Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2020 Jun 3;9:e56754. doi: 10.7554/eLife.56754

A size principle for recruitment of Drosophila leg motor neurons

Anthony W Azevedo 1, Evyn S Dickinson 1, Pralaksha Gurung 1, Lalanti Venkatasubramanian 2, Richard S Mann 2, John C Tuthill 1,
Editors: Ronald L Calabrese3, Ronald L Calabrese4
PMCID: PMC7347388  PMID: 32490810

Abstract

To move the body, the brain must precisely coordinate patterns of activity among diverse populations of motor neurons. Here, we use in vivo calcium imaging, electrophysiology, and behavior to understand how genetically-identified motor neurons control flexion of the fruit fly tibia. We find that leg motor neurons exhibit a coordinated gradient of anatomical, physiological, and functional properties. Large, fast motor neurons control high force, ballistic movements while small, slow motor neurons control low force, postural movements. Intermediate neurons fall between these two extremes. This hierarchical organization resembles the size principle, first proposed as a mechanism for establishing recruitment order among vertebrate motor neurons. Recordings in behaving flies confirmed that motor neurons are typically recruited in order from slow to fast. However, we also find that fast, intermediate, and slow motor neurons receive distinct proprioceptive feedback signals, suggesting that the size principle is not the only mechanism that dictates motor neuron recruitment. Overall, this work reveals the functional organization of the fly leg motor system and establishes Drosophila as a tractable system for investigating neural mechanisms of limb motor control.

Research organism: D. melanogaster

eLife digest

In the body, spindly nerve cells called motor neurons connect the brain to the muscles. Their role is to control movement, as they translate the electrical signals from the brain into instructions to the muscles. In humans, it takes over 150,000 motor neurons to control the movement of one leg; in contrast, fruit flies only need 50 neurons to operate a leg, despite also executing a variety of movements.

Fruit flies are commonly used in laboratories to study an array of biological processes, yet little is known about how their motor neurons direct movements. In particular, it was unclear whether the same principles that control how muscles contract in mammals also applied in the tiny fruit fly.

To begin investigating, Azevedo et al. mapped out the arrangement of motor neurons that control muscles in the fruit fly leg. As the leg moved, the activity of both the neurons and the muscles they controlled was recorded, as well as the force that had been generated.

The experiments showed that each motor neuron controls a certain range of leg force and speed: some produced small, slow motion important for posture and dexterity, while others created large, fast movements essential to running or escape. In addition, the neurons activate in a particular order – cells that control slow movements fire first, and those that direct fast maneuvers later. These processes are also found in other organisms, but the difference is that flies have so few neurons, allowing scientists to reliably identify each motor neuron. Future experiments will therefore be able to test how flies recruit the right neurons to create specific movement sequences.

Fruit flies are often used to research human illnesses that affect movement, such as motor neuron disease. A better understanding of the way their neural circuits coordinate the body could help reveal how these conditions emerge.

Introduction

Dexterous motor behaviors require precise neural control of muscle contraction to coordinate force production and timing across dozens to hundreds of muscles. This coordination is mediated by populations of motor neurons, which translate commands from the central nervous system into dynamic patterns of muscle contraction. Although motor neurons are the final common output of the brain, the scale and complexity of many motor systems have made it challenging to understand how motor neuron populations collectively control muscles and thus generate behavior. For example, a human leg is innervated by over 150,000 motor neurons and a single calf muscle is innervated by over 600 motor neurons (Kernell, 2006). How can the nervous system coordinate the activity of such large motor neuron populations to flexibly control the force, speed and precision of limb movements?

One way to streamline motor control is to establish a hierarchy among neurons controlling a particular movement, such as flexion of a joint. This hierarchy allows premotor circuits to excite different numbers of motor neurons depending on the required force: motor neurons controlling slow or weak movements are recruited first, followed by motor neurons that control progressively stronger, faster movements (Denny-Brown, 1929). A recruitment order for vertebrate motor neurons innervating a single muscle was first postulated over 60 years ago (Henneman, 1957). Subsequent work identified mechanisms associated with the recruitment order and synthesized these findings as the size principle, which states that small motor neurons, with lower spike thresholds, are recruited prior to larger neurons, which have higher spike thresholds (Henneman et al., 1965a; Henneman et al., 1965b; Henneman and Olson, 1965; Mcphedran et al., 1965; Mendell, 2005; McPhedran et al., 1965). Evidence for the size principle has been provided by electrophysiological analysis of motor neurons in a number of species, from crayfish to humans (Gabriel et al., 2003; Hill and Cattaert, 2008; McLean and Dougherty, 2015; Milner-Brown et al., 1973; Sasaki and Burrows, 1998). These studies have also described systematic relationships between motor neuron electrical excitability, recruitment order, conduction velocity, force production, and strength of sensory feedback and descending input (Bawa et al., 1984; Binder et al., 1983; Fleshman et al., 1981; Heckman and Binder, 1988; Kernell and Sjöholm, 1975; Zengel et al., 1985).

A simplifying assumption of the size principle is that all motor neurons within a pool receive identical presynaptic input, such that recruitment order is entirely dictated by the gradient of motor neuron excitability. However, recordings in vertebrates have found that presynaptic inputs may vary within a motor pool (McLean and Dougherty, 2015) and that recruitment order can change during specific movements (Desnedt and Gidaux, 1981; Kishore et al., 2014; Menelaou and McLean, 2012; Smith et al., 1980). Are these violations of the size principle a fundamental feature of motor control circuits or rare exceptions to the rule? Answering this question would be greatly aided by a tractable system in which it was possible to genetically target identified motor neurons for recording, manipulation, and mapping of presynaptic inputs.

The leg of the fruit fly, Drosophila melanogaster, contains 14 muscles which are innervated by just 53 motor neurons (Baek and Mann, 2009; Brierley et al., 2012; Maniates-Selvin et al., 2020; Soler et al., 2004). In spite of this tiny scale, the fly leg supports a variety of fast and flexible behaviors. During forward walking, the fly grips the substrate with the distal segment of its front leg (the tarsus) and flexes the femur-tibia joint, pulling the body forward (Video 4). The femur-tibia joint of a walking fly flexes and extends 10–20 times per second, reaching swing speeds of several thousand degrees per second (DeAngelis et al., 2019; Gowda et al., 2018; Mendes et al., 2013; Strauss and Heisenberg, 1990; Wosnitza et al., 2013). Flies also use their legs to target other body parts during grooming (Hampel et al., 2015; Seeds et al., 2014), for social behaviors like aggression and courtship (Clowney et al., 2015; Hoopfer et al., 2015), and to initiate flight take-off (Card and Dickinson, 2008; Zumstein et al., 2004) and landing (Ache et al., 2019). These behaviors require a wide range of muscle force production across multiple timescales. However, little is currently known about the organization and function of leg motor control circuits in the fly. While a great deal of progress has been made on understanding the processing of sensory signals in the Drosophila brain, we lack an understanding of how this information is translated into behavior by the fly’s ventral nerve cord (VNC). Investigating motor control in Drosophila is important because the fly’s compact nervous system and identified cell types make it a tractable system for comprehensive circuit analysis (Tuthill and Wilson, 2016a).

In this study, we investigate motor control of the Drosophila tibia. We first mapped the organization of tibia flexor motor units using calcium imaging from leg muscles in behaving flies (Figure 1). With electrophysiology, we then discovered that motor neurons controlling tibia flexion (Figure 2) lie along a gradient of anatomical and physiological properties (Figure 3) that correlate with muscle force production (Figure 4). Slow motor neurons produce <0.1 µN per spike, while fast motor neurons produce ~10 µN per spike, approximately equal to the fly’s weight. Recordings during spontaneous leg movements revealed a recruitment hierarchy: slow motor neurons typically fire first, followed by intermediate, then fast neurons (Figure 5). Interestingly, all tibia flexor motor neurons receive feedback from proprioceptors at the femur/tibia joint, but these sensory signals vary in amplitude, sign, and dynamics across the different motor neuron types (Figure 6). Optogenetic manipulation of each motor neuron type had unique and specific effects on the behavior of walking flies (Figure 7), consistent with their roles in controlling distinct force regimes.

Together, these data establish the organization and function of a key motor control module for the fly leg. Overall, we found that motor neurons controlling the fly tibia exhibit many features consistent with the size principle. However, we also observed that tibia motor neurons receive distinct proprioceptive feedback signals and that recruitment order is occasionally violated. Thus, in addition to the size principle, heterogeneous input from premotor circuits is likely to play an important role in coordinating neural activity within a motor pool.

Results

Organization and recruitment of motor units controlling the femur-tibia joint of Drosophila

We first sought to understand how muscles in the fly femur control movement of the tibia (Figure 1). Fly leg muscles are each composed of multiple fibers (Soler et al., 2004) and innervated by distinct motor neurons (Baek and Mann, 2009; Brierley et al., 2012). A motor neuron and the muscle fibers it innervates are referred to as a motor unit. In most invertebrate species, multiple motor neurons can innervate the same muscle fiber, so motor units may be overlapping (Hoyle, 1983).

Figure 1. Functional organization and recruitment order among muscles controlling the Drosophila leg.

(A) Muscles of the right prothoracic leg of a female Drosophila (MHC-LexA; 20XLexAop-GFP). (B) Muscles controlling tibia movement in the fly femur. Top and bottom are confocal sections through the femur. Anterior-posterior axis refers to the leg in a standing posture (Soler et al., 2004). (C) Top: K-means clustering of calcium signals (MHC-Gal4;UAS-GCaMP6f) based on correlation of pixel intensities during 180 s of self-generated leg movements in an example fly (unloaded waving, see methods). Bottom: average change in fluorescence for each cluster, in each frame, when the leg is extended (femur-tibia joint >120°, 20772 frames) vs. flexed (<30°, 61,860 frames), n = 5 flies. Flexion activity was consistently higher (p=0.01 for cluster 5, p<10−6 for all other clusters, 2-way ANOVA, Tukey-Kramer correction). (D) Schematic of the experimental setup. The fly is fixed in a holder so that it can pull on a calibrated force probe with the tibia while calcium signals are recorded from muscles in the femur. (E) Calcium activity in tibia muscles while the fly pulls on the force probe (bottom trace). Cluster six was obscured by the probe and not included. The middle row shows the smoothed, rectified derivative of the cluster fluorescence (dFi/dt) for the two brightest clusters (1 and 2), which we refer to as Flexors. Highlighted periods indicate that both Flexors 1 and 2 are active simultaneously (gray, dF/dt > 0.005), or that Flexor 2 alone is active (magenta). (F) 2D histogram of probe position and velocity, for all frames (n = 13,504) for a representative fly. The probe was often stationary (velocity = 0), either because the fly let go of the probe (F = 0), or because the fly pulled the probe as far as it could (F ~ 85 µN), reflected by the hotspots in the 2D histogram. In F–H), the white circles indicate the centroids of the distributions. (G) Top - 2D histogram of probe position vs. velocity when Flexor 2 fluorescence increased, but not Flexor 1 (n = 637 frames, same fly as F). Gray squares indicate hotspots in F), which are excluded here. Color scaled to log(50 frames). Bottom – example of instance in which Flexor 2 alone is active (magenta shading). (H) Top - Same as G), when Flexor 1 AND 2 fluorescence increased simultaneously (n = 1449 frames). Bottom – gray shading indicates instances of activity in both Flexors 1 and 2. (I) Fraction of total frames for each fly in which both Flexor 1 and 2 fluorescence increased (gray), Flexor 2 alone increased (magenta), or Flexor 1 alone increased. Number of frames for each of five flies: 32,916, 13,504, 37,136, 37,136, 24,476. (J) Shift in the centroid of the 2D histogram when Flexor 1 fluorescence is increasing along with Flexor 2 fluorescence (gray), compared to when Flexor 2 fluorescence alone is increasing (p<0.01, Wilcoxon rank sum test). Black line indicates example cell in G) and H).

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Wide-field calcium imaging of muscles in the femur.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

(A) Schematic of light path for widefield calcium imaging of femur muscles. Infrared illumination is used to track leg movement. GCaMP emission is reflected by a longpass dichroic mirror to a second camera. (B) Tracking the femur and tibia with DeepLabCut. A network was trained to detect six points on the femur and six points on the tibia in the IR illuminated videos of the leg. We calculated the center of the tibia from the six detected points. The elliptical arc of the centroid across frames allowed us to estimate the angle of elevation of the tibia relative to the plane of the metal holder (black background). (C) K-means clustering of GCaMP activity in muscle fibers during unloaded movements of the tibia. Clusters were similar across five flies, including the ventral distal cluster 1, the proximal cluster 2, the more dorsal cluster 3, and a thin dorsal cluster 4. Clusters 5 and 6 were more variable in shape and location. (D) Example epoch from Fly one showing the femur-tibia angle and the fluorescence of each cluster. (E) Averaged fluorescence across all frames in which the tibia was extended (top) vs. flexed (bottom), for Fly 1, normalized to the maximum ΔF/F. Note the lack of signal during extension. (F) K-means clustering with different numbers of clusters for Fly 1, shown in Figure 1. With fewer than five clusters, the proximal cluster tended to be much larger, incorporating much of the region labeled as cluster 3. With more than six clusters, the smallest and least modulated clusters tended to divide, not providing any further information. When k = 6, pixels in the extension region clustered together, but we did not see large increases in fluorescence with extension (Figure 1).
Figure 1—figure supplement 2. Calibration of the force probe dynamic properties.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2.

(A) The force probe tip was positioned over an analytical balance, and the base of the force probe was displaced. The weight (force) at each displacement is shown in blue, the linear fit is shown in red. The slope of the line is the spring constant, k = 0.2234 N/m. (B) To measure the effective mass and drag constant of the probe in saline, we flicked the force probe by displacing the tip with a glass hook until it lost contact and snapped back to rest. In blue is the displacement in each frame for 16 different flicks. In orange is the velocity. We used the ode45 solver in Matlab to fit the mass (m = 0.17 mg) and drag coefficient (c = 0.14E-3 kg/s). (C) Using the dynamical parameters from b, we calculated the portion of the force due to elastic properties of the force probe (blue), drag (red), and inertia (yellow). Here, a single spike evoked optogenetically in a fast motor neuron produced a small movement of the probe (as in Figure 4A, Video 2). In Figure 4D–F, we calculated force by including drag and inertia, but in other figures we report leg displacement and the approximation of force, assuming that drag and inertia are negligible. (D) The probe obscured the distal leg during spontaneous movements, so we limited the ROI for clustering to the proximal leg.
Figure 1—figure supplement 3. Flexor 2 signal is high whenever Flexor 1 is active.

Figure 1—figure supplement 3.

(A) The force probe histogram for frames when only Flexor 1 was active, scaled according to Figure 1H. (B) Flexor 2 ΔF/F for all frames across N = 5 flies in which 1) Flexor 2 is activating (21962 frames); 2) Flexor 2 alone is activating (6686 frames); 3) when Flexor 1 alone is active (1805 frames); 4) random values of cluster 2 ΔF/F (1805 values). The mean Flexor 2 ΔF/F is higher when Flexor 1 alone is active than when Flexor 2 is active (asterisk, p<10–8, 2-way ANOVA, Tukey-Kramer correction for multiple comparisons), indicating that Flexor 2 is also likely contracting even though ΔF/F is not increasing. (C) Example epochs in which the derivative of Flexor 1 fluorescence, but not Flexor 2 (blue shading) is positive. These epochs make clear that these periods arise when Flexor 2 and 1 are active and contracting together (Flexor 2 ΔF/F high as in Figure 1H), but the derivative of Flexor 2 ΔF/F is either negative or simply not large. The first example occurs near the end of a trial, when the LED was turned off (green arrow).
Figure 1—figure supplement 4. GFP control for k-means clustering of calcium activity.

Figure 1—figure supplement 4.

(A) K-means clustering of GFP expression in muscles during spontaneous movements. Clusters were noisy, interleaved and dispersed. In this case, the fly pulled on the probe and the clusters are superimposed over the image of the fly leg. (B) Changes in fluorescence of clusters calculated from videos of GFP expression during spontaneous movements. GFP expression was bright (as shown in Figure 1A), but did not change with movements.

We used widefield fluorescence imaging of muscle calcium signals with GCamp6f (Chen et al., 2013; Lindsay et al., 2017) to map the spatial organization of motor units in the fly femur during spontaneous tibia movements (Figure 1C, Figure 1—figure supplement 1, and Video 1). We observed that spatial patterns of calcium activity were different for different movements (e.g., tibia flexion vs. extension).

Video 1. Calcium imaging of femur muscles.

Download video file (6.3MB, mp4)

The video (1) illustrates the arrangement of musculature controlling the fly’s tibia; (2) schematizes the position of a restrained fly relative to the force probe fiber, together with wide-field calcium imaging from muscles in the leg and body; and then (3) shows simultaneous videos of the fly pulling on the force with muscle calcium signals in the femur, together with the movement of the probe, an EMG recording from the fast neuron, and the time course of cluster ΔF/F. Videos are slowed 3X.

To quantify this spatial organization, we performed unsupervised clustering so that pixels with correlated activity were grouped together into activity clusters. We assigned arbitrary numbers to these clusters, from ventral to dorsal (Figure 1—figure supplement 1). Three clusters significantly increased their fluorescence when the leg was flexed (Figure 1C); we refer to these clusters as Flexors 1, 2, and 3. Flexors 2 and 3 were active during most periods of tibia flexion, whereas Flexor 1 was only active only during large, fast movements (Figure 1E). This organization was similar across flies and robust to changes in clustering parameters (Figure 1—figure supplement 1). Likely because of the orientation of the leg in these experiments, we did not observe any clusters whose activity increased specifically during tibia extension (see Figure 1—figure supplement 1 and Materials and Methods for further discussion). For the remainder of this study, we focus our efforts on motor control of tibia flexion.

For Flexors 1 and 2, we could record and identify extracellular motor neuron spikes in the femur (electromyogram: EMG), which confirmed that the activity clusters identified through calcium imaging correspond to distinct motor units (Figure 1E and see Figure 2D, below). In other words, we propose that activity clusters 1 and 2 reflect the firing patterns of specific motor neurons. Below, we elaborate on this relationship further when discussing specific motor neurons.

Figure 2. Motor neurons controlling tibia flexion.

(A) Schematic of the muscle fibers innervated by motor neurons labeled by the following Gal4 lines: left: R81A07-Gal4, center: R22A08-Gal4, and right: R35C09-Gal4. (B) Biocytin fills of leg motor neurons from whole-cell patch clamp recordings, labeled in fixed tissue with strepavidin-Alexa-568 (green), counter stained with phalloidin-Alexa-633 (red) labeling actin in muscles, and autofluorescence of the cuticle (cyan). (C) Images of biocytin fills in the VNC following whole-cell patch recordings (schematic). Prothoracic neuromere is outlined. Cells were traced and ‘filled’ with the simple neurite tracer plugin in Fiji. (D) Example recordings from each motor neuron type during spontaneous tibia movement: whole-cell current clamp recordings with detected spike times above (top row, corrected for a liquid junction potential of −13 mV), EMG recordings (middle), and tibia movement (force probe position; bottom). We often observed EMG events from the fast and intermediate neurons. We did not observe EMG events associated with slow neuron firing, though we often observed flexion of the tibia without seeing EMG activity, as seen here.

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Central anatomy of flexor motor neurons.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(A) Maximum intensity projections of fast flexor motor neuron fill (green, neurobiotin). In magenta is the neuropil (nc82). (B) Traced neuron from A), using the simple neurite tracer plugin in FIJI. Also shown in Figure 2C. (C) Maximum intensity projections of intermediate flexor motor neuron fill (green, neurobiotin). In blue is GFP expression driven by R22A08-Gal4. Note, the cell body of the filled neuron was ripped off, but the motor neuron cell body on the contralateral side remains. (D) Traced neuron from C, as shown in Figure 2C. (E) Maximum intensity projections of slow flexor motor neuron fill (green, neurobiotin). In blue is GFP expression driven by R35C09-Gal4. (F) Traced neuron from E), as shown in Figure 2C.

Muscles controlling the same joint may exert different levels of force, depending on fiber type composition and musculoskeletal biomechanics. To understand the relationship between motor unit activity and force production, we allowed the fly to pull on a flexible probe with its tibia (Figure 1D). We calibrated the force required to deflect the probe a given distance and measured a spring constant of 0.22 μN/μm (Figure 1—figure supplement 2). Flies were able to move the force probe up to 400 μm, reaching speeds of 8 mm/s (Figure 1F). That is, the fly was capable of producing close to 100 μN of force at the tip of the tibia and changes in force of ~1.3 mN/s. For comparison, the mass of the fly is ~1 mg for a weight of ~10 μN; this means that the femur-tibia joint can produce enough force to lift approximately ten times the fly’s body weight.

We used the activity clusters computed from unloaded leg movements (Figure 1C) to examine whether different flexor motor units control different levels of tibia force production. We observed that Flexor 2 activity increased across a range of tibia velocities and forces, but Flexor 1 activity increased only during the fastest, most powerful movements. To quantify this relationship, we compared probe force and velocity when clusters were recruited either alone or together (Figure 1G–J). The kinetics of GCaMP6f are slow relative to fly tibia movements, so we examined only periods when the derivative of the fluorescence signal was positive, i.e. muscle contraction was increasing (Figure 1E). The highest probe forces and velocities were achieved only when both Flexors 1 and 2 were active together (Figure 1G–H). When Flexor 2 was active alone, probe velocities were always lower (Figure 1J). Occasionally, the derivative of Flexor 1 fluorescence alone was high (Figure 1I), but in these rare instances the intensity of Flexor 2 was also high (Figure 1—figure supplement 3), indicating that Flexor 2 was contracting. These results indicate that distinct motor units control distinct levels of force production, and that they are recruited in a specific order, with motor units controlling low forces firing prior to motor units controlling higher forces.

The results of Figure 1 illustrate two organizational features of fly leg motor control. First, fly tibia flexion is controlled by a number of distinct motor units in the femur that are active at different levels of force production. Second, the sequential activity of tibia flexor motor units is consistent with a hierarchical recruitment order. The spatial organization of tibia flexor motor units also provides a template that can be used to identify genetic driver lines that label specific motor neurons.

A gradient of electrophysiological and anatomical properties among motor neurons controlling flexion of the femur-tibia joint

We visually screened a large collection of Gal4 driver lines (Jenett et al., 2012) for expression in motor neurons that innervate the activity clusters we identified through calcium imaging (Figure 1). We focus our analysis on three lines that each label a single tibia flexor motor neuron in the femur. The first line (R81A07-Gal4) labels a motor neuron that innervates the high-force motor unit (Figure 2, left) that we identified through calcium imaging (Flexor 1). The second (R22A08-Gal4) labels a neuron that projects to the proximal femur (Figure 2, middle), one of several likely candidates that controls Flexor 2 (Baek and Mann, 2009). The muscle fibers innervated by these two neurons connect to the same tibia flexor tendon (Soler et al., 2004). The third line (R35C09-Gal4) labels a single motor neuron that projects to the distal part of the femur, where the signals in our calcium imaging experiments were weak and noisy (Figure 2, right). The muscle fibers in this distal region have been collectively referred to as the reductor muscle (Baek and Mann, 2009; Brierley et al., 2012; Soler et al., 2004), but their alignment and attachment points suggest they control flexion of the tibia (see Materials and Methods for further details).

For consistency with the literature on other insects (Burrows, 1996), and based on their functional properties described below, we refer to these three motor neurons as fast, intermediate, and slow. Previous studies of motor neuron development suggest that the fast motor neuron is a unique, embryonically born neuron that persists through metamorphosis, whereas the intermediate and slow motor neurons belong to larger cohorts born during metamorphosis (Baek and Mann, 2009; Brierley et al., 2012). Those studies estimated that 2–5 neurons innervate the proximal muscle fibers in the femur, similar to the intermediate motor neuron, and 8–9 neurons innervate distal muscle fibers, similar to the slow motor neuron.

Using in vivo whole-cell patch-clamp electrophysiology, we recorded the membrane potential of each motor neuron while the fly pulled on the force probe. The recording configuration was similar to that used for calcium imaging (Figure 1). We observed increases in the firing rate of each motor neuron type during tibia flexion (Figure 2D). Simultaneous EMG recordings from motor neuron axons in the femur confirmed that the fast motor neuron corresponds to Flexor 1, identified via calcium imaging (Figure 1). The intermediate neuron contributes to the activity of Flexor 2, likely along with one to two other similar intermediate neurons. Perhaps because of the small size of its axon, we were unable to identify slow motor neuron spikes with extracellular recordings.

The three identified motor neurons have conspicuous differences in the size of their axons, dendrites, and cell bodies (Figure 3A). The fast motor neuron has an exceptionally large soma (for a Drosophila neuron) and thick dendritic branches. Its axon has a wide diameter and branches extensively in the femur (Figure 2B). The intermediate motor neuron has a smaller cell body, thinner dendritic branches, and smaller axonal arborization in the femur. The slow motor neuron has the smallest cell body, dendrites, and axon of the three. Each motor neuron has a unique dendritic branching pattern within the VNC neuropil. For example, the intermediate neuron projects toward the midline, and the arborizations of the fast and intermediate neurons branch more extensively than the slow neuron (Figure 2C, Figure 2—figure supplement 1). Thus, each motor neuron is anatomically positioned to receive distinct presynaptic inputs.

Figure 3. A gradient of intrinsic properties among tibia flexor motor neurons.

Figure 3.

(A) Left) Maximum intensity projection of a Neurobiotin fill (green) of the fast tibia flexor motor neuron, with nc82 counterstain. Right) Diameters of identified neuron 1) somas, 2) primary neurites, and 3) axons (p<0.01, 2-way ANOVA, Tukey-Kramer correction for multiple comparisons). (B) Example traces showing voltage responses to current injection in whole-cell recordings from each motor neuron type. (C) Average resting membrane potential for fast (n = 15), intermediate (n = 11), and slow tibia flexor motor neurons (n = 14) (p<0.001, 2-way ANOVA, Tukey-Kramer correction for multiple comparisons). (D) Average spontaneous firing rate in fast (n = 15), intermediate (n = 11), and slow neurons (n = 14) (p<10−5). (E) Input resistance in fast (n = 15), intermediate (n = 11), and slow neurons (n = 14) (p<0.001, 2-way ANOVA, Tukey-Kramer correction for multiple comparisons). All intrinsic properties in C-E) were calculated from periods when the fly was not moving.

The intrinsic electrical properties of the tibia flexor motor neurons varied along a continuum (Figure 3B–E). The average resting potential of fast motor neurons was lower (−68 mV) than that of intermediate (−60 mV) and slow (−48 mV) motor neurons (Figure 3C). While the fast and intermediate neurons were silent at rest, the slow neuron had a resting spike rate of approximately 30 Hz (Figure 3D). We also observed a gradient in input resistance, which we calculated from the voltage responses to small hyperpolarizing current injections before each trial (−5 pA). The input resistance was 150 MΩ for fast motor neurons, 300 MΩ for intermediate motor neurons, and 700 MΩ for slow motor neurons. Current injection in the fast and intermediate neurons failed to reliably trigger action potentials, based on recordings from both the cell body (whole-cell) and axon (EMG). This is likely because of intrinsic morphological or electrophysiological properties that electrically isolate the soma from the spike initiation zone (Sasaki and Burrows, 1998). By contrast, injecting current into the slow neuron effectively modulated the spike rate (Figure 3B).

Overall, our electrophysiological characterization of tibia flexor motor neurons (Figures 23) revealed a systematic relationship between motor neuron anatomy, input resistance, resting membrane potential, and spontaneous firing rate. These properties accompany differences in the force generated by a spike in each neuron, which we quantify next.

A gradient of force per spike across motor neurons

How much force does a single spike in a motor neuron produce? How does force-per-spike, or gain, vary across neurons within a motor pool? To answer these questions, we measured probe displacement as a function of firing rate in each motor neuron type. To evoke spikes in the fast and intermediate neurons, we optogenetically stimulated motor neuron dendrites in the VNC using expression of Chrimson (Klapoetke et al., 2014), a red-shifted channelrhodopsin (Figure 4A–B). Brief (10–20 ms) flashes of increasing intensity produced increasing numbers of spikes, corresponding spikes in the EMG, and movement of the tibia detected by small displacements of the force probe (Video 2). Aligning probe movement to spike onset showed that a single fast motor neuron spike produced ~10 μN of force, resulting in a 50 μm movement of the force probe (Figure 4A). An intermediate neuron spike produced ~1 μN that moved the tibia 5 μm (Figure 4B). For comparison, during take-off, the peak force production of the fly leg is ~100 μN (Zumstein et al., 2004).

Figure 4. A gradient of force production among tibia flexor motor neurons.

(A) Optogenetic activation of a fast flexor motor neuron expressing CsChrimson (50 ms flash from a 625 nm LED,~2 mW/mm2). Traces show average membrane potential for trials with one (top) and two (bottom) spikes, the average EMG (top) or overlaid EMGs (bottom), and the resulting tibia movement for each trial (50 µm = 11 µN). The jitter in the force probe movement traces results from variability of when a spike occurs relative to the video exposure (170 fps). (B) Same as A for an example intermediate flexor motor neuron. (C) Tibia movement resulting from current injection in a slow flexor motor neuron. Top: spike rasters from an example cell during current injection. Firing rates are shown below, color coded according to current injection value, followed by the baseline subtracted average movement of the probe (5 µm = 1.1 µN). Bottom: spike rates and probe movement in the presence of the cholinergic antagonist MLA (1 µM), which reduces excitatory synaptic input to the motor neuron. (D) Peak average force vs. number of spikes for fast (blue), intermediate (magenta), and slow (green) motor neurons. The number of spikes in slow neurons is computed as the average number of spikes during positive current injection steps minus the baseline firing rate; that is, the number of additional spikes above baseline. The black line is a linear fit to the slow motor neuron data points, with the slope indicated below. (E) Peak probe displacement for 2 spikes vs. one spike in fast (blue) and intermediate motor neurons (magenta). (F) Summary data showing that zero spikes in fast (n = 7) and intermediate neurons (n = 6) does not cause probe movement, but that hyperpolarization in slow motor neurons (n = 9) causes the fly to let go of the probe, that is, decreases the applied force. The number of spikes (x-axis) is computed as the average number of spikes per trial during the hyperpolarization.(G) Delay between a spike in the cell body and the EMG spike (conduction delay). Note there may be a delay from the spike initiation zone to the cell body that is not captured (n = 5 intermediate cells), p=0.6, Wilcoxon rank sum test. (H) Time to half maximal probe displacement for fast (blue) and intermediate cells (magenta), p=0.2, rank sum test. (I) Estimates of the maximum velocity of tibia movement in each fast (blue) and intermediate motor neuron (magenta), p=0.0012, rank sum test. A line was fit to the rising phase of probe points aligned to single spikes as in B) and D).

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Example recordings from other tibia flexor neurons.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

(A) Confocal image of an intermediate flexor motor neuron axon (neurobiotin fill is shown in green; red is phalloidin) from UAS-GFP;R81A06-Gal4. The filled neuron targets the proximal femur. Scale bar is 50 µm. The axon appears to target different fibers than the intermediate neuron in R22A08-Gal4. (B) Membrane potential, spikes, and probe movement during spontaneous movements. Membrane potential reflects and slightly precedes flexion events, and spikes occur at low forces. (C) Single trial example of sensory feedback. The neuron rests at −67 mV. Passive flexion of the leg (60 μm = 8°) hyperpolarizes the intermediate neuron in R81A06-Gal4. Extension causes an 8 mV EPSP. Compare to Figure 6. (D) Confocal image of a flexor motor neuron axon in the leg, neurobiotin fill is shown in green; red is phalloidin. The recording was from an unidentified, non-GFP-expressing neuron in a UAS-GFP;R35C09-Gal4 fly. The GFP-expressing slow motor neuron is seen in cyan in the right image, at a different z position. The filled neuron targets fibers more proximal than the slow motor neuron. (E) This neuron rested at −55 mV and did not spike at rest. Current injection could evoke spikes, and moderate spike rates caused steady state tension similar in magnitude to a single intermediate neuron spike (~5 um displacement of the probe). Interestingly, the resulting load on the force probe was sufficient to rapidly force the leg into an extended position. This in turn caused a depolarization and spike in the motor neuron, as the fly returned the probe to the resting position by placing more force on the probe. (F) Single trial example of sensory feedback. A slow extension of the leg (60 μm = 8°) depolarized the neuron and drove spikes. (G) Confocal image of a flexor motor neuron axon from UAS-GFP;R81A06-Gal4. The filled neuron appears to have a more extensive axon terminal than the slow motor neuron labeled by R35C09-Gal4. (H) This neuron rested around −53 mV, with a spontaneous spike rate of 12 Hz. Large increases in spike rate increased the force on the probe. The recording ended prematurely and we did not capture the probe returning to rest, but note the depolarization and small increase in the spike rate following the end of the current injection, similar to the neuron in panels D-F). Noise in probe position is due to poor lighting and is not time locked to spikes. (I) Single trial example of sensory feedback. Extension of the leg (60 μm = 8°) of the probe depolarized the neuron and drove spikes. (J) Confocal image of a flexor motor neuron axon from UAS-GFP;R81A04-Gal4. (K) This neuron rested around −51 mV, with a spontaneous spike rate of 28 Hz, an input resistance of 486 MΩ, and could increase flexion force more than the slow neuron labeled by R35C09-Gal4. (L) Single trial example of sensory feedback. Extension of the leg (60 μm = 8°) of the probe depolarized the neuron and drove spikes.

Video 2. Motor neuron electrophysiology, force production, and tibia movement.

Download video file (8.6MB, mp4)

The video introduces 1) the dendritic morphology and axon projection of the three neurons we studied: fast, intermediate and slow; 2) shows video of individual trials from a fast neuron in which one or four spikes are driven with optogenetic stimulation in the VNC, together with probe movement, the whole-cell current clamp recording from the soma, and the EMG record from the leg; 3) shows video of individual trials from an intermediate neuron in which one or four spikes are driven with optogenetic stimulation in the VNC; 4) shows video of a trial from a slow motor neuron in which current injection at the soma depolarizes the neuron and drives ~ 100 spikes, and a trial in which the slow neuron is hyperpolarized, reducing the force on the probe. Videos are slowed 3X.

Increasing the spike rate of the slow motor neuron with current injection produced small (~1 µm) and slow tibia movements (Figure 4C). Decreasing slow motor neuron firing produced tibia extension (Figure 4C,E), suggesting that spontaneous firing in this cell contributes to the resting force on the probe. Bath application of 1 μM MLA, an antagonist of nicotinic acetylcholine receptors, led to a decrease in the spontaneous firing rate and reduced the resting force on the probe by ~1.5 μN, or ~15% of the fly’s weight (Figure 4C). This suggests that the spontaneous firing rate in slow motor neurons is set by excitatory synaptic input.

A comparison of force production as a function of firing rate revealed that fast, intermediate and slow motor neurons occupy distinct force production regimes which span three orders of magnitude (Figure 4D). The shapes of these force production curves were also distinct. For fast and intermediate neurons, the force produced by two spikes was ~1.6X the force produced by a single spike (Figure 4E) and the force-per-spike curves saturated at ~10 spikes (Figure 4D). These observations suggest that the fast and intermediate muscle fibers fatigue during repeated stimulation. In contrast, the resting spike rate of slow motor neurons maintains constant force on the probe (Figure 4C,F). Thus, slow motor neurons may be used to maintain body posture, while intermediate and fast motor neurons are transiently recruited to execute body movements. Consistent with this hypothesis, we did not observe that flies maintained sufficient resting force on the probe (>5 s) to require sustained firing of fast or intermediate neurons (Figure 1E).

The rates of force generation for the fast and intermediate motor units were greater than the slow motor unit. For example, the effect of a spike in the fast and intermediate motor neurons reached half maximal force in ~8.5 ms (Figure 4H), whereas the effect of increasing the spike rate in slow motor neurons was gradual and did not reach its peak within 500 ms. Hyperpolarizing slow motor neurons required ~100 ms to reach its maximal effect on force production, which could reflect slow release of muscle tension or could be due to activity of other motor neurons in the population that influence resting force on the probe.

We recorded from several other motor neurons controlling tibia flexion, none of which had more extreme properties than the fast and slow motor neurons in Figure 4. Importantly, these cells exhibited similar relationships in morphology, intrinsic properties, and force production (Figure 4—figure supplement 1), which provides confidence that these correlations are not an artifact of the motor neurons we have chosen to highlight in this study.

Motor neurons are recruited in a temporal sequence from weakest to strongest

Calcium imaging from leg muscles suggested that distinct motor units are active during distinct tibia movement regimes, and that this activity follows a recruitment order (Figure 1). To test these hypotheses more rigorously, we examined recordings from single cells and pairs of tibia flexor motor neurons during spontaneous leg movements (Figure 5A–C).

Figure 5. Motor neurons are recruited in a specific order across different motor regimes.

(A) Membrane potential, EMG, and probe movement, for two example epochs of spontaneous leg movement during a whole-cell recording of the fast motor neuron. Highlighted events are plotted in D. The EMG was recorded in the proximal femur to pick up both fast and intermediate spikes (scale is 10 pA). (B) Same as A, but for an intermediate motor neuron. The EMG for the intermediate neuron was recorded in the proximal part of the femur, the largest events reflect activity in the fast motor neuron (scale is 100 pA). (C) Same as A-B, but for a slow motor neuron. The EMG was recorded in the proximal femur, near the terminal bristle, and large events are likely from the intermediate neuron (scale is 100 pA). (D) Example force probe trajectories from movement examples. The grayscale in D matches that of the trajectories in A-C. Each vertex is a single frame; vertices with dots indicate the occurrence of a spike in the whole-cell recording. (E) 2D histograms of probe force and velocity for video frames within 25 ms following a spike across recordings in cells of each type (fast – five neurons, 4666 frames; intermediate – four neurons, 11,833 frames; slow – nine neurons, 65,473 frames). Crosses indicate the centroids of the histograms for individual neurons. Dots outlined in white indicate centroid of the 2D histograms of all frames, within and outside the 25 ms window. Color scale shows the log number of frames in each bin, normalized to the total number of frames. (F) Schematic illustrating firing rate predictions of the recruitment hierarchy. As force increases, left to right, first the slow neuron begins firing, then the intermediate neuron (magenta line). Once the fast neuron spikes, both intermediate and slow neurons are already firing (blue dotted line). (G) Left) Spike rate of intermediate neuron in 30 ms preceding fast EMG spike (n = 4 pairs); center) slow neuron spike rate preceding fast EMG spike (n = 2 pairs); right) and slow neuron spike rate preceding intermediate EMG spike (n = 3 pairs).

Figure 5.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1. Testing the recruitment hierarchy of motor neurons, in paired recordings and as a function of force probe position and velocity.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1.

(A) The effective spike rate of fast, intermediate and slow motor neurons, over the course of all trials with spontaneous leg movements. Instantaneous spike rates could be much higher. (B) From the 2D spike-triggered phase histograms and Bayes’ rule, we calculated a likelihood function in order to compare spiking regimes in different neurons. If the force probe has a particular position, p, and velocity, q, the likelihood that a motor neuron fired a spike in the preceding 25 ms (right column, linear color scale from 0 to 1) is given by the number of frames with p,q that follow a spike (middle column, shown in Figure 5) divided by the full distribution of p,q (left column). The centroids of the full histograms (left column) are shown in Figure 5. (C) Comparison of activity in recordings of pairs of motor neurons: the number of EMG spikes preceded by a spike in the whole-cell recording, i.e. in a neuron lower down the recruitment hierarchy. The left hand axis indicates the probability of a spike in each millisecond bin for 30 ms before the EMG spike. The right hand axis indicates the cumulative probability of observing a spike in the period before the EMG spike. We found that a small number of intermediate neurons were not preceded by slow neuron spikes (N = 110/3082 spikes). (D) An example trial in which the normal recruitment hierarchy was violated, which only occurred when the leg was unloaded (tibia angle, bottom). During rapid shaking of the leg (highlighted region), the intermediate neuron could spike before the slow motor neuron and have a higher instantaneous spike rate.

The membrane potential of each tibia flexor motor neuron reflected probe movement, even during periods of purely subthreshold activity (Figure 5A–C). This shows that motor neurons that are not currently spiking are receiving synaptic input that, if shared across the motor pool, could drive other motor neurons to spike (Gabriel and Büschges, 2007). This synaptic input could arise from premotor neurons or proprioceptive feedback.

To understand the relationship between motor neuron spiking and tibia movement, we compared force and velocity production in the 25 ms following each motor neuron spike (Figure 5D–E). Spikes in each motor neuron type tended to occur during specific regimes of force and velocity. Plots of probe position and movement during example epochs (Figure 5D) illustrate that fast motor neurons typically spiked when the tibia was already flexed and moving. In comparison, the only period during which slow motor neurons were silent was when the tibia was extending (negative probe velocity) or fully extended (zero probe position). As a result, the spike-triggered distribution of probe dynamics for the slow motor neuron is broad (Figure 5E, right). Large spike rate modulations in the slow motor neuron often coincided with gradual changes in the position of the probe, consistent with the slow kinetics of force production measured using current injection (Figure 4). In comparison, spikes in fast motor neurons caused the probe to rapidly accelerate and approach maximum velocity (Figure 5D–E). Intermediate neuron spikes could also produce small but measurable increases in force (Figure 5B, inset).

Overall, this analysis shows that each motor neuron type has a different regime of force probe position and velocity in which it is likely to spike (Figure 5—figure supplement 1). These regimes are overlapping. For example, in the regime where fast motor neuron spikes are likely, both intermediate and slow spikes are likely as well. Similarly, the slow motor neuron is likely to be spiking throughout the intermediate neuron’s regime. These relationships between motor neuron spiking and force production are consistent with the existence of a hierarchical recruitment order (Figure 5F).

We tested this model by examining paired recordings of somatic (whole-cell) and axonal (EMG) spikes from different motor neurons. Neurons lower in the recruitment hierarchy were consistently active when a neuron producing more force was recruited (Figure 5G). In other words, activity in slow neurons preceded that of intermediate neurons, and activity in intermediate neurons preceded that of fast neurons.

Interestingly, we sometimes observed violations of this recruitment order (Figure 5—figure supplement 1). The exceptions occurred when the fly was rapidly waving its leg, rather than pulling on the force probe. In these cases, the slow motor neuron spike rate appeared to be suppressed by inhibition. This phenomenon is similar to rapid paw shaking behavior in cats (Smith et al., 1980) and rapid swimming in zebrafish (Menelaou and McLean, 2012). Such violations suggest that motor neuron excitability is not the only factor that determines recruitment order. Rather, differences in presynaptic input may selectively de-recruit slow motor neurons through inhibition.

A gradient of sensory input to motor neurons

So far, we have shown that tibia flexor motor neurons exhibit a gradient of functional properties (Figures 24) and a recruitment order during natural leg movements (Figure 5). We next asked how motor neurons respond to proprioceptive sensory feedback. A key function of proprioceptive feedback is to maintain stability by counteracting sudden perturbations, such as when an animal stumbles during walking (Tuthill and Azim, 2018). Larger perturbations require more corrective force, so as feedback increases it should result in the recruitment of additional motor neurons with increasing force production capacity.

We first compared the amplitude and dynamics of proprioceptive feedback in flexor motor neurons in response to ramping movements of the tibia (Figure 6A–B). Passive extension of the tibia caused excitatory postsynaptic potentials (PSPs) in all three flexor motor neurons, a response known as a resistance reflex. PSP amplitude was largest in slow motor neurons and smallest in fast neurons (Figure 6C–D). The slope of the rising phase of the PSP increased with the rate of extension. In the fast and intermediate neurons, sensory-evoked PSPs typically failed to elicit spikes, whereas the firing rate of slow motor neurons was significantly modulated. Slow motor neurons were exceptionally sensitive to passive tibia movement: a 1° change in tibia angle resulted in a significant change in firing rate (Figure 6F). Extremely fast extension movements were sufficient to evoke single spikes in intermediate neurons (Figure 6—figure supplement 1), but we never observed feedback-evoked spikes in fast motor neurons. The amplitude of the PSP evoked by proprioceptive feedback did not vary across different resting femur-tibia joint angles (Figure 6—figure supplement 1).

Figure 6. A gradient of proprioceptive feedback to motor neurons controlling tibia flexion.

(A) Whole-cell recordings of fast, intermediate, and slow motor neurons in response to flexion (black) and extension (gray) of the tibia. Each trace shows the average membrane potential of a single neuron (n = 7 trials). A 60 μm movement of the probe resulted in an 8° change in femur-tibia angle. (B) Spike rasters and firing rates for the same slow neuron as in A. (C) Amplitude of the postsynaptic potential (PSP) vs. angular velocity of the femur-tibia angle (n = 7 fast, seven intermediate, nine slow cells). Responses to fast extension (−123°/s) are not significantly different for fast and intermediate neurons (p=0.5) but are different for slow neurons (p<10−5, 2-way ANOVA, Tukey-Kramer correction for multiple comparisons). (D) Amplitude of firing rate changes in slow motor neurons vs. angular velocity of the femur-tibia angle. (E) Time-course of normalized average responses (± s.e.m. of baseline subtracted responses) to tibia extension (left) and flexion (right, n = 7 fast, seven intermediate, nine slow cells). (F) Sensitivity of firing rate to small tibia movements of 6 μm (0.8°) or 18 μm (2.4°). Spike rasters are from representative cells, the average spike rate across cells is plotted in green below.

Figure 6.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1. Properties of sensory feedback to motor neurons.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1.

(A) The EPSP evoked by fast ramping extension stimuli did not significantly depend on leg angle. (B) We measured the onset of the sensory evoked EPSP (magenta, intermediate neuron) in the motor neurons by fitting a line (black) to the rising phase of the average response. We measured the time from the start of the command to the piezoelectric actuator for the largest step stimulus (8° extension, shown here as reference). Sensory delays in the fast and intermediate neurons were similar. We did not measure the sensory delays in slow neurons because 1) the spike rate was calculated with an acausal smoothing filter and 2) the average EPSPs were not smooth because of the presence of spikes. (C) An example of reflex reversal in a slow motor neuron. Top) A normal resistance reflex in which an extension of the leg depolarizes the membrane potential and increases the spike rate. Bottom) When the fly was pulling on the force probe and the EMG activity was increased, the extension stimulus instead hyperpolarized the neuron. (D) To test faster stimuli than we could deliver with a piezoelectric actuator, we pulled on the probe with a hook until the hook lost contact, and the probe snapped back to rest. We identified intermediate EMG spikes using optogenetic excitation of R22A08-Gal4 motor neurons. This ballistic stimulus was able to evoke intermediate neuron spikes, but not fast neuron spikes (not shown).
Figure 6—figure supplement 2. Optogenetic activation of proprioceptive sensorimotor circuits.

Figure 6—figure supplement 2.

(A) Optogenetic activation of proprioceptive feedback to tibia motor neurons. The axons of femoral chordotonal neurons, labeled by iav-LexA, were stimulated with Chrimson activation during whole-cell recordings. (B) Example responses in an intermediate neuron to a high intensity 10 ms light flash that drove activity in chordotonal neurons expressing Chrimson (iav-LexA >Chrimson). The membrane potential and spikes from three trials are shown above (magenta shading) and the simultaneous force probe movement is shown below. Like the flicking stimulus in Figure 5—figure supplement 1, this large stimulus drove intermediate neurons, but not fast neurons, to spike. The effect of the light stimulus was surprisingly long-lasting, producing membrane potential fluctuations and movements over ~200 ms. (C) Expression of Chrimson decreases gain of proprioceptive feedback. Peak of the average EPSP in fast (blue), intermediate (magenta), and slow (green) motor neurons in response to a ramping extension stimulus (123 °/s), in flies that either express Chrimson in FeCO neurons (Chr+ reflex, empty circles) or not (WT reflex, filled circles). Chrimson expression reduces EPSPs, p<0.01 for all neurons, Wilcoxon rank-sum test. (D) Example recordings showing membrane potential, spiking activity, and probe position following an LED flash. Top: low intensity flashes caused the fly to mainly pull on the force probe, from individual fast (5.5 mW/mm2), intermediate (4.8 mW/mm2), and slow neurons (1.3 mW/mm2). Each trace is a single trial; the black traces belong to the same trial. Bottom: higher LED intensities caused the fly to let go of the force probe in fast (10.9 mW/mm2), intermediate (7.8 mW/mm2), and slow neurons (6.3 mW/mm2). (E) Summary of optogenetically-evoked membrane potential changes in motor neurons for low (flexion-producing) and high (extension-producing) LED intensities. Fast and intermediate neurons depolarized initially, so the amplitude of the depolarization is quantified. Slow motor neurons exhibited stronger hyperpolarization.

In addition to differences in amplitude, the sign and dynamics of proprioceptive feedback varied across fast, intermediate, and slow motor neurons. Fast and slow motor neurons both hyperpolarized slightly at flexion onset, while intermediate neurons depolarized once the flexion event ceased (Figure 6E). These results demonstrate that the different tibia flexor motor neurons receive distinct feedback signals from leg proprioceptors.

As reported in recordings from other insect species (Bässler and Büschges, 1998), we occasionally observed a reflex reversal, in which the sign of the proprioceptive PSP reversed (Figure 6—figure supplement 1). In most instances, tibia extension led to excitatory responses in flexor motor neurons. But occasionally, typically when the fly was actively moving as the stimulus was delivered, extension produced an inhibitory PSP. Reflex reversal is an important mechanism that could allow the fly to switch between active and passive motor states (Bässler and Büschges, 1998; Tuthill and Wilson, 2016a).

A major source of proprioceptive information about tibia position is the femoral chordotonal organ (FeCO). The Drosophila FeCO is comprised of ~150 mechanosensory neurons that encode position, movement, and vibration of the femur-tibia joint (Mamiya et al., 2018). We used optogenetic activation (iav-LexA > Chrimson) to measure proprioceptive feedback from the FeCO to each motor neuron type (Figure 6—figure supplement 2). Stimulating FeCO neurons caused the fly to extend and then flex its tibia in a rapid and stereotyped manner. Motor neuron responses to FeCO stimulation increased from slow motor neurons to fast motor neurons, consistent with responses to passive stimulation. In interpreting these experiments, it is important to note that Chrimson expression in FeCO neurons reduced motor neuron responses to passive leg movements compared to control flies (Figure 6—figure supplement 2). We speculate that this side-effect of Chrimson expression is caused by a homeostatic decrease in the gain of proprioceptive feedback pathways following increased excitability.

Overall, these data demonstrate that the FeCO provides proprioceptive feedback to tibia flexor motor neurons, and that the amplitude and dynamics of proprioceptive feedback signals vary across the different motor neurons. This gradient of sensory feedback amplitude is likely shaped by the concurrent gradient of motor neuron intrinsic properties (Figure 3). We observed differences in the sign and dynamics of proprioceptive feedback, which indicate that tibia flexor motor neurons receive distinct presynaptic inputs. These differences also suggest that proprioceptive feedback signals may be specialized for controlling particular motor neurons.

Optogenetic perturbation of single motor neurons alters walking behavior

We have thus far measured force production and recruitment order of slow, intermediate, and fast tibia flexor motor neurons under controlled conditions. We next used optogenetics to alter motor neuron activity in tethered, walking flies, in order to test how sensorimotor feedback loops respond to perturbations of normal activity patterns. Flies flex the tibia of the front leg to pull the body forward during walking (Video 4), so we examined how each motor neuron contributes to this movement. We focused a green laser at the ventral thorax, at the base of the left front (T1) leg to optogenetically activate (Gal4 >CsChrimson) (Klapoetke et al., 2014) or silence (Gal4 >gtACR1) (Mohammad et al., 2017) each motor neuron type. As a control, we used an empty-Gal4 driver line, which has the same genetic background but lacks Gal4 expression.

To verify our optogenetic manipulations and characterize basic leg reflexes, we first measured the movement of the femur-tibia joint in headless flies with their legs unloaded (i.e., the fly was suspended in air, Figure 7A). Activation of fast motor neurons caused rapid flexion of the tibia (Figure 7B,C, Video 3). We observed repeated patterns of tibia flexion and extension, suggesting that the fast neuron fired single spikes and that the resulting flexion was immediately countered by a resistance reflex. Activation of intermediate neurons caused slower tibia flexion, while activation of slow neurons caused even slower contractions, often followed by unexpected, prolonged tibia extension. Silencing each motor neuron type did not consistently evoke leg movements, though silencing the full motor neuron population resulted in paralysis (Video 4). These results are consistent with our measures of force production during electrophysiology (Figures 45) and provide a useful baseline for interpreting results from walking flies.

Figure 7. Optogenetic perturbation of motor neurons in behaving flies.

(A) Example frame illustrating behavioral effects of optogenetically activating leg motor neurons in headless flies. A green laser (530 nm) is focused at the coxa-body joint of the fly’s left front leg (outlined in white) and the femur-tibia joint (yellow) is monitored with high-speed video. (B) Example traces of femur-tibia joint angles during optogenetic activation (Gal4 >CsChrimson) of each motor neuron type in headless flies. The control line has the same genetic background as motor neuron lines but lacks Gal4 expression (BDP-Gal4 >CsChrimson).( C) Average (± sem) tibia flexion speed during the first 300 ms of CsChrimson activation (n = 5 flies of each genotype). (D) Schematic of the behavioral setup, in which a tethered fly walks on a spherical treadmill. The treadmill and fly are tracked with separate cameras. As in A), a green laser is focused on the coxa of the left front leg. (E) Average treadmill forward velocity (± sem) of walking flies, when activating (top row) or silencing (bottom row) control (n = 11, 13), fast (n = 15, 14), intermediate (n = 9,12), or slow (n = 11, 13) motor neurons for 90 ms (dark colors) or 720 ms (light colors). Black traces indicate trials with no laser (0 ms). Gray boxes indicate the period of optogenetic stimulation. Black dashes above fast and slow flexor activation traces indicate the time points in panel G. (F) Percent change (change in speed/pre-stimulus speed) in the average running speed of flies shown in C during stimulation+200 ms for activation (top) and silencing (bottom). Asterisks indicate p<0.01 relative to control (bootstrapping with false discovery rate correction; see methods for details). (G) Example frames showing representative behavioral responses of running flies during the first 200 ms of fast (top) and slow (bottom) MN activation. (H) Probability that a stationary fly initiated a walking bout during the 500 ms following either MN activation (left) or silencing (right). Dark colors are for the 90 ms stimulus (or no stimulus – black) and light colors are for the 720 ms stimulus (or no stimulus – gray). Asterisks indicate p<0.01 relative to control (bootstrapping with false discovery rate correction; see Methods for details).

Figure 7.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1. Effects of tibia motor neuron silencing and activation on leg kinematics.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1.

(A) Femur-tibia joint angles during silencing (Gal4 >gtACR1) of the three motor neuron types and a control empty Gal4 driver, in headless, suspended flies. (B) Average (± sem) flexion speed during the initial 300 ms of silencing, on trials where the leg moved. (C) Scatter plot of instantaneous flexion speed vs. leg angle for each video frame, when activating (left) or silencing (right) the three motor neuron types (and empty Gal4 control) in headless suspended flies. Extension would be seen as negative and is not shown. The flexion speeds are largest near 80° for trajectories caused by fast neuron activation (blue points).

Video 3. Optogenetic activation of motor neurons in headless flies.

Download video file (1.2MB, mp4)

The video shows the movements of the flies left front tibia caused by optogenetic activation of the fast, intermediate and slow neurons and a control line (BDP-Gal4). The video shows 1) the tibia flexion during the entire 720 ms laser stimulation period slowed 10X; 2) the first 300 ms of the same event, slowed 40X.

Video 4. Optogenetic silencing in all motor neurons controlling the front leg.

Download video file (4.6MB, mp4)

Motor neurons labelled by OK371-Gal4 expressed gtACR1. The video shows the behavior of different flies on different trials while 1) flies were walking during a 90 ms laser stimulus; 2) flies were stationary during a 90 ms laser stimulus; 3) flies were walking during a 720 ms laser stimulus; 4) flies were stationary during a 720 ms laser stimulus. Videos of behavior are slowed 10X.

To optogenetically manipulate motor neuron activity during walking, we positioned tethered flies on a spherical treadmill (i.e., a Styrofoam ball) within a visual arena (Reiser and Dickinson, 2008Figure 7D). Previous studies have found that walking speed and gait on the treadmill are similar to freely walking flies (Szczecinski et al., 2018). In our setup, the fly was motivated to walk forward with a wiggling stripe (Figure 7E), and we tracked the treadmill and the fly’s behavior with multiple high-speed cameras.

We first asked how flies would respond to transient optogenetic activation of the different tibia flexor motor neurons. Activation of the fast motor neuron caused the tibia of the front leg to persistently flex, thus, interrupting walking (Figure 7E, Video 5). Consequently, flies appeared to slow down, as measured by a decrease in forward walking velocity. Activation of the slow motor neurons also interrupted walking (Figure 7E), but this was because the fly reacted by extending its tibia, as when the legs were unloaded (Video 7). Based on the muscle fibers innervated by the slow motor neuron, we speculate that optogenetic activation of this neuron may increase twisting at the joint, which the fly reacts to with a compensatory extension of the leg. Unlike the other two cells, activation of the intermediate motor neuron appeared to cause the flies to transiently increase their walking speed. Activation of the intermediate motor neuron in stationary flies also caused them to start walking (Figure 7E–F, Video 6). Extra spikes in the intermediate tibia flexor motor neuron may accelerate the rotation of the treadmill, forcing the other legs to move faster in response. Overall, these results show that activation of different motor neurons drive distinct behavioral responses that depend on the fly’s behavioral state (e.g., walking vs. standing).

Video 5. Optogenetic activation and silencing of fast motor neurons in behaving flies.

Download video file (7MB, mp4)

The video shows the behavior of different flies on different trials while 1) flies were walking and the fast neuron expressed CsChrimson; 2) flies were stationary, and the fast neuron expressed CsChrimson; 4) flies were walking, and the fast neuron expressed gtACR1; 3) flies were stationary, and the fast neuron expressed gtACR1. Videos of behavior are slowed 10X.

Video 6. Optogenetic activation and silencing of intermediate motor neurons in behaving flies.

Download video file (8.6MB, mp4)

The video shows the behavior of different flies on different trials while 1) flies were walking and the intermediate neuron expressed CsChrimson; 2) flies were stationary, and the intermediate neuron expressed CsChrimson; 3) flies were walking, and the intermediate neuron expressed gtACR1; 4) flies were stationary, and the intermediate neuron expressed gtACR1. Videos of behavior are slowed 10X.

Video 7. Optogenetic activation and silencing of slow motor neurons in behaving flies.

Download video file (8.5MB, mp4)

The video shows the behavior of different flies on different trials while 1) flies were walking and the slow neuron expressed CsChrimson; 2) flies were stationary, and the slow neuron expressed CsChrimson; 3) flies were walking, and the slow neuron expressed gtACR1; 4) flies were stationary, and the slow neuron expressed gtACR1. Videos of behavior are slowed 10X.

Optogenetically silencing motor neurons had less pronounced effects on walking speed (Figure 7E). However, silencing of fast and intermediate neurons decreased the probability that a stationary fly would initiate walking compared to controls (Figure 7H, Videos 6 and 7). These results suggest that motor neurons with a high force production capacity (Figure 4) may be dispensable for normal walking behavior, but their activity may be important for producing the high levels of force required to initiate walking.

Discussion

In this study, we characterized the cellular morphology, electrical excitability, force production, and proprioceptive feedback among motor neurons that control flexion of the Drosophila tibia. We discovered a gradient of properties that co-varies across the motor pool (Figures 12). At one end of the spectrum is the fast motor neuron, which has a larger diameter axon and dendrites, low input resistance, weak sensory input, and produces enough force with each spike to support the fly’s entire body weight (Figures 34). At the other end, the slow motor neuron has fine axons and dendrites and high input resistance; each spike in the slow motor neuron produces <1% the force of a fast motor neuron spike. The slow motor neuron also has a high spontaneous firing rate that maintains resting muscle tension (Figure 4), and it receives strong proprioceptive feedback that continuously modulates its firing rate (Figure 6).

We found that leg motor neurons are recruited in a specific order: small, low gain motor neurons that generate weak forces are recruited first, and larger, more powerful motor neurons are recruited later (Figure 5). Consistent with this recruitment order, proprioceptive feedback has the greatest effect on the slow motor neuron: tibia movements of less than 1° significantly impact slow motor neuron firing (Figure 6). By comparison, proprioceptive feedback has a much smaller effect on the firing rate of intermediate and fast motor neurons. Fly behavior is also quite sensitive to aberrant optogenetic recruitment of motor neurons (Figure 7). For example, flies fail to initiate walking if unable to recruit slow motor neurons at the bottom of the recruitment hierarchy.

A relationship between force production and recruitment order is a common feature of vertebrate motor systems (Henneman et al., 1965a; Henneman and Olson, 1965; Kernell, 2006; Mcphedran et al., 1965; McPhedran et al., 1965). This organization has been proposed to confer a number of computational and energetic advantages. Recruitment of additional motor units increases force nonlinearly, overcoming suppressive nonlinearities in spike rates and muscle force production (Bakels and Kernell, 1994; Monster and Chan, 1977). Furthermore, the relative increase in force does not decrease with successive recruitment, as it would if all motor units produced similar amounts of force. Thus, much like Weber’s law describes the constant sensitivity to relative stimulus intensity (Fechner, 1860), a recruitment hierarchy maximizes the resolution of motor unit force while also simplifying the dimensionality of the motor system (Henneman et al., 1965b; Mendell, 2005).

Violations of the size principle and their implications for premotor circuit organization

A key assumption of the original size principle is that all motor neurons within a pool receive the same presynaptic input (Henneman et al., 1965a); the alternative is that different premotor neurons provide input to different subsets of motor neurons. Our results suggest the motor system controlling the fly tibia operates in a middle ground between these two extremes. Although tibia motor neurons generally follow a recruitment order in accordance with the size principle (Figure 5), we found that the dynamics of proprioceptive feedback vary across motor neurons within the pool (Figure 6). We also observed a small but significant proportion of behavioral events in which recruitment order was violated (Figure 5—figure supplement 1). These data suggest that although the tibia flexor motor neuron pool may share some presynaptic inputs, they are not identical. Thus, motor control of the fly tibia is more complex than a straightforward implementation of the size principle.

Similar exceptions to the strict dogma of the size principle have been observed in vertebrate species. For instance, the effect of proprioceptive feedback varies across a pool of motor neurons controlling the cat leg (Heckman and Binder, 1988; Heckman and Binder, 1991). During rapid escape behaviors in zebrafish, slow motor neurons can completely drop out of the population firing pattern (Menelaou and McLean, 2012). Muscles controlling human fingers can change recruitment order based on movement direction (Desmedt and Godaux, 1977). These examples suggest that some behaviors exhibit more degrees of freedom than can be supported by recruitment of motor neurons based on their intrinsic excitability alone.

Investigations of the vertebrate spinal cord have also identified circuit motifs that support flexible control of motor neurons within a pool (Kiehn, 2016; McLean and Dougherty, 2015). For example, recordings in turtles and zebrafish have shown that motor neurons receive coincident excitation and inhibition, which could underlie selective de-recruitment (Berg et al., 2007; Kishore et al., 2014). Zebrafish spinal cord premotor neurons provide patterned inhibition to speed-specific circuits (Callahan et al., 2019; Menelaou et al., 2019) and can flexibly switch between different motor neuron recruitment patterns (Bagnall and McLean, 2014).

Our characterization of identified tibia motor neurons provides a handle to investigate similar premotor circuit motifs for flexible limb motor control in the fly. Little is currently known about the leg premotor circuitry in Drosophila. However, the hypotheses generated by this work should soon be testable using connectomic reconstruction of identified motor neurons and their presynaptic inputs in the Drosophila VNC (Maniates-Selvin et al., 2020).

Organization of motor pools controlling the Drosophila tibia

The muscles that control flexion of the Drosophila tibia are innervated by approximately 15 motor neurons (Baek and Mann, 2009; Brierley et al., 2012). Here, we chose to analyze three identified neurons that span the extremes of this motor pool. Each of these motor neurons was stereotyped across flies in its anatomy, physiology, and function. The fast tibia flexor motor neuron labeled by R81A07-Gal4 is the only motor neuron that innervates the large tibia flexor muscle fibers in the middle of the femur (Figure 2B). Because it has the largest axon of any motor neuron in the femur, the fast motor neuron also produces the largest extracellular spikes (Figure 2D). The slow tibia flexor motor neuron labeled by R35C09-Gal4 is one of 8–9 motor neurons that innervates tibia flexor muscle fibers located at the distal tip of the femur. Measurements of force production from other motor neurons that innervated this distal region (Figure 4—figure supplement 1) suggest that the R35C09-Gal4 neuron is among the weakest within this group. Finally, we studied an intermediate motor neuron labeled by R22A08-Gal4 that innervates muscle fibers separate from the fast and slow neurons. Previous anatomical studies suggest that 2–5 neurons innervate nearby muscle fibers in the proximal region of the femur. Our recordings of other tibia flexor motor neurons (Figure 4—figure supplement 1) were consistent with the gradient of properties we describe in detail for three identified motor neurons, which suggests that the relationship between anatomy, physiology, and force production applies to other neurons in the tibia flexor motor pool.

The structure of the leg motor system in Drosophila has several similarities to other well-studied walking insects. In the metathoracic leg of the locust, a single tibia flexor muscle is innervated by nine motor neurons that were also classified into three groups: fast, intermediate, and slow (Burrows and Hoyle, 1973; Phillips, 1981; Sasaki and Burrows, 1998). Different motor neurons within this pool lie along a gradient of intrinsic properties (Sasaki and Burrows, 1998) and are sensitive to different types of proprioceptive feedback, such as position vs. velocity or fast vs. slow movements (Field and Burrows, 1982; Newland and Kondoh, 1997). The femur of the stick insect is innervated by flexor motor neurons that were also described as slow, semi-fast, and fast, based on their intrinsic properties and firing patterns during behavior (Bässler, 1993; Schmidt et al., 2001).

Much of the work in bigger insects and crustaceans has focused on the most reliably identifiable neurons, the fast and slow extensor tibiae (FETi and SETi), antagonists to the more diverse tibia flexor motor neurons. Consequently, details of how sensory feedback and local interneurons recruit specific subsets of flexor motor neurons has been relatively understudied (Clarac et al., 2000) (but see Gabriel et al., 2003; Gabriel and Büschges, 2007; Hill and Cattaert, 2008). Our work exemplifies the advantage of using Drosophila genetics to identify cell types, and even individual cells, within a diverse motor pool. Genetic markers have also recently been identified for tibia extensor motor neurons in flies (Venkatasubramanian et al., 2019), which will enable future investigation of premotor input to antagonist motor neurons.

Flies differ from these other invertebrates in one major respect: while most arthropods possess GABAergic motor neurons that directly inhibit leg muscles, holometabolous insects such as Drosophila do not (Schmid et al., 1999; Witten and Truman, 1998). Indeed, we found that a transgenic line for GABAergic neurons (Gad1-Gal4; Diao et al., 2015) does not label any axons in the Drosophila leg (data not shown). The presence of inhibitory motor neurons has been proposed as a key underlying reason why insects have been able to achieve flexible motor control with small numbers of motor neurons (Belanger, 2005; Wolf, 2014). That flies lack this capability means that other mechanisms must be at play. Fly legs are innervated by neurons that release neuromodulators, such as octopamine, which could underlie flexible tuning of muscle excitability (Zumstein et al., 2004).

Behavioral function of different motor neurons within a pool

The large range of gain, or force production, at the femur-tibia joint implies that different subgroups of flexor neurons will be recruited during distinct behaviors. Activity in slow motor neurons produces small, low force movements, while fast motor neuron activity produces fast, high amplitude movements. Consistent with this division, the output of slow motor neurons is more strongly influenced by feedback from leg proprioceptors (Figure 6). Thus, proprioceptive feedback may continuously modulate the firing rate of slow motor neurons for precise stabilization of posture during standing or grooming. When the slow motor neuron is optogenetically activated (Figure 7), flies stop walking and extend their legs. The reason for this response is unclear but could reflect the fly’s reaction to a loss of autonomous motor control.

In contrast to the postural movements controlled by slow motor neurons, rapid, stereotyped movements like escape (Card and Dickinson, 2008; Zumstein et al., 2004) are likely to use fast motor neurons whose activity is less dependent on sensory feedback. This division of labor may also provide energy efficiency: slow contracting muscle fibers use aerobic metabolism and take advantage of energy stores in the form of glycogen, while fast muscle fibers are anaerobic and lack energy stores, leading to more rapid fatigue (Kernell, 2006).

Even for a specific behavior, such as walking, different motor neurons may be recruited in different environments and contexts. We saw that optogenetically activating intermediate motor neurons caused stationary flies to start walking and walking flies to walk faster (Figure 7). In a similar manner, stick insects walking on treadmills typically recruit fast motor neurons only during fast walking and, in that case, late in the stance phase. But as friction on the treadmill increases, fast neurons are recruited earlier during stance (Gabriel et al., 2003). Leg kinematics and force production also change as insects walk up or down inclines (Dallmann et al., 2019; Wöhrl et al., 2017). When locusts (Duch and Pflüger, 1995) and cockroaches (Larsen et al., 1995; Watson and Ritzmann, 1997) are forced to walk upside-down, fast flexor neurons are recruited to allow the animal to grip the substrate. This context-dependence is not limited to walking: small changes in body posture can have a large effect on which motor neurons are recruited during target reaching movements in locusts (Page et al., 2008).

In this study, we chose to focus on flexion of the femur-tibia joint of the fly’s front leg. How might these results compare to motor neuron pools of antagonist muscles? Tibia flexor motor neurons outnumber the extensor neurons (Baek and Mann, 2009; Brierley et al., 2012), and thus are likely to possess a shallower gradient of intrinsic and functional properties, as has been found in crayfish (Hill and Cattaert, 2008). Invertebrate muscles also exhibit polyneural innervation, such that a given muscle fiber may be innervated by multiple motor neurons (Brierley et al., 2012; Hoyle, 1983; Sasaki and Burrows, 1998). Polyneural innervation could allow independent activation of motor neurons in order to use the same muscle fibers during different contexts, or it could make the force produced by one motor neuron dependent on coincident activity in another motor neuron (Sasaki and Burrows, 1998).

Summary

In this study, we describe an organizing architecture for motor control of tibia movement in Drosophila. These neurons constitute the output of motor circuits that flexibly control a wide range of fly behaviors, from walking to aggression to courtship. Studies of motor control in the fly VNC are now possible thanks to an increasing catalog of genetically-identified cell types (Lacin et al., 2019; Namiki et al., 2018; Shepherd et al., 2019), connectomic reconstruction with serial-section EM (Maniates-Selvin et al., 2020) and the ability to image from VNC neurons in walking flies (Chen et al., 2018). The leg motor neurons we describe also provide valuable targets for understanding the coordinated development of motor neuron and muscle properties (Enriquez et al., 2015; Venkatasubramanian et al., 2019). We anticipate that Drosophila will be a useful complement to other model organisms in understanding the neural basis of flexible motor control.

Materials and methods

Key resources table.

Reagent type
(species) or resource
Designation Source or reference Identifiers Additional
information
Genetic reagent (D. melanogaster) ‘w[*]; P{w[+mC]=Mhc-GAL4.K}2’ Bloomington Drosophila Stock Center RRID:BDSC_55133
Genetic reagent (D. melanogaster) ‘P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=20XUAS-IVS-mCD8::GFP}attP2’ Bloomington Drosophila Stock Center RRID:BDSC_32194
Genetic reagent (D. melanogaster) ‘P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=GMR22A08-GAL4}attP2’ Bloomington Drosophila Stock Center RRID:BDSC_47902
Genetic reagent (D. melanogaster) ‘P{JFRC7-20XUAS-IVS-mCD8::GFP} attp40’ Other FBrf0212432 Barret Pfeiffer, Janelia Farm, HHMI
Genetic reagent (D. melanogaster) ‘MHC-LexA’ Other Richard Mann, Columbia University
Genetic reagent (D. melanogaster) ‘13XLexAop2-IVS-GCaMP6f-p10}su(Hw)attP5’ Bloomington Drosophila Stock Center RRID:BDSC_44277
Genetic reagent (D. melanogaster) ‘Berlin-K’ Bloomington Drosophila Stock Center RRID:BDSC_8522
Genetic reagent (D. melanogaster) ‘P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=GMR81A07-GAL4}attP2’ Bloomington Drosophila Stock Center RRID:BDSC_40100
Genetic reagent (D. melanogaster) ‘P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=GMR35 C09-GAL4}attP2’ Bloomington Drosophila Stock Center RRID:BDSC_49901
Genetic reagent (D. melanogaster) ‘10XUAS-syn21-Chrimson88-tDT3.1 (attP18)’ DOI:10.1016/j.neuron.2017.03.010 Michael Reiser, Janelia Farm, HHMI
Genetic reagent (D. melanogaster) ‘P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=20 XUAS-IVS-CsChrimson.mVenus}attP2’ Bloomington Drosophila Stock Center RRID:BDSC_55136
Genetic reagent (D. melanogaster) ‘iav-LexA’ Bloomington Drosophila Stock Center RRID:BDSC_52246
Genetic reagent (D. melanogaster) ‘13XLexAop2-IVS-Syn-21-Chrimson::tdTomato (attP2)’ DOI: 10.1038/nmeth.2836 Barret Pfeiffer, Janelia Farm, HHMI
Genetic reagent (D. melanogaster) ‘P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=BDP-GAL4}attP2’ DOI:10.7554/eLife.08758.027 Andrew Seeds, Steffi Hampel, UNIVERSITY OF PUERTO RICO
Genetic reagent (D. melanogaster) ‘P{w[+mW.hs]=GawB}VGlut[OK371]’ Bloomington Drosophila Stock Center RRID:BDSC_26160
Antibody nc82 (mouse monoclonal) Developmental Studies Hybridoma Bank, RRID:AB_2314866 1:50
Chemical compound, drug MLA Tocris TOCRIS_1029 ‘1 μM methyllycaconitine citrate’
Software, algorithm MATLAB Mathworks RRID:SCR_001622
Software, algorithm DeepLabCut DOI:10.1038/s41593-018-0209-y Mathis Lab, Rowland Institute, Harvard University https://github.com/AlexEMG/DeepLabCut
Software, algorithm FIJI PMID:22743772 RRID:SCR_002285
Software, algorithm Fictrac DOI:10.1016/j.jneumeth.2014.01.010
Other Force probe fiber Proform CS2.5 AS This paper “PBT fiber from a synthetic paint brush, cut to have a spring constant = 0.22 N/m. Find at any Proform retailer, e.g. Amazon.com
Other Green CST DPSS laser, Besram Technology, Inc ‘532 nm’
Other Phalloidin ThermoFisherScientific FISHER: A22284 ‘633 nm, 1 unit per leg’
Other streptavidin-Alexa Fluor ThermoFisherScientific FISHER: S11226 ‘568 nm’

Fly husbandry

Drosophila melanogaster were raised on cornmeal agar food on a 14 hr dark/10 hr light cycle at 25°C and 70% relative humidity. We used female flies, 1–4 days post eclosion, for all experiments except tethered behavior experiments. Both male and female dark-reared flies, between 2–10 days post-eclosion, were used for tethered walking behavior experiments. For experiments involving optogenetic reagents (Chrimson variants and gtACR1), adult flies were placed on cornmeal agar with all-trans-retinal (100 µL of 35 mM ATR in 95% EtOH, Santa Cruz Biotechnology) for 24 hr prior to the experiment. Vials were wrapped in foil to reduce optogenetic activation during development.

Electrophysiology and calcium imaging preparation

Flies were positioned in a custom steel holder as described in Tuthill and Wilson, 2016b, with modifications to allow us to image movement of the fly leg. Each fly was anesthetized on ice for two minutes, so that she could be positioned ventral side up with her head and thorax fixed in place with UV-cured glue. The front legs were glued to the horizontal top of the holder, the coxa aligned with the thorax, and the femur positioned at a right angle to the coxa and body axis. In this configuration, the fly could freely wave her tibia in an arc at an angle of ~50–65° to the top surface of the holder. The holder was placed in the imaging plane of a Sutter SOM moveable objective microscope. All recordings were performed in extracellular fly saline (recipe below) at room temperature.

We used a water immersion 40X objective (Nikon) for patch-clamping and a 5X air objective (Nikon) to view the fly’s right front femur and tibia through the saline during spontaneous movements of the leg. Videos of the preparation were acquired at 170 Hz through the 5X objective with a Basler acA1300-200um machine vision camera. Custom acquisition code written in MATLAB (Azevedo, 2020a; copy archived at https://github.com/elifesciences-publications/FlySound) controlled generation and acquisition of digital and analog signals through a DAQ (National Instruments). Input signals were digitized at 50 kHz.

Force measurement and spring constant calibration

To measure forces produced by leg movements, we imaged the position of a flexible ‘force probe’ as the fly pulled against it. The force probe was a PBT filament fiber from a synthetic paint brush (Proform CS2.5 AS), threaded through the end of a glass micropipette (1.5 mm OD, 1.1 mm ID, WPI). To create a force probe, UV-cured glue (KOA 300, KEMXERT) was sucked up into the micropipette, the fiber was threaded into the glue, leaving 1–2 cm protruding out from the tip of the glass, and the glue was cured. The micropipette allowed us to mount the force probe in a custom holder and to couple it to a micromanipulator (Sutter MP-285). Videos of the force probe were acquired at 170 Hz through the 5X objective with a Basler acA1300-200um machine vision camera. One pixel equaled 1.03 μm2. We wrote custom machine vision code that detects the position of the force probe in each frame of the video by 1) allowing the user to draw a line along the probe in the video, 2) rotate the image of the probe perpendicular to the line, 3) average down the rows of the rotated image to get a single intensity profile, with a peak at the probe’s location, and then 4) find the center of mass of the intensity peak,±FWHM above baseline. A similar technique employing a probe to measure force has been used in Drosophila in previous studies (Elliott and Sparrow, 2012).

At steady state, the position of the force probe was related to the force through a spring constant, k, F = -kx (Figure 1—figure supplement 2). We measured the spring constant by positioning the force probe over an analytical balance. A glass coverslip was oriented vertically on edge in a piece of wax on the balance, and the tip of the force probe was positioned at the top edge of the coverslip. We then moved the micromanipulator to different positions. The ‘mass’ of the force probe was multiplied by gravity to give the force at that position. We then fit a linear relationship between force and position to measure the spring constant (Figure 1—figure supplement 2A). The force probe we used for experiments in this study had a spring constant of k = 0.2234 µN/µm and protruded approximately 1.5 cm past the end of the micropipette.

The force probe was not only a spring. It also had mass and was placed in saline, so inertia (m) and drag (c) affected its dynamics: F=md2x/dt2+cdx/dt+kx. To measure these properties, we ‘flicked’ the force probe by moving it to different positions with a glass micropipette, abruptly letting go, and allowing the probe to relax back to rest (Figure 1—figure supplement 2B). We imaged the position of the probe at 1.2 kHz with a restricted region of interest, and extracted dynamical parameters m = 0.1702 mg and c = 0.1377 kg/s. While not zero, the effective mass and drag were negligible (Figure 1—figure supplement 2C). The probe was slightly under-damped with a relaxation time constant of 2.5 ms and oscillation period of 5.8 ms, such that when imaging spontaneous movements at 170 Hz, the probe would effectively come to rest within one frame. The relaxation time constant was much faster than the fly’s spontaneous movements, even when the fly was attempting to let go of the force probe.

In Figure 4D–F, we calculate force by including drag and inertia, but in other figures we report leg displacement and the approximation of force, assuming that drag and inertia are negligible. We easily captured the lateral movement of the force probe across the frame but avoided estimating the vertical movement as the fly pulled the probe closer to its leg. As a result, the displacement (and thus the force) measured by the probe in Figures 15 may be slightly underestimated.

Mechanical stimulation of the leg

To move the leg and passively stimulate proprioceptive feedback, we mounted the force probe perpendicularly on a piezoelectric actuator with a 60 μm travel range (Physik Instrumente). The axial position of the probe was controlled by an amplifier (Physik), with voltage commands generated in MATLAB and delivered through the DAQ board (National Instruments). The output of the actuator’s strain gauge was used to control the position of the actuator through closed-loop feedback. The strain gauge sensor output was sampled at 50 kHz. The probe tip was positioned near the end of the tibia, giving a lever arm of 417 ± 7 (s.d.) μm across flies (n = 8). We then moved the tibia through its range of motion until it was approximately at 90° to the femur. To measure the effect of leg angle on the amplitude of sensory feedback, we then moved the probe to a range of axial positions (−150 μm = −21°, −75 μm = −10°, 75 μm = 10° and 150 μm = 21°, negative direction is extension) and repeated the stimuli (Figure 6—figure supplement 1).

We delivered ramp stimuli that moved the leg 60 μm (8°) with varying speeds, in both flexion (+) and extension (-) directions. We measured the actual speed of step stimuli by finding the maximum derivative of the strain gauge signal during the step onset. The range of angular velocities produced by the probe span the range shown to activate position- and velocity-sensitive femoral chordotonal neurons in the femur (Mamiya et al., 2018). Though the force probe was flexible, when we imaged the displacement of the force probe we saw that the probe tip matched the strain gauge feedback (errors < 5%), suggesting that passive or muscle forces did not impact these small stimuli.

To generate larger, faster movements than we could deliver with the probe, we whacked the leg by flicking the probe, similar to how we calibrated the probe dynamics (Figure 6—figure supplement 1).

Leg tracking

In trials where the fly was free to wave its leg rather than pull on the force probe, we tracked the leg using DeepLabCut (Mathis et al., 2018). For a training set, we labeled the tibia position for ~45 frames for three different videos from each fly using custom labeling code (Azevedo, 2020b; copy archived at https://github.com/elifesciences-publications/LabelSelectedFramesForDLC) . We labeled six points on the stationary femur, six points on the tibia (Figure 1—figure supplement 1B), as well as prominent bright objects that would otherwise often be falsely identified as part of the legs, such as the EMG electrode, the force probe, and several specular creases in the steel holder. The resnet50 network used in DeepLabCut served as the starting point for training, but as we added more flies to the training set, we initiated further training from the previously trained network. We found that the networks failed to generalize across flies but that ~150 labeled frames were sufficient to ensure >99% accuracy on other frames for that fly (Figure 1—figure supplement 1B).

In post-processing, we measured the distribution of pairwise nearest neighbor distances between the six detected tibia points and assumed that outliers indicated that a point was poorly identified. If only a single point was misidentified (~0.7%), we filled in the point with random draws from the nearest-neighbor pairwise distance distributions. The network misidentified more than one point 0.2% of the time, typically when the fly moved its leg particularly quickly, causing the image to blur. We excluded such frames. We median-filtered the x, y coordinates across video frames, and found the centroid of the six points, approximately the middle of the tibia. The centroid points traced out an ellipse that was the projection of the circular arc of the leg in the plane of the camera. Fitting an ellipse to the centroids allowed us to calculate the azimuthal angle of the leg arc (~50–65°) and the real angle between the stationary femur and the moving leg. We then used the real angle of the leg to detect when the leg was extended (>120°) or flexed (<30°) (Figure 1—figure supplement 1E).

Calcium imaging

We imaged the calcium influx into muscles with GCaMP6f (Figure 1, Figure 1—figure supplement 1A), driven by MHC-LexA expression in muscles. Epifluorescent 488 nm illumination excited GCaMP6f fluorescence. A long-pass dichroic (560 nm, Semrock) passed IR wavelengths to the leg imaging camera and reflected GCaMP6f emission to a second Basler camera imaging at 50–60 Hz. The imaging window of the GCaMP camera was restricted to the femur. Video frames were registered (Guizar-Sicairos et al., 2008) to remove vibrations due to movements of the saline meniscus (Video 1).

In the dark, the fly tended not to move its leg. However, the fly began to struggle and move its leg as soon as the blue epifluorescent light turned on to excite the calcium sensor. We imaged spontaneous movements under two conditions. 1) The fly could pull on the force probe with its tibia or 2) the fly waved its leg around spontaneously without the force probe. In the second case, a glass hook was placed near the femur as a barrier to prevent the fly from completely flexing the tibia and obscuring the calcium signals in the femur.

Clustering of calcium signals

We used the k-means algorithm in MATLAB to segment calcium signals into clusters. We used trials in which the fly waved its leg with no force probe. We drew an ROI around the femur, excluding only points near the femur tibia joint where intensity changes were dominated by the tibia obscuring the femur. We used the correlation of pixel intensity as the distance metric and varied the number of clusters, k = 3–8. Once pixels were assigned to a cluster, we applied a Gaussian kernel to the pixel cluster assignments and excluded pixels which fell below 0.75, indicating that less than ¾ of the surrounding pixels were of the same cluster. This resulted in clearly defined clusters with separation between them but no major gaps, indicating that similar clusters were also grouped anatomically (Figure 1—figure supplement 1C). To confirm that the clustering indicated changes in calcium influx rather than muscle movement, we also ran the same clustering routines on flies expressing GFP in the muscles (Figure 1—figure supplement 4). In this case, clusters were dispersed, fluctuated very little in intensity and bore no similarity to musculature.

We found that six clusters produced broadly similar clusters across flies. We numbered the six clusters as follows: Cluster one was large and distal/ventral; Cluster two was proximal and ventral; Cluster three ran down the center of the leg, neighboring Cluster 2; Clusters 4, 5, six were assigned from proximal to distal. With fewer than five clusters, the proximal cluster tended to be much larger, incorporating much of the region labeled as cluster 3 (Figure 1—figure supplement 1F). With more than six clusters, the smallest and least modulated clusters tended to divide, not giving us any further information. Six clusters potentially allowed for pixels that were most correlated with extension of the leg to cluster together, but we did not see large increases in fluorescence with extension.

We used the same clusters to measure fluorescence changes in trials where the fly pulled on the force probe. When the leg was flexed, the force probe could obscure the femur (e.g. Figure 1—figure supplement 4 and Figure 1—figure supplement 2), so we included only proximal portions of the clusters. If the force probe still happened to obscure >40% of the pixels in a cluster, that cluster intensity for that frame was excluded from the analysis.

The time constant of the calcium indicator was slower than the fly’s movements, such that fluorescence built up over subsequent contractions. Thus, pixel intensity (ΔF/F) was not directly related to contraction of the muscle. We took positive increases in cluster intensity to indicate muscle activation, i.e. neural activity. We applied a Sovitzky-Golay filter to interpolate cluster calcium signals (50 Hz) to the leg movements (170 Hz), which also computed the time-derivative of the local spline for each cluster (sgolay_t function in MATLAB, by Tiago Ramos, N = 7, F = 9). GCaMP6f decay was slow relative to leg movements (Figure 1E and Figure 1—figure supplement 1E), so negative derivatives reflected noise in the cluster fluorescence. We used this estimate of the noise (two standard deviations) to threshold the positive cluster derivatives, and thus find cluster ‘activations’.

Surprisingly, we did not identify any clusters that increased their calcium activity during tibia extension. Flies occasionally held their legs extended (Video 1), at which point we expected to see some clusters increase fluorescence (Figure 1—figure supplement 1D–E). On average, the leg musculature was dim during these periods Figure 1—figure supplement 1E), whereas the fluorescence of superficial flexors muscle fibers could increase more than six-fold during flexion events. Diffuse emission from the bright and slowly fading flexors may have obscured small increases in extensor fluorescence. We still found it curious that contractions of extensors did not produce brighter events. We speculate that calcium influx and contractile forces may be larger in flexor muscles than extensors because flies use flexion of the forelimb tibia to support their weight, to hold onto the substrate, and to pull their body during walking, whereas extensors generally lift up unloaded limbs when swinging them forward.

Whole-cell patch clamp electrophysiology

To perform whole-cell patch clamp recordings, we first covered the fly in a drop of extracellular saline and dissected a window in the ventral cuticle of the thorax to expose the VNC. The perineural sheath surrounding the VNC was ruptured manually with forceps, near the midline, anterior to the T1 neuromeres. We first used a large bore cleaning pipette (~7–10 μm opening) to remove debris and gently blow cell bodies apart, clearing a path from the ruptured hole in the sheath to the targeted motor neuron soma. The recording chamber was then transferred to the microscope and perfused with saline at a rate of 2–3 mL/min.

The extracellular saline solution was composed of (in mM) 103 NaCl, 3 KCl, 5 TES, eight trehalose, 10 glucose, 26 NaHCO3, 1 NaH2PO4, 4 MgCl2, 1.5 CaCl2. Saline pH was adjusted to 7.2 and osmolality was adjusted to 270–275 mOsm. Saline was bubbled with 95% O2/5% CO2.

Whole-cell patch pipettes were pulled with a P-97 linear puller (Sutter Instruments) from borosilicate glass (OD 1.5 mm, ID 0.86 mm) to have approximately 5 MOhm resistance. Pipettes were then pressure-polished (Goodman and Lockery, 2000) using a microforge equipped with a 100X inverted objective (ALA Scientific Instruments). Polished pipettes had resistances of approximately 12 MOhms. The polished surface allowed for high seal resistances (>50 GΩ) to limit the impact of seal conductance on Vrest (<1 mV). We used a Multiclamp 700A amplifier (Molecular Devices) for all recordings. The bridge resistance was balanced before sealing onto a soma. The pipette capacitance was compensated after the seal was made.

The internal solution for whole-cell recordings was composed of (in mM) 140 KOH, 140 aspartic acid, 10 HEPES, 2 mM EGTA, 1 KCl, 4 MgATP, 0.5 Na3GTP, 13 neurobiotin, with pH adjusted using KOH to 7.2 and osmolality adjusted to 268 mOsm. The neurobiotin was a generous gift from Rachel Wilson at Harvard Medical School.

The liquid junction potential for the whole cell recordings was −13 mV (Gouwens and Wilson, 2009). We corrected the membrane voltages reported in the paper by post hoc subtraction of the junction potential.

We applied 1 μM methyllycaconitine citrate (MLA, Tocris) to block cholinergic transmission in the VNC. To encourage spontaneous self-driven movements, we sometimes bath applied 0.5 mM caffeine (Sigma-Aldrich) during whole cell recordings, which prolonged periods of struggling in response to the epifluorescent light. Figure 5 includes trials recorded in caffeine: 45 of the 90 trials from one fast cell, and 50 of the 100 trials from a second fast cell. Caffeine’s effects have been reported to be due to dopamine receptor agonism (Nall et al., 2016). Caffeine application did not increase activity on the leg electrodes during periods of rest.

Identifying Gal4 lines that label leg motor neurons

We screened the Janelia FlyLight collection (Jenett et al., 2012) to find Gal4 lines that sparsely label leg motor neurons. We obtained flies from the Bloomington Drosophila Stock Center (BDSC), and imaged leg expression of GFP to characterize muscle innervation patterns.

Soler et al., 2004 described the leg musculature in Drosophila, which also serves as the basis for the leg motor neuron nomenclature (Baek and Mann, 2009; Brierley et al., 2012). For clarity, however, we refer to muscles of the femur as flexors or extensors, rather than as depressors or levators, which refer to the natural stance of the insect. Soler et al. in turn based their nomenclature on Miller, 1950. There appears to be a discrepancy between the two: the muscle named the tibia reductor muscle by Soler et al. is described as one of two depressor muscles by Miller, muscles 40 and 41. Miller applied the nomenclature of Snodgrass, 1935 to Drosophila leg muscles. Muscles that control the tibia are located in the femur. Snodgrass described a reductor of the femur that was actually located in the trochanter, perhaps leading to this misreading. The trochanter-femur joint in most insects has limited mobility. Presumably, Snodgrass named it a reductor muscle because depressor or levator would be inaccurate. We agree with the characterization of Miller and Soler that muscles 40 and 41 are two distinct muscles as they attach via distinct tendons, as seen in X-ray images of the leg musculature (Pacureanu et al., 2019). Because the function of muscle 41 is to flex the tibia, we refer to it here as a tibia flexor.

Extracellular recordings from leg motor neurons

We recorded electrical activity in the leg by inserting finely pulled glass electrodes (OD 1.5 mm, ID 0.86 mm) into the cuticle of the femur, taking care to avoid impaling the muscle. The electrodes were filled with extracellular saline. Extracellular currents were recorded in voltage clamp to improve the signal-to-noise. We confirmed that injecting currents of similar size did not move the leg nor produce additional electrical activity. The currents we recorded likely reflect spikes from the motor neuron axons, commensurate with the short latencies between somatic spikes and the events on the leg electrode (Figure 4). If we observed extracellular leg spikes that were time locked with whole-cell-recorded somatic spikes, we then never observed unmatched somatic spikes. If we were observing muscle action potentials or muscle EPSPs, we would have expected some failures. For simplicity we refer to activity recorded in the leg as the electromyogram (EMG), consistent with the use of the term in other organisms.

The content of EMG signals recorded in the leg depended on the placement of the electrode. To improve our chances of recording spikes from specific neurons, we used femur bristles (macrochaetes) as landmarks to place the electrode near the branched axon terminals. These include four large distal macrochaetes (‘bristles 0–3’) and one very proximal bristle which we call here the ‘terminal bristle’. Fast motor neuron spikes were most likely found by impaling the leg between bristle 2 and 3, while large intermediate spikes were often found near the terminal bristle. Even still, the polarity, amplitude, and shape of events from identified neurons could vary substantially. When the electrode was placed near the third macrochaete, we tended to record very large units of >200 pA from the fast motor neuron. Fast neuron units were by far the largest amplitude events in the femur. When the electrode was placed in the proximal part of the femur, near the terminal bristle, the spikes from the fast neuron tended to be smaller but still identifiable. We could not unambiguously detect EMG units associated with Flexor 3 in Figure 1. We ran our spike detection routines (see below) on EMG records only in cases where they could be clearly identified or when EMG spikes aligned with the somatic spikes.

Optogenetic activation of leg motor neurons during electrophysiology

To measure force production as a function of motor neuron activity, we drove the expression of CsChrimson (Klapoetke et al., 2014) with 81A07-Gal4, and the expression of Chrimson88 (Strother et al., 2017) with 22A08-Gal4. CsChrimson expression in 22A08-Gal4 prevented straightening of the wings and caused the front legs to be bent midway through each segment. We drove expression of Chrimson (Klapoetke et al., 2014) in sensory neurons with iav-LexA. We activated Chrimson by placing a fiber-coupled cannula (105 μm diameter, Thorlabs) next to the ventral window in the cuticle and illuminating the T1 neuropil with a 625 nm LED (Thorlabs). We used short flashes of 10 or 20 ms to activate neurons, increasing intensity by increasing the voltage supplied to the LED driver (Thorlabs). We measured the power output of the LED for each voltage we used.

Spike detection from whole-cell recordings and EMG recordings

To detect spikes in current clamp recordings of membrane potential, we applied the following analysis steps to our electrophysiology traces (digitized at 50 kHz): 1) filter, 2) identify events with large peaks above a threshold, 3) compare the shape of the filtered events to a template (distance metric), 4) threshold events based both on the shape and on the amplitude of the unfiltered spike. The parameter space for each of these steps was explored in an interactive spike detection interface (Azevedo, 2020c; copy archived at https://github.com/elifesciences-publications/spikeDetection) .

  1. We high-pass filtered the first derivative of each trace with a 3-pole Butterworth filter with a cutoff at 209 Hz, a low-pass filter with a 3-pole Butterworth filter with a 898 Hz cutoff. Empirically derived, this procedure resulted in a large positive peak associated with the rapid reversal of the membrane potential at the top of a spike.

  2. Events were identified by threshold crossing. Thresholds were in the range of 2–10 *10^−5 mV/s. The threshold was set as low as possible to reject baseline noise. Changes in membrane voltage associated with current injection and large secondary peaks associated with the filtered spike waveform oscillations often passed above threshold.

  3. Events that passed threshold were then compared with a template using a dynamic time warping procedure over a 251 sample window centered on the event. The template was calculated for each cell, and though the amplitude varied with spike amplitude, the template time course (shape) was generally similar across cells and genotypes.

  4. We then calculated the amplitude of the voltage fluctuation in the raw record associated with each event. Thus, each detected peak in the filtered data had a shape metric value and a spike amplitude. Events with both a similar shape (i.e. time course) as the template and a large amplitudes were identified as spikes.

We inspected records by eye to reject occasional false positives, such as changes in membrane voltage caused by current injection.

The algorithm was generally effective once the parameters were tuned for each cell. However, two cases typically caused spikes to become very difficult to unambiguously identify in slow motor neurons. First, during large current injections and the resulting depolarization, the spikes became very small and difficult to identify (Figure 4). High frequency spikes were clear in the raw voltage, and the leg moved, so we do not believe the neurons entered depolarization block. In such cases we hand tuned the parameters and inspected the identified spikes by eye to estimate the spike rate. Second, prolonged self-generated flexion could also depolarize slow motor neurons to the point that spike detection was difficult, we speculate because of large synaptic conductances that decreased input resistance. By contrast, spike detection worked perfectly during high spike rates evoked by sensory feedback (Figure 6).

Since we were interested in measuring spike latencies and conduction velocities, we calculated the second derivative of the raw voltage trace, smoothed over five samples, for each identified spike. We found the peak of the second derivative, using that point as the onset/acceleration of the spike.

We used the same algorithm to detect spikes from EMG current records, with different thresholding parameters in Step four for each recording due to the variability in EMG event size.

Immunohistochemistry

After a whole-cell motor neuron recording, we dissected the VNC but left the legs and head attached. We placed the tissue in 4% paraformaldehyde in phosphate-buffered saline (PBS) for 20 min, then separated and retained the VNC and the right leg with the filled motor neuron.

The VNC tissue was washed in PBST (PBS + Triton, 0.2% w/w), placed in blocking solution (PBST + 5% normal goat serum) for 20 min, and then placed for 24 hr in blocking solution containing a primary antibody for neuropil counterstain (1:50 mouse anti-Bruchpilot, Developmental Studies Hybridoma Bank, nc82 s) and a streptavidin-Alexa Fluor conjugate to label the neurobiotin-filled motor neuron (Invitrogen). We washed the tissue again in PBST, and then placed the VNC in blocking solution containing secondary antibodies for 24 hr (1:250 goat anti-mouse Alexa Fluor conjugate from Invitrogen; streptavidin-Alexa Fluor).

The leg was first incubated in blocking solution, like the VNC. Then it was placed in PBST containing 0.01% sodium azide (Thermo Fisher), 1 unit of phalloidin (Thermo Fisher) and the streptavidin-Alexa Fluor, and allowed to incubate for two weeks at 4°C, with occasional nutation.

Following staining, the tissue was mounted in Vectashield (Vector Labs) and imaged on a Zeiss 510 confocal microscope (Zeiss). Cells were traced in FIJI (Rueden et al., 2017; Schindelin et al., 2012), using the Simple Neurite Tracing plug-in (Longair et al., 2011). Images in Figure 2 show the results of the filling function in the FIJI plug-in. In some cases, the neuropil counterstain (anti-Bruchpilot) was omitted and the native autofluorescence of the tissue (along with nonspecific binding of streptavidin and GFP fluorescence) was used as reference.

To quantify morphology (Figure 3), we measured the soma diameter, the width of the neurite entering the neuropil, and the width of the axon, as close to the exit of the neuropil as possible. We made two measurements for each image and location and averaged the values.

Fly preparation for walking experiments

Fly wings were clipped under cold anesthesia (<4 mins) 24 hr before walking experiments. the fly’s dorsal thorax was attached to a tungsten wire (0.1 mm diameter) with UV-curing glue (KOA 300, KEMXERT). Tethered flies were given only water for 2–5 hr prior to being placed on the treadmill. In some experiments, the tethered flies were then decapitated under cold anesthesia and allowed to recover for 5–20 min prior to the experiment.

Intact tethered flies were positioned on a hand-milled foam treadmill ball (density: 7.3 mg/mm3, diameter: 9.46 mm) that was suspended on a stream of air (5 l/min) and that freely rotated under the fly’s movement (Figure 7D). The ball and fly were illuminated by three IR lights. In experiments on headless flies, we removed the spherical treadmill, leaving the flies suspended in air. For all trials, the temperature in the chamber was maintained between 26–28°C with a relative humidity of 58–65%.

Tethered behavior assay

We coaxed flies to walk on the ball by displaying visual stimuli on a semi-circular green LED display (Götz and Wenking, 1973; Reiser and Dickinson, 2008). To elicit forward walking, we displayed a single dark bar (width 30°) on a light background which oscillated across 48.75° about the center of the fly’s visual field, at 2.7 Hz. During periods between trials, the LED panels displayed a fixed dark stripe (30°) on a bright background in front of the tethered fly.

To characterize the role of the motor neurons in behaving tethered flies, we optogenetically activated or silenced genetically targeted motor neurons. A green laser (532 nm, CST DPSS laser, Besram Technology, Inc), pulsed at 1200 Hz with a 66% duty cycle, passed through a converging lens and a pinhole (50 µm diameter) with a resulting power of 87 mW/mm2 at the target. It was aimed at the fly’s left thoracic coxa-body wall joint, thus targeting the motor neuron axons and the T1 neuromere below the cuticle. Experiments using a driver line labeling all motor neurons (OK371-Gal4) indicated that optogenetic stimulation primarily effected neurons innervating the left prothoracic leg, though we cannot rule out effects on other VNC neurons (Video 8).

Video 8. Optogenetic stimulation of a control line (BDP-Gal4).

Download video file (7.8MB, mp4)

The video shows the behavior of different control flies on different trials while 1) UAS-CsChrimson flies were walking; 2) UAS-CsChrimson flies were stationary; 3) UAS- gtACR1 flies were walking; 4) UAS- gtACR1 flies were stationary. Videos of behavior are slowed 10X.

Each trial was four seconds long. We presented walking flies with the visual stimulus, the flies reached a steady running speed at ~1.5 s (Figure 7E), and the laser stimulus began at 2 s. We omitted the laser in some trials (0 ms), or the laser stimulus was either 90 ms or 720 ms in duration, interleaved in random order. Trials were separated by a 25 s period during which video data were written to disk and the LED panels displayed a fixed, stationary stripe.

Quantification of fly behavior

We used Fictrac (Moore et al., 2014) to calculate fly walking trajectories (position, speed, and rotational velocity) from live video of the spherical treadmill’s rotation (Point Grey Firefly camera, imaging at 30 Hz). Trajectories were then converted from pixels to mm using the spherical treadmill’s diameter of 9.46 mm. Leg movements were captured from six simultaneously triggered cameras (Basler acA800-510µm, imaging at 300 Hz) that were spatial distributed around the fly’s body. Digital and analog data signals were collected with a DAQ (PCIe-6321, National Instruments) sampling at 10 kHz and recorded with custom MATLAB scripts.

When headless flies were suspended above the ball, we manually tracked the position of the left femur and tibia during the 720 ms optogenetic stimulus period using manual annotation of video frames in FIJI (Rueden et al., 2017; Schindelin et al., 2012). We then calculated the femur-tibia joint angle from the position measurements in MATLAB.

When flies were in contact with the ball, we visually categorized the fly’s behavior in the 200 ms preceding the optogenetic stimulus as ‘Stationary’, ‘Walking/turning’, ‘Grooming’ or ‘Other’. Flies that took no steps for the duration of the categorization period were classified as Stationary. Flies that took at least four coordinated steps over the duration of the 200 ms period were classified as Walking/turning, irrespective of any distance traveled. Trials in which the fly switched behaviors, groomed or did not display clear markers for walking/turning during the categorization period were classified as Other/Grooming and excluded from analyses.

For Walking data, we calculated the average forward velocity over time, for each stimulus length, for each fly. We computed the percent change in walking speed for each fly by averaging walking speed during the stimulation (90 ms and 720 ms) and the subsequent 200 ms period, subtracting the average walking speed at stimulus onset, then dividing by the walking speed at stimulus onset. For Stationary trials, we calculated the percent of trials in which stationary flies reached steady state walking, i.e. sustained walking (>3 mm/s) in the half second following laser stimulation.

We noticed that the laser stimulus caused the empty Gal4 flies (BDP-Gal4) to decrease their walking speed slightly, likely because the flies could see the stimulus. This was particularly noticeable when we first used a fiber-coupled LED with a 105 µm diameter cannula. This prompted us to focus the laser on the fly’s left leg in order to further reduce the spot size and minimize the behavioral artifact. Even so, the optogenetic stimulus increased the probability that stationary flies would start walking (Figure 7H). To control for this effect, we compared the change in speed in motor neuron lines for each stimulus duration, to the change in speed in the control empty Gal4 flies (see statistical analysis below). In no-light trials, walking initiation (Figure 7H, black and gray bars) and changes in speed (Figure 7E, black traces) did not vary across different genotypes, although baseline walking speed varied slightly across the different lines.

Statistical analyses

For electrophysiology and calcium imaging results in Figures 16, no statistical tests were performed a priori to decide upon sample sizes, but sample sizes are consistent with conventions in the field. Unless otherwise noted, we used the non-parametric Mann-Whitney-Wilcoxon rank-sum test to compare two populations (e.g. Figure 4) and 2-way ANOVA with Tukey-Kramer corrections for multiple comparisons across three populations (e.g. Figure 3). To compare changes in fluorescence across multiple clusters and extension vs flexion (Figure 1), we used a 2-way ANOVA modeling an interaction between clusters and flexion vs. extension, with Tukey-Kramer corrections for multiple comparisons. To compare cluster ΔF/F of multiple clusters (Figure 1—figure supplement 1), we used a 2-way ANOVA with Tukey-Kramer corrections for multiple comparisons. All statistical tests were performed with custom code in MATLAB.

For fly walking behavior in Figure 7, we used bootstrap simulations with 10,000 random draws to compare both the likelihood of stationary flies to start walking, as well as changes in walking speed (Saravanan et al., 2019). Stationary trials were assigned a binary value to indicate that the fly began walking (1) or not (0). For a given stimulus duration and optogenetic condition (Chrimson or gtACR), the binary values for the empty Gal4 control flies and a motor neuron line were combined and then drawn randomly with replacement in proportion to the number of trials for each genotype. As a metric, we measured the difference in the fraction of flies that began walking. The p-value was the fraction of instances in which the randomly drawn distribution produced a value of our metric more extreme then we saw in the data (two-tailed) (Figure 7H). For trials in which the fly was already walking at the onset of the laser stimulus (Walking trials, Figure 7E), we compared the relative change in speed following the stimulus for a given motor neuron line to the empty Gal4 line. We randomly assigned trials to each genotype and calculated the average speed change as above. We used the Benjamini-Hochberg procedure to calculate the false-discovery-rate for either activation or silencing.

Table of genotypes

Figure 1A W[*]; P{w[+mC]=Mhc-GAL4.K}2, P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=20XUAS-IVS-mCD8::GFP}attP2
Figure 1B w[1118]; P{JFRC7-20XUAS-IVS-mCD8::GFP} attp40/+; P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=GMR22A08-GAL4}attP2/+
Figure 1C–I w[1118]; MHC-LexA,w[13XLexAop2-IVS-GCaMP6f-p10}su(Hw)attP5/Berlin WT; +/Berlin WT;
Figure 2 w[1118]; P{JFRC7-20XUAS-IVS-mCD8::GFP} attp40/+; P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=GMR81A07-GAL4}attP2/+ w[1118]; P{JFRC7-20XUAS-IVS-mCD8::GFP} attp40/+; P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=GMR22A08-GAL4}attP2/+ w[1118]; P{JFRC7-20XUAS-IVS-mCD8::GFP} attp40/+; P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=GMR35 C09-GAL4}attP2/+
Figure 3 w[1118]; P{JFRC7-20XUAS-IVS-mCD8::GFP} attp40/+; P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=GMR81A07-GAL4}attP2/+ w[1118]; P{JFRC7-20XUAS-IVS-mCD8::GFP} attp40/+; P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=GMR22A08-GAL4}attP2/+ w[1118]; P{JFRC7-20XUAS-IVS-mCD8::GFP} attp40/+; P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=GMR35 C09-GAL4}attP2/+
Figure 4 w[1118]; P{JFRC7-20XUAS-IVS-mCD8::GFP} attp40/+; P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=GMR81A07-GAL4}attP2/P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=20 XUAS-IVS-CsChrimson.mVenus}attP2
w[1118], 10XUAS-syn21-Chrimson88-tDT3.1 (attP18); P{JFRC7-20XUAS-IVS-mCD8::GFP} attp40/+; P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=GMR22A08-GAL4}attP2/w[1118]; P{JFRC7-20XUAS-IVS-mCD8::GFP} attp40/+; P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=GMR35 C09-GAL4}attP2/+
Figure 5 w[1118]; P{JFRC7-20XUAS-IVS-mCD8::GFP} attp40/+; P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=GMR81A07-GAL4}attP2/+ w[1118]; P{JFRC7-20XUAS-IVS-mCD8::GFP} attp40/+; P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=GMR22A08-GAL4}attP2/+ w[1118]; P{JFRC7-20XUAS-IVS-mCD8::GFP} attp40/+; P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=GMR35 C09-GAL4}attP2/+
Figure 6A–E w[1118]; P{JFRC7-20XUAS-IVS-mCD8::GFP} attp40/+; P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=GMR81A07-GAL4}attP2/+ w[1118]; P{JFRC7-20XUAS-IVS-mCD8::GFP} attp40/+; P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=GMR22A08-GAL4}attP2/+ w[1118]; P{JFRC7-20XUAS-IVS-mCD8::GFP} attp40/+; P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=GMR35 C09-GAL4}attP2/+
Figure 6G–H w[1118]; P{JFRC7-20XUAS-IVS-mCD8::GFP} attp40/iav-LexA; P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=GMR81A07-GAL4}attP2/13XLexAop2-IVS-Syn-21-Chrimson::tdTomato (attP2) w[1118]; P{JFRC7-20XUAS-IVS-mCD8::GFP} attp40/iav-LexA; P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=GMR22A08-GAL4}attP2/13XLexAop2-IVS-Syn-21-Chrimson::tdTomato (attP2) w[1118]; P{JFRC7-20XUAS-IVS-mCD8::GFP} attp40/iav-LexA; P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=GMR35 C09-GAL4}attP2/13XLexAop2-IVS-Syn-21-Chrimson::tdTomato (attP2)
Figure 7 w[1118]; +/+; P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=GMR81A07-GAL4}attP2/P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=20 XUAS-IVS-CsChrimson.mVenus}attP2
w[1118]; +/+; P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=GMR35 C09-GAL4}attP2/P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=20 XUAS-IVS-CsChrimson.mVenus}attP2
w[1118]; +/+; P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=GMR22A08-GAL4}attP2/P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=20 XUAS-IVS-CsChrimson.mVenus}attP2
w[1118]; +/+; P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=BDP-GAL4}attP2/P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=20 XUAS-IVS-CsChrimson.mVenus}attP2
w[1118]; +/+; P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=GMR81A07-GAL4}attP2/P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=20 XUAS-IVS- gtACR1}attP2
w[1118]; +/+; P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=GMR35 C09-GAL4}attP2/P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=20XUAS-IVS-gtACR1}attP2
w[1118]; +/+; P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=GMR22A08-GAL4}attP2/P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=20 XUAS-IVS- gtACR1}attP2
w[1118]; +/+; P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=BDP-GAL4}attP2/P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=20 XUAS-IVS- gtACR1}attP2
w[1118]; P{w[+mW.hs]=GawB}VGlut[OK371]/+; +/P{y[+t7.7] w[+mC]=20 XUAS-IVS- gtACR1}attP2

Data and software availability

Data will be made available from the authors website. Acquisition code is available at https://github.com/tony-azevedo/FlySound. Analysis code is available at https://github.com/tony-azevedo/FlyAnalysis.

Acknowledgements

We thank Jim Truman, Wei-Chung Allen Lee, Jasper Maniates-Selvin, Eiman Azim, Randy Powers, Marc Binder, and Brendan Lehnert, as well as members of the FlyLoops U19 team (Tom Clandinin, Michael Dickinson, Shaul Druckmann, Richard Murray, and Rachel Wilson) for helpful discussions, and members of the Tuthill laboratory for feedback on the manuscript. We thank Peter Detwiler, Fred Rieke, and Rachel Wong for generous sharing of equipment, Shellee Cunnington for preparation of solutions, Eric Martinson and Bryan Venema for technical assistance, and Michael Reiser and Michael Dickinson for sharing fly stocks. Stocks obtained from the Bloomington Drosophila Stock Center (NIH P40OD018537) were used in this study. We also acknowledge support from the NIH (S10 OD016240) to the Keck Imaging Center at UW, and the assistance of its manager, Nathaniel Peters. AWA was partially supported by NIH fellowship F32 DC013928. ED and PG were both supported by post-baccalaureate fellowships from the UW Institute for Neuroengineering (UWIN). LV was supported by NIH grant R01NS070644. This work was funded by a Searle Scholar Award, a Sloan Research Fellowship, a Pew Biomedical Scholar Award, and a McKnight Scholar Award to JCT, and NIH grant U19NS104655 to JCT and RS.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

John C Tuthill, Email: tuthill@uw.edu.

Ronald L Calabrese, Emory University, United States.

Ronald L Calabrese, Emory University, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • National Institutes of Health U19NS104655 to Anthony W Azevedo, Evyn S Dickinson, Pralaksha Gurung, Lalanti Venkatasubramanian, Richard S Mann, John C Tuthill.

  • Searle Foundation Scholar Award to Anthony W Azevedo, Evyn S Dickinson, Pralaksha Gurung, John C Tuthill.

  • McKnight Foundation Scholar Award to Anthony W Azevedo, Evyn S Dickinson, Pralaksha Gurung, John C Tuthill.

  • Pew Biomedical Trust Scholar Award to Anthony W Azevedo, Evyn S Dickinson, Pralaksha Gurung, John C Tuthill.

  • Alfred P. Sloan Foundation Research Fellowship to Anthony W Azevedo, Evyn S Dickinson, Pralaksha Gurung, John C Tuthill.

  • National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders F32 DC013928 to Anthony W Azevedo.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing - original draft, Writing - review and editing.

Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology.

Data curation, Investigation.

Resources.

Resources.

Conceptualization, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Writing - original draft, Project administration, Writing - review and editing.

Additional files

Transparent reporting form

Data availability

All data is publicly available on Dryad https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.76hdr7stb.

The following dataset was generated:

Azevedo AW, Dickinson ES, Gurung P, Venkatasubramanian L, Mann R, Tuthill JC. 2020. Data for: A size principle for recruitment of Drosophila leg motor neurons. Dryad Digital Repository.

References

  1. Ache JM, Namiki S, Lee A, Branson K, Card GM. State-dependent decoupling of sensory and motor circuits underlies behavioral flexibility in Drosophila. Nature Neuroscience. 2019;22:1132–1139. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0413-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Azevedo A. FlySound.  51a456b Github. 2020a https://github.com/tony-azevedo/FlySound
  3. Azevedo A. LabelSelectedFramesForDLC. 144fb7dGithub. 2020b https://github.com/tuthill-lab/LabelSelectedFramesForDLC
  4. Azevedo A. spikeDetection. e8c4f78Github. 2020c https://github.com/tony-azevedo/spikeDetection
  5. Baek M, Mann RS. Lineage and birth date specify motor neuron targeting and dendritic architecture in adult Drosophila. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:6904–6916. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1585-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bagnall MW, McLean DL. Modular organization of axial microcircuits in zebrafish. Science. 2014;343:197–200. doi: 10.1126/science.1245629. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bakels R, Kernell D. Threshold-spacing in motoneurone pools of rat and cat: possible relevance for manner of force gradation. Experimental Brain Research. 1994;102:69–74. doi: 10.1007/BF00232439. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bässler U. The femur-tibia control system of stick insects--a model system for the study of the neural basis of joint control. Brain Research Reviews. 1993;18:207–226. doi: 10.1016/0165-0173(93)90002-H. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bässler U, Büschges A. Pattern generation for stick insect walking movements--multisensory control of a locomotor program. Brain Research Reviews. 1998;27:65–88. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(98)00006-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bawa P, Binder MD, Ruenzel P, Henneman E. Recruitment order of motoneurons in stretch reflexes is highly correlated with their axonal conduction velocity. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1984;52:410–420. doi: 10.1152/jn.1984.52.3.410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Belanger JH. Contrasting tactics in motor control by vertebrates and arthropods. Integrative and Comparative Biology. 2005;45:672–678. doi: 10.1093/icb/45.4.672. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Berg RW, Alaburda A, Hounsgaard J. Balanced inhibition and excitation drive spike activity in spinal half-centers. Science. 2007;315:390–393. doi: 10.1126/science.1134960. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Binder MC, Bawa P, Ruenzel P, Henneman E. Does orderly recruitment of motoneurons depend on the existence of different types of motor units? Neuroscience Letters. 1983;36:55–58. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(83)90485-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Brierley DJ, Rathore K, VijayRaghavan K, Williams DW. Developmental origins and architecture of Drosophila leg motoneurons. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2012;520:1629–1649. doi: 10.1002/cne.23003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Burrows M. The Neurobiology of an Insect Brain. Oxford, New York: Oxford University Press; 1996. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  16. Burrows M, Hoyle G. Neural mechanism underlying behavior in the locust Schistocerca gregaria. 3. topography of limb motorneurons in the metathoracic ganglion. Journal of Neurobiology. 1973;4:167–186. doi: 10.1002/neu.480040207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Callahan RA, Roberts R, Sengupta M, Kimura Y, Higashijima SI, Bagnall MW. Spinal V2b neurons reveal a role for ipsilateral inhibition in speed control. eLife. 2019;8:e47837. doi: 10.7554/eLife.47837. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Card G, Dickinson M. Performance trade-offs in the flight initiation of Drosophila. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2008;211:341–353. doi: 10.1242/jeb.012682. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Chen TW, Wardill TJ, Sun Y, Pulver SR, Renninger SL, Baohan A, Schreiter ER, Kerr RA, Orger MB, Jayaraman V, Looger LL, Svoboda K, Kim DS. Ultrasensitive fluorescent proteins for imaging neuronal activity. Nature. 2013;499:295–300. doi: 10.1038/nature12354. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Chen CL, Hermans L, Viswanathan MC, Fortun D, Aymanns F, Unser M, Cammarato A, Dickinson MH, Ramdya P. Imaging neural activity in the ventral nerve cord of behaving adult Drosophila. Nature Communications. 2018;9:4390. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-06857-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Clarac F, Cattaert D, Le Ray D. Central control components of a 'simple' stretch reflex. Trends in Neurosciences. 2000;23:199–208. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(99)01535-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Clowney EJ, Iguchi S, Bussell JJ, Scheer E, Ruta V. Multimodal chemosensory circuits controlling male courtship in Drosophila. Neuron. 2015;87:1036–1049. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.07.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Dallmann CJ, Dürr V, Schmitz J. Motor control of an insect leg during level and incline walking. The Journal of Experimental Biology. 2019;222:jeb188748. doi: 10.1242/jeb.188748. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. DeAngelis BD, Zavatone-Veth JA, Clark DA. The manifold structure of limb coordination in walking Drosophila. eLife. 2019;8:e46409. doi: 10.7554/eLife.46409. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Denny-Brown D. On the nature of postural reflexes. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. 1929;104:252–301. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1929.0010. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  26. Desmedt JE, Godaux E. Ballistic contractions in man: characteristic recruitment pattern of single motor units of the tibialis anterior muscle. The Journal of Physiology. 1977;264:673–693. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1977.sp011689. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Desnedt HE, Gidaux E. Spinal motoneuron recruitment in man: rank deordering with direction but not with speed of voluntary movement. Science. 1981;214:933–936. doi: 10.1126/science.7302570. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Diao F, Ironfield H, Luan H, Diao F, Shropshire WC, Ewer J, Marr E, Potter CJ, Landgraf M, White BH. Plug-and-Play genetic access to Drosophila Cell types using exchangeable exon cassettes. Cell Reports. 2015;10:1410–1421. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2015.01.059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Duch C, Pflüger H. Motor patterns for horizontal and upside down walking and vertical climbing in the locust. The Journal of Experimental Biology. 1995;198:1963–1976. doi: 10.1242/jeb.198.9.1963. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Elliott CJ, Sparrow JC. In vivo measurement of muscle output in intact Drosophila. Methods. 2012;56:78–86. doi: 10.1016/j.ymeth.2011.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Enriquez J, Venkatasubramanian L, Baek M, Peterson M, Aghayeva U, Mann RS. Specification of individual adult motor neuron morphologies by combinatorial transcription factor codes. Neuron. 2015;86:955–970. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.04.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Fechner GT. Elemente Der Psychophysik. Breitkopf Und Härtel; 1860. [Google Scholar]
  33. Field LH, Burrows M. Reflex effects of the femoral chordotonal organ upon leg motor neurones of the locust. The Journal of Experimental Biology. 1982;101:265–285. [Google Scholar]
  34. Fleshman JW, Munson JB, Sypert GW. Homonymous projection of individual group Ia-fibers to physiologically characterized medial gastrocnemius motoneurons in the cat. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1981;46:1339–1348. doi: 10.1152/jn.1981.46.6.1339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Gabriel JP, Scharstein H, Schmidt J, Büschges A. Control of flexor motoneuron activity during single leg walking of the stick insect on an electronically controlled treadwheel. Journal of Neurobiology. 2003;56:237–251. doi: 10.1002/neu.10237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Gabriel JP, Büschges A. Control of stepping velocity in a single insect leg during walking. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society A: Mathematical, Physical and Engineering Sciences. 2007;365:251–271. doi: 10.1098/rsta.2006.1912. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Goodman MB, Lockery SR. Pressure polishing: a method for re-shaping patch pipettes during fire polishing. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 2000;100:13–15. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0270(00)00224-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Götz KG, Wenking H. Visual control of locomotion in the walking fruit fly Drosophila. Journal of Comparative Physiology. 1973;85:235–266. doi: 10.1007/BF00694232. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  39. Gouwens NW, Wilson RI. Signal propagation in Drosophila central neurons. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:6239–6249. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0764-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Gowda SBM, Paranjpe PD, Reddy OV, Thiagarajan D, Palliyil S, Reichert H, VijayRaghavan K. GABAergic inhibition of leg motoneurons is required for normal walking behavior in freely moving Drosophila. PNAS. 2018;115:E2115–E2124. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1713869115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Guizar-Sicairos M, Thurman ST, Fienup JR. Efficient subpixel image registration algorithms. Optics Letters. 2008;33:156–158. doi: 10.1364/OL.33.000156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Hampel S, Franconville R, Simpson JH, Seeds AM. A neural command circuit for grooming movement control. eLife. 2015;4:e08758. doi: 10.7554/eLife.08758. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Heckman CJ, Binder MD. Analysis of effective synaptic currents generated by homonymous Ia afferent fibers in motoneurons of the cat. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1988;60:1946–1966. doi: 10.1152/jn.1988.60.6.1946. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Heckman CJ, Binder MD. Analysis of Ia-inhibitory synaptic input to cat spinal motoneurons evoked by vibration of antagonist muscles. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1991;66:1888–1893. doi: 10.1152/jn.1991.66.6.1888. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Henneman E. Relation between size of neurons and their susceptibility to discharge. Science. 1957;126:1345–1347. doi: 10.1126/science.126.3287.1345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Henneman E, Somjen G, Carpenter DO. Excitability and inhibitability of motoneurons of different sizes. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1965a;28:599–620. doi: 10.1152/jn.1965.28.3.599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Henneman E, Somjen G, Carpenter DO. Functional significance of cell size in spinal motoneurons. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1965b;28:560–580. doi: 10.1152/jn.1965.28.3.560. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Henneman E, Olson CB. Relations between structure and function in the design of skeletal muscles. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1965;28:581–598. doi: 10.1152/jn.1965.28.3.581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Hill AA, Cattaert D. Recruitment in a heterogeneous population of motor neurons that innervates the depressor muscle of the crayfish walking leg muscle. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2008;211:613–629. doi: 10.1242/jeb.006270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Hoopfer ED, Jung Y, Inagaki HK, Rubin GM, Anderson DJ. P1 interneurons promote a persistent internal state that enhances inter-male aggression in Drosophila. eLife. 2015;4:e11346. doi: 10.7554/eLife.11346. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Hoyle G. Muscles and Their Neural Control. New York: Wiley; 1983. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  52. Jenett A, Rubin GM, Ngo TT, Shepherd D, Murphy C, Dionne H, Pfeiffer BD, Cavallaro A, Hall D, Jeter J, Iyer N, Fetter D, Hausenfluck JH, Peng H, Trautman ET, Svirskas RR, Myers EW, Iwinski ZR, Aso Y, DePasquale GM, Enos A, Hulamm P, Lam SC, Li HH, Laverty TR, Long F, Qu L, Murphy SD, Rokicki K, Safford T, Shaw K, Simpson JH, Sowell A, Tae S, Yu Y, Zugates CT. A GAL4-driver line resource for Drosophila neurobiology. Cell Reports. 2012;2:991–1001. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2012.09.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Kernell D. The Motoneurone and Its Muscle Fibres. Oxford University Press; 2006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  54. Kernell D, Sjöholm H. Recruitment and firing rate modulation of motor unit tension in a small muscle of the cat's foot. Brain Research. 1975;98:57–72. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(75)90509-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Kiehn O. Decoding the organization of spinal circuits that control locomotion. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2016;17:224–238. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Kishore S, Bagnall MW, McLean DL. Systematic shifts in the balance of excitation and inhibition coordinate the activity of axial motor pools at different speeds of locomotion. Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;34:14046–14054. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0514-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Klapoetke NC, Murata Y, Kim SS, Pulver SR, Birdsey-Benson A, Cho YK, Morimoto TK, Chuong AS, Carpenter EJ, Tian Z, Wang J, Xie Y, Yan Z, Zhang Y, Chow BY, Surek B, Melkonian M, Jayaraman V, Constantine-Paton M, Wong GK, Boyden ES. Independent optical excitation of distinct neural populations. Nature Methods. 2014;11:338–346. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2836. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Lacin H, Chen HM, Long X, Singer RH, Lee T, Truman JW. Neurotransmitter identity is acquired in a lineage-restricted manner in the Drosophila CNS. eLife. 2019;8:e43701. doi: 10.7554/eLife.43701. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Larsen GS, Frazier SF, Fish SE, Zill SN. Effects of load inversion in cockroach walking. Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 1995;176:229–238. doi: 10.1007/BF00239925. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Lindsay T, Sustar A, Dickinson M. The function and organization of the motor system controlling flight maneuvers in flies. Current Biology. 2017;27:345–358. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.12.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Longair MH, Baker DA, Armstrong JD. Simple neurite tracer: open source software for reconstruction, visualization and analysis of neuronal processes. Bioinformatics. 2011;27:2453–2454. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btr390. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Mamiya A, Gurung P, Tuthill JC. Neural coding of leg proprioception in Drosophila. Neuron. 2018;100:636–650. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.09.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Maniates-Selvin JT, Hildebrand DGC, Graham BJ, Kuan AT, Thomas LA, Nguyen T, Buhmann J, Azevedo AW, Shanny BL, Funke J, Tuthill JC, W-Ca L. Reconstruction of motor control circuits in adult Drosophila using automated transmission electron microscopy. bioRxiv. 2020 doi: 10.1101/2020.01.10.902478. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  64. Mathis A, Mamidanna P, Cury KM, Abe T, Murthy VN, Mathis MW, Bethge M. DeepLabCut: markerless pose estimation of user-defined body parts with deep learning. Nature Neuroscience. 2018;21:1281–1289. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0209-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. McLean DL, Dougherty KJ. Peeling back the layers of locomotor control in the spinal cord. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2015;33:63–70. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2015.03.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Mcphedran AM, Wuerker RB, Henneman E. Properties OF motor units in a homogeneous red muscle (SOLEUS) OF the cat. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1965;28:71–84. doi: 10.1152/jn.1965.28.1.71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. McPhedran AM, Wuerker RB, Henneman E. Properties OF motor units in a heterogeneous pale muscle (M. gastrocnemius) OF the cat. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1965;28:85–99. doi: 10.1152/jn.1965.28.1.85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Mendell LM. The size principle: a rule describing the recruitment of motoneurons. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2005;93:3024–3026. doi: 10.1152/classicessays.00025.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Mendes CS, Bartos I, Akay T, Márka S, Mann RS. Quantification of gait parameters in freely walking wild type and sensory deprived Drosophila melanogaster. eLife. 2013;2:e00231. doi: 10.7554/eLife.00231. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Menelaou E, Kishore S, McLean DL. Distinct spinal V2a and V0d microcircuits distribute locomotor control in larval zebrafish. bioRxiv. 2019 doi: 10.1101/559799. [DOI]
  71. Menelaou E, McLean DL. A gradient in endogenous rhythmicity and oscillatory drive matches recruitment order in an axial motor pool. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:10925–10939. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1809-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Miller A. The internal anatomy and histology of the imago of Drosophila melanogaster. In: Demerec M, editor. Biology of Drosophila. John Wiley and Sons; 1950. pp. 420–534. [Google Scholar]
  73. Milner-Brown HS, Stein RB, Yemm R. The orderly recruitment of human motor units during voluntary isometric contractions. The Journal of Physiology. 1973;230:359–370. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1973.sp010192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Mohammad F, Stewart JC, Ott S, Chlebikova K, Chua JY, Koh TW, Ho J, Claridge-Chang A. Optogenetic inhibition of behavior with anion channelrhodopsins. Nature Methods. 2017;14:271–274. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.4148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Monster AW, Chan H. Isometric force production by motor units of extensor digitorum communis muscle in man. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1977;40:1432–1443. doi: 10.1152/jn.1977.40.6.1432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Moore RJ, Taylor GJ, Paulk AC, Pearson T, van Swinderen B, Srinivasan MV. FicTrac: a visual method for tracking spherical motion and generating fictive animal paths. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 2014;225:106–119. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2014.01.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Nall AH, Shakhmantsir I, Cichewicz K, Birman S, Hirsh J, Sehgal A. Caffeine promotes wakefulness via dopamine signaling in Drosophila. Scientific Reports. 2016;6:20938. doi: 10.1038/srep20938. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Namiki S, Dickinson MH, Wong AM, Korff W, Card GM. The functional organization of descending sensory-motor pathways in Drosophila. eLife. 2018;7:e34272. doi: 10.7554/eLife.34272. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Newland PL, Kondoh Y. Dynamics of neurons controlling movements of a locust hind leg II. flexor tibiae motor neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1997;77:1731–1746. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.77.4.1731. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Pacureanu A, Maniates-Selvin J, Kuan AT, Thomas LA, Chen C-L, Cloetens P, Lee W-CA. Dense neuronal reconstruction through X-ray holographic nano-tomography. bioRxiv. 2019 doi: 10.1101/653188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  81. Page KL, Zakotnik J, Dürr V, Matheson T. Motor control of aimed limb movements in an insect. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2008;99:484–499. doi: 10.1152/jn.00922.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Phillips CE. Organization of motor neurons to a multiply innervated insect muscle. Journal of Neurobiology. 1981;12:269–280. doi: 10.1002/neu.480120307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Reiser MB, Dickinson MH. A modular display system for insect behavioral neuroscience. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 2008;167:127–139. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.07.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Rueden CT, Schindelin J, Hiner MC, DeZonia BE, Walter AE, Arena ET, Eliceiri KW. ImageJ2: imagej for the next generation of scientific image data. BMC Bioinformatics. 2017;18:529. doi: 10.1186/s12859-017-1934-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Saravanan V, Berman GJ, Sober SJ. Application of the hierarchical bootstrap to multi-level data in neuroscience. bioRxiv. 2019 doi: 10.1101/819334. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  86. Sasaki K, Burrows M. Innervation pattern of a pool of nine excitatory motor neurons in the flexor tibiae muscle of a locust hind leg. The Journal of Experimental Biology. 1998;201 (Pt 12:1885–1893. doi: 10.1242/jeb.201.12.1885. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Schindelin J, Arganda-Carreras I, Frise E, Kaynig V, Longair M, Pietzsch T, Preibisch S, Rueden C, Saalfeld S, Schmid B, Tinevez JY, White DJ, Hartenstein V, Eliceiri K, Tomancak P, Cardona A. Fiji: an open-source platform for biological-image analysis. Nature Methods. 2012;9:676–682. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Schmid A, Chiba A, Doe CQ. Clonal analysis of Drosophila embryonic neuroblasts: neural cell types, axon projections and muscle targets. Development. 1999;126:4653–4689. doi: 10.1242/dev.126.21.4653. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Schmidt J, Fischer H, Büschges A. Pattern generation for walking and searching movements of a stick insect leg. II. control of motoneuronal activity. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2001;85:354–361. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.85.1.354. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Seeds AM, Ravbar P, Chung P, Hampel S, Midgley FM, Mensh BD, Simpson JH. A suppression hierarchy among competing motor programs drives sequential grooming in Drosophila. eLife. 2014;3:e02951. doi: 10.7554/eLife.02951. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Shepherd D, Sahota V, Court R, Williams DW, Truman JW. Developmental organization of central neurons in the adult Drosophila ventral nervous system. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2019;527:2573–2598. doi: 10.1002/cne.24690. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Smith JL, Betts B, Edgerton VR, Zernicke RF. Rapid ankle extension during paw shakes: selective recruitment of fast ankle extensors. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1980;43:612–620. doi: 10.1152/jn.1980.43.3.612. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Snodgrass RE. Principles of Insect Morphology. McGraw-Hill Book Company; 1935. [Google Scholar]
  94. Soler C, Daczewska M, Da Ponte JP, Dastugue B, Jagla K. Coordinated development of muscles and tendons of the Drosophila leg. Development. 2004;131:6041–6051. doi: 10.1242/dev.01527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Strauss R, Heisenberg M. Coordination of legs during straight walking and turning in Drosophila melanogaster. Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 1990;167:403–412. doi: 10.1007/BF00192575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Strother JA, Wu ST, Wong AM, Nern A, Rogers EM, Le JQ, Rubin GM, Reiser MB. The emergence of directional selectivity in the visual motion pathway of Drosophila. Neuron. 2017;94:168–182. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.03.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Szczecinski NS, Bockemühl T, Chockley AS, Büschges A. Static stability predicts the continuum of interleg coordination patterns in Drosophila. The Journal of Experimental Biology. 2018;221:jeb189142. doi: 10.1242/jeb.189142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Tuthill JC, Azim E. Proprioception. Current Biology. 2018;28:R194–R203. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.01.064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Tuthill JC, Wilson RI. Mechanosensation and adaptive motor control in insects. Current Biology. 2016a;26:R1022–R1038. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.06.070. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Tuthill JC, Wilson RI. Parallel transformation of tactile signals in central circuits of Drosophila. Cell. 2016b;164:1046–1059. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2016.01.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Venkatasubramanian L, Guo Z, Xu S, Tan L, Xiao Q, Nagarkar-Jaiswal S, Mann RS. Stereotyped terminal axon branching of leg motor neurons mediated by IgSF proteins DIP-α and Dpr10. eLife. 2019;8:e42692. doi: 10.7554/eLife.42692. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Watson JT, Ritzmann RE. Leg kinematics and muscle activity during treadmill running in the cockroach, Blaberus discoidalis : I. Slow running. Journal of Comparative Physiology A: Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology. 1997;182:11–22. doi: 10.1007/s003590050153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Witten JL, Truman JW. Distribution of GABA-like immunoreactive neurons in insects suggests lineage homology. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1998;398:515–528. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19980907)398:4&#x0003c;515::AID-CNE4&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Wöhrl T, Reinhardt L, Blickhan R. Propulsion in hexapod locomotion: how do desert ants traverse slopes? The Journal of Experimental Biology. 2017;220:1618–1625. doi: 10.1242/jeb.137505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Wolf H. Inhibitory motoneurons in arthropod motor control: organisation, function, evolution. Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 2014;200:693–710. doi: 10.1007/s00359-014-0922-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Wosnitza A, Bockemühl T, Dübbert M, Scholz H, Büschges A. Inter-leg coordination in the control of walking speed in Drosophila. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2013;216:480–491. doi: 10.1242/jeb.078139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Zengel JE, Reid SA, Sypert GW, Munson JB. Membrane electrical properties and prediction of motor-unit type of medial gastrocnemius motoneurons in the cat. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1985;53:1323–1344. doi: 10.1152/jn.1985.53.5.1323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Zumstein N, Forman O, Nongthomba U, Sparrow JC, Elliott CJ. Distance and force production during jumping in wild-type and mutant Drosophila melanogaster. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2004;207:3515–3522. doi: 10.1242/jeb.01181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Ronald L Calabrese1
Reviewed by: Chris Q Doe2

In the interests of transparency, eLife publishes the most substantive revision requests and the accompanying author responses.

Acceptance summary:

Azevedo et al. investigate how different motor neurons are coordinated during Drosophila leg movements and find that leg motor neurons are typically recruited following a "size principle." This work beautifully combines different techniques: Ca-imaging in muscles, electrophysiology in motor neurons, and genetic manipulations with behavioral read-outs including force generation that highlight Drosophila as a particularly advantageous system for determining the neural architectures and the mechanistic principles for the control of limbs.

Decision letter after peer review:

Thank you for submitting your article "A size principle for leg motor control in Drosophila" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by Ronald Calabrese as the Senior Editor and Reviewing Editor. The following individual involved in review of your submission has agreed to reveal their identity: Chris Q Doe (Reviewer #1).

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

We would like to draw your attention to changes in our revision policy that we have made in response to COVID-19 (https://elifesciences.org/articles/57162). Specifically, we are asking editors to accept without delay manuscripts, like yours, that they judge can stand as eLife papers without additional data, even if they feel that they would make the manuscript stronger. Thus the revisions requested below only address clarity and presentation.

Summary:

In this paper, Azevedo et al. investigate how different motor neurons are coordinated during Drosophila leg movements. They found that leg motor neurons are typically recruited following a "size principle," where slower, smaller motor neurons are activated first, followed by larger more powerful motor neurons. As in vertebrates, the activation thresholds explain the order of recruitment. This work combines different techniques: Ca2+ imaging in muscles, electrophysiology in motor neurons, and genetic manipulations with behavioral read-outs including force generation. The limited number of motor neurons moving the fly leg make it an advantageous place to explore the control architecture and the principles are comparable to the more complicated vertebrate limbs.

Revisions:

All comments should be addressed; no further experiments are requested. We encourage the authors to revise expeditiously so we can publish quickly.

Reviewer #1:

1) I did not understand Figure 1C. What is the leg doing at the time of imaging? Is it flexing or extending during the 180 second imaging period?

2) Figure 2 does not show recruitment data for slow MNs, although it does for fast and intermediate MNs. I understand that Figure 5 covers this ground later, but it was distracting to have it missing in Figure 2. Perhaps adding foreshadowing text saying fast and intermediate are described here, but slow will be upcoming in Figure 5?

3) Please consider common labeling for Figure 1, 2, and 5 so that readers can track the same MNs/muscles through the paper. As it stands, the muscle groups are introduced in Figure 1 but they have different names in Figure 2. One possibility is to use the fast/intermediate/slow nomenclature even in Figure 1, but say "these muscle/MN groups are named fast/intermediate/slow for reasons that will be shown below."

4) The citation to the VNC TEM volume is distracting as no EM data are shown. If you have identified these MNs in the volume then it is probably worth showing them in a Supplemental Figure. Then readers can compare the neurobiotin morphology to the EM morphology. Without this, I don't see any value in citing that dataset.

5) The discussion is interesting but quite long, and a bit wandering in places. This is a very minor comment, but it might be helpful to copy edit the Discussion to reduce redundancy.

Reviewer #2:

The term "size principle" may not be immediately familiar to the broader scientific audience. I suggest expanding the title to "A size principle governs the recruitment order of Drosophila leg motor neurons."

The calcium imaging identifies different motor units, named Flexor 3, 2, and 1, where 1 is the last recruited, biggest, and most forceful. The numbering is a bit unfortunate unless they were aiming for a countdown. Do the activity clusters correspond exactly to specific muscles? The mapping between these regions, muscles, and motor neurons was tricky to follow. Also, Figure 1G,H lower panels are not explained in the caption.

There were some aspects of the Introduction and Discussion that I found challenging to follow. The authors could be encouraged to expand. Each muscle is actually innervated by several motor neurons. These data are from representative example motor neurons. The legs do need to activate these muscles in different order to perform different movements. There is some discussion that different pre-synaptic inputs and different proprioceptive feedback could explain the flexibility. The optogenetic activation and silencing experiments use light targeted to a single leg – what does this say about communication/coordination between legs? Flexion is a pulling movement – which is what the authors measure with their force probe – it would be nice to describe when in walking or other limb behaviors this pulling is used. To lift the leg for a step probably doesn't use all the force that can be generated. It would be nice to know what this looks like on the extension side, where the leg has to push off the weight of the body. A description of how to map between the experimental tests and the normal behavior would be helpful.

Figure 5E Explain the white dots more clearly in the caption: they indicate centroid of the 2D histograms of all frames both within and outside the 25 ms windows.

Figure 7 is confusing. In Figure 7A, the authors monitored femur-tibia joint angle during motor neurons activation in headless flies. Activation of slow neurons caused very little contractions followed by prolonged tibia extension. Authors mentioned that the extension is caused by resistance reflex for joint twist. But the same response was not observed in Figure 4C, where the current injection causes continuous tibia flexion. Is there an explanation for the difference? The activation and silencing of different motor neurons affect walking speed or probability of walking, with intermediate neurons contributing in a surprising way. Why does activation of intermediate neurons increase speed while slow and fast neurons slow it (7E)? Overall, Figure 7 could be improved to help connecting the walking behavior data to the organizational model suggested by the size principle.

Reviewer #3:

My comments are geared more on the conceptual and presentation side, outlined below in no particular order.

1) They divide the types of motor neurons into three classes. On one extreme is the large, fast, high force, low sensory feedback type; the other is small, slow low force, high proprioceptive feedback type; the third is the intermediate. How "true" is this subdivision? Is this a case like there are three distinct sizes of shirts---small, medium and large, or is it more a continuum and they just happen to have three motor neurons that represents three points along this continuum?

2) There is a strong emphasis on the size principle of muscle recruitment, with small neurons being recruited prior to large neurons, together with statements from their work that this principle can be violated (even the word dogma is invoked). I´m not an expert in this field, but I´m wondering whether this is the only conceptual framework that´s available for muscle recruitment. Are there competing theories? Maybe because I don´t know the history that I find it difficult to understand why the authors so heavily rely on this. Also, why the emphasis on this principle, when the assumption for the principle is likely not true in this case, i.e. the premotor inputs are likely different to the three motor neurons they are studying.

3) The description of the muscles and motor neurons are not so clear. Could they provide a figure where they outline all the muscles and all the motor neurons with their target muscle fibers, and then point out where the three motor neurons they are studying fits into this broad scheme (6 muscles, and at least 15 motor neurons; which of these are the three motor neurons discussed here) ? Also, they should try to be more consistent in how they refer to the muscles and movements, e.g. in Figure 1B, they say femur muscles, with flexors, etc. ; in 2A, they say tibia flexor/Flexor (I assume they mean femur muscles that move the tibia). This was a bit confusing at first. And when they say tibia flexor motor neuron, is this the motor neuron that innervates the femur muscle, whose action leads to tibia movement?

4) Figure 2 in general: there are separate letterings, but are these essentially one integrated figure? Meaning, e.g., does the left side of a,b,c,d refer to fast tibia flexor? If so, the presence of fly recording scheme in c is rather confusing; would be better put elsewhere so a consistent structure is maintained in the figure.

5) Figure 3. I realize they have the quantitation on the soma size, but it would be nice to actually have a picture for all three types as a visual comparison in addition to the numbers. Also, how do the size of the motor neurons correlate with size of the muscles/fibers? (see also point 3).

6) Figure 5F is a very helpful summary figure, but it could be made clearer. For example, the red marked Intermediate recruited lies just above the green Slow…; the legend can also be a bit more explanatory, e.g. start from low force and then to high, instead of just saying when fast spikes, slow and intermediate should already be firing.

7) Behavior description of walking phenotype: I think it would help if the description does not go from walking slowing down, and then saying it´s because the legs lock up. This seems trivial, but I was expecting that the walking rate would be slowing (of course if your leg is locked up, that´s going to affect your walking speed…). Is there a clear idea of the sequence of muscle activity that moves the thoracic leg during Drosophila adult walking or escaping? It might be helpful to provide a scheme of this if such knowledge exists.

8) They mention that there is a hierarchy of recruitment but now always. But do they see certain pairs that are always linked, and others that can be linked hierarchically but can be decoupled under different situations?

9) As an outsider and out of curiosity, how convincing is the argument that there are no GABA inhibitory neurons in Drosophila? There is a case of people saying the identity of the neurotransmitter used by the mushroom body kenyon cells is unknown, and after decades of saying it is a novel transmitter, one group found it to be cholinergic.

10) Finally, going back to point 2 above, the authors write in the discussion that a "key assumption of the size principle is that all motor neurons within a pool receive the same synaptic input…These data suggest that although the tibia flexor motor neuron pool may share some presynaptic inputs, they are not completely homogeneous." I found this rather striking because I never would even have thought to think it was homogeneous. With the known analysis of motor neuron inputs from connectomic work in other systems (e.g., Drosophila larval locomotion or feeding behaviors), it seems abundantly clear that different motor neurons innervating different muscles will not have homogeneous presynaptic inputs.

eLife. 2020 Jun 3;9:e56754. doi: 10.7554/eLife.56754.sa2

Author response


Revisions:

All comments should be addressed; no further experiments are requested. We encourage the authors to revise expeditiously so we can publish quickly.

A theme across several comments was a request for clarity about the anatomy of the leg motor neuron system and how that anatomy relates to functional measures, particularly the calcium imaging in Figure 1. We agree with the reviewers that it is important to be clear about what is and is not known. This is particularly challenging because there are few prior studies of the Drosophila leg motor system.

Comments on the theme of “the neuroanatomy of the femur and how does Figure 1 relate to that neuroanatomy” include: reviewer 1’s comments 3, 4; reviewer 2’s comment 3; and reviewer 3’s comment 3.

Two key points of confusion were:

The labels “Slow”, “Intermediate”, and “Fast” describe motor neuron force production and recruitment order, but they do not indicate the number of neurons in each of these categories, nor whether there are strong boundaries between them.

The activity clusters we find through calcium imaging bear similarities to the morphology and recruitment order of specific motor neurons, but we are currently unable to definitively map the correspondence between muscle and motor neuron activity.

The concrete changes we have made to the Results section to better describe the neuromuscular anatomy include:

As we introduce the activity clusters in Figure 1, we state in the text that the numbering is arbitrary and generally moves dorsal to ventral. This was stated only in the Materials and methods before.

In the section where we introduce the motor neurons, we now define the proximal tibia flexor muscle as those fibers that connect to the tibia flexor tendon, whereas the distal tibia flexor muscle consists of fibers that flex the tibia but connect to other structures on the cuticle. We use this terminology to describe motor neuron innervation patterns and to interpret calcium imaging.

As we introduce the neurons in Figure 2, we rely on studies that used MARCM clone anatomy to estimate there is only 1 fast neuron; 2-5 intermediate neurons that innervate the proximal fibers of the proximal tibia flexor muscle; and 8-9 slow neurons that innervate the distal flexor muscle.

We make clear that “slow” ,”intermediate” and “fast” are both functional and anatomical designations, and they encapsulate the rank ordering of neuronal properties and recruitment order.

We clarify that we think at least two different intermediate neurons underlie signals in activity cluster Flexor 2 in Figure 1, while the single fast neuron governs the high force contractions in Flexor 1.

We clearly restate this model following measurements of force in Figure 4 and measurements from other flexor neurons.

Finally, we restate this model in the Discussion.

Our understanding of the architecture of the leg motor system is pieced together from three sources: MARCM clones of leg motor neurons, an x-ray tomography volume of the leg and VNC (Pacureanu et al., 2019. bioRxiv 653188), and a serialsection EM volume of the VNC (Maniates-Selvin et al., 2020).

We refer here and in our paper to the distal flexor muscle and proximal flexor muscle fibers. These are clearly distinct muscles in the sense that they attach to different tendons. All the proximal fibers attach to a single tendon (tibia flexor tendon), whereas the distal fibers connect to cuticular structures very close to where the flexor tendon attaches. The distal fibers originate on both sides of the leg and stretch to this point, forming a chevron shape to make room for the extensor muscle. To distinguish these more clearly, we refer in this response to the distal flexor muscle as muscle 41, and the proximal flexor muscle as muscle 40, as described in Miller, 1950.

Muscle 40 (the proximal flexor muscle) can in turn be divided into more distal muscle fibers and more proximal muscle fibers. The distal muscle fibers are controlled by the fast tibia flexor motor neurons, which we think corresponds to muscle activity cluster 1. The more proximal part of the muscle is controlled by intermediate tibia flexor motor neurons and corresponds to activity cluster 2 in Figure 1.

We are confident that the motor neuron we call “fast” is the only neuron that innervates cluster 1. It innervates long muscle fibers in the middle part of the femur, the distal part of muscle 40, that attach to the tibia flexor tendon. Extracellular recordings near those fibers reveal large spikes that always match one-to-one with spikes recorded from the cell body of the fast neuron (81A07-Gal4). This gives us confidence that we are not missing an additional fast motor neuron.

We believe the neuron that we call “intermediate” (22A08-Gal4), is one of two neurons that innervates the proximal muscle fibers of muscle 40, which connect to the same tendon as the “fast” muscle fibers connect to. We are certain there are at least two, the one labelled by 22A08-Gal4 that we extensively study, and one labelled by 81A06-Gal4 that is reported in Figure 4—figure supplement 1. 81A06-Gal4 labels ~6-8 motor neurons, so we did not use it extensively for recording. Of these two neurons, the one in 81A06-Gal4 leaves the nerve more proximally than the one in 22A08-Gal4, and it innervates the most proximal muscle fibers. The 22A08-Gal4 neuron innervates more distal fibers, but overlaps minimally with the fast neuron. In terms of force production, spikes in the 81A06-Gal4 flexor neuron produced small but visible twitches, similar to the 22A08-Gal4 intermediate neuron, but we are not certain about the relative amounts of force production.

We think that there are only two intermediate neurons that innervate the proximal fibers of the flexor muscle. Between the fast neuron and the two intermediate neurons, their axonal arbors appear to target most of the proximal muscle fibers. (Each muscle fiber is innervated by just one motor neuron.) We see this in our light microscopic images reported here and in the x-ray data. However, based on MARCM clone expression in the leg, Baek and Mann, 2009 found that six neurons – including the “fast” neuron, their LinH neuron – innervate the tibia flexor muscle, whereas Brierley and Williams, 2012, found only two, so it is possible that more may exist. In particular, such neurons likely innervate and control fibers that give rise to the calcium activity cluster, Flexor 3.

Finally, the motor neuron we define as slow innervates the distal tibia flexor muscle, muscle 41 (formerly known as the tibia abelled muscle). From the MARCM studies, there are 8-9 neurons that innervate this distal flexor muscle. We recorded from three other such neurons, shown in Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Two of them resemble the slow motor neuron in 35C09-Gal4 in that they are spontaneously spiking while the fly’s leg rests on the probe. They are quite similar to each other, both functionally and morphologically, so they might be the same neuron abelled by different Gal4 lines. The third rests below spike threshold but can be recruited by sensory stimuli. This third neuron was found accidentally, following a failed attempt to record from the fluorescently-labeled slow neuron 35C09-Gal4, so we will have to wait for additional tools to record from it systematically.

To summarize: 8-9 slow motor neurons innervate the distal tibia flexor muscle (muscle 41). 35C09-Gal4 is likely one of the weakest. 2-5 intermediate neurons innervate the proximal portion of the tibia flexor muscle that attaches to the tibia flexor tendon (muscle 40); 1 fast neuron innervates the rest of the tibia flexor muscle fibers (muscle 40).

How does motor neuron innervation then relate to the activity clusters we find in Figure 1? The activity cluster 1 reflects the distal portion of muscle 40, controlled by the fast neuron, whose activity can be seen in the EMG. Activity cluster 2 then reflects the proximal portion of muscle 40. However, we know this portion to be under the control of at least two different intermediate neurons, which we were not able to differentiate between. Finally, we cannot see a single cluster that looks like the distal flexor muscle 41. As we mention, the distal femur region is generally dim, the leg and cuticle are changing shape near the distal flexor, and the extensor muscle is moving and contracting within that region. Another explanation might be that the physiology of muscle 41 (low-force) is distinct from that of muscle 40 (high-force), including larger calcium concentrations during contractions.

We want to emphasize that the three motor neurons we focus on in this study cause flexion of the tibia. This is key to using the “size principle” as a framework. The size principle is the dominant, largely successful concept that describes the relationships between motor neurons that control the same direction of movement. It was formulated from studies of the gastrocnemius and soleus muscles in the cat. Those muscles both extend the foot, but they have separate origins and insertion points. The size principle thus can apply to multiple muscles, like our framework here, but not to antagonistic muscles, muscles in different segments of the leg, or muscle groups that have different effects on movements. It thus is not relevant for muscles under control of a single motor neuron, such as those in the Drosophila flight system.

The other muscles in the femur are the tibia extensor and the long tendon muscle. Both likely contribute to the calcium signals we record in Figure 1, but they are dominated by flexor activity and probably unresolvable without optical sectioning. What we know about the extensor and long tendon muscles comes from work in larger insects. The large body of work on the slow extensor tibiae (SETi) and the fast extensor tibiae (FETi) motor neurons help to establish our naming conventions, but our work shows why the terms are harder to apply to the more diverse flexor neurons. In this paper, we ignore the long tendon muscle entirely because its action is surely complex. These muscles are described in more detail in the references we cite, but we’d also like to stress that the work in Drosophila has been purely anatomical thus far, so we do not have functional measurements for extension or the role of the long tendon muscle.

Reviewer #1:

1) I did not understand Figure 1C. What is the leg doing at the time of imaging? Is it flexing or extending during the 180 second imaging period?

To cluster pixels across coactivated regions, we imaged the fly leg during spontaneous, un-loaded movements; that is, this experiment did not involve a force probe. This meant that the force probe was not obscuring part of the leg, as depicted in Figure 1G, enabling us to visualize more of the leg.

We have changed the figure legend for Figure 1C to state: “K-means clustering of calcium signals (MHC-GAL4;UASGcamp6f) based on correlation of pixel intensities during 180 s of self-generated leg movements in an example fly (unloaded waving, see Materials and methods).” We also added a label to Figure 1C, and labelled Figure 1D to emphasize that the addition of the force probe means the leg movements are loaded.

2) Figure 2 does not show recruitment data for slow MNs, although it does for fast and intermediate MNs. I understand that Figure 5 covers this ground later, but it was distracting to have it missing in Figure 2. Perhaps adding foreshadowing text saying fast and intermediate are described here, but slow will be upcoming in Figure 5?

We believe the reviewer is referencing the fact that there is little EMG activity in the right-hand plot of Figure 2D, compared to EMG recordings during fast and intermediate neuron recordings. The intention behind showing the EMG in this case was to show that we could not record EMG signals associated with slow motor neuron spiking. This is important in order to make sense of the finding that the fly places some constant force on the force probe at rest, yet we see little activity in the EMG. This could have been more clear.

To clarify our point, we now specifically reference in the text and the figure legend the fact that EMG signals from the fast neuron are readily detectable. We had made this point only in the Materials and methods. We now also state in the Materials and methods and figure legend that we were unable to observe EMG events that correlated with the slow motor neuron, which was not made explicit before. We believe this is due to the axon diameter and the distal innervation of the axon terminals. The slow motor neuron connects to fibers near the femur-tibia joint, and we could not impale that region of the cuticle without interfering with movement of the leg.

3) Please consider common labeling for Figure 1, 2, and 5 so that readers can track the same MNs/muscles through the paper. As it stands, the muscle groups are introduced in Figure 1 but they have different names in Figure 2. One possibility is to use the fast/intermediate/slow nomenclature even in Figure 1, but say "these muscle/MN groups are named fast/intermediate/slow for reasons that will be shown below."

We are certain that the calcium activity cluster we call Flexor 1 represents the activity of the fast neuron. Flexor 2, on the other hand, likely represents the coordinated activity of at least two intermediate neurons. As we layout above, we have improved our explanations in the paper for how the neurons we describe relate to the activity clusters we find in Figure 1.

We feel the terminology introduced in Figure 1 is helpful for guiding the reader through that figure, so we try to do a better job indicating this is ultimately a loose relationship with the underlying neural activity. We appreciate the reviewer’s suggestion on how to clarify the text.

4) The citation to the VNC TEM volume is distracting as no EM data are shown. If you have identified these MNs in the volume then it is probably worth showing them in a Supplemental Figure. Then readers can compare the neurobiotin morphology to the EM morphology. Without this, I don't see any value in citing that dataset.

Per the reviewer’s advice, we have moved the citation of the EM work to the Discussion of the paper so as not to distract from the data we present here. The correspondence between EM connectomics and genetic strains is an ongoing effort, sure to be expanded upon in future projects.

5) The discussion is interesting but quite long, and a bit wandering in places. This is a very minor comment, but it might be helpful to copy edit the Discussion to reduce redundancy.

We thank Dr. Doe for his close reading, and we have attempted to streamline the Discussion. We eliminated discussion of spike timing, which will better accompany a more complete characterization of walking kinematics; we tightened up our discussion of violations of recruitment order and the implications for premotor circuitry; and we emphasized our understanding about how many fast, intermediate and slow neurons exist and their relationship to the musculature.

Reviewer #2:

The term "size principle" may not be immediately familiar to the broader scientific audience. I suggest expanding the title to "A size principle governs the recruitment order of Drosophila leg motor neurons."

We appreciate the reviewer’s perspective and we have changed the title to "A size principle for recruitment of Drosophila leg motor neurons". We are hesitant to suggest that any of the properties that are encompassed in the term “size principle” are causal mechanisms, without the details of the synaptic input. The idea that neuron size and excitability alone can account for the recruitment order has been precisely the attractive but unresolvable hypothesis that has made the size principle a useful but unsatisfying concept.

The calcium imaging identifies different motor units, named Flexor 3, 2, and 1, where 1 is the last recruited, biggest, and most forceful. The numbering is a bit unfortunate unless they were aiming for a countdown. Do the activity clusters correspond exactly to specific muscles? The mapping between these regions, muscles, and motor neurons was tricky to follow. Also, Figure 1G,H lower panels are not explained in the caption.

We have added text to the figure legend to better explain what the lower panels in Figure 1G and H show. As for the numbering scheme, we take the reviewer’s point. This numbering scheme was somewhat arbitrary (generally from ventral to dorsal), yet it suggests an underlying structure. To address this and similar comments, our revisions to the text clarify the connection between the calcium imaging and the details of the underlying neural control, e.g that Flexor 2 is likely controlled by two neurons. We hope this underscores the numbering scheme, rather than the recruitment order we use the calcium imaging to demonstrate.

There were some aspects of the Introduction and Discussion that I found challenging to follow. The authors could be encouraged to expand. Each muscle is actually innervated by several motor neurons. These data are from representative example motor neurons. The legs do need to activate these muscles in different order to perform different movements. There is some discussion that different pre-synaptic inputs and different proprioceptive feedback could explain the flexibility. The optogenetic activation and silencing experiments use light targeted to a single leg – what does this say about communication/coordination between legs? Flexion is a pulling movement – which is what the authors measure with their force probe – it would be nice to describe when in walking or other limb behaviors this pulling is used. To lift the leg for a step probably doesn't use all the force that can be generated. It would be nice to know what this looks like on the extension side, where the leg has to push off the weight of the body. A description of how to map between the experimental tests and the normal behavior would be helpful.

Our study avoids detailed descriptions of walking kinematics, partly because most of the experiments involved a reduced preparation, and partly because several ongoing studies will take advantage of new 3D markerless tracking technology developed in our lab (anipose.org) to set a new benchmark for kinematics during walking. However, the reviewer is right that simple functional details could better set up the role of tibia flexion during typical behaviors, like those in Figure 7. To this end, we have distilled the following information into new statements in the Introduction and the Discussion, and we refer the reader to our videos of walking behavior.

The fly’s front legs stick out in front of them when they walk, and a typical stride involves placing the front tarsus on the ground/ball by flexing the tibia. It then pulls through by flexing the tibia. Then it flexes the femur to lift the leg (controlled by coxal neurons), and extends the tibia to reach forward before placing it down again. The rear legs, by contrast, extend the tibia to push the fly forward. This can be seen in our videos. The short example clip at anipose.org of tracked legs during walking is illustrative, but Bender, Simpson, and Ritzmann, 2010, show this in detail for cockroach legs. In our Discussion we cite studies that measure how the distribution of pulling and pushing forces change as ants and stick insects walks up inclines, for example, and flies will also likely shift how they walk as conditions change. As the reviewer notes, extension of the tibia (lifting of the leg) is unlikely to require the same force or the same graded control as flexion. Researchers have speculated this accounts for the greater number of flexor neurons (Sasaki and Burrows, “Innervation Pattern of a Pool of Nine Excitatory Motor Neurons in the Flexor Tibiae Muscle of a Locust Hind Leg.”).

In Figure 7, optogenetic activation of intermediate neurons increases walking speed, and silencing of intermediate neurons decreases the likelihood of starting to walk. The reviewer is right that these results will make more intuitive sense if we clearly state that flies flex their front tibia during each stride.

Figure 5E Explain the white dots more clearly in the caption: they indicate centroid of the 2D histograms of all frames both within and outside the 25 ms windows.

We have added this explanation to the Figure 5E legend.

Figure 7 is confusing. In Figure 7A, the authors monitored femur-tibia joint angle during motor neurons activation in headless flies. Activation of slow neurons caused very little contractions followed by prolonged tibia extension. Authors mentioned that the extension is caused by resistance reflex for joint twist. But the same response was not observed in Figure 4C, where the current injection causes continuous tibia flexion. Is there an explanation for the difference? The activation and silencing of different motor neurons affect walking speed or probability of walking, with intermediate neurons contributing in a surprising way. Why does activation of intermediate neurons increase speed while slow and fast neurons slow it (7E)? Overall, Figure 7 could be improved to help connecting the walking behavior data to the organizational model suggested by the size principle.

Connecting the results of Figure 1-6 with those of Figure 7 illustrates a general challenge facing the motor control field: we have characterized elements of the system under controlled conditions, and then we want to know how they work when sensorimotor feedback loops are free to influence their activity. Optogenetic activation during walking was the obvious experiment. The reviewer points out two of the more confounding findings, 1) why does slow motor activation strongly disrupt walking, and 2) why does intermediate neuron activation cause walking? Because the experiment is somewhat orthogonal to the organizational model, it results in a messy narrative structure. To improve this, we have changed the text connecting Figures 1-6 to Figure 7 to say that feedback loops are now in play and that the experiment should really be seen as testing the effects of activating neurons outside of their normal recruitment patterns and outside of the fly’s control.

In that sense, we feel like our findings in Figures 1-6 better explain the effect of activating and silencing intermediate neurons. We know the intermediate neurons produce forces on the scale of ~1/10th of the fly’s weight and are quite fast. So, if the fly does not intend to recruit them, their activation creates a sudden but not disruptive perturbation on the ball, which is dealt with by walking forward. This is also consistent with tibia flexion moving the fly forward, so that general information would be helpful to have, see response above.

We were more puzzled that optogenetic activation of slow neurons causes tibia extension in the walking behavior experiments. We placed the same flies in the force probe setup, though we did not simultaneously patch the slow neurons. For the most part, we observed the fly flexing against the force probe, as the fly did following current injection at the cell body in Figure 4. With higher light intensities, this pulling could be more forceful than we observed in whole cell recordings, suggesting that chrimson activation could be more effective at changing the spike rate than current injection, as we observed for the fast and slow neuron. We observed extension in some instances when the leg was unloaded (no force probe). We did occasionally observe extension with the loaded leg. Since the slow neuron innervates fibers on one side of the leg, we speculate that in the unloaded leg, lateralized activation of muscle fibers causes a twisting motion that the fly reacts to.

We think a careful examination of the differences between current injection and Chrimson activation will involve more biophysical modelling of motor neurons and is beyond the scope of the paper. Generally, this approach might address why current injection at the soma is so ineffective, particularly in fast and intermediate neurons. More importantly, however, biophysical modeling would allow us to understand the intrinsic conductances in the motor neurons and integration of inputs, a critical part of going beyond the size principle framework for peripheral motor control.

Reviewer #3:

My comments are geared more on the conceptual and presentation side, outlined below in no particular order.

1) They divide the types of motor neurons into three classes. On one extreme is the large, fast, high force, low sensory feedback type; the other is small, slow low force, high proprioceptive feedback type; the third is the intermediate. How "true" is this subdivision? Is this a case like there are three distinct sizes of shirts---small, medium and large, or is it more a continuum and they just happen to have three motor neurons that represents three points along this continuum?

The reviewer has vividly posed an important question. Above, we have tried to clarify what we know about the divisions of motor neurons, but we can repurpose the reviewer’s metaphor to restate. We think the slow motor neurons are like a box of assorted sizes of small t-shirt, some close fitting v-necks down to skimpy tube tops (like our slow motor neuron), whereas the intermediate neurons are a separate box of 2-5 long sleeve shirts. The fast neuron would then be like a unique XXL sweatshirt.

2) There is a strong emphasis on the size principle of muscle recruitment, with small neurons being recruited prior to large neurons, together with statements from their work that this principle can be violated (even the word dogma is invoked). I´m not an expert in this field, but I´m wondering whether this is the only conceptual framework that´s available for muscle recruitment. Are there competing theories? Maybe because I don´t know the history that I find it difficult to understand why the authors so heavily rely on this. Also, why the emphasis on this principle, when the assumption for the principle is likely not true in this case, i.e. the premotor inputs are likely different to the three motor neurons they are studying.

In our understanding of the literature, the size principle is not only a conceptual framework, it is a well-documented empirical description that links the electrical properties of motor neurons with the gradients of force production across motor units. It was an early success in understanding the physiology of motor neurons, generally, and in describing structure in a population of neurons. The corresponding gradients of excitability and force production also suggested exquisite developmental processes to establish these linked properties, and spurred on the search for similarly compact descriptions elsewhere in the brain. The linked gradients at the core of the concept have been found in vertebrates (cats, fish, humans, etc) and in each systematic investigation of motor pools in invertebrates. As a shorthand for an intricate set of properties, the size principle is thus the dominant concept for motor neurons and motor control in the periphery in animals with limbs. As noted above, the size principle only applies to neurons controlling the same movement, such as when muscle fibers all connect to the same tendon, but still, this helps to collapse a lot of possible arrangements into an orderly low dimensional arrangement. In short, there is no competing conceptual framework for this situation, and since we find that it applies to Drosophila as well, we use it.

However, as a successful and dominant concept in the field, the size principle is also ripe for oversimplification, namely that a gradient of excitability alone governs the recruitment order. This is a possibility, but the relevant information required to augment the theory is an understanding of the different inputs to neurons within the same pool. We agree with the reviewer that it seems unlikely all the neurons in a pool get similar input, even when a motor pool controls one direction of movement in a rigid limb, but it remains to be rigorously tested. This should be possible in flies once we identify the leg motor neurons in the EM volume and map their inputs, a non-trivial but doable project.

Work in the Drosophila larva has paved the way for this approach (Zarin et al., “A Multilayer Circuit Architecture for the Generation of Distinct Locomotor Behaviors in Drosophila.”). There, each motor neuron receives distinct inputs, together creating patterns of contraction that distort the larva’s soft body. The same types of gradients in neuron properties have not been found in larvae (Zwart et al., “Selective Inhibition Mediates the Sequential Recruitment of Motor Pools.”)

Detailed mapping of motor neuron inputs has not yet been possible in vertebrates, though. The scale of the problem (e.g. 600 neurons controlling extension of a cat’s foot) is a large and the task daunting. As a result, attempts to modify the concept of a recruitment order – specifically one governed simply by motor neuron properties – have been limited to finding phenomena that cannot be explained by this simple mechanism. We find it notable that we have also seen similar phenomena in the fly, e.g. instances when the recruitment order was reversed, increasing the likelihood that what we find will apply to organisms with larger nervous systems. We look forward to the day when we can conceptualize motor neuron recruitment as the result of structured inputs to a structured neuromuscular periphery.

3) The description of the muscles and motor neurons are not so clear. Could they provide a figure where they outline all the muscles and all the motor neurons with their target muscle fibers, and then point out where the three motor neurons they are studying fits into this broad scheme (6 muscles, and at least 15 motor neurons; which of these are the three motor neurons discussed here) ? Also, they should try to be more consistent in how they refer to the muscles and movements, e.g. in Figure 1B, they say femur muscles, with flexors, etc. ; in 2A, they say tibia flexor/Flexor (I assume they mean femur muscles that move the tibia). This was a bit confusing at first. And when they say tibia flexor motor neuron, is this the motor neuron that innervates the femur muscle, whose action leads to tibia movement?

The introduction to our rebuttal attempts to address the main concerns raised in this comment (see above). We are still working towards a complete and accurate understanding of the detailed neuroanatomy and biomechanics of the leg. We have changed the text as indicated above to clarify what we know. Our best information on biomechanics comes from light microscopy studies and is limited, but newer datasets such as the XRay volume we reference will help. Our understanding of leg neurons comes from two studies of MARCM clones, a technique that can give repeated examples but not a complete overview of the system.

We have used established terminology where appropriate, but we have made a few changes. Specifically, in the literature “tibia flexor” neurons are called “tibia depressor” neurons, since they tend to lower the tibia when the insect is standing. “Depressor” is a confusing term, in part because the fly is inverted in our experiments. The term “tibia flexor neuron” is thus a compromise that better explains what the neuron is doing. As the reviewer notes, this can cause confusion, even to us and we found several instances where we referred incorrectly to “femur flexors”. We have edited the text to clearly state the name of each neuron, its function, and its innervation of the femur.

4) Figure 2 in general: there are separate letterings, but are these essentially one integrated figure? Meaning, e.g., does the left side of a,b,c,d refer to fast tibia flexor? If so, the presence of fly recording scheme in c is rather confusing; would be better put elsewhere so a consistent structure is maintained in the figure.

We experimented with moving the schematic to Figure 3, but the recording configuration was important to have for interpreting panels C (biocytin fills) and D (whole-cell recordings). We have added a shaded box to the schematic inset, to make it more clear that the traced neurons should be compared.

5) Figure 3. I realize they have the quantitation on the soma size, but it would be nice to actually have a picture for all three types as a visual comparison in addition to the numbers. Also, how do the size of the motor neurons correlate with size of the muscles/fibers? (see also point 3).

We have added a supporting figure showing the images from which the traces in Figure 2C were created. How the size and properties of the muscle fibers relates to the number of fibers in a motor unit and the force generated will be a fascinating future direction. Light microscopy cannot sufficiently resolve the edges of each muscle fiber to be able to segment them, currently. EM and XRay approaches will improve this going forward. Muscle ATPase, kinases and components of metabolism are likely to differ across muscle fibers, as do sarcomere lengths, which we observe.

6) Figure 5F is a very helpful summary figure, but it could be made clearer. For example, the red marked Intermediate recruited lies just above the green Slow…; the legend can also be a bit more explanatory, e.g. start from low force and then to high, instead of just saying when fast spikes, slow and intermediate should already be firing.

We have taken the reviewer’s suggestion and changed the legend to walk through this recruitment order schematic starting from low force to high force.

7) Behavior description of walking phenotype: I think it would help if the description does not go from walking slowing down, and then saying it´s because the legs lock up. This seems trivial, but I was expecting that the walking rate would be slowing (of course if your leg is locked up, that´s going to affect your walking speed…). Is there a clear idea of the sequence of muscle activity that moves the thoracic leg during Drosophila adult walking or escaping? It might be helpful to provide a scheme of this if such knowledge exists.

Reviewer 2 raised a similar concern above, helping us to realize that we did not clearly state that the front tibia flexes during normal walking to pull the fly forward, while the tibia of the rear legs extends to push the fly forward. We have added this important information to the Introduction and Discussion. We have also taken the reviewer’s suggestion and clarified our interpretation of the effect of stimulating the fast motor neuron.

8) They mention that there is a hierarchy of recruitment but now always. But do they see certain pairs that are always linked, and others that can be linked hierarchically but can be decoupled under different situations?

In short, all three neurons we studied, together with the examples from Figure 4—figure supplement 1, do appear hierarchically linked, but we also see instances that suggest that order can be softened. Just to restate the result from our paired recordings in Figure 5—figure supplement 1C-D, whenever the fast neuron was spiking, we observed spikes in the intermediate neuron, suggesting this hierarchy was strict. In the vast majority of cases, we found that slow neuron spikes preceded intermediate neuron spikes. However, in a few cases, intermediate neuron spikes were not directly preceded by slow spikes (110/3082).

To expand on this result, in those cases where we observed intermediate spikes before slow spikes, the slow neurons were spiking, but were just slower to spike and with lower instantaneous rates. These instances occurred when the fly was rapidly moving its leg. If, as we lay out above, the slow neuron is a member of 8-9 neurons controlling the distal flexor muscle, then perhaps we would see a similar reordering of spikes if we could record from the intermediate neuron along with some of those other slow neurons, like those in Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

We draw attention in the text to similar notable findings in zebrafish when the fish are swimming very quickly. In those cases the slow neurons drop out completely. Both cases suggest that when the organism is trying to make some rapid, alternating movement, slowly contracting muscles might get in the way and it would be better to de-recruit them or not drive them as hard. A simple mechanism could be selective inhibition to slow motor neurons that accompanies motor commands to move quickly.

As a final note, we also observed that suddenly turning on the epifluorescent lamp could elicit a spike in the fast neuron in some rare cases. We could not replicate this event with any regularity, we think it would happen when the epifluorescent lamp had not been turned on for some time and the fly was startled. Time was of the essence during whole-cell recordings, so we did not test this. It would make sense for there to be a pathway for rapid recruitment of the fast neuron: flexion of the tibia would be required to anchor the fly to the substrate, if, say, a strong breeze threatened to carry the fly away. We mention it because it is possible that this startle response also recruits the fast neuron out of order. One motif to look for in EM connectomics work would be descending neurons from the brain that might preferentially synapse onto the fast neuron but not the intermediate neuron.

9) As an outsider and out of curiosity, how convincing is the argument that there are no GABA inhibitory neurons in Drosophila? There is a case of people saying the identity of the neurotransmitter used by the mushroom body kenyon cells is unknown, and after decades of saying it is a novel transmitter, one group found it to be cholinergic.

We are relatively convinced that Drosophila have no inhibitory motor neurons. In insects with inhibitory neurons, the neurons are GABAergic. The most obvious way we thought of to see inhibitory motor neurons was to image expression of GABAergic markers in the leg. When we did this in preliminary studies with the Gad1-Gal4 line we saw no expression in the leg, and we didn’t even take images (regretfully). So, while the Gad1-Gal4 line labels many GABAergic neurons in the VNC, none of them appear to innervate muscles in the leg.

More convincingly, work on the development of neurons in the VNC has not detected inhibitory MNs, specifically from the neuroblast (NB5-5) that gives rise to them in other insects (Schmid, Chiba, and Doe, “Clonal Analysis of Drosophila Embryonic Neuroblasts.”).

10) Finally, going back to point 2 above, the authors write in the Discussion that a "key assumption of the size principle is that all motor neurons within a pool receive the same synaptic input…These data suggest that although the tibia flexor motor neuron pool may share some presynaptic inputs, they are not completely homogeneous." I found this rather striking because I never would even have thought to think it was homogeneous. With the known analysis of motor neuron inputs from connectomic work in other systems (e.g., Drosophila larval locomotion or feeding behaviors), it seems abundantly clear that different motor neurons innervating different muscles will not have homogeneous presynaptic inputs.

We completely agree with the reviewer that the assumption of homogeneous inputs is quite unlikely. In particular, as the reviewer notes, neurons innervating different muscles are sure to have different inputs, as is the case in the larva, or the opposing resistance reflexes in flexor vs extensor motor neurons (Burrows, The Neurobiology of an Insect Brain).

The question that applies here is, do neurons that control the same movement – either through innervation of different muscles (slow vs. intermediate and fast), or through innervation of distinct muscle fibers of the same muscle (fast vs. intermediate) – do they receive input from the same set of premotor neurons? Believe it or not, this has been a persistent question, mainly because the precise input to such a motor pool has not been mapped in any system as it has in the larva (and perhaps C. elegans).

All three of the neurons we study here are depolarized by extension of the leg, and some portion of that response is likely to come through a common pathway from proprioceptors to motor neurons. What portion, and what the rest of the input does to coordinate recruitment, is precisely the question our study will allow us to ask in the future, with the tools we report here. Fortunately, our study recapitulates several of the (surprisingly sparse) findings in vertebrates that indicate that some sources of input are specific to distinct members of a motor pool, such that recruitment order can be modified. This suggests that addressing this question in flies will have broad impact.

The unanswered question of whether the input to a motor pool is homogeneous, as it is referred to in the vertebrate motor neuron literature, is really about whether identical information about motor state and commands is conveyed to all members of the motor pool (same direction of movement). Given the measured gradient of excitability in a motor pool, an attractive model has been that more neurons could be recruited as that hypothetically identical input gets larger. Based in part on this fundamental finding, theoretical studies of neural networks often include a gradient of spike thresholds and time constants, in networks of linear integrate and fire neurons for example (Gerstner et al., Neuronal Dynamics), which simply indicates the usefulness of this concept. We agree with the reviewer that this ignores what is likely to be an important part of the puzzle. Frankly, in the past, authors have often used the possibility of identical inputs, as we have, as a simplifying assumption just to indicate what we do not yet understand.

To illustrate an analogous assumption at work in larval motor control, the field assumes the circuit motifs in a given segment of the larva will repeat in neighboring segments. This logical simplification rests on detailed development work and permits modelling efforts to understand coordinated contraction within and across segments. Interestingly, recent work on larval motor control relies on a reconstruction of A1, which receives different input than other abdominal segments, requiring additional assumptions in order to model a repeating structure (Zarin et al., “A Multilayer Circuit Architecture for the Generation of Distinct Locomotor Behaviors in Drosophila.”). Likewise, the distinct proprioceptive input to fast, intermediate and slow neurons, or the reversal of recruitment order in select cases, both indicate that the functions of the motor neurons within a recruitment hierarchy can diverge and spurs us on to find mechanisms underlying these differences.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Data Citations

    1. Azevedo AW, Dickinson ES, Gurung P, Venkatasubramanian L, Mann R, Tuthill JC. 2020. Data for: A size principle for recruitment of Drosophila leg motor neurons. Dryad Digital Repository. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]

    Supplementary Materials

    Transparent reporting form

    Data Availability Statement

    Data will be made available from the authors website. Acquisition code is available at https://github.com/tony-azevedo/FlySound. Analysis code is available at https://github.com/tony-azevedo/FlyAnalysis.

    All data is publicly available on Dryad https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.76hdr7stb.

    The following dataset was generated:

    Azevedo AW, Dickinson ES, Gurung P, Venkatasubramanian L, Mann R, Tuthill JC. 2020. Data for: A size principle for recruitment of Drosophila leg motor neurons. Dryad Digital Repository.


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES