Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2020 Jul 17.
Published in final edited form as: Dev Psychopathol. 2019 Dec;31(5):1911–1921. doi: 10.1017/S0954579419000828

Trajectories and Predictors of Children’s Early-Starting Conduct Problems: Child, Family, Genetic, and Intervention Effects

Daniel S Shaw 1, Chardée A Galán 1, Kathryn Lemery-Chalfant 2, Thomas J Dishion 2, Kit K Elam 2, Melvin N Wilson 3, Frances Gardner 4
PMCID: PMC7366854  NIHMSID: NIHMS1604183  PMID: 31370912

Abstract

Several research teams have previously traced patterns of emerging conduct problems (CP) from early or middle childhood. The current study expands on this previous literature by using a genetically-informed, experimental, and long-term longitudinal design to examine trajectories of early-emerging conduct problems and early childhood discriminators of such patterns from the toddler period to adolescence. The sample represents a cohort of 731 toddlers and diverse families recruited based on socioeconomic, child, and family risk, varying in urbanicity and assessed on nine occasions between ages 2 and 14. In addition to examining child, family, and community level discriminators of patterns of emerging conduct problems, we were able to account for genetic susceptibility using polygenic scores and the study’s experimental design to determine whether random assignment to the Family Check-Up (FCU) discriminated trajectory groups. In addition, in accord with differential susceptibility theory, we tested whether the effects of the FCU were stronger for those children with higher genetic susceptibility. Results augmented previous findings documenting the influence of child (inhibitory control [IC], gender) and family (harsh parenting, parental depression, and educational attainment) risk. In addition, children in the FCU were overrepresented in the persistent low versus persistent high CP group, but such direct effects were qualified by an interaction between the intervention and genetic susceptibility that was consistent with differential susceptibility. Implications are discussed for early identification and specifically, prevention efforts addressing early child and family risk.

Keywords: behavioral genetics, conduct disorder, early intervention, maternal depression


Research on the development of antisocial behavior (AB) is important because of the direct cost of AB to society not only in terms of damaged property and disruption of normal patterns of living but also because of the difficulty of treating delinquent youth and the potential emergence of later adult criminality, substance use, and other disorders (Odgers et al., 2008; Shaw, Hyde, & Brennan, 2012). Studies suggest that most antisocial youth can be classified into one of two groups: youth with onset in early childhood (i.e., early-starters), and youth with an onset in adolescence (i.e., late-starters). Children with the early-starting pattern of conduct problems (CP) have been found to show high rates of AB in the elementary school years and beyond (e.g., Campbell, Pierce, Moore, Marakovitz, & Newby, 1996; Shawet al., 2012) and a higher risk of continued AB and other problem behaviors in adulthood relative to late-starters (Moffitt, Caspi, Harrington, & Milne, 2002). This is particularly the case for boys, for whom early patterns of CP take on more serious forms of AB (Brame, Nagin, & Tremblay, 2001; Stevenson & Goodman, 2001).

In the past two decades investigators have employed person-oriented, growth mixture modeling to identify different patterns of growth and factors that discriminate trajectories of early-emerging CP (NICHD, 2004; Shaw, Gilliom, Ingoldsby, & Nagin, 2003; Tremblay et al., 2004). However, growth mixture modeling studies have been somewhat limited by focusing primarily on boys, using normative samples, and typically relying on passive, nongenetically-informed, longitudinal designs. The current study seeks to take advantage of an experimental, genetically-informed, longitudinal trial using a sample of 731 low-income toddlers screened based on socioeconomic, family, and child risk.

Developmental Transitions: The Toddler Period

Our current theoretical perspective emphasizes the unfolding of disruptive behavior during key developmental periods, accounting for critical developmental challenges for children and parents (Shaw & Bell, 1993). These key developmental transitions are marked by biological transformations in the child’s cognitive and physical functioning that have critical implications for the child’s socioemotional functioning and present challenges to parents coping with their child’s newfound status (Shaw et al., 2012). The toddler period represents one of these critical developmental transitions, marked by the child’s increased physical mobility but immature cognitive and regulatory abilities. Stressors generated from individual differences in child characteristics, parental well-being and caregiving skills, as well as community-level factors, might be exacerbated during the toddler period when parents and children are often struggling in negotiating a toddler’s increasing physical mobility and need for greater autonomy. In part, because of stressors associated with the “terrible twos,” recent research has suggested that toddlerhood is an age period when parents are open to feedback and implementing more effective parenting management strategies (Dishion et al., 2008; Shaw, Dishion, Supplee, Gardner, & Arnds, 2006).

Within the family, aspects of caregiving have been highlighted as central causal factors in the development of early-starting CP, as postulated by social learning and attachment theorists (Greenberg & Speltz, 1988; Patterson, Reid, & Dishion, 1992). From a social learning perspective, parenting management practices that model and reinforce disruptive behavior are hypothesized to be associated with increasingly frequent and severe CP that begin during the terrible twos and escalate during the preschool and school-age years (Campbell et al., 1996; Shaw et al., 1998). Conversely, parents who demonstrate high levels of responsivity, involvement, and/or proactive anticipation during early childhood have children with low levels of early-starting CP (Dishion et al., 2008; Gardner et al., 2007; Wakschlag & Hans, 1999). However, longitudinal follow-up typically has been limited to the preschool or early school-age period, with only a few studies on parenting and antisocial outcomes that have spanned from early childhood to adolescence (Aguilar, Sroufe, Egeland, & Carlson, 2000; Caspi et al., 2002; Fergusson & Woodward, 1999; Shaw et al., 2012).

Aside from caregiving quality, perhaps the most consistent family predictor of problem behavior during early childhood has been maternal depression, including both clinical and subclinical symptoms (Goodman et al., 2011). Maternal depression is thought to increase risk of child CP directly through genetic transmission and compromised caregiving quality, the latter by increasing harshness and critical behavior but also reducing involvement and activity level with the child. Results from a plethora of studies document associations between maternal depressive symptoms and early-starting CP (Aguilar et al., 2000; Shaw et al., 2003, 2012; Webster-Stratton, 2000), relations that persist longitudinally through adolescence and young adulthood (Gilliam et al., 2015).

In terms of child characteristics, in addition to toddler levels of oppositional and aggressive behavior (Tremblay et al., 2004), negative emotionality (Sanson, Oberklaid, Pedlow, & Prior, 1991), language skills (Moffitt, 1990), and callous-unemotional behavior (Hyde et al., 2013; Willoughby, Mills-Koonce, Gottfredson, & Wagner, 2014), fearlessness or low behavioral inhibition (Shaw et al., 2003; Waller, Shaw, & Hyde, 2017) and IC (Olson, Choe, & Sameroff, 2017) have been consistently linked to early-starting CP. In both cases, fearlessness and low IC are thought to be directly associated with later CP and indirectly influential by eliciting more hostile and inconsistent care from caregivers.

In addition to child-level characteristics, meta-analytic findings from twin and adoption studies suggest that CP is moderately to highly heritable (Rhee & Waldman, 2002).

There is also extensive literature linking single candidate genes to aggression, violence, and other forms of CP through both direct and interactive associations with environmental risk (e.g., Burt & Mikolajewski, 2008). However, single genetic polymorphisms typically account for a small amount of variance in CP. Rather, complex traits and behavior are thought to have an underlying polygenic architecture in which multiple genetic variants account for differences in behavior (Wray et al., 2014). More clearly, polygenic risk scores aggregate across multiple single nucleotide polymorphism to create cumulative measures of genetic predisposition. Polygenic risk scores are often generated based on summary statistics from an existing genome-wide association study (GWAS). Accordingly, the current study capitalizes on advances in molecular genetic techniques by including a polygenic risk score created from a recent meta-GWAS of aggression in middle childhood (Pappa et al., 2016). Based on prior research suggesting that comparable polygenic risk scores based on the aforementioned meta-GWAS are predictive of early-starting CP (Elam, Chassin, & Pandika, 2018), we used a polygenic risk score for aggression to test whether it discriminated higher and more persistent trajectories of CP from early childhood through adolescence.

Outside of the family and the child, there are contextual factors associated with poverty that have been consistently linked to emerging child CP and more serious forms of AB (Brooks-Gunn, Duncan, & Aber, 1997; Sampson, 1997), particularly as children move into middle childhood and adolescence and experience greater autonomy from parents. However, as prior research with predominantly low-income samples has found neighborhood deprivation independently related to child CP for children as young as age 3 (Supplee, Unikel, & Shaw, 2007; Xue, Leventhal, Brooks-Gunn, & Earls, 2005), we investigated the possibility of effects for neighborhood deprivation in the current sample of low-income families beginning between children ages 2 and 3.

The Family Check-Up

One preventive intervention that has been used with some success during the toddler period, particularly in engaging families in nontraditional settings for preventive mental health, is the Family Check-Up (FCU; Dishion & Stormshak, 2007; Shaw et al., 2016). Consistent with this Special Issue’s focus on the contributions of Tom Dishion, the FCU was developed by Dishion (Dishion & Kavanaugh, 2003; Dishion & Stormshak, 2007) as a preventive intervention for emerging adolescents at high-risk for maladaptive outcomes. As noted in the Introduction to the Special Issue, the FCU likely will be one of Tom’s most lasting contributions to the fields of prevention science and more broadly, developmental psychopathology. The FCU emphasizes and supports parenting practices that are empirically linked to children’s growth in problem behavior. It was designed to motivate parents to maintain their skillful efforts to promote positive child adjustment and to engage in interventions services for parenting practices that need attention.

Subsequent to its use with adolescents and their families, Dishion and Shaw adapted the FCU for early childhood (Dishion et al., 2008; Shaw, Dishion, Supplee, Gardner, & Arnds, 2006) and tested its validity in Women, Infants, and Children Nutritional Supplement (WIC) programs in the US. In two independent samples using a randomized control trial design, families screened on the basis of socioeconomic, family (e.g., maternal depression), and child (e.g., conduct problems) risk were found to show improvements in parenting (Dishion et al., 2008) and reductions in later problem behavior during preschool (Dishion et al., 2008; Shaw et al., 2006). Using the larger of these two cohorts, the current sample has shown improvements in CP at home and school through middle childhood (Dishion et al., 2014; Shaw et al., 2016), with collateral effects on maternal depression and child emotional problems (Shaw, Dishion, Connell, Wilson, & Gardner, 2009) and co-occurring child CP and emotional problems (Connell et al., 2008).

Following up on prior research suggesting the FCU to be effective in preventing early-starting CP through the preschool (Dishion et al., 2008) and school-age periods (Dishion et al., 2014; Shaw et al. 2016), we sought to examine whether random assignment to the FCU would discriminate membership in more versus less elevated and persistent trajectories of CP. In addition, consistent with the premise of differential susceptibility theory (Belsky & Pluess, 2009), we were also interested in examining potential polygenic moderation of intervention effects, such that intervention might prove more efficacious for genetically susceptible individuals than others. Accordingly, we expected intervention effects to be most visible between genetically susceptible children in the intervention and the control groups, the latter of whom we expected to be at higher risk for persistently high trajectories of CP than other children in the control group.

The Current Study

In the current study, we sought to advance our understanding of early-starting patterns of CP and genetic, child, family, and neighborhood factors that discriminate such trajectories from one another. We expected that predictors of early-starting AB would be identifiable during early childhood from these multiple domains, discriminating persistently high from persistently low or decreasing trajectories of CP. We were also interested in whether previous variable-centered findings from this study documenting intervention effects on child CP would continue to be found following middle childhood (Dishion et al., 2008, 2014). Specifically, we expected those children in persistently low and initially high then desisting trajectories to be overrepresented in the FCU intervention group. In addition and consistent with a differential susceptibility perspective, we expected that any intention-to-treat intervention effects would be qualified by genetic susceptibility, such that those children with greater genetic susceptibility would respond more favorably to the intervention than those children at lower genetic susceptibility. A final goal was to validate parent-reported trajectory group patterns of CP to youth and teacher reports of more serious AB at age 14.

Methods

Participants

Participants included 731 caregiver–child dyads recruited between 2002 and 2003 from WIC programs in and around Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, Eugene, Oregon, and Charlottesville, Virginia (Dishion et al., 2008; Shaw et al., 2016). Families were invited to participate if they had a child between 2 years 0 months and 2 years 11 months of age, following a screening to ensure that they met study criteria by having family, socioeconomic, and/or child risk factors for future behavioral problems. Families were eligible to participate if they scored at least one standard deviation above the normative mean in at least two of the three domains of risk: (a) familial risk (e.g., maternal depressive symptoms; (b) socio-demographic risk (e.g., low parental education); and (c) child risk (e.g., CP).

Recruitment

Of the 1,666 families who had children in the appropriate age range and were approached at WIC programs across the three study sites, 879 met eligibility requirements (52% in Pittsburgh, 57% in Eugene, and 49% in Charlottesville). Before the first home assessment at age 2, families were randomly assigned to the FCU or control conditions, the latter receiving WIC services as usual. At the time of recruitment, primary caregivers across sites self-identified as European American (50%), African American (28%), biracial (13%), and other groups (9%; e.g., American Indian, Native Hawaiian), and 13.4% of the sample reported being Hispanic American. The primary caregivers who participated in the assessment tasks at age 2 were predominantly biological mothers (96%), and in all other cases, were biological fathers, grandmothers, or other nonmaternal custodial caregivers. The sample was primarily of lower socioeconomic status, with more than two-thirds of the families enrolled in the project reporting an annual income less than $20,000. Forty-one percent of the primary caregivers had a high school diploma or general education diploma (GED), and an additional 32% had 1 to 2 years of post-high school training. For more information about sample characteristics, see Dishion et al. (2008).

Retention

For the current study, analyses were limited to those participants with available genetic data at age 14, which resulted in 515 participants, which was 86.7% of the sample retained for home assessments at age 14. Among those who participated at age 14, there were no significant differences between youth who provided genetic samples versus those who did not with respect to intervention status, project site, parental education, race, gender, parental depression, behavioral inhibition, IC, or child CP at age 2.

Procedures

All assessments were conducted in the home at ages 2 to 14 with primary caregivers and children. Assessments were identical for control and intervention group participants and involved structured and unstructured play activities for the target child and caregiver. During these assessments, parents completed questionnaires regarding sociodemographic characteristics, parenting, neighborhood conditions, and child behavior. The first 50 to 55 minutes of the age 2 assessment began with a series of parent–child observation tasks, varied in stress level (e.g., free play, clean-up, teaching), with all tasks videotaped for later coding. Relevant to the current study are coded measures of parenting and behavioral inhibition. Parental written consent was obtained for all families, and assent was obtained from participating children beginning at age 14. Participants were compensated for their time at each age, and all study protocols were approved by the University of Pittsburgh’s Institutional Review Board.

The Family Check-Up

The FCU is an annual, brief three-session intervention that is individually tailored to the needs of children and families based on results obtained via an ecological assessment. The three meetings include an initial contact session, a home-based multi-informant ecological observational assessment session, and a feedback session (Dishion & Stormshak, 2007), which followed the same order and structure each year of the current trial. During the assessment session, a parent consultant explores parent concerns, focusing on family issues that are critical to the child’s well-being. Feedback emphasizes parenting and family strengths, yet draws attention to possible areas of change.

During the current trial, FCUs were offered to families when children were ages 2 to 5 and 7.5 to 10.5 annually, with engagement rates ranging from 77% at age 2 to 45% at age 10.5.

Measures

Polygenic risk scores

Polygenic risk scores were based on a recent meta-GWAS of aggression in middle childhood (Pappa et al., 2016). Summary statistics including single nucleotide polymorphisms reference number, risk allele, and p-value were drawn from this meta-GWAS. Polygenic risk scores were created using PRSice v2 (Euesden, Lewis, & O’Reilly, 2015) and PLINK v1.9 (Purcell et al., 2007) from overlapping SNPs in the meta-GWAS and those genotyped in the current sample and included 1200 SNPs at the p < .01 threshold. In the current score, SNPs were coded additively and unit weighted such that greater values reflected greater predisposition for aggression.

Population admixture

We conducted a principal components analysis of all autosomal SNPs to represent population admixture using PLINK. We extracted the first 20 components, with the first component (PC1) having an eigenvalue of 28.84 and differentiating European-American and Latino groups from African-American groups, with most biracial participants falling in the middle. The second component (PC2) had an eigenvalue of 5.62 and differentiated non-Latino participants (European and African American) from Latino participants. The remaining components had eigenvalues ranging from 1.45 to 1.21 and were excluded from these analyses.

Behavioral inhibition

At age 2, child behavioral inhibition was coded based on reactions to an approach by an adult stranger (2 minutes) and two novel objects (2 minutes each): a tunnel and a mechanically-operated robot. Based on a system developed by Kochanska (1991) and adapted by Shaw et al. (2003), approach and proximity to caregivers, avoidance or wary response to examiner, playing with free play toys, and approach to novel objects were each coded in 30-second intervals on a 4-point scale. In addition, coders assigned one global rating for the child’s level of inhibition (kappas ranged from .68–.83). Each item was standardized and summed to generate a total score for inhibition, with higher scores indicating greater inhibitory behavior (α = .63).

Inhibitory control

The 13-item Inhibitory Control subscale of the Children’s Behavior Questionnaire (CBQ; Rothbart, Ahadi, Hershey, & Fisher, 2001) was used to measure children’s ability to suppress immediate behavioral reactions at age 2. Primary caregivers indicated the extent to which statements were true of their child over the past six months using a 7-point scale (α = .76).

Maternal depressive symptoms

At age 2, primary caregivers (98% mothers) rated their depressive symptoms using the 20-item Center for Epidemiological Studies of Depression Scale (CES-D; Radloff, 1977). Caregivers reported how frequently they experienced depressive symptoms during the past week on a 4-point scale. Items were summed to create an overall depressive symptoms score (α = .74).

Harsh Parenting

Harsh parenting at age 2 was assessed using a composite index (α = .75; see Moilanen, Shaw, Dishion, Gardner, & Wilson, 2009) formed from three duration proportions from the Relationship Process Code (RPC; Jabson, Dishion, Gardner, & Burton, 2004) and five items from the Coder Impressions Inventory (COIMP; Dishion et al., 2008). For more information on specific items used, the reader is referred to Moilanen et al. (2009).

Positive behavioral support

Positive behavioral support at age 2 was assessed using a composite index formed from the RPC, COIMP, and the Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment inventory (HOME; Bradley, Corwyn, McAdoo, & Garcia-Coll, 2001). This index of positive behavior support included measures of parental involvement, positive reinforcement, engaged parent–child interaction time, and proactive parenting (see Dishion et al., 2008 for more information on how this score was computed).

Neighborhood deprivation

Neighborhood deprivation was ascertained by geocoding residential addresses using 2000 U.S. Census data when children were age 2. Block-level data, the smallest unit for which all census data are available, were used to approximate families’ immediate neighborhood. Scores were created for each block group from eight items selected in large-scale studies of public health outcomes and principal component analyses of 20 census items (Messer et al., 2006). Each item was transformed into a z-score, and an overall mean score was computed (α = .81).

Child CP and AB

At each assessment from ages 2 to 14, a measure of child oppositional and aggressive behavior was created from the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) for ages 1.5–5 and ages 6–18 (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001). The eight items for the oppositional-aggressive (Opp-Agg) factor that appeared on both versions of the CBCL were chosen based on their ability to map on to DSM-IV criteria for oppositional defiant or conduct disorder (e.g., gets in many fights, is defiant). Internal consistencies for primary caregiver reports on the Opp-Agg scale ranged from .71 at age 2 to .84 at age 14 (M = .80). A similar Opp-Agg scale was generated from the Achenbach Teacher Report Form (TRF; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001) and completed by teachers when youth were age 14 (α = .89). Youth also reported on the 33-item Self-Report of Delinquency at age 14 (SRD, Elliott, Huizinga, & Ashton, 1985), which assesses the frequency with which an individual has engaged in aggressive and delinquent behavior, including alcohol and drug use, during the prior year (α = .85).

Data Analytic Plan

In the first step of analyses, we estimated a single-class latent growth curve model of CP based on parent-report on the CBCL from ages 2 through 14 (Jung & Wickrama, 2008). Next, a series of growth mixture models estimating 1–5 classes were generated. Linear, quadratic, and cubic models were fit to the data, and the cubic model provided the best model fit. Thus, a cubic growth model was estimated to allow trajectory shapes to change across time, and quadratic and cubic variances were fixed to zero within groups to allow for model estimation (Jung & Wickrama, 2008). Selection of the most optimal trajectory group model was guided primarily by the sample-size adjusted Bayesian Information Criterion (Adj. BIC; Schwarz, 1978), with lower BIC values indicating better model fit. Other criteria for selecting the optimal number of classes included a high entropy value, high posterior probabilities for class membership, and no less than 5% of participants in each group (Andruff, Carraro, Thompson, & Gaudreau, 2009; Jung & Wickrama, 2008). After discerning trajectories of CP and classifying each individual into a distinct group based on posterior probabilities, class assignment data were transferred to SPSS 23. Chi-square tests and analysis of variance (ANOVA) with Bonferroni post hoc tests were performed to examine associations between trajectory group membership and all potential discriminator variables. Associations were also examined between group membership and both youth and teacher report of antisocial behavior at age 14. Next, the 3-step approach (Vermunt & Vermunt, 2010) was implemented in MPlus 7.3 to investigate the unique effects of each risk factor on CP group membership, also controlling for other predictors in the model and study site, parental education, child race, child gender, and the first two ancestry principle components (PC1 and PC2). After identifying the optimal number of trajectory groups (i.e., step #1) and saving group membership (i.e., step #2), multinomial logistic regression was used to regress group membership on intervention status and genetic, child, family, and neighborhood risk factors at age 2 (step #3). We also included a product term representing the interaction between polygenic score and intervention status. In this final step, logit values for misclassification error rates from step #1 were manually fixed. By accounting for the degree of uncertainty in classification, the three-step approach ensures that class membership is not affected by the inclusion of covariates, in contrast to the one-step approach, which can result in substantial changes to the number and shape of identified trajectories once covariates are introduced into the model.

Results

Identification of CP Trajectories

First, CP trajectories were estimated from ages 2 to 14 using a cubic growth model. Model fit indices for 1–5 groups are reported in Supplemental Table S1, which shows that the four-group model had lower AIC and sample-size adjusted BIC scores relative to the three-group model. Despite improved BIC and AIC scores, the five-group model resulted in a group consisting of less than 5% of the sample; hence, the four-group solution was retained as the best fitting model. The posterior probabilities for groups 1–4 were 91.9, 86.1, and 83.0%, respectively, well above the lower recommended threshold for assignment of 0.70 (Nagin, 2005).

Figure 1 depicts the trajectories of CPs for the final four-group model. Class 4, which we term the “Persistently Low Group,” accounted for 65.5% of the sample and consisted of children who had persistently low levels of CP from ages 2 to 14. The second largest group, Class 3 (16.2%), which we term the “Initially High-Quickly Desisting Group,” included children with the highest CP at age 2, but immediately and consistently declining symptoms from ages 2 to 9.5, and then consistently low levels from ages 9.5 to 14. In the third largest group, Class 1 (9.7%), which we term the “Initially High-Late Desisting Group,” CP measures were initially high and increasing from ages 2 to 7.5, but subsequently declined from ages 7.5 to 14 (i.e., late desisting). Finally, Class 2, which we term the “Persistently High Group,” accounted for the smallest proportion of the sample (8.6%). These children showed initially high symptoms that gradually increased from ages 2 to 14, with by far the highest rate of CP at age 14 compared to all other groups.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Trajectories of conduct problems from ages 2 to 14.

Risk Factors and Outcomes for CP Trajectory Membership

After establishing the trajectories of CP, chi-square tests were computed to examine whether study covariates were associated with trajectory group membership. While the four groups did not differ on intervention status, significant group differences in geographic location and gender composition were evident. The Persistently Low Group consisted of a greater percentage of girls and had greater representation from non-Pittsburgh sites compared with all other groups. Further, the Persistently High and the Initially High-Late Desisting Groups included more children from Pittsburgh compared with the other groups.

After examining group differences with respect to study covariates, a series of one-way ANOVAs were performed to test associations between trajectory group membership and child, family, and neighborhood risk factors at age 2. As shown in Table 1, there were significant associations between group membership and parent-reported IC, maternal depression, and observed positive behavior support and harsh parenting. Finally, significant associations were identified between trajectory group membership and teacher- and youth-reported AB at age 14 (see Supplemental Table S2). Specifically, according to teacher and youth reports, children in the Persistently High group demonstrated significantly greater AB at age 14 compared with the Persistently Low and Initially High-Quickly Desisting groups. Teachers also reported children in the Initially High-Late Desisting group as having more AB than children in Persistently Low and Initially High-Quickly Desisting groups.

Table 1.

Child, family, and neighborhood differences across trajectory groups of conduct problems from 2 to 14 years (N = 515)

Study Variables Class 1 Class 2 Class 3 Class 4 Chi-Square Test (χ2) or
Analysis of Variance (ANOVA)
Results
Intervention 47.9% Control, 52.1% FCU 59.0% Control, 41.0% FCU 53.8% Control, 46.2% FCU 48.0% Control, 52.0% FCU χ2 (3) = 2.344, p = .504
Geographic Location 54.2% Pittsburgh, 33.3% Eugene, 12.5% Charlottesville 56.4% Pittsburgh, 23.1% Eugene, 20.5% Charlottesville 42.3% Pittsburgh, 38.5% Eugene, 19.2% Charlottesville 34.6% Pittsburgh, 36.8% Eugene, 28.6% Charlottesville Pittsburgh vs. Non-Pittsburgh: χ2 (3) = 12.817, p < .01
Child Gender 58.3% Boys, 41.7% Girls 61.5% Boys, 38.5% Girls 60.3% Boys, 39.7% Girls 46.6% Boys, 53.4% Girls χ2 (3) = 8.183, p = .042
Child Inhibitory Control Age 2 (Parent-Report) 3.78 (0.77)1 < 4 3.77 (0.91)2 < 4 3.61 (0.75)3 < 4 4.13 (0.75)4 > 1,2,3 F (3, 507) = 12.29, p < .001a
Child Behavioral Inhibition Age 2 (Observation) 2.18 (0.44) 2.29 (0.61) 2.10 (0.67) 2.13 (0.60) F (3, 487) = 1.022, p = .382
Parental Depressive Symptoms Age 2 (Parent-Report) 21.94 (12.03)1 > 4 20.02 (10.43) 17.53 (10.35) 15.89 (10.23)4 < 1 F (3, 513) = 6.052, p < .001a
Positive Behavior Support Age 2 (Observation) 5.91 (1.17) 5.80 (0.91) 5.72 (1.03) 6.06 (1.04) F (3, 512) = 2.84, p < .05a
Harsh Parenting (Observation) 0.27 (0.89)1 > 4 0.16 (0.67) 0.14 (0.79)3 > 4 −0.08 (0.69)4 < 1, 3 F (3, 439) = 6.534, p < .001a
Neighborhood Deprivation Age 2 (US Census Data) 0.45 (0.74) 0.38 (0.83) 0.29 (0.63) 0.36 (0.73) F (3, 493) = 0.461 p = .709
Dopamine Risk Proportion 0.20 (0.03) 0.19 (0.04) 0.18 (0.03) 0.19 (0.03) F (3, 514) = 2.431 p = .064

Note: FCU = Family Check-Up; Means (standard deviations) are provided in columns with corresponding F-tests. Superscript numbers denote significant differences in mean scores between conduct problems classes, ps < .05.

Next, to examine the unique contributions of child, family, and neighborhood risk factors in discriminating CP trajectories, a multinomial logistical regression was computed in which group membership was regressed on all early childhood risk factors (see Table 2). When comparing the Persistently High group with the Persistently Low group, intervention status, parental education, child IC, and behavioral inhibition significantly discriminated group status in expected directions. Specifically, children had a higher probability of belonging to the Persistently Low group than to the Persistently High Group if they were less behaviorally inhibited, had higher levels of IC, and had parents with more education. There was also evidence of an intervention effect, as those assigned to the FCU had a higher probability of belonging to the Persistently Low group than to the Persistently High Group compared with youth in the control group. Despite no evidence of genetic main effects on group membership, the aggression polygenic score interacted with intervention status to discriminate the Persistently Low group from the Persistently High group. At low polygenic scores (i.e., 1 SD below the mean), individuals assigned to the FCU and control conditions did not differ with respect to trajectory group membership. However, at high polygenic scores (i.e., 1 SD above the mean), individuals assigned to the FCU had a higher likelihood of being in the Persistently Low group, whereas individuals in the control group were more likely to follow a Persistently High trajectory.

Table 2.

Multinomial logistic regressions predicting conduct problems trajectory membership

Class 1a
Class 2a
Class 3a
Predictors B (SE) OR B (SE) OR B (SE) OR
Intervention −0.14(0.46) 0.87 −0.54(0.25) 0.58* −0.31(0.22) 0.74
Child Gender 0.91(0.93) 2.48 0.70(0.52) 2.02 0.65(0.42) 1.92
Pittsburgh Study Site 0.45(0.89) 1.57 0.85(0.74) 2.33 0.70(0.66) 2.01
Virginia Study Site −0.70(0.82) 0.50 −0.50(0.76) 0.61 −0.68(0.71) 0.51
Parental Education 0.06(0.50) 1.07 −0.86(0.29) 0.43** −0.40(0.27) 0.67
Child Inhibitory Control Age 2 (Parent-Report) −0.94(0.43) 0.39* −0.83(0.35) 0.44* −0.99(0.23) 0.37**
Child Behavioral Inhibition Age 2 (Observation) 0.20(0.28) 1.22 0.55(0.25) 1.74* 0.19(0.24) 1.21
Parental Depressive Symptoms Age 2 (Parent-Report) 0.81(0.25) 2.24** 0.41(0.24) 1.51 0.25(0.22) 1.28
Harsh Parenting Age 2 (Observation) 0.83(0.36) 2.30* 0.43(0.39) 1.54 0.54(0.35) 1.72
Positive Behavior Support Age 2 (Observation) −0.18(0.50) 0.84 −0.31(0.27) 0.73 −0.48(0.27) 0.62
Neighborhood Deprivation Age 2 (US Census Data) 0.015(0.28) 1.02 −0.17(0.31) 0.85 −0.25(0.24) 0.78
Dopamine Polygenic Risk Score
(Proportion)
0.47(0.39) 1.60 −0.15(0.29) 0.86 −0.12(0.18) 0.89

Note: Intervention (Control = −1; Family Check-Up Intervention = 1); Child Gender (Girls = 0; Boys = 1); Pittsburgh Study Site (Non-Pittsburgh = 0; Pittsburgh = 1); Virginia Study Site (Non-Virginia = 0; Virginia = 1).

a

The Persistently High group is used as the reference group.

p < .10.

*

p < .05.

**

p < .01.

Discussion

This study sought to examine trajectories of CP from ages 2 to 14 and to examine predictors and outcomes of these trajectories.

Consistent with prior research on early- and late-starting patterns of AB, four groups were identified (Broidy et al., 2003; Moffitt, 2006; Nagin & Tremblay, 1999; Shaw et al., 2003). However, based on the high-risk nature of the sample, the inclusion of boys and girls, the initiation of the study during the terrible twos, and the study’s experimental design, some atypical variation in the patterning of trajectory groups was evident compared with studies of lower-risk samples including only boys and/or using passive longitudinal designs (Nagin & Tremblay, 2001; NICHD ECCRN, 2004; Shaw et al., 2003). Similar to other studies of early-starting CP, we identified persistently high and persistently low groups. These two groups were complemented by initially-high-late-desisting and initially-high-quickly-desisting groups. These two initially high and desisting groups could have been anticipated based on requiring high levels of child CP and initiating the study during the toddler period, as well as including girls, who show faster levels of desistance in aggression and oppositional behavior from the toddler to school-age period than boys (Brennan & Shaw, 2013).

Turning to early discriminators of trajectories, results confirm past research suggesting the importance of children’s gender, IC, maternal depression, harsh parenting, and parental education in discriminating early-starting and persistent CP (Aguilar et al., 2000; Moffitt & Caspi, 2001). All of the aforementioned predictor variables discriminated early-starting from persistently-low trajectories in both univariate and multivariate analyses.

Perhaps the three most striking findings were evident in comparing the persistently high group with the persistently low group. Children in the persistently high group stood out because of their initially high and further ascending trajectory of CP. First, in relation to the persistently low group, persistently high children demonstrated less parental education, IC, and a trend for greater maternal depressive symptoms, as well as an unexpectedly higher level of behavioral inhibition, the latter perhaps reflecting co-occurring externalizing and internalizing concerns. Second, intervention effects were evident, corroborating and extending previous research with this sample documenting FCU intervention effects on child CP and IC through preschool and middle childhood (Chang et al., 2014; Dishion et al., 2008, 2014; Shaw et al., 2016), as well as maternal depression (Shaw et al., 2009) during early childhood. Third, this intervention effect was qualified by an interaction involving polygenic risk for aggression. Specifically, among youth with low polygenic scores, those children assigned to the FCU and control conditions did not differ with respect to their trajectory of CP from ages 2 to 14. However, at high polygenic risk for aggression, children in the FCU group were significantly more likely to be in the Persistently Low versus Persistently High group, whereas control children were more likely to follow a Persistently High trajectory. Polygenic scores moderated FCU effects on trajectory class membership. This pattern of results is consistent with Differential Susceptibility Theory (Belsky & Pluess, 2009), such that the intervention was efficacious for genetically susceptible individuals but not for others.

There are less than a handful of studies that have considered polygenic moderation of intervention effects with polygenic scores based on results from large discovery GWASs in order to provide valid indices of genetic variation that create multiple small genetic effects. Of these studies, Keers et al. (2016) and Lemery-Chalfant, Clifford, Dishion, Shaw, and Wilson (2018) focused on the outcome of internalizing problems in middle childhood, and Musci, Masyn, Uhl, Maher, Kellam, and Ialongo (2016) and Musci et al. (2018) examined onset of smoking tobacco and cannabis use in adolescence. In our previous study with the Early Steps Multsite sample, we used a polygenic score that indexed susceptibility to the environment (i.e., genetic variants in this score predicted identical twin differences in emotional problems in middle childhood in a discovery GWAS) (Lemery-Chalfant et al., 2018). We reported a significant Polygenic × Intervention interaction, such that genetically susceptible children in the FCU condition had fewer internalizing symptoms than those in the control condition. Regions of significance indicated that these differences were significant beginning at approximately half a standard deviation above the mean on polygenic susceptibility, or for about 25% of the sample. Together, our findings suggest that psychosocial interventions such as the FCU likely have large effects on some individuals but rather than small effects on everyone, and this field could benefit from taking a personalized medicine approach and providing multiple interventions tailored to individual differences.

Unanticipated null findings also need to be acknowledged for neighborhood risk and formal G × E interactions. Null findings for neighborhood risk were not surprising based on prior research indicating that such effects do not begin to emerge until at least ages 3 or 4 even in low-income urban samples (Brooks-Gunn et al., 1997; Supplee et al., 2007). In the current sample, neighborhood effects on CP at school have been reported as children move from early to middle childhood (Shaw et al., 2016). Traditional G × E interactions were not found between polygenic risk and intervention or between polygenic risk and family or community risk, although there was a trend for a direct effect of polygenic risk. These null G × E findings are inconsistent with both diathesis stress and differential susceptibility models. Although one might expect to find evidence of genetic susceptibility to contextual risk only (diathesis stress) or susceptibility to both contextual risk and protective factors (differential susceptibility), especially during early childhood (Galan & Shaw, 2018), actual support for these forms of G × E interactions have not been universally found (Belsky & Pluess, 2013).

Finally, with the untimely passing of our dear colleague, Tom Dishion, the subject of this Special Issue, we would be remiss not to acknowledge his brilliance in developing the Family Check-Up as a way to engage and improve child and adolescent outcomes. The current intention-to-treat findings spanning 12 years after the initiation of the FCU in this ethnically diverse, low-income sample, albeit qualified by genetic moderation, represent another testament of the FCU’s efficacy in promoting long-term functioning among low-income samples of high-risk youth. The current findings echo similar intervention effects found for the current sample on problem behavior during the preschool (Connell et al., 2008; Dishion et al., 2008) and school-age (Dishion et al., 2014; Reuben et al. 2015; Shaw et al., 2016) periods. In addition, similar long-term intervention effects on multiple types of problem behavior have been evident for the FCU using a large cohort of at-risk youth when initiated in early adolescence (Caruthers, Van Ryzin, & Dishion, 2014; Connell, Klostermann, & Dishion, 2011; Stormshak, Connell, & Dishion, 2009). Although Dr. Dishion is no longer with us, the influence of his Family Check-Up lives on and will likely increase in the coming years and decades.

Limitations

Despite having several methodological strengths, including the long-term follow-up of an experimental trial of the FCU using a large sample of low-income, ethnically-diverse girls and boys from urban, rural, and suburban communities and the use of multiple methods and informants, the study is not without methodological limitations. First, the extent to which the findings would generalize to other samples, particularly children from different ethnic/racial backgrounds or higher income households, might be limited. Second, we relied heavily on primary caregiver reports to measure trajectories of child CP and multiple discriminators of trajectory groups, including maternal depression and child IC, and parental education, all of which discriminated persistently high versus persistently low groups. Parental informant and method bias might have inflated these associations, and they should be interpreted with more caution than discriminators such as intervention status, harsh parenting (observed), and child gender that were not subject to reporting or method bias and also were found to discriminate trajectory group status. In terms of relying on primary caregiver reports of child CP from age 2 to 14, it should be noted that differences in CP were generally corroborated by youth and teacher reports at age 14. Finally, cell sizes for three of the four trajectory groups were relatively small (<10%), including persistently high and initially high then desisting groups, making it challenging to detect group differences on predictors among these initially high groups.

Conclusions

The present study extends previous research that has used person-oriented, latent growth modeling to identify different trajectories of early-starting CP with a high-risk community sample and largely corroborates child and family discriminators of early-starting persistent patterns. Importantly and novel to the current study, by accounting for genetic risk and employing an experimental design, findings revealed an intervention effect for the FCU between children with persistently high and persistently low trajectories.

Supplementary Material

Shaw 2019 Supplemental Material

Acknowledgments.

The research reported in this paper was supported by grants to the first, fourth, and sixth authors from the National Institute on Drug Abuse (R01 DA25630 and R01 DA26222), and the National Institute on Drug Abuse and National Institutes of Health: Office of the Director, and Office of Behavioral and Social Sciences Research to the fifth author (K01 DA042828). We are grateful to the staff of the Early Steps Multisite Project for their years of service, and to our study families for making the research possible.

References

  1. Achenbach TM, & Rescorla LA (2001). Manual for the ASEBA School-Age Forms & Profiles. Burlington, VT: ASEBA. [Google Scholar]
  2. Aguilar B, Sroufe L, Egeland B, & Carlson E (2000). Distinguishing the early-onset/persistent and adolescence-onset antisocial behavior types: From birth to 16 years. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 109–132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Andruff H, Carraro N, Thompson A, & Gaudreau P (2009). Latent class growth modelling: A tutorial. Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 5(1), 11–24. [Google Scholar]
  4. Belsky J, & Pluess M (2009). Beyond diathesis stress: differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Psychological Bulletin, 135(6), 885–908. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bradley RH, Corwyn RF, McAdoo HP, & Garcia-Coll C (2001). The home environments of children in the United States part 1: Variations by age, ethnicity, and poverty status. Child Development, 72, 1844–1867. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Brame B, Nagin DS, & Tremblay RE (2001). Developmental trajectories of physical aggression from school entry to late adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 503–512. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Brennan L, & Shaw DS (2013). Revisiting data related to the age of onset and developmental course of female conduct problems. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 16, 35–58, PMC3684009. 10.1007/s10567-012-0125-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Broidy LM, Nagin DS, Tremblay RE, Bates JE, Brame R, Dodge KA, et al. (2003). Developmental trajectories of childhood disruptive behaviors and adolescent delinquency: A six-site, cross-national study. Developmental Psychology, 39, 222–245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Brooks-Gunn J, Duncan GJ, & Aber JL (1997). Lessons learned in future directions for research on the neighborhoods in which children live In Brooks-Gunn J, Duncan GJ, Aber JL (Eds.), Neighborhood poverty: Vol 1.Context and consequences for children (pp. 279–297). New York: Russell Sage Foundation. [Google Scholar]
  10. Burt SA, & Mikolajewski AJ (2008). Preliminary evidence that specific candidate genes are associated with adolescent-onset antisocial behavior. Aggressive Behavior, 34(4), 437–445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Campbell SB, Pierce EW, Moore G, Marakovitz S, and Newby K (1996). Boys’ externalizing problems at elementary school: Pathways from early behavior problems, maternal control, and family stress. Development and Psychopathology, 8, 701–720. [Google Scholar]
  12. Caruthers AS, Van Ryzin MJ, & Dishion TJ (2014). Preventing high-risk sexual behavior in early adulthood with family interventions in adolescence: Outcomes and developmental processes. Prevention Science, 15, 59–69. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Caspi A, McClay J, Moffitt TE, Mill J, Martin J, Craig IW et al. (2002). Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science, 297, 851–854. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Chang H, Shaw DS, Dishion TJ, & Wilson MN (2014). Direct and indirect effects of the Family Check-Up on self-regulation from toddlerhood to early school-age. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 42, 1117–1128. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Connell A, Bullock BM, Dishion TJ, Shaw D, Wilson M, & Gardner F (2008). Family intervention effects on co-occurring behavior and emotional problems in early childhood: A latent transition analysis approach. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 1211–1225. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Connell A, Klostermann S, & Dishion TJ (2011). Family Check Up effects on adolescent arrest trajectories: Variation by developmental subtype. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 22, 367–380. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Dishion TJ, Brennan LM, McEachern A, Shaw DS, Wilson MN, & Weaver CM (2014). The prevention of early onset problem behavior through periodic family check ups in early childhood: A health maintenance model with indigent, high risk families. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 42, 343–354. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Dishion TJ, & Kavanagh K (2003). Intervening in adolescent problem behavior: A family-centered approach. New York: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
  19. Dishion TJ, Shaw D, Connell A, Gardner F, Weaver C, & Wilson M (2008). The family check-up with high-risk indigent families: Preventing problem behavior by increasing parents’ positive behavior support in early childhood. Child Development, 79(5), 1395–1414. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Dishion TJ, & Stormshak E (2007). Intervening in children’s lives: An ecological, family-centered approach to mental health care. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. [Google Scholar]
  21. Elam KK, Chassin L, & Pandika D (2018). Polygenic risk, family cohesion, and adolescent aggression in Mexican-American and European-Am4erican families: Developmental pathways to alcohol use. Development and Psychopathology. Epub ahead of print. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Elliott DS, Huizinga D, & Ageton SS (1985). Explaining delinquency and drug use. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. [Google Scholar]
  23. Erickson MF, Sroufe LA, & Egeland B (1985). The relationship between quality of attachment and behavior problems in preschool in a high-risk sample In Bretherton I & Waters E (Eds.), Growing points of attachment theory and research; Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50, Nos. 1–2, 147–167. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Eusden J, Lewis CM, & O’Reilly PF (2015). PRSice: Polygenic risk score software. Bioinformatics, 31(9), 1466–1468. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Fergusson DM & Woodward LJ (1999). Maternal age and educational and psychosocial outcomes in early adulthood. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 479–489. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Forbes EE, Brown SM, Kimak M, Ferrell RE, Manuck SB, & Hariri AR (2009). Genetic variation in components of dopamine neuro-transmission impacts ventral striatal reactivity associated with impulsivity. Molecular Psychiatry, 14(1), 60–70. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Gálan CA, Shaw DS, & Belsky J (in preparation). Taking stock of GxE Interaction involving dopaminergic Genes and alternative conceptual models: A meta-analytic investigation.
  28. Gardner F, Shaw DS, Dishion TJ, Supplee LA, & Burton J (2007). Randomized prevention trial for early conduct problems: Effects on proactive parenting and links to toddler disruptive behavior. Journal of Family Psychology, 21, 398–406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Gilliam M, Forbes EE, Gianaros PJ, Erickson KI, Brennan LM, & Shaw DS (2015). Maternal depression in childhood and aggression in young adulthood: evidence for mediation by offspring amygdala–hippocampal volume ratio. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56, 1083–1091. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Goodman SH, Rouse MH, Connell A, Broth MR, Hall CM, & Heyward D (2011). Maternal depression and child psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 14, 1–27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Greenberg MT, & Speltz ML (1988). Contributions of attachment theory to the understanding of conduct problems during the preschool years In Belsky J & Nezworski T (Eds.), Clinical implications of attachment (pp. 177–218). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
  32. Hyde LW, Shaw DS, Gardner F, Cheong J, Dishion TJ, & Wilson MN (2013). Dimensions of callousness in early childhood: Links to problem behavior and family intervention effectiveness. Development and Psychopathology, 25, 347–363. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Jabson JM, Dishion TJ, Gardner FEM, & Burton J (2004). Relationship Process Code v-2.0 Training Manual A system for coding relationship interactions. Unpublished coding manual. Child and Family Center, University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon. [Google Scholar]
  34. Jung T, & Wickrama KA (2008). An introduction to latent class growth analysis and growth mixture modeling. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2(1), 302–317. [Google Scholar]
  35. Keers R, Coleman JR, Lester KJ, Roberts S, Breen G, Thastum M, … & Nauta M (2016). A genome-wide test of the differential susceptibility hypothesis reveals a genetic predictor of differential response to psychological treatments for child anxiety disorders. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 85, 146–158. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Kochanska G (1991). Patterns of inhibition to the unfamiliar in children of normal and affectively ill mothers. Child Development, 62, 250–263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Lemery-Chalfant K, Clifford S, Dishion T, Shaw D, & Wilson M (2018). Genetic moderation of the effects of the Family Check-Up intervention on children’s internalizing symptoms: A longitudinal study with a racial/ethnically diverse sample. Development and Psychopathology. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Messer LC, Laraia BA, Kaufman JS, Eyster J, Holzman C, Culhane J, …O’Campo P (2006). The development of a standardized neighborhood deprivation index. Journal of Urban Health: Bulletin of the New York Academy of Medicine, 83, 1041–1062. 10.1007/s11524-006-9094-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Moffitt TE (1990). Juvenile delinquency and Attention Deficit Disorder: Boys’ developmental trajectories from age 3 to age 15. Child Development, 61, 893–910. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Moffitt TE (2006). Life-course-persistent and adolescent-limited antisocial behavior In Cicchetti D & Cohen DJ (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: Vol. 3. Risk, disorder, and adaptation (pp. 570–598). New York: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
  41. Moffitt TE, & Caspi A (2001). Childhood predictors differentiate life-course persistent and adolescence-limited antisocial pathways among males and females. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 355–375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Harrington H, & Milne BJ (2002). Males on the life-course-persistent and adolescence-limited antisocial pathways: Follow-up at age 26 years. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 179–207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Moilanen KL, & Shaw DS, Dishion TJ, Gardner F, & Wilson M (2009). Longitudinal growth and predictors of inhibitory control in early childhood. Social Development, 19, 326–347. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Musci RJ, Fairman B, Masyn KE, Uhl G, Maher B, Sisto DY, … & Ialongo NS (2018). Polygenic score × intervention moderation: An application of discrete-time survival analysis to model the timing of first marijuana use among urban youth. Prevention Science, 19, 6–14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Musci RJ, Masyn KE, Uhl G, Maher B, Kellam SG, & Ialongo NS (2015). Polygenic score × intervention moderation: An application of discrete-time survival analysis to modeling the timing of first tobacco use among urban youth. Development and Psychopathology, 27, 111–122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Nagin DS (2005). Group-based modeling of development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
  47. Nagin DS, & Tremblay RE (1999). Trajectories of boys’ physical aggression, opposition, and hyperactivity on the path to physically violent and nonviolent juvenile delinquency. Child Development, 70, 1181–1196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Nagin DS, & Tremblay RE (2001). Parental and early childhood predictors of persistent physical aggression in boys from kindergarten to high school. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58, 389–394. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2004). Are child developmental outcomes related to before-and after-school care arrangements? Results from the NICHD Study of Early Child Care. Child Development, 75, 280–295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Odgers CL, Moffitt TE, Broadbent JM, Dickson N, Hancox RJ, Harrington H et al. (2008). Female and male antisocial trajectories: From childhood origins to adult outcomes. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 673–716. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Olson S, Choe D, & Sameroff A (2017). Trajectories of child externalizing problems between ages 3 and 10 years: Contributions of children’s early effortful control, theory of mind, and parenting experiences. Development and Psychopathology, 29, 437–453. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Pappa I, St Pourcain B, Benke K, Cavadino A, Hakulinen C, Nivard MG, & Tiemeier H (2016). A genome-wide approach to children’s aggressive behavior: The EAGLE consortium. American Journal of Medical Genetics, Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 171(5), 562–572. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Patterson GR, Reid JB, & Dishion TJ (1992). Antisocial boys. Eugene, OR: Castalia. Publishing Company. [Google Scholar]
  54. Purcell S, Neale B, Todd-Brown K, Thomas L, Ferreira MA, & Bender D (2007). PLINK: tool set for whole-genome association and population-based linkage analyses. American Journal of Human Genetics, 81(3), 559–575. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Radloff LS (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385–401. [Google Scholar]
  56. Reuben J, Shaw DS, Brennan LM, Dishion TJ, & Wilson MN (2015). A family-based intervention for improving children’s emotional problems through effects on maternal depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 83, 1142–1148. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Rhee SH, & Waldman ID (2002). Genetic and environmental influences on antisocial behavior: A meta-analysis of twin and adoption studies. Psychological Bulletin, 128(3), 490–529. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Richman M, Stevenson J, & Graham PJ (1982). Preschool to school: A behavioral study. London: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
  59. Rothbart MK, Ahadi SA, Hershey KL, & Fisher P (2001). Investigations of temperament at three to seven years: The Children’s Behavior Questionnaire. Child Development, 72, 1394–1408. 10.1111/1467-8624.00355 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Sampson RJ (1997). Collective regulation of adolescent misbehavior: Validation results from eighty Chicago neighborhoods. Journal of Adolescent Research, 12, 227–244. [Google Scholar]
  61. Sanson A, Oberklaid F, Pedlow R, & Prior M (1991). Risk indicators: Assessment of infancy predictors of pre-school behavioural maladjustment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 609–626. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Schwarz G (1978). Estimating the Dimension of a Model. The Annals of Statistics, 6(2), 461–464. Retrieved from http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0090-5364%28197803%296%3A2%3C461%3AETDOAM%3E2.0.CO%3B2-5 [Google Scholar]
  63. Shaw DS, & Bell RQ (1993). Developmental theories of parental contributors to antisocial behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 21, 493–518. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Shaw DS, Dishion TJ, Connell A, Wilson MN, & Gardner F (2009). Improvements in maternal depression as a mediator of intervention effects on early child problem behavior. Development and Psychopathology, 21, 417–439. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Shaw DS, Dishion TJ, Supplee LH, Gardner F, & Arnds K (2006). A family-centered approach to the prevention of early-onset antisocial behavior: Two-year effects of the family check-up in early childhood. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74, 1–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Shaw DS, Gilliom M, Ingoldsby EM, & Nagin D (2003). Trajectories leading to school-age conduct problems. Developmental Psychology, 39, 189–200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Shaw DS, Hyde LW, & Brennan LM Early predictors of boys’ antisocial trajectories (2012). Development and Psychopathology, 24, 871–888. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Shaw DS, Owens EB, Vondra JI, Keenan K & Winslow EB (1996). Early risk factors and pathways in the development of early disruptive behavior problems. Development and Psychopathology, 8, 679–699. [Google Scholar]
  69. Shaw DS, Sitnick S, Brennan LM, Choe DE, Dishion TJ, Wilson MN, & Gardner F (2016). The long-term effectiveness of the Family Check-Up on school-age conduct problems: Moderation by neighborhood deprivation. Development and Psychopathology, 28, 1471–1487. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Shaw DS, Winslow EB, Owens EB, Vondra JI, Cohn JF, & Bell RQ (1998). The development of early externalizing problems among children from low-income families: A transformational perspective. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26, 95–107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Stevenson J, & Goodman R (2001). Association between behaviour at age 3 years and adult criminality. British Journal of Psychiatry, 179, 197–202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Stormshak EA, Connell A, & Dishion TJ (2009). An adaptive approach to family-centered interventions in schools: Linking intervention engagement to academic outcomes in middle and high school. Prevention Science, 10, 221–235. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Supplee LH, Unikel E, & Shaw DS (2007). Physical environmental adversity and the protective role of maternal monitoring in relation to early child conduct problems. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 28, 166–183. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Tremblay RE, Nagin DS, Seguin JR, Zoccolillo M, Zelazo PD, Boivin M, et al. (2004). Physical aggression during early childhood: Trajectories and predictors. Pediatrics, 114, E43–E50. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Vermunt JK, Vermunt K,J (2010). Latent Class Modeling with Covariates: Two Improved Three-Step Approaches. Political Analysis, 18 (4), 450–469. Retrieved from https://econpapers.repec.org/article/cuppolals/v_3a18_3ay_3a2010_3ai_3a04_3ap_3a450-469_5f01.htm [Google Scholar]
  76. Wakschlag LS, & Hans SL (1999). Relation of maternal responsiveness during infancy to the development of behavior problems in high-risk youths. Developmental Psychology, 35, 569–579. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Waller R, Shaw DS, & Hyde LW (2017). Observed fearlessness and positive parenting interact to predict childhood callous-unemotional behaviors among low-income boys. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58, 282–291. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Webster-Stratton C (1990). Long-term follow-up of families with young conduct problem children: From preschool to grade school. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 19(2), 144–149. [Google Scholar]
  79. Willoughby MT, Mills-Koonce WR, Gottfredson NC, & Wagner NJ (2014). Measuring callous unemotional behaviors in early childhood: Factor structure and the prediction of stable aggression in middle childhood. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 36, 30–42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Wray NR, Lee SH, Mehta D, Vinkhuyzen AA, Dudbridge F, & Middeldorp CM (2014). Research review: polygenic methods and their application to psychiatric traits. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55(10), 1068–1087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Xue Y, Leventhal T, Brooks-Gunn J, & Earls FJ (2005). Neighborhood residence and mental health problems of 5-11 year olds. Achives of General Psychiatry, 62, 554–563. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Shaw 2019 Supplemental Material

RESOURCES