Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2021 Aug 1.
Published in final edited form as: Environ Pollut. 2020 Mar 26;263(Pt B):114444. doi: 10.1016/j.envpol.2020.114444

The association between prenatal exposure to perfluoroalkyl substances and childhood neurodevelopment

Miranda J Spratlen a, Frederica P Perera a, Sally Ann Lederman b, Virginia A Rauh b, Morgan Robinson c, Kurunthachalam Kannan c,d, Leonardo Trasande e,f,g, Julie Herbstman a
PMCID: PMC7362877  NIHMSID: NIHMS1583274  PMID: 32272335

Abstract

Perfluoroalkyl substances (PFAS) were among various persistent organic pollutants suspected to have been released during the collapse of the World Trade Center (WTC) on 9/11. Evidence on the association between prenatal PFAS exposure and child neurodevelopment is limited and inconsistent. This study evaluated the association between prenatal PFAS exposure and child cognitive outcomes measured at 5 different time points in a population prenatally exposed to the WTC disaster. The study population included 302 pregnant women in the Columbia University WTC birth cohort enrolled between December 13, 2001 and June 26, 2002 at three hospitals located near the WTC site: Beth Israel, St. Vincent’s, and New York University Downtown. We evaluated the association between prenatal exposure to four PFAS (perfluorooctane sulfonate (PFOS), perfluorooctanoic acid (PFOA), perfluorohexanesulfonic acid (PFHxS), perfluorononanoic acid (PFNA)) and child neurodevelopment measured using the Bayley Scales of Infant Development (BSID-II) at approximately 1, 2 and 3 years of age and using The Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence (WPPSI) at approximately 4 and 6 years of age. Geometric mean (range) concentrations of PFAS were 6.03 (1.05, 33.7), 2.31 (0.18, 8.14), 0.43 (<LOQ, 10.3) and 0.67 (<LOQ, 15.8) ng/mL for PFOS, PFOA, PFNA and PFHxS, respectively. Several PFAS were associated with increases in cognitive outcomes in females and overall (males and females combined). Child sex modified the association between PFOS and the mental development index measured using BSID-II, with the observed relationship being positive for females and negative for males. Through principal component analyses, we observed a negative relationship between PFNA and the psychomotor development index measured using BSID-II and the verbal IQ measured using WPPSI. Our results suggest a sex- and compound-specific relationship between prenatal PFAS exposures and childhood neurodevelopment.

Keywords: perfluoroalkyl substances, cord blood, World Trade Center disaster, cognitive outcomes

Graphical Abstract

graphic file with name nihms-1583274-f0003.jpg

INTRODUCTION

Perfluoralkyl substances (PFAS) are a class of synthetic chemicals that have been used in commercial and industrial products, including fire-fighting foam, carpets, food packaging, clothing and non-stick cookware, since the 1940s.1 Recently, the main PFAS manufacturers in the US have mostly phased out production of the two most widespread PFAS, perfluorooctane sulfonate (PFOS) and perfluorooctanoic acid (PFOA); however, their long half-lives, global dispersion, and resistance to degradation suggest that exposure to these compounds will remain a public health concern for some time.1 Indeed, despite reductions in levels of exposure, national estimates for PFOS and PFOA indicate that they remain at levels that have been associated with adverse health effects in humans.24 Further, other PFAS, including replacement PFAS, may actually be increasing: levels of perfluorononanoic acid (PFNA) in women of child-bearing age increased between 1999–2000 and 2007–2008, before decreasing back to earlier levels in 2013–2014.5 Exposure in humans occurs through ingestion of contaminated food and water, inhalation/ingestion of dust and fumes from PFAS-containing products in homes and offices, and occupational exposure in workplaces that produce or use PFAS.6,7 PFAS have been shown to cross the placental barrier resulting in in utero fetal exposure, and in turn, have been associated with reductions in fetal growth in both humans and animals.810

Development during the prenatal period is particularly vulnerable to neurotoxic exposures because of the rapid and fundamental developmental processes that occur during this time. Some1115, but not all16, experimental studies suggest prenatal PFAS exposure may have neurodevelopmental effects. Alterations in offspring behavior and motor function13,14, as well as in levels of proteins required for normal brain development12, have been observed in mice exposed prenatally to PFOS and PFOA. Several mechanisms have been proposed to explain the neurotoxic effect of PFAS including modifications in the expression of calcium-related signaling molecules in the hippocampus12,17, changes to the functioning of the cholinergic system16, and interference with thyroid hormone homeostasis.18,19

Evaluation of the relationship of prenatal PFAS exposure and cognitive outcomes in humans is understudied and findings have been inconsistent. Of the five studies that have looked at PFAS and measures of IQ specifically, two studies, conducted in Japan20 and Taiwan21, found significant inverse associations. However, studies conducted in the US22,23 and Denmark24 found evidence of a positive association between PFAS and IQ. Studies that have looked at prenatal PFAS exposure and other childhood cognitive outcomes have also reported inconsistent findings. A study conducted among British girls found both positive and negative associations between Perfluorononanoic acid (PFNA), PFOA, PFOS and early communication development, and only positive associations for perfluorohexanesulfonic acid (PFHxS) with early communication development outcomes at 15 and 28 months.25 A study conducted in the Danish Birth Cohort found positive and negative associations between PFOS and developmental milestones measured at 6 and 18 months, respectively, but null associations for PFOA.26 A US-based study found associations between higher PFOA, PFOS and PFNA and better reading skills at 5 and 8 years.27 Finally, another US-based study found positive associations between PFNA, PFHxS and PFOS and visual memory, but negative associations for PFOA and PFHxS with visual motor abilities.28 Several studies have also reported sex-specific associations between PFAS and neurodevelopment in humans20,23,24 and animals11,14.

In this study, we attempt to better understand the inconsistent associations that have been observed between prenatal PFAS exposure and neurodevelopment through the evaluation of four PFAS (PFOS, PFOA, PFHxS, PFNA) in a population of pregnant mothers that delivered in New York City hospitals and whose children had childhood cognitive outcomes assessed from age 1 through 6 years.

METHODS

Study Population

We included mother-child dyads from a Columbia University birth cohort originally selected to evaluate the effects of exposure to the World Trade Center (WTC) disaster on September 11, 2001 (9/11) on pregnancy outcomes and child development in women who were pregnant at that time. Detailed methods have been described previously.29 Briefly, 329 women with singleton pregnancies were enrolled between December 13, 2001 and June 26, 2002 at one of three hospitals located near the WTC site: Beth Israel, St. Vincent’s, and New York University Downtown. Eligibility requirements included: maternal age between 18 and 39 years, had not smoked during pregnancy (<1 cigarette/day at any time), self-report of no diabetes, hypertension, HIV infection or AIDS, and no use of illegal drugs in the last year. Participants provided at least one blood sample (maternal blood at the time of delivery and/or cord blood), access to their medical record and to their newborn’s medical record, and completion of a 30- to 45-minute interview after delivery. PFAS was measured in either cord or maternal blood, and participants missing both PFAS measurements (n=27) were excluded from this analysis, resulting in a sample size of 302. Participants missing maternal IQ (n=104), maternal race (n=19), pre- pregnancy BMI (n=4), parity (n=1), maternal demoralization (n=4) were excluded from complete case sensitivity analyses. Participants were further excluded from age-specific analyses if they were missing cognitive measures at these time points, resulting in the following sample sizes for complete case sensitivity analyses: 1 year (n=156), 2 years (n=157), 3 years (n=127), 4 years (n=124) and 6 years (n=110) (Figure 1).

Figure 1. Study Flow Diagram.

Figure 1.

Abbreviations: Perfluoralkyl substances (PFAS); Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence (WPPSI).

Sociodemographic and Risk Factor Variables

The postpartum interview was administered at the hospital, post-delivery and prior to discharge, in the woman’s preferred or native language (English, Spanish, or Chinese). Information on maternal education, date of birth, race, parity, material hardship during pregnancy, marital status and family smoking exposure was collected through a structured questionnaire during the interview. Two study-specific variables relating to exposure to the 9/11 WTC event were also drawn from the structured questionnaire; these variables were trimester on 9/11 and residential and occupational (if applicable) distance to the 9/11 site. Gestational age on 9/11 was used to determine trimester on that day. Mothers were classified as being in their first trimester on 9/11 if their child had a gestational age of ≤91 days on 9/11, and in their second or third trimester if their child had a gestational age >91 days. Eighteen participants were not pregnant yet on 9/11 but were still included in the study in the first trimester group, because exposures to the disaster persisted for months following the initial collapse. Distance to the 9/11 site was categorized into two groups: those who either lived or worked within 2 miles of the 9/11 site versus those who did not, using geocoded residential and work addresses (for the 4 weeks starting on and following 9/11). Maternal pre-pregnancy BMI was calculated using weight in kilograms divided by height in meters squared, both abstracted from the participants’ medical chart. In the case of missing height (n=41) or weight (n=52) from the medical record, self-reported information on these variables from the hospital interview was used. Among participants with both self-reported and medical record weight and height, correlations were very high (r=0.99 and r=0.91, respectively). Child sex and date of birth were abstracted from the child’s medical record. Gestational age in days was also abstracted from the medical record (if missing (n=15), date of last menstrual period from the interview minus the child’s date of birth was used). Maternal age at delivery was determined by subtracting the child’s date of birth from the mother’s date of birth. Maternal demoralization was measured during the post-partum interview using the Psychiatric Epidemiology Research Instrument Demoralization scale (PERI-D), which provides a measure of nonspecific psychological distress, with demonstrated reliability across different ethnic groups.3032 Maternal intelligence was evaluated during the first study visit, which took place at approximately 12 months post-partum, using the Test of Non-Verbal Intelligence, Second Edition (TONI-2), a validated instrument for measuring general cognitive ability, considered to be free of cultural bias.33 Institutional Review Board approval was obtained before enrollment began and all women gave written informed consent before delivery.

Child Cognitive Outcomes

Child neurodevelopment was measured using the Bayley Scales of Infant Development (BSID-II) at approximately 1, 2 and 3 years of age. The BSID-II is a tool to evaluate neurodevelopment in infants and toddlers aged 1–42 months through two indices, the Mental Development Index (MDI), which measures memory, problem solving, sensory perception, hand–eye coordination, imitation and early language, and the Psychomotor Development Index (PDI), which measures fine and gross motor development.34 The assessment provides a developmental quotient (raw score/chronological age), generating continuous MDI and PDI scores, which are normed and have a mean of 100 and a standard deviation of 15, with higher scores indicating better development. The BSID-II has demonstrated reliability and validity.34 Child neurodevelopment at 4 and 6 years of age was measured using The Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence (WPPSI). WPPSI-R, the first revision to the scale and the version used in this study, is a standardized assessment designed to measure the cognitive development of children ages 3 years to 7 years and 3 months.35 The scale is composed of 12 core subtests which are used to develop three main index scores: verbal, performance and full scale IQ. Not all children were available for all developmental assessments, resulting in different numbers of children tested at each age. Assessments were conducted in the first language of the child (English or Chinese) by trained research technicians. In some cases, when the primary language of the child was not English or Chinese (e.g., Yiddish), we relied on maternal translation. The majority of follow-up assessments were conducted at the Columbia Center for Children’s Environmental Health, however a small number of assessments were conducted in the child’s home if the parents were unable or unwilling to travel to the Center.

PFAS Collection and Measurements

Blood samples from the umbilical cord were collected at the time of delivery; maternal samples were typically collected on the day after delivery. On average, 30.7 mL blood was collected from the umbilical cord, and 30–35 mL blood was collected from the mothers. Blood samples were transported to Columbia University laboratory facilities in Northern Manhattan and processed within hours of collection. The buffy coat, packed red blood cells and plasma were separated and stored at −70 °C.

Twelve PFAS [PFOS, PFOA, PFHxS, PFNA, perfluorodecanesulfonic acid (PFDS), perfluorobutanesulfonic acid (PFBS), perfluorooctane sulfonamide (PFOSA), perfluorohexanoic acid (PFHxA), perfluoroheptanoic acid (PFHpA), perfluorodecanoic acid (PFDA), perfluoroundecanoic (PFUnDA) and perfluorododecanoate (PFDoDA)] were measured in maternal plasma (n=55) and cord blood (n=247) using a solid phase extraction procedure and high-performance liquid chromatograph interfaced with an electrospray tandem mass spectrometer, at the New York State Department of Health Wadsworth Center Laboratory, using methods similar to those used in prior studies.36,37 Internal standards for C-labeled PFAS were added into plasma samples prior to the addition of reagents for extraction.38 Solvents and method blanks (blinded to the laboratory) were tested for the presence of the PFAS. Target chemicals were not found in procedural blanks at concentrations above the limits of quantification (LOQs). The LOQs of target chemicals ranged from 0.08 to 0.20 ng/mL. A standard reference material from the National Institute of Standards and Technology was analyzed with every batch of 50 samples, and recoveries of target chemicals were between 90% and 115% of the certified values. Recoveries of target chemicals passed through the entire analytical procedure ranged between 100% and 124%. Quantification was by isotope dilution and target chemicals were monitored by multiple reaction monitoring mode under negative ionization.

Statistical Analyses

All statistical analyses were conducted in R software (version 3.5.1; R Project for Statistical Computing). A threshold of P < 0.05 was used to define associations as statistically significant. PFAS assessment was restricted to compounds detected in ≥50% of samples (PFOS, PFOA, PFNA, PFHxS and PFDS). To maximize sample size, we used both maternal plasma and cord blood concentrations in analyses. However, to account for differences in maternal versus cord blood samples, we used a prediction model developed using 78 paired cord blood and maternal plasma PFAS samples from the HOME study39, a US-based cohort with comparable PFAS concentrations, to transform maternal PFAS concentrations to cord blood concentrations in participants with maternal measurements but no cord blood measurements in our study as previously described.40 We were unable to create prediction models from our own data because we did not have paired samples. Instead, participants had either a cord PFAS measurement or maternal PFAS measurement. Separate prediction models were run for PFOS, PFOA, PFNA and PFHxS. Prediction models were not available for PFDS; therefore, analyses focused on the previously listed four PFAS compounds and did not include PFDS. All analyses report concentrations and associations using both transformed maternal-to-cord concentrations and cord blood concentrations together. PFOS, PFOA, PFNA and PFHxS were log-transformed to account for right-skewed distributions. Both PFOA and PFOS were detected in 100% of samples. Two samples (<1%) were below the LOQ (0.08 ng/ml) for PFHxS and 40 samples (13%) were below the LOQ (0.20 ng/ml) for PFNA. In accord with published practices,41 samples <LOQ were imputed as the LOQ divided by the square root of 2.

We used separate linear models to evaluate the association between log-transformed PFAS variables and cognitive outcomes measured at 1, 2 and 3 years of age through BSID-II (outcomes = MDI and PDI indices) and at 4 and 6 years of age through WPPSI (outcomes = Performance, Verbal and Full IQ scales). We also evaluated the combined effect of exposure to all four PFAS variables on cognitive outcomes through principal component analyses, a data reduction technique in which the linear relationships between observed correlated variables are captured into a smaller number of principal components, as has been done in previous PFAS epidemiologic studies.42 Each input variable, in this case the four PFAS variables, is given a “factor loading”, reflecting the correlation of each PFAS with that component.43 We followed the Kaiser criterion44 and included all principal factors with eigenvalues ≥1.0.45 Main analyses were conducted using imputed data for missing variables and regressions were run with and without covariate adjustments. Adjustments for all models were selected a priori based on previous literature and included maternal age; material hardship during pregnancy (defined as having gone without either food, shelter, gas/electric, clothing, or medication/medical care because of financial constraint); pre-pregnancy BMI; maternal IQ; maternal race (Black, White, Asian, Native American/other); maternal education (< high school degree, high school degree, > high school degree); home smoking exposure (no reported family member smoking in household, any reported family member smoking in household); marital status (not married, married); parity (primiparous, multiparous); child’s gestational age at birth; exact child age on test date; child’s sex; trimester on 9/11 (first trimester, second/third trimester); maternal demoralization score; and child breastfeeding history (ever breastfed or never). To evaluate the potential for sex-specific associations, we included an interaction term between the child’s sex and the PFAS variable and reported the P-value from a two- sided Wald t-test on the coefficient for the 2-way multiplicative term.

Due to substantial missing data for outcome variables (cognitive measures) and maternal intelligence, we imputed variables using multivariate imputation by chained equations (MICE). MICE is a highly flexible statistical imputation method that generates multiple predictions for missing values by creating multiple complete datasets based on the observed values for a given individual and its relationship with the observed values in the data for other participants. This method takes into account uncertainty in missing value imputation and therefore, yields accurate standard errors. Described in detail by Schafer and Graham (2002)46, MICE is now a common statistical method for dealing with missing data in both observational and clinical trials with demonstrated effectiveness in handling substantial missing data, including in outcome variables.4749 Proportions of variables with imputed values in our data ranged from 0% to 52.6% and included maternal race, maternal pre-pregnancy BMI, parity, breastfeeding status, maternal IQ, maternal demoralization and all cognitive outcome measures. Imputation methods included predictive mean modeling for continuous variables and logistic regression for binary variables. For our imputation model, as generally advised, we used all variables potentially predictive of the exposure (PFAS) – outcome (cognition) relationship, covariates or missingness.50 These variables included all model covariates (listed above), exposure and outcome variables, in addition to marital status and proximity to the 9/11 site, a variable relevant to this dataset specifically. Setting model iterations to 20 and imputations to 30 achieved healthy convergence of the imputation model, evaluated visually through trace and density plots. To visually display sex-specific trends, results from adjusted imputed PFOS models at all time-points, overall and by sex, were plotted (Figure 2).

Figure 2. Mean Difference (95% Confidence Interval) in Cognitive Outcomes per Log Unit Changes in Perfluorooctane Sulfonate (PFOS).

Figure 2.

MDI and PDI were measured using the Bayley Scales of Infant Development (BSID-II) when the child was approximately 1, 2 and 3 years of age. Per (Performance), Ver (Verbal) and Full scale IQ scores were measured using The Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence (WPPSI) when the child was approximately 4 and 6 years of age. WPPSI-R, is the first revision to the scale and the version used in this study. Models were adjusted for maternal age, material hardship, parity, pre-pregnancy BMI, maternal IQ, maternal race, maternal education, family smoking status, child age at testing, child’s gestational age at birth, child sex and child breastfeeding history. The number following the outcome (MDI or PDI, etc.) is the age in years at testing. * indicates significance (p<0.05) of an interaction by sex. Abbreviations: Mental development index (MDI); per (performance); perfluorooctane sulfonate (PFOS); psychomotor development index (PDI), ver (Verbal)

In sensitivity analyses, we ran regressions using only complete cases at each age (i.e., 1, 2, 3, 4 and 6 years).

RESULTS

Participant Characteristics

Geometric mean (range) concentrations of PFAS variables (cord blood plus maternal-to-cord transformed) were 6.03 (1.05, 33.7) ng/ml for PFOS, 2.31 (0.18, 8.14) ng/ml for PFOA, 0.43 (<LOQ, 10.3) ng/ml for PFNA and 0.67 (<LOQ, 15.8) ng/ml for PFHxS (Table 1). Since there were no missing data for maternal age, child sex, trimester on 9/11, gestational age, material hardship during pregnancy, marital status, maternal education and family smoking status, none of these data were imputed. Median (interquartile range) maternal age was 30.6 (26.8, 34.5). Most women reported no family smoking (81.5%) and no material hardship during pregnancy (90.7%). The majority of women were married (79.8%) and pregnant less than 91 days (≤1st trimester) on 9/11 (69.5%). Half of the children born were female (49.7%) and among all children born, they had a median (IQR) gestational age of 280 (273, 280) days at birth. The majority of women had greater than a high school degree (65.2%), with 16.9% having a high school degree and 17.9% reporting less than a high school degree. Data on race, pre-pregnancy BMI, parity, maternal demoralization, breastfeeding status, maternal IQ and child cognitive outcomes were missing to varying degrees. The distributions of the variables with missing data did not differ substantially between participants with observed data and those with imputed data and are described in Table 2. Two distinct patterns of PFAS exposure were observed through principal component analyses. The first pattern, PFAS principal component (PC1), explained 57% of the variance and had high positive loadings for all four PFAS, reflecting higher overall PFAS exposure (Table 3). The second pattern, PFAS principal component (PC2), explained 25% of the variance and was dominated by positive loadings for PFNA, followed by negative loadings for PFOA and PFHxS.

Table 1.

Geometric Mean Concentrations (ng/mL) and Percent Above Detection of Perfluoroalkyl Substances (PFAS) in Cord Blood, Maternal Plasma, and Cord + Transformed Cord Blood.

PFAS N LOQ % Above LOQ Geometric Mean (Range)

PFOS
 Cord Blood 247 0.20 100% 6.27 (1.05, 33.7)
 Maternal Plasma 55 0.20 100% 11.9 (2.90, 30.9)
 Cord Blood + Transformed Cord Blood 302 0.20 100% 6.03 (1.05, 33.7)
PFOA
 Cord Blood 247 0.08 100% 2.37 (0.18, 8.14)
 Maternal Plasma 55 0.08 100% 2.42 (0.88, 5.06)
 Cord Blood + Transformed Cord Blood 302 0.08 100% 2.31 (0.18, 8.14)
PFNA
 Cord Blood 247 0.20 86% 0.45 (<LOQ, 10.3)
 Maternal Plasma 55 0.20 96% 0.45 (<LOQ, 1.93)
 Cord Blood + Transformed Cord Blood 302 0.20 88% 0.43 (<LOQ, 10.3)
PFHxS
 Cord Blooddu 247 0.08 99% 0.69 (<LOQ, 15.8)
 Maternal Plasma 55 0.08 100% 0.94 (0.35, 3.20)
 Cord Blood + Transformed Cord Blood 302 0.08 99% 0.67 (<LOQ, 15.8)
PFDS
 Cord Blood 247 0.08 97% 0.13 (<LOQ, 0.64)
 Maternal Plasma 55 0.08 98% 0.16 (<LOQ, 0.82)
PFOSA
 Cord Blood 247 0.08 0% <LOQ
 Maternal Plasma 55 0.08 0% <LOQ
PFBS
 Cord Blood 247 0.08 <1% <LOQ (<LOQ, 0.28)
 Maternal Plasma 55 0.08 0% <LOQ
PFHxA
 Cord Blood 247 0.08 15% <LOQ (<LOQ, 10.8)
 Maternal Plasma 55 0.08 19% <LOQ (<LOQ, 6.01)
PFHpA
 Cord Blood 247 0.08 36% <LOQ (<LOQ, 0.59)
 Maternal Plasma 55 0.08 23% <LOQ (<LOQ, 0.23)
PFDA
 Cord Blood 247 0.08 45% <LOQ (<LOQ, 1.69)
 Maternal Plasma 55 0.08 75% 0.13 (<LOQ, 0.75)
PFUnDA
 Cord Blood 247 0.20 20% <LOQ (<LOQ, 3.27)
 Maternal Plasma 55 0.20 13% <LOQ (<LOQ, 0.87)
PFDoDA
 Cord Blood 247 0.20 7% <LOQ (<LOQ, 0.63)
 Maternal Plasma 55 0.20 0% <LOQ

Abbreviations: Limit of Quantification (LOQ); Interquartile Range (IQR); perfluorohexanesulfonic acid (PFHxS); perfluorononanoic acid (PFNA); perfluorooctane sulfonate (PFOS); perfluorooctanoic acid (PFOA)

Geometric mean is listed as <LOQ if >50% of observations are <LOQ.

Table 2.

Participant Characteristic Distributions of Observed and Imputed Data Predicted through Multiple Imputation by Chained Equations (MICE).

Variable Number Missing (%) Median (IQR) or %
Observed Imputed

Total 302
Maternal Age 0 30.6 (26.8, 34.5) NA
Child Sex 0
 Female 49.7 NA
 Male 50.3 NA
Trimester on 9/11 0
 ≤1st Trimester 69.5 NA
 2nd or 3rd Trimester 30.5 NA
Gestational Age at Birth (days) 0 280 (273, 280) NA
Racea 19 (6.3)
 Black 16.6 17.1
 White 43.5 43.6
 Asian 36.0 34.5
 Native American/Other 3.9 4.7
Pre-pregnancy BMIa 3 (1.0) 22.0 (20.2, 24.4) 22.0 (20.2, 24.4)
Paritya 1 (0.33)
 Primiparous 56.1 56.0
 Multiparous 43.9 44.0
Material Hardship 0
 Yes 9.3 NA
 No 90.7 NA
Maternal Demoralizationa 4 (1.3) 0.70 (0.44, 1.11) 0.70 (0.44, 1.11)
Marital Status 0
 Married 79.8 NA
 Not Married 20.2 NA
Education 0
 < High School Degree 17.9 NA
 High School Degree 16.9 NA
 > High School Degree 65.2 NA
Family Smoking Status 0
 No Family Smoking 81.5 NA
 Any Family Smoking 18.5 NA
Breastfeeding Historya 6 (2.0)
 Ever Breastfed 77.7 77.4
 Never Breastfed 22.3 22.6
Maternal IQ a 104 (34.4) 94.0 (84.0, 106.5) 94.0 (83.0, 105.5)
Age at Test 1 (years)a 96 (31.8) 1.04 (1.01, 1.13) 1.04 (1.01, 1.13)
Age at Test 2 (years)a 99 (32.8) 2.01 (1.98, 2.07) 2.01 (1.98, 2.07)
Age at Test 3 (years)a 115 (38.1) 3.00 (2.96, 3.11) 3.01 (2.96, 3.13)
Age at Test 4 (years)a 145 (48.0) 4.05 (4.01, 4.11) 4.05 (4.01, 4.12)
Age at Test 6 (years)a 159 (52.6) 6.17 (6.10, 6.26) 6.17 (6.10, 6.26)
MDI Age 1a 119 (39.4) 95.0 (89.0, 102.0) 95.0 (89.0, 102.0)
PDI Age 1a 119 (39.4) 102.0 (90.0, 110.0) 101.0 (90.0, 110.0)
MDI Age 2a 119 (39.4) 96.0 (86.0,105.5) 96.0 (86.0, 105.0)
PDI Age 2a 121 (40.1) 96.0 (92.0, 106.0) 96.0 (90.0, 107.0)
MDI Age 3a 136 (45.0) 97.0 (87.3, 105.0) 95.0 (87.0, 104.0)
PDI Age 3a 141 (46.7) 97.0 (89.0, 108.0) 96.0 (88.0, 108.0)
Verbal Age 4a 145 (48.0) 94.0 (84.0, 103.0) 94.0 (84.0, 103.0)
Performance Age 4a 145 (48.0) 100.0 (89.0, 106.0) 98.0 (86.0, 106.0)
Full IQ Age 4a 145 (48.0) 95.0 (88.0, 103.0) 94.0 (88.0, 103.0)
Verbal Age 6a 159 (52.6) 99.0 (86.5, 109.0) 99.0 (87.0, 109.0)
Performance Age 6a 159 (52.6) 108.0 (97.0, 115.0) 108.0 (94.0, 116.0)
Full IQ Age 6a 159 (52.6) 103.0 (90.0, 113.0) 102.0 (90.0, 111.0)
a

Variable includes imputed values in analyses

Abbreviations: Body Mass Index (BMI); Gestational Age (GA); Interquartile Range (IQR); Mental Development Index (MDI); Psychomotor Development Index (PDI)

Table 3.

Principal Components of Perfluoroalkyl Substances

PFAS Principal Component 1 Principal Component 2

Standard Deviation 1.51 1.01
Proportion of Variance 0.57 0.25
Weight for PFOS 0.59 0.27
Weight for PFOA 0.46 −0.48
Weight for PFNA 0.47 0.65
Weight for PFHxS 0.46 −0.53

Abbreviations: perfluorohexanesulfonic acid (PFHxS); perfluorononanoic acid (PFNA); perfluorooctane sulfonate (PFOS); perfluorooctanoic acid (PFOA)

Association between PFAS and Cognitive Outcomes at 1, 2 and 3 Years

There were no significant associations or apparent trends between PFAS concentrations and MDI or PDI at 1 year of age in unadjusted (Table S3) or adjusted (Table 4) analyses. At 2 and 3 years of age, in general, increases in PFAS concentrations were associated with higher MDI scores, for the combined male and female data. In unadjusted analyses, these associations with MDI were significant for PFOA (β=4.74, 95% CI: 0.88, 8.60) and PC1 (β=1.44, 95% CI: 0.30, 2.58) at 3 years and for PFHxS at both 2 (β=4.29, 95% CI: 1.27, 7.31) and 3 years (β=4.87, 95% CI: 2.13, 7.61) (Table S3). In adjusted analyses, these findings were similar but slightly attenuated (β=3.93, 95% CI: 0.08, 7.77 for PFOA; β=3.30, 95% CI: 0.70, 5.90 for PFHxS; and β=1.23, 95% CI: 0.13, 2.32 for PC1 at 3 years) and no longer significant for PFHxS at 2 years (Table 4). In unadjusted analyses, there were no interactions by sex at 1, 2 or 3 years. In adjusted analyses, at 2 years, there appeared to be a trend of stronger positive associations between PFAS and MDI for females compared to males. This sex-specific trend was significant only for PFOS (P interaction = 0.04) (Figure 2). There were no significant associations between PFAS and PDI at 1 or 2 years. However, at 3 years, PC2 was associated with lower PDI scores in both unadjusted (β=−2.28, 95% CI: −4.53, −0.03) and adjusted models (β=−3.51, 95% CI: −6.01, −1.02). PC2 reflects higher PFNA and lower PFOA and PFHxS (Table 3); therefore, the associations between PC2 and PDI suggest a positive relationship for PFOA and PFHxS with PDI and a negative relationship for PFNA.

Table 4.

Mean Difference (95% Confidence Interval) in Cognitive Outcomes Assessed using Bayley Scales of Infant Development (BSID-II) per Log Unit Increase in Perfluoroalkyl Substances at 1, 2 and 3 Years (n=302)

Year 1 Year 2 Year 3
MDI PDI MDI PDI MDI PDI

PFOS
 Overall −0.61 (−3.17, 1.95) −0.07 (−4.56, 4.43) 2.36 (−1.23, 5.94) −1.34 (−4.26, 1.57) 1.96 (−1.24, 5.16) −0.55 (−5.34, 4.23)
 Female −0.88 (−4.33, 2.57) −0.17 (−6.05, 5.71) 5.52 (0.64, 10.4) −0.04 (−4.12, 4.04) 4.15 (−0.2, 8.5) −1.44 (−7.5, 4.62)
 Male −0.29 (−3.93, 3.35) 0.05 (−5.41, 5.51) −1.35 (−7.09, 4.39) −2.88 (−10.4, 4.67) −0.63 (−5.41, 4.15) 0.49 (−5.29, 6.27)
P−Interaction 0.8 0.95 0.04 0.33 0.12 0.59
PFOA
 Overall −1.10 (−3.83, 1.63) −1.05 (−6.02, 3.92) 1.26 (−2.64, 5.16) 0.23 (−3.27, 3.74) 3.93 (0.08, 7.77) 2.35 (−2.84, 7.54)
 Female −1.34 (−5.12, 2.44) −1.41 (−7.60, 4.78) 3.00 (−1.96, 7.96) 0.47 (−4.18, 5.12) 4.09 (−0.97, 9.15) 4.03 (−2.87, 10.9)
 Male −0.85 (−5.23, 3.53) −0.67 (−7.12, 5.78) −0.57 (−6.04, 4.90) −0.02 (−4.79, 4.75) 3.76 (−4.94, 12.46) 0.58 (−5.78, 6.94)
P−Interaction 0.85 0.84 0.29 0.88 0.92 0.41
PFNA
 Overall −0.14 (−2.30, 2.02) −0.43 (−4.00, 3.14) 1.71 (−1.78, 5.20) −1.62 (−4.18, 0.94) 2.05 (−0.80, 4.89) −2.10 (−6.01, 1.81)
 Female −0.38 (−2.91, 2.15) −0.14 (−4.28, 4.00) 2.15 (−2.01, 6.31) −1.23 (−4.44, 1.98) 2.38 (−0.87, 5.63) −2.77 (−7.32, 1.78)
 Male 0.23 (−2.43, 2.89) −0.89 (−5.85, 4.07) 1.02 (−3.4, 5.44) −2.24 (−8.25, 3.77) 1.51 (−2.96, 5.98) −1.04 (−6.19, 4.11)
P−Interaction 0.68 0.75 0.63 0.62 0.67 0.46
PFHxS
 Overall 0.20 (−2.06, 2.45) 0.23 (−3.26, 3.71) 1.71(−1.13, 4.54) 0.41 (−2.38, 3.21) 3.30 (0.70, 5.90) 2.620 (−1.270, 6.50)
 Female 0.03 (−2.71, 2.77) 0.11 (−3.75, 3.97) 2.46 (−0.93, 5.85) 0.58 (−2.75, 3.91) 2.39 (−1, 5.78) 2.19 (−2.28, 6.66)
 Male 0.45 (−2.69, 3.59) 0.38 (−4.94, 5.7) 0.62 (−3.82, 5.06) 0.17 (−3.92, 4.26) 4.62 (−5.08, 14.3) 3.23 (−5.13, 11.6)
P−Interaction 0.83 0.93 0.49 0.87 0.41 0.76
PC1
 Overall −0.17 (−1.06, 0.71) −0.12 (−1.70, 1.46) 0.80 (−0.49, 2.09) −0.26 (−1.30, 0.77) 1.23 (0.13, 2.32) 0.255 (−1.49, 2.00)
 Female −0.29 (−1.47, 0.89) −0.13 (−2.07, 1.81) 1.58 (−0.11, 3.27) 0.01 (−1.4, 1.42) 1.51 (0.04, 2.98) 0.09 (−2.07, 2.25)
 Male −0.03 (−1.35, 1.29) −0.12 (−2.23, 1.99) −0.13 (−1.98, 1.72) −0.58 (−2.82, 1.66) 0.89 (−1.11, 2.89) 0.45 (−1.66, 2.56)
P-Interaction 0.74 1.00 0.12 0.55 0.56 0.77
PC2
 Overall 0.09 (−1.54, 1.72) 0.02 (−2.38, 2.42) 0.05 (−2.13, 2.23) −1.48 (−3.77, 0.81) −1.70 (−3.93, 0.54) −3.51 (−6.01, −1.02)
 Female 0.00 (−1.9, 1.9) 0.24 (−2.54, 3.02) 0.14 (−2.56, 2.84) −1.22 (−3.89, 1.45) −0.88 (−3.47, 1.71) −4.17 (−7.27, −1.07)
 Male 0.21 (−1.92, 2.34) −0.28 (−3.79, 3.23) −0.07 (−3.05, 2.91) −1.85 (−7.01, 3.31) −2.83 (−9.73, 4.07) −2.61 (−9.3, 4.08)
P-Interaction 0.86 0.78 0.91 0.69 0.23 0.44

Models adjusted for maternal age,material hardship, parity, pre-pregnancy BMI, maternal IQ, maternal race, maternal education, family smoking status, child age at testing, child’s gestational age at birth, maternal demoralization, trimester on 9/11, child’s sex, and child’s breastfeeding history.

Abbreviations: mental development index (MDI); perfluorohexanesulfonic acid (PFHxS); perfluorononanoic acid (PFNA); perfluorooctane sulfonate (PFOS); perfluorooctanoic acid (PFOA); principal component (PC); psychomotor development index (PDI).

Findings from complete case sensitivity analyses were similar (Table S1). Although associations between PFHxS and PC1 with MDI at 3 years were not significant as seen in adjusted imputed analyses, they were consistent in magnitude and direction. Further, the sex-specific trend of stronger positive associations between PFAS and MDI for females in adjusted imputed analyses was more apparent in complete case analyses. The significant interaction by sex for PFOS with MDI at 2 years was stronger (P- interaction <0.001) and also significant at 3 years (P-interaction = 0.03). In addition, there was a significant interaction by sex for PC1 and MDI at 2 years (P-interaction = 0.003).

Association between PFAS and Cognitive Outcomes at 4 and 6 Years

In unadjusted models, similar to findings observed for MDI at 3 years, there were positive associations between PFOA and PFHxS and cognitive outcomes at 4 and 6 years. These associations were significant for verbal and full scale IQ at 4 years, and just verbal IQ at 6 years (Table S4). Results were similar but attenuated and no longer significant in adjusted models (Table 5). In unadjusted models, PC2 was associated with significantly lower verbal IQ scores at 4 (β=−4.19, 95% CI: −6.40, −1.98) and 6 (β=−3.14, 95% CI: −5.55, −0.73) years (Table S4). PC2 was also associated with lower Full scale IQ scores (β=−2.93, 95% CI: −5.01, −0.85) at 4 years. In adjusted models, the association between PC2 and lower verbal IQ at 4 years was attenuated but remained significant (β=−2.67, 95% CI: −5.14, −0.20) (Table 5). This finding is consistent with the positive associations between PFOA and PFHxS with MDI at 3 years, as PC2 reflects higher PFNA but lower PFOA and PFHxS. In adjusted models, similar to findings for MDI at 2 and 3 years, there appeared to be a sex-specific trend between PFAS exposure and some cognitive outcomes (verbal and full scale IQ only) at 4 and 6 years, suggesting stronger positive associations for females compared to males (Table 5). For example, a log-unit increase in PFOA in adjusted models was associated with significantly higher verbal IQ scores among females (β=5.97, 95% CI: 0.34, 11.6) but not males (β=1.92, 95% CI: −4.76, 11.6) at 4 years. Further, although the interaction was not significant, at 4 years, PFOS was associated with 2.60 (95% CI: −3.18, 8.38) and 1.13 (95% CI: −4.04, 6.3) points higher verbal and full scale IQ scores, respectively, for females, but 3.44 (95% CI: −12.7, 5.82) and 2.21 (−9.12, 4.70) points lower verbal and full scale IQ scores, respectively, for males. This interaction by sex between PFAS and verbal and full scale IQ scores at 4 years reached significance in complete case analyses for PFOS (P-interaction = 0.02 for verbal IQ), PFOA (P-interaction = 0.04 for verbal and full scale IQ) and PC1 (P-interaction = 0.01 for verbal IQ and 0.04 for full scale IQ) (Table S2). The significant negative association between PC2 and verbal IQ at 4 years in adjusted imputed analyses did not reach significance in complete case analyses but was similar in magnitude and direction.

Table 5.

Mean Difference (95% Confidence Interval) in Cognitive Outcomes Assessed using The Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence (WPPSI) per Log Unit Increase in Perfluoroalkyl Substances at 4 and 6 Years (n=302)

   Year 4    Year 6

Per Ver Full Per Ver Full

PFOS
 Overall −0.05 (−4.56, 4.46) −0.19 (−4.5, 4.12) −0.41 (−4.25, 3.43) 2.81 (−2.29, 7.91) 2.67 (−2.56, 7.9) 2.81 (−1.84, 7.46)
 Female −0.32 (−6.14, 5.5) 2.60 (−3.18, 8.38) 1.13 (−4.04, 6.30) 4.20 (−1.9, 10.3) 4.15 (−2.08, 10.38) 4.49 (−1.14, 10.1)
 Male 0.27 (−5.20, 5.74) −3.44 (−12.7, 5.82) −2.21 (−9.12, 4.70) 1.16 (−5.51, 7.83) 0.90 (−5.83, 7.63) 0.80 (−5.02, 6.62)
P-Interaction 0.87 0.10 0.30 0.41 0.39 0.27
PFOA
 Overall 0.64 (−4.12, 5.4) 3.99 (−0.34, 8.32) 2.50 (−1.15, 6.15) −1.37 (−6.25, 3.51) 3.02 (−2.49, 8.53) 0.87 (−3.89, 5.63)
 Female 1.58 (−4.61, 7.77) 5.97 (0.34, 11.6) 4.17 (−0.87, 9.21) −0.28 (−6.2, 5.64) 4.03 (−2.93, 10.99) 2.23 (−3.73, 8.19)
 Male −0.35 (−6.69, 5.99) 1.92 (−4.76, 8.60) 0.75 (−4.3, 5.8) −2.50 (−11.3, 6.28) 1.95 (−5.49, 9.39) −0.56 (−6.82, 5.7)
P-Interaction 0.64 0.29 0.33 0.59 0.61 0.45
PFNA
 Overall −0.35 (−3.96, 3.26) 0.21 (−3.18, 3.6) −0.23 (−3.37, 2.91) 1.42 (−2.32, 5.16) 1.53 (−2.55, 5.61) 1.55 (−2.12, 5.22)
 Female −0.61 (−4.84, 3.62) 0.95 (−3.11, 5.01) 0.1 (−3.62, 3.82) 1.82 (−2.47, 6.11) 1.83 (−2.72, 6.38) 1.98 (−2.21, 6.17)
 Male 0.07 (−4.57, 4.71) −0.99 (−5.92, 3.94) −0.76 (−5.14, 3.62) 0.79 (−3.78, 5.36) 1.06 (−4.08, 6.2) 0.85 (−3.56, 5.26)
P-Interaction 0.79 0.43 0.70 0.66 0.75 0.61
PFHxS
 Overall −2.45 (−5.9, 1) 2.35 (−0.98, 5.68) 0.04 (−2.78, 2.86) −0.63 (−4.45, 3.19) 0.24 (−3.48, 3.96) −0.34 (−3.71, 3.03)
 Female −2.61 (−6.75, 1.53) 2.90 (−1.24, 7.04) 0.35 (−3.20, 3.90) 0.27 (−3.9, 4.44) 0.88 (−3.49, 5.25) 0.57 (−3.13, 4.27)
 Male −2.24 (−9.44, 4.96) 1.56 (−3.90, 7.02) −0.41 (−4.84, 4.02) −1.92 (−8.97, 5.13) −0.7 (−6.46, 5.06) −1.64 (−8.07, 4.79)
P-Interaction 0.90 0.66 0.78 0.46 0.62 0.44
PC1
 Overall −0.28 (−1.77, 1.21) 0.66 (−0.77, 2.09) 0.16 (−1.07, 1.39) 0.30 (−1.39, 1.99) 0.81 (−0.93, 2.55) 0.56 (−1.01, 2.13)
 Female −0.33 (−2.21, 1.55) 1.42 (−0.46, 3.30) 0.60 (−1.05, 2.25) 0.79 (−1.19, 2.77) 1.24 (−0.82, 3.30) 1.11 (−0.73, 2.95)
 Male −0.20 (−2.30, 1.90) −0.24 (−2.31, 1.83) −0.35 (−2.35, 1.65) −0.28 (−2.72, 2.16) 0.29 (−1.94, 2.52) −0.10 (−2.24, 2.04)
P-Interaction 0.91 0.17 0.37 0.39 0.46 0.29
PC2
 Overall 0.89 (−1.93, 3.71) −2.67 (−5.14, −0.20) −1.16 (−3.37, 1.05) 2.23 (−0.46, 4.92) 0.16 (−2.7, 3.02) 1.31 (−1.28, 3.9)
 Female 0.60 (−2.83, 4.03) −2.38 (−5.34, 0.58) −1.15 (−3.91, 1.61) 2.10 (−1.04, 5.24) 0.19 (−3.14, 3.52) 1.24 (−1.8, 4.28)
 Male 1.29 (−2.92, 5.50) −3.09 (−10.7, 4.49) −1.19 (−5.38, 3.00) 2.42 (−3.13, 7.97) 0.13 (−3.29, 3.55) 1.42 (−2.49, 5.33)
P-Interaction 0.76 0.72 0.98 0.87 0.98 0.92

Models adjusted for maternal age, material hardship, parity, pre-pregnancy BMI, maternal IQ, maternal race, maternal education, family smoking status, child’s age at testing, child’s gestational age at birth, maternal demoralization, trimester on 9/11, child’s sex, and child’s breastfeeding history.

Abbreviations: full IQ (Full); perfluorohexanesulfonic acid (PFHxS); perfluorononanoic acid (PFNA); perfluorooctane sulfonate (PFOS); perfluorooctanoic acid (PFOA); performance IQ (Per); principal component (PC); verbal IQ (Ver)

DISCUSSION

In this prospective birth cohort of 302 mother-child dyads recruited in New York City, we evaluated the association between prenatal PFAS exposure and cognitive outcomes in children at 1, 2, 3, 4 and 6 years of age. We found a trend of generally higher MDI scores at 2 and 3 years with higher PFAS exposure, reaching significance at 3 years for PFOA, PFHxS and for our summary measure of higher overall PFAS (PC1). We also found evidence of sex-specific relationships between PFAS and cognition, with results suggesting that higher PFAS may be associated with higher scores for some measures of cognition among females but not males. We observed significantly higher cognitive scores with increases in PFOS at 2 years and with increases in PFOA at 4 years, for females but not males. An interaction by sex was significant between PFOS and MDI at 2 years. We also found some evidence of divergent associations across PFAS, such that there were significant associations between the second PFAS principal component and PDI at 3 years and verbal IQ at 4 years, reflecting a positive relationship for PFOA and PFHxS and an inverse relationship for PFNA.

PFAS are widespread and stable environmental toxicants, highly resistant to biotransformation and environmental degradation. Their ability to cross the placental barrier has led to increasing interest in understanding the effect of exposure to PFAS on the developing fetus. Epidemiologic studies evaluating the association between prenatal PFAS exposure and neurodevelopment have been limited and inconsistent. Two studies have reported significant inverse associations between prenatal PFAS and childhood cognitive outcomes. A study from Taiwan21 found higher PFNA and PFUNDA to be associated with lower IQ at 5 and 8 years, and a study from Japan20 found higher PFOA to be associated with lower MDI in females at 6 months but not 18 months. In contrast, a recent study in the Danish Birth Cohort evaluated seven different PFAS and found an association between PFNA and higher verbal IQ among 5- year olds in the overall population and in females in sex-stratified analyses.24 Similarly, US-based studies have reported positive associations of prenatal PFOA22,23 and PFNA23 with cognitive outcomes, with evidence of sex-specific relationships for PFOA and PFOS. Another US-based study reported variable associations across PFAS compounds, cognitive measures and timing of cognitive assessments (early versus mid-childhood).28 For example, for PFNA, there was a general trend of better cognition scores with increasing exposure; however, for PFOA, greater exposure was associated with higher design memory and composite scores, but lower verbal IQ and visual-motor scores. Studies evaluating PFAS with other measures of neurodevelopment (i.e., non-IQ measures) have also reported inconsistent findings. A study conducted among British girls found both positive and negative associations of PFOS, PFOA and PFNA with early communication development at 15 and 38 months, depending on the outcome measured and age of the mother, but only positive associations reported for PFHxS.25 In addition, a recent US study reported better reading skills at 5 and 8 years with higher prenatal exposure to PFOA, PFOS and PFNA.27

Our findings are reflective of the apparent complicated nature of the relationship between PFAS and neurodevelopment. The significant associations we observed between PFOA, PFHxS and PC1 (i.e., higher PFAS exposure overall) and better MDI scores at 3 years, are inconsistent with the results of studies from Taiwan and Japan, but consistent with the studies outlined above that have reported surprising protective associations between various PFAS and cognitive outcomes. The mechanism by which PFAS could exert neuroprotective effects is not clear, however, their activation of human peroxisome proliferator-activated receptor (PPAR) alpha and PPARγ in several experimental studies5153 has been identified as one potential pathway, since other PPARγ agonists have been shown to be neuroprotective.5456 Still, in vivo and in vitro models have also suggested PFAS have neurotoxic potential through effects on the cholinergic system13,16,57, neuronal differentiation58, protein levels necessary for proper brain development12,17 and thyroid homeostasis19.

Inconsistencies across studies could be due to differences in study populations including ethnicity and exposure levels. For example, the populations in the studies from Japan20 and Taiwan21 were Asian, whereas the populations in other studies were all24,25, or predominately22,23, Caucasian. Further, the range of exposure for PFOS and PFOA across studies was substantial: the highest reported median PFOA concentration22 was 36x the level of the lowest reported median.20 In addition, associations between PFAS and cognitive outcomes varied in our study based on age and type of assessment used to measure cognition and likely contribute to differences observed in findings across studies. Inconsistent findings may also result from biological mechanisms that possibly vary by sex and PFAS compounds. For example, one indicated neurotoxic mechanism of PFAS in thyroid homeostasis is through competitive binding to the human thyroid hormone transport protein, transthyretin (TTR), and PFAS TTR binding potency has been shown to differ across PFAS compounds.58,59 Our finding of a significant negative association between PC2 and PDI at 3 years and verbal scores at 4 years, suggesting a positive relationship with neurodevelopment for PFHxS and PFOA but negative for PFNA, adds evidence that neurotoxic mechanisms of action may differ across PFAS. Further, the sex-specific trends we observed of better cognitive outcomes with higher prenatal PFAS among females but not males suggest that PFAS neurotoxic mechanisms may also differ by sex. Our sex-specific findings are consistent with two recent studies that also found evidence of a positive relationship between PFAS and cognitive scores in females but not males. Still, these studies observed these trends for PFNA,24 PFOA23 and PFHxS23 , whereas our only significant interaction by sex was for PFOS. Sex-specific associations have also been observed between prenatal PFAS exposure and childhood behavioral outcomes in epidemiologic22,60 and animal11,14 studies. Mechanistic evidence on the sex-specific associations between PFAS and cognitive outcomes is currently unexplored. However, DNA methylation may be one pathway: in a Faroese birth cohort, in males only, cord blood PFOS was associated with DNA methylation changes that were predicted to dysregulate genes involved in nervous system development.61 PFAS toxicokinetic differences between males and females may also play a role. Animal studies have consistently shown shorter PFAS half-lives and lower accumulation in tissues in females compared with males.6264 Analysis of NHANES data revealing higher concentrations of all PFAS evaluated (PFOS, PFOA, PFNA and PFHxS) in males versus females, suggest these trends may also be true in humans.65 Still these findings do not provide evidence for the positive association between PFAS and cognitive outcomes. As discussed earlier, the ability of PFAS to activate PPARγ has been identified as one pathway through which PFAS could exert neuroprotective effects. Clinical trials have reported sex-differences in the efficacy of PPARγ agonist treatments, possibly due to sex-specific expression of PPARγ, with greater effects (in response to diabetes) seen among females.66,67 In theory, PPARγ therefore provides a potential mechanistic link between PFAS and positive cognitive outcomes in females, however, in vivo research on the mediating role of PPARγ in the sex-specific relationship between PFAS and cognitive outcomes is needed to test this hypothesis.

This study population is unique in that participants were initially recruited to evaluate the effects of the 9/11 event on pregnancy outcomes and child development. The collapse of the WTC resulted in the generation of thousands of tons of toxic chemicals dispersed across the surrounding area, with fires that burned for three months following the disaster.68 PFAS were a likely component of the toxic plume caused by the WTC, given their use as surfactants and stain-resistant coatings on numerous products that may been involved in the WTC collapse and response (e.g., carpets, food packaging, textiles, leather and fire-fighting foam).1 Indeed, proximity to the 9/11 site has been reported previously to be associated with higher cord blood PFAS levels in this population (prenatally) 40 and other WTC-exposed groups.69,70 However, the range of prenatal exposures in this population is similar to other US-based studies,39,71 and findings should therefore be generalizable to other multi-ethnic US populations. Despite the strength of participants in this study being drawn from a well-designed, rich longitudinal cohort, with information on many important confounders, our study has limitations that should be considered when interpreting findings, notably, the small sample size and substantial loss to follow-up. Still, MICE is an established statistical imputation technique shown to provide unbiased estimates when missing data are missing at random, including dealing with missing outcome data due to loss to follow-up in longitudinal studies.72 Further, the validity of multiple imputation has been reported to have less to do with the fraction of data missing, and more on the appropriate inclusion of predictors of missingness.73 In addition, due to the number of regressions run (multiple exposure variables, multiple outcome variables, and five different time points), we cannot exclude the possibility of spurious significant associations resulting from multiple comparisons.

CONCLUSIONS

Our findings suggest sex-specific associations between prenatal PFAS and childhood neurodevelopment, with certain positive associations seen among females but not males. Further, our principal component analysis provided evidence of divergent neurotoxic findings across PFAS compounds, with positive associations observed for PFOA and PFHxS, but negative associations observed for PFNA, using a psychomotor-based scale at 3 years and verbal scores at 4. Our results highlight the complex and inconsistent relationships between prenatal PFAS exposures and childhood neurodevelopment. Given the large amount of missing data, as well as the number of statistical tests, these findings should be interpreted with caution. However, they emphasize the need for both experimental studies, to better elucidate the biological mechanisms behind these relationships, and large-scale epidemiologic studies with high retention to reduce any effects of missing data. Further, additional research should be dedicated to evaluating possible sex-specific associations between PFAS and neurodevelopment.

Supplementary Material

1

Highlights.

  • Perfluoralkyl substances (PFAS) are widespread environmental toxicants

  • PFAS cross the placenta, exposing the fetus during vulnerable developmental periods

  • Findings on prenatal PFAS exposure and child neurodevelopment are inconsistent

  • We found sex- and compound-specific trends for this relationship

Main Findings:

We found sex- and compound-specific relationships between prenatal PFAS exposures and childhood neurodevelopment.

Funding: This work was supported by grants 1U01-OH010714–01A1, 1U01-OH010394–01A1, 1U01- OH011299–01A1 from NIOSH; and the National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences grant ES09089.

Footnotes

Declaration of interests

The authors declare the following financial interests/personal relationships which may be considered as potential competing interests:

DISCLOSURE STATEMENT: The authors have nothing to disclose

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

REFERENCES

  • 1.US Environmental Protection Agency, 2017a. Technical Fact Sheet – perfluorooctane sulfonate (PFOS) and perfluorooctanoic acid (PFOA). Washington, DC: Retrieved from: https://www.epa.gov/sites/production/files/2017- 12/documents/ffrrofactsheet_contaminants_pfos_pfoa_11-20-17_508_0.pdf [Google Scholar]
  • 2.US Environmental Protection Agency, 2016a. Health Effects Support Document for Perfluorooctanoic Acid (PFOA). (EPA 822-R-16-003) Washington, DC: Retrieved from: https://www.epa.gov/sites/production/files/2016-05/documents/pfoa_hesd_final-plain.pdf [Google Scholar]
  • 3.US Environmental Protection Agency, 2016b. Health Effects Support Document for Perfluorooctane Sulfonate (PFOS). (EPA 822-R-16-002) Washington, DC: Retrieved from: https://www.epa.gov/sites/production/files/2016-05/documents/pfos_hesd_final_508.pdf [Google Scholar]
  • 4.US Environmental Protection Agency, 2016c. Lifetime health advisories and health effects support documents for perfluorooctanoic acid and perfluorooctane sulfonate federal register. (EPA-HQ-OW-2014-0138FRL- 9946-91-OW) Retrieved from: https://www.govinfo.gov/content/pkg/FR-2016-05-25/pdf/2016-12361.pdf [Google Scholar]
  • 5.US Environmental Protection Agency, 2017b. ACE: Biomonitoring - Perfluorochemicals (PFCs). Retrieved from: https://www.epa.gov/ace/ace-biomonitoring-perfluorochemicals-pfcs [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Trudel D, Horowitz L, Wormuth M, Scheringer M, Cousins IT, Hungerbuhler K. Estimating consumer exposure to PFOS and PFOA. Risk Anal. 2008;28(2):251–269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Fraser AJ, Webster TF, Watkins DJ, Nelson JW, Stapleton HM, Calafat AM, Kato K, Shoeib M, Vieira VM, McClean MD. Polyfluorinated compounds in serum linked to indoor air in office environments. Environ Sci Technol. 2012;46(2):1209–1215. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Olsen GW, Butenhoff JL, Zobel LR. Perfluoroalkyl chemicals and human fetal development: an epidemiologic review with clinical and toxicological perspectives. Reprod Toxicol. 2009;27(3–4):212–230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Bach CC, Bech BH, Brix N, Nohr EA, Bonde JP, Henriksen TB. Perfluoroalkyl and polyfluoroalkyl substances and human fetal growth: a systematic review. Crit Rev Toxicol. 2015;45(1):53–67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Lau C, Thibodeaux JR, Hanson RG, Narotsky MG, Rogers JM, Lindstrom AB, Strynar MJ. Effects of perfluorooctanoic acid exposure during pregnancy in the mouse. Toxicol Sci. 2006;90(2):510–518. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Fuentes S, Colomina MT, Vicens P, Domingo JL. Influence of maternal restraint stress on the long-lasting effects induced by prenatal exposure to perfluorooctane sulfonate (PFOS) in mice. Toxicol Lett. 2007;171(3):162–170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Johansson N, Eriksson P, Viberg H. Neonatal exposure to PFOS and PFOA in mice results in changes in proteins which are important for neuronal growth and synaptogenesis in the developing brain. Toxicol Sci. 2009;108(2):412–418. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Johansson N, Fredriksson A, Eriksson P. Neonatal exposure to perfluorooctane sulfonate (PFOS) and perfluorooctanoic acid (PFOA) causes neurobehavioural defects in adult mice. Neurotoxicology. 2008;29(1):160–169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Onishchenko N, Fischer C, Wan Ibrahim WN, Negri S, Spulber S, Cottica D, Ceccatelli S. Prenatal exposure to PFOS or PFOA alters motor function in mice in a sex-related manner. Neurotox Res. 2011;19(3):452–461. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Butenhoff JL, Ehresman DJ, Chang SC, Parker GA, Stump DG. Gestational and lactational exposure to potassium perfluorooctanesulfonate (K+PFOS) in rats: developmental neurotoxicity. Reprod Toxicol. 2009;27(3–4):319–330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Lau C, Thibodeaux JR, Hanson RG, Rogers JM, Grey BE, Stanton ME, Butenhoff JL, Stevenson LA. Exposure to perfluorooctane sulfonate during pregnancy in rat and mouse. II: postnatal evaluation. Toxicol Sci. 2003;74(2):382–392. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Liu X, Liu W, Jin Y, Yu W, Wang F, Liu L. Effect of gestational and lactational exposure to perfluorooctanesulfonate on calcium-dependent signaling molecules gene expression in rats’ hippocampus. Arch Toxicol. 2010;84(1):71–79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Ballesteros V, Costa O, Iniguez C, Fletcher T, Ballester F, Lopez-Espinosa MJ. Exposure to perfluoroalkyl substances and thyroid function in pregnant women and children: A systematic review of epidemiologic studies. Environ Int. 2017;99:15–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Mariussen E Neurotoxic effects of perfluoroalkylated compounds: mechanisms of action and environmental relevance. Arch Toxicol. 2012;86(9):1349–1367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Goudarzi H, Nakajima S, Ikeno T, Sasaki S, Kobayashi S, Miyashita C, Ito S, Araki A, Nakazawa H, Kishi R. Prenatal exposure to perfluorinated chemicals and neurodevelopment in early infancy: The Hokkaido Study. Sci Total Environ. 2016;541:1002–1010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Wang Y, Rogan WJ, Chen HY, Chen PC, Su PH, Chen HY, Wang SL. Prenatal exposure to perfluroalkyl substances and children’s IQ: The Taiwan maternal and infant cohort study. Int J Hyg Environ Health. 2015;218(7):639–644. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Stein CR, Savitz DA, Bellinger DC. Perfluorooctanoate and neuropsychological outcomes in children. Epidemiology. 2013;24(4):590–599. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Vuong AM, Yolton K, Xie C, Dietrich KN, Braun JM, Webster GM, Calafat AM, Lanphear BP, Chen A. Prenatal and childhood exposure to poly- and perfluoroalkyl substances (PFAS) and cognitive development in children at age 8 years. Environ Res. 2019;172:242–248. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Liew Z, Ritz B, Bach CC, Asarnow RF, Bech BH, Nohr EA, Bossi R, Henriksen TB, Bonefeld-Jorgensen EC, Olsen J. Prenatal Exposure to Perfluoroalkyl Substances and IQ Scores at Age 5; a Study in the Danish National Birth Cohort. Environ Health Perspect. 2018;126(6):067004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Jeddy Z, Hartman TJ, Taylor EV, Poteete C, Kordas K. Prenatal concentrations of Perfluoroalkyl substances and early communication development in British girls. Early Hum Dev. 2017;109:15–20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Fei C, McLaughlin JK, Tarone RE, Olsen J. Fetal growth indicators and perfluorinated chemicals: a study in the Danish National Birth Cohort. Am J Epidemiol. 2008;168(1):66–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Zhang H, Yolton K, Webster GM, Ye X, Calafat AM, Dietrich KN, Xu Y, Xie C, Braun JM, Lanphear BP, Chen A. Prenatal and childhood perfluoroalkyl substances exposures and children’s reading skills at ages 5 and 8years. Environ Int. 2018;111:224–231. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Harris MH, Oken E, Rifas-Shiman SL, Calafat AM, Ye X, Bellinger DC, Webster TF, White RF, Sagiv SK. Prenatal and childhood exposure to per- and polyfluoroalkyl substances (PFASs) and child cognition. Environ Int. 2018;115:358–369. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Lederman SA, Rauh V, Weiss L, Stein JL, Hoepner LA, Becker M, Perera FP. The effects of the World Trade Center event on birth outcomes among term deliveries at three lower Manhattan hospitals. Environ Health Perspect. 2004;112(17):1772–1778. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Vernon SW, Roberts RE. Measuring nonspecific psychological distress and other dimensions of psychopathology. Further observations on the problem. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1981;38(11):1239–1247. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Dohrenwend BP, Shrout PE, Egri G, Mendelsohn FS. Nonspecific psychological distress and other dimensions of psychopathology. Measures for use in the general population. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1980;37(11):1229–1236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Dohrenwend BP, Dohrenwend BS, Warheit GJ, Bartlett GS, Goldsteen RL, Goldsteen K, Martin JL. Stress in the community: a report to the President’s Commission on the Accident at Three Mile Island. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1981;365:159–174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Brown L, Sherbenou R, Johnsen S. Test of Non-Verbal Intelligence, Third Edition Examiner’s Manual; Austin, TX: 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Bayley N Bayley Scales of Infant Development, Second Edition San Antonio, TX: 1993. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Wechsler D Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-Revised San Antonio, TX: 1989. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Kannan K, Corsolini S, Falandysz J, Fillmann G, Kumar KS, Loganathan BG, Mohd MA, Olivero J, Van Wouwe N, Yang JH, Aldoust KM. Perfluorooctanesulfonate and related fluorochemicals in human blood from several countries. Environ Sci Technol. 2004;38(17):4489–4495. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Taniyasu S, Kannan K, So MK, Gulkowska A, Sinclair E, Okazawa T, Yamashita N. Analysis of fluorotelomer alcohols, fluorotelomer acids, and short- and long-chain perfluorinated acids in water and biota. J Chromatogr A. 2005;1093(1–2):89–97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Sakr CJ, Kreckmann KH, Green JW, Gillies PJ, Reynolds JL, Leonard RC. Cross-sectional study of lipids and liver enzymes related to a serum biomarker of exposure (ammonium perfluorooctanoate or APFO) as part of a general health survey in a cohort of occupationally exposed workers. J Occup Environ Med. 2007;49(10):1086–1096. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Apelberg BJ, Goldman LR, Calafat AM, Herbstman JB, Kuklenyik Z, Heidler J, Needham LL, Halden RU, Witter FR. Determinants of fetal exposure to polyfluoroalkyl compounds in Baltimore, Maryland. Environ Sci Technol. 2007;41(11):3891–3897. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Spratlen MJ, Perera FP, Lederman SA, Robinson M, Kannan K, Trasande L, Herbstman J. Cord blood perfluoroalkyl substances in mothers exposed to the World Trade Center disaster during pregnancy. Environ Pollut. 2019;246:482–490. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Kataria A, Trachtman H, Malaga-Dieguez L, Trasande L. Association between perfluoroalkyl acids and kidney function in a cross-sectional study of adolescents. Environ Health. 2015;14:89. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Hoffman K, Webster TF, Weisskopf MG, Weinberg J, Vieira VM. Exposure to polyfluoroalkyl chemicals and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder in U.S. children 12–15 years of age. Environ Health Perspect. 2010;118(12):1762–1767. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Birgisdottir BE, Knutsen HK, Haugen M, Gjelstad IM, Jenssen MT, Ellingsen DG, Thomassen Y, Alexander J, Meltzer HM, Brantsaeter AL. Essential and toxic element concentrations in blood and urine and their associations with diet: results from a Norwegian population study including high-consumers of seafood and game. Sci Total Environ. 2013;463–464:836–844. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Kaiser H The application of electronic computer to factor analysis. Educational and Psychological Measurement. 1960;20:141–151. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Kaiser H The varimax criterion for analytic rotation in factor analysis. Psychometrika. 1958;23:187–200. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Schafer JL, Graham JW. Missing data: our view of the state of the art. Psychol Methods. 2002;7(2):147–177. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Morisot A, Bessaoud F, Landais P, Rebillard X, Tretarre B, Daures JP. Prostate cancer: net survival and cause-specific survival rates after multiple imputation. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2015;15:54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Hayati Rezvan P, Lee KJ, Simpson JA. The rise of multiple imputation: a review of the reporting and implementation of the method in medical research. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2015;15:30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Moons KG, Donders RA, Stijnen T, Harrell FE Jr, . Using the outcome for imputation of missing predictor values was preferred. J Clin Epidemiol. 2006;59(10):1092–1101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Collins LM, Schafer JL, Kam CM. A comparison of inclusive and restrictive strategies in modern missing data procedures. Psychol Methods. 2001;6(4):330–351. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Maloney EK, Waxman DJ. trans-Activation of PPARalpha and PPARgamma by structurally diverse environmental chemicals. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol. 1999;161(2):209–218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Takacs ML, Abbott BD. Activation of mouse and human peroxisome proliferator-activated receptors (alpha, beta/delta, gamma) by perfluorooctanoic acid and perfluorooctane sulfonate. Toxicol Sci. 2007;95(1):108–117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Vanden Heuvel JP, Thompson JT, Frame SR, Gillies PJ. Differential activation of nuclear receptors by perfluorinated fatty acid analogs and natural fatty acids: a comparison of human, mouse, and rat peroxisome proliferator-activated receptor-alpha, -beta, and -gamma, liver X receptor-beta, and retinoid X receptor-alpha. Toxicol Sci. 2006;92(2):476–489. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Power MC, Webster TF, Baccarelli AA, Weisskopf MG. Cross-sectional association between polyfluoroalkyl chemicals and cognitive limitation in the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. Neuroepidemiology. 2013;40(2):125–132. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Watson GS, Cholerton BA, Reger MA, Baker LD, Plymate SR, Asthana S, Fishel MA, Kulstad JJ, Green PS, Cook DG, Kahn SE, Keeling ML, Craft S. Preserved cognition in patients with early Alzheimer disease and amnestic mild cognitive impairment during treatment with rosiglitazone: a preliminary study. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2005;13(11):950–958. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Liu J, Wang LN, Jia JP. Peroxisome proliferator-activated receptor-gamma agonists for Alzheimer’s disease and amnestic mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review and meta- analysis. Drugs Aging. 2015;32(1):57–65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Viberg H, Lee I, Eriksson P. Adult dose-dependent behavioral and cognitive disturbances after a single neonatal PFHxS dose. Toxicology. 2013;304:185–191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Slotkin TA, MacKillop EA, Melnick RL, Thayer KA, Seidler FJ. Developmental neurotoxicity of perfluorinated chemicals modeled in vitro. Environ Health Perspect. 2008;116(6):716–722. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Weiss JM, Andersson PL, Lamoree MH, Leonards PE, van Leeuwen SP, Hamers T. Competitive binding of poly- and perfluorinated compounds to the thyroid hormone transport protein transthyretin. Toxicol Sci. 2009;109(2):206–216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Oulhote Y, Steuerwald U, Debes F, Weihe P, Grandjean P. Behavioral difficulties in 7-year old children in relation to developmental exposure to perfluorinated alkyl substances. Environ Int. 2016;97:237–245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Leung YK, Ouyang B, Niu L, Xie C, Ying J, Medvedovic M, Chen A, Weihe P, Valvi D, Grandjean P, Ho SM. Identification of sex-specific DNA methylation changes driven by specific chemicals in cord blood in a Faroese birth cohort. Epigenetics. 2018;13(3):290–300. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Khazaee M, Guardian MGE, Aga DS, Ng CA. Impacts of sex and exposure duration on gene expression in zebrafish following perfluorooctane sulfonate (PFOS) exposure. Environ Toxicol Chem. 2019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Dzierlenga AL, Robinson VG, Waidyanatha S, DeVito MJ, Eifrid MA, Gibbs ST, Granville CA, Blystone CR. Toxicokinetics of perfluorohexanoic acid (PFHxA), perfluorooctanoic acid (PFOA) and perfluorodecanoic acid (PFDA) in male and female Hsd:Sprague dawley SD rats following intravenous or gavage administration. Xenobiotica. 2019:1–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Huang MC, Dzierlenga AL, Robinson VG, Waidyanatha S, DeVito MJ, Eifrid MA, Granville CA, Gibbs ST, Blystone CR. Toxicokinetics of perfluorobutane sulfonate (PFBS), perfluorohexane-1- sulphonic acid (PFHxS), and perfluorooctane sulfonic acid (PFOS) in male and female Hsd:Sprague Dawley SD rats after intravenous and gavage administration. Toxicol Rep. 2019;6:645–655. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Kato K, Wong LY, Jia LT, Kuklenyik Z, Calafat AM. Trends in exposure to polyfluoroalkyl chemicals in the U.S. Population: 1999–2008. Environ Sci Technol. 2011;45(19):8037–8045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.ACTOS. Indianapolis, IN1999. [Google Scholar]
  • 67.AVANDIA, Prescribing Information. Research Triangel Park, NC: 2007. [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Landrigan PJ, Lioy PJ, Thurston G, Berkowitz G, Chen LC, Chillrud SN, Gavett SH, Georgopoulos PG, Geyh AS, Levin S, Perera F, Rappaport SM, Small C, Group NWTCW. Health and environmental consequences of the world trade center disaster. Environ Health Perspect. 2004;112(6):731–739. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Trasande L, Koshy TT, Gilbert J, Burdine LK, Attina TM, Ghassabian A, Honda M, Marmor M, Chu DB, Han X, Shao Y, Kannan K. Serum perfluoroalkyl substances in children exposed to the world trade center disaster. Environ Res. 2017;154:212–221. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Tao L, Kannan K, Aldous KM, Mauer MP, Eadon GA. Biomonitoring of perfluorochemicals in plasma of New York State personnel responding to the World Trade Center disaster. Environ Sci Technol. 2008;42(9):3472–3478. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Kato K, Wong LY, Chen A, Dunbar C, Webster GM, Lanphear BP, Calafat AM. Changes in serum concentrations of maternal poly- and perfluoroalkyl substances over the course of pregnancy and predictors of exposure in a multiethnic cohort of Cincinnati, Ohio pregnant women during 2003–2006. Environ Sci Technol. 2014;48(16):9600–9608. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Rawlings AM, Sang Y, Sharrett AR, Coresh J, Griswold M, Kucharska-Newton AM, Palta P, Wruck LM, Gross AL, Deal JA, Power MC, Bandeen-Roche KJ. Multiple imputation of cognitive performance as a repeatedly measured outcome. Eur J Epidemiol. 2017;32(1):55–66. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Madley-Dowd P, Hughes R, Tilling K, Heron J. The proportion of missing data should not be used to guide decisions on multiple imputation. J Clin Epidemiol. 2019;110:63–73. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

1

RESOURCES