Abstract
Background
Deficits of inhibitory control in early childhood are linked to externalizing behaviors and attention problems. While environmental factors and physiological processes are associated with its etiology, few studies have examined how they jointly predict inhibitory control. This study examined whether respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) functioned as a mediator or moderator of both cumulative sociodemographic risk and parenting behaviors on inhibitory control during early childhood.
Methods
The sample included 206 children and their biological mothers. At 24, 30, and 36 months of child age dyads participated in a series of laboratory visits in which sociodemographic, parenting, and baseline RSA (RSAB) data were collected. Inhibitory control was assessed at 36 months using a gift-wrap delay task.
Results
A series of structural equation models yielded no evidence that RSAB mediated the relations of risk or parenting on inhibitory control. RSAB moderated the effects of risk, such that high-risk children with low RSAB performed more poorly on tasks of inhibitory control, while high-risk children with high RSAB did not.
Conclusions
These results suggest that higher levels of RSAB may mitigate the influence of environmental risk in early childhood.
Keywords: risk factors, parenting, self-control, RSA, vagal tone
Introduction
Inhibitory control is the ability to inhibit a dominant behavioral response (Kochanska, Coy, & Murray, 2001). Ecological-systems models (e.g., Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 2006) emphasize the influence of both distal and proximal environmental factors on child development, while family stress models (Conger & Elder, 1994) suggest that the influence of distal environmental factors (e.g., cumulative sociodemographic risk) on child outcomes may be mediated by proximal factors (e.g., parenting behaviors). However, whether neurophysiological processes should be treated as links in a mediational chain connecting distal and proximal environmental factors to developmental outcomes or as moderators of environmental influence remains unclear. Understanding the etiology of inhibitory control has important implications for clinicians, given that deficits of inhibitory control have been linked to externalizing and attention problems (Schoemaker et al., 2012). In this paper we explore different conceptualizations of the role of one physiological process – the function of the parasympathetic nervous system – in relation to environmental influences on inhibitory control.
Inhibitory control: environmental influences in early childhood
Performance on behavioral measures of inhibitory control improves during early childhood (Carlson, 2005), with rapid gains from 24 to 36 months on tasks with minimal working memory demands, such as delay of gratification tasks (e.g., Kochanska, Murray, & Harlan, 2000; Gagne & Saudino, 2016). Deficits in inhibitory control during this period are strongly associated with symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD; Pauli-Pott & Becker, 2011), and these deficits may be traced, in part, to adverse environmental influence. For example, higher levels of poverty are linked to lower levels of inhibitory control (assessed via delay tasks) among preschoolers (e.g., Evans & Rosenbaum, 2008) and higher levels of cumulative sociodemographic risk are associated with poorer performance on delay tasks among school-aged children (e.g, Noble, Norman, & Farah, 2005).
Family stress models, however, suggest that distal environmental influences on inhibitory control are mediated by proximal processes, such as parenting (Conger & Elder, 1994). Maternal responsiveness (Kochanska et al., 2000) and the degree of dyadic connectedness (i.e., mutual engagement, shared pleasure, and reciprocity) between mothers and their children predicts better performance on a delay tasks (Li-Grining, 2007), whereas harsh parenting is associated with lower levels of compliance (Karreman, van Tuijl, van Aken, & Deković, 2006) and self-regulation (Colman, Hardy, Albert, Raffaelli, & Crockett, 2006) among preschoolers and school-age children. This, together with research indicating that high levels of cumulative sociodemographic risk undermine positive parenting behaviors while promoting negative ones (Kochanska, Aksan, Penney & Boldt, 2007; Popp, Spinrad, & Smith, 2008), outlines a model in which parenting behaviors mediate the effects of risk on inhibitory control. However, evidence supporting this model is sparse: to date only Lengua and colleagues (2007) have reported that the effects of risk on inhibitory control (at 40 months) were mediated by maternal limit-setting and scaffolding.
Inhibitory control and the activity of the parasympathetic nervous system
The parasympathetic nervous system (PNS) is one branch of the autonomic nervous system, and its activity is commonly indexed by respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA; Lewis, Furman, McCool, & Porges, 2012). According to polyvagal theory (e.g., Porges & Furman, 2011), high resting or “basal” levels of RSA (RSAB) reflect greater sensitivity to environmental influence and an enhanced capacity to regulate behavior in response to shifting environmental demands (Beauchaine, 2001). Bioecological models (e.g., Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 2006) stipulate that RSA functions as a mediator of environmental influence. There is evidence that both risk (for a review, see Propper, 2012) and parenting (Kennedy, Rubin, Hastings, & Maisel, 2004; Moore, 2010) influence RSAB, and that higher RSAB is associated with better performance on measures of sustained attention (Suess, Porges, & Plude, 1994), effortful control (Taylor, Eisenberg, & Spinrad, 2015), and executive function (Marcovitch et al., 2010). A recent meta-analysis found that higher RSAB is modestly associated with better performance on multiple measures of self-regulation (Holzman & Bridgett, 2017). Alternatively, the theory of biological sensitivity to context (BSC; Ellis & Boyce, 2008) suggests that RSA moderates the effects of environment on inhibitory control. Recent studies found that RSAB moderated the effects of socioeconomic status (SES) on delay-task performance (Sturge-Apple et al., 2016) and the influence of parenting on executive functions (which encompasses inhibitory control; Gueron-Sela et al., 2016), though the findings diverged as to whether low (Gueron-Sela et al., 2016) or high (Sturge-Apple et al., 2016) RSAB exacerbated the influence of low SES. These findings are not necessarily contrary to those reporting a direct link between RSAB and self-regulation: in studies conducted with sociodemographically diverse samples, no direct effect of RSAB on inhibitory control was observed (Sturge-Apple et al., 2016), while in studies conducted with low-income samples, high RSAB predicted poorer performance on tasks of behavioral self-regulation (e.g., Davies, Sturge-Apple, Cicchetti, Manning, & Zale, 2009).
Current study
This study addresses how sociodemographic risk, parenting, and RSAB may influence the development of inhibitory control by examining whether RSAB functions as a mediator or moderator of these environmental influences. Given the conflicting literature, we make no hypothesis as to which function for RSAB is more likely. If RSAB functions as a mediator, we anticipate that higher levels of risk and negative parenting behaviors will be associated with lower RSAB, which will in turn be associated with poorer inhibitory control, while higher levels of positive parenting behaviors will be associated with higher RSAB and better inhibitory control. If RSAB functions as a moderator, we expect that the effects of risk and negative parenting behaviors on inhibitory control will be exacerbated for children with higher RSAB and that the effects of positive parenting behaviors will be amplified.
Methods
Participants
Participants were healthy full-term infants recruited for a longitudinal study at 3 months of age. The sample included 206 children (48.5% female) and their biological mothers, over half of whom were African American (n = 121, 58.7%; European American, n = 85, 41.3%). At 3 months (the first age of data collection) 25.9% of families were below 100% of the federal poverty level (Mean income-to-needs ratio = 2.69, SD = 2.42). For additional details about sample demographics, see Holochwost et al., 2016. During the first three years 27 participants (13.1% of the initial sample) were lost to attrition. Likelihood of attrition was unrelated to child gender (p = .970), ethnicity (p = .676), or cumulative risk (p = .526).
Procedures
Children and their mothers participated in home and/or laboratory visits at 24, 30, and 36 months of child age. Visits included a four-minute period for the collection of resting cardiac data and a ten-minute period of free play in which the mother played with her child “like she would if she had free time during the day.” Free-play sessions were videotaped and subsequently coded (see below). At each visit mothers completed questionnaires.
During the 36-month visit children participated in the gift-wrap task (Kochanska et al., 2000). In this task an experimenter brings a gift and wrapping supplies into a room where the child is waiting. During the wrapping phase, the child is told to sit with his or her back to the experimenter for one minute without peeking while the gift is wrapped. The experimenter then leaves the room to retrieve a bow, thus initiating the bow phase, returning after two minutes. During this time the child is told not to open the gift. The child’s behavior was videotaped and subsequently coded (see below).
Ethical considerations
Mothers provided informed consent and permission for the children to participate, and all procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Board at the University of North Carolina Chapel Hill.
Measures
Cumulative risk (30 and 36 months)
Mothers provided data regarding their marital status, highest degree/certification, number of children in their household (age ≤ 18 years), annual pre-tax household income, and whether they had given birth while a teenager. Responses were dichotomized to indicate sociodemographic risk using thresholds based on previous research (e.g., Burchinal, Roberts, Hooper, & Zeisel, 2000) and cumulative risk scores at each age were calculated as the proportion of risk factors to the number of risk factors for which data were provided (Mistry, Benner, Biesanz, Clark, & Howes, 2010). Cumulative risk scores were highly correlated (r (176) = .84, p < .001) at 30 (M = .23, SD = .26) and 36 months (M = .24, SD = .26). Therefore, a composite risk score was calculated as the mean of these proportion scores for cases where scores were available for at least one age (α = .91; see Holochwost et al., 2016).
Parenting behaviors (24 and 36 months)
Maternal behaviors during free-play were rated by two observers on seven scales (NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, 1997). Average inter-rater reliability was .90 (24 months) and .92 (36 months). Sensitivity scores were calculated as the mean of the five scale scores that loaded on a sensitivity factor (α = .90 and .84 for the 24- and 36-month scores, respectively), while negative-intrusiveness scores were calculated as the mean of the two scores that loaded on a negative-intrusiveness factor (α = .82 at both ages). Scores at 24- and 36-months were positively correlated (sensitivity: r (152) = .51, p < .001, negative-intrusiveness: r (152) = .60, p < .001) and therefore cross-age composites were calculated (after Propper, Willoughby, Halpern, Carbone, & Cox, 2007). For further detail, see Mills-Koonce, Propper, and Barnett (2012).
Respiratory Sinus Arrhythmia (24, 30, and 36 months)
Cardiac data were collected during a 4-min period of rest using a pair of pediatric electrodes affixed to the child’s chest. Output was amplified and sent to a heart inter-beat interval (IBI) monitor (Mini Logger; Mini-Mitter/Respironics, Bend, OR) for R-wave detection. Files containing IBI data were edited for movement artifacts using MXEdit software (Delta Biometrics, Bethesda, MD), and files requiring editing of more than 10% of data were excluded from subsequent analyses. Measures of RSA were extracted using Porges’ (1985) moving 21-polynomial method (band-pass filter set to 0.24 – 1.04 Hz). Mean RSA was calculated for every 15-s epoch during the 2-min period of rest (after Huffman et al., 1998), and RSAB was calculated as the mean of these epochs. As values of RSAB were significantly inter-correlated (r (41, 82) = [.38, .49], p = [< .001, .014]), a cross-age composite measure of RSAB was calculated for all children who had RSAB values for at least two ages (N = 112, M = 5.16, SD = 1.26; results reported below were robust to the inclusion of children with RSAB values at only one age).
Inhibitory control (36 months)
The gift-wrap task developed by Kochanska and her colleagues (2000) is a simple delay task that is appropriate for children ages two to four (Spinrad et al., 2007). The task yields several latency scores for the two-minute bow phase: latency to touch (M = 54.07 s, SD = 52.86 s), lift (M = 79.52, SD = 51.90), and open the gift (M = 105.19, SD =36.32), all of which are calculated as time elapsed from the start of the phase. Following Kochanska et al. (2000), we calculated a composite score for the bow phase as the mean of the latency to touch, lift, and open the gift (α = .79; M = 79.59, SD = 40.06). The composite score for the bow phase was used as our measure of inhibitory control, given that performance on delay tasks in early childhood is most predictive of outcomes when it is assessed absent adult guidance (Mischel, Shoda, & Rodriguez, 1989).
Missing data
Data could be missing due to attrition or because a mother or child did not complete a measure during a lab visit. Cumulative risk scores were available for 183 children, parenting behaviors for 176 mother-child dyads (i.e., behaviors were available at either 24 months, 36 months, or both ages), RSAB for 112 children (with measures at two or three ages), and gift-wrap scores for 164 children. Missing data for these measures were regressed on all other measures and child gender and ethnicity. African American children were more likely to be missing RSAB data at a rate approaching significance (B = 0.72, Wald (1) = 3.67, p = .056). Missingness of RSAB was classified as missing at random (MAR) and ethnicity was included as a covariate in subsequent models (Alison, 2009).
Data analysis
Our hypotheses were evaluated via a series of structural equation models. The first set of models established relations among risk, parenting, and inhibitory control (see Figure 1a). The next set of models (Models 1 and 2) specified RSAB as a mediator of the effects of both risk and parenting on inhibitory control (see Figure 1b), while the third and final set of models (Models 3 and 4) specified RSAB as a moderator (see Figure 1c). Separate models were run for each dimension of parenting (sensitivity and negative-intrusiveness) to maximize the likelihood of obtaining reliable parameter estimates in a small sample (Bollen, 1989) and to prevent attenuating the estimates of parenting’s effects on inhibitory control (Preacher & Hayes, 2008). Risk, parenting, and RSAB were measured as cross-age composites. Therefore, we examined an alternate set of models in which parenting and RSAB at 36 months mediated the effects of risk at 30 months. While sacrificing the more stable measures offered by the cross-age composites, this alternate establishes the temporal precedence of risk.
All models were tested in MPlus 7.0 (Muthén & Muthén, 2015) using full-information maximum likelihood estimation. Each model was assessed using multiple fit indices, with the Akaike (AIC) and Bayesian Information Criteria (BIC) used to compare the relative fit of each pair of models specifying RSAB as either a mediator or moderator of risk and each dimension of parenting. Specific indirect effects and their standard errors were estimated using the products of coefficients approach and the delta method, respectively (Bollen, 1989). Moderation was assessed via interaction terms between mean-centered variables and significant interactions were probed using online utilities (Preacher, Curran, & Bauer, 2006).
Results
Preliminary Analyses
Table 1 presents descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations among study variables. Female (r (164) = −.22, p = .004) and European-American (r (164) = .30, p < .001) participants performed better on the gift-wrap task; therefore, these were covariates in all models. European-American ethnicity was associated with lower levels of cumulative risk (r (183) = −.39, p < .001), higher levels of sensitivity (r (176) = .38, p < .001), lower levels of negative-intrusiveness (r (176) = −.46, p < .001), and lower levels of RSAB (r (112) = .24, p = .012). Cumulative risk was associated with lower levels of sensitivity (r (174) = −.48, p < .001) and higher levels of negative-intrusiveness (r (174) = .54, p < .001), while higher levels of sensitivity (r (164) = .32, p < .001) and lower levels of negative-intrusiveness (r (164) = −.48, p < .001) were associated with better performance on the gift-wrap task. The orthogonal relation of RSAB and performance on the gift-wrap task raises questions about the veracity of models in which RSAB is specified as a mediator.
Table 1:
1. | 2. | 3. | 4. | 5. | 6. | 7. | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Gender (male) | --- | ||||||
2. Ethnicity (EA) | .06 | --- | |||||
3. Cumulative Risk (30 & 36 mos.) | −.04 | −.39** | --- | ||||
4. Sensitivity (24 & 36 mos.) | −.05 | .38** | −.48** | --- | |||
5. Negative-Intrusiveness (24 & 36 mos.) | .13 | −.46** | .54** | −.63** | --- | ||
6. RSAB (24 – 36 mos.) | −.03 | −.33** | .24* | −.24* | .13 | --- | |
7. Inhibitory control (36 mos.) | .22** | .30** | −.36** | .32** | −.48** | 0 | --- |
N | 206 | 206 | 183 | 176 | 176 | 112 | 164 |
M | .51 | .41 | .24 | 4.45 | 2.78 | 5.16 | 79.59 |
SD | .50 | .49 | .25 | 1.07 | 1.26 | 1.26 | 40.06 |
Note: For pairwise correlations N = [109, 206]. EA = European American.
p < .05
p < .01
Model Specification and Testing
Preliminary models
Results of our preliminary models are presented in the Supplementary Materials. In brief, higher levels of cumulative risk predicted lower levels of maternal sensitivity (B = −.39, p < .001) and inhibitory control (B = −.24, p = .003). Maternal sensitivity was not related to inhibitory control (p = .131) and did not mediate the effects of risk on inhibitory control (specific indirect effect: p = .143). In contrast, higher levels of risk predicted higher negative intrusiveness (B = .44, p < .001), which predicted lower levels of inhibitory control (B = −.33, p < .001). Negative-intrusiveness mediated the effects of risk on inhibitory control (specific indirect effect: B = .14, p < .001).
RSAB as a mediator: Models 1 and 2
Based on these results, models 1 and 2 were specified differently: in model 1, RSAB and sensitivity were allowed to mediate the effects of risk on inhibitory control in parallel, whereas in model 2 RSAB was allowed to mediate the effects of negative-intrusiveness on inhibitory control, which in turn mediated the effects of risk. Model 1 was a good fit to the data (see Table 2). While there was a significant direct effect of risk on both sensitivity (B = −.39, p < .001) and inhibitory control (B = −.25, p = .001), neither the effect of sensitivity (B = .13, p = .102) nor RSAB on inhibitory control was significant (B = .15, p = .091; see Figure 2a). The test of the specific indirect effect of risk on inhibitory control through RSAB was also not significant (p = .317).
Table 2:
Model 1 (Figure 3a) | Model 2 (Figure 3b) | Model 3 (Figure 4a) | Model 4 (Figure 4b) | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Parenting behavior | Sensitivity | Negative-Intrusiveness | Sensitivity | Negative-Intrusiveness |
Putative role of RSA | Mediator | Mediator | Moderator | Moderator |
X2 (df), p | 3.12 (3), .373 | 3.24 (2), .198 | 5.13 (8), .744 | 10.39 (7), .168 |
RMSEA, 90% CI | .015, [0, .130] | .060, [0, .173] | 0, [0, .080] | .070 [.014, .169] |
CFI | .999 | .992 | 1.0 | .988 |
TLI | .995 | .950 | 1.0 | .941 |
SRMR | .023 | .019 | .030 | .040 |
AIC | 2475.1 | 2492.1 | 1767.1 | 1766.8 |
BIC | 2522.5 | 2542.6 | 1810.6 | 1813.0 |
Note: RMSEA = Root mean square error of approximation; CI = confidence interval; CFI = Confirmatory fit index; TLI = Tucker-Lewis index; SRMR = Standardized root mean square residual; AIC = Akaike Information Criterion; BIC
Model 2 was an acceptable fit to the data (see Table 2). Higher levels of cumulative risk were associated with higher levels of negative-intrusiveness (B = .44, p < .001) and lower levels of inhibitory control (B = −.18, p = .007); higher levels of negative-intrusiveness were associated with lower levels of inhibitory control (B = −.36, p < .001; see Figure 2b). However, negative-intrusiveness was not significantly related to RSAB (B = −.15, p = .203), nor was RSAB associated with inhibitory control (B = .05, p = .498). The test of the specific indirect effect from risk to inhibitory control through negative-intrusiveness and then RSAB was not significant (p = .552). The alternative models used to establish temporal precedence yielded similar results (see Supplementary Material).
RSAB as a moderator: Models 3 and 4
In model 3 RSAB was allowed to moderate the effects of risk and sensitivity on inhibitory control. This model was a good fit to the data (see Table 2). The interaction between RSAB and risk was significant (B = .22, p = .017), while the interaction between RSAB and sensitivity was not (B = .07, p = .455; see Figure 3a). The smaller values of both the AIC and BIC for this model indicated that it was a superior fit to the data (i.e., the divergence from the true model is smaller) than model 1, in which RSAB mediated the effects of risk. Moreover, while adding RSAB as a mediator uniquely accounted only for 2% of variance in inhibitory control (ΔR2 = .02), including RSAB as a moderator uniquely accounted for 7% of the variance, or nearly a third of the proportion collectively accounted for by risk, ethnicity, gender, and sensitivity.
In model 4 RSAB was allowed to moderate the effects of risk and negative-intrusiveness on inhibitory control. The model was an acceptable fit to the data (see Table 2). The AIC and BIC indicated that this model was a better fit to the data than model 2. The results were similar to those produced by the moderation model including sensitivity: RSAB moderated the effects of risk (B = .24, p = .011), but not negative-intrusiveness (B = −.17, p = .089; see Figure 3b).
Post-hoc analyses
Given the similar results across models 3 and 4, the model including sensitivity was selected for post-hoc probing. Simple slopes for the relation between risk and inhibitory control were calculated at low (−1 SD) and high (+1 SD) levels of RSAB. Among children with low RSAB, higher levels of risk were associated with significantly lower levels of inhibitory control (B = −.81.7 (SE = 22.3), t = −3.66, p < .001). Among children with high RSAB, no significant decrement in inhibitory control was observed at high levels of risk (p = .571).
Discussion
This study examined whether RSAB functioned as a mediator or moderator of environmental influence on inhibitory control. While our preliminary models indicated that negative-intrusiveness fully mediated the effects of risk on inhibitory control, there was no evidence that RSAB mediated the effects of risk or either dimension of parenting on inhibitory control. This finding is consistent with results reported by Sturge-Apple et al. (2016, study 2), and underscore the possibility that in socioeconomically-diverse samples a direct relation between RSAB and inhibitory control may not be observed. In contrast, our models indicated that RSAB moderated the effects of risk on inhibitory control: for children with low RSAB, there was a significant, negative relation between higher risk and poorer performance on the gift-wrap task; for children with high RSAB, this relation was not observed. Although this finding is consistent with previous studies that found high RSAB to be a protective factor (e.g., Gueron-Sela et al., 2016), it contradicts our hypothesis that high RSAB would exacerbate the effects of risk. This hypothesis was based, in part, on studies reporting high RSAB to be a susceptibility factor (Conradt et al., 2013) and Sturge-Apple et al. (2016)’s finding that children from low-income backgrounds with high RSAB exhibited poorer performance on a snack delay task than low-income children with low RSAB.
Some of these differences may be explained by the fact that the role of RSAB was examined with respect to different outcomes (as in the case of Conradt et al., 2013; Gueron-Sela et al., 2016), as well as differences in study design, including the precise constructs measured (risk versus poverty), the ages at which measurement occurred, and the measures used. For example, Sturge-Apple et al. (2016) used a snack delay task in which children were given the choice to eat a small amount of candy now or a larger amount later. The authors argued that for children reared in poverty, consuming a small amount of candy now would be adaptive. We would add that consuming the candy immediately does not entail a violation of a rule or instruction. This is in contrast to the gift-wrap task, in which touching, lifting, or unwrapping the present during the bow phase violates a rule. For children from high-risk environments, rule violation is maladaptive, given the severe consequences relative to those experienced by children from more affluent backgrounds (e.g., Hannon, DeFina, & Bruch, 2013). Thus, in the context of the gift-wrap task, high levels of RSAB support the adaptive behavior of delaying or abstaining from violating the rule among children from high-risk environments.
Conclusion
Clinical Implications and Future Directions
Our results have implications for clinicians who work with young children and high-risk families. Consistent with prior research (e.g., Noble et al., 2005), we found that higher levels of sociodemographic risk predict lower levels of inhibitory control, as do negative-intrusive parenting behaviors. However, our results also suggest that certain patterns of neurophysiological activity can buffer the effects of risk. These findings, together with the fact that deficits of inhibitory control are implicated in the etiology of externalizing disorders (Pauli-Pott & Becker, 2011; Schoemaker et al., 2012), suggest two courses for intervention designed to reduce the prevalence and severity of these disorders, one reducing negative parenting behaviors by bolstering parents’ ability to facilitate their child’s exploration while fostering their autonomy, and a second identifying children who are most likely to develop deficits of inhibitory control based on their neurophysiological activity.
While this study focused on RSAB, future research should examine the mediating or moderating role of RSA withdrawal, given that the degree to which children recruit parasympathetic resources is also linked to inhibitory control (Holzmann & Bridgett, 2017). Moreover, while we found evidence that RSA functions as a moderator during the third year of life, physiological systems may be more open to environmental influence earlier in development, and therefore may be more likely to function as mediators. As the activity of physiological systems becomes increasingly consolidated, this activity may be more likely to function as a moderator. Understanding how the role of physiological systems change over the course of early childhood – and how that heterochronicity varies with respect to different developmental outcomes – is an important direction for future research.
Our study makes several contributions to the literature. It is the first study of which we are aware to systematically examine whether RSAB functions as a mediator or moderator of both distal (i.e., cumulative risk) and proximal (parenting) factors on inhibitory control, and it addresses this question during a particularly sensitive period for the development of inhibitory control. It is also the first study that demonstrates that negative and intrusive parenting behaviors mediate the effects of risk on inhibitory control. These findings do not settle the question of how RSAB functions with respect to inhibitory control; rather, they raise new questions about the role of physiological function with respect to environment, how that role may change, and how efforts to prevent and treat externalizing and attention problems can best be informed by an understanding of physiological function in early childhood.
Supplementary Material
Key points.
Inhibitory control is a crucial developmental outcome of early childhood, and deficits in inhibitory control have been linked to externalizing problems.
While both environment and physiological function are associated with the etiology of inhibitory control, few studies have systematically examined the role of the parasympathetic nervous system (PNS) with respect to environment and inhibitory control.
This study demonstrated that baseline activity of the PNS, indexed by respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA), moderated, rather than mediated, the influence of risk on inhibitory control, such that children exposed to high levels of risk who exhibited high baseline RSA performed better on a gift-wrap delay task of inhibitory control.
While elucidating the role of RSA with respect to inhibitory control during a sensitive period in its development, this study also underscores the need to consider how that role may change over time.
Acknowledgements
This study was supported by the North Carolina Child Development Research Collaborative which was funded by the National Science Foundation. The authors would like to thank all the parents who participated in the Durham Child Health and Development Study and the research assistants for their valuable help in collecting the data. The authors have declared that they have no competing or potential conflicts of interest.
Footnotes
Conflict of interest statement: No conflicts declared.
Supporting information
Additional Supporting Information may be found in the online version of this article:
References
- Alison PD (2009). Missing data In Millsap RE, & Maydeu-Olivares A (Eds.), The sage handbook of quantitative methods in psychology (pp. 72–89). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. [Google Scholar]
- Beauchaine T (2001). Vagal tone, development, and Gray’s motivational theory: Toward and integrated model of autonomic nervous system functioning in psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 183–214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bollen KA (1989). A new incremental fit index for general structural equation models. Sociological Methods & Research, 17, 303–316. [Google Scholar]
- Bronfenbrenner U, & Morris PA (2006). The bioecological model of human development In Lerner RM (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (pp. 793–828). Hoboken, New Jersey: John Wiley & Sons. [Google Scholar]
- Burchinal MR, Roberts JE, Hooper S, & Zeisel SA (2000). Cumulative risk and early cognitive development: A comparison of statistical risk models. Developmental Psychology, 36, 793–807. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carlson SM (2005). Developmentally sensitive measures of executive function preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28, 595–616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colman RA, Hardy SA, Albert M, Raffaelli M, & Crockett L (2006). Early predictors of self-regulation in middle childhood. Infant and Child Development, 15, 421–437. [Google Scholar]
- Conger RD, & Elder GH (1994). Families in troubled times: Adapting to change in rural America. Hawthorne, New York: Aldine de Gruyter. [Google Scholar]
- Conradt E, Measelle J, & Ablow JC (2016). Poverty, problem behavior, and promise: Differential susceptibility among infants reared in poverty. Psychological Science, 24, 235–242. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davies PT, Sturge-Apple ML, Cicchetti D, Manning LG, & Zale E (2009). Children’s patterns of emotional reactivity to conflict as explanatory mechanisms in links between interpartner aggression and child physiological functioning. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 1384–1391. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ellis BJ, & Boyce WT (2008). Biological sensitivity to context. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 183–187. [Google Scholar]
- Evans GW, & Rosenbaum J (2008). Self-regulation and the income-achievement gap. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 23, 504–514. [Google Scholar]
- Gagne JR, & Saudino KJ (2016). The development of inhibitory control in early childhood: A twin study from 2–3 years. Developmental Psychology, 52, 391–399. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gueron-Sela N, Wagner NJ, Propper C, & Cox MJ (2016). The interaction between child respiratory sinus arrhythmia and early sensitive parenting in the prediction of children’s executive functions. Infancy, 22, 171–189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hannon L, DeFina R, & Bruch S (2013). The relationship between skin tone and school suspension for African Americans. Race and Social Problems, 5, 281–295. [Google Scholar]
- Holochwost SJ, Gariépy J-L, Propper CB, Gardner-Neblett N, Volpe V, Neblett E, & Mills-Koonce WR (2016). Sociodemographic risk, parenting, and executive functions in early childhood: The role of ethnicity. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 36, 537–549. [Google Scholar]
- Holzman JB, & Bridgett DJ (2017). Heart-rate variability indices as bio-markers of top-down self-regulatory mechanisms: A meta-analytic review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Review, 74, 233–255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huffman LC, Bryan YE, del Carmen R, Pedersen FA, Doussard-Roosevelt JA, & Porges SW (1998). Infant temperament and cardiac vagal tone: Assessments at twelve weeks of age. Child Development, 69, 624–635. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karreman A, van Tuijl C, van Aken MAG, & Deković M (2006). Parenting and self-regulation in preschoolers: A meta-analysis. Infant and Child Development, 15, 561–579. [Google Scholar]
- Kennedy AE, Rubin KH, Hastings PD, Maisel B (2004). Longitudinal relations between child vagal tone and parenting behavior: 2 to 4 years. Developmental Psychology, 45, 10–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kochanska G, Aksan N, Penney SJ, & Boldt LJ (2007). Parental personality as an inner resource that moderates the impact of ecological adversity on parenting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 136–150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kochanska G, Coy KC, & Murray KT (2001). The development of self-regulation in the first four years of life. Child Development, 72, 1091–1111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kochanska G, Murray KT, & Harlan ET (2000). Effortful control in early childhood: Continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development. Developmental Psychology, 36, 220–232. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lengua LJ, Honorado E, & Bush NR (2007). Contextual risk and parenting as predictors of effortful control and social competence in preschool children. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 28, 40–55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewis GF, Furman SA, McCool MF, & Porges SW (2012). Statistical strategies to quantify respiratory sinus arrhythmia: Are commonly used metrics equivalent? Biological Psychology, 89, 349–364. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li-Grining CP (2007). Effortful control among low-income preschoolers in three cities: Stability, change, and individual differences. Developmental Psychology, 43, 208–221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marcovitch S, Leigh J, Calkins SD, Leerks EM, O’Brien M, & Blankson AN (2010). Moderate vagal withdrawal in 3.5-year-old children is associated with optimal performance on executive function tasks. Developmental Psychobiology, 52, 603–608. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mills-Koonce WR, Propper CB, & Barnett M (2012). Poor infant soothability and later insecure-ambivalent attachment: Developmental change in phenotypic markers of risk or two measures of the same construct? Infant Behavior & Development, 35, 215–225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mischel W, Shoda Y, & Rodriguez M (1989). Delay of gratification in children. Science, 244, 933–938. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mistry R, Benner AD, Biesanz JC, Clark SL, & Howes C (2010). Family and social risk, and parental investments during the early childhood years as predictors of low-income children’s school readiness outcomes. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 25, 432–449. [Google Scholar]
- Moore GA (2010). Parent conflict predicts infants’ vagal regulation in social interaction. Development and Psychopathology, 22, 23–33. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muthén LK, & Muthén BO (2015). Mplus user’s guide. Los Angeles: Muthén & Muthén. [Google Scholar]
- NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (1997). The effects of infant child care on infant–mother attachment security: Results of the NICHD study of early child care. Child Development, 68, 860–879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noble KG, Norman MF, & Farah MJ. (2005). Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten children. Developmental Science, 8, 74–87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pauli-Pott U, & Becker K (2011). Neuropsychological basic deficits in preschoolers at risk for ADHD: A meta-analysis. Child Psychology Review, 31, 626–637. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Popp TK, Spinrad TL, & Smith CL (2008). The relation of cumulative demographic risk to mothers’ responsivity and control. Examining the role of toddler temperament. Infancy, 13, 496–518. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Porges SW (1985). Method and apparatus for evaluating rhythmic oscillations in aperiodic response systems. Patent no. 4510944.
- Porges SW, & Furman SA (2011). The early development of the autonomic nervous system provides a neural platform for social behavior: A polyvagal perspective. Infant and Child Development, 20, 106–118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Preacher KJ, Curran PJ, & Bauer DJ (2006). Computational tools for probing interactions in multiple linear regression, multilevel modeling, and latent curve analysis. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 31, 437–448. [Google Scholar]
- Preacher KJ, & Hayes AF (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879–891. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Propper C (2012). The early development of vagal tone: Effects of poverty and elevated contextual risk In King R & Maholmes V (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of poverty and child development (pp. 103–123). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Propper C, Willoughby M, Halpern CT, Carbone MA, & Cox M (2007). Parenting quality, DRD4, and the prediction of externalizing and internalizing behaviors in early childhood. Developmental Psychobiology, 49, 619–632. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoemaker K, Bunte TL, Wiebe SA, Espy KA, Deković M, & Matthys W (2012). Executive function deficits in preschool children with ADHD and DBD. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 111–119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spinrad TL, Eisenberg N, Gaertner B, Popp T, Smith CL, Kupfer A, Greving K, Liew J, & Hofer C (2007). Relations of maternal socialization and toddlers’ effortful control to children’s adjustment and social competence. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1170–1186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sturge-Apple ML, Suor JH, Davies PT, Cicchetti D, Skibo MA, & Rogosch FA (2016). Vagal tone and children’s delay of gratification: Differential sensitivity in resource-poor and resource-rich environments. Psychological Science, 27, 885–893. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suess PE, Porges SW, & Plude DJ (1994). Cardiac vagal tone and sustained attention in school-age children. Psychophysiology, 31, 17–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor ZE, Eisenberg N, & Spinrad TL (2015). Respiratory sinus arrhythmia, effortful control, and parenting predictors of children’s sympathy across childhood. Developmental Psychology, 51, 17–25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.