01

***p<0.001. Fc: F value after corrected with IQ,gender, and age as covariant; pc: p value after corrected with IQ, gender, and age as covariant; ASD: autism spectrum disorders; TDC: typically developed controls; AQ: Autism Spectrum Quotient; SWM: Spatial Working Memory; VMI-Visual: The Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration-visual perception subtest.; VMI-Motion: The Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration-motion coordination subtest; VMI-integration: The Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration-integration subtest.

Correlations among neuropsychological tests and symptom severity

We performed Pearson correlation analysis among neuropsychological test performances in ASD group using IQ, gender, and age as covariant. The correlations between SWM, VMI and AQ score were not significant (p>0.05). We further explored the correlations between SWM and VMI tests. There was negative correlation between SWM total errors with visual perception (r = -0.592, p<0.001), and motion coordination (r = -0.398, p = 0.004). (Fig 1) It indicated that spatial working memory dysfunction was related with visual perception in children with ASD.

Fig 1. Correlations between Spatial Working Memory and VMI performances in children with ASD.

Fig 1

The Pearson correlation analyses were conducted with age and IQ as covariant. The left picture showed a correlation between spatial working memory and visual perception (r = -0.592, p<0.001); The right figure indicated a correlation between spatial working memory and motion coordination (r = -0.398, p = 0.004).

In the mean time we conducted Pearson correlation analysis in healthy control children. Results showed that there was no correlation among spatial working memory, visual perception and ASD symptom severity(p>0.05). (Fig 2) The results showed the correlation between SWM and visual perception in children with ASD did not repeat in typically developing controls.

Fig 2. Scatter diagrams of Spatial Working Memory and VMI performances in typically developing children.

Fig 2

The Pearson correlation analyses were conducted with age and IQ as covariant. The left picture showed the correlation between spatial working memory and visual perception was not significant (r = -0.214, p = 0.256). The right picture showed the correlation between spatial working memory and motor coordination was not significant (r = 0.099, p = 0.604).

Community population study

Community population study was conducted in three sites, after stratified random sampling and quality control the final participants demographics characteristics were as follows, higher trait group n = 115,lower trait group n = 105.There was no significant differences between the two groups in age, gender, and IQ (p>0.05).(described in Table 3.)

Neuropsychological test performance differences among different degrees of autistic-like traits

Independent t test results revealed that individuals with higher ALTs behaved worse than children with lower ALTs in spatial working memory. (t = 2.117, df = 119, p = 0.035) We did not find group differences in visual perception. (p>0.05) (see Table 4 for descriptive statistics).

Table 4. Tasks performance differences between different trait group on Spatial Working Memory (SWM) and Berry-VMI.
H-trait group(n = 115) L-trait group(n = 105) t p
SWM total errors 49.70±19.35 44.02±20.48 2.117 0.035*
VMI-Integration 102.16±13.69 103.23±13.17 0.591 0.555
VMI-Visual 107.47±11.87 109.61±12.54 1.300 0.195
VMI-Motion 100.60±16.35 101.96±13.61 0.668 0.505

*p<0.05, **p<0.05, ***p<0.001; H-trait group was abbreviation for higher trait group; L-trait group was abbreviation for lower trait group. IQ: Intelligence Quotient; SWM: Spatial Working Memory; VMI-Visual: The Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration-visual perception subtest.; VMI-Motion: The Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration-motion coordination subtest; VMI-integration: The Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration-integration subtest.

Correlations among neuropsychological tests and autistic traits

We performed Pearson correlation analysis to determine the association between neuropsychological test performances and ASD Symptoms in the community population samples. There was negative correlation between SWM total errors with VMI scores (SWM total errors and VMI-visual perception: r = -0.210, p = 0.003; SWM total errors and VMI-motion coordination: r = -0.138, p = 0.041). (see in Fig 3) The correlations between SWM, VMI and AQ score were not significant (p>0.05).

Fig 3. Correlations between spatial working memory and VMI performances in community population.

Fig 3

The Pearson correlation analyses were conducted with age and IQ as covariant. The left picture showed the correlation between spatial working memory and visual perception (r = -0.201, p = 0.003). The right picture showed the correlation between spatial working memory and motor coordination (r = -0.138, p = 0.041).

Discussion

This study found that children with ASD had abnormalities in spatial working memory, visual perception compared to typically developing controls. Furthermore, the results indicated a correlation between the cognitions in the ASD group. Impairment of spatial working memory in children with ASD has been demonstrated in population study, with children with high ALTs performing worse on spatial working memory than children with low ALTs. The correlation between visual perception and spatial working memory was confirmed in general population.

Working memory was defined as a limited capacity system that maintains information ‘‘on-line” over brief periods of time [64]. Spatial working memory is responsible for the temporary storage of visual information, and its ‘‘on-line” manipulation. This study found the spatial working memory deficits in both children with ASD and children with high ALTs, which is consistent with previous studies. Studies indicated that children with ASD had dysfunction in the spatial working memory [21, 22]. Spatial working memory abnormalities were found to be correlated with autism symptom severities in previous study [23]. On contrary, we found that the association between SWM and AQ was not statistically significant. It may be related to different age ranges. Previous studies were conducted in a wide range of age from children to adolescents, and our study was conducted in children aged from 6 to 12 years. Secondly, the tools used are different. A questionnaire, the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function, BRIEF was used in the previous study; however, the present study used CANTAB which was a common and reliable measure. The BRIEF is not specific enough to capture separate EFs based on the subscales [65]. Furthermore, symptoms may be affected by many factors, and the effect of cognitive function on symptoms may be related to other mediating factors. Interaction between cognitive functions may also have an impact on the results that SWM impairment found in ASD and high ALTs is not associated with overall level of autism symptom severity measured with the AQ. Another possible explanation for this pattern could be that spatial working memory is related to aspects of autism that may not be covered well by the AQ. Additional studies using larger samples of participants in all age periods that combine laboratory and informant-based measures are necessary to clarify this issue. However, recently one study conducted in unaffected siblings, showed spatial working memory wasn’t a broader autism phenotype of ASD [48]. The previous study didn’t identify spatial working memory as broader autism phenotype of ASD, which may be explained by the small sample size and the limitation of the term ‘broader autism phenotype’; and in our study we applied AQ to measure ALTs. Children with high ALTs have also been found to have spatial working memory deficits [66]. Furthermore, Previous neurobiological studies using electroencephalograph (EEG) [67] or fMRI [21] results suggested that children with ASD have abnormal activation of brain regions associated with working memory. Investigating executive function components that contribute to the severity of autistic symptomology provides insight for the development of effective interventions for individuals with ASD. Previous study proposed that the working memory trains improved classroom performances of children with ASD [68]. We speculated that spatial working memory was an autistic-like trait; and professional spatial working memory training might alleviate the severity of the symptoms.

Abnormal sensory perception is one character of autism. However, previous studies on visual processing had inconsistent results due to different focus points [13, 1517, 69]. This study focused on overall visual perception. In our study, the visual perception function deficit was found in ASD group when compared with typically developing children. Previous studies have found that individuals with autism have visual perception dysfunctions, which was predictor to the severity of symptoms [7075]. Except that Green et al. did not repeat the results in children with ASD [63]. However, the population study results indicated that higher trait group behave comparable of lower trait group. This is at odds with previous research suggesting that people with high autistic traits have impaired visual illusions [76], and visual search [77]. It should be noted that the AQ also correlates with IQ [78], and it could be IQ that produces the no significant results of higher trait group. In addition, the visual perception inconsistency of the ALTs prompted us to carry out better visual psychophysics using more sensitive measures of visual perception in general population with various degrees of autistic traits. Future brain mapping studies could provide additional insight into the neural underpinnings of how visual perception might and might not be affected in autism.

It is challenging for traditional case-control studies to invest diseases that distributed normally in population. However, population study is just helpful to explore the diseases with a continuous distribution of characteristics in the population [79]. Therefore, in the current study, we investigate the spatial working memory and visual perception by using a general population approach. Besides comparing a group with a clinical diagnosis of ASD to a group with typical development, we also compare individuals with higher ALTs to individuals with lower ALTs. ASD is commonly operationalized as the extreme end of a phenotypic continuum of autistic traits normally distributed in the general population [80]. Studies found that high ALTs individuals have behavioral and cognitive characteristics similar to those of ASD [8183]. This study consists of two parts, case control study and population study. The results confirmed that the high ALTs had similar changes in neurocognitive function as in children with ASD, suggesting that spatial working memory may be a core symptom of ASD. Besides, the present study offered support to the approaches that regard autistic-like traits as a continuous variable in general population rather than qualitative variable.

Studies have shown that working memory and other executive functions are important contributing factors influencing visual processing performance [38, 39]. When the subjects were asked to complete the VMI visual perception test, they require the ability to visualize an image, hold the image in short-term memory, and transfer the image with the correct proportions onto paper. The present study found that visual perception deteriorated as working memory performance decreased in the ASD group. While the relationship didn’t repeat in control group, possibly because copying geometric figures in the VMI tasks was not so hard for typically developing children to remember; at the same time children could check the geometric figures again if they forgot them. Also, the small sample size may have an impact on the results of control group, and we tried to address it by community population study with a larger sample size, and results of the general population children suggested a positive correlation between SWM and visual perception. Researches in preschool children suggested working memory has no impact on the VMI skills [40], the age range might explain the controversy. Further study was needed to explore mechanism of the relationship between working memory and visual perception using more meticulous measures. Neuroimaging studies found that executive function and visual perception are connected by overlapping neural networks in individuals with ASD [8486]. Further intensive structural equation model is needed to clarify the relationship between visual perception and spatial working memory, and to find out the underneath visual processing mechanism of neuropsychology in ASD.

Our study design includes population-based study that increase the effectiveness of finding core traits of ASD. The spatial working memory deficits found both in children with ASD and in ALTs individuals reflect that the cognitive characteristic may be distributed across the general population. Furthermore, we investigated the correlation between spatial working memory and visual perception, attempting to verify the mutually relationship between different cognitions in visual processing. The weakness of the study was that the gender ratio of the clinical samples was unbalanced which will have some impact on the final results when concerning the cognitive function impairments. The situation may be caused by a higher incidence of boys. When compared ASD with TDC in the cognitions, gender was set as a covariant to control potential bias. Another disadvantage is that the instrument we used, the VMI, cannot describe the visual processing in detail. In the future, more elaborate task design is needed to explore the visual processing of autism. Finally, our study is a cross-sectional study, long-term follow-up studies can be conducted in the future, considering the impact of development on children.

Conclusions

The findings of our study indicated spatial working memory played an important role in the ASD symptoms. Our results suggested that spatial working memory deficit may be a characteristic of autism, and may be distributed across the general population. Furthermore, we speculated a correlation between spatial working memory and visual perception in children with ASD and general population. The future study should be focused on the detailed mechanisms of the relationship between spatial working memory and visual perception.

Supporting information

S1 Data

(XLSX)

S1 File. STROBE statement—checklist of items that should be included in reports of observational studies.

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all the families for generously donating their time so that this study could be possible. We are also grateful to the research staffs for their dedication to this project.

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting Information files.

Funding Statement

This work was supported by the National Key Research& Development Program of China (NO.2016YFC1306100) and The National Natural Science Foundation of China (NO.81371495).

References

  • 1.US, A.P.A.D.-T.F.A.V., Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5™ (5th ed) Codas, 2013. 25(2): p. 191 10.1590/s2317-17822013000200017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Baranek G.T., et al. , Hyporesponsiveness to social and nonsocial sensory stimuli in children with autism, children with developmental delays, and typically developing children. Development & Psychopathology, 2013. 25(2): p. 307–320. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Estes A., et al. , Behavioral, cognitive, and adaptive development in infants with autism spectrum disorder in the first 2 years of life. J Neurodev Disord, 2015. 7(1): p. 1–24. 10.1186/1866-1955-7-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Boyd B.A., et al. , Sensory features and repetitive behaviors in children with autism and developmental delays. Autism Res, 2010. 3(2): p. 78–87. 10.1002/aur.124 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Spencer T.D. and Kruse L., Beery-Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration. 2013: Springer; New York: 494–494. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Bayley N., Bayley Scales of Infant Development. 1993: The Psychological Corporation; 233. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Keehn B. and Joseph R.M., Exploring What's Missing: What Do Target Absent Trials Reveal About Autism Search Superiority? J Autism Dev Disord, 2016. 46(5): p. 1686–1698. 10.1007/s10803-016-2700-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Song Y., et al. , Can They See It? The Functional Field of View Is Narrower in Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder. PLoS One, 2015. 10(7): p. e0133237 10.1371/journal.pone.0133237 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Eussen M.L., et al. , Superior Disembedding Performance in Childhood Predicts Adolescent Severity of Repetitive Behaviors: A Seven Years Follow-Up of Individuals With Autism Spectrum Disorder. Autism Res, 2016. 9(2): p. 282–291. 10.1002/aur.1510 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Kaldy Z., et al. , Toddlers with Autism Spectrum Disorder are more successful at visual search than typically developing toddlers. Dev Sci, 2011. 14(5): p. 980–988. 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01053.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Baranek G.T., et al. , Hyporesponsiveness to social and nonsocial sensory stimuli in children with autism, children with developmental delays, and typically developing children. Dev Psychopathol, 2013. 25(2): p. 307–320. 10.1017/S0954579412001071 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Turner-Brown L.M., et al. , The First Year Inventory: a longitudinal follow-up of 12-month-old to 3-year-old children. Autism, 2013. 17(5): p. 527–540. 10.1177/1362361312439633 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Bertone A., et al. , Enhanced and diminished visuo-spatial information processing in autism depends on stimulus complexity. Brain A Journal of Neurology, 2005. 128(Pt 10): p. 2430–2441. 10.1093/brain/awh561 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Takarae Y., Pursuit eye movement deficits in autism. Dkgest of the World Latest Medical Information, 2005. 127(12): p. 2584–2594. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Hagmann C.E., et al. , Children with Autism Detect Targets at Very Rapid Presentation Rates with Similar Accuracy as Adults. J Autism Dev Disord, 2016. 46(5): p. 1762–1772. 10.1007/s10803-016-2705-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Gregory B.L. and Plaisted-Grant K.C., The Autism-Spectrum Quotient and Visual Search: Shallow and Deep Autistic Endophenotypes. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 2016. 46(5): p. 1503–1512. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Sheppard E., Ropar D., and Mitchell P., Drawing the line: how people with autism copy line drawings of three-dimensional objects. Perception, 2009. 38(7): p. 1104–1106. 10.1068/p6449 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Freyberg J., Robertson C.E., and Baron-Cohen S., Typical magnitude and spatial extent of crowding in autism. J Vis, 2016. 16(5): p. 1–10. 10.1167/16.5.1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Grubb M.A., et al. , Endogenous spatial attention: evidence for intact functioning in adults with autism. Autism Research, 2013. 6(2): p. 108–118. 10.1002/aur.1269 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Pellicano E., et al. , Abnormal global processing along the dorsal visual pathway in autism: A possible mechanism for weak visuospatial coherence? Neuropsychologia, 2005. 43(7): p. 1044–1053. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.10.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Kaufmann L., et al. , Brief report: CANTAB performance and brain structure in pediatric patients with Asperger syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord, 2013. 43(6): p. 1483–1490. 10.1007/s10803-012-1686-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Jiang Y.V., Capistrano C.G., and Palm B.E., Spatial working memory in children with high-functioning autism: intact configural processing but impaired capacity. J Abnorm Psychol, 2014. 123(1): p. 248–257. 10.1037/a0035420 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Schuh J.M., Eigsti I.M., and Mirman D., Discourse comprehension in autism spectrum disorder: Effects of working memory load and common ground. Autism Res, 2016. 9(12): p. 1340–1352. 10.1002/aur.1632 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Gilotty L., et al. , Adaptive skills and executive function in autism spectrum disorders. Child Neuropsychology, 2002. 8(4): p. 241–248. 10.1076/chin.8.4.241.13504 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Sachse M., et al. , Executive and Visuo-motor Function in Adolescents and Adults with Autism Spectrum Disorder. J Autism Dev Disord, 2013. 43(5): p. 1222–1235. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Macizo P., Soriano M.F., and Paredes N., Phonological and Visuospatial Working Memory in Autism Spectrum Disorders. J Autism Dev Disord, 2016. 46(9): p. 2956–2967. 10.1007/s10803-016-2835-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Zimmerman D., et al. , Associations between executive functions and mental health outcomes for adults with autism spectrum disorder. Psychiatry Res, 2017. 253: p. 360–363. 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.04.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Gardiner E., et al. , Assessment of executive function in young children with and without ASD using parent ratings and computerized tasks of executive function. Clin Neuropsychol, 2017. 31(8): p. 1283–1305. 10.1080/13854046.2017.1290139 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Wang Y., et al. , A Meta-Analysis of Working Memory Impairments in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Neuropsychol Rev, 2017. 27(1): p. 46–61. 10.1007/s11065-016-9336-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Goldberg M.C., et al. , Subtle Executive Impairment in Children with Autism and Children with ADHD. J Autism Dev Disord, 2005. 35(3): p. 279–293. 10.1007/s10803-005-3291-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Corbett B.A., et al. , Examining executive functioning in children with autism spectrum disorder, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and typical development. Psychiatry Res, 2009. 166(2–3): p. 210–222. 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.02.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Happé F. and Frith U., The Weak Coherence Account: Detail-focused Cognitive Style in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 2006. 36(1): p. 5–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Yonemoto S., Recent studies on cognitive dysfunctions of autism. The theory of mind, weak central coherence and executive function deficits: A review. Hokusei Gakuen University Graduate School Social Welfare Review, 2000. 4. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Sharon A. Cermak L.A.D., Sensory Processing in the Postinstitutionalized Child. The American JournaL of OccupationaL Therapy, 1997. 51: p. 7 10.5014/ajot.51.1.7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Mottron L., Belleville S., and Ménard E., Local bias in autistic subjects as evidenced by graphic tasks: perceptual hierarchization or working memory deficit? Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 1999. 40(5): p. 743–755. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Rinehart N.J., et al. , Atypical interference of local detail on global processing in high-functioning autism and Asperger's disorder. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 2000. 41(6): p. 769–778. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Happe F. and Frith U., The weak coherence account: detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord, 2006. 36(1): p. 5–25. 10.1007/s10803-005-0039-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Böhm B., Lundequist A., and Smedler A.C., Visual-motor and executive functions in children born preterm: the Bender Visual Motor Gestalt Test revisited. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 2010. 51(5): p. 376–384. 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2010.00818.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Decker S.L., et al. , Cognitive and developmental influences in visual-motor integration skills in young children. Psychol Assess, 2011. 23(4): p. 1010–1016. 10.1037/a0024079 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Fang Y., et al. , The Relationship of Motor Coordination, Visual Perception, and Executive Function to the Development of 4-6-Year-Old Chinese Preschoolers' Visual Motor Integration Skills. Biomed Res Int, 2017. 2017: p. 1–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Parkington K.B., et al. , Visual-motor association learning in undergraduate students as a function of the autism-spectrum quotient. Experimental Brain Research, 2015. 233(10): p. 2883–2895. 10.1007/s00221-015-4358-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Hoekstra R.A., et al. , Heritability of autistic traits in the general population. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med, 2007. 161(4): p. 372–377. 10.1001/archpedi.161.4.372 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Jobe L.E. and White S.W., Loneliness, social relationships, and a broader autism phenotype in college students. Personality & Individual Differences, 2007. 42(8): p. 1479–1489. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Ingersoll B., Broader autism phenotype and nonverbal sensitivity: evidence for an association in the general population. J Autism Dev Disord, 2010. 40(5): p. 590–598. 10.1007/s10803-009-0907-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Sabatino DiCriscio A. and Troiani V., Brief Report: Autism-like Traits are Associated With Enhanced Ability to Disembed Visual Forms. J Autism Dev Disord, 2017. 47(5): p. 1568–1576. 10.1007/s10803-017-3053-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Bayliss A.P. and Tipper S.P., Gaze and arrow cueing of attention reveals individual differences along the autism spectrum as a function of target context. Br J Psychol, 2005. 96(Pt 1): p. 95–114. 10.1348/000712604X15626 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Grinter E.J., et al. , Brief report: visuospatial analysis and self-rated autistic-like traits. J Autism Dev Disord, 2009. 39(4): p. 670–677. 10.1007/s10803-008-0658-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Rosa M., et al. , Broad Cognitive Profile in Children and Adolescents with HF-ASD and in Their Siblings: Widespread Underperformance and its Clinical and Adaptive Correlates. J Autism Dev Disord, 2017. 47(7): p. 2153–2162. 10.1007/s10803-017-3137-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Skorich D.P., et al. , Exploring the Cognitive Foundations of the Shared Attention Mechanism: Evidence for a Relationship Between Self-Categorization and Shared Attention Across the Autism Spectrum. J Autism Dev Disord, 2017. 47(5): p. 1341–1353. 10.1007/s10803-017-3049-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Gokcen E., et al. , Sub-threshold autism traits: the role of trait emotional intelligence and cognitive flexibility. Br J Psychol, 2014. 105(2): p. 187–199. 10.1111/bjop.12033 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Trent G., et al. , Most genetic risk for autism resides with common variation. Nature Genetics, 2014. 46(8): p. 881–885. 10.1038/ng.3039 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Ronald A. and Hoekstra R.A., Autism spectrum disorders and autistic traits: a decade of new twin studies. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 2011. 156(3): p. 255–274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Baron-Cohen S., et al. , The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. J Autism Dev Disord, 2001. 31(1): p. 5–17. 10.1023/a:1005653411471 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Lau Y.P., et al. , Psychometric properties of the Chinese version of the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ). Research in Developmental Disabilities, 2013. 34(1): p. 294–305. 10.1016/j.ridd.2012.08.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Lord C., Rutter M., and Le C.A., Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 1994. 24(5): p. 659–685. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Gau S.F., et al. , Psychometric properties of the Chinese version of the Social Communication Questionnaire. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2011. 5(2): p. 809–818. [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Lord C., et al. , The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule—Generic: A Standard Measure of Social and Communication Deficits Associated with the Spectrum of Autism. J Autism Dev Disord, 2000. 30(3): p. 205–223. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Cubero N.S. and Wechsler D., WAIS-III: Escala de Inteligencia de Wechsler para Adultos III. Manual técnico; 1999: TEA Ediciones. [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Petrides M. and Milner B., Deficits on subject-ordered tasks after frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia, 1982. 20(3): p. 249–262. 10.1016/0028-3932(82)90100-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Beery K.E., Buktenica N.A., and Beery N.A., The Beery-Buktenica developmental test of visual-motor integration: VMI, with supplemental developmental tests of visual perception and motor coordination: administration, scoring and teaching manual. Vol. 40 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Bloch M.H., et al. , Poor fine-motor and visuospatial skills predict persistence of pediatric-onset obsessive-compulsive disorder into adulthood. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 2011. 52(9): p. 974–983. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Sutton G.P., et al. , Beery-Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration performance in children with traumatic brain injury and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychological Assessment, 2011. 23(3): p. 805–809. 10.1037/a0023370 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Green R.R., et al. , Beery VMI performance in autism spectrum disorder. Child Neuropsychol, 2016. 22(7): p. 795–817. 10.1080/09297049.2015.1056131 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Buchweitz A., Models of working memory:Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control Ilha Do Desterro, 2008. 41(43): p. 179–184. [Google Scholar]
  • 65.de Vries M., et al. , The association between the behavior rating inventory of executive functioning and cognitive testing in children diagnosed with a brain tumor. Child Neuropsychol, 2018. 24(6): p. 844–858. 10.1080/09297049.2017.1350262 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Richmond L.L., et al. , Individual differences in autistic trait load in the general population predict visual working memory performance. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 2013. 66(6): p. 1182–1195. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Urbain C.M., Pang E.W., and Taylor M.J., Atypical spatiotemporal signatures of working memory brain processes in autism. Transl Psychiatry, 2015. 5: p. e617 10.1038/tp.2015.107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Kerns K.A., et al. , Attention and working memory training: A feasibility study in children with neurodevelopmental disorders. Appl Neuropsychol Child, 2017. 6(2): p. 120–137. 10.1080/21622965.2015.1109513 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Endogenous Spatial Attention: Evidence for Intact Functioning in Adults With Autism. Autism Research Official Journal of the International Society for Autism Research, 2013. 6(2): p. 108–118. 10.1002/aur.1269 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Gliga T., et al. , Enhanced Visual Search in Infancy Predicts Emerging Autism Symptoms. Curr Biol, 2015. 25(13): p. 1727–1730. 10.1016/j.cub.2015.05.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.B, A., et al. , Neurophysiological Indices of Atypical Auditory Processing and Multisensory Integration are Associated with Symptom Severity in Autism. J Autism Dev Disord 2015. 45: p. 230–244. 10.1007/s10803-014-2212-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Sutherland A. and Crewther D.P., Magnocellular visual evoked potential delay with high autism spectrum quotient yields a neural mechanism for altered perception. Brain, 2010. 133(Pt 7): p. 2089–2097. 10.1093/brain/awq122 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Flevaris A.V. and Murray S.O., Orientation-specific surround suppression in the primary visual cortex varies as a function of autistic tendency. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 2015. 8: p. 1017 10.3389/fnhum.2014.01017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Mayer J.L., The Relationship Between Autistic Traits and Atypical Sensory Functioning in Neurotypical and ASD Adults: A Spectrum Approach. J Autism Dev Disord, 2017. 47(2): p. 316–327. 10.1007/s10803-016-2948-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Horder J., et al. , Autistic traits and abnormal sensory experiences in adults. J Autism Dev Disord, 2014. 44(6): p. 1461–1469. 10.1007/s10803-013-2012-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Chouinard P.A., et al. , Global processing during the Müller-Lyer illusion is distinctively affected by the degree of autistic traits in the typical population. Experimental Brain Research.experimentelle Hirnforschung.experimentation Cerebrale, 2013. 230(2): p. 219–231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Reed P., Lowe C., and Everett R., Perceptual learning and perceptual search are altered in male university students with higher Autism Quotient scores. Personality & Individual Differences, 2011. 51(6): p. 732–736. [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Wakabayashi A., Baron-Cohen S., and Wheelwright S., Are autistic traits an independent personality dimension? A study of the Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ) and the NEO-PI-R. Personality & Individual Differences, 2006. 41(5): p. 873–883. [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Ronald A. and Hoekstra R.A., Autism spectrum disorders and autistic traits: a decade of new twin studies. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet, 2011. 156B(3): p. 255–274. 10.1002/ajmg.b.31159 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Baron-Cohen S. and Hammer J., Parents of Children with Asperger Syndrome: What is the Cognitive Phenotype? 1997. 548–554. 10.1162/jocn.1997.9.4.548 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Bayliss A.P. and Kritikos A., Brief Report: Perceptual Load and the Autism Spectrum in Typically Developed Individuals. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 2011. 41(11): p. 1573–1578. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Russell-Smith S.N., et al. , Support for a Link Between the Local Processing Bias and Social Deficits in Autism: An Investigation of Embedded Figures Test Performance in Non-Clinical Individuals. J Autism Dev Disord, 2012. 42(11): p. 2420–2430. 10.1007/s10803-012-1506-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Alexandra S. and Crewther D.P., Magnocellular visual evoked potential delay with high autism spectrum quotient yields a neural mechanism for altered perception. Brain, 2010. 133(Pt 7): p. 2089–2097. 10.1093/brain/awq122 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Nouchine H., et al. , Early visual cortex organization in autism: an fMRI study. Neuroreport, 2004. 15(2): p. 267–270. 10.1097/00001756-200402090-00011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Rippon G., et al. , Disordered connectivity in the autistic brain: challenges for the "new psychophysiology". Int J Psychophysiol, 2007. 63(2): p. 164–172. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.03.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Bhaumik R., et al. , Predicting Autism Spectrum Disorder Using Domain-Adaptive Cross-Site Evaluation. Neuroinformatics, 2018. 16: p. 197–205. 10.1007/s12021-018-9366-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Inmaculada Riquelme

27 Apr 2020

PONE-D-20-04238

Exploring the spatial working memory and visual perception in autism children and general population with high autism-like traits

PLOS ONE

Dear Pro. Huang,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Minor methodological and expression details should be corrected and some references must be included, according to the reviewers' remarks. Particularly, authors should further elaborate the Discussion, incluiding a statement explaining how potential bias were addressed. In addition, an effort for the use of including language is required along the manuscript (e.g. children with ASD instead of ASD children, autistic children or ASD).

We would appreciate receiving your revised manuscript by Jun 07 2020 11:59PM. When you are ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter.

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). This letter should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Manuscript'.

Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Inmaculada Riquelme

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements:

1.    Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at http://www.plosone.org/attachments/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and http://www.plosone.org/attachments/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. In your Methods section, please provide additional information about the participant recruitment method and the demographic details of your participants for both the case-control study and the community population study. Please ensure you have provided sufficient details to replicate the analyses such as:

a) the recruitment date range (month and year),

b) a description of any inclusion/exclusion criteria that were applied to participant recruitment,

c) a table of relevant demographic details,

d) a specific description of how participants were recruited online for the case-control study

e) descriptions of the specific locations where participants were recruited (names of the three primary schools for the community population study).

3. Please provide a sample size and power calculation in the Methods, or discuss the reasons for not performing one before study initiation.

4.  Please include additional information regarding the questionnaires or scales used in the study (ADOS-G, WISC-III, Beery VMI and SVM and ensure that you have provided sufficient details that others could replicate the analyses. For instance, if you developed a questionnaire as part of this study and it is not under a copyright more restrictive than CC-BY, please include a copy, in both the original language and English, as Supporting Information.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: 1. As the unbalanced gender ratios in ASD and TD group is a reckoned limitation, what has been done to address this?

2. In the discussion, 2nd paragraph, it was suggested that the lack of significant correlation between SWM and AQ could be explained by the heterogenous nature of ASD. Would the authors elaborate on this?

3. In the discussion, 2nd paragraph, it was postulated that the inconsistent SWM findings in subjects with high ALT was due to the lack of objective indicators of ALT. Do the authors mean that the ALTs they identify are not actually ALT? Such that the subsequent conclusions can not really be drawn?

4. On page 10, first paragraph, would the authors elaborate on the statement 'it enlightens that spatial working memory is a specifically cognitive elements in patients with ASD'. Following that, it was mentioned that SWM training might improve the performance of autism and alleviate the severity of the symptoms. Would the authors provide a reference for this?

5. The authors failed to provide an explanation on the inconsistent findings of normal visual processing skills in subjects with high ALT as compared to the existing literature.

6. The presence of correlation between SWM and visual perception in ASD and in the general population, and the lack of such correlation in the TD group was explained by ‘the interaction between cognitive functions’ as on page 11 line 11. Can the authors elaborate on this?

7. The conclusion suggested that the study results confirmed the deficits in neurocognitive functioning are autism-like traits and have a continuous distribution in the population. I wonder if the correlation analyses done in the population study can confer such a finding.

8. There are some typos (second sentence on page 4, second paragraph of discussion "which might be explainED") and the 1st sentence of the 3rd paragraph of P.10 was hard to read.

9. On page 9, second paragraph, the reference 46 was cited to support that idea that unaffected siblings showed normal SWM in high ALT. It was misleading as the study only compared HF-ASD, HF-ASD siblings and controls without assessment of autistic traits in the non-ASD groups.

Reviewer #2: Thank you for the opportunity to review this manuscript, in which the authors analyze spatial working memory and visual perception in children with Autism Spectrum Disorder and general population with high autism-like traits. This is a very interesting topic.

Despite the interest of the paper, I have some comments and recommendations for the authors.

First of all, the title refers to “autism children”, but using the expression “children with autism spectrum disorder” may be more appropriate.

Introduction

When they say “Previous researches had provided inconclusive evidence about whether children with ASD are impaired in working memory. Many investigations suggested working memory impairment in ASD”, the authors should be more precise about the age range they are referring to: do they include also preschoolers? Or only school age children? Do they include adolescents in their analysis?

When describing the results of the meta-analysis by Wang et al (2017), the authors should specify the age group on which this meta-analysis is focused, executive functions change deeply during development, the age group considered could have an important meaning to the inconsistent results in the field.

The authors discuss theories about the psychopathological mechanisms of autism, references are needed for these theories.

When they describe the aims of the study, the authors mention the clarification of inner mechanisms of ASD, but these are difficult to explore without a longitudinal analysis.

Materials and methods

Participants

Using DSM-5 diagnostic criteria that include descriptive specifiers for a more precise, and appropriate, nomenclature could be a better choice than the general distinction between low and high functioning individuals.

Age group of the 52 participants (minimum and maximum) should be specified. It is not clear what the iq value reported refers to, is it mean IQ? In this case, the authors should specify it and add sd and minimum and maximum IQ.

The authors should add information about the demographic features of the second sample, such as age and sex. There should be a detailed description of the sample in this section.

Measures

The AQ factors should be briefly described to allow a better understanding of the instrument.

Results

Part 1 – correlations among Neuropsychological Tests and Symptom Severity

Understanding mechanisms is only possible with longitudinal data, cross sectional data can provide associations, but not cause. It would be better if the authors did not imply that Pearson correlations can detect inner mechanisms of autism.

Part II – Degrees of Autism-like traits.

It is not clear here whether there was a matching procedure or if the authors mean that the groups did not significantly differ in age and iq. This should be clarified.

Discussion

When the authors mention the inconsistent results due to different focus points (page 10, line 6), references should be added.

Page 10, line 18: the expression broader autism phenotype (BAP) is usually referred to individuals somehow related to people diagnosed with ASD which is not the case here. BAP can be defined as a milder expression of the underlying genetic liability for autism, as present in relatives of individuals with ASD. I am not sure this is the most appropriate expression to use here.

Page 10, line 25: this sentences should be rephrased, as its meaning is not very clear.

Page 10, line 29: I think the authors meant typical development, instead of typically

Page 10, line 37: “Look it at the other way”: this sentences should be rephrased

Page 11, line 13: “the relationship that cannot be repeated might because of the samples and the measures”: a verb is missing after might

I think the study has a solid design and interesting conclusions and implications. Nevertheless, it could be strengthened by adding the clarifications and modifications required.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Lai Chun Lun Eric

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files to be viewed.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email us at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2020 Jul 9;15(7):e0235552. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0235552.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


25 May 2020

Dear Editor,

Thank you for your valuable comments on our manuscript. We have revised the manuscript according to the reviewers’ comments and carefully proofread it to minimize errors.

Our point-to-point response to the reviewers’ comments are given below. 

Part Ⅰ:

Journal requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements:

Thank you for your valuable comments on our manuscript. We are sorry for all the mistakes. We have written the following information in the manuscript.

1.Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming.  

Thanks. We have checked the style requirements.

2. In your Methods section, please provide additional information about the participant recruitment method and the demographic details of your participants for both the case-control study and the community population study. Please ensure you have provided sufficient details to replicate the analyses such as:

a) the recruitment date range (month and year),

page6 line5 during the period from September 2016 to June 2018.

b) a description of any inclusion/exclusion criteria that were applied to participant recruitment,

page 7 second paragraph line3 The exclusion criteria of our study included: a history of psychotic disorder, severe head injury, syndromic genetic disorder associated with autism (e.g. fragile X syndrome), intellectual disability (i.e. IQ <70), Tourette’s disorder, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, any other medical condition significantly affecting brain function; unable to cooperate to complete the test.

c) a table of relevant demographic details,

we have added Table 1(page10) and Table 3 (page13)to show demographic details of case-control study and population study respectively.

d) a specific description of how participants were recruited online for the case-control study

page6 line6 Our online recruitment advertises were on the social platform, WeChat official account and were distributed over a period of time to recruit more volunteers.

e) descriptions of the specific locations where participants were recruited (names of the three primary schools for the community population study).

Page 6 second paragraph line6 Pi Xian is a county with 750,000 population consists of 13 districts. The population study was conducted in Pi Xian county, Xi Pu district from October 2016 to January 2017. Two thousand nine hundred ninety-four school aged students in three public primary schools -Si Yuan experimental primary school, Xi Pu experimental primary school, and Xi Pu foreign language primary school- were included in our study which can stand for the characteristics of the children population of the district.

3. Please provide a sample size and power calculation in the Methods, or discuss the reasons for not performing one before study initiation.

Page 6 second paragraph line12 According to the sample size calculation formula N= (UαS/δ) 2, (Uα is the U value corresponding to the inspection level α, set as Uα=1.96; S is the standard deviation, δ is the allowable error; assume S / δ = 5), the estimated sample size is N= 99. Considering the loss of interview and quality control, we chose all the students in the population study. 

4.  Please include additional information regarding the questionnaires or scales used in the study (ADOS-G, WISC-III, Beery VMI and SVM and ensure that you have provided sufficient details that others could replicate the analyses. For instance, if you developed a questionnaire as part of this study and it is not under a copyright more restrictive than CC-BY, please include a copy, in both the original language and English, as Supporting Information.

Thank you for your valuable comments on our manuscript.

Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ), additional information:

Page 7 third paragraph line4 AQ-35 Chinese version consisted of five tightly semantically coherent subscale constructs, named as Socialness, Mindreading, Patterns, Attention to Details and Attention Switching. The items clustered in a way that depicted distinctive dimensions of ASD symptomatology.

Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R) and Autism Diagnostic Observation Scale-General (ADOS-G), additional information:

Page 8 line5 The Chinese ADI-R was approved by the World Psychological Association in 2007 (56). The ratings were based on an assessment under current conditions and under the most severe state at 4–5 years, as recalled by the caregivers. It covers most developmental and behavioral aspects of ASD, including reciprocal social interaction, communication, and repetitive behaviors and stereotyped patterns. The ADOS-G is a semi-structured, standardized assessment of social interaction, communication, play, and imaginative use of materials (57). It has four different units for different levels of development and verbal abilities. ADOS-G provides scores that are distinct in three domains: language and communication score, reciprocal social interaction scores and total scores.

Chinese Version of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale (short-version), additional information:

Page 8 third paragraph line4 For the present investigation, FSIQ was measured using the WISC–III.

Spatial Working Memory (SWM), additional information:

Page 9 line1 Touching any box in which a blue token has already been found is an error. The number of boxes is gradually increased from three to eight boxes. The color and position of the boxes used are changed from trial to trial to discourage the use of stereotyped search strategies.

The Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration (Beery VMI), additional information:

Page 9 second paragraph line4 Considering the longitudinal nature of the parent project, the 3-5th editions of the Beery VMI were used during the various stages of data collection. The stimuli in the various editions did not change.

Part Ⅱ

Reviewer #1:

Q1. As the unbalanced gender ratios in ASD and TD group is a reckoned limitation, what has been done to address this?

Thanks for your valuable comments. We are sorry about the mistake, and we tried to address the problem as follows. To minimize the effect of unmatched IQ, age, and gender ratio between groups, the ANCOVA was conducted with IQ, gender, and age as covariant.

Table2. Performance differences on Spatial Working Memory (SWM) and Berry-VMI between ASD and control.

Q2. In the discussion, 2nd para

graph, it was suggested that the lack of significant correlation between SWM and AQ could be explained by the heterogenous nature of ASD. Would the authors elaborate on this?

Thanks for your valuable comments. We worked to discuss this issue as follows.

Page15 third paragraph Line8 On contrary, we found that the association between SWM and AQ was not statistically significant. It may be related to different age ranges. Previous studies were conducted in a wide range of age from children to adolescents, and our study was conducted in children aged from 6 to 12 years. Secondly, the tools used are different. A questionnaire, the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function, BRIEF was used in the previous study; however, the present study used CANTAB which was a common and reliable measure. The BRIEF is not specific enough to capture separate EFs based on the subscales(66). Furthermore, symptoms may be affected by many factors, and the effect of cognitive function on symptoms may be related to other mediating factors. Interaction between cognitive functions may also have an impact on the results that SWM impairment found in ASD and high ALTs is not associated with overall level of autism symptom severity measured with the AQ. Another possible explanation for this pattern could be that spatial working memory is related to aspects of ASD that may not be covered well by the AQ. Additional studies using larger samples of participants in all age periods that combine laboratory and informant-based measures are necessary to clarify this issue.

Q3. In the discussion, 2nd paragraph, it was postulated that the inconsistent SWM findings in subjects with high ALT was due to the lack of objective indicators of ALT. Do the authors mean that the ALTs they identify are not actually ALT? Such that the subsequent conclusions can not really be drawn?

Thanks for pointing this out. We are sorry about that we mentioned the reasons of the inconsistent in an inappropriate way. We have elaborated on this as follows.

Page16 Line9 However, recently one study conducted in unaffected siblings, showed spatial working memory wasn’t a broader autism phenotype of ASD [48]. The previous study didn’t identify spatial working memory as broader autism phenotype of ASD, which may be explained by the small sample size and the limitation of the term ‘broader autism phenotype’; and in our study we applied AQ to measure ALTs.

Q4. On page 10, first paragraph, would the authors elaborate on the statement 'it enlightens that spatial working memory is a specifically cognitive elements in patients with ASD'. Following that, it was mentioned that SWM training might improve the performance of autism and alleviate the severity of the symptoms. Would the authors provide a reference for this?

Thanks for pointing this out. We are sorry about the inappropriate expressions.

Page16 Line18 Investigating executive function components that contribute to the severity of autistic symptomology provides insight for the development of effective interventions for individuals with ASD. Previous study proposed that the working memory trains improved classroom performances of children with ASD (68). We speculated that spatial working memory was an autism-like trait; and professional spatial working memory training might alleviate the severity of the symptoms.

Q5. The authors failed to provide an explanation on the inconsistent findings of normal visual processing skills in subjects with high ALT as compared to the existing literature.

Thanks for your valuable comments. we have rewritten the discussion of this issue.

Page 16 second paragraph line3 In our study, the visual perception function deficit was found in ASD group when compared with typically developing children. Previous studies have found that individuals with autism have visual perception dysfunctions, which was predictor to the severity of symptoms (70-75). Except that Green et al. did not repeat the results in children with ASD (64). However, the population study results indicated that higher trait group behave comparable of lower trait group. This is at odds with previous research suggesting that people with high autistic traits have impaired visual illusions (76), and visual search(77). It should be noted that the AQ also correlates with IQ(78), and it could be IQ that produces the no significant results of high trait group. In addition, the visual perception inconsistency of the ALTs prompted us to carry out better visual psychophysics using more sensitive measures of perception in general population with various degrees of autistic traits. Future brain mapping studies could provide additional insight into the neural underpinnings of how visual perception might and might not be affected in autism.

Q6. The presence of correlation between SWM and visual perception in ASD and in the general population, and the lack of such correlation in the TD group was explained by ‘the interaction between cognitive functions’ as on page 11 line 11. Can the authors elaborate on this?

Thanks for your valuable comments. we have rewritten the discussion of this issue.

Page 18 line3 The present study found that visual perception deteriorated as working memory performance decreased in the ASD group. While the relationship didn’t repeat in control group, possibly because copying geometric figures in the VMI tasks was not so hard for typically developing children to remember; at the same time children could check the geometric figures again if they forgot them. Also, the small sample size may have an impact on the results of control group, and we tried to address it by community population study with a larger sample size, and results of the general population children suggested a positive correlation between SWM and visual perception. Researches in preschool children suggested working memory has no impact on the VMI skills[40], the age range might explain the controversy. Further study was needed to explore mechanism of the relationship between working memory and visual perception using more meticulous measures.

Q7. The conclusion suggested that the study results confirmed the deficits in neurocognitive functioning are autism-like traits and have a continuous distribution in the population. I wonder if the correlation analyses done in the population study can confer such a finding.

Thanks for pointing this out. We are sorry about the inappropriate expressions. We have rewritten the sentence.

Page 19 second paragraph line2 Our results suggested that spatial working memory deficit was a characteristic of autism, and may be distributed across the general population. Furthermore, we speculated a correlation between spatial working memory and visual perception in children with ASD and general population.

Q8. There are some typos (second sentence on page 4, second paragraph of discussion "which might be explainED") and the 1st sentence of the 3rd paragraph of P.10 was hard to read.

Thanks for pointing this out. We are sorry about the mistakes we have made.

1.second sentence on page 4: Visual perception and executive function were studied in the same task, The results suggest that visual perception is dissociable with executive function(39).

Page 5 line4 Karisa and his team, using a sophisticated measure that tested visual processing and executive function, found that visual perception is dissociable with executive function in individuals with various degree of ALTs(41).

2.second paragraph of discussion "which might be explained

we have rewritten the paragraph, and the sentence has been deleted already.

3.1st sentence of the 3rd paragraph of P.10: Traditional case-control studies are difficult to find the characteristics of quantitative phenotypes such as ASD, while population study can overcome this shortcoming

Page 17 second paragraph line1 It is challenging for traditional case-control studies to invest diseases that distributed normally in population. However, population study is just helpful to explore the diseases with a continuous distribution of characteristics in the population (79).

Q9. On page 9, second paragraph, the reference 46 was cited to support that idea that unaffected siblings showed normal SWM in high ALT. It was misleading as the study only compared HF-ASD, HF-ASD siblings and controls without assessment of autistic traits in the non-ASD groups.

Thank you for your valuable comments on our manuscript. We are sorry about the mistake. What we mentioned may be ‘broader autism phenotype’ but not high ALT.

Page 16 line9 However, recently one study conducted in unaffected siblings, showed spatial working memory wasn’t a broader autism phenotype of ASD [48]. The previous study didn’t identify spatial working memory as broader autism phenotype of ASD, which may be explained by the small sample size and the limitation of the term ‘broader autism phenotype’; and in our study we applied AQ to measure ALTs.

Part Ⅲ

Reviewer #2:

Thank you for the opportunity to review this manuscript, in which the authors analyze spatial working memory and visual perception in children with Autism Spectrum Disorder and general population with high autism-like traits. This is a very interesting topic.

Despite the interest of the paper, I have some comments and recommendations for the authors.

Q1. First of all, the title refers to “autism children”, but using the expression “children with autism spectrum disorder” may be more appropriate.

Thank you for your valuable comments on our manuscript. We have changed the title as ‘Exploring the spatial working memory and visual perception in autism children with Autism Spectrum Disorder and general population with high autism-like traits’, and in the manuscript all the words refer to autism,autistic children, are using ‘children with ASD’ instead.

Introduction

Q2. When they say “Previous researches had provided inconclusive evidence about whether children with ASD are impaired in working memory. Many investigations suggested working memory impairment in ASD”, the authors should be more precise about the age range they are referring to: do they include also preschoolers? Or only school age children? Do they include adolescents in their analysis?

When describing the results of the meta-analysis by Wang et al (2017), the authors should specify the age group on which this meta-analysis is focused, executive functions change deeply during development, the age group considered could have an important meaning to the inconsistent results in the field.

Thank you for your valuable comments on our manuscript. We have carefully reread the references and described the age ranges of these researches in the manuscript.

Page4 line1 Investigations of school aged children and adolescents suggested working memory impairment in ASD (21, 22). Working memory deficit is associated with symptoms of children and adolescents with ASD such as communication and socialization impairments (23, 24), restrictive and repetitive interests and behaviors (25). While some researches indicated normal working memory in preschoolers and adults with ASD(26-28). The mixed results indicated that adolescents with ASD showed remarkable working memory dysfunction, and indicated that age had important influences in the field. A Meta-Analysis (29) of working memory in individuals ranged from school-age children to adults suggested that spatial working memory was more severely impaired than verbal working memory.

Q3. The authors discuss theories about the psychopathological mechanisms of autism, references are needed for these theories.

Thanks for pointing this out. We are sorry about this mistake; we have attached the references:32-33

Page4 second paragraph line2 There have been many theories about the psychopathological mechanisms of ASD, such as ‘executive function deficit’, and ‘weak central coherence’[32, 33]

Q4. When they describe the aims of the study, the authors mention the clarification of inner mechanisms of ASD, but these are difficult to explore without a longitudinal analysis.

Thanks for pointing this out. We agree with you that the correlation analysis couldn’t stand for the relationship of cause and result, so it is unsuitable to mention inner mechanisms.

Page5 third paragraph line5 Furthermore, we detected the correlations between visual perception and spatial working memory.

Materials and methods

Participants

Q5. Using DSM-5 diagnostic criteria that include descriptive specifiers for a more precise, and appropriate, nomenclature could be a better choice than the general distinction between low and high functioning individuals.

Thanks for pointing this out. We have changed ‘high functioning ASD’ to ‘ASD without intellectual disability comorbidity’

Page5 fourth paragraph line2 We recruited ASD without intellectual disability comorbidity

Q6. Age group of the 52 participants (minimum and maximum) should be specified. It is not clear what the iq value reported refers to, is it mean IQ? In this case, the authors should specify it and add sd and minimum and maximum IQ.

Thank you for your valuable comments on our manuscript. We are sorry about this mistake. We have added those demographic features in the manuscript.

Page5 fourth paragraph line2 We recruited ASD without intellectual disability comorbidity(n=52, mean age=9.23 years, SD=3.35 years, Min–Max=4-17 years; mean IQ=95.27,SD=19.18 , Min–Max=70-132; Male/Female=37/7) and typically developed control children (n=32, mean age=10.63 years, SD=3.15,Min–Max=6-16years; IQ=108.22, SD=13.94 , Min–Max=81-136; Male/Female=16/16). (see in table1.)

Q7. The authors should add information about the demographic features of the second sample, such as age and sex. There should be a detailed description of the sample in this section.

Thanks for pointing this out. We have added those demographic features in the manuscript.

Page7 second paragraph line7:Finally, the demographic features of the population study participants were as follows, higher trait group(n=115, mean age=8.35 years, SD=1.42 years, Min–Max=6-11 years; mean IQ=117.37, SD=12.25, Min–Max=86-139; Male/Female=44/71), lower trait group(n=105, mean age=8.03 years, SD=1.50 years, Min–Max=6-11 years; mean IQ=116.16,SD=10.74 , Min–Max=82-137; Male/Female=42/63).(see in table 3.)

Measures

Q8. The AQ factors should be briefly described to allow a better understanding of the instrument.

Thank you for your valuable comments on our manuscript. We agree with you that more detailed about the AQ subscales should be mentioned.

Page7 third paragraph line4:AQ-35 Chinese version consisted of five tightly semantically coherent subscale constructs, named as Socialness, Mindreading, Patterns, Attention to Details and Attention Switching. The items clustered in a way that depicted distinctive dimensions of ASD symptomatology.

Results

Q9. Part 1 – correlations among Neuropsychological Tests and Symptom Severity

Understanding mechanisms is only possible with longitudinal data, cross sectional data can provide associations, but not cause. It would be better if the authors did not imply that Pearson correlations can detect inner mechanisms of autism.

Thanks for pointing this out. We are sorry for the mistake we have made. We agree that it is not appropriate to say that correlation analysis can imply inner mechanism, and such expression has been deleted

Page12 fourth paragraph Line3 The results showed the correlation between SWM and visual perception in children with ASD did not repeat in typically developing controls.

Part II – Degrees of Autism-like traits.

Q10. It is not clear here whether there was a matching procedure or if the authors mean that the groups did not significantly differ in age and iq. This should be clarified.

Thank you for your valuable comments on our manuscript. In the present study we mean that ‘Higher trait group and lower trait group did not significantly differ in age and IQ.’

Discussion

Q11. When the authors mention the inconsistent results due to different focus points (page 10, line 6), references should be added.

Thanks for pointing this out. We are sorry for the mistake we have made. we have attached the references,13, 15-17, 70

Page16 second paragraph line1 However, previous studies on visual processing had inconsistent results due to different focus points (13, 15-17, 70).

Q12. Page 10, line 18: the expression broader autism phenotype (BAP) is usually referred to individuals somehow related to people diagnosed with ASD which is not the case here. BAP can be defined as a milder expression of the underlying genetic liability for autism, as present in relatives of individuals with ASD. I am not sure this is the most appropriate expression to use here.

Thanks for pointing this out. We have rewritten the paragraph and the sentence has been deleted already.

Q13. Page 10, line 25: this sentence should be rephrased, as its meaning is not very clear.

Thanks for pointing this out. We are sorry for the mistake. we have rephrased the sentences as follows.

Page 17, second paragraph line1 It is challenging for traditional case-control studies to invest diseases that distributed normally in population. However, population study is just helpful to explore the diseases with a continuous distribution of characteristics in the population(79).

Page 10, line 29: I think the authors meant typical development, instead of typically

Thanks for pointing this out. We are sorry for the mistake, and we have corrected the error.

Page 10, line 37: “Look it at the other way”: this sentence should be rephrased

Thanks for pointing this out. We are sorry for the mistake, and we have rephrased the sentence.

Page 17, second paragraph line17 Besides, the present study offered support to the approaches that regard autistic-like traits as a continuous variable in general population rather than qualitative variable.

Page 11, line 13: “the relationship that cannot be repeated might because of the samples and the measures”: a verb is missing after might

Thanks for pointing this out. We are sorry for the mistake. We have rephrased this sentence as follows.

Page 18, line11 Researches in preschool children suggested working memory has no impact on the VMI skills[40], the age range might explain the controversy.

I think the study has a solid design and interesting conclusions and implications. Nevertheless, it could be strengthened by adding the clarifications and modifications required.

Thanks for your valuable comments!

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.doc

Decision Letter 1

Inmaculada Riquelme

18 Jun 2020

Exploring the spatial working memory and visual perception in children with Autism Spectrum Disorder and general population with high autism-like traits

PONE-D-20-04238R1

Dear Dr. Huang,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Inmaculada Riquelme

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: LAI CHUN LUN ERIC

Acceptance letter

Inmaculada Riquelme

26 Jun 2020

PONE-D-20-04238R1

Exploring the spatial working memory and visual perception in children with Autism Spectrum Disorder and general population with high autism-like traits

Dear Dr. Huang:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Inmaculada Riquelme

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Data

    (XLSX)

    S1 File. STROBE statement—checklist of items that should be included in reports of observational studies.

    (DOCX)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.doc

    Data Availability Statement

    All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting Information files.


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES