Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2021 Jan 1.
Published in final edited form as: Psychol Med. 2019 Jul 1;50(9):1519–1529. doi: 10.1017/S0033291719001521

Do neighborhood social processes moderate the etiology of youth conduct problems?

S Alexandra Burt 1, D Angus Clark 2, Amber L Pearson 3, Kelly L Klump 1, Jenae M Neiderhiser 4
PMCID: PMC7370405  NIHMSID: NIHMS1571581  PMID: 31258102

Abstract

Background:

Prior work has robustly suggested that social processes in the neighborhood (i.e., informal social control, social cohesion, norms) influence child conduct problems (CP) and related outcomes, but has yet to consider how these community-level influences interact with individual-level genetic risk for CP. The current study sought to do just this, evaluating neighborhood-level social processes as etiologic moderators of child CP for the first time.

Methods:

We made use of two nested samples of child and adolescent twins within the Michigan State University Twin Registry (MSUTR): 5,649 families who participated in in the Michigan Twins Project (MTP) and 1,013 families who participated in the Twin Study of Behavioral and Emotional Development (TBED-C). The neighborhood social processes of informal social control, social cohesion, and norms were assessed using neighborhood sampling techniques, in which residents of each twin family’s neighborhood reported on the social processes in their neighborhood. Standard biometric GxE analyses evaluated the extent to which they moderated the etiology of CP.

Results:

The ‘no moderation’ model provided the best fit to the data in nearly all cases, arguing against neighborhood social processes as etiologic moderators of youth CP.

Conclusions:

The neighborhood social processes evaluated here do not appear to exert their effects on child CP via etiologic moderation. The documented links between neighborhood social processes and child CP are thus likely to reflect a different etiologic process. Possibilities include environmental main effects of neighborhood social processes on child CP, or genotype-environment correlations.

Keywords: Conduct Problems, GxE, informal social control, social cohesion


There is now substantial evidence that neighborhood conditions predict youth academic achievement, social competence, and conduct problems (e.g., Sampson et al., 1997). Experimental studies (Ludwig et al., 2001, Damm and Dustmann, 2014) leveraging quasi-randomized or randomized neighborhood assignment (i.e., refugee immigrants assigned to neighborhoods, housing vouchers) have indicated that, in the case of youth conduct problems, this association is at least partially causal (although for an excellent debate of these findings, see Ludwig et al., 2008, Sampson, 2008). A quasi-experimental comparison of cousins residing in neighborhoods with different levels of poverty (Goodnight et al., 2012) further supported the possibility of causal effects on conduct problems.

Although such work brings needed attention to the role of neighborhood in youth conduct problems, studies of neighborhood poverty, crime, and disadvantage tell us little about how these neighborhood structural characteristics influence child outcomes. Contemporary theoretical models have convincingly argued that neighborhood social processes (i.e., social support, willingness to intervene for the common good) serve as core mechanisms of neighborhood effects on child outcomes, such that neighborhood disadvantage is primarily related to increases in youth conduct problems when accompanied by low levels of informal social control and/or social cohesion (Leventhal and Brooks-Gunn, 2000, Bronfenbrenner, 1979, Jencks and Mayer, 1990, Coie et al., 2000).

Three neighborhood social processes have emerged as particularly important in this literature (Henry et al., 2014): informal social control, social cohesion, and norms. Informal social control involves the supervision of children by other adults in the community and the willingness of neighbors to intervene for the common good (Henry et al., 2014). These interventions can focus on a number of issues, including safeguarding child welfare, preventing or managing youth problem behaviors, and/or intervening when youth are disrupting public spaces. The common theme, however, is that, rather than relying on formal mechanisms of control such as the police, “…neighborhood residents take responsibility for and authority to help regulate each other’s behavior collectively and informally” (pg. 190; Tolan et al., 2014).

Social cohesion is defined via social support amongst neighbors, and a sense of belonging to the community (Henry et al., 2014). It has been argued that social cohesion is particularly important in stressed neighborhoods (Leventhal & Brooks-Gunn, 2000), as parents can more easily navigate the environmental, developmental, and structural challenges that characterize those neighborhoods when bolstered by connections with others also facing those challenges (Tolan et al., 2014). These theoretical arguments have been borne out in both correlational and intervention studies demonstrating that high levels of social cohesion protect adolescents in disadvantaged neighborhoods from developing youth conduct problems, among other things (Tolan et al., 2004, Simon et al., 2009, Wight et al., 2006, Sampson, 2003, Morenoff et al., 2001).

Neighborhood norms index shared beliefs about acceptable or expected behaviors and are typically assessed in the areas of child welfare and management, youth behavior, crime, and citizen responsibility (Henry et al., 2014). Recent empirical evidence indicates that when neighbors approve of deviant behavior, youth are more likely to engage in those behaviors (Ahern et al., 2009, Reed et al., 2011, Wright and Fagan, 2013).

In short, years of sociological studies have suggested that neighborhood social processes partially shape the development of youth conduct problems. Although already important, the broader contribution of this work has been limited by the fact that it has yet to meaningfully consider, either theoretically or empirically, the role of the child’s individual attributes in his or her outcome, and specifically the ways in which neighborhood social processes might interact with individual genetic risk for conduct problems. This is no small omission in the neighborhood literature. Several meta-analyses (Burt, 2009b, Burt, 2009a) have robustly indicated that youth conduct problems are moderately-to-strongly heritable, with genetic influences accounting for roughly 48–65% of the variance across the population. How do we integrate this compelling evidence of individual-level genetic risk with the results of neighborhood studies pointing to the importance of emergent neighborhood social processes for resident outcomes? One obvious approach would be via considerations of genotype-environment interplay. GxE is defined as differential responsiveness to environmental risk as a function of genetic variability (Plomin et al., 1977, Rutter et al., 1999b, Rutter et al., 1999a), and is thought to constitute a core mechanism though which genes influence mental health (Johnson, in press, Moffitt et al., 2006), including antisocial behavior (Hicks et al., 2009).

What might GxE look like in this case? There are two models of GxE that appear important for youth antisocial behavior, although the model appears to vary with both the specific environmental moderator and the child’s level of development (Burt, 2011). Under the oft-discussed diathesis-stress model, GxE would manifest as stronger genetic effects in the presence of environmental risk, such that genetic influences on conduct problems were ‘activated’ by exposure to environmental risk (Hicks et al., 2009). Under the lesser known bioecological GxE model, by contrast, deleterious environments would amplify environmental influences, whereas genetic influences would be more important under normal environmental conditions (Pennington et al., 2009, Bronfenbrenner and Ceci, 1994, Burt et al., 2016). The logic of the latter model is best illustrated through Lewontin’s analogy of genetically variable seeds planted in either nutrient-rich or nutrient-deprived soil (Lewontin, 1995). Because all plants receive adequate nutrition in nutrient-rich soil, individual differences in plant height would be largely a consequence of genetic differences between plants. The environmental adversity conferred by the deprived soil, by contrast, should eventuate in field populated largely by short plants, regardless of the plants’ genetic predispositions for height. Put differently, it may be that some adverse experiences provide such a strong “social push” for a given outcome that the importance of genetic factors in these environments is effectively diminished (Raine, 2002).

Current Study

The current study sought to evaluate, for the first time, neighborhood social processes as etiologic moderators of youth conduct problems. We made use of two nested studies. We first examined families in the Twin Study of Behavioral and Emotional Development (TBED-C), the only twin study in the world (to our knowledge) to have incorporated neighborhood directly into the inclusion criteria as recommended in the neighborhood literature (see Leventhal and Brooks-Gunn, 2000). We augmented the TBED-C’s cutting-edge neighborhood sampling approach with state-of-the-art measurement of neighborhood social processes, collecting multiple informant-reports of informal social control, cohesion, and norms from randomly-selected individuals residing in the families’ neighborhoods. We then linked these neighbor informant-reports back to both the TBED-C and the Michigan Twins Project, the large-scale, population-based twin registry from which the TBED-C was initially recruited. The current project was thus well-positioned, both in its design and analytic approach, to explore whether and how neighborhood social processes moderated the etiology of youth conduct problems. Given the absence of prior genetically-informed studies examining neighborhood social processes, we did not have any specific hypotheses about the presence or specific pattern of etiologic moderation of youth conduct problems by neighborhood social processes.

METHODS

PARTICIPANTS

Twin families

The current study made use of two nested samples within the population-based Michigan State University Twin Registry (MSUTR; Burt and Klump, 2013, Klump and Burt, 2006): the Michigan Twins Project (MTP) and the TBED-C. The primary aim of the on-going MTP is to collect health data on a large sample of child and adolescent twins (current N ~12,000 families) that can be used either for data analysis or to select twin families for follow-up research (as was done in the TBED-C, see below). The MTP twins were 50.1% female, and ranged in age from 3 to 17 years (mean age = 8.80 years, SD = 4.6 years) at the time of their assessment, although a few pairs (n=43) had turned 18 by the time their assessment was completed. Twins belonged to racial groups at rates comparable to the lower Michigan Census (e.g., Black: 7.9%, White: 82.0%, Multiracial: 5.3%, respectively) (Burt and Klump, 2012). A parent provided informed consent for themselves and their children.

The TBED-C was recruited out of the MTP, and includes both a population-based arm (n=528 families) and an independent ‘at-risk’ arm (n=502 families). To be eligible for participation in either arm of the TBED-C, neither twin could have a cognitive or physical condition (e.g., a significant developmental delay) as assessed via parental screen that would preclude completion of the assessment. Additional inclusion criteria for the ‘at-risk’ arm of the study specified that participating twin families lived in modestly- to severely-impoverished Census tracts. As expected, this additional inclusion criterion eventuated in a less advantaged sample. While twins participating in the population-based arm of the TBED-C belonged to racial groups at rates comparable to local area inhabitants (e.g., Black: 5.4%, White: 86.4%), twins in the at-risk arm were significantly more racially diverse (14.2% Black and 76.3% White). The at-risk arm also reported lower family incomes (Cohen’s d effect size = −.38), higher paternal felony convictions (d = .30), and higher rates of youth conduct problems and hyperactivity (d = .34 and .27, respectively), although they did not differ in youth emotional problems (d = .08, ns). The TBED-C twins collectively ranged in age from 6 to 11 years (mean = 7.99, SD = 1.49) and were 49% female. Other recruitment and sampling details are detailed extensively in prior publications (e.g., Burt & Klump, 2013b; Burt et al., 2016). Children provided informed assent, while parents provided informed consent for themselves and their children.

Neighbors

The protocol for the at-risk arm of the TBED-C included the recruitment and assessment of randomly-chosen neighbors. Following the participation of a given family in the ‘at-risk’ study, we sent mailings to 10 randomly-chosen addresses in that family’s Census tract, inviting one adult resident per household to complete a survey. When a particular randomly-chosen address was no longer inhabited (i.e., the letter was returned as undeliverable), one attempt was made to find a replacement address. This approach resulted in a sample of 1,880 neighbors (63.2% women; 80.6% White, 11.6% Black, 7.8% other ethnic group memberships; average age of 52.6 with a range of 18–95 years). The response rate was 70%, of which 70% agreed to participate (for a final participation rate of 49%). All participants provided informed consent.

To maximize the number of MTP families with available neighborhood informant data, we also included an independent sample of 1,430 neighborhood informants (46.7% women; 86.2% non-Hispanic Caucasian, average age of 27.9 with a range of 18–70 years) nested in 997 census tracts across the state of Michigan. Participants in this second sample were recruited via the web-based Amazon marketplace MTurk (Buhrmester et al., 2011). MTurk has a large (N>100,000) and diverse ‘workforce’ of individuals who complete surveys, writing, and other such tasks on-line. For the current study, we required that all participants resided in Michigan, and paid $1.50 for completion of the assessment. Assessments were identical to those in the above neighborhood informant sample.

MEASURES

Zygosity

Zygosity was established using physical similarity questionnaires administered to the twins’ primary caregiver (Peeters et al., 1998). On average, the physical similarity questionnaires used by the MSUTR have accuracy rates of at least 95% as compared to DNA. Data structure by zygosity is presented in Table 1.

Table 1.

Sample sizes and operationalization of neighborhood

Sample Geographic unit Number of neighbor informants Number of twin families
Mean (SD) Median Min Max Total MZ DZ
MTP Census tract 2.87 (2.33) 2 1 17 5649 1618 4031
TBED-C Neighborhood informant nearest to the twin family 1.0 (0.0) 1 1 1 1013 416 597
All neighborhood informants within 5 km 13.1 (11.0) 10 1 47 847 332 515
All neighborhood informants within 1 km 3.1 (2.1) 3 1 16 459 163 296

Note. MZ and DZ indicate monozygotic and dizygotic twin pairs, respectively.

Conduct Problems (CP)

In the MTP, primary caregivers (nearly always the mother) completed the Strengths and Difficulty Questionnaire (SDQ; Goodman and Scott, 1999), along with a handful of additional items. The SDQ is highly correlated with other measures of psychopathology (e.g., the Child Behavior Checklist) and demonstrates good predictive validity for related diagnoses (Goodman and Scott, 1999). We specifically focused here on the Conduct Problems scale (i.e., stealing, hot temper, physical fights; 5-items). However, given the relatively low reliability of this 5-item scale (α =.64), we added two items assessing other behaviors that are also characteristic of CP (i.e., destroys things that belong to others; thinks things out before acting, reverse-scored). The addition of these two items increased the internal consistency reliability (α = .72). Moreover, an exploratory factor analysis yielded evidence of a clean break between the one- and two-factor solutions, with only one eigen value above 1.0 (3.907, next one was 0.855) and a reasonable RMSEA (.07). The seven items evidenced high loadings on that factor (ranging from .63 to .76). Only 5.5% of twins had missing CP data. The mean CP score was 2.49 (SD=2.20), with a range of 0 to 14 (skew was 1.38). To adjust for this positive skew, the data were log-transformed prior to analysis to better approximate normality (skew after transformation was −0.02).

In the TBED-C, mothers and fathers completed the Achenbach Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach and Rescorla, 2001) separately for each twin, while the twins’ teacher(s) completed the corresponding Achenbach Teacher Report Form (TRF; Achenbach and Rescorla, 2001). The teachers of 119 twins were not available for assessment (because the twins were home-schooled, because parental consents to contact the teachers were completed incorrectly, etc.), and our final teacher participation rate across the TBED-C was 83%. The twins themselves completed the Semi-structured Clinical Interview for Children and Adolescents (SCICA; McConaughy and Achenbach, 2001), the corresponding interview for youth ages 6–18, in separate rooms with different interviewers.

For the current study, we made use of the DSM-oriented CP scale (Achenbach and Rescorla, 2001, McConaughy and Achenbach, 2001), which comprises 17 CBCL items, 13 TRF items, and 19 SCICA items (with nearly identical item content) viewed as “very consistent” with the DSM-IV diagnostic category of Conduct Disorder (e.g., stealing, fighting, setting fires, cruelty to animals, etc.). Further validation work (Achenbach et al., 2001) indicated that the DSM-oriented CP scale accurately captures conduct disordered behavior and DSM diagnoses. Internal consistency reliabilities for the CBCL and TRF scales were adequate (α = .82, .87, and .77 for mother, teacher, and father informant reports, respectively). Roughly 10% of SCICA interviews were videotaped to obtain inter-rater reliability (the average intraclass correlation across raters was .88). The various informant-reports were combined to form multi-informant composites of child CP. Only 4 twins had missing composite scores. Consistent with manual recommendations (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001), analyses were conducted on the raw scale scores. The mean CP composite was 1.52 (SD=1.86), with a range of 0 to 15 (skew was 2.50). To adjust for this positive skew, the data were log-transformed prior to analysis to better approximate normality (skew after transformation was 0.64).

Neighborhood social processes

We assessed social processes from neighbors using the Neighborhood Matters questionnaire (Henry et al., 2014). The Social Cohesion scale consists of 30 items (α>.95) assesses perceptions of neighborhood support, help, and trust (e.g., would neighbors intervene if a fight broke out?). The 29-item Informal Social Control scale (α>.94) assesses the degree to which residents perceive an expectation among community residents to undertake activities that maintain social order (e.g., what would someone in your neighborhood do if … someone is trying to sell drugs to kids?). The 22-item Norms scale (α>.94) assesses perceptions of behavioral norms in the neighborhood, with a focus on norms regarding child welfare and neighborhood safety. Descriptive data for the neighborhood process variables are presented in Supplementary Table 1.

Operationalization of ‘neighborhood’

We made use of several approaches to operationalizing the twin family’s neighborhood, and thus the measurement of social processes in those neighborhoods (see Table 1). For the TBED-C, we geocoded all neighbor and twin family addresses with a 99.9% success rate using an “.hmtl” code that uses Google Maps address data to assign coordinates. We then mapped the geocoded coordinates using ArcGIS v10.3 (ESRI, Redlands, CA). We verified the spatial accuracy of 20 random geocoded locations by comparing the tabular data to ensure that the assigned county and city names correspond with the Census tract found in the original dataset. Using the geocoded coordinates, we computed the distance to the nearest neighbor in the dataset (median distance was 1.25km, mean = 2.5km, SD = 2.9km, with a range of 0.25m to 13.9km; families in which the nearest neighbor was more than 14km away were omitted from this particular operationalization). We also calculated average perceptions of neighborhood social processes for each twin family residential location using ArcMap software, averaging the informant-reports of all neighbors residing within 1km of the twins and all neighbors residing within 5km of the twins, respectively. Descriptive statistics for these various spatial covariates were then calculated using Stata v13 (College Station, TX).

Because the Department of Vital Records within the Michigan Department of Health and Human Services makes use of confidential driver’s license and birth record data to locate each family’s address for recruitment into the MTP, they do not release family addresses to researchers at MSU until the families indicate an interest in participating in a specific study (as families did with the TBED-C). However, they will release County and Census tract identifiers for MTP families. This allowed us to link the MTP data to the neighborhood informant data, calculating average perceptions of neighborhood social processes at the level of the Census tract. Slightly fewer than half of the MTP families (N=5,649) resided in a Census tract for which we had at least one neighborhood informant (see Table 1).

ANALYSES

Classical twin studies leverage the difference in the proportion of genes shared between monozygotic or MZ twins (who share 100% of their genes) and dizygotic or DZ twins (who share an average of 50% of their segregating genes) to estimate the relative contributions of genetic and environmental influences to the variance within observed behaviors or characteristics (phenotypes). More information on twin studies is provided elsewhere (Neale and Cardon, 1992). In the current study, we fitted the ‘univariate GxE’ model (Purcell, 2002), as shown in Supplementary Figure 1, to evaluate whether a given social process variable moderated the etiology of youth CP. Although prone to false positives when twin pairs are imperfectly correlated on the moderator (van der Sluis et al., 2012), it is the most appropriate GxE model when the twins are perfectly concordant on the moderator (van der Sluis et al., 2012), as is this case here.

Mx (Neale et al., 2003) was used to fit the GxE models to the data using Full-Information Maximum-Likelihood techniques. When fitting models to raw data, variances, covariances, and means are first freely estimated to get a baseline index of fit (minus twice the log-likelihood; −2lnL). Model fit was evaluated using four information theoretic indices that balance overall fit with model parsimony: the Akaike’s Information Criterion (AIC; Akaike, 1987), the Bayesian Information Criteria (BIC; Raftery, 1995), the sample-size adjusted Bayesian Information Criterion (SABIC; Sclove, 1987), and the Deviance Information Criterion (DIC; Spiegelhalter et al., 2002). The lowest or most negative AIC, BIC, SABIC, and DIC among a series of nested models is considered best. Because fit indices do not always agree (they place different values on parsimony, among other things), we reasoned that the best fitting model should yield lower or more negative values for at least 3 of the 4 fit indices.

Prior to analyses, each moderator variable was floored at 0 and divided by its maximum, providing a continuous measure of each social process variable that ranged from 0 to 1. Twin sex and age were regressed out of all twin data, in keeping with prior recommendations (McGue and Bouchard, 1984). Finally, as it is recommended that unstandardized or absolute parameter estimates be presented in etiologic moderation models (Purcell, 2002), we standardized our log-transformed and residualized child CP scores to have a mean of zero and a standard deviation of one to facilitate interpretation of the unstandardized value.

RESULTS

Phenotypic and intraclass correlations

Phenotypic correlations with CP are presented in Table 2. As seen there, the three social process variables demonstrated weak correlations with twin CP regardless of the operationalization of neighborhood (note, however, that the magnitude of the phenotypic correlation between a moderator and an outcome has no bearing on the presence of etiologic moderation; Purcell, 2002). Correlations across the various operationalizations within the TBED-C are presented in Table 3, separately for each social process variable. The strongest associations were observed between the nearest neighbor and the 1 km radius (rs were .57 to .76), followed by their respective associations with the 5 km radius (rs were .33 to .49). We were also able to examine associations with the MTP Census tract operationalizations, given that the TBED-C is nested within the MTP. Census tract evidenced its clearest associations with the 1km radius (rs were .32 to .48) and somewhat weaker associations with nearest neighbor and 5km (rs were .18 to .29).

Table 2.

Phenotypic and Intraclass Correlations

Construct Operationalization Correlations
r with CP rMZ rDZ
Social cohesion Census tract1 −.05* .70*/.68* .39*/.35*
Nearest neighbor2 −.05* .61*/.60* .38*/.36*
All neighbors within 5 km2 −.06* .62*/.61* .34*/.39*
All neighbors within 1 km2 −.11* .63*/.65* .43*/.34*
Informal social control Census tract1 −.03* .69*/.68* .36*/.38*
Nearest neighbor2 −.07* .61*/.59* .38*/.35*
All neighbors within 5 km2 −.09* .56*/.66* .39*/.34*
All neighbors within 1 km2 −.09* .61*/.64* .42*/.37*
Norms Census tract1 −.02 .71*/.67* .38*/.36*
Nearest neighbor2 −.03 .61*/.58* .36*/.39*
All neighbors within 5 km2 −.02 .56*/.67* .37*/.36*
All neighbors within 1 km2 −.01 .66*/.61* .40*/.40*

Note.

1

and 2 indicate the MTP and TBED-C samples, respectively. rMZ and rDZ indicate the intraclass twin correlations for MZ and DZ twin pairs, respectively

*

correlation is greater than zero at p<.05

Table 3.

Correlations among measures of neighborhood social processes in the TBED-C

Social Cohesion Informal Social Control Norms
Geographic unit 1 2 3 4 1 2 3 4 1 2 3 4
1. Nearest neighbor
2. All neighbors within 1 km .76** .57** .66**
3. All neighbors within 5 km .42** .49** .34** .49** .33** .41**
4. Census tract .21** .48** .29** .18** .44** .25** .20** .32** .23**

Note.

**

p<.01

Intraclass correlations are also presented in Table 2, separately by zygosity and level of each social process variable (the latter were divided at the median for these analyses, although note that they were analyzed continuously in the GxE analyses below). As seen there, the MZ correlations for CP were significantly larger than the DZ correlations in all cases, regardless of sample or neighborhood operationalization. In no case, however, did either the MZ or the DZ correlation significantly change with level of the social process variable (i.e., the MZ correlation was .70 at low levels of Census tract social cohesion and .68 at high levels). Such findings preliminarily argue against the presence of etiologic moderation of CP by neighborhood social processes.

GxE results

Formal tests of moderation were conducted next. Individual GxE model fitting results are presented in Table 4. As seen there, the no moderation model generally provided the best fit to the data, regardless of sample or neighborhood operationalization. To enhance discussion, however, estimated paths and moderators from the full linear models are presented in Table 5. Results are discussed in turn.

Table 4.

Fit Indices for the GxE analyses

Sample -2lnL df AIC BIC SABIC DIC
SOCIAL COHESION
Census tract
  Non-linear moderation 28116.79 10557 7002.79 −31204.19 −14430.83 −21502.96
  Linear moderation 28119.07 10560 6999.07 −31215.92 −14437.79 −21511.93
  No moderation 28120.35 10563 6994.35 −31228.14 −14445.24 −21521.39
Nearest neighbor
  Non-linear moderation 5370.67 1989 1392.67 −4184.43 −1025.84 −2356.66
  Linear moderation 5372.32 1992 1388.32 −4193.96 −1030.61 −2363.44
  No moderation 5380.67 1995 1390.67 −4200.15 −1032.04 −2366.87
All neighbors within 5 km
  Non-linear moderation 4567.95 1681 1205.95 −3381.43 −712.26 −1836.70
  Linear moderation 4573.95 1684 1205.95 −3388.54 −714.61 −1841.05
  No moderation 4580.66 1687 1206.66 −3395.30 −716.60 −1845.05
All neighbors within 1 km
  Non-linear moderation 2520.44 907 706.44 −1519.31 −80.03 −685.83
  Linear moderation 2522.21 910 702.21 −1527.62 −83.58 −691.38
  No moderation 2528.16 913 702.16 −1533.83 −85.04 −694.84
INFORMAL SOCIAL CONTROL
Census tract
  Non-linear moderation 27916.42 10479 6958.42 −30931.24 −14281.82 −21301.68
  Linear moderation 27923.48 10482 6959.48 −30940.56 −14286.37 −21308.25
  No moderation 27925.57 10485 6955.57 −30952.37 −14293.41 −21317.30
Nearest neighbor
  Non-linear moderation 5418.70 2011 1396.70 −4247.40 −1053.85 −2399.41
  Linear moderation 5423.02 2014 1395.02 −4255.62 −1057.31 −2404.87
  No moderation 5434.56 2017 1400.56 −4260.23 −1057.15 −2406.73
All neighbors within 5 km
  Non-linear moderation 4559.95 1681 1197.95 −3385.43 −716.26 −1840.70
  Linear moderation 4563.14 1684 1195.14 −3393.95 −720.01 −1846.45
  No moderation 4577.75 1687 1203.75 −3396.75 −718.06 −1846.50
All neighbors within 1 km
  Non-linear moderation 2511.60 907 697.60 −1523.72 −84.45 −690.25
  Linear moderation 2517.01 910 697.01 −1530.22 −86.18 −693.98
  No moderation 2528.67 913 702.67 −1533.58 −84.78 −694.59
NORMS
Census tract
  Non-linear moderation 27952.46 10495 6962.46 −30989.74 −14314.89 −21345.48
  Linear moderation 27958.57 10498 6962.57 −30999.54 −14319.92 −21352.52
  No moderation 27965.30 10501 6963.30 −31009.03 −14324.65 −21359.25
Nearest neighbor
  Non-linear moderation 5401.19 1997 1407.19 −4200.78 −1029.48 −2365.66
  Linear moderation 5402.81 2000 1402.81 −4210.34 −1034.28 −2372.47
  No moderation 5404.77 2003 1398.77 −4219.73 −1038.90 −2379.09
All neighbors within 5 km
  Non-linear moderation 4576.82 1681 1214.82 −3377.00 −707.83 −1832.26
  Linear moderation 4579.92 1684 1211.92 −3385.56 −711.63 −1838.07
  No moderation 4580.77 1687 1206.77 −3395.24 −716.55 −1844.99
All neighbors within 1 km
  Non-linear moderation 2523.17 907 709.17 −1517.94 −78.66 −684.46
  Linear moderation 2526.82 910 706.82 −1525.31 −81.27 −689.07
  No moderation 2327.29 913 701.29 −1534.27 −85.47 −695.28

Note. The best fitting model for a given set of analyses is highlighted in bold font, and is indicated by the lowest AIC (Akaike’s Information Criterion), BIC (Bayesian Information Criterion), SABIC (sample size adjusted Bayesian Information Criterion), and DIC (Deviance Information Criterion) values for at least 3 of the 4 fit indices. When neither of two models clearly provided a better fit relative to the other, both are italicized.

Table 5.

Unstandardized path and moderator parameter estimates for the full linear moderation models

PATHS LINEAR MODERATORS
a c e A1 C1 E1
SOCIAL COHESION
MTP, Census tract-level 0.83* 0.23 0.50* −0.05 −0.07 0.06
TBED-C, nearest neighbor 0.76* 0.68 0.71* −0.05 −0.63 −0.16
TBED-C, all neighbors within 5 km 0.78* 0.43 0.66* −0.04 −0.29 −0.13
TBED-C, all neighbors within 1 km 0.79* 0.56 0.69* −0.10 −0.34 −0.14
INFORMAL SOCIAL CONTROL
MTP, Census tract-level 0.82* 0.36 0.50* −0.02 −0.22 0.05
TBED-C, nearest neighbor 0.84* 0.26 0.75* −0.18 0.12 −0.17
TBED-C, all neighbors within 5 km 0.78* 0.28 0.68* −0.05 0.05 −0.13*
TBED-C, all neighbors within 1 km 0.80* 0.44 0.86* −0.11 −0.04 −0.31*
NORMS
MTP, Census tract-level 0.79* 0.23 0.43* 0.03* −0.07 0.15*
TBED-C, nearest neighbor 0.43 0.61 0.79* 0.37 −0.36 −0.23
TBED-C, all neighbors within 5 km 0.72* 0.26 0.62* 0.08 0.04 −0.03
TBED-C, all neighbors within 1 km 0.67* 0.40 0.69* 0.10 −0.03 −0.11

Note. A, C, and E (upper and lower case) respectively represent genetic, shared, and non-shared environmental parameters on child conduct problems (CP). Because lower levels of each neighborhood social process variable were coded as 0, the genetic and environmental contributions to child CP at this level can be obtained by squaring the path estimates (i.e., a, c, and e). At higher levels, linear moderators (i.e., A1, C1, E1) were added to the paths using the following equation: Unstandardized VarianceTotal = (a + A1(informal social control))2 + (c + C1(informal social control))2 + (e + E1(informal social control))2. Bold font and an asterisk indicate that the parameter estimate was significant at p<.05.

For social cohesion, the no moderation model fitted the data better than the moderation model regardless of sample or operationalization. The genetic and non-shared environmental moderators were uniformly non-significant and small in magnitude. Although moderators for the shared environment were similarly non-significant, they were more variable in magnitude, ranging from −.07 (census tracts) to −.63 (nearest neighbor). The direction of these non-significant effects is particularly interesting, since it tracks prior findings for the structural effects of disadvantage (i.e., shared environmental influences are lower in less disadvantaged neighborhoods). Such findings raise the possibility that social cohesion may moderate the etiology of CP, but that we are unable to detect this effect in these analyses because we are underpowered. To directly examine this possibility, we evaluated our statistical power to detect etiologic moderation, conducting a series of simulations. Results are plotted in Figure 1. As seen there, we were sufficiently powered to detect small non-shared environmental moderators and moderate genetic moderators in samples of 1,001 families, but underpowered to detect shared environmental moderators until they were large in magnitude (i.e., we had 80.5% power to detect C moderators of 0.6). In samples of 5,502 pairs and larger, however, we were sufficiently powered to detect shared environmental moderators as small as .30 and genetic moderators as small as .20. Such findings argue against low power as an explanation for our non-significant results in the large MTP sample.

Figure 1. Power analyses.

Figure 1.

Note. We simulated data containing etiologic moderators of varying magnitude, from small (0.2) to large (0.6), in two sample sizes (N=1,001 pairs and 5,502 pairs). Our power to detect genetic (A), shared environmental (C), and non-shared environmental (E) moderators of varying sizes are indicated for each sample size, respectively. We assumed that our sample was 35% MZ and 65% DZ (the average across the various samples examined herein), and that a, c, and e path estimates were .72, .30, and .60, respectively.

For informal social control, the linear moderation model fitted the data as well as, but not better than, the no moderation model for 3 of the 4 operationalizations. When examining the estimated parameters, the non-shared environmental moderator was statistically significant (albeit small) for the 1km and 5km radii, potentially highlighting a small reduction in the importance of non-shared environmental influences on CP with increasing informal social control. However, these findings did not persist when examining perceptions of informal social control in the nearest neighbor or by Census tracts (the latter of which was signed in the opposite direction, a notable discrepancy given the sample size for those analyses). Such findings diminish our confidence regarding non-shared environmental moderation of CP by informal social control, but do not rule it out entirely.

For norms, the no moderation model uniformly fitted the data best, regardless of sample or operationalization. Even so, the genetic and non-shared environmental moderators were statistically significant for the Census tract analyses, indicating the possibility of very small increases in genetic influences and small increases in non-shared environmental influences with stronger norms. That said, the direction of the non-shared environmental moderators varied across the various operationalizations of neighborhood, undercutting our confidence in the presence of meaningful non-shared environmental moderation.

Discussion

The primary goal of the current study was to evaluate, for the first time, whether and how neighborhood social processes might moderate the etiology of child CP. Results provided no consistent evidence that neighborhood social processes moderated the etiology of child CP. The linear moderation model never provided a better fit to the CP data, regardless of operationalization or sample. What’s more, when significant moderators did emerge, they were not replicated across the other operationalizations of that neighborhood social process, a key test given both the replicability crisis in psychology and the acknowledged potential for spurious findings in studies focused on statistical interactions (Eaves, 2006, Cohen et al., 2013). Power analyses indicated that these null findings are not likely to be a function of low statistical power. We thus conclude that, if there is any etiologic moderation of child CP by neighborhood social processes, these effects are small to very small, and inconsistent.

Of note, these findings stand in rather sharp contrast to those of GxE studies examining neighborhood structural characteristics as etiologic moderators of youth CP (Tuvblad et al., 2006, Cleveland, 2003, Burt et al., 2016). Tuvblad and colleagues (2006), for example, examined 1,133 population-based twin pairs in Sweden, and found that shared environmental influences on antisocial behavior were more important for adolescents residing in disadvantaged neighborhoods (i.e., those with blight, crime, etc.), while genetic influences were more important for adolescents residing in advantaged neighborhoods. We have since extended these findings in the TBED-C, evaluating Census reports of the % of families residing below the poverty line (Burt et al., 2016), maternal reports of neighborhood disadvantage, and neighbor informant-reports (i.e., nearest neighbor, all within 1km, all within 5km; Burt et al., submitted). As with prior work, results pointed squarely to stronger shared environmental influences on youth antisocial behavior in disadvantaged neighborhoods as compared to advantaged neighborhoods. The consistency of the findings for the structural characteristics of neighborhood disadvantage across independent research teams is rather remarkable, given the use of different samples with unique measurement strategies. Such findings collectively argue that, consistent with the bioecological model of GxE (Bronfenbrenner and Ceci, 1994, Scarr, 1992), youth CP is more environmental in origin in structurally disadvantaged neighborhood contexts.

Limitations

There are several limitations to the current study. First, although we collected resident perceptions of neighborhood social processes, we did not collect resident perceptions of neighborhood boundaries. It is thus possible that, when they completed their questionnaires, neighbors were referencing a neighborhood different from (but overlapping with or in close proximity to) the one in which the twin family resides. That said, the notable consistency of our results across the various combinations of neighbor informant-reports (and especially the nearest neighborhood informant) suggests that this limitation is unlikely to be the source of our null findings. Regardless, future work should more precisely evaluate neighborhood boundaries when attempting to constructively replicate the current results.

Second, analyses of at-risk samples inevitably raise concerns regarding the generalizability of the results (i.e., do they also extent to population-based samples that include proportionally fewer at-risk youth?). This concern is allayed here by the fact that our findings in the TBED-C extended to a large population-based sample of twins (the MTP), indicating that these effects may generalize beyond at-risk samples. Next, given the power hungry nature of these models, we also regressed sex out of the child CP data prior to analysis. Fortunately, this decision is in keeping with prior meta-analyses arguing against sex differences in heritability estimates for child CP (Burt, 2009a), as well as recent work indicating the clear absence of joint etiologic moderation of CP by sex and neighborhood disadvantage (Burt et al., 2018). Next, our CP data were log-transformed prior to analysis to adjust for positive skew, which can either artefactually inflate or suppress evidence of GxE. Other transformations of the data could theoretically yield somewhat different results.

Finally, although our neighborhood informant sample includes several participants per neighborhood, it is unclear whether participating neighbors were representative of adults in their neighborhoods or how perceptions of social processes might align with more objective measures of these social processes (i.e., direct observations of neighborhood social processes). To preliminarily evaluate the first issue, we examined whether ethnicity data in the Census predicted neighbor self-reports of ethnicity. The two were highly correlated .68 (p<.001), suggesting that neighbors in our sample may be representative of their overall neighborhoods. However, future work should explore this issue in more depth.

Conclusions

The current results are not consistent with the notion that neighborhood social processes moderate the genetic and environmental origins of child CP. This conclusion has two key implications. First, although the current results might seem to undermine the importance of neighborhood social processes, we would argue against this interpretation, as they instead only suggest that neighborhood social processes do not exert their effects on child CP via etiologic moderation. Our results are silent in regards to other possible etiologic connections. For example, the links between neighborhood social processes and child CP could reflect main effects of neighborhood social processes, in that they protect youth from engaging in high levels of CP regardless of their genetic risk. Alternately, associations could reflect genotype-environment correlations, whereby children at lower genetic risk for CP seek out protective environments (perhaps via strong relationships with particular neighbors) in order to buffer themselves from the conditions in their neighborhoods. Unfortunately, we were unable to examine these alternate possibilities here, since Neighbor perceptions of neighborhood-level social processes do not vary across twins, and thus we cannot decompose the covariance between child CP and neighborhood social processes into their genetic and environmental components. Future studies should collect twin perceptions of neighborhood social processes, which are amenable to variance/covariance decomposition, thereby allowing us to better understand the origins of the association between neighborhood social processes and child CP.

Second, when viewed in conjunction with extant studies evaluating neighborhood structural characteristics as etiologic moderators, the current findings suggest that efforts to identify the specific neighborhood-level factors that moderate the etiology of CP in childhood should focus on structural characteristics linked to neighborhood disadvantage. What might these neighborhood-level characteristics be? One possibility relates to exposure to neighborhood crime and/or CP among neighbors, as prior theory and empirical work has indicated youth CP may be influenced in part by the phenomenon of social contagion (Papachristos et al., 2015, Jencks and Mayer, 1990, Burt et al., 2019), or the spread of particular outcomes across social networks (Christakis and Fowler, 2013). Another possibility centers on exposure to environmental contaminants, as such exposures are experienced disproportionately by those living in impoverished environments (Perera et al., 2002). This may be particularly the case in Rust Belt states with high levels of lead exposure. For example, of the 32,973 Detroit children younger than 6 years of age who were tested for lead exposure in 2004 (this corresponds to 35.3% of all young children in Detroit at that time), fully one third had blood lead levels greater than the CDC cut-off (i.e., 5ug/dL) for high lead exposure (https://www.michigan.gov/mdhhs/0,5885,7-339-73971_4911_4913---,00.html). Such numbers are tragic, given the now uncontested effects of lead exposure on later antisocial behavior (Nevin, 2007). Future work should seek to examine these possibilities.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary Material

Acknowledgements

This project was supported by R01-MH081813 from the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH), R01-HD066040 from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute for Child Health and Human Development (NICHD), Strategic Partnership Grant #11-SPG-2408 and institutional funds from Michigan State University. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIMH, NICHD, or the National Institutes of Health. The primary author had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. The authors thank all participating twins and their families for making this work possible.

Footnotes

None of the authors report any conflicts of interest.

References

  1. ACHENBACH TM, DUMENCI L & RESCORLA LA 2001. Ratings of relations between DSM-IV diagnostic categories and items of the CBCL/6–18, TRF, and YSR, Burlington, VT, University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families; Available at www.ASEBA.org. [Google Scholar]
  2. ACHENBACH TM & RESCORLA LA 2001. Manual for ASEBA School-Age Forms & Profiles, Burlington, VT, University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families. [Google Scholar]
  3. AHERN J, GALEA S, HUBBARD A & SYME SL 2009. Neighborhood smoking norms modify the relation between collective efficacy and smoking behavior. Drug & Alcohol Dependence, 100, 138–145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. AKAIKE H 1987. Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika, 52, 317–332. [Google Scholar]
  5. BRONFENBRENNER U 1979. The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design, Cambridge, MA, Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
  6. BRONFENBRENNER U & CECI SJ 1994. Nature-nurture reconceptualized in developmental perspective: A bioecological model. Psychological Review, 101, 568–586. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. BUHRMESTER M, KWANG T & GOSLING SD 2011. Amazon’s Mechanical Turk: A New Source of Inexpensive, Yet High-Quality, Data? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 3–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. BURT SA 2009a. Are there meaningful etiological differences within antisocial behavior? Results of a meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 29, 163–178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. BURT SA 2009b. Rethinking environmental contributions to child and adolescent psychopathology: A meta-analysis of shared environmental influences. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 608–637. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. BURT SA 2011. Some key issues in the study of gene-environment interplay: Activation, deactivation, and the role of development. Research in Human Development, 8, 192–210. [Google Scholar]
  11. BURT SA & KLUMP KL 2012. How does the inclusion of twins conceived via fertility treatments influence the results of twin studies? Twin Research and Human Genetics, 15, 746–752. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. BURT SA & KLUMP KL 2013. The Michigan State University Twin Registry (MSUTR): An update. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 16, 344–350. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. BURT SA, KLUMP KL, GORMAN-SMITH D & NEIDERHISER JM 2016. Neighborhood disadvantage alters the origins of children’s non-aggressive conduct problems. Clinical Psychological Science. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  14. BURT SA, PEARSON AL, NEIDERHISER JM & KLUMP KL submitted. Non-aggressive antisocial behavior is more environmental in origin in disadvantaged neighborhood contexts [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  15. BURT SA, PEARSON AL, RZOTKIEWICZ A, KLUMP KL & NEIDERHISER JM 2019. It really does take a village: The role of neighbors in the etiology of non-aggressive rule-breaking. Development & Psychopathology, 31, 713–725. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. BURT SA, SLAWINSKI BL & KLUMP KL 2018. Are there sex differences in the etiology of youth antisocial behavior? Journal of abnormal psychology, 127, 66. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. CHRISTAKIS NA & FOWLER JH 2013. Social contagion theory: examining dynamic social networks and human behavior. Statistics in medicine, 32, 556–577. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. CLEVELAND HH 2003. Disadvantaged neighborhoods and adolescent aggression: Behavioral genetic evidence of contextual effects. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 13, 211–238. [Google Scholar]
  19. COHEN J, COHEN P, WEST SG & AIKEN LS 2013. Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences, Routledge. [Google Scholar]
  20. COIE JD, MILLER-JOHNSON S & BAGWELL C 2000. Prevention science. In: SAMEROFF AJ, LWIS M & MILLER S (eds.) Handbook of developmental psychopathology. 2 ed. Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers. [Google Scholar]
  21. DAMM AP & DUSTMANN C 2014. Does growing up in a high crime neighborhood affect youth criminal behavior? The American Economic Review, 104, 1806–1832. [Google Scholar]
  22. EAVES LJ 2006. Genotype x environment interaction in psychopathology: Fact or artifact? Twin Research and Human Genetics, 9, 1–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. GOODMAN R & SCOTT S 1999. Comparing the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire and the Child Behavior Checklist: Is small beautiful? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 27, 17–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. GOODNIGHT JA, LAHEY BB, VAN HULLE CA, RODGERS JL, RATHOUZ PJ, WALDMAN ID & D’ONOFRIO BM 2012. A quasi-experimental analysis of the influence of neighborhood disadvantage on child and adolescent conduct problems. Journal of abnormal psychology, 121, 95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. HENRY DB, GORMAN-SMITH D, SCHOENY ME & TOLAN PH 2014. “Neighborhood Matters”: Assessment of Neighborhood Social Processes. American journal of community psychology, 54, 187–204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. HICKS BM, SOUTH SC, DIRAGO AC, IACONO WG & MCGUE M 2009. Environmental adversity and increasing genetic risk for externalizing disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 66, 640–648. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. JENCKS C & MAYER E 1990. The social consequences of growing up in a poor neighborhood. In: LYNN L & MCGEARY M (eds.) Inner-city poverty in the United States. Washington, D.C: National Academy Press. [Google Scholar]
  28. JOHNSON W in press. Genetic and environmental influences on behavior: Capturing all the interplay. Psychological Review. [DOI] [PubMed]
  29. KLUMP KL & BURT SA 2006. The Michigan State University Twin Registry (MSUTR): Genetic, environmental, and neurobiological influences on behavior across development. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 9, 971–977. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. LEVENTHAL T & BROOKS-GUNN J 2000. The neighborhoods they live in: The effects of neighborhood residence on child and adolescent outcomes. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 309–337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. LEWONTIN R 1995. Human Diversity, New York, Scientific American Library. [Google Scholar]
  32. LUDWIG J, DUNCAN GJ & HIRSCHFIELD P 2001. Urban poverty and juvenile crime: Evidence from a randomized housing-mobility experiment. QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF ECONOMICS, 116, 655–680. [Google Scholar]
  33. LUDWIG J, LIEBMAN JB, KLING JR, DUNCAN GJ, KATZ LF, KESSLER RC & SANBONMATSU L 2008. What can we learn about neighborhood effects from the Moving to Opportunity experiment. American Journal of Sociology, 114, 144–188. [Google Scholar]
  34. MCCONAUGHY SH & ACHENBACH TM 2001. Manual for the Semistructured Interview for Children and Adolescents Burlington, VT, University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families. [Google Scholar]
  35. MCGUE M & BOUCHARD TJJ 1984. Adjustment of twin data for the effects of age and sex. Behavior Genetics, 14, 325–343. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. MOFFITT TE, CASPI A & RUTTER M 2006. Measured gene-environment interactions in psychopathology. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 5–27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. MORENOFF JD, SAMPSON RJ & RAUDENBUSH SW 2001. Neighborhood inequality, collective efficacy, and the spatial dynamics of urban violence. Criminology, 39, 517–558. [Google Scholar]
  38. NEALE MC, BOKER SM, XIE G & MAES HH 2003. Mx: Statistical Modeling, VCU Box 900126, Richmond, VA 23298: Department of Psychiatry, 6th Edition. [Google Scholar]
  39. NEALE MC & CARDON LR 1992. Methodology for Genetic Studies of Twins and Families, Boston, MA, Kluwer Academic Publishers. [Google Scholar]
  40. NEVIN R 2007. Understanding international crime trends: the legacy of preschool lead exposure. Environmental research, 104, 315–336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. PAPACHRISTOS AV, WILDEMAN C & ROBERTO E 2015. Tragic, but not random: The social contagion of nonfatal gunshot injuries. Social Science & Medicine, 125, 139–150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. PEETERS H, VAN GESTEL S, VLIETINCK R, DEROM C & DEROM R 1998. Validation of a telephone zygosity questionnaire in twins of known zygosity. Behavior Genetics, 28, 159–161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. PENNINGTON BF, MCGRATH LM, ROSENBERG J, BARNARD H, SMITH SD, WILLCUTT EG, FRIEND A, DEFRIES JC & OLSON RK 2009. Gene X environment interactions in reading disability and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Developmental Psychology, 45, 77–89. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. PERERA FP, ILLMAN SM, KINNEY PL, WHYATT EM, KELVIN EA, SHEPHARD P, EVANS D, FULLILOVE M, FORD J, MILLER RL, MEYER IH & RAUH VA 2002. The challenge of preventing environmentally related disease in young children: community-based research in New York City. Environmental Health Perspectives, 110, 197–204. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. PLOMIN R, DEFRIES JC & LOEHLIN JC 1977. Genotype-environment interaction and correlation in the analysis of human behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 84, 309–322. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. PURCELL S 2002. Variance components model for gene-environment interaction in twin analysis. Twin Research, 5, 554–571. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. RAFTERY AE 1995. Bayesian model selection in social research. Sociological Methodology, 25, 111–163. [Google Scholar]
  48. RAINE A 2002. Biosocial studies of antisocial and violent behavior in children and adolescents: A review. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 30, 311–326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. REED E, SILVERMAN JG, RAJ A, DECKER MR & MILLER E 2011. Male perpetration of teen dating violence: Associations with neighborhood violence involvement, gender attitudes, and perceived peer and neighborhood norms. Journal of Urban Health, 88, 226–239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. RUTTER M, SILBERG J, O’CONNOR TJ & SIMONOFF E 1999a. Genetics and child psychiatry: I Advances in quantitative and molecular genetics. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 3–18. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. RUTTER M, SILBERG J, O’CONNOR TJ & SIMONOFF E 1999b. Genetics and child psychiatry: II Empirical research findings. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 19–55. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. SAMPSON RJ 2003. The neighborhood context of well-being. Perspectives in biology and medicine, 46, S53–S64. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. SAMPSON RJ 2008. Moving to inequality: Neighborhood effects and experiments meet structure. AJS; American journal of sociology, 114, 189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. SAMPSON RJ, RAUDENBUSH SW & EARLS FJ 1997. Neighborhoods and violent crime: A multilevel study of collective efficacy. Science, 277, 918–924. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. SCARR S 1992. Developmental theories for the 1990s: Developmental and individual differences. Child Development, 63, 1–19. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. SCLOVE LS 1987. Application of model-selection criteria to some problems in multivariate analysis. Psychometrika, 53, 333–343. [Google Scholar]
  57. SIMON TR, IKEDA RM, SMITH EP, REESE LRE, RABINER DL, MILLER S, WINN D-M, DODGE KA, ASHER SR & HORNE AM 2009. The ecological effects of universal and selective violence prevention programs for middle school students: A randomized trial. Journal of consulting and clinical psychology, 77, 526. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. SPIEGELHALTER DJ, BEST NG, CARLIN BP & VAN DER LINDE A 2002. Bayesian measures of model complexity and fit. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B 64, 583–639. [Google Scholar]
  59. TOLAN P, GORMAN-SMITH D & HENRY D 2004. Supporting families in a high-risk setting: proximal effects of the SAFEChildren preventive intervention. Journal of consulting and clinical psychology, 72, 855. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. TUVBLAD C, GRANN M & LICHTENSTEIN P 2006. Heritability for adolescent antisocial behavior differs with socioeconomic status: Gene-environment interaction. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 734–743. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. VAN DER SLUIS S, POSTHUMA D & DOLAN CV 2012. A note on false positives and power in GxE modeling of twin data. Behavior Genetics, 42, 170–186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. WIGHT RG, BOTTICELLO AL & ANESHENSEL CS 2006. Socioeconomic context, social support, and adolescent mental health: A multilevel investigation. Journal of youth and adolescence, 35, 109. [Google Scholar]
  63. WRIGHT EM & FAGAN AA 2013. The cycle of violence in context: Exploring the moderating roles of neighborhood disadvantage and cultural norms. Criminology, 51, 217–249. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Material

RESOURCES