Abstract
This report introduces the COVID‐19 Family Environment Scale (CHES), which aims to measure the impact of social distancing due to COVID‐19 on household conflict and cohesion. Existing measures do not capture household experiences relevant to the pandemic, in which families are largely confined to their homes while sharing a life‐threatening situation. Using best practice guidelines, we developed a pool of items and revised them with review by a panel of experts, and cognitive interviewing with community respondents. We administered the CHES by online survey to 3,965 adults. The CHES consists of 15 items for each of two subscales, household conflict (α = .847) and household cohesion (α = .887). Exploratory factor analysis yielded two factors, corresponding to the intended conflict and cohesion items, which accounted for 29% of variance. Confirmatory factor analysis partially supported the 2‐factor model (RMSEA = .057; CFI = .729, TLI = .708, and SRMR = .098). The CHES also contains 25 optional items to describe respondent and household characteristics, and household‐level COVID‐19 exposure. The CHES, publicly available at https://elcentro.sonhs.miami.edu/research/measures-library/covid-19/index.html, provides a tool for measuring the impact of the COVID‐19 pandemic on important determinants of resilience in the face of major stressful events. Further work is needed to address the factor structure and establish validity of the CHES.
Keywords: Family Cohesion, Family Conflict, Family Environment, Family Stress, COVID‐19
Resumen
Este informe presenta la Escala del Entorno Familiar de la COVID‐19 (COVID‐19 Family Environment Scale, CHES), cuyo fin es medir el efecto del distanciamiento social debido a la COVID‐19 en el conflicto familiar y la cohesión familiar. Las herramientas de medición actuales no captan las experiencias familiares relevantes de la pandemia, en la cual las familias están en su gran mayoría confinadas en sus hogares mientras comparten una situación que pone en riesgo la vida. Utilizando pautas de mejores prácticas, desarrollamos un conjunto de ítems y los revisamos con un grupo de expertos, e hicimos entrevistas cognitivas a personas de la comunidad. Administramos la CHES mediante una encuesta en línea a 3965 adultos. La CHES consta de 15 ítems para cada una de dos subescalas, la de conflicto familiar (α = .847), y la de cohesión familiar (α = .887). El análisis factorial exploratorio dio dos factores correspondientes a los ítems planeados de conflicto y cohesión, que representaron el 22 % de la varianza. El análisis factorial confirmatorio respaldó parcialmente el modelo de dos factores (RMSEA = .057; CFI = .729, TLI = .708 y SRMR = .098). La CHES también contiene 25 ítems opcionales para describir las características de los encuestados y la familia, y el nivel de exposición de la familia a la COVID‐19. La CHES, disponible públicamente en https://elcentro.sonhs.miami.edu/research/measures-library/covid-19/index.html ofrece una herramienta para medir el efecto de la pandemia de la COVID‐19 en determinantes importantes de resiliencia ante situaciones de gran estrés. Se necesitan más trabajos para abordar la estructura factorial y establecer la validez de la CHES.
摘要
本文介绍了COVID‐19家庭环境量表(CHES),该量表旨在衡量COVID‐19导致的社交距离对家庭冲突和凝聚力的影响。现有的量表不能反映出与新冠疫情有关的家庭经历,在这种情况下,家庭大多数都被限制在家中,同时又面临着共同的对生命有威胁的情况。我们使用了最佳实践指南,开发了一个项目集合,并由专家小组的审查之后修订,还有在和社区受访者的认知访谈完成之后也会加以修改。我们通过在线调查对3965名成年人进行了CHES测试。CHES包括两个子量表,即家庭冲突(α=0.847)和家庭凝聚力(α=0.887)每个子量表有15个项目。探索性因子分析得出两个因子,对应于预期的冲突和凝聚力项目,占方差的22%。验证性性因子分析部分支持了双因子模型(RMSEA = .057;CFI = .729,TLI = .708,SRMR = .098)。CHES还包含25个可选项目,用于描述受访者和家庭特征,以及家庭层面的暴露在COVID‐19情形下的相关情况。CHES的公开网址是,它提供了一种工具,用于衡量COVID-19大流行对重大压力事件中,对复原力有着重要决定性的因素的影响。本研究提出有必要进行进一步的研究专门去针对因素结构和如何建立CHES量表的效度。
The pandemic of coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID‐19) is a unique life‐threatening worldwide event. In addition to the stress that comes from the risk of illness and death, families are suffering economic losses, insecurity about having adequate stores of food and supplies, separation from loved ones, a barrage of frightening news, and uncertainty about the future. While there are behaviors that can reduce risk of infection, many aspects of the pandemic are beyond the control of individuals, thus increasing the stressful nature of the event.
Social distancing, defined by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention in reference to COVID‐19 as the practice whereby individuals “maintain a distance of at least six feet (2 m) from others [and] stay out of crowded places,” has been widely adopted as a recommended, and sometimes government‐mandated, means of containing the pandemic (Centers for Disease Control & Prevention, 2020). A recent review of the literature undertaken in response to the COVID‐19 crisis revealed that the psychological effects of quarantine include anxiety, depression, and post‐traumatic stress (Brooks, et al., 2020). Although social distancing is less restrictive of movement than a quarantine, both share the elements of life disruption, fear, uncertainty, and financial repercussions.
Social distancing requires household members to be together for extended time periods, often with limited personal space. Furthermore, the activities that individuals typically would do in other spaces are taking place in the home. With most schools closed, children are schooling at home, and parents are responsible for supervising the children’s education and recreation. Many adults are also working from home, often without designated quiet space. Others have been laid off from jobs, with corresponding financial strains, or are required to report to jobs that expose their households to risk of contagion. In short, the COVID‐19 pandemic sets up a home situation with tremendous potential for generating conflict as household member spend nearly unlimited hours together in a limited physical space while confronting a stressful event. The situation also provides opportunities for increased cohesion, as members of the household share more time together, confront challenges together, and get up‐close glimpses of each other’s roles normally conducted outside of the home.
The stress process model (Pearlin, 1999), which provides a framework for understanding the interplay of stress‐related factors, contains four major categories of dynamically interrelated constructs: (1) sources of stress, including chronic or acute stressors, (2) proximal outcomes, (3) resources that can moderate the consequences of stress, and (4) outcomes or manifestations of stress. All of these processes can occur at the individual level or at a larger, social/contextual level. The role of family functioning in the stress process can be considered within any of the categories of constructs: as a source of stress, as is the case for overburdened family caregivers; as a proximal outcome, as in the case of marital conflict that arises from economic pressures; as a resource, such as family cohesion that stems from solving problems together; or as an outcome, such as is in divorce (Avison, 2010). Furthermore, although the stress process model is typically applied to individuals, we can conceptualize family‐level stress as an extension of individual stress (Hobfoll & Spielberger, 1992).
Family Conflict and Cohesion
Numerous studies have reported the extent to which family conflict is associated with negative outcomes, particularly among children and adolescents, including poor emotional development (Amir, 2017; Borst, 2015), greater physical symptomatology (Chen, Brody, & Miller, 2017), psychological distress (Constantine & Flores, 2006), problem behaviors (Cummings, Koss, & Davies, 2015), obesity (Frontini, Canavarro, & Moreira, 2018), chronic pain (Voerman, et al., 2015), substance abuse (Cano, et al., 2018; Foxcroft & Lowe, 1995), anxiety disorders (Priest & Denton, 2012), post‐traumatic stress disorder (Norris & Uhl, 1993) (North & Pfefferbaum, 2013), and depression (Cummings, Koss, & Davies, 2015; Essau, 2004). In general, children and adolescents exposed to increased family conflict carry a disproportionate burden of chronic disease into adulthood and report poorer health status and quality of life (Borst, 2015; Chen, Brody, & Miller, 2017; Driscoll, et al., 2015; Repetti, Taylor, & Seeman, 2002; Shonkoff, Boyce, & McEwen, 2009).
In contrast, family cohesion is a protective factor that can mitigate negative outcomes associated with life stress (Hobfoll & Spielberger, 1992). Among youth, family cohesion has been associated with lower levels of substance abuse and sexual risk behavior (Cano, et al., 2018), positive psychological development (Perrino, et al., 2015; Uruk, Sayger, & Cogdal, 2007) more favorable self‐esteem and well‐being (Leidy, Guerra, & Toro, 2010), and more successful relationships in adulthood (Constantine & Flores, 2006). Family cohesion has also been found to be protective among adults facing stress from family caregiving (Francesco, Barsanti, Bongioanni, Bogliolo, & Rossi, 2014; Magaña & Smith, 2006; Yi, 2009), acculturative stress (Joel Wong, Uhm, & Li, 2012; Leong, Park, & Kalibatseva, 2013; Zapata Roblyer, Carlos, Merten, & Gallus, 2017), and living with HIV (Sauceda, Wiebe, Chan, Kutner, & Simoni, 2018).
There is evidence for family conflict and cohesion as important determinants of physical and mental health outcomes for family members that have experienced acute and/or persistent stressful situations, such as financial strains. Numerous studies examined family functioning in the context of the Great Recession of 2007–2009, which, like the current pandemic, was experienced globally, leaving millions unemployed, and disproportionately affected racial and ethnic minorities, and people with lower levels of education and income (Currie, Duque, & Garfinkel, 2015; Grusky, Western, & Wilmer, 2011; Hoynes, Miller, & Schaller, 2012). Forbes and Krueger (2019) analyzed longitudinal surveys before and after the Great Recession and found that individuals who had experienced a greater number of recession impacts (i.e., job loss, increased debt, bankruptcy, housing loss) were at higher odds of reporting increased rates of marital disputes and family conflict, which have been associated with greater incidence of child neglect and abuse (Schneider, Waldfogel, & Brooks‐Gunn, 2017), psychological distress, suicidal behavior, and problematic parental substance abuse (Margerison‐Zilko, Goldman‐Mellor, Falconi, & Downing, 2016). On the other hand, stable and cohesive familial relationships have been seen as protective to child development by contributing to the development of familial resilience (Conger & Conger, 2002; Orthner, Jones‐Sanpei, & Williamson, 2004). After the recession, children that came from cohesive families were less likely to develop externalizing behaviors or to have substance abuse problems (Giuliano & Spilimbergo, 2013; Meadows, Sell, Blinkoff, Williams, & Repcheck, 2015).
There is also evidence for family conflict and cohesion as important determinants of mental health outcomes for children and adults who have experienced natural disasters. Exposure to disaster events can inflate levels of stress and instability, can challenge family functioning, and, as a result, increase family conflict (McDermott & Cobham, 2012). In 2008, Bokszczanin studied a group of 533 schoolchildren and high school students 28 months after a devastating flood in Poland. She found that increased family conflict predicted levels of post‐traumatic stress disorder among the adolescents (Bokszczanin, 2008). However, family cohesion, which plays an important role in a family’s ability to recover and cope with disasters, is often seen as a buffer to family conflict (McDermott & Cobham, 2012). Sprague et al. (2015) found that in the face of disaster, stronger family connectedness and cohesion were associated with positive outcomes in youth. In China, after the Wenchuan earthquake, Cao, Jiang, Li, Lo, and Li (2013) found that family cohesion was significantly correlated with less emotional distress among bereaved adults. Some studies have also found that in the face of disaster, families may initially experience a period of cohesiveness, but chronic stress, poor coping mechanisms, and disaster‐driven displacement can significantly increase family conflict after a disaster (Bonanno, Brewin, Kaniasty, & La Greca, 2010; Le, Tracy, Norris, & Galea, 2013). Such findings highlight the need for longitudinal studies that can track changes in conflict and cohesion at different stages during and after a disaster.
Purpose of the Report
The goal of this report is to describe the development of the COVID‐19 Household Environment Scale (CHES). There are existing and widely used measures of family conflict and cohesion, such as the Family Environment Scale (Moos & Moos, 1994) and Family Life Questionnaire (Foxcroft & Lowe, 1995). However, these measures do not address the unique family and household circumstances that are inherent in the context of social distancing in the COVID‐19 pandemic. Our aspiration with the CHES was to capture the discord and the unity that can result from every‐day experiences in the home within the social distancing context. We sought to measure how the COVID‐19 context would introduce or elevate household‐level conflict, as an indicator of household stress, as well as increase available household‐level cohesion that could mitigate stress. Our intention is to provide a means of measuring change in the social and emotional climate of the home, as a source of family stress (Kazak, 1992), and resources, important determinants of the mental health effects of the pandemic.
Methods
We followed the process outlined by Boateng and colleagues which described best practices for scale development in the behavioral sciences, consisting of three phases: Item Development, Scale Development, and Scale Evaluation, each with various steps (Boateng, Neilands, Frongillo, Melgar‐Quinonez, & Young, 2018). The project was approved by the University of Miami Social and Behavioral Sciences Institutional Review Board. Participants were not compensated.
Phase 1. Item Development
Step 1. Identification of the domains and item generation
The domains of interest for the measure were conflict and cohesion, specifically the extent to which these domains had either increased or decreased in the household due to social distancing during the COVID‐19 pandemic. We defined household conflict as “active opposition between family members that can take a wide variety of forms (i.e., verbal, physical, and psychological)” (Marta & Alfieri, 2014), and cohesion as having a “close connected relationship” (Olson, Russel, & Sprenkle, 1983). We used the CDC definition of social distancing described above.
We developed an initial pool of 14 conflict and 14 cohesion items by listing home activities or circumstances likely to be relevant in the context of social distancing. We sought to include content domains and processes established in the research literature as sources of family strain, such as parenting (Creasey & Reese, 1996), work–family balance (Afifi, et al., 2019; French, Dumani, Allen, & Shockley, 2018; Nohe, Meier, Sonntag, & Michel, 2015), caregiving (Brown, Whiting, Kahumoku‐Fessler, Witting, & Jensen, 2020; Cousino & Hazen, 2013), and finances (Camacho‐Thompson, Gillen‐O’Neel, Gonzales, & Fuligni, 2016). We also sought to include sources of family support in the context of stressful life circumstances such as communication and mutuality (Brown, Whiting, Kahumoku‐Fessler, Witting, & Jensen, 2020; Landers, Dimitropoulos, Mendenhall, Kennedy, & Zemanek, 2019), religion and spirituality (Olson, 2018), recreation (Buswell, Zabriskie, Lundberg, & Hawkins, 2012), and family meals (Skeer & Ballard, 2013; Utter, et al., 2013). The specific items in these domains were developed based on family social distancing experiences that were widely described in social media postings and news reports.
In addition to the conflict and cohesion items, we also generated a list of 20 descriptive items including demographic information about individual respondents (e.g., age, country), household characteristics (e.g., number of and age categories of household members), and household‐level COVID‐19 experiences (e.g., whether anyone in the home had been diagnosed or ill from COVID‐19, lost their job, or worked in a job with risk for exposure to the virus). We included these items to provide context for interpreting responses to the conflict and cohesion scales.
We had some additional considerations in designing the items. First, the unit of measurement was an important consideration. We decided to use the term “household” rather than “family” in order to be inclusive of nontraditional household arrangements, and to set a boundary around the relational unit being rated. Further, since we sought to measure household‐level functioning, we decided to ask respondents to describe conflict and cohesion experiences of the household overall, rather than limiting the questions to interactions that the respondent was personally involved in. Second, given the global nature of the pandemic, we intended the CHES to be used beyond the United States and aimed to design indicators that could be universally relevant. For example, rather than ask about household income, which would not provide a common metric across different countries, we included home ownership as an indicator of household socioeconomic status (Johnson, et al., 2013). Third, because our research group has a strong focus on Latinx families, we intended to develop English and Spanish versions simultaneously. Therefore, while we started with an English version, we phrased the items and instructions so that they could be equivalent in both English and Spanish. The measure content was translated to Spanish and back‐translated to English, and then the two English versions (original and back‐translation) were compared for equivalence of meaning.
Step 2. Content validity
We sent the initial item pool to a panel of expert judges to determine content relevance and representation. The panel consisted of 13 doctoral level researchers, representing the disciplines of psychology, nursing, social work, and epidemiology. Areas of research expertise included family process and interventions, stress, chronic illness, family caregiving, mental and substance use disorders, and psychometrics. The members of the expert panel were active in domestic and international research with racial/ethnic and sexual/gender minorities and with populations across the age spectrum.
Communicating via email, we asked the experts to rate the relevance of each item with respect to (1) the specific construct of interest, and (2) the COVID‐19 social distancing experience. Additionally, we asked the experts whether each item was clear, to suggest alternate wording, and to suggest additional items that were important to fully represent the constructs in the context of COVID‐19.
Phase 2: Scale Development
Step 3. Pretesting questions through cognitive interviews
Next, we revised the measure based on the expert feedback and added it to Research Electronic Data Capture (REDCap) in English and Spanish. We then conducted pretests and cognitive interviews to ensure that the CHES could be easily completed via the online interface, and to receive feedback from a small sample intended to represent the target audience. We planned the method for the cognitive interviews to address clarity and relevance that could inform item refinement and English/Spanish equivalence. Interviews were conducted by phone or through a telecommunication platform.
Participation in the cognitive interviews was limited to adults practicing social distancing and living with at least one other person. Participants were emailed informed consent and a link to the survey prior to the interview. Basic demographic information was collected at the start of the interview. Ten cognitive interviews were conducted, seven in English and three in Spanish. Participants included six women and four men, including four who identified as Latinx, three American Indian, one Black, one Asian/Pacific Islander, and one White/non‐Latinx. Mean age of participants was 36.8 (SD = 12.7), educational level ranged from associate degree to doctoral degree, and number in the household ranged from two to six people.
After participants completed each section of the CHES, the interviewer asked probing questions about that section. For example, in the first section, participants were asked whether they or any members of their household were working in a job that they considered to be high risk for contracting COVID‐19. The interviewer asked respondents to define “high risk” and to provide an example of a job that they considered to be high risk. The interviewer also probed based on participant answer choices and asked participants to define, in their own words, some of the conflict and cohesion items. For example, the interviewer asked questions like “We asked you about showing concern or emotional support. In your own words, what does this mean, and what does that look like in your household?” Each cognitive interview lasted approximately 40 minutes and ended with a request for general comments.
Step 4. Survey administration
We administered the CHES via an anonymous survey on REDCap from April 21 to May 5, 2020. Recruitment for the survey was conducted through social media, including ads in Facebook and Instagram and postings in LinkedIn and Twitter. We also asked our professional networks to disseminate announcements about the survey. The recruitment materials, which were in English or Spanish, included a link to the consent document and survey, in which respondents could select the language. Eligibility criteria for the survey were the same as for the cognitive interview, that is, adults who were currently practicing or who had practiced social distancing, and living with at least one other person.
Step 5. Item reduction analysis
Using a classical test theory (CTT) approach, we examined item difficulty and discrimination parameters to determine whether it was necessary to remove any items from the measure. Item‐total correlations were also examined.
Step 6. Extraction of factors
The factorability of the CHES was evaluated using an exploratory factor analysis using maximum likelihood factoring and oblimin rotation with Kaiser normalization using SPSS Version 25 (IBM Corp, 2017). Eigenvalues, scree plots, and rotated factor loadings were evaluated to interpret the results of the analysis.
Phase 3: Scale Evaluation
Step 7. Tests of dimensionality
Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was conducted using Mplus (Muthén & Muthén, 2017). Model fit indices evaluated included chi‐square test of model fit, root mean‐squared error of approximation (RMSEA), Tucker–Lewis index (TLI), comparative fit index (CFI), and the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR). These fit indices were evaluated using the interpretation parameters summarized by Boateng et al., (2018).
Step 8. Tests of reliability
Cronbach’s alpha was used to evaluate the reliability of the subscales identified by the factor analysis. In addition, item‐total correlations were evaluated as well as Cronbach’s alpha if item is deleted to determine whether subscales could be improved if any items were eliminated.
Step 9. Tests of validity
As a preliminary test of validity, we examined the extent to which family functioning was related to various items from Section 1 of the CHES describing respondent and household characteristics and COVID‐19 experience variables. We used appropriate statistical tests based upon level of measurement of the Section 1 variables (Kruskal–Wallis, Mann–Whitney U test, and Pearson correlations).
Results
Phase 1. Item Development
Content validity
Mean, standard deviations, and percent agreement of ratings on relevance and word clarity by the 13 experts were calculated using SPSS. Relevance ratings could range from 1 to 3 points, with higher ratings denoting higher relevance. There were no mean relevance ratings below 2.57 for any section. Ratings of word clarity could range from 1 to 2 points, with lower ratings denoting better clarity. The highest (worst) mean word clarity rating was 1.10. The lowest percent agreement among expert raters was .5.
The experts provided suggestions for revisions to clarify meaning and provide examples of behaviors under some items, for collapsing some of the items with overlapping meaning, and to add items with respect to severity of exposure (e.g., whether any family members died from COVID‐19), conflict (e.g., personal hygiene and use of social media), and cohesion (e.g., physical affection).
Phase 2. Scale Development
Pretesting/cognitive interviewing
Overall, the CHES was well received by those who were interviewed. Participants reported that sections of personal information, household information, and COVID‐19‐specific information were generally clear. Two of our participants came from a background in disability services and suggested that we rephrase our item about children with special needs to “children with a disability or special health care needs.” Every participant interviewed said that the examples provided in the conflict and cohesion items contributed to their understanding.
Participants from both languages indicated that word clarity was satisfactory, but made suggestions for improving some items, resulting in several items being revised. For example, some English speakers told us that they struggled with the definition of the word “argument”; which they interpreted as an “increase of discussion around a subject” rather than its intended definition, which is that of active opposition. As a result, we decided to change the word from “argument” to “conflict.” Based on participant suggestions, we added four items to the section on background information, one item to the conflict subscale, and one item to the cohesion subscale.
The CHES as it emerged from the development process includes two sections. Section 1 contains 25 descriptive items to characterize individual (e.g., age, gender) and household characteristics (e.g., number of people in the home), and household exposure to COVID‐19 (e.g., whether anyone in the home had a positive test for the virus; and job loss due to the pandemic). This section is optional and not scored, and we did not subject it to psychometric analysis. Section 2 contains the family functioning subscales: household conflict and household cohesion, each containing 15 items, asking the respondent about change in conflict, or “togetherness” (cohesion), related to specific household activities during social distancing as compared to the period before social distancing. Items are rated on a scale of 1 (“much less than before social distancing”) to 5 (“much more than before…”).
Scores on the family functioning scales could range from 1 to 5 on each item, with five indicating much more [conflict or cohesion] than before the COVID‐19 pandemic. Possible scores on each subscale range from 15 to 75. To derive a total family functioning score, we reverse‐scored the items on the household conflict subscale and summed the conflict and cohesion subscales. This method of scoring is similar to the Family Environment Scale where scales are combined to create a single‐family interpersonal relationships scale score (Moos & Moos, 1994). Possible scores on family functioning range from 30 to 150, with higher scores indicating better family functioning.
Survey administration
The sample for the survey included 3,965 adults. Respondent demographic information, household characteristics, and household COVID‐19 exposure information are presented in Table 1. Respondents were from 81 different countries in the continents of Africa, Asia, Australia, Europe, North America, and South America.
Table 1.
Item | n (%) | Mean family functioning score (SD) | p |
---|---|---|---|
Respondent information | |||
Full sample | 3,965 (100) | 81.4 (14.0) | |
Gender (n = 3,934) | |||
Male | 286 (7.3) | 80.8 (14.5) | <.0001 a |
Female | 3,603 (91.6) | 81.6 (14.0) | |
Nonbinary | 45 (1.1) | 71.8 (10.6) | |
Language (n = 3,957) | |||
English | 2,405 (60.8) | 80.4 (13.2) | <.0001 b |
Spanish | 1,552 (39.2) | 82.9 (15.0) | |
Age (n = 3,965) | |||
Median age (range) | 35 (18–85) | <.0001 c | |
Education (n = 3,784) | |||
Less than high school completion | 109 (2.9) | 78.4 (13.4) | <.0001 a |
High school graduate | 551 (14.6) | 79.4 (15.3) | |
Some college | 958 (25.3) | 79.6 (14.0) | |
College graduate | 2,166 (57.2) | 82.7 (13.3) | |
Household information | |||
Household composition (n = 3,965) | |||
Mean (SD) | 4.0 (2.1) | <.0001 c | |
Households with children | |||
Yes | 2,550 (65.5) | 83.8 (14.2) | <.0001 b |
No | 1,346 (34.5) | 77.2 (12.6) | |
Households with adults aged 65+ | |||
Yes | 818 (21.5) | 79.7 (14.0) | .0001 b |
No | 2,988 (78.5) | 81.8 (14.0) | |
Household COVID‐19 information | |||
Time social distancing (n = 3,965) | |||
One month or less | 1,108 (27.9) | 82.0 (13.8) | .064 b |
More than one month | 2,857 (72.1) | 81.2 (14.1) | |
Employment affected by COVID‐19 | |||
Stopped working | |||
Yes | 2,161 (56.5) | 81.1 (14.2) | .182 b |
No | 1,661 (43.5) | 81.7 (13.7) | |
Working from home | |||
Yes | 2,800 (71.5) | 82.2 (13.7) | <.0001 b |
No | 1,115 (28.5) | 79.3 (14.6) | |
Working in a job with high risk for COVID‐19 exposure | |||
Yes | 1,458 (36.9) | 80.3 (14.4) | <.0001 b |
No | 2,488 (63.1) | 82.1 (13.7) | |
Working in health care with direct patient contact | |||
Yes | 437 (11.1) | 80.4 (13.9) | .174 b |
No | 3,510 (88.9) | 81.5 (14.0) |
Kruskal–Wallis.
Mann–Whitney U test.
Pearson correlation: r = .152, and .056 for age and household composition, respectively.
Item reduction analysis
Examination of item difficulty, discrimination parameters, and item‐total correlation showed that item reduction was not necessary. Item‐total correlations are presented in Table 2. While few items were found to have item‐total correlations that fell below .40, removal of these items would have resulted in a decrease in reliability. As a result, these items were retained.
Table 2.
Item | M (SD) |
Factor 1 Loading |
Factor 2 Loading |
Item‐total Correlation Factor 1 |
Item‐total Correlation Factor 2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Compared to before the COVID‐19 pandemic, during social distancing, were/are there more conflicts in your household about: | |||||
How to spend leisure time | 2.67 (1.16) | .012 | .465 | .497 | |
Parenting or childcare | 2.60 (1.13) | .074 | .502 | .470 | |
Children’s schoolwork | 2.44 (1.23) | −.091 | .316 | .386 | |
Decisions about how people should take care of their health | 2.45 (1.07) | −.010 | .520 | .522 | |
Decisions about going out | 2.57 (1.38) | −.020 | .291 | .525 | |
Decisions about visitors to the home | 2.75 (1.48) | −.045 | .279 | .460 | |
Home maintenance | 2.30 (1.11) | −.003 | .647 | .527 | |
Personal hygiene | 2.60 (1.08) | .052 | .675 | .468 | |
Food | 2.48 (1.09) | −.016 | .596 | .542 | |
Work or employment | 2.60 (1.21) | −.101 | .239 | .478 | |
Finances | 2.57 (1.15) | .000 | .329 | .487 | |
Privacy or personal space | 2.63 (1.20) | −.101 | .157 | .428 | |
News or social media | 2.27 (1.10) | −.026 | .487 | .529 | |
Alcohol, tobacco, or drug use | 3.07 (1.04) | .006 | .155 | .333 | |
Politics | 2.90 (1.05) | −.050 | .275 | .394 | |
Compared to before the COVID‐19 pandemic, during social distancing, was/is there more togetherness in your household because of: | |||||
Spending leisure time together | 3.86 (1.13) | .810 | .033 | .602 | |
Engaging in conversation | 3.81 (1.09) | .880 | .020 | .695 | |
Doing exercise or fitness activities together | 3.27 (1.22) | .429 | .111 | .462 | |
Getting involved in the children’s education | 3.71 (1.08) | .388 | −.061 | .521 | |
Facing challenges/solving problems together | 3.59 (1.04) | .601 | .010 | .702 | |
Helping each other | 3.71 (1.04) | .576 | −.086 | .701 | |
Sharing household tasks | 3.65 (1.08) | .514 | −.101 | .624 | |
Going on errands together | 2.51 (1.37) | .155 | .052 | .307 | |
Eating together | 3.77 (1.09) | .532 | −.110 | .590 | |
Showing concern/emotional support | 3.62 (1.08) | .551 | −.097 | .673 | |
Showing affection | 3.18 (1.20) | .454 | .084 | .589 | |
Physical intimacy | 2.64 (1.17) | .191 | .188 | .375 | |
Sharing religious or spiritual activities | 3.21 (1.07) | .275 | −.018 | .475 | |
Sharing material resources | 3.25 (0.96) | .349 | −.033 | .561 | |
Helping others together | 3.03 (1.10) | .215 | −.023 | .425 |
Extraction of factors
An exploratory factor analysis (EFA) of the 30 conflict and cohesion items extracted three factors with eigenvalues greater than one. These eigenvalues were 4.92, 3.79, and 1.34, respectively. However, factor 3 did not have a sufficient number of items with loadings >.4 to justify retaining it as a factor. The rotated factor loadings accounted for 29% of the total variance. The rotated factor loadings and the item‐total correlations for factors 1 and 2 are displayed in Table 2. The first 15 items loaded onto factor 2 (conflict), while the next 15 items loaded onto factor 1 (cohesion).
Phase 3: Scale Evaluation
Confirmatory factor analysis
A confirmatory factor analysis was conducted in MPlus with items 1–15 loading onto the conflict scale and items 6–30 loading onto the cohesion scale. When examining fit indices, the RMSEA was indicative of fair fit (RMSEA = .057). However, all other fit indices did not show acceptable fit without model modifications. Specifically, CFI = .729, TLI = .708, and SRMR = .098. Modification indices to improve model fit included the recommendation for three cohesion items to load onto the conflict subscale and two conflict items to load onto the cohesion subscale. Additionally, interitem correlations both within subscales and across subscales were also suggested.
Reliability analysis
The 15 items that measure conflict showed good reliability (Cronbach’s = .842). Item‐total correlations were between .333 and .542. For the 15 items that measure cohesion, good reliability was also established (Cronbach’s = .884) with item‐total correlations ranging from .307 to .702. Additionally, since two‐factor model was only partially supported, we examined the reliability of the full scale with all 30 items included. The reliability for all 30 items from the conflict and cohesion together was Cronbach’s = .808, demonstrating good reliability for the overall measure of household functioning.
Tests of validity
The relationships between the single dimension family functioning score and select items from Section 1 of the CHES are shown in Table 1. Respondent age, language, and education were related to family functioning. Household characteristics and COVID‐19 experiences related to family functioning included household composition (children in the home, persons age 65 or above living in the home), working from home, and working in a high‐exposure job.
Discussion
We have presented the development of the COVID‐19 Household Environment Scale (CHES) which we developed in response to the global pandemic that is endangering and disrupting the lives of millions of people around the world. The household experience of social distancing, in the context of threats to physical and economic survival, sets up a home environment unlike anything the world has experienced, and therefore merits an assessment tool custom‐made for this event. Through our psychometric analysis, with a large sample of people around the world, we found evidence to support reliability of the conflict and cohesion subscales of the CHES as well of the overall family functioning measure.
Family conflict and cohesion are important stress‐related factors that impart risk or can serve as mitigating resources, respectively, for the psychological consequences of COVID‐19 on youth and adults. High levels of conflictive interactions in the family contribute to a potentially toxic environment which serves as a stressor that interacts with, and exacerbates, the negative effects of major stressful life events (Avison, 2010). On the other hand, having close, connected interactions with family and other household members can offset the psychological consequences of major stressors (Conger & Conger, 2002; Gard, McLoyd, Mitchell, & Hyde, 2020).
Although there are established measures of family conflict and cohesion, in our estimation they would not be sensitive to the unique circumstances of the social distancing phenomenon. The magnitude of the COVID‐19 pandemic and the profound impact that it is likely to have on societies around the world is not yet understood, but it will undoubtedly generate the need, and the opportunity, for new knowledge not only in the basic and clinical sciences, which are most directly involved in prevention, treatment and cure of the illness, but also in the social and behavioral science disciplines. The pandemic has led many researchers to develop measures of its social impact, several of which, along with the CHES, have been included in the NIH’s PhenX Toolkit of common data elements for social determinants of health in the COVID‐19 pandemic (NIH, 2020). However, we are not aware of any measures designed to specifically address family functioning in the context of the current pandemic. With the CHES, we sought to develop a tool that could be useful for ongoing family studies that were disrupted by the pandemic, and for new COVID‐19 research and clinical initiatives aimed at informing or measuring the impact of family interventions.
Our process for developing the CHES has followed best practices as recommended by Boateng et al. (2018). However, the time sensitivity of facilitating on‐the‐ground research about the social and psychological impact of COVID‐19 necessitated expedited dissemination. The fluidity of these family processes during the course of a disaster event and in the period following the event (Bonanno, Brewin, Kaniasty, & La Greca, 2010; Le, Tracy, Norris, & Galea, 2013) raises the importance of assessing these family variables longitudinally and ideally starting during the early stages of the crisis. Our measure development process was therefore somewhat compressed and work remains to be done. For example, although our analysis provides support for the content validity of the CHES and we have presented the relationship of family functioning with various descriptive indicators, further work is needed to assess the relationship of the CHES with measures of family functioning that can provide construct validation, and with measures of mental health outcomes for criterion validity. Moreover, given the disparities in physical and social impact of COVID‐19, we recommend that future users of the CHES supplement the descriptive indicators included in the first section of the CHES to include ethnicity, and more extensive socioeconomic and cultural indicators.
Further work is also needed to address the factor structure of the family functioning scales. Specifically, while there is evidence of a two‐factor model assessing conflict and cohesion based on the results of the EFA, this model was not confirmed using CFA. Model fit may have been improved by modeling correlations between items or cross‐loading of items. Additional examination of the factor structure is needed in future studies with new respondent samples.
The COVID‐19 pandemic will leave an indelible mark on individuals and families for years to come. In addition to illness and loss of loved ones, scholars have predicted that the economic turmoil following the COVID‐19 pandemic will be significantly greater than that of the Great Recession; once again, disparately affecting racial and ethnic minorities, single parent households and families of low socioeconomic status (Artiga, Garfield, & Orgera, 2020). The imprint that the pandemic will leave on mental health will be colored, in part, by the emotional climate in the home during the practice of social distancing. Intervention strategies are needed to prevent and mitigate mental consequences of the pandemic, including family‐based interventions that address relational stressors and build upon positive family structures that emerge from the shared experience.
The global nature of this crisis calls for a global response. We have seen unprecedented international collaboration to identify effective treatments and to develop a vaccine. We also need global initiatives to understand and intervene in the mental health repercussions for individuals and families. An example of a multinational effort is seen in the ECLB‐COVID‐19 Multicenter Study that sought to understand the mental health repercussions of COVID‐19 home confinement. They have found that social distancing has exacerbated psychological strain, particularly in high‐risk populations (Ammar, et al., 2020). We are hopeful that family researchers will also embark on multinational projects. To this end, we designed the CHES to be relevant for households around the world, and the reliability of the measure in our large international sample supports the universality of the social distancing experience in homes around the world. In the short time since we began this work, the CHES has been translated to Portuguese and Mandarin and a translation to Haitian Creole is in progress. In response to requests from researchers, we have also developed an adaptation for adolescents. The CHES is publicly available at https://elcentro.sonhs.miami.edu/research/measures‐library/covid‐19/index.html.
This work was completed with support from the Center for Latino Health Research Opportunities ‐U54MD002266 (PI: Behar‐Zusman) and the Center for HIV and Research in Mental Health –P30MH116867 (PI: Safren). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.
References
- Afifi, T. , Granger, D. , Ersig, A. , Tsalikian, E. , Shahnazi, A. , Davis, S. et al. (2019). Testing the theory of resilience and relational load (TRRL) in families with type I diabetes. Health Communication, 34, 1107–1119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Amir, H. (2017) Psychology – Child Emotions The Link between Inter‐Parental Conflict and Health Emotional Development of Children. Journal of Psychology & Clinical Psychiatry, 7 (2). 10.15406/jpcpy.2017.07.00431 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ammar, A. , Mueller, P. , Trabelsi, K. , Chtourou, H. , Boukhris, O. , Masmoudi, L. et al. (2020). Emotional consequences of COVID‐19 home confinement: The ECLB‐COVID19 multicenter study. BMJ Yale. https://www.medrxiv.org/content/10.1101/2020.05.05.20091058v1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Artiga, S. , Garfield, R. , & Orgera, K. (2020, April 25). Communities of color at higher risk for health and economic challenges due to COVID‐19. Retrieved May 20, 2020, from Kaiser Family Foundation https://www.kff.org/disparities‐policy/issue‐brief/communities‐of‐color‐at‐higher‐risk‐for‐health‐and‐economic‐challenges‐due‐to‐covid‐19/ [Google Scholar]
- Avison, W. R. (2010). Family structure and women's lives: A life course perspective. In Avison W. R., Aneshensel C. S., Schieman S., & Wheaton B. (Eds.), Advances in the conceptualization of the stress process: Essays in honor of Leonard I. Pearlin (pp. 71–92). New York: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- Boateng, G. O. , Neilands, T. B. , Frongillo, E. A. , Melgar‐Quinonez, H. R. , & Young, S. L. (2018). Best practices for developing and validating scales for health, social, and behavioral research: a primer. Frontiers in Public Health, 6, 1‐18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bokszczanin, A. (2008). Parental support, family conflict, and overprotectiveness: Predicting PTSD symptom levels of adolescents 28 months after a natural disaster. Anxiety, Stress & Coping, 21, 325–335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bonanno, G. A. , Brewin, C. R. , Kaniasty, K. , & La Greca, A. M. (2010). Weighing the costs of disaster: Consequences, risks and resilience in individuals, families and communities. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 11, 1–49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borst, J. B. (2015). A systematic review of the effects of family conflict: Focusing on divorce, style infidelty and attachment. St. Thomas, ON: St. Catherine University. [Google Scholar]
- Brooks, S. K. , Webster, R. K. , Smith, L. E. , Woodland, L. , Wessely, S. , & Greenberg, N. (2020). The psychological impact of quarantine and how to reduce it: rapid review of the evidence. Lancet, 395, 912–920. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown, M. , Whiting, J. , Kahumoku‐Fessler, E. , Witting, A. B. , & Jensen, J. (2020). A dyadic model of stress, coping, and marital satisfaction among parents of children with Autism. Family Relations, 69, 138–150. [Google Scholar]
- Buswell, L. , Zabriskie, R. B. , Lundberg, N. , & Hawkins, A. J. (2012). The Relationship between father involvement in family leisure and family functioning: The importance of daily family leisure. Leisure Sciences, 34, 172–190. [Google Scholar]
- Camacho‐Thompson, D. , Gillen‐O’Neel, C. , Gonzales, N. , & Fuligni, A. (2016). Financial strain, major family life events, and parental academic involvement during adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 45, 1065–1074. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cano, M. A. , Sanchez, M. , Rojas, P. , Ramirez‐Ortiz, D. , Polo, K. L. , Romano, E. et al. (2018). Alcohol use severity among adult hispanic immigrants: Examining the roles of family cohesion, social support, and gender. Substance Use & Misuse, 53, 668–676. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cao, X. , Jiang, X. , Li, X. , Lo, M. J. , & Li, R. (2013). Family functioning and its predictors among disaster bereaved individuals in China: Eighteen months after the Wenchuan earthquake. PLoS One, 8(4), e60738. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2020, April 4). Coronavirus disease 2019. Retrieved May 20, 2020, from Centers of Disease Control and Prevention https://www.cdc.gov/coronavirus/2019‐ncov/php/risk‐assessment.html [Google Scholar]
- Chen, E. , Brody, G. H. , & Miller, G. E. (2017). Childhood close family relationships and health. American Psychologist, 72, 555–566. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conger, R. , & Conger, K. (2002). Resilience in Midwestern families: Selected findings from the first decade of a prospective, longitudinal study. Journal of Marriage and Family, 64, 361–373. [Google Scholar]
- Constantine, M. G. , & Flores, L. Y. (2006). Psychological distress, perceived family conflict, and career development issues in college students of color. Journal of Career Assessment, 14, 354–369. [Google Scholar]
- Cousino, M. K. , & Hazen, R. A. (2013). Parenting stress among caregivers of children with chronic illness: A systematic review. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 38, 809–828. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Creasey, G. , & Reese, M. (1996). Mothers' and fathers' perceptions of parenting hassles: Associations with psychological symptoms, nonparenting hassles, and child behavior problems. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 17, 393–406. [Google Scholar]
- Cummings, M. E. , Koss, K. J. , & Davies, P. T. (2015). Prospective Relations between family conflict and adolescent maladjustment: Security in the family system as a mediating process. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 43, 503–515. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Currie, J. , Duque, V. , & Garfinkel, I. (2015). The great recession and mother's health. The Economic Journal, 125, F311–F346. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Driscoll, M. A. , Higgins, D. M. , Seng, E. K. , Buta, E. , Goulet, J. L. , Heapy, A. A. et al. (2015). Trauma, social support, family conflict, and chronic pain in recent service veterans: does gender matter? Pain Medicine, 16, 1101–1111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Essau, C. A. (2004). The association between family factors and depressive disorders in adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 33, 365–372. [Google Scholar]
- Forbes, M. K. , & Kreuger, R. F. (2019). The great recession and mental health in the United States. Clinical Psychology Science, 7, 900–913. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foxcroft, D. , & Lowe, G. (1995). Adolescent drinking, smoking and other substance use involvement: Links with perceived family life. Journal of Adolescent Health, 18, 159–177. [Google Scholar]
- Francesco, T. , Barsanti, I. , Bongioanni, P. , Bogliolo, C. , & Rossi, B. (2014). A permanent emergency: A longitudinal study on families coping with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Families, Systems & Health, 32, 271–279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- French, K. A. , Dumani, S. , Allen, T. D. , & Shockley, K. M. (2018). A meta‐analysis of work‐family conflict and social support. Psychological Bulletin, 144, 284–314. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frontini, R. , Canavarro, M. C. , & Moreira, H. (2018). Family cohesion and psychopathological symptoms in pediatric obesity: Is there an indirect effect? Children's Health Care, 47, 101–117. [Google Scholar]
- Gard, A. M. , McLoyd, V. C. , Mitchell, C. , & Hyde, L. W. (2020). Evaluation of a longitudinal family stress model in a population‐based cohort. Social Development, 1–21. 10.1111/sode.12446 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giuliano, P. , & Spilimbergo, A. (2013). Growing up in a recession. The Review of Economic Studies, 81, 787–817. [Google Scholar]
- Grusky, D. B. , Western, B. , & Wilmer, C. (2011). The great recession. New York: Russell Sage Foundation. [Google Scholar]
- Hobfoll, S. , & Spielberger, C. (1992). Family stress: Integrating theory and measurement. Journal of Family Psychology, 6, 99–112. [Google Scholar]
- Hoynes, H. , Miller, D. L. , & Schaller, J. (2012). Who suffers during recessions? Journal of Economic Perspectives, 26, 27–48. [Google Scholar]
- IBM Corp. (2017). IBM SPSS statistics for MacOS, Version 25.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp. [Google Scholar]
- Joel Wong, Y. , Uhm, S. Y. , & Li, P. (2012). Asian Americans' family cohesion and suicide ideation: Moderating and mediating effects. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 82, 309–318. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson, M. D. , Urm, S. H. , Jung, J. A. , Yun, H. D. , Munitz, G. E. , Tsigrelis, C. et al. (2013). Housing data‐based socioeconomic index and risk of invasive pneumococcal disease: An exploratory study. Epidemiology and Infection, 141(4), 880–887. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kazak, A. (1992). Stress, change, and families: Theoretical and methodological considerations. Journal of Family Psychology, 6, 120–124. [Google Scholar]
- Landers, A. , Dimitropoulos, G. , Mendenhall, T. , Kennedy, A. , & Zemanek, L. (2019). Backing the blue: Trauma in law enforcement spouses and couples. Family Relations, 69(2), 308–319. [Google Scholar]
- Le, F. , Tracy, M. , Norris, F. H. , & Galea, S. (2013). Displacement, county social cohesion, and depression after a large‐scale traumatic event. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 48, 1729–1741. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leidy, M. S. , Guerra, N. G. , & Toro, R. I. (2010). Positive parenting, family cohesion, and child social competence among immigrant Latino families. Journal of Family Psychology, 24, 252–260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leong, F. , Park, Y. S. , & Kalibatseva, Z. (2013). Disentangling immigrant status in mental health: Psychological protective and risk factors among Latino and Asian American immigrants. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 83, 361–371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Magaña, S. , & Smith, M. J. (2006). Psychological distress and well‐being of Latina and non‐Latina white mothers of youth and adults with an autism spectrum disorder: Cultural attitudes towards coresidence status. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 76, 346–357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Margerison‐Zilko, C. , Goldman‐Mellor, S. , Falconi, A. , & Downing, J. (2016). Health impacts of the great recession: A critical review. Current Epidemiology Reports, 3, 81–91. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marta, E. , & Alfieri, S. (2014). Family conflicts. Retrieved June 23, 2020, from Encyclopedia of Quality of Life and Well‐Being Research https://link.springer.com/referenceworkentry/10.1007%2F978‐94‐007‐0753‐5_997 [Google Scholar]
- McDermott, B. M. , & Cobham, V. E. (2012). Family functioning in the aftermath of a natural disaster. BMC Psychiatry, 12, 12–55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meadows, R. , Sell, K. , Blinkoff, E. , Williams, N. , & Repcheck, L. (2015). The effect of the great recession on child well‐being. Philadelphia, PA: First Focus. [Google Scholar]
- Moos, R. , & Moos, B. (1994). Family environment scale manual: Development, applications, research. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. [Google Scholar]
- Muthén, L. , & Muthén, B. (2017). Mplus user's guide. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén. [Google Scholar]
- NIH (2020, May 8). COVID‐19 protocols. Retrieved June 29, 2020, from Phenx Toolkit https://www.phenxtoolkit.org/covid19/ [Google Scholar]
- Nohe, C. , Meier, L. L. , Sonntag, K. , & Michel, A. (2015). The chicken or the egg? A meta‐analysis of panel studies of the relationship between work‐family conflict and strain. Journal of Applied Psychology, 100, 522–536. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Norris, F. , & Uhl, G. (1993). Chronic stress as a mediator of acute stress: The case of Hurricane Hugo. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 23, 1263–1284. [Google Scholar]
- North, C. S. , & Pfefferbaum, B. (2013). Mental health response to community disasters a systematic review. Journal of the American Medical Association, 310, 507–518. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olson, D. H. , Russel, C. S. , & Sprenkle, D. H. (1983). Circumplex model of marital and family systems: Vl. Theoretical update. Family Process, 22, 69–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olson, J. R. (2018). Individual, family, and community predictors of PTSD symptoms following military deployment. Family Relations, 67, 615–629. [Google Scholar]
- Orthner, D. K. , Jones‐Sanpei, H. , & Williamson, S. (2004). The resilience and strengths of low‐income families. Family Relations Interdisciplinary Journal of Applied Family Science, 53, 159–167. [Google Scholar]
- Pearlin, L. I. (1999). Stress and mental health: A conceptual overview. In Pearlin L. I. (Ed.), A handbook for the study of mental health: Social contexts, theories, and systems (pp. 161–175). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Perrino, T. , Pantin, H. , Huang, S. , Brincks, A. , Brown, H. , & Prado, G. (2015). Reducing the risk of internalizing symptoms among high‐risk Hispanic youth through a family intervention: A randomized controlled trial. Family Process, 55, 91–106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Priest, J. B. , & Denton, W. (2012). Anxiety disorders and Latinos: The role of family cohesion. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 34, 557–575. [Google Scholar]
- Repetti, R. L. , Taylor, S. E. , & Seeman, T. E. (2002). Risky families: Family social environments and the mental and physical health of offspring. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 330–366. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sauceda, J. A. , Wiebe, J. S. , Chan, K. , Kutner, B. A. , & Simoni, J. M. (2018). Acculturation, family cohesion, and mental health among Latinos living with HIV on the U.S.‐Mexico border. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 24, 453–458. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schneider, W. , Waldfogel, J. , & Brooks‐Gunn, J. (2017). The great recession and risk for child abuse and neglect. Children and Youth Services Review, 72, 71–81. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shonkoff, J. P. , Boyce, W. T. , & McEwen, B. S. (2009). Neuroscience, molecular biology, and the childhood roots of health disparities building a new framework for health promotion and disease prevention. Journal of the American Medical Association, 301, 2252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skeer, M. R. , & Ballard, E. L. (2013). Are family meals as good for youth as we think they are? A review of the literature on family meals as they pertain to adolescent risk prevention. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 42, 943–963. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sprague, C. M. , Kia‐Keating, M. , Felix, E. , Afifi, T. , Reyes, G. , & Afifi, W. (2015). Youth psychosocial adjustment following wildfire: the role of family resilience, emotional support, and concrete support. Child & Youth Care Forum, 44, 433–450. [Google Scholar]
- Uruk, A. C. , Sayger, T. V. , & Cogdal, P. A. (2007). Examining the influence of family cohesion and adaptability on trauma symptoms and psychological well‐being. Journal of College Student Psychotherapy, 22, 51–63. [Google Scholar]
- Utter, J. , Denny, S. , Robinson, E. , Fleming, T. , Ameratunga, S. , & Grant, S. (2013). Family meals and the well‐being of adolescents. Journal of Paediatrics and Child Health, 49, 906–911. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Voerman, J. , Vogel, I. , de Waart, F. , Westendorp, T. , Timman, R. , Busschbach, J. et al. (2015). Bullying, abuse and family conflict as risk factors for chronic pain among Dutch adolescents. European Journal of Pain, 19, 1544–1551. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yi, J. (2009). Cultural influences on the survivorship of families affected by childhood cancer: A case for using family systems theories. Families, Systems & Health, 27, 228–236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zapata Roblyer, M. I. , Carlos, F. L. , Merten, M. J. , & Gallus, K. (2017). Psychosocial factors associated with depressive symptoms among Latina immigrants living in a new arrival community. Journal of Latina/o Psychology, 5, 103–117. [Google Scholar]