Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2020 Jul 14;9:e53125. doi: 10.7554/eLife.53125

Recurrent circuitry is required to stabilize piriform cortex odor representations across brain states

Kevin A Bolding 1, Shivathmihai Nagappan 1, Bao-Xia Han 1, Fan Wang 1, Kevin M Franks 1,
Editors: Stephen Liberles2, Laura L Colgin3
PMCID: PMC7360366  PMID: 32662420

Abstract

Pattern completion, or the ability to retrieve stable neural activity patterns from noisy or partial cues, is a fundamental feature of memory. Theoretical studies indicate that recurrently connected auto-associative or discrete attractor networks can perform this process. Although pattern completion and attractor dynamics have been observed in various recurrent neural circuits, the role recurrent circuitry plays in implementing these processes remains unclear. In recordings from head-fixed mice, we found that odor responses in olfactory bulb degrade under ketamine/xylazine anesthesia while responses immediately downstream, in piriform cortex, remain robust. Recurrent connections are required to stabilize cortical odor representations across states. Moreover, piriform odor representations exhibit attractor dynamics, both within and across trials, and these are also abolished when recurrent circuitry is eliminated. Here, we present converging evidence that recurrently-connected piriform populations stabilize sensory representations in response to degraded inputs, consistent with an auto-associative function for piriform cortex supported by recurrent circuitry.

Research organism: Mouse

Introduction

Recognition occurs at the interface of perception and memory: it requires being able to reliably identify a familiar object, even when the stimulus is noisy or degraded, or when behavioral states change. Sensory systems must, therefore, be able to generate representations of the stimuli that are robust to changes in input or ongoing brain activity. Theoretical studies have shown that this retrieval can occur in recurrent neural networks through processes called auto-associative recall, attractor dynamics, or pattern completion (Hopfield, 1982; Marr, 1971; Haberly and Bower, 1989; Hasselmo et al., 1990; Treves and Rolls, 1994; Eichenbaum, 2004; Rolls, 2013; Guzman et al., 2016; Chaudhuri and Fiete, 2016). Phenomenological evidence for pattern completion has been described in various recurrent networks, including hippocampus (Nakazawa et al., 2002; Wills et al., 2005; Neunuebel and Knierim, 2014), piriform cortex (Chapuis and Wilson, 2012), and neocortex (Suzuki and Gottlieb, 2013; Carrillo-Reid et al., 2016; Inagaki et al., 2019). However, despite recurrent collateral connections being a defining feature of auto-associative networks, whether these phenomena indeed require recurrent connectivity has not been demonstrated.

Odors are initially encoded as combinations of co-active glomeruli in the olfactory bulb (OB). This elemental odor code is then integrated in the piriform cortex (PCx) where a synthetic or configural representation of the odor object is first formed (Gottfried, 2010; Wilson and Sullivan, 2011; Howard and Gottfried, 2014) but see Barwich, 2019. Projections from OB to PCx are diffuse and overlapping (Sosulski et al., 2011; Ghosh et al., 2011; Miyamichi et al., 2011) allowing individual principal neurons in PCx to integrate inputs from different and possibly random subsets of glomeruli. The diffuse projections from OB to PCx activate odor-specific ensembles of neurons distributed across PCx [Stettler and Axel, 2009; Roland et al., 2017] whose concerted activity encodes odor identity (Miura et al., 2012; Bolding and Franks, 2017). Alone, these afferent inputs would render PCx odor representations wholly dependent on their inputs from the OB. However, principal cells make excitatory synaptic connections onto other PCx cells with sparse but uniform connection probabilities across millimeters of PCx, forming an extensive recurrent network, similar in synaptic organization to hippocampal CA3 (Johnson et al., 2000; Franks et al., 2011; Hagiwara et al., 2012; Guzman et al., 2016). Selective strengthening of recurrent synapses between co-responsive PCx neurons can lead to the formation of cortical odor cell assemblies that can generate stable odor representations when OB inputs are noisy or degraded (Hasselmo et al., 1990; Ambros-Ingerson et al., 1990; Haberly, 2001).

PCx populations can demonstrate pattern separation or pattern completion-like responses depending on recent training history (Chapuis and Wilson, 2012). However, these and other major observations regarding associative functions and odor-evoked activation patterns in PCx were obtained in either ex vivo (Hasselmo and Barkai, 1995; Barkai et al., 1994) or anesthetized preparations (Barnes et al., 2008Chapuis and Wilson, 2012). Odor responses in both OB and PCx are reported to depend strongly on global brain state (Murakami et al., 2005; Kato et al., 2012; Rinberg et al., 2006). Previous studies, using different methods, have reported that OB responses increase (Kato et al., 2012; Rinberg et al., 2006) or remain constant (Kollo et al., 2014) under anesthesia. Curiously, we found that OB responses reliably degraded shortly after a single injection of a ketamine-xylazine anesthetic cocktail. We leveraged this observation to ask how PCx odor ensembles change when odor input is degraded. Interestingly, we found little difference in the quality of PCx representations across brain states, suggesting that PCx circuits may transform noisy or degraded OB inputs into stable cortical odor representations. Using a novel transgenic line and selective disruption of PCx output, we find that intracortical synaptic connections within PCx are required for this stabilization, consistent with the crucial role for recurrent connectivity in implementing cortical pattern completion.

Results

Odor responsivity is state-dependent in OB but not PCx

We simultaneously recorded spiking activity in populations of OB mitral cells and layer II neurons in PCx using 32-site silicon probes in head-fixed mice before and after inducing ketamine/xylazine anesthesia (k/x; Figure 1a). Anesthesia induced pronounced changes in respiration patterns, oscillatory activity, and spontaneous spiking in both OB and PCx (Figure 1, b and c; Figure 1—figure supplement 1); see also [Murakami et al., 2005; Rinberg et al., 2006; Fontanini and Bower, 2005; Li et al., 2012]. Previous reports found reduced OB firing rates under anesthesia, though estimates of spontaneous OB firing rates vary widely (Rinberg et al., 2006; Kollo et al., 2014; Shusterman et al., 2011). Our measures of spontaneous OB activity (n = 187 cells; awake: (mean ± st.dev.) 6.58 ± 6.47 Hz; k/x: 2.62 ± 4.37 Hz) are similar to those observed using whole-cell recordings in vivo by Kollo et al. (n = 60 cells, awake: (mean ± st.dev.) 4.1 ± 6.7 Hz; k/x: n = 84 cells, 2.7 ± 4.6 Hz) (Kollo et al., 2014), but are substantially lower than those made with single extracellular electrodes by Rinberg et al. (n = 11 cells; awake: (mean ± st.dev.) 27.4 ± 9.7 Hz; k/x: 9.3 ± 4.4 Hz) (Rinberg et al., 2006). These differences likely stem from improved identification of lower firing-rate units using multi-site extracellular electrodes, longer-duration recordings, and improved spike-sorting algorithms.

Figure 1. Odor responsivity is state-dependent in OB but not PCx.

(a) Recording and experimental schematics. Timing of first and last trials designated as anesthetized for analysis, starting from the onset of behavioral indicators of anesthesia and continuing for 10–15 presentations of all odors (n = 12 experiments in six mice). Error bars are mean ± SEM. (b) Example recording showing irregular respiration (top) and desynchronized spiking in awake OB (middle) and PCx (bottom). Ticks indicate action potentials and each row represents a different cell. Gray shading indicates inhalation, blue shading indicates odor delivery, red trace is an example PID trace monitored at the exhaust port of the switched final delivery valve. (c) Same recording as b after k/x injection. Respiration becomes rhythmic and spontaneous spiking in OB and PCx slows and becomes entrained to respiration under anesthesia. (d) Example OB cell-odor pair responses during awake and k/x trials showing loss of significant odor responses under anesthesia. Arrows indicate significant responses. (e) Example PCx cell-odor pair responses during awake and k/x trials showing preserved odor responses under anesthesia. Arrows indicate significant responses. (f) Percent of OB cell-odor pairs with significant activation or suppression (n = 187 cells, 12 experiments). Asterisks indicate p<0.05 in bootstrap difference test (Activation: p=0.007; Suppression: p<0.001). Boxes indicate quartiles and whiskers indicate ± 2.7 st. dev. from mean. Data points outside this range are shown as circles. (g) Lifetime and population sparseness in OB (bootstrap difference test, p<0.05) across all cells and odors. Asterisks indicate p<0.05 on bootstrap difference test. Lifetime: p<0.001; Population: p<0.001. (h–i) As in f-g, but in PCx (n = 640 cells, 11 experiments). Activation: p=0.003; Suppression: p<0.001; Lifetime: p=0.45; Population: p=0.22. (j–k) Trial-by-trial pseudopopulation correlation matrices sorted by odor within OB (j) and PCx (k) in awake and anesthetized conditions. Odors are (1) ethyl butyrate, (2) isoamyl acetate, (3) 2-hexanone, (4) hexanal, (5) ethyl tiglate, (6) ethyl acetate. (l) Bootstrapped distributions of within- and across-odor trial-to-trial correlations in OB and PCx computed by sampling cells with replacement and computing the mean correlation within and across odors 1000 times. Means are shown as filled circles. (m) Separation (mean within-odor correlations - mean across-odor correlations) of odor representations in OB and PCx over 1000 bootstrap iterations. (n–o) Odor classification accuracy as a function of pseudopopulation size in OB (n) and PCx (o) in awake and anesthetized states using a multiclass linear support vector machine. Mean ± 95% bootstrapped confidence intervals.

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. State-dependent changes in respiration, local field potential and spontaneous spiking.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

(a) Respiration rate (top) and coefficient of variation (CV, bottom) measured in a sliding 60 s window before and during anesthesia (n = 12 experiments, mean ± sem). (b) Average respiration rate did not differ between awake (gray) and anesthetized periods (blue); paired t-test, t(11) = −1.07, p=0.30. (c) Variability in the rate and amplitude or respiration decreases under anesthesia (paired t-tests; rate: t(11) = 6.93, p=2.48e-5; amplitude: t(11) = 6.98, p=2.32e-5.) Error bars are mean ± sem. (d–e) Example local field potential traces from OB (d) and PCx (e) from awake and anesthetized periods. Awake LFP in both regions is desynchronized, while anesthetized LFP becomes strongly coupled to respiration. (f) Example spectrogram showing LFP power in OB throughout an experiment. After k/x injection, high frequency power rapidly diminishes and is replaced by strong low frequency oscillations. (g) Average power spectra during awake and anesthetized periods in OB (n = 12, mean ± sem). (h–i) As in f and g but for PCx (n = 11, mean ± sem). (j) Spontaneous spiking activity is reduced and more couple to respiration in OB under anesthesia. Left, cumulative distribution of spontaneous firing rates in awake (black) and anesthetized (blue) periods. Mean shown as unfilled circles. Paired t-test, t(186) = 14.46, p=6.22e-34. Right, cumulative distribution of pairwise phase-consistency of spiking relative to respiration phase. Paired t-test, t(186) = −15.03, p=3.07e-32. (k) As in j, but for PCx. Paired t-tests: Spontaneous rate, t(639) = 21.66, p=1.88e-78. Pairwise phase consistency, t(639) = −29.52, p=1.81e-121.
Figure 1—figure supplement 2. Criteria for maintaining sorted unit identity across states.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2.

(a) Left, Overall firing rates for all sorted OB units. Units falling below one spike in 0.01 Hz in either state were excluded. Middle, Fold-change in overall firing rate across states. Units with a greater than 100-fold change were excluded. Right, Change in waveform amplitude across states. Spike amplitudes for each unit were measured on the channel with the largest peak-to-peak waveforms. Units with greater than 50 μV changes in peak-to-peak amplitude were excluded. (b) Fraction of cells considered stable under varying rate (left), rate-change (middle), and waveform amplitude (right) criteria. (c–d) Primary experimental results do not depend strongly on the choice of stability criteria. The percentage of cell-odor pairs significantly activated by odor in OB is always substantially lower under anesthesia than during awake trials despite varying the rate-change (c) or waveform (d) criteria. Stricter waveform stability criteria tend to enhance the effect. (e–h) As in (a–d) but for PCx.
Figure 1—figure supplement 3. Odor response characteristics during awake and anesthetized trials.

Figure 1—figure supplement 3.

(a) Average odor response for all OB cell-odor pairs during awake (black) or k/x (blue) trials (n = 1122 cell-odor pairs, mean ± sem). (b–f) Responses with any peak in the 500 ms following inhalation were identified and their response features characterized. Awake, n = 1059; K/X, n = 799 cell-odor pairs. (b) Response peak is the maximum of the cell-odor PSTH within 500 ms. (c) Onset latency is the latency to cross a threshold of 2.5 s.d. above the 3 s pre-odor baseline. (d) Peak latency is the time of the maximum of the cell-odor PSTH within 500 ms. (e) Response duration measured as full-width at half-maximum in the first 500 ms. (f) Response duration measures as time above a 2.5 s.d. threshold. This accounts for multiphasic responses that may have sharp initial peaks followed by later, lower amplitude rate increases. Awake responses tended to be larger and lower latency, with slightly wider peaks but shorter overall responses. (g–l) As in a-f, but for PCx.
Figure 1—figure supplement 4. Degraded OB decoding due to weaker odor-evoked responses.

Figure 1—figure supplement 4.

We evaluated three factors possibly contributing to degraded OB decoding under anesthesia: 1)Decreased sensitivity of OB cells to odors under anesthesia 2)Increased variability of OB responses under anesthesia 3)The presence of global background activity pattern under anesthesia (a-b) Change in response magnitude and change in baseline firing rate from awake to anesthetized trials for awake-only (c) or robust (d) OB cell-odor pairs. Black circles are cell-odor pairs. Red circles are means. Mean odor response decreases more strongly for awake-only responses and the decrease is not matched by a decrease in baseline rate. (c-d) Fano factor for odor evoked responses across all awake or anesthetized trials for all cell-odor pairs (e, aw, n = 1082, k/x, n = 785; Unpaired t-test: t(1865) = 3.35, p=8.34e-04) or awake-only cell-odor pairs (f, aw, n = 81, k/x, n = 69, Unpaired t-test: t(148) = 0.49, p=0.63). Fano factors are lower (all pairs) or not significantly changed (awake-only pairs) during anesthetized trials, excluding the possibility that increased variability explains loss of significant responses under anesthesia. (e) Distribution of correlation coefficients for population activity four sniffs prior to odor exposure in awake or k/x conditions. Background correlations increase to a greater extent in PCx than OB, contrasting with the greater degradation of OB decoding accuracy.
Figure 1—figure supplement 5. Trial-trial correlation changes after anesthesia reflect contributions of ‘background’ patterns and reduced reliability of OB odor responses.

Figure 1—figure supplement 5.

Increases in both within-odor and across-odor correlations observed in OB after anesthesia are not expected given a simple loss of OB responsivity. We therefore investigated the origins of increased OB correlations. Correlations could be driven by imposition of an odor-independent ‘background’ pattern that has a greater influence in anesthetized OB than awake OB or PCx in either state. (a–b) Scatterplot and histograms showing distributions of mean and standard deviation of spike counts across all odors in either state for OB (a) or PCx (b). Anesthetized OB (blue) has a large number of low-rate and low-s.d. responses, indicating a large portion of cells response weakly but very similarly to every trial of every odor. Including these responses in trial-trial comparisons can drive very large correlations within and across odors. (c–d) Illustration of trial-trial correlation calculation for one pair of within-odor trials (black) and one pair of across-odor trials (green) for raw spike counts without background subtraction in OB (c) and PCx (d). Correlation coefficients for these example trial-trial comparisons are indicated to the right of each linear fit line. Note small difference in within and across odor correlations in anesthetized OB. However, all correlations are large and positive, due to large numbers of consistently low-firing cells. (e–f) As in c-d, but for trial-trial comparisons after subtracting mean response across all odors within a state from each trial’s response. In this condition, across-odor correlations are often negative. (g–i) As in Figure 1j–l, but for background-subtracted population responses. After background subtraction, anesthetized OB tends to have weaker within-odor correlations than other conditions indicating reduced reliability of OB odor responses in this condition. Thus, after removing ‘background patterns’, OB responses remain degraded relative to PCx or awake OB responses.
Figure 1—figure supplement 6. Similar outcomes for simultaneously recorded PCx populations and pseudopopulations.

Figure 1—figure supplement 6.

(a–b) Within-across odor population vector correlation difference for awake trials (a) or k/x trials (b) in OB and PCx. Each line is a separate simultaneous OB-PCx recording (n = 11). Circles show means for OB (red) and PCx (black). Pseudopopulation correlations measures from Figure 1 are shown as shaded violin plots for comparison. (c–d) Classifier accuracy for simultaneously recorded PCx populations up to 60 cells in awake (c) or k/x trials (d). Each light green line is a separate PCx recording. The average accuracy across recordings is shown as a bold green line. Classifier performance from Figure 1 is shown as a dashed line for comparison.
Figure 1—figure supplement 7. Background activity or non-specific odor responses do not significantly impair OB decoding under k/x anesthesia.

Figure 1—figure supplement 7.

(a) Odor classification accuracy as a function of pseudopopulation size in OB for anesthetized trials using a multiclass linear support vector machine with raw spike counts (solid blue line) or with ‘background activity’ subtracted (dashed blue line). For each permutation, a random pseudopopulation of n cells was selected and differences in classification accuracy using the raw and background-subtracted responses was determined. The solid black line indicates the mean difference across permutations and grey shading indicates 99% confidence intervals across permutations. (b) As in a but with average response across all odors subtracted instead of background activity.

We examined odor-evoked spiking activity in individual cells in OB and PCx during the first sniff after odor delivery (Figure 1, d and e). OB neurons were less responsive under anesthesia, with many fewer significantly activated or suppressed neurons, and increased lifetime and population sparseness (Figure 1, f and g). We ensured that this effect was indeed due to changes in spiking activity and not to unit drift or instabilities over the course of the recording (Figure 1—figure supplement 2). Yet, despite reduced input from OB, PCx responsivity shifted only subtly and, in fact, toward greater activation (Figure 1, h and i; Figure 1—figure supplement 3), due largely to an increase in signal-to-noise ratio as spontaneous activity levels in PCx dropped under anesthesia. Individual cell-odor pair responses rarely switched from significantly activated to suppressed or vice versa (OB: act. to supp.: 0.98% of activated responses.; supp. to act.: 3.7% of suppressed responses; PCx: act. to supp.: 0.81%; supp. to act: 5.0%). Tuning curves for neurons in either region did not exhibit an overall tendency toward either maintained ordering or reconfiguration such that there was only a modest, positive average correlation between individual neuron tuning curves for awake and anesthetized responses (OB: 0.13 ± 0.47; PCx: 0.16 ± 0.48). Although we did find some reliable OB responses after k/x injection, consistent with other anesthesia regimes (Yokoi et al., 1995; Dhawale et al., 2010; Buonviso et al., 1992), the reduced OB odor responsivity we observed was unexpected given that previous studies have reported larger and longer-duration odor responses under k/x anesthesia (Kato et al., 2012; Rinberg et al., 2006). These differences could result from differences in the anesthesia methods, recording methods, odorant concentrations, or behavioral states (please see Discussion). In our experiments, the loss of OB responsivity was largely due to decreased odor-evoked spiking under k/x anesthesia, rather than an increase in response variability or increased baseline firing (Figure 1—figure supplement 4). Crucially, however, we found that PCx responsivity was preserved even though OB responsivity was consistently suppressed.

To examine odor responses at the population level we constructed pseudopopulation vectors of OB or PCx firing rates for each odor trial and measured the similarity of responses using trial-to-trial correlations within or across odors (Figure 1j-l). In these and other analyses (except those presented in Figure 6), we excluded the first ~5 trials to minimize the contribution of rapid sniffing in response to the first few odor presentations, and we used an equivalent number of trials during stable anesthesia, shortly after initial induction (Figure 1a). Responses in both regions became more correlated under anesthesia (Figure 1j-l). This was due primarily to a subset of cells in OB and PCx that exhibited weak and stereotyped responses to all odors under k/x anesthesia, driving up both within- and across-trial correlations, although this was especially pronounced in OB (Figure 1—figure supplement 5). However, even as all responses became more correlated, the difference between within- versus across-odor response correlations decreased significantly more in OB than PCx (Figure 1m; Figure 1—figure supplement 5i). Greater inter-odor response separation in PCx than OB under anesthesia was also observed when comparing population vector correlations for simultaneously recorded OB and PCx neuron populations (Figure 1—figure supplement 6, a and b).

Next, we determined how accurately odors could be identified from the spiking activity of populations of OB and PCx neurons. To do this, we trained and tested classifiers on either awake or anesthetized odor trials from pseudopopulations of OB or PCx neurons. Decoding accuracy was markedly worse under anesthesia in OB (Figure 1n), whereas responses in PCx were decoded equally well in either state (Figure 1o). OB response decoding was not statistically significantly improved after accounting for changes in baseline firing rate or the imposition of odor-dependent but non-odor-specific ‘background patterns’ that may emerge under k/x anesthesia (Figure 1—figure supplement 7), indicating that OB decoding degraded due to weakened odor responses rather than the imposition of a global background activity pattern. Finally, classifier performance using individual PCx experiments with simultaneously-recorded populations of up to 60 cells did not differ significantly from accuracies for size-matched pseudopopulations (Figure 1—figure supplement 6, c and d).

PCx stabilizes odor representations across activity regimes

These data indicate that PCx maintains odor responsivity when OB input is degraded, but not whether the cortical odor representations themselves are preserved across states. To address this question, we focused first on cells that had statistically significant increases in odor-evoked spiking. We defined activated cell-odor pairs as either robust if they were activated in both regimes or as state-specific if they were only activated in awake or only in anesthetized states (Figure 2a and b). There were many more robust responses in PCx than OB (OB, 21/102 total awake responses; PCx, 118/237 total awake responses, Figure 2c, top). To avoid simply classifying responses as ‘activated’ or ‘not-activated’ according to an arbitrary statistical threshold, we again considered single-trial population responses using spike rates for all OB and PCx neurons. Compared to OB, PCx population responses to the same odor were more correlated across states (Figure 2d) and were more separable from responses to other odors (Figure 2e), indicating again that population odor representations are better preserved across states in PCx.

Figure 2. PCx stabilizes odor representations across activity regimes.

(a–b) Example population responses to six odors for three OB (a) and one PCx recordings (b). The response index indicates the reliability of the difference between pre-odor and odor-evoked spiking (i.e. auROC*2–1). Awake-only (black), k/x-only (cyan), and robust cell-odor pairs (magenta), which were activated in both states ('activated' = p<0.05 rank-sum test), are indicated at right. PCx cells are sorted by their dorsal-ventral location. Note more robust PCx responses in deeper, (i.e. more dorsal) layer II. (c) Top, the fraction of significant awake cell-odor pair responses that are preserved under anesthesia in OB (n = 187 cells, 12 experiments) and PCx (n = 640 cells, 11 experiments). Bottom, the fraction of significant cell-odor pair responses under anesthesia that are observed in awake condition. Asterisks indicate p<0.05 on bootstrap difference test (aw to k/x: p<0.001; k/x to aw: p=0.023). (d) Cross-state trial-by-trial pseudopopulation correlation matrices sorted by odor within OB and PCx illustrate the similarity between awake and anesthetized population responses. The horizontal bands in these matrices indicate awake trials that had consistently high (light bands) or consistently low correlations (dark bands) with k/x responses regardless of odor. Typically, dark bands occur on awake trials with higher firing rates. Note that for these analyses, we used trials 6–13, and the regularity of these bands, especially in OB, reflects the progressive adaptation of responses, which are described in detail in Figure 6e. Separation (mean within-odor correlations - mean across-odor correlations) of cross-state odor representations in OB and PCx over 1000 bootstrap iterations. Asterisks indicate p<0.05 in bootstrap difference test (OB vs PCx, p<0.001). Means are shown as filled circles. (f) Cross-state decoding accuracy (trained on awake trials, tested on k/x trials) in OB and PCx. Mean ± 95% bootstrapped confidence intervals. (g) OB pseudopopulation activity projected onto the first three stimulus-dependent demixed PCA components and shown as mean ± 1 s.d. ellipsoids across trials of the same odor. Different colors correspond to different odors. Filled ellipsoids are awake responses, unfilled ellipsoids are anesthetized responses. (h) As in g, but for PCx. Note both the overall larger separation between odors in either state and the greater overlap of same-odor responses across states. (i) Overlap between awake and k/x trial response distributions projected onto their first three stimulus-dependent components in OB (red) and PCx (black). Dots show overlap scores for individual odors. Overlap is calculated using Matusita’s overlap measure (see Materials and methods). Unpaired t-test, n = 6 odors, t(10) = −3.41, p=0.007.

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. State-dependent low-dimensional representations in OB and PCx.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(a) OB pseudopopulation responses to six different odors (different colors) recorded in awake filled spheres) and anesthetized states (open spheres) projected onto the first three principal components. Spheres are centered on response mean and describe ±1 s.d. ellipsoids. (b) As in a, but for PCx.

To determine how similar odor-evoked activity patterns were across states, we trained a classifier using responses recorded in the awake state and tested the classifier on responses recorded under anesthesia. Cross-state decoding was better in PCx than OB (Figure 2f), indicating that PCx could extract and selectively represent stimulus-specific information from partial and noisy OB input. Nevertheless, cross-state decoding using spike counts was relatively poor in both OB and PCx, and Principal Components Analysis (PCA) indicated that state accounted for most of the variance in responses across states in both regions (Figure 2—figure supplement 1). Thus, if pattern stability is a reflection of overlap in a low-dimensional neural activity space, then neither region maintained similar responses across states. We therefore considered an alternative model, in which a downstream decoder could adapt to overall state-dependent changes and still maintain the ability to distinguish stimuli. To explore this possibility, we used demixed PCA to isolate the stimulus-specific features of the population responses (Kobak et al., 2016a). This analysis revealed that OB responses to different odors were clearly separable and responses to the same odor overlapped partly in awake and anesthetized states (Figure 2g). However, in PCx responses to the same odor overlapped almost completely , indicating that the stimulus-specific features of the cortical odor response are almost identical across states and better preserved than their inputs (Figure 2h and i). Thus, regardless of whether downstream processing is fixed or adaptive, PCx actively transforms degraded OB output to represent the stimulus more faithfully than the input it receives.

Robust PCx representations derive from short-latency OB responses

To generate stable cortical odor representations using degraded and noisy OB input, PCx must over-represent the impact of the few robust OB responses. What features of the OB response does PCx use to selectively extract this information? Peak firing rate distributions of robust and state-specific OB responses were broad and overlapped substantially in either regime (Figure 3, a-e), and though robust responses appeared slightly larger, on average this difference was not statistically significant. Instead, robust OB responses had significantly shorter latencies than state-specific responses (Figure 3f). Thus, PCx could over-represent early inputs to produce a stable output (Bolding and Franks, 2018). However, robust responses in PCx not only had shorter latencies (Figure 3, g-l), but were also substantially stronger than state-specific ones (Figure 3k), indicating that robust responses are actively amplified within PCx.

Figure 3. Robust PCx representations derive from short-latency OB responses.

Figure 3.

(a,e) All cell-odor pair responses for example simultaneously recorded OB (a) and PCx (e) populations sorted by onset latency determined by 2.5 st. dev. threshold crossing. Latencies are marked with filled circles. Robust, awake-only, and non-significant responses are magenta, black, and gray, respectively. (b–c) Overlay plots of all OB awake-only responses (b) or robust responses (c). The bold line is the mean response. (d) Mean (solid line) and median (dashed line) awake-only (black) and robust (magenta) OB responses. (f–h) As in b-d, but for PCx. (i–j) Peak firing rates (i) and onset latencies (j) for robust vs awake-only responses in OB. Boxes indicate quartiles and whiskers indicate ± 2.7 st. dev. from mean. Data points outside this range are shown as circles. Asterisks indicate p<0.05 in unpaired t-test. n = 81 awake-only OB cell-odor pairs and 21 robust OB cell-odor pairs. Peak: t(100) = −0.82, p=0.42; Latency: t(100) = 3.06, p=0.003. (k–l) As in i-j but for PCx. n = 125 awake-only PCx cells and 116 robust PCx cells. Peak: t(239) = −4.49, p=1.12e-5; Latency: t(236) = 2.47, p=0.01.

Pattern recovery depends on intracortical synaptic inputs

Piriform cortex is a recurrent cortical circuit that resembles auto-associative or discrete attractor networks. The ability to generate stable output patterns using degraded input is a property of such networks. If PCx is indeed such a network then the recurrent connectivity between PCx neurons should be essential for stabilizing odor representations across states. PCx contains two distinct classes of principal cells: pyramidal cells (PYRs), which are located primarily in deeper layer II and receive both OB and recurrent collateral inputs, and semilunar cells (SLs), which are more superficial and receive strong OB input but weak or no recurrent input (Figure 4aSuzuki and Bekkers, 2006; Choy et al., 2015). To test whether recurrent connectivity predicts response stability, we generated a Netrin G1-Cre mouse line (Ntng1-Cre) that selectively expresses Cre recombinase in SLs in PCx (Figure 4, b and c). We then virally expressed either Cre-dependent Archaeorhodopsin-3 (Arch), Jaws, or Channelrhodopsin-2 (ChR2) in PCx to optogenetically differentiate SLs from PYRs during population recordings (Figure 4, d-f, Figure 4—figure supplement 1; 22.4 ± 16.0% opto-tagged, putative SL cells, n = 12 recordings). Indeed, odor responses were better-preserved across states in PYRs than SLs: (Figure 4, g and h). Ntng1-Cre mice were generally more sensitive to k/x anesthesia than the Emx1-Cre mice that we used in our control recordings, resulting in greater suppression of overall activity in Ntng1-Cre mice, and therefore a slightly lower fraction of total robust responses. SLs also recapitulated the degradation of OB representations under anesthesia (Figure 4, i and j) and the poor cross-state decoding (Figure 4k), whereas PYRs successfully recovered representations across states. Thus, two PCx cell populations, receiving the same feedforward input but distinguished by their recurrent connectivity, differentially recover odor representations when OB input is degraded.

Figure 4. Pattern recovery requires recurrent circuits.

(a) Schematic of inputs to excitatory cell-types in PCx. Semilunar cells only receive OB input; pyramidal cells receive OB and recurrent collateral inputs. (b–c) Selective expression in PCx semilunar cells using the Ntng1-Cre driver line. (b) Strong expression of Cre-dependent tdTomato in PCx layer IIa of Ntng1-Cre X Ai14 mice. Scale bars: 500 μm and 100 μm. (c) Sparse Cre-dependent GCaMP6 expression shows Ntng1+ cells restricted to superficial layer II (i.e. layer IIa) and lacking basal dendrites. Scale bar: 100 μm. (d) Identifying Ntng1+ (semilunar, SL) and Ntng1- (pyramidal, PYR) cells in vivo using optogenetic inhibition. Injection of AAV expressing Cre-dependent Jaws in anterior piriform cortex restricts expression to cells with semilunar localization and morphologies. Scale bars: 100 μm and 20 μm. (e) Simultaneously recorded example cells exhibiting unambiguous suppression (top) or residual spiking (bottom) in response to 1 s, 532 nm light pulses. Traces show mean ± SEM responses over 40 laser pulses. Optogenetic tagging and unit stability criteria identified 108 SL and 234 PYR cells in 12 experiments from seven mice. (f) Odor responses during awake and k/x trials for example SL (top) and PYR cells (bottom). (g) State-specific (black, awake-only; cyan k/x only) and robust (magenta) responses in simultaneously recorded populations of SL and PYR cells from two example experiments. (h) The fraction of significant awake cell-odor pair responses that are preserved under anesthesia in SL and PYR. Asterisk indicates p<0.05 on bootstrap difference test (p=0.036). (i–j) Odor classification accuracy as a function of pseudopopulation size using SL (red) and PYR (black) cells in awake (i) and anesthetized (j) states. Mean ± 95% bootstrapped confidence intervals. (k) Cross-state decoding accuracy using SL (red) and PYR (black) cells. Mean ± 95% bootstrapped confidence intervals. (l) Strategy for disabling recurrent circuits in PCx. Expression of tetanus toxin in all PCx excitatory cells disrupts recurrent connectivity. (m–n) Odor classification accuracy as a function of pseudopopulation size in TeLC-infected (green) and contralateral control (black) PCx in awake (m) and anesthetized (n) states. Mean ± 95% bootstrapped confidence intervals. Pseudopopulations were built from 241 cells recorded in the control hemisphere in 4 experiments with 4 mice and 214 cells from the TeLC hemisphere in 6 experiments with five mice. (o) Cross-state decoding accuracy in TeLC- (green) and control (black) PCx. Mean ±95% bootstrapped confidence intervals.

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Criteria for identifying opto-tagged Ntng1+ cells.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

(a) Heatmaps showing trial-averaged response to 20, 1 s laser pulses in presumptive Arch+/Jaws+ (top) and Arch-/Jaws- (bottom). Arch or Jaws-expressing cells show rapid onset and deep suppression during exposure to 532 nm or 640 nm light, respectively. (b) Two criteria were applied to identify Arch+/Jaws+ cells: 1) p<0.0001 in rank-sum test of spiking in the 1 s preceding and during laser stimulation, 2) median last-spike latency during laser pulse <0.01 ms (shown as red dots). Additionally, cells with overall firing rates < 0.175 Hz or a peak-trough time <0.35 ms in their average waveform were excluded from classification either as Arch+/Jaws+ or Arch-/Jaws- cells. Arch+/Jaws+ cells (red dots circled in red) identified with these criteria were subtly shifted toward more superficial recording locations compared to the total recorded population. (c) Two experiments (out of 12) used excitation with ChR2 for opto-tagging. Heatmaps show trial-averaged responses for all recorded cells to ~200, 1 ms pulses of 473 nm light, delivered at 4 Hz. Cells are sorted within heatmap by estimated DV position. ChR2+ cells show rapid and reliable stimulus-locked spiking. (d) Two criteria were applied to identify ChR2+ cells: (1) p-value<0.001 in Stimulus-Associated spike Latency Test (see Materials and methods), (2) latency-to-peak response in PSTH <0.003 ms. Cells with a peak-trough time <0.35 in their average waveform were excluded from classification as ChR2+ or ChR2-. ChR2+ cells (red dots circled in red) tended toward more superficial recording locations compared to the total recorded population.
Figure 4—figure supplement 2. Across-experiment variability in response preservation depends on preserved spontaneous activity.

Figure 4—figure supplement 2.

Fraction of significant awake PCx cell-odor pair responses that are preserved under anesthesia vs. mean change in spontaneous firing rate in Emx1-cre recordings (black, n = 11 experiments) and NTNG1-cre recordings (blue n = 14 experiments). Dashed lines are linear fits. Solid lines are mean ± s.e.m. All recorded PCx cells are considered in this analysis, rather than dividing SL and PYR responses. Spontaneous rates were more strongly affected by anesthesia in some NTNG1 experiments, accompanied by a lower rate of response preservation.
Figure 4—figure supplement 3. Decoding from TeLC-ipsilateral and contralateral OB populations.

Figure 4—figure supplement 3.

(a–b) Odor classification accuracy as a function of pseudopopulation size in OB ipsilateral (green) or contralateral (black) to TeLC-infected PCx in awake (a) and anesthetized (b) states. Mean ±95% bootstrapped confidence intervals. (c) Cross-state decoding accuracy in TeLC-ipsilateral (green) or contralateral (black) OB. Mean ±95% bootstrapped confidence intervals.

Next, we asked whether eliminating recurrent connectivity in PCx abolished the ability to recover odor representations across states. To test this prediction, we injected conditional viral vectors to express tetanus toxin (AAV-DIO-GFP-TeLC) into PCx of Emx1-Cre mice to express TeLC in all PCx excitatory neurons (Figure 4l), effectively converting PCx into a pure feedforward circuit driven by OB (Bolding and Franks, 2018). We then obtained simultaneous bilateral recordings from TeLC-infected and contralateral control PCx hemispheres before and during anesthesia. Interestingly, odor responses could be classified equally accurately in awake responses from control and TeLC PCx (Figure 4m), and TeLC-PCx decoding was only slightly impaired under anesthesia (Figure 4n). Critically, however, cross-state decoding in PCx was markedly degraded in TeLC PCx (Figure 4o), indicating that recovery of the odor-specific features of the cortical response during anesthesia requires recurrent connections. To understand why within-state decoding was only minimally affected by TeLC expression, we examined decoding in OB ipsi- and contralateral to TeLC expression. Because TeLC expression blocks all PCx output from infected neurons, including strong projections from PCx to OB inhibitory neurons, OB output is substantially larger ipsilateral to TeLC-PCx (Bolding and Franks, 2018), and this may compensate for the loss of recurrent circuitry within PCx. Indeed, decoding using OB responses ipsilateral to TeLC-PCx was substantially better than in contralateral control OB in both awake and anesthetized conditions (Figure 4—figure supplement 2a). However, cross-state decoding in TeLC ipsilateral OB remained poor (Figure 4—figure supplement 2b), indicating that stronger ipsilateral TeLC-PCx OB responses can support reliable within-state decoding, but cannot fully rescue a state-dependent reconfiguration of OB odor representations.

Timescale-dependent noise correlations in PCx populations

Our working hypothesis, that odor-selective PCx ensembles are recruited through recurrent connections, predicts some coordinated activity between simultaneously recorded PCx cell-pairs. However, previous work by Miura et al. reported near-zero noise correlations during PCx odor responses, appearing to preclude strong intracortical recruitment (Miura et al., 2012). Our data are consistent, both with these previous results and with the idea that an attractor network should show some weakly correlated activity across trials. Using Miura et al.'s calculation methods (in a 120 ms window following inhalation onset), noise correlations are indeed near-zero (Figure 5). However, correlated activity generated by an interconnected recurrent network should show correlations on timescales more consistent with synaptic time constants. When we used smaller bin sizes, consistent with coordinated local activity, piriform cell-pairs do show weak noise-correlations just-after the peak of the odor-evoked response. In general, noise correlations are higher at all time points under anesthesia, reflecting low-frequency coordination (i.e. rhythmic spiking). Thus, at short timescales, noise-correlations can be observed in PCx during odor responses, consistent with the ability of PCx cells to recruit each other and retrieve stored odor-specific patterns.

Figure 5. Noise correlations in awake and anesthetized PCx.

Figure 5.

(a–c) Average noise correlations in a sliding window surrounding odor inhalation for all awake (black) or k/x (blue) odor trials using a 120 ms (a), 60 ms (b) or 30 ms (c) bin size for spike counts. Mean ± s.e.m, n = 11 experiments.

Rapid pattern formation in PCx populations

The enhanced stability of pattern activation across conditions in PCx is consistent with a pattern completion-like process occurring in PCx recurrent circuits. For stable population activity patterns to be retrieved they must first be formed; this is commonly thought to occur through experience. We therefore sought evidence for odor pattern formation in PCx in our recordings. If learning occurs during early trials then odor responses should systematically converge toward a stable pattern. We tested this prediction by measuring distances in neural activity space for population responses. Responses on initial trials were highly variable but then stabilized over subsequent trials (Figure 6a-c). Much of this variability can be explained by increased sniffing (Figure 6d), and therefore stronger overall responses, on early trials, however a multiple linear regression analysis revealed a significant effect of trial number alone (Figure 6e). Recording instabilities or changes in cell health did not account for this shift as corresponding changes did not occur for pre-trial activity (Figure 6b). Trial-by-trial stabilization curves were similar across odors and experiments (Figure 6—figure supplement 1). We considered the possibility that some part of this increased stability could be inherited from, and imparted to, OB. Our ability to interpret single-trial OB responses is limited by relatively low number of simultaneously recorded OB neurons, nevertheless we found that OB responses indeed stabilized over early trials (Figure 6f–j). OB stabilization dynamics were more rapid than in PCx, and there was a decrease in odor-evoked overall firing rates in PCx that was not apparent in OB. However, the contribution of centrifugal inputs from PCx to OB makes it impossible to determine whether the trial-dependent effects observed in OB reflects changes in OB circuitry that are then imparted to PCx or changes in PCx circuitry that are then imparted to OB.

Figure 6. Rapid pattern formation in PCx population responses.

(a, f) PCA trajectories for the first 15 presentations of three odors in an example simultaneous PCx (a) and OB (f) recording. The third odor in OB data occupied overlapping PC space and was omitted for visual clarity. The area occupied during the designated ‘stable’ trials is shown as mean ± 1 s.d. ellipsoids. Different colors correspond to different odors. (b, g) Average Euclidean distance from trial population vectors to stable trials normalized by the average distance between stable trials (b, PCx: n = 132 experiment-odor pairs, g, OB: n = 45 experiment-odor pairs, mean ± SEM). The shaded area shows distances computed using pre-odor baseline activity (mean ± SEM). (c, h) Average population firing rates during odor response (mean ± SEM) or pre-odor baseline (shaded area, mean ± SEM) for PCx (c) or OB (h). (d, i) Average sniffing rates during odor response (mean ± SEM) or pre-odor baseline (shaded area, mean ± SEM) for PCx recordings (d) and OB recordings (i). (e) Left, multiple linear regression coefficients for effects of sniff rate, population firing rate, and trial number on population distance to stable in PCx (mean ± SEM). All main and interaction coefficients are significant (p<0.05). Right, Residuals plot of multiple linear regression on distance-to-stable fit with only sniff rate and population firing rate predictors, showing decreasing distance with trial number independent of these predictors. (j) As in e, but for OB recordings. (k, l) As in e, but for TeLC-infected PCx (k, n = 36 experiment-odor pairs) and contralateral control PCx (l, n = 24 experiment-odor pairs). Distance changes are fully explained by sniff rate and overall population firing rate in TeLC-PCx, but depend on trial number in control PCx.

Figure 6.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1. Similar trial-trial population response stabilization across odors.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1.

Normalized neural distance from stable (as in Figure 5) for each PCx experiment-odor pair across trials. Mean ± s.e.m stabilization curves for all odors are shown in the bottom right. Stabilization curves were similar across odors.

To account for these effects, as well as receptor desensitization, sniffing, and arousal, we compared the stabilization of responses across trials in bilateral recordings from control versus TeLC-infected PCx in the same animals. Under these conditions, we again observed a significant trial-number effect in contralateral control hemispheres, but sniffing and overall rate fully accounted for response variability in TeLC-infected PCx, with no significant trial number effect (Figure 6k and l). These data support the hypothesis that learning is instantiated in PCx by recurrent circuitry in a trial-dependent manner.

Short-term pattern stability in recurrently-connected PCx cells

Activity patterns within an attractor network should persist even after input is removed. To examine the temporal stability of PCx odor representations, we trained and tested a linear decoder on population responses (smoothed with a 200 ms kernel) from odor onset until several seconds after odor offset in responses from TeLC-infected and contralateral control hemispheres. In control PCx, odors could be accurately identified using activity at least 4 s after odor offset, but odor information decayed much more rapidly after odor offset in TeLC-infected PCx (Figure 7, a-c). We then asked if odor representations remained stable at later time points or whether patterns of activity evolved dynamically across time (Friedrich and Laurent, 2001Bathellier et al., 2008). To do this we trained our decoder on responses at odor offset and then tested on subsequent epochs. We could accurately classify responses long after odor offset in responses from control hemispheres, whereas decoding accuracy from TeLC-PCx decayed more rapidly (Figure 7, b-d). Similarly, odor-evoked activation patterns in SLs were less stable than those in simultaneously recorded PYRs in Ntng1-Cre mice (Figure 7, e-h). These results are consistent with a role for recurrent circuitry preserving odor representations in PCx after the stimulus has ended. However, we note that OB representations decayed only slightly more rapidly than in PCx (Figure 7, i-l), leaving open the possibility that OB-PCx interactions contribute to short-term maintenance of odor representations.

Figure 7. Short-term pattern stability in recurrently-connected PCx cells.

Figure 7.

(a–c) Average cross-time decoding accuracy for pseudopopulations of 200 cells recorded from control (b) or TeLC-infected (c) PCx. Responses were aligned to the first sniff after odor offset. (a) Decoding accuracy for training and testing on identical time bin at increasing times from odor offset for control (black) and TeLC- (green) PCx. Mean ± 95% bootstrapped confidence intervals. (d) Mean decoding accuracy at increasing temporal distance between the training bin (t = 0) and test bin in control (black) and TeLC- (green) PCx. These data are reflected in the rightmost dashed vertical lines in a and b. Mean ± 95% bootstrapped confidence intervals. (e–h) As in a-d, but for pseudopopulations of 100 SLs (f, red) or PYRs (g, black). (i–l) As in a-d, but for pseudopopulations of 170 OB cells (j, red) or PCx cells (k, black).

Discussion

PCx activity exhibits several features that are consistent with attractor network function. First, cortical sensory representations are preserved even when upstream representations are substantially degraded. Second, odor responses stabilize over multiple presentations, reflecting, in part, the formation of cortical odor templates. Third, odor representations persist after the stimulus has ended. And, two lines of evidence indicate that recurrent cortical circuitry underlies these phenomena. First, responses destabilize and do not converge in a trial-number dependent manner after blocking transmitter release from PCx principal neurons. Second, odor responses are more stable in recurrently connected PYRs than in SLs.

Limitations of chemogenetic approach for disabling recurrent circuits

Recurrent circuitry is classically defined as connections specifically between a single class of neurons – in this case, excitatory synaptic connections between pyramidal cells in PCx. We used the expression of TeLC in PCx excitatory cells to block synaptic output from principal cells (both SL and PYR) and thus eliminate recurrent circuity. However, in addition to blocking potential ensemble recruitment through excitatory-excitatory connections, this strategy also eliminates (1) cortical feedback projections primarily driving inhibitory granule cells in OB, (2) recruitment of feedback inhibitory circuits within PCx, and (3) any signaling to downstream targets. Pattern recovery may occur through coordinated activity in the PCx-OB loop which is disrupted by TeLC expression, but this is unlikely given that pattern recovery also degrades in SLs when the centrifugal PCx-OB connections are intact. Given that robust OB inputs occur earlier than state-dependent ones, cortical feedback inhibition likely enhances cross-state decoding by making PCx less responsive to later OB inputs, in the same way these circuits help maintain odor identity representations across odor concentrations (Bolding and Franks, 2018). However feedback inhibition cannot explain why robust PCx responses are significantly stronger than state-dependent ones; instead, this observation argues for an active amplification of this information by excitatory recurrent inputs. We cannot disprove that stabilization involves an elaborate loop from PCx to a downstream area and then back to PCx. Moving forward, recent technical developments that enable optogenetic control of the specific spatiotemporal patterns of OB output (Chong et al., 2020) could be used to examine pattern formation and recovery in PCx more directly.

Stable odor representations in PCx

Even under normal, stable behavioral conditions, the olfactory system is challenged with identifying complex odors and odorant mixtures, with fluctuating odorant concentrations, and inherent variability at the level of receptor binding and odorant sensory neuron activity. Recurrent connections within PCx are capable of undergoing plasticity and forming self-reinforcing neural ensembles (Kanter and Haberly, 1990; Jung et al., 1990; Poo and Isaacson, 2007), which endows the circuit with the ability to form coherent sensory representations from noisy input (Wilson and Sullivan, 2011; Haberly, 2001). The destabilized and degraded OB odor responses that we induced by anesthesia throw the stabilizing function of PCx processing into stark relief, but we also see subtle evidence of this stabilizing function in greater odor separability in PCx over OB populations in the awake state (Figure 1m).

Previously, we described how long-range recurrent circuits recruit strong, global feedback inhibition, implementing a ‘temporal winner-take-all’ filter that allows the earliest-active OB inputs to largely define the PCx response and thereby make PCx odor responses robust to changes in odorant concentration (Bolding and Franks, 2018). This net-inhibitory process is complementary to the more constructive role for recurrent circuits that we propose here, in which recurrent connections help selectively recruit specific subsets of similarly responsive PCx principal cells.

We have described a trial-dependent stabilization of PCx odor representations, which we propose emerges through a Hebbian learning process at recurrent synapses between co-active PCx principal cells. Interestingly, (Jacobson et al., 2018) recently reported a trial-dependent shift in odor representations in the zebrafish homolog of PCx that appeared to require synaptic plasticity – that is, it was NMDA receptor-dependent – but their responses did not appear to stabilize over eight trials. Instead, our results are consistent with a recent study from Pashkovski et al., 2020, who showed that recurrent circuits transform OB inputs to form a systematic and somewhat-invariant map of chemical odor space that preserves correlational structure across odors, presumably by the same unsupervised learning process that we are proposing here. Thus, the stabilization we observe may reflect a default, activity-dependent sculpting that occurs in PCx during passive odor sensing. However, rodents can either be trained to either discriminate between or generalize across similar odors, depending on context (Chapuis and Wilson, 2012). The balance between generalization or discrimination may be modulated at multiple processing stages to serve changing behavioral contexts (Chapuis and Wilson, 2012; Koldaeva et al., 2019; Chu et al., 2016). Intriguingly, cholinergic (Hasselmo and Bower, 1992), noradrinergic (Hasselmo et al., 1997), and GABAergic (Tang and Hasselmo, 1994; Franks and Isaacson, 2005) modulators selectively affect recurrent but not afferent synapses in PCx, providing a mechanism to coherently and flexibly implement task-dependent computations in the same circuit.

State-dependent odor responses in OB

We saw a decrease in OB odor responsivity after injection of k/x, which allowed us to examine how PCx responds with degraded input. However, previous studies have reported either no change (Kollo et al., 2014) or an increase in OB responses under anesthesia (Kato et al., 2012; Rinberg et al., 2006). Stimulus panels in each of these previous studies consisted primarily of neutral monomolecular odorants, similar to those we used, also diluted in the 0.1–10% range. We recorded from a similar population of ventrally located M/T cells and used an identical dose of ketamine/xylazine to Rinberg et al., 2006. The most substantive methodological differences between our study and theirs are 1) their freely-moving, awake animals were able to actively truncate the odor presentation by removing their nose from the odor port, whereas in our experiments head-fixed animals passively received odors for a full second and 2) their odor responses were aligned to nose withdrawal rather than to inhalation. These would both have the effect of decreasing the apparent strength of awake responses and may largely account for the differences in our results.

It is more difficult to compare the results from our extracellular recordings with calcium imaging results from Kato et al., 2012. We made additional OB recordings matching the length of their odor stimulus (4 s), accounting for the smoothing and loss of individual spikes associated with calcium imaging, and using a range of ketamine doses from 70 to 200 mg/kg, however we were unable to identify conditions in which we observed larger and/or more reliable odor responses in anesthetized OB (data not shown).

We also compared the changes we observed in spontaneous and odor-evoked activity to those reported by Kollo et al., 2014 using in vivo whole-cell recordings. By contrast with their intracellular data, we saw an overall decrease in firing rather than a narrowing of firing rate distributions in anesthetized OB activity. These differences may be partially explained by the large population of so-called ‘silent’ mitral cells, which we likely underrepresent with extracellular spike-sorting procedures. Nevertheless, we found that even very low firing rate cells in our recordings tend to decrease their firing under anesthesia and do not appear more likely to have strong odor responses. Future experiments will be required to resolve these discrepancies. However, regardless of why our results are different, they do not impact our fundamental observation that PCx odor representations remain robust when OB responses are degraded.

Odor representations in semilunar and pyramidal cells

Our findings indicate functional segregation between the coding properties of SLs and PYRs. Recent studies have demonstrated that local connectivity, molecular identity, and projection targets vary with depth in PCx. Superficial/SL cells receive input from OB and target PCx PYRs as well as more posterior targets such as lateral entorhinal cortex and cortical amygdala (Diodato et al., 2016; Mazo et al., 2017). Because SLs receive little or no input from other PCx excitatory neurons, their responses are minimally affected by local cortical computations (Choy et al., 2015; Suzuki and Bekkers, 2011). These cells may thus simply integrate converging inputs from OB with no mechanisms to correct response variability inherited from OB. As such, the downstream targets of SLs may receive more variable, state-dependent olfactory representations.

Hippocampal area CA3 has been modeled as an auto-associative network capable of storing and retrieving a large number of unique representations due to the presence of an extensive network of recurrent excitatory collaterals (Treves and Rolls, 1994). Similarly, because PCx PYRs receive inputs from other local PCx excitatory neurons, they could retrieve previously-stored odor-evoked activity patterns via recurrent reactivation (Johnson et al., 2000; Franks et al., 2011). We propose the initial activation of a small subset of superficial PCx neurons can drive reactivation of a stable ensemble of deeper cells, enabling the recovery of PCx odor responses despite degraded OB input. Interestingly, PCx cells in deep layer II project back to OB (Diodato et al., 2016; Mazo et al., 2017; Luskin and Price, 1983), suggesting that an accurate representation of the current stimulus is returned to the OB, allowing comparison between ongoing input and the retrieved activation patterns. This loop allows SLs to receive OB input that is updated by feedback from PYRs, and may explain why odor representations in SLs are better preserved after odor offset than those in TeLC-PCx (Figure 6).

We hypothesize that PCx circuits undergo rapid plasticity induced by stimulus exposure which embeds attractor states in the PCx synaptic architecture and later biases the trajectories of PCx population activity to previously visited states. We have demonstrated that these processes require recurrent excitation in PCx but the detailed mechanisms remain to be explored. The bulk of experience-driven changes we observe in PCx are accompanied by decreases in overall stimulus-evoked spiking, suggesting that plasticity in both excitatory and inhibitory connectivity may effect response stabilization (Vogels et al., 2011; D'amour and Froemke, 2015Frank et al., 2019). Also, it remains unclear whether the instability in TeLC-PCx is due to the inability to retrieve patterns or to store them in the first place. Addressing these questions will require the development of temporally restricted and synapse-type specific interventions. Nevertheless, our results demonstrate that computations carried out by recurrently connected cells within PCx enable pattern stabilization and strongly suggest that PCx serves as a locus of memory storage for odor representations acquired through incidental sensory experience.

Materials and methods

All experimental protocols were approved by Duke University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. The methods for head-fixation, data acquisition, electrode placement, stimulus delivery, and analysis of single-unit and population odor responses are adapted from those described in detail previously (Bolding and Franks, 2017). A portion of the data reported here (5 of 12 simultaneous OB-PCx experiments) were also described in that previous report.

Mice

All mice except Ntng1-cre mice were adult (>P60, 20–24 g) offspring of Emx1-Cre (+/+) breeding pairs obtained from The Jackson Laboratory (005628).

Ntng1-Cre knock-in mouse was generated using CRISPR based method in which sequences encoding the Cre recombinase followed by the bovine growth hormone polyA sequences were inserted at the start codon ATG of Ntng1 gene. During CRISPR mediated homologous recombination, an extra 162 base pairs was also inserted in front of the ATG start codon of Cre (note that these 162 base pairs are in-frame with Cre). The 162 extra DNA sequences are: atgtatttgtcaagattcctgtcgatccatgccctgtgggtgacagtgtcctctgtgatgcagccctaccttacattatcagatctgaattcactagtcgcgcccggggagcccaaaggttaccccagttggggcgggcccgaacgaaaaggtagggctgcc .

Thus, the resulting allele contains Cre with an extra 5’ end 34 amino acids inserted into the start codon of the Ntng1 gene. The knock-in allele was verified using both genomic PCR and Southern blot.

Head-fixation

Mice were habituated to head-fixation and tube restraint for 15–30 min on each of the two days prior to experiments. The head post was held in place by two clamps attached to ThorLabs posts. A hinged 50 ml Falcon tube on top of a heating pad (FHC) supported and restrained the body in the head-fixed apparatus.

Ketamine/xylazine anesthesia

To induce anesthesia, Emx1-Cre animals were injected with a bolus ketamine/xylazine cocktail (100/10 mg/kg, ip). This induced stable anesthesia lasting 30–45 min. In initial experiments, many Ntgn1-cre mice died shortly after administering this highly standardized k/x dose, and although dosage was decreased 10–20% in subsequent experiments, these mice appeared ‘more deeply anesthetized’. Throughout the recording, body temperature was maintained using a heating pad (FHC). During anesthesia breathing became metronomic and animals ceased spontaneous forelimb movements.

We defined a subset of 7 awake trials and 7 k/x trials for all analyses except the analysis of odor representation stabilization in Figure 5A–E. Awake trials were selected as the last seven trials prior to k/x injection, to minimize sniff-related variability which was prevalent in early trials. K/X trials were defined as the seven trials following behavioral and electrophysiological onset of anesthesia effects, most prominently expressed in the loss of variable breathing frequency and increase in low-frequency LFP power.

Data acquisition

Electrophysiological signals were acquired with 32-site polytrode acute probes (A1 × 32-Poly3-5mm-25s-177, Neuronexus) through an A32-OM32 adaptor (Neuronexus) connected to a Cereplex digital headstage (Blackrock Microsystems). Unfiltered signals were digitized at 30 kHz at the headstage and recorded by a Cerebus multichannel data acquisition system (BlackRock Microsystems). Experimental events and respiration signal were acquired at 2 kHz by analog inputs of the Cerebus system. Respiration was monitored with a microbridge mass airflow sensor (Honeywell AWM3300V) positioned directly opposite the animal’s nose. Negative airflow corresponds to inhalation and negative changes in the voltage of the sensor output.

Electrode placement

The recording probe was positioned in the anterior piriform cortex using a Patchstar Micromanipulator (Scientifica). For piriform cortex recordings, the probe was positioned at 1.32 mm anterior and 3.8 mm lateral from bregma. Recordings were targeted 3.5–4 mm ventral from the brain surface at this position with adjustment according to the local field potential (LFP) and spiking activity monitored online. Electrode sites on the polytrode span 275 µm along the dorsal-ventral axis. The probe was lowered until a band of intense spiking activity covering 30–40% of electrode sites near the correct ventral coordinate was observed, reflecting the densely packed layer II of piriform cortex. For simultaneous ipsilateral olfactory bulb recordings, a micromanipulator holding the recording probe was set to a 10-degree angle in the coronal plane, targeting the ventrolateral mitral cell layer. The probe was initially positioned above the center of the olfactory bulb (4.85 AP, 0.6 ML) and then lowered along this angle through the dorsal mitral cell and granule layers until encountering a dense band of high-frequency activity signifying the targeted mitral cell layer, typically between 1.5 and 2.5 mm from the bulb surface.

Spike sorting and waveform characteristics

Individual units were isolated using Spyking-Circus (https://github.com/spyking-circus) (Yger, 2018Yger et al., 2018). Clusters with >1% of ISIs violating the refractory period (<2 ms) or appearing otherwise contaminated were manually removed from the dataset. Pairs of units with similar waveforms and coordinated refractory periods in the cross-correlogram were combined into single clusters. Unit position with respect to electrode sites was characterized as the average of all electrode site positions weighted by the wave amplitude on each electrode. Relative dorsal-ventral unit position was determined by fitting the waveform positions within a recording with a truncated normal distribution (truncated at 0 and 275) and then subtracting the mean of this fit.

Unit stability criteria

To assure stable identification of cells across states, sorted units were subjected to further stability criteria (Figure 1—figure supplement 2). Units were excluded if their overall rate fell below 0.01 Hz, if their rate changed more than 100-fold across states, or if their average peak-to-peak waveform amplitude changed by more than 50 μV across states. 107/294 OB units and 63/703 PCx units were discarded based on these criteria.

Percent responding and sparseness bootstrap analyses

Cell-odor pair responsivity was visualized using a response index (2*auROC-1) comparing the distribution of odor-evoked spike counts to the pre-odor baseline. Cell-odor pairs were labeled as significantly odor-responsive using a rank-sum test (p<0.05) on trial-by-trial spike counts over the first sniff after odor delivery compared to spike counts over the last pre-odor sniff. Because of variability in cell yield for olfactory bulb experiments we computed population responsivity measures on the population of all recorded cells rather than for each experiment. We established confidence bounds for these measures (percent activated responses, lifetime and population sparseness) by sampling these with replacement from the population of all recorded cells 1000 times. For significance testing, a null distribution of mean differences (between awake and k/x samples) was constructed by randomly selecting equivalent-sized samples from the combined population 1000 times, and the p-value was the fraction of null responses more extreme than the empirical mean difference.

Trial-trial population vector correlations

The similarity of population odor responses, defined as spike count vectors within the first sniff, was quantified using the Pearson correlation coefficient. Population responses were combined across experiments to form a pseudopopulation, and correlations for all trial-pairs were calculated (i.e. seven trials for two odors = 49 correlations). The correlation between two stimuli (across odor) or between a stimulus and itself (within odor) was then taken as the average of these correlations. Separation of population firing rate vectors in neural space was the average within-odor correlation minus the average across-odor correlation. Bootstrap significance testing on pseudo-population correlation measures were determined with the null hypothesis that mean differences between OB and PCx could be generated from a homogenous population containing cells from both regions. We combined OB and PCx responses and sampled this distribution with replacement to match the recorded population size of OB and PCx cells and then computed the mean difference between correlation separation for these populations 1000 times. The p-value was then the fraction of null differences that were more extreme than the empirical mean difference.

Noise correlations

Noise correlations were defined as the correlation in across-trial variability around the mean stimulus-evoked response between simultaneously recorded cell-pairs and were calculated according to the methods described in Miura et al., 2012. Briefly, spike count responses within a specified temporal window around inhalation were z-scored across trials for a given stimulus. Then, correlation coefficients were computed across trials for each stimulus and for each cell-pair in each simultaneously recorded population. These values were first averaged within an experiment across cell-pairs and stimuli and then averaged across experiments.

Population decoding analysis

Odor classification accuracy based on population responses was measured using a linear multi-class SVM classifier with 10-fold cross-validation (LIBLINEAR, solver 4 (Crammer and Singer method), https://www.csie.ntu.edu.tw/~cjlin/liblinear/ (Fan et al., 2008). Responses to six distinct monomolecular odorants presented at 0.3% v/v were used as the training and testing data. The feature vectors for spike count classification were the spike counts for each cell during the 480 ms following inhalation. For decoding across states, the classifier was trained on all awake trials and accuracy was assessed across all anesthetized trials.

Classification accuracy was measured across multiple pseudopopulation sizes. To estimate the mean accuracy at each size, we constructed pseudopopulations by randomly subsampling from the entire recorded population 200 times. Bootstrap confidence intervals on the mean accuracy for each population size were estimated by sampling with replacement from the distribution of accuracies and re-computing the mean 1000 times.

Local field potential analysis

30 kHz raw recordings were downsampled to 1 kHz and filtered between 0.05–500 Hz with a 3-pole Butterworth filter. Average power spectra and OB-PCx coherence were obtained using the multi-taper spectrum utilities in the Chronux package (www.chronux.org). For visualization, LFPs were pre-whitened with an 2nd-order autoregressive filter, and spectrograms were computed in a 30 s sliding window in 3 s steps.

Spontaneous activity and pairwise phase consistency

Spontaneous activity was computed using spikes that occurred >3 s after odor offset and >2 s before odor onset. The relationship of each unit’s spiking to the ongoing respiratory oscillation was quantified using pairwise phase consistency (PPC) as in Vinck et al., 2010. Each spike was assigned a phase by interpolation between inhalation (0 degrees) and exhalation (180 degrees). Each spike was then treated as a unit vector and PPC was taken as the average of the dot products of all pairs of spikes.

Demixed PCA overlap analysis

Demixed PCA projections were computed using the Machens lab, MATLAB implementation (https://github.com/machenslab/dPCA) (Kobak et al., 2016bKobak et al., 2016a). Demixed PCA finds separate reduced-rank regression solutions that best reconstruct population responses averaged over each experimenter-defined factor. This is conceptually similar but not identical to performing PCA on, for instance, the stimulus-averaged population response separately from a PCA performed on state-averaged population responses. Trial-by-trial pseudopopulation responses were constructed as above from all recorded cells using a 500 ms response window. PCs were then computed on Stimulus, State, and Stimulus X State interaction marginalizations. Trial responses were projected onto the top 3 Stimulus components and the trial-mean and covariance of these 3D projections for each odor in each state were determined. These measures were then used to compute an overlap score for each odor across states according to Matusita’s measure (Minami and Shimizu, 1999; Matusita, 1966).

ξ=2p2Σ114Σ214Σ1+Σ212exp-14(μ1-μ2)T(Σ1+Σ2)-1(μ1-μ2)

Optogenetic tagging of Ntng1+ cells

In 5/12 experiments with Ntng1-Cre mice, putative semilunar cells were tagged by Cre-dependent expression of Jaws (a red-shifted variant of halorhodopsin), and in 5/12 experiments with Ntng1-Cre mice, putative semilunar cells were tagged by Cre-dependent expression of Archaeorhodopsin (Arch). Cells expressing either inhibitory opsin show rapid onset and deep suppression during exposure to 532 nm (Arch) or 640 nm (Jaws) light. Normal odor response recordings were made with an optic fiber attached probe, and 20 1 s laser pulses were delivered at the end of the experiment. Two criteria were applied to identify Arch+ cells: 1) p<0.0001 in rank-sum test of spiking in the 1 s preceding and during laser stimulation, 2) median last-spike latency during laser pulse <0.01 ms (shown as red dots). Additionally, cells with overall firing rates < 0.175 Hz or a peak-trough time <0.35 ms in their average waveform were excluded from classification either as Arch+ or Arch- cells.

2/12 experiments used excitation with channelrhodopsin for opto-tagging. Cells were stimulated using ~200, 1 ms pulses of 473 nm light, delivered at 4 Hz at the end of the experiment. Two criteria were applied to identify ChR2+ cells: 1) p-value<0.001 in Stimulus-Associated spike Latency Test (Kvitsiani et al., 2013), 2) latency-to-peak response in PSTH <0.003 ms. Cells with a peak-trough time <0.35 in their average waveform were excluded from classification as ChR2+ or ChR2-.

Neural distance-to-stable and regression analysis

For each experiment, population firing rate vectors for each odor trial were constructed from spiking during the 500 ms after odor inhalation or from a control period 2000 to 1500 ms before odor inhalation. Mean Euclidean distance from each trial population response to the last five awake odor responses (stable trials) was normalized to the mean Euclidean distance between stable trials to estimate population distance-to-stable. For the main analysis of PCx and OB population trajectories, PCx data were combined from simultaneous OB-PCx recordings and Ntng1-Cre recordings, and three OB-PCx experiments which had <15 awake trials prior to k/x injection were omitted (n = 22 PCx recordings; n = 9 OB recordings).

To examine the influence of sniffing, overall firing rates, and odor experience over trials on the stabilization of population responses, we fit a multiple linear regression model using the fitlm function in the MATLAB Statistics and Machine Learning toolbox with sniff rate, firing rate, and trials, as predictors of distance-to-stable. Sniff rate was estimated as the reciprocal of the first breath duration following odor presentation. Population firing rates were taken as the mean response across neurons on each trial. All predictors were z-scored to allow comparison of the magnitude of regression coefficients. To further visualize stabilization across trials that is independent of sniffing and overall firing rates, we fit a reduced model including only sniff rate and firing rate as predictors, and examined the residuals of this model as a function of trials.

Cross-time decoding and temporal stability

To assess stability of odor representations across short timescales, we trained and tested an SVM classifier (as above) on different time bins following odor offset. Smooth pseudopopulation responses were built from kernel density functions (200 ms kernel) aligned to the first inhalation after odor offset, and the classifier was trained and tested on each combination of time points up to 1.5 s before and 4 s after inhalation. Classification accuracy was assessed with leave-one-out cross-validation and no time bins from the test trial were included in the training data for each fold.

Acknowledgements

We thank J Beck and G Field for helpful discussions and A Fleischmann, L Glickfeld, S Lisberger and A Schaefer for helpful comments on earlier versions of the manuscript. This work was supported by grants from NIDCD (DC015525 and DC016782) and the Edward Mallinckrodt Jr. Foundation.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Kevin M Franks, Email: franks@neuro.duke.edu.

Stephen Liberles, Harvard Medical School, United States.

Laura L Colgin, University of Texas at Austin, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders DC015525 to Kevin M Franks.

  • National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders DC016782 to Kevin M Franks.

  • National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke U19 NS112953 to Kevin M Franks.

  • National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke NS 077986 to Fan Wang.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology.

Data curation, Investigation, Visualization.

Resources.

Resources.

Conceptualization, Resources, Data curation, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Project administration.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: This study was performed in strict accordance with the recommendations in the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals of the National Institutes of Health. All experimental protocols were approved by Duke University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (protocols A177-18-07). All surgeries were performed under either ketamine/xylazine and/or isofluorane anesthesia, and every effort was made to minimize suffering.

Additional files

Transparent reporting form

Data availability

Raw data and code are available on Dryad (https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.n2z34tmtj) and GitHub (https://github.com/FranksLab/eLife2020-recurrents-stabilize; copy archived at https://github.com/elifesciences-publications/eLife2020-recurrents-stabilize), respectively.

The following dataset was generated:

Bolding KA, Nagappan S, Han BX, Wang F, Franks KM. 2020. Data from: Recurrent circuitry is required to stabilize piriform cortex odor representations across brain states. Dryad Digital Repository.

References

  1. Ambros-Ingerson J, Granger R, Lynch G. Simulation of paleocortex performs hierarchical clustering. Science. 1990;247:1344–1348. doi: 10.1126/science.2315702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Barkai E, Bergman RE, Horwitz G, Hasselmo ME. Modulation of associative memory function in a biophysical simulation of rat piriform cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1994;72:659–677. doi: 10.1152/jn.1994.72.2.659. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Barnes DC, Hofacer RD, Zaman AR, Rennaker RL, Wilson DA. Olfactory perceptual stability and discrimination. Nature Neuroscience. 2008;11:1378–1380. doi: 10.1038/nn.2217. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Barwich AS. A critique of olfactory objects. Frontiers in Psychology. 2019;10:1337. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01337. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bathellier B, Buhl DL, Accolla R, Carleton A. Dynamic ensemble odor coding in the mammalian olfactory bulb: sensory information at different timescales. Neuron. 2008;57:586–598. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.02.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bolding KA, Franks KM. Complementary codes for odor identity and intensity in olfactory cortex. eLife. 2017;6:e22630. doi: 10.7554/eLife.22630. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bolding KA, Franks KM. Recurrent cortical circuits implement concentration-invariant odor coding. Science. 2018;361:eaat6904. doi: 10.1126/science.aat6904. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Buonviso N, Chaput MA, Berthommier F. Temporal pattern analyses in pairs of neighboring mitral cells. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1992;68:417–424. doi: 10.1152/jn.1992.68.2.417. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Carrillo-Reid L, Yang W, Bando Y, Peterka DS, Yuste R. Imprinting and recalling cortical ensembles. Science. 2016;353:691–694. doi: 10.1126/science.aaf7560. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Chapuis J, Wilson DA. Bidirectional plasticity of cortical pattern recognition and behavioral sensory acuity. Nature Neuroscience. 2012;15:155–161. doi: 10.1038/nn.2966. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Chaudhuri R, Fiete I. Computational principles of memory. Nature Neuroscience. 2016;19:394–403. doi: 10.1038/nn.4237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Chong E, Moroni M, Wilson C, Shoham S, Panzeri S, Rinberg D. Manipulating synthetic optogenetic odors reveals the coding logic of olfactory perception. Science. 2020;368:eaba2357. doi: 10.1126/science.aba2357. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Choy JMC, Suzuki N, Shima Y, Budisantoso T, Nelson SB, Bekkers JM. Optogenetic mapping of intracortical circuits originating from semilunar cells in the piriform cortex. Cerebral Cortex. 2015;31C:bhv258. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv258. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Chu MW, Li WL, Komiyama T. Balancing the robustness and efficiency of odor representations during learning. Neuron. 2016;92:174–186. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.09.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. D'amour JA, Froemke RC. Inhibitory and excitatory spike-timing-dependent plasticity in the auditory cortex. Neuron. 2015;86:514–528. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.03.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Dhawale AK, Hagiwara A, Bhalla US, Murthy VN, Albeanu DF. Non-redundant odor coding by sister mitral cells revealed by light addressable glomeruli in the mouse. Nature Neuroscience. 2010;13:1404–1412. doi: 10.1038/nn.2673. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Diodato A, Ruinart de Brimont M, Yim YS, Derian N, Perrin S, Pouch J, Klatzmann D, Garel S, Choi GB, Fleischmann A. Molecular signatures of neural connectivity in the olfactory cortex. Nature Communications. 2016;7:12238. doi: 10.1038/ncomms12238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Eichenbaum H. Hippocampus: cognitive processes and neural representations that underlie declarative memory. Neuron. 2004;44:109–120. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.08.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Fan RE, Chang KW, Hsieh CJ, Wang XR, Lin CJ. LIBLINEAR: a library for large linear classification. Journal of Machine Learning Research. 2008;9:1871–1874. [Google Scholar]
  20. Fontanini A, Bower JM. Variable coupling between olfactory system activity and respiration in ketamine/xylazine anesthetized rats. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2005;93:3573–3581. doi: 10.1152/jn.01320.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Frank T, Mönig NR, Satou C, Higashijima SI, Friedrich RW. Associative conditioning remaps odor representations and modifies inhibition in a higher olfactory brain area. Nature Neuroscience. 2019;22:1844–1856. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0495-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Franks KM, Russo MJ, Sosulski DL, Mulligan AA, Siegelbaum SA, Axel R. Recurrent circuitry dynamically shapes the activation of piriform cortex. Neuron. 2011;72:49–56. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.08.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Franks KM, Isaacson JS. Synapse-specific downregulation of NMDA receptors by early experience: a critical period for plasticity of sensory input to olfactory cortex. Neuron. 2005;47:101–114. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.05.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Friedrich RW, Laurent G. Dynamic optimization of odor representations by slow temporal patterning of mitral cell activity. Science. 2001;291:889–894. doi: 10.1126/science.291.5505.889. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Ghosh S, Larson SD, Hefzi H, Marnoy Z, Cutforth T, Dokka K, Baldwin KK. Sensory maps in the olfactory cortex defined by long-range viral tracing of single neurons. Nature. 2011;472:217–220. doi: 10.1038/nature09945. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Gottfried JA. Central mechanisms of odour object perception. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2010;11:628–641. doi: 10.1038/nrn2883. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Guzman SJ, Schlögl A, Frotscher M, Jonas P. Synaptic mechanisms of pattern completion in the hippocampal CA3 network. Science. 2016;353:1117–1123. doi: 10.1126/science.aaf1836. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Haberly LB. Parallel-distributed processing in olfactory cortex: new insights from morphological and physiological analysis of neuronal circuitry. Chemical Senses. 2001;26:551–576. doi: 10.1093/chemse/26.5.551. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Haberly LB, Bower JM. Olfactory cortex: model circuit for study of associative memory? Trends in Neurosciences. 1989;12:258–264. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(89)90025-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Hagiwara A, Pal SK, Sato TF, Wienisch M, Murthy VN. Optophysiological analysis of associational circuits in the olfactory cortex. Frontiers in Neural Circuits. 2012;6:18. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2012.00018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Hasselmo ME, Wilson MA, Anderson BP, Bower JM. Associative memory function in piriform (olfactory) cortex: computational modeling and neuropharmacology. Cold Spring Harbor Symposia on Quantitative Biology; 1990. pp. 599–610. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Hasselmo ME, Linster C, Patil M, Ma D, Cekic M. Noradrenergic suppression of synaptic transmission may influence cortical signal-to-noise ratio. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1997;77:3326–3339. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.77.6.3326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Hasselmo ME, Barkai E. Cholinergic modulation of activity-dependent synaptic plasticity in the piriform cortex and associative memory function in a network biophysical simulation. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1995;15:6592–6604. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-10-06592.1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Hasselmo ME, Bower JM. Cholinergic suppression specific to intrinsic not afferent fiber synapses in rat piriform (olfactory) cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1992;67:1222–1229. doi: 10.1152/jn.1992.67.5.1222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Hopfield JJ. Neural networks and physical systems with emergent collective computational abilities. PNAS. 1982;79:2554–2558. doi: 10.1073/pnas.79.8.2554. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Howard JD, Gottfried JA. Configural and elemental coding of natural odor mixture components in the human brain. Neuron. 2014;84:857–869. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.10.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Inagaki HK, Fontolan L, Romani S, Svoboda K. Discrete attractor dynamics underlies persistent activity in the frontal cortex. Nature. 2019;566:212–217. doi: 10.1038/s41586-019-0919-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Jacobson GA, Rupprecht P, Friedrich RW. Experience-Dependent plasticity of odor representations in the telencephalon of zebrafish. Current Biology. 2018;28:1–14. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.11.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Johnson DM, Illig KR, Behan M, Haberly LB. New features of connectivity in piriform cortex visualized by intracellular injection of pyramidal cells suggest that "primary" olfactory cortex functions like "association" cortex in other sensory systems. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2000;20:6974–6982. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-18-06974.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Jung MW, Larson J, Lynch G. Long-term potentiation of monosynaptic EPSPs in rat piriform cortex in vitro. Synapse. 1990;6:279–283. doi: 10.1002/syn.890060307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Kanter ED, Haberly LB. NMDA-dependent induction of long-term potentiation in afferent and association fiber systems of piriform cortex in vitro. Brain Research. 1990;525:175–179. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(90)91337-G. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Kato HK, Chu MW, Isaacson JS, Komiyama T. Dynamic sensory representations in the olfactory bulb: modulation by wakefulness and experience. Neuron. 2012;76:962–975. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Kobak D, Brendel W, Constantinidis C, Feierstein CE, Kepecs A, Mainen ZF, Qi XL, Romo R, Uchida N, Machens CK. Demixed principal component analysis of neural population data. eLife. 2016a;5:e10989. doi: 10.7554/eLife.10989. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Kobak D, Brendel W, Machens C. Demixed Principal Component Analysis. 0.1Github. 2016b doi: 10.7554/eLife.10989. https://github.com/machenslab/dPCA [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  45. Koldaeva A, Schaefer AT, Fukunaga I. Rapid task-dependent tuning of the mouse olfactory bulb. eLife. 2019;8:e43558. doi: 10.7554/eLife.43558. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Kollo M, Schmaltz A, Abdelhamid M, Fukunaga I, Schaefer AT. 'Silent' mitral cells dominate odor responses in the olfactory bulb of awake mice. Nature Neuroscience. 2014;17:1313–1315. doi: 10.1038/nn.3768. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Kvitsiani D, Ranade S, Hangya B, Taniguchi H, Huang JZ, Kepecs A. Distinct behavioural and network correlates of two interneuron types in prefrontal cortex. Nature. 2013;498:363–366. doi: 10.1038/nature12176. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Li A, Zhang L, Liu M, Gong L, Liu Q, Xu F. Effects of different anesthetics on oscillations in the rat olfactory bulb. Journal of the American Association for Laboratory Animal Science : JAALAS. 2012;51:458–463. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Luskin MB, Price JL. The topographic organization of associational fibers of the olfactory system in the rat, including centrifugal fibers to the olfactory bulb. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1983;216:264–291. doi: 10.1002/cne.902160305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Marr D. Simple memory: a theory for archicortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 1971;262:23–81. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1971.0078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Matusita K. A Distance and Related Statistics in Multivariate Analysis. Academic Press; 1966. [Google Scholar]
  52. Mazo C, Grimaud J, Shima Y, Murthy VN, Lau CG. Distinct projection patterns of different classes of layer 2 principal neurons in the olfactory cortex. Scientific Reports. 2017;7:8282. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-08331-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Minami M, Shimizu K. Estimation of similarity measure for multivariate normal distributions. Environmental and Ecological Statistics. 1999;6:229–248. doi: 10.1023/A:1009678412953. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  54. Miura K, Mainen ZF, Uchida N. Odor representations in olfactory cortex: distributed rate coding and decorrelated population activity. Neuron. 2012;74:1087–1098. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.04.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Miyamichi K, Amat F, Moussavi F, Wang C, Wickersham I, Wall NR, Taniguchi H, Tasic B, Huang ZJ, He Z, Callaway EM, Horowitz MA, Luo L. Cortical representations of olfactory input by trans-synaptic tracing. Nature. 2011;472:191–196. doi: 10.1038/nature09714. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Murakami M, Kashiwadani H, Kirino Y, Mori K. State-dependent sensory gating in olfactory cortex. Neuron. 2005;46:285–296. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.02.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Nakazawa K, Quirk MC, Chitwood RA, Watanabe M, Yeckel MF, Sun LD, Kato A, Carr CA, Johnston D, Wilson MA, Tonegawa S. Requirement for hippocampal CA3 NMDA receptors in associative memory recall. Science. 2002;297:211–218. doi: 10.1126/science.1071795. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Neunuebel JP, Knierim JJ. CA3 retrieves coherent representations from degraded input: direct evidence for CA3 pattern completion and dentate gyrus pattern separation. Neuron. 2014;81:416–427. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.11.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Pashkovski SL, Iurilli G, Brann D, Chicharro D, Drummey K, Franks K, Panzeri S, Datta SR. Structure and flexibility in cortical representations of odour space. Nature. 2020;26 doi: 10.1038/s41586-020-2451-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Poo C, Isaacson JS. An early critical period for long-term plasticity and structural modification of sensory synapses in olfactory cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:7553–7558. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1786-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Rinberg D, Koulakov A, Gelperin A. Sparse odor coding in awake behaving mice. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:8857–8865. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0884-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Roland B, Deneux T, Franks KM, Bathellier B, Fleischmann A. Odor identity coding by distributed ensembles of neurons in the mouse olfactory cortex. eLife. 2017;6:e26337. doi: 10.7554/eLife.26337. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Rolls ET. The mechanisms for pattern completion and pattern separation in the Hippocampus. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience. 2013;7:74. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00074. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Shusterman R, Smear MC, Koulakov AA, Rinberg D. Precise olfactory responses tile the sniff cycle. Nature Neuroscience. 2011;14:1039–1044. doi: 10.1038/nn.2877. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Sosulski DL, Bloom ML, Cutforth T, Axel R, Datta SR. Distinct representations of olfactory information in different cortical centres. Nature. 2011;472:213–216. doi: 10.1038/nature09868. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Stettler DD, Axel R. Representations of odor in the piriform cortex. Neuron. 2009;63:854–864. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.09.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Suzuki N, Bekkers JM. Neural coding by two classes of principal cells in the mouse piriform cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:11938–11947. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3473-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Suzuki N, Bekkers JM. Two layers of synaptic processing by principal neurons in piriform cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:2156–2166. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5430-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Suzuki M, Gottlieb J. Distinct neural mechanisms of distractor suppression in the frontal and parietal lobe. Nature Neuroscience. 2013;16:98–104. doi: 10.1038/nn.3282. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Tang AC, Hasselmo ME. Selective suppression of intrinsic but not afferent fiber synaptic transmission by baclofen in the piriform (olfactory) cortex. Brain Research. 1994;659:75–81. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(94)90865-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Treves A, Rolls ET. Computational analysis of the role of the Hippocampus in memory. Hippocampus. 1994;4:374–391. doi: 10.1002/hipo.450040319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Vinck M, van Wingerden M, Womelsdorf T, Fries P, Pennartz CM. The pairwise phase consistency: a bias-free measure of rhythmic neuronal synchronization. NeuroImage. 2010;51:112–122. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Vogels TP, Sprekeler H, Zenke F, Clopath C, Gerstner W. Inhibitory plasticity balances excitation and inhibition in sensory pathways and memory networks. Science. 2011;334:1569–1573. doi: 10.1126/science.1211095. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Wills TJ, Lever C, Cacucci F, Burgess N, O'Keefe J. Attractor dynamics in the hippocampal representation of the local environment. Science. 2005;308:873–876. doi: 10.1126/science.1108905. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Wilson DA, Sullivan RM. Cortical processing of odor objects. Neuron. 2011;72:506–519. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.10.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Yger P, Spampinato GLB, Esposito E, Lefebvre B, Deny S, Gardella C, Stimberg M, Jetter F, Zeck G, Picaud S, Duebel J, Marre O. A spike sorting toolbox for up to thousands of electrodes validated with ground truth recordings in vitro and in vivo. eLife. 2018;7:34518. doi: 10.7554/eLife.34518. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Yger P. SpyKING CIRCUS. 0.5b3Github. 2018 https://github.com/spyking-circus
  78. Yokoi M, Mori K, Nakanishi S. Refinement of odor molecule tuning by dendrodendritic synaptic inhibition in the olfactory bulb. PNAS. 1995;92:3371–3375. doi: 10.1073/pnas.92.8.3371. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Stephen Liberles1

In the interests of transparency, eLife publishes the most substantive revision requests and the accompanying author responses.

Acceptance summary:

This study reveals a role for recurrent neural circuits in stabilizing odor representations in the olfactory cortex. The authors observe that anesthesia degrades odor representations in the olfactory bulb but not in the olfactory cortex. They then use genetic approaches to perturb synaptic function and reveal a role for recurrent circuits in maintaining odor-evoked cortical responses. The study advances mechanistic understanding of networks involved in olfactory processing. The study is also likely to be of interest to others who do not study olfaction but are generally interested in recurrent networks.

Decision letter after peer review:

Thank you for submitting your article "Recurrent circuitry is required to stabilize piriform cortex odor representations across brain states" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by Laura Colgin as the Senior Editor, a Reviewing Editor, and three reviewers. The reviewers have opted to remain anonymous.

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

The reviewers thought the conclusions of the manuscript were potentially exciting, but that further analysis of existing data was required to validate key claims. The two key points of debate concerned (1) the validity of claims related to the attractor network and recurrent circuitry and (2) the potentially gross effects of anesthesia. Essential revisions related to these general points, and other major and minor points, are detailed below.

Summary:

The authors demonstrate that while anesthesia disrupts olfactory bulb (OB) representations of odors profoundly, the representations of odors between wakefulness and anesthesia remain relatively stable in the piriform cortex (PCx). Also, silencing output of PCx excitatory neurons reduced separability of odor representations in PCx. Odor responses in PCx were initially unstable but approached a stable pattern after repeated odor stimulation. The authors conclude that these features of the PCx resemble aspects of self-stabilizing / attractor type of networks and are a result of the recurrent circuitry in PCx, given that odor representations of optogenetically identified semilunar cells in PCx (cells that lack recurrent collaterals) degrade under anesthesia, whereas odor representations of optogenetically identified pyramidal cells in PCx (cells that receive collateral inputs) are largely preserved across states. Reviewers felt that the use of anaesthetized vs. awake states as the key comparison for degraded vs. preserved OB input was a weakness, and that it would be very compelling in the future to explicitly provide degraded input (e.g. optogenetically or through odor mixtures missing components). However, they felt that the results were still compelling, and they agreed that such experiments were well beyond the scope of the present study. Suggestions for future experiments that were provided by the reviewers are mentioned below for the authors' information, although they were ultimately not considered to be Essential Revisions.

Essential revisions:

1) One major concern is that the changes in odor-induced activity underlying the observed effects of anesthesia on correlations and separability of odor representations are not described in detail. A more detailed analysis of these effects is important to better understand how anesthesia changes odor representations in the OB, and why odor representations in PCx are not affected in the same way. Several specific comments related to this general point are provided below:

a) Reviewers were surprised by the poor odor representation in the anaesthetized OB, given results of prior studies. There is a long list of papers demonstrating seemingly robust, odor dependent responses in the OB of anaesthetized rodents (e.g., Yokoi et al., 1999, Dhawale, 2010, Buonviso et al., 1992, admittedly none of them directly demonstrating decoding ability). For a long time, the general conclusion was that waking up mice and rats resulted in MC activity becoming less odor- and more context-sensitive, less robust etc (e.g. RInberg et al., 2006, Kay and Laurent, 1999). While a lot of the apparent lack of reliable coding in the awake state was traced back to variability in sniff recording, the puzzle remains as to why Bolding et al. see such poor odor representation in the anaesthetized OB that even an ensemble of 150 (putative Mitral and Tufted) cells does not allow for efficient classification of a panel of 6 quite different monomolecular odors (Figure 1N). Can the authors pinpoint why they observe such poor odor representation in the OB? Are they using e.g. much lower concentration than previous studies? Is it the nature of the odor panel? As the lack of accurate representation in the OB is the basis for their use of the anaesthetized input to PCX as a proxy for a corrupted input, this observation deserves more explanation / exploration. In other words, the authors come to very different conclusions about the relationship between awake and anesthetized activity in the OB than what has been presented in the literature. The authors discuss this at length in subsection “State-dependent odor responsivity”. Nevertheless, this is a puzzling and possibly important point and more emphasis in the abstract and Results section might be needed to highlight this appropriately – and possibly help to resolve contradicting conclusions of published papers. Importantly, the statement in subsection “Odor responsivity is state-dependent in OB but not PCx” that their findings are consistent with previous studies is misleading. The authors need to explicitly discuss these inconsistencies at that point in the text.

b) It is important to better understand the changes in odor responses that cause high correlations and low separability in the OB under k/x anesthesia. One possibility is that there is a pattern of odor-independent "background activity" from a subset of cells that respond to all (most) odors. The horizontal "band-like" structure in the OB correlation matrix in Figure 2D hints at this. It is also possible that tuning curves of all cells become more similar. The origin of high correlations need to be analyzed in more detail; this is important to understand the effects of anesthesia and the difference between brain states, and to interpret effects in piriform cortex.

c) The authors should also analyze correlations in spontaneous activity across populations and how they change under anesthesia. Does anesthesia induce a pattern of spontaneous activity that persists during odor responses?

d) It is possible that anesthesia has little effect on the odor response itself but induces a global activity pattern that is additive to the odor response and present in all odor responses (and perhaps also in spontaneous activity). This possibility is quite different from the possibility that odor responses change globally and become more overlapping. The authors should try to distinguish between these (and other possibilities) and consider the consequences for the interpretation of k/x effects in PCx.

e) How do excitatory and inhibitory responses change under anesthesia? Are both affected equally? How often did responses change character (from excitatory to inhibitory or vice versa)? What was the correlation between responses of single neurons to odors (tuning curves) in the awake and anesthetized state?

f) The mean spontaneous firing rates in the OB and PCx in awake state and during k/x should be reported explicitly in the manuscript. Effects of k/x on spontaneous firing should be compared to those observed previously, e.g. by Rinberg et al., 2006.

2) General points were raised regarding the authors' claims regarding attractor networks and recurrent circuitry. Specific points related to this are provided below.

a) Do the authors actually show that it is "recurrent circuitry" (implying recurrent excitation as in most models of attractor networks)? All their work is certainly consistent with recurrent excitation underlying the observed features of an attractor network. They show that SL cells – that are thought to lack input from SL or PYR – do not show the same "pattern completion" stability features and that blocking the output of pyramidal cells similarly blocks "pattern completion". Both are consistent with recurrent excitation in PCX playing a key role – however, it is also consistent with simply "needing piriform cortical PYR input" (recurrent or feedback or feedforward) or e.g. a key role for FBI (as in Bolding and Franks, 2018, maybe technically also recurrency, albeit not in the sense it is usually used in the description of pattern completion and attractor networks). This needs to be actively discussed, and the Abstract and Discussion section should be phrased more carefully to better reflect results (e.g., "Recurrent connections are required…" "We find that PCx is an attractor network by virtue of its recurrent activity"). For example, in the first sentence of Discussion section: this is too strong a statement. It is possible that the underlying observations also involve other brain areas. PCx shows signatures of an attractor network, but it is not fully resolved whether it is an attractor network in the classical mechanistic sense.

b) The adjustment of sniff frequency (i.e. the initial rapid sniffing mentioned in subsection “Odor responsivity is state-dependent in OB but not PCx” and excluded for analysis) should be an excellent example of "altered/corrupted input to PCX". How does PCX deal with such altered OB inputs? Wouldn't one expect some kind of stabilization acting here as well?

c) Stabilization is an important phenomenon that is somewhat underrepresented in the manuscript. It would be interesting to see some further analyses of this phenomenon (e.g. is stabilization observed for all odors? Does the time course of stabilization (number of trials) depend on the odor? How does it depend on inter-trial interval?).

d) Correlations in PCx increase globally under anesthesia; the effect looks in first approximation as if a global "background correlation" is added to the pattern of correlations in the awake state. How can a "background correlations" arise in an attractor network? This is not consistent with the classical view of canonical attractor networks and should be discussed. Can the increased background correlation be explained by the observed changes in activity in the OB?

3) The results and their interpretation by the authors suggest that PCx neurons should show correlated variability of activity across trials. This is inconsistent with results reported previously by Miura et al., 2012, who report low noise correlations. The authors should perform a similar analysis of noise correlations and compare their results to those of Miura et al. The open comparison of present results to those of previous studies would be appreciated in the Discussion section, even when there are discrepancies. The authors may consider including a comparison to Miura et al., here; this paper is currently not discussed in any detail.

4 Figure 4H: the fraction of preserved responses in both types of neurons is lower than the fraction of preserved responses pooled over all neurons in Figure 2C. Is this due to inter-animal variation? Please explain.

5) How well can odors be decoded from OB activity after stimulus offset? The analysis in Figure 6 should also be performed for OB responses.

6) It would be useful to see not only peak firing rates and onset of robust vs non-robust responses but also their full time courses. The authors could for example plot an overlay of all robust responses and their mean, same for non-robust responses.

7) Figure 2A,B show that there are fewer robust cells in the OB, but there are also fewer non-robust responses (black and blue). This figure suggests that there are more cell-odor-pairs responding to odors in PCx than in the OB, which is at odds with Figure 1F-I. Please explain. What is the response index? Please define. The fraction of preserved responses should be reported both ways (wrt awake responses and wrt to k/x responses), and wrt responses in either state.

8) The argumentation why OB output is not degraded under anesthesia when TeLC is expressed ipsilaterally is not consistent with the simplified interpretation that PCx representations do not degrade under anesthesia (in normal animals not expressing TeLC). In normal animals, the back-projection to the OB does not appear to degrade representations in the OB (results from awake animals). Then, removing back-projections should not improve representations in TeLC animals, unless there is a complex interaction between back-projections and anesthesia that is not understood.

9) The usage of "degraded" is somewhat unclear. It may be useful to use a more precise description here because "degraded" may be expected to mean "more noisy", which is expected to decrease correlations. This semantic issue is linked to the question of whether the increase in correlation can be explained by a decrease in noise. This should be addressed by an analysis of noise and intertrial variation of responses.

[Editors' note: further revisions were suggested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

Thank you for resubmitting your article "Recurrent circuitry is required to stabilize piriform cortex odor representations across brain states" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by Laura Colgin as the Senior Editor, a Reviewing Editor, and three reviewers. The reviewers have opted to remain anonymous.

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

We would like to draw your attention to changes in our revision policy that we have made in response to COVID-19 (https://elifesciences.org/articles/57162). Specifically, when editors judge that a submitted work as a whole belongs in eLife but that some conclusions require a modest amount of additional new data, as they do with your paper, we are asking that the manuscript be revised to either limit claims to those supported by data in hand, or to explicitly state that the relevant conclusions require additional supporting data.

Our expectation is that the authors will eventually carry out the additional experiments and report on how they affect the relevant conclusions either in a preprint on bioRxiv or medRxiv, or if appropriate, as a Research Advance in eLife, either of which would be linked to the original paper.

The reviewers felt that the revised manuscript was substantially improved, and after discussion, there was one point in particular that reviewers felt was not properly addressed. Many of the conclusions related to attractor network behavior assume, implicitly, that anesthesia "degrades" odor discrimination because it decreases signal to noise, but the data do not seem to support this view. An alternative possibility is that anesthesia results in some common mode of activity such that activity becomes less odor-specific. This should be addressable by some simple analysis, and discussed appropriately in a few sentences.

Reviewer comments are provided in full below in case they are helpful.

Reviewer #1:

The authors have responded to the comments. Overall, I feel that with the current changes the paper does provide reasonable support for its claims.

1) Anesthesia and correlations/separability of odor representations.

The authors do a detailed comparison with prior work and better place their observations in context with, and in contrast to, what has previously been seen.

1b). Poor odor representation in anaesthetized OB.

Again, the authors now discuss the prior work and acknowledge where there are differences.

2) Attractor networks.

The authors have put in further discussions and tempered their interpretations.

They have not addressed some other suggestions about testing for attractor behavior in the network based on sharp transitions between response states triggered by removal of components from a multi-component odor, and similar manipulations. As this is substantial work, it is fair enough to defer this for future study.

3) Noise correlations:

The authors do show that their results on long (120 ms) windows match earlier work from Miura, but point out that shorter windows exhibit some (weak) correlations.

Reviewer #2:

In the revision Bolding and Franks discuss all major points raised. They can't really resolve the key issues but lay them open rather transparently and discuss them clearly. Regarding the discrepancy of their recordings under anesthesia from the published literature they discuss potential reasons for the discrepancy and compare their results with the literature in detail. The very first statement they make is still somewhat misleading and it should rather be pointed out that their results differ from the literature upfront (l 51: "Anesthesia induced pronounced changes in respiration patterns, oscillatory activity and spontaneous spiking in both OB and PCx (Figure 1B and C; Figure 1—figure supplement 1), consistent with previous work ").

Subsection “Odor responsivity is state-dependent in OB but not PCx” are somewhat clumsy and confusing – they emphasize that reliable responses can be seen in their data, yet decoding is substantially impaired. This needs rephrasing pointing out that reliable responses are somewhat rare in their data compared to the published literature.

Reviewer #3:

The authors have addressed some but not all comments.

Comments:

1) The most important issue is that the authors have not elucidated how anesthesia "degraded" odor representations in the OB. They observe that across-trial correlations go up, which tells us that a decrease in signal or an increase in noise cannot easily account for the effect of anesthesia. Their explanation that "degraded" decoding is due to reduced responses is inconsistent with the observation that all correlations increase (if activity is weakened and the signal to noise ratio decreases, correlations should converge to zero. If signal to noise ratio increases, which is possible when anesthesia reduces baseline activity, the difference between within and across odor correlations should increase, not decrease). An obvious possibility is that odor stimulation evokes a non-specific activity pattern under anesthesia that is independent of odor identity. This needs to be tested, as was put forward under main point 1 in the previous review. The authors have addressed this point incompletely because they only tested whether a background pattern present during spontaneous activity could increase correlations during odor responses (by subtracting mean spontaneous activity), but they did not test whether a non-specific "background" pattern is evoked by odor stimulation. There are many obvious ways to test this possibility (identifying a common mode in patterns by PCA or so and subtracting it out, or asking whether it is always the same cells that are responsible for different across-odor correlations, etc). As pointed out before, this issue is important because understanding the effects in the OB – which are obviously not a simple noise increase – is essential to understand the effects in PCx. Most predictions related to attractor networks consider inputs with some form of "random" noise, but anesthesia does not seem to induce "random" noise.

2) Along the same lines: Subtracting background rates did have a clear effect on accuracy (probably significant; please test) that is somewhat understated in the manuscript. It increases with the number of cells, as expected for a "background pattern". Is this effect enhanced during odor stimulation?

3) Along the same lines: the horizontal bands in Figure 2D, left panel, have not been explained. Please test whether they come from a background pattern (that may be observed only during odor stimulation, or enhanced by odor stimulation). If not, what else could it be?

4) The components in activity patterns responsible for increased across-odor correlations in piriform cortex under anesthesia have not been identified. These across-odor correlations are not predicted by attractor networks. They seem to argue against attractor networks. The authors should try to identify the sources for these correlations and address in the Discussion section whether or not they are consistent with an attractor network hypothesis.

5) The question whether the gradual stabilization of odor responses is due to plasticity of responses in the OB or PCx can be addressed in TeLC-PC mice: if the site of pasticity is PCx, stabilization of responses in the OB should be absent. Figure 6K seems to support this possibility but showing more data for TeLC-PC mice (as in Figure 6G) could clarify this question further.

6) In Emx1-Cre mice, decoding seems to be even improved under k/x as compared to awake (compare Figure 4N to 4M), both in control and in TeLC expressing mice. Please explain.

7) The conclusion that robust responses are actively amplified within PCx appears to strong because it is based only on a rather crude analysis of peak firing rates.

8) It would be interesting to see correlation matrices such as Figure 1K that are generated only from SL or PYR cells.

eLife. 2020 Jul 14;9:e53125. doi: 10.7554/eLife.53125.sa2

Author response


Summary:

The authors demonstrate that while anesthesia disrupts olfactory bulb (OB) representations of odors profoundly, the representations of odors between wakefulness and anesthesia remain relatively stable in the piriform cortex (PCx). Also, silencing output of PCx excitatory neurons reduced separability of odor representations in PCx. Odor responses in PCx were initially unstable but approached a stable pattern after repeated odor stimulation. The authors conclude that these features of the PCx resemble aspects of self-stabilizing / attractor type of networks and are a result of the recurrent circuitry in PCx, given that odor representations of optogenetically identified semilunar cells in PCx (cells that lack recurrent collaterals) degrade under anesthesia, whereas odor representations of optogenetically identified pyramidal cells in PCx (cells that receive collateral inputs) are largely preserved across states. Reviewers felt that the use of anaesthetized vs. awake states as the key comparison for degraded vs. preserved OB input was a weakness, and that it would be very compelling in the future to explicitly provide degraded input (e.g. optogenetically or through odor mixtures missing components). However, they felt that the results were still compelling, and they agreed that such experiments were well beyond the scope of the present study. Suggestions for future experiments that were provided by the reviewers are mentioned below for the authors' information, although they were ultimately not considered to be Essential Revisions.

Essential revisions:

1) One major concern is that the changes in odor-induced activity underlying the observed effects of anesthesia on correlations and separability of odor representations are not described in detail. A more detailed analysis of these effects is important to better understand how anesthesia changes odor representations in the OB, and why odor representations in PCx are not affected in the same way. Several specific comments related to this general point are provided below:

In response to this important point we carried out additional analyses on changes in OB responses across states to further examine the origin of degraded OB odor responses and representations under anesthesia.

1) Reviewers suggested that a strong ‘background’ activity pattern could dominate anesthetized activity, making it difficult to resolve distinct odor-evoked responses. To examine this, we looked at spontaneous activity correlations (sniff-sniff population vector correlations for activity during the pre-odor baseline). Results were similar to the across-odor correlationsin Figure 1L. That is, activity in both OB and PCx is more correlated across sniffs under anesthesia (Figure 1—figure supplement 4A). If increased background correlations explain poor separation of odor representations, we would expect an equal or even greater decrement in PCx separation and decoding. The fact that background correlations increase in both regions, but the decreased response separation is only observed in OB suggest that background correlations are not the primary explanatory factor.

2) Nevertheless, to further examine the effect of background activity on OB response classification, we found the mean rate during baseline for all cells and subtracted that from the odor responses before classifying, thus eliminating any consistent ‘background pattern’. OB decoding is only very slightly improved by this procedure (Figure 1—figure supplement 4B).

3) A naïve prediction of the background activity hypothesis is that baseline activity increases under anesthesia, especially in OB cells that lose their responses. However, we cannot find evidence for this in our data. Instead, our data suggest that OB cells lose their responses under anesthesia because their odor-evoked firing rates decrease (Figure 1—figure supplement 4B and 4B). We therefore think that OB decoding accuracy is poor under anesthesia because there are fewer/weaker OB responses.

4) OB decoding could also fail because responses become more variable under anesthesia. However, on average across all OB responses, the Fano factors are nearly the same in awake and k/x trials (awake: 2.72 ± 2.11, k/x: 2.43 ± 1.27; Figure 1—figure supplement 4E). We further broke this analysis down to examine Fano factors for awake-only or robust responses. There is some increased variability in the awake-only responses that could contribute to poor decoding, but maintained responses are even more reliable (Figure 1—figure supplement 4F). The bigger effect and probably greatest contributor to the decoding issue is simply the weaker and sparser responses under anesthesia.

a) Reviewers were surprised by the poor odor representation in the anaesthetized OB, given results of prior studies. There is a long list of papers demonstrating seemingly robust, odor dependent responses in the OB of anaesthetized rodents (e.g., Yokoi et al., 1999, Dhawale, 2010, Buonviso et al., 1992, admittedly none of them directly demonstrating decoding ability). For a long time, the general conclusion was that waking up mice and rats resulted in MC activity becoming less odor- and more context-sensitive, less robust etc (e.g. RInberg et al., 2006, Kay and Laurent, 1999).

We were also initially surprised by this result, given the existing literature. Nevertheless, we reliably observed that the anesthetic cocktail we injected reduced both responsivity and classification accuracy for responses in olfactory bulb. We wish to emphasize that our results, showing degraded OB responses and decoding in ketamine-xylazine (k/x) anesthetized mice should not be interpreted as a general statement about the effects of different anesthetics in different systems or even about k/x anesthesia effects across anesthesia protocols. Thus, we are not comfortable making direct comparisons to Yokoi et al., 1995, who used urethane anesthesia in rabbits; to Dhawale et al., 2010 who used a 40% lower dose than ours but sustained administration of the cocktail for >10 hours; or to Buonviso et al., 1992, where equitesin was used in anesthetized rats. We can indeed observe reliable odor responses amongst a portion of the mitral/tufted cells in these recordings (~5% of cell-odor pairs) and agree that it is possible to do so in a variety of anesthetized preparations. We have revised the text describing our observed anesthesia effects to clarify that we do not interpret this as a general result reflecting all forms of anesthesia.

While a lot of the apparent lack of reliable coding in the awake state was traced back to variability in sniff recording, the puzzle remains as to why Bolding et al. see such poor odor representation in the anaesthetized OB that even an ensemble of 150 (putative Mitral and Tufted) cells does not allow for efficient classification of a panel of 6 quite different monomolecular odors (Figure 1N). Can the authors pinpoint why they observe such poor odor representation in the OB? Are they using e.g. much lower concentration than previous studies? Is it the nature of the odor panel? As the lack of accurate representation in the OB is the basis for their use of the anaesthetized input to PCX as a proxy for a corrupted input, this observation deserves more explanation / exploration. In other words, the authors come to very different conclusions about the relationship between awake and anesthetized activity in the OB than what has been presented in the literature. The authors discuss this at length in subsection “State-dependent odor responsivity”.

As noted by the reviewer, we address this discrepancy and our efforts to explain the difference with studies directly comparable to ours (Kato et al., 2012, Rinberg et al., 2006, Kollo et al., 2014) in the Discussion section. We have added a concise description of odor stimuli across these studies. The choice of stimuli is unlikely to explain the differences we observed. Absent a direct comparison of results obtained from these diverse activity monitoring techniques in the same experimental preparation, we cannot confidently ascribe the differences in our anesthesia results to a specific experimental factor beyond the factors we proposed in the Discussion section.

Nevertheless, this is a puzzling and possibly important point and more emphasis in the abstract and Results section might be needed to highlight this appropriately – and possibly help to resolve contradicting conclusions of published papers. Importantly, the statement in subsection “Odor responsivity is state-dependent in OB but not PCx” that their findings are consistent with previous studies is misleading. The authors need to explicitly discuss these inconsistencies at that point in the text.

The point is well-taken and we have modified the text accordingly.

b) It is important to better understand the changes in odor responses that cause high correlations and low separability in the OB under k/x anesthesia. One possibility is that there is a pattern of odor-independent "background activity" from a subset of cells that respond to all (most) odors. The horizontal "band-like" structure in the OB correlation matrix in Figure 2D hints at this. It is also possible that tuning curves of all cells become more similar. The origin of high correlations need to be analyzed in more detail; this is important to understand the effects of anesthesia and the difference between brain states, and to interpret effects in piriform cortex.

See above (response to Essential revision 1).

c) The authors should also analyze correlations in spontaneous activity across populations and how they change under anesthesia. Does anesthesia induce a pattern of spontaneous activity that persists during odor responses?

See above (response to Essential revision 1).

d) It is possible that anesthesia has little effect on the odor response itself but induces a global activity pattern that is additive to the odor response and present in all odor responses (and perhaps also in spontaneous activity). This possibility is quite different from the possibility that odor responses change globally and become more overlapping. The authors should try to distinguish between these (and other possibilities) and consider the consequences for the interpretation of k/x effects in PCx.

See above (response to Essential revision 1).

e) How do excitatory and inhibitory responses change under anesthesia? Are both affected equally? How often did responses change character (from excitatory to inhibitory or vice versa)? What was the correlation between responses of single neurons to odors (tuning curves) in the awake and anesthetized state?

We have now included a description of changes in significant suppressed odor responses in OB and PCx under anesthesia in Figure 1(F and H), and include statistics for response sign-switching and tuning curve correlations in the main text.

f) The mean spontaneous firing rates in the OB and PCx in awake state and during k/x should be reported explicitly in the manuscript. Effects of k/x on spontaneous firing should be compared to those observed previously, e.g. by Rinberg et al., 2006.

We now include these measures in the Results section and compare our results to those from previous studies.

2) General points were raised regarding the authors' claims regarding attractor networks and recurrent circuitry. Specific points related to this are provided below.

a) Do the authors actually show that it is "recurrent circuitry" (implying recurrent excitation as in most models of attractor networks)? All their work is certainly consistent with recurrent excitation underlying the observed features of an attractor network. They show that SL cells – that are thought to lack input from SL or PYR – do not show the same "pattern completion" stability features and that blocking the output of pyramidal cells similarly blocks "pattern completion". Both are consistent with recurrent excitation in PCX playing a key role – however, it is also consistent with simply "needing piriform cortical PYR input" (recurrent or feedback or feedforward) or e.g. a key role for FBI (as in Bolding and Franks, 2018, maybe technically also recurrency, albeit not in the sense it is usually used in the description of pattern completion and attractor networks). This needs to be actively discussed, and the Abstract and Discussion section should be phrased more carefully to better reflect results (e.g., "Recurrent connections are required…" "We find that PCx is an attractor network by virtue of its recurrent activity"). For example, in the first sentence of Discussion section: this is too strong a statement. It is possible that the underlying observations also involve other brain areas. PCx shows signatures of an attractor network, but it is not fully resolved whether it is an attractor network in the classical mechanistic sense.

The reviewer is saying that our results are consistent with recurrent circuitry being required for attractor dynamics and pattern completion, but also consistent with the simpler idea that piriform simply “needs PYR input”. Given that, by far, the biggest distinguishing feature of SLs vs. PYRs is that SLs are driven almost exclusively by OB input and PYRs are driven by both direct OB input and input from other SLs and PYRs, we think these two statements are practically, if not formally, equivalent. Moreover, our observation that robust and awake-only OB responses have similar magnitudes, but robust responses are stronger than awake-only responses in PCx indicates that these responses are actively amplified in PCx, which is difficult to explain as a function solely of recurrent feedback excitation, as in Bolding and Franks, 2018. We therefore think that we are justified is stating that our results show that recurrent circuitry is required to support these phenomena. Nevertheless, we do take the reviewers’ point that more baroque explanations, such as loops from PCx to other brain areas and back again, could also be involved and we have added a section in the Discussion section that describes the limitations in the interpretation, especially of the TeLC expression experiments. We have also tempered some the particularly strongly worded phrases.

b) The adjustment of sniff frequency (i.e. the initial rapid sniffing mentioned in subsection “Odor responsivity is state-dependent in OB but not PCx” and excluded for analysis) should be an excellent example of "altered/corrupted input to PCX". How does PCX deal with such altered OB inputs? Wouldn't one expect some kind of stabilization acting here as well?

In principle, an analysis of sniff frequency could be useful. However, this analysis is confounded in these experiments because, in addition to having very few (~3-5) variable sniff trials, variable sniffing occurs primarily in the earliest trials, when we predict the initial formation of the odor template occurs. In our working model, this ‘learned’ representation is the basis for pattern stabilization. We therefore predict that odor responses would not be especially stable in these early trials, before the pattern is ‘learned’.

c) Stabilization is an important phenomenon that is somewhat underrepresented in the manuscript. It would be interesting to see some further analyses of this phenomenon (e.g. is stabilization observed for all odors? Does the time course of stabilization (number of trials) depend on the odor? How does it depend on inter-trial interval?).

We now show stabilization traces for all population-odor pairs and show that the phenomenon is roughly equivalent with all odors. Inter-trial interval was held stable in the current experiments. We agree that examining the dependence on inter-trial interval would be interesting, and varying this parameter will be an important element of future experiments, but is beyond the scope of this study.

d) Correlations in PCx increase globally under anesthesia; the effect looks in first approximation as if a global "background correlation" is added to the pattern of correlations in the awake state. How can a "background correlations" arise in an attractor network? This is not consistent with the classical view of canonical attractor networks and should be discussed. Can the increased background correlation be explained by the observed changes in activity in the OB?

See above (response to Essential revision 1).

3) The results and their interpretation by the authors suggest that PCx neurons should show correlated variability of activity across trials. This is inconsistent with results reported previously by Miura et al., 2012, who report low noise correlations. The authors should perform a similar analysis of noise correlations and compare their results to those of Miura et al. The open comparison of present results to those of previous studies would be appreciated in the Discussion section, even when there are discrepancies. The authors may consider including a comparison to Miura et al., here; this paper is currently not discussed in any detail.

We have now performed an extensive analysis of noise correlations and describe the outcomes of this analysis in the Results section (Figure 5). This is a valuable addition to our paper and we thank the reviewers for this suggestion. As we now describe in the text, our data are consistent, both with Miura and with the idea that an attractor network should shows some weak correlated activity across trials. We find near-zero noise correlation when using a 120 ms time window, as Miura et al., did, but we do find weak noise correlations when using time windows more in line with synaptic time constants. This warrants a more thorough investigation because there are numerous factors that can contribute to noise correlations, including spiking rate and oscillatory activity, but that is beyond the scope of this paper.

4 Figure 4H: the fraction of preserved responses in both types of neurons is lower than the fraction of preserved responses pooled over all neurons in Figure 2C. Is this due to inter-animal variation? Please explain.

Yes, we noticed this too. This difference almost certainly depends on animal strain, as we observed that Ntng1-Cre mice were considerably more sensitive to k/x. We have elaborated on this observation in Figure 4—figure supplement 2, which shows that the rate of response preservation can be partially predicted by the maintenance of spontaneous activity and that overall rates are more sensitive to anesthesia in a portion of Ntng1-Cre recordings. We consider the comparisons of simultaneously recorded cell populations in the same genetic background to be most reliable.

5) How well can odors be decoded from OB activity after stimulus offset? The analysis in Figure 6 should also be performed for OB responses.

We have included this analysis in (now) Figure 7.

6) It would be useful to see not only peak firing rates and onset of robust vs non-robust responses but also their full time courses. The authors could for example plot an overlay of all robust responses and their mean, same for non-robust responses.

We have included the suggested illustration in Figure 3.

7) Figure 2A,B show that there are fewer robust cells in the OB, but there are also fewer non-robust responses (black and blue). This figure suggests that there are more cell-odor-pairs responding to odors in PCx than in the OB, which is at odds with Figure 1F-I. Please explain. What is the response index? Please define. The fraction of preserved responses should be reported both ways (wrt awake responses and wrt to k/x responses), and wrt responses in either state.

We thank the reviewers for pointing out this inconsistency. These examples were generated from an older version of analysis code that included the earliest awake trials and longer periods of anesthesia, neither of which could be described as a steady-state. Our final analysis was limited to late awake trials after odor-evoked sniffing had subsided and a shorter, stable period of anesthesia that excluded early trials after induction and later trials during which recovery may add to response variability. Examples and summary data are now generated using the same procedure and we have replaced the examples in Figure 2A and B with more representative data. We now define the response index in the methods. We now also report preserved responses both ways, though we argue the awake-to-k/x preservation is the relevant measure for our analysis.

8) The argumentation why OB output is not degraded under anesthesia when TeLC is expressed ipsilaterally is not consistent with the simplified interpretation that PCx representations do not degrade under anesthesia (in normal animals not expressing TeLC). In normal animals, the back-projection to the OB does not appear to degrade representations in the OB (results from awake animals). Then, removing back-projections should not improve representations in TeLC animals, unless there is a complex interaction between back-projections and anesthesia that is not understood.

Our previous presentation was unnecessarily confusing and our explanation was garbled, and we apologize for this. We have simplified the presentation of the OB decoding figure with TeLC (Figure 4—figure supplement 3A) to show OB decoding accuracy in awake vs. anesthetized states in OBs ipsi- and contralateral to TeLC on the same graph. As can now be seen clearly, although OB decoding is substantially better ipsilateral to TeLC-PCx in both awake and anesthetized conditions, OB decoding does decrease under anesthesia in both cases. Note however that ipsilateral OB decoding under anesthesia is still actually slightly better than decoding in awake contralateral control OB. We think that this stronger OB output improves PCX decoding and helps compensate for the loss of recurrent circuitry in both awake and anesthetized conditions.

9) The usage of "degraded" is somewhat unclear. It may be useful to use a more precise description here because "degraded" may be expected to mean "more noisy", which is expected to decrease correlations. This semantic issue is linked to the question of whether the increase in correlation can be explained by a decrease in noise. This should be addressed by an analysis of noise and intertrial variation of responses.

We have revised the text to clarify our usage of this term, and we include an analysis of response variability in Figure 1—figure supplement 4.

[Editors' note: further revisions were suggested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

The reviewers felt that the revised manuscript was substantially improved, and after discussion, there was one point in particular that reviewers felt was not properly addressed. Many of the conclusions related to attractor network behavior assume, implicitly, that anesthesia "degrades" odor discrimination because it decreases signal to noise, but the data do not seem to support this view. An alternative possibility is that anesthesia results in some common mode of activity such that activity becomes less odor-specific. This should be addressable by some simple analysis, and discussed appropriately in a few sentences.

As we understand this, there are two concerns about the background pattern.

First, there is the nature of the origin of increased correlations in OB under anesthesia. We have now included a new set of analyses that show that there is indeed an activity pattern – a population of weak but reliable and non-selective odor responses, i.e. a background pattern – which drives higher correlations under anesthesia. This occurs in both OB and PCx but is much more pronounced in OB. This was apparent in Figure 1l, but we have now explicitly demonstrated this effect in additional materials (Figure 1—figure supplement 5A-F), in which we subtracted the average odorevoked responses and find that residual responses are no longer correlated across odors.

Second, and most crucially, there is the question about whether this background pattern explains our main result: i.e. the preservation of PCx odor representations despite degraded OB representations. We stand by our original contention that OB odor responses degrade because some strong, odor-specific responses drop off under k/x anesthesia. We believe that we have demonstrated with multiple methods that this the degradation of OB odor representations is not dependent on the presence or absence of a background pattern in anesthetized OB:

- First, the ‘correlation separation’ shown in Figure 1m measures within-odor correlations that are above and beyond those expected from any across-odor correlation. Here, we directly subtracted off the across-odor correlation that is produced by background patterns in this analysis and show that separation is higher in PCx than OB.

- Second, any background pattern that is common across stimuli would not survive training of our SVM classifier, or any classifier. Cells that respond to all odors would have 0-weights in determining the classification boundary. The degraded classifier performance in OB under anesthesia (Figure 1N) demonstrates that it is difficult to find cells that respond consistently differentially to at least some subset of our odors.

- Third, in the dPCA analysis in Figure 2G and H), we explicitly removed overall statedependent patterns from our data. These are defined on the odor-evoked responses, so they effectively do remove any state-specific background patterns. Here again, we see PCx outperforming OB in maintaining similarity between awake and anesthetized responses.

- Finally, in the new material provided, we find that after explicitly subtracting state-specific background patterns before our correlation analyses, there remain weakened within-odor correlations in anesthetized OB in comparison with awake OB or awake or anesthetized PCx (Figure 1—figure supplement 5G,I), demonstrating again that there are less reliable odor representations in OB under anesthesia, while they remain more reliable in PCx.

In summary, two separate phenomena that occur under k/x anesthesia. First, there is an odor-dependent but not odor-specific component that increases within- and across-odor response correlations in both OB and PCx. On top of this, there is a pronounced degradation of OB responses due to a decrease in firing of some strong odor-specific responses that PCx is nevertheless able to accommodate.

Reviewer comments are provided in full below in case they are helpful.

Reviewer #3):

The authors have addressed some but not all comments.

Comments:

1) The most important issue is that the authors have not elucidated how anesthesia "degraded" odor representations in the OB. They observe that across-trial correlations go up, which tells us that a decrease in signal or an increase in noise cannot easily account for the effect of anesthesia. Their explanation that "degraded" decoding is due to reduced responses is inconsistent with the observation that all correlations increase (if activity is weakened and the signal to noise ratio decreases, correlations should converge to zero. If signal to noise ratio increases, which is possible when anesthesia reduces baseline activity, the difference between within and across odor correlations should increase, not decrease). An obvious possibility is that odor stimulation evokes a non-specific activity pattern under anesthesia that is independent of odor identity. This needs to be tested, as was put forward under main point 1 in the previous review. The authors have addressed this point incompletely because they only tested whether a background pattern present during spontaneous activity could increase correlations during odor responses (by subtracting mean spontaneous activity), but they did not test whether a non-specific "background" pattern is evoked by odor stimulation. There are many obvious ways to test this possibility (identifying a common mode in patterns by PCA or so and subtracting it out, or asking whether it is always the same cells that are responsible for different across-odor correlations, etc). As pointed out before, this issue is important because understanding the effects in the OB – which are obviously not a simple noise increase – is essential to understand the effects in PCx. Most predictions related to attractor networks consider inputs with some form of "random" noise, but anesthesia does not seem to induce "random" noise.

As stated above, this concern is two-pronged: first, why do response correlations increase, and second, do these increases in response correlations underlie/undermine our main conclusion that OB responses degrade under k/x anesthesia while PCx responses are comparatively robust?

Indeed, we do find that weak but reliable and non-specific odor responses emerge under k/x anesthesia, and these are especially pronounced in OB. These non-specific responses increase both within- and across-odor response correlations (Figure 1—figure supplement 5A-F).

However, although this background pattern increases correlations, it does not explain or trivialize the observation that PCx responses remain relatively robust under k/x anesthesia while OB responses degrade. Any ‘background pattern’ would be manifested in the average response across all odors. To test whether a non-specific ‘background’ pattern is the cause of the degraded representations in OB under anesthesia, we therefore computed and subtracted this average for both OB and PCx in awake and k/x trials, and then re-examined our correlation results using the residuals. This new analysis removes all non-odor-specific patterns of activity and clearly shows that although responses in OB degrade under k/x anesthesia while responses in PCx remain robust within- vs. across-odor (Figure 1—figure supplement 5G-I).

As an aside, there is no requirement in the definition of a fixed-point attractor that the attractor be perturbed by random noise. The attractor dynamics simply demand that specific points in state space are stable while others are not, such that, when the system is initiated to any unstable state – be it a random state or one caused by the superimposition of a stimulus-specific pattern with a background pattern – it will rapidly evolve toward a stable state and then remain there until perturbed again. Well-known examples of putative attractor-like dynamics do not assume ‘random’ noise. Rather, they often consist of morphing experiments, such as those suggested by reviewer 1, where an input pattern is subtly perturbed and recovers the original rather than an intermediate representation. Our study has much in common with Neunuebel et al., 2014, wherein DG and CA3 are monitored simultaneously and a cue manipulation is used to ‘degrade’ DG inputs to CA3. The population vector representations they observe at each degree position in their radial maze are analogous to the distinct odors in our experiments, and across-degree correlations can be quite high in DG under ‘degraded’ conditions, as are across-odor conditions in OB in our anesthetized condition. Yet CA3 population vectors maintain well-separated representations for each degree-bin despite degraded DG input. This is very like the sense in which we are interpreting these data as attractor-like, and we are cautious in our description because the system fails to perfectly recover this hypothetical stable state in our experiments. Nevertheless, the system moves in the direction of the known awake odor-specific state, consistent with a partial pattern recovery.

2) Along the same lines: Subtracting background rates did have a clear effect on accuracy (probably significant; please test) that is somewhat understated in the manuscript. It increases with the number of cells, as expected for a "background pattern". Is this effect enhanced during odor stimulation?

Subtracting either baseline firing or average odor-evoked firing patterns appeared to increase decoding accuracy slightly, however neither of these improvements was significant, as determined by confidence intervals between raw vs. subtracted performance across multiple permutations (Figure 1—figure supplement 7).

3) Along the same lines: the horizontal bands in Figure 2D, left panel, have not been explained. Please test whether they come from a background pattern (that may be observed only during odor stimulation, or enhanced by odor stimulation). If not, what else could it be?

The horizontal bands in Figure 2D occur when some awake trials have either more (light bands) or less (dark bands) correlated activity with all anesthetized trials. For the most part, these are due to some ‘early’ awake trials, which have had stronger responses (i.e. higher firing rates. Note that for these analyses we compared trials 6-13. Note also that PCx responses stabilized rapidly (i.e. within ~5 trials) whereas OB responses exhibited a slower and more modest trial-dependent adaptation (see Figure 6), and so many of the dark bands especially evident in OB confusion matrix can be explained by slightly higher overall firing rates during early OB trials. We have explicated this briefly in the Figure 2 legend.

4) The components in activity patterns responsible for increased across-odor correlations in piriform cortex under anesthesia have not been identified. These across-odor correlations are not predicted by attractor networks. They seem to argue against attractor networks. The authors should try to identify the sources for these correlations and address in the Discussion section whether or not they are consistent with an attractor network hypothesis.

Like OB, the increase in response correlations is due to a small subset of highly reliable, nonspecific odor responses. However, these are considerably less pronounced in PCx than OB, as evidenced by the greater separability of PCx vs. OB responses in awake vs. k/x trials.

5) The question whether the gradual stabilization of odor responses is due to plasticity of responses in the OB or PCx can be addressed in TeLC-PC mice: if the site of pasticity is PCx, stabilization of responses in the OB should be absent. Figure 6K seems to support this possibility but showing more data for TeLC-PC mice (as in Figure 6G) could clarify this question further.

We were able to analyze and observe stabilization across trials at the population level for individual PCx experiments because each PCx recording yielded a substantial number of simultaneously recorded neurons. Yields in OB recordings were often much smaller and would produce a very noisy and difficult to interpret outcome tracking the dynamics of stabilization on an individual experiment basis. These experiments would thus require an additional series of experiments in which we first inject TeLC into PCx and then recorded from multiple probes targeted to ipsi- and contralateral OB before and after k/x administration. While this is an interesting point, it is a little tangential, and we are presently months away from being able to even start these experiments. We therefore think that this question is beyond the scope of this study.

6) In Emx1-Cre mice, decoding seems to be even improved under k/x as compared to awake (compare Figure 4N to 4M), both in control and in TeLC expressing mice. Please explain.

Yes, the reviewer is correct, and we cannot really explain this observation. We have noticed previously (Bolding and Franks, 2018) that after TeLC expression, responses in the so-called contralateral control hemisphere are similar yet slightly different (i.e. ‘worse’) than true, unperturbed control PCx. We suspect this could be due to the modest contralateral projections, either directly from PCx or indirectly via AON or OB.

7) The conclusion that robust responses are actively amplified within PCx appears to strong because it is based only on a rather crude analysis of peak firing rates.

We disagree with the reviewer – peak firing rates (more accurately, spike counts within the first sniff) seem the most appropriate measure for whether or not a response is robust (see also Miura et al., 2012; Bolding and Franks, 2017).

8) It would be interesting to see correlation matrices such as Figure 1K that are generated only from SL or PYR cells.

Yes, however a detailed characterization of responses in SL vs. PYR cells is ongoing in the lab, will be described in another manuscript by Ms. Nagappan, and is not immediately relevant to the present study.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Data Citations

    1. Bolding KA, Nagappan S, Han BX, Wang F, Franks KM. 2020. Data from: Recurrent circuitry is required to stabilize piriform cortex odor representations across brain states. Dryad Digital Repository. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]

    Supplementary Materials

    Transparent reporting form

    Data Availability Statement

    Raw data and code are available on Dryad (https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.n2z34tmtj) and GitHub (https://github.com/FranksLab/eLife2020-recurrents-stabilize; copy archived at https://github.com/elifesciences-publications/eLife2020-recurrents-stabilize), respectively.

    The following dataset was generated:

    Bolding KA, Nagappan S, Han BX, Wang F, Franks KM. 2020. Data from: Recurrent circuitry is required to stabilize piriform cortex odor representations across brain states. Dryad Digital Repository.


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES