Abstract
Previous (predominantly) laboratory studies reported positive relations of physical activity (or everyday bodily movement) with executive functioning, some even showed effects on creative thinking. Furthermore, positive-activated affect was found to be positively associated with everyday bodily movements and creativity. The mechanisms, however, underlying these relationships are poorly understood. The aim of this study was twofold: Firstly, we investigated whether everyday bodily movement was associated with creative performance. Secondly, we examined if positive-activated affect may mediate the association between bodily movement and creative performance. In a sample of 79 participants everyday bodily movement was recorded during five consecutive days using accelerometers. Creativity in the figural and verbal domain was assessed with performance tests, along with self-reported positive-activated affect as a trait. Findings revealed that creativity, positive-activated affect, and everyday bodily movement were associated with each other. However, positive-activated affect did not mediate the association between everyday bodily movement and creative performance. The pattern of findings argues for shared variance between bodily movement and creativity (fluency and originality) that is largely independent from variations in positive-activated affect.
Subject terms: Psychology, Quality of life
Introduction
It is well-known that physical activity has many beneficial effects on physical health. A physically active lifestyle goes along with decreases in blood pressure, protects against chronic (e.g., type 2 diabetes) and cardiovascular diseases, and decreases the risk for several types of cancer1–3. Beyond physical health and life expectancy4,5, beneficial effects of physical activity may expand to mental health (e.g., decreased depressive symptoms6) and psychological well-being7–11.
In addition, studies showed a positive association between physical activity and a broad range of cognitive skills12–17, however, Young and colleagues18 did not find an association between physical activity and cognition. Performance gains were often seen in comparatively basic types of cognitive functions, such as indicators derived from the Stroop task, or speed/accuracy measures in different attention or memory tasks12,17. Only a limited number of studies investigated the effects of physical activity on cognitive functions such as divergent thinking/creativity, which has indeed been found to be sensitive to physical activity interventions19,20.
Gondola and Tuckman21 were among the first who showed improved verbal creative performance (measured by means of an Alternate Uses task, AUT22) after participation in a running intervention23,24. Cavallera et al.25 found a positive association between the elaboration performance in a figural creative ideation task (Torrance Test of Creative Thinking, TTCT26) and the self-reported hours of sport activities per week (but see the work of Ramocki27 for a verbal creativity task). The association between self-reported physical activity and creative performance nicely corresponds to the finding that physically more fit school-aged children outperformed their less fit peers in a number of creative (and cognitive) thinking tasks28. Importantly, beneficial effects were not only found for chronic aspects of physical activity, such as exercise and fitness29, but also for more acute mechanisms accompanying (or following) bodily movement. To avoid conceptual confusion, we use the term bodily movement throughout this article when referring to daily life whole-body movements of different intensity (e.g., walking, running, but also sitting and lying). For example, Blanchette et al.30 found improved creative performance (in the TTCT) directly after moderate aerobic exercise20,24,27,31,32. Oppezzo and Schwartz19 found that even low-dose movement interventions (i.e., brief walking sessions) resulted in improved creative performance, however, others did not find increases after a walk or single bouts of exercise33–36.
While the majority of findings were based on intervention studies in controlled experimental settings (e.g., laboratory assessment, physical exercise training program21,32) and cross-sectional studies using a broad range of physical fitness measures25,28, studies using ecologically valid and objective measures of everyday physical activity are lacking. Consequently, it is still unknown if the association between physical activity and creativity generalizes to everyday life situations37. Therefore, the present study goes one step further and investigated this association by continuously monitoring participants’ everyday bodily movement via acceleration sensors throughout a time period of five days—including the weekend38. This ambulatorily assessed degree of physical activity was then correlated with the participants’ total creative performance assessed by summing up the originality of ideas of a verbal and figural creativity task (AUT, TTCT).
Although literature generally indicates a link between physical activity and creativity, only few studies investigated why physical activity is associated with creativity and addressed potential underlying mechanisms that might—at least partly—explain this relationship20,39. One potential factor is positive affect, which has been found to play a major role in both creativity and physical activity. The association between physical activity and positive affect is a well-established finding with small to medium effect size7,40–42, which was also found in ambulatory assessment studies43,44 (similar findings are available for arousal32,36). Specifically, positive-activated affect (PAA) and perceived energy seem to increase with increasing physical activity45,46, which was also shown in meta-analyses40,41.
Most important for the present investigation, Dreu and Collegues47 established a relationship between PAA and creative performance48,49. They assumed that PAA leads to higher creative fluency and originality by enhanced cognitive flexibility and reduced perseverance. Strikingly, a similar association was indicated by ecological ambulatory assessments studies reporting a simultaneous increase of positive affect and everyday creative activities50–54. The meta-analysis of Baas et al.55 estimated this correlational effect of small size (r = 0.08). Therefore, the second aim of the present study was to examine whether PAA may mediate the association between physical activity and creativity17,20,47 and therefore serve as a potential explanation of the link between physical activity and creativity.
It was firstly hypothesized, given the results of Oppezzo and Schwartz19 and Schwerdtfeger et al.44, that everyday bodily movement was associated with creativity and PAA. Secondly, given the assumption of Steinberg et al.20 it was hypothesized, that the association between everyday bodily movement and creativity was mediated by PAA. A significant mediation effect would indicate that PAA may at least partly explain the association between physical activity and creativity. Since specific prior evidence about the effect sizes of the correlations between physical activity, creativity, and positive affect exists, we decided for the use of Bayesian mediation analysis. This statistical approach allows incorporating prior information in the analysis and improves the efficiency of estimation and the statistical power of the analysis56.
Results
Association between everyday bodily movement and total creative performance
In accordance with the first hypothesis, everyday bodily movement (CPM) was positively correlated with total creative performance (r = 0.32, p = 0.004). Furthermore, PAA was positively associated with both total creative performance (r = 0.28, p = 0.014) and everyday bodily movement (r = 0.23, p = 0.040; see Fig. 1).
Mediation effect of PAA on the association between bodily movement and total creative performance
In line with the Pearson correlations, the Bayesian mediation analysis revealed that bodily movement was positively associated with both creative performance (b = 0.27, lower CI = 0.06, upper CI = 0.49) and PAA (b = 0.23, lower CI = 0.01, upper CI = 0.46). These findings are depicted in Table 1.
Table 1.
Estimate (SE) | l-95% CI | u-95% CI | Effective sample | Rath | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Total creative performance (intercept) | 0.00 (0.11) | − 0.21 | 0.21 | 24,908 | 1.00 |
PAA (intercept) | 0.00 (0.11) | − 0.22 | 0.22 | 23,482 | 1.00 |
Bodily movement (CPM)—total creative performance | 0.27 (0.11) | 0.06 | 0.49 | 21,405 | 1.00 |
Bodily movement (CPM)—PAA | 0.23 (0.11) | 0.01 | 0.46 | 26,751 | 1.00 |
Total creative performance—PAA | 0.21 (0.11) | − 0.01 | 0.44 | 24,868 | 1.00 |
PAA positive-activated affect, CPM counts per minute, CI credible intervals.
SE in parenthesis.
The total effect of the mediation model was b = 0.32 (lower CI = 0.10, upper CI = 0.54) and the indirect effect was b = 0.04 (lower CI = -0.02, upper CI = 0.13). The indirect effect added only 13.51% (lower CI = -12.85%, upper CI = 39.87%) to the direct effect of bodily movement on creative performance (b = 0.27, lower CI = 0.06, upper CI = 0.49). This argues against a strong mediating effect of PAA on the relationship between bodily movement and creativity, since the CIs include a negative percentage. Due to the absence of a clear mediation, the pattern of findings indicates that activity and PAA are independently linked with creative performance (see Table 1). These results remained virtually unchanged when including age, gender, and BMI as covariates.
Additional analyses
Proportion of time spent with different types of bodily movement
In order to evaluate the association of physical activity with creative performance and PAA in more detail, exploratory correlation analyses were calculated with the proportion of time spent with bodily movements of four different intensity levels. As illustrated in Fig. 1, total creative performance showed significant positive associations with time spent in moderate (e.g., walking) and a negative association with movements of no to light intensity (e.g., sitting and lying). However, PAA was associated with time spent with very vigorous physical activity, but not with the proportion of time spent with sedentary behaviour or movement at low and moderate intensities. This pattern of findings indicate that the variance of CPM associated with PAA might be qualified by very vigorous activity.
Originality and fluency of creative performance
Since the measure of total creative performance has been criticized due to its high correlation with the fluency of ideas57–59, we also investigated, which aspects of creativity (originality, fluency) were linked with bodily movement. The regression analysis showed that both originality (sr = 0.23, p = 0.038) and fluency (sr = 0.25, p = 0.024) were positively and independently from each other associated with physical activity. The significant regression analysis indicated that the quantity and the quality of ideas together shared 9.6% of variance with physical activity (F(2,76) = 4.06, p = 0.021). A similar regression analysis investigating the association of fluency and originality with PAA slightly failed to show significance (F(2,76) = 3.00, p = 0.056). Only fluency (sr = 0.27, p = 0.018), but not originality (sr = 0.10, p = 0.354) was associated with the PAA. Originality (r = 0.52, p < 0.001) and fluency (r = 0.70, p < 0.001) were correlated with total creative performance.
Discussion
This study examined firstly, whether everyday bodily movement is associated with creativity and secondly, if this association is mediated by participants’ PAA. Importantly, this study replicated the reported positive relationship between bodily movement and creativity, which to date was predominantly found in laboratory studies21,25. We assessed bodily movement in everyday life by means of acceleration sensors. In contrast to former methods, the applied ecological assessment methodology objectively measures the level of spontaneous bodily movement in daily life—free from systematic errors of subjective self-reports38.
A more detailed examination of bodily movement (i.e., categorization into different intensity levels) indicated that not only time spent with activities of high intensity, like sport and bouts of exercise, go along with increased creative performance21,28,30, but also everyday physical activities of a moderate intensity level19. The negative relationship between the proportion of time spent with light intensities—including sedentary behavior—and creativity further underlines the a priori assumption that physical activity and bodily movement have beneficial effects on creative cognition19. This is also nicely in accordance with the anecdotal view that creative people use bodily movement to overcome mental blocks and lacks of inspiration20,32,60,61 (but see62).
Bodily movement was also linked to PAA40,41, which in turn was associated with creativity35,47,55. Importantly and critically, while all three variables of interest were interrelated, the Bayesian mediation analysis indicated that PAA did not explain the relationship between bodily movement and creativity. This finding corresponds to a former study. Steinberg et al.20 reported an impact of physical activity on creative performance and positive affect. However, they did not find evidence for a mediation. Importantly, together with Steinberg et al.20, the present finding suggests an additive effect of PAA and physical activity on creativity. This conclusion may also have important consequences: (1) Other psychological mechanisms have to be considered responsible for the observed relationship between physical activity and creativity. It might be hypothesized that the personality trait openness to experiences may serve as a potentially mediating variable, because it goes along with creativity and physical activity63,64. (2) To promote creativity, the induction of PAA plus the engagement in physical activity separately might be more fruitful in contrast to one strategy alone. This is in line with the observation that PAA was mainly associated with the fluency of ideas, but bodily movement seemed to be linked with the quantity and quality of ideas.
In accordance with this, Curnow and Turner39 reported an additional effect in a music (i.e., arousal) and exercise combined group compared to exercise and music alone groups in TTCT fluency, but they did not find differences in originality. However, this study investigated acute effects of exercise and emotional arousal on creativity, while the present study focused on the chronic aspect of this relationship, thus hampering a direct comparison of results. To overcome this limitation of solely focusing on chronic or acute mechanisms in future studies, experience-sampling methods (i.e., ecological momentary assessment of everyday life behavior), which enable the measurement of creativity, affective states, and bodily movement in the very same moment of time, should be applied44,65. This innovative approach may allow a more detailed insight into the mechanisms responsible for the relationship between physical activity and creativity. In conjointly using ecological momentary assessment and multi-level analyses, the chronic as well as acute effects of physical activity (and positive affect) on creative performance may be distinguished.
While in accordance with previous research higher PAA was associated with more bodily movement41,44–46 the more detailed analyses—using the proportion of time spent with bodily movement at different intensity levels—showed a divergent pattern of correlations. PAA was linked with very vigorous physical activity, but not with moderate and low intensities40,42. These additional analyses indicate one further explanation for the absence of a mediation effect. While a sedentary lifestyle seems to be associated with lower creativity scores, it shows no association with the affect. With other words: Because affect and creativity are associated with different intensity levels of everyday bodily movement, movement associated changes in affect might therefore be independent from movement associated changes in creativity. This conclusion is well in line with an additive effect of affect and physical activity on creativity. Despite the promises of ecological momentary assessment studies as outlined above, the present study was interested to assess the trait component of all variables and tested whether variance of the proposed mediator (i.e., PAA) overlaps with the shared variance of the predictor (i.e., bodily movement) and the criterion (i.e., creative performance).
This study is not without limitations. It is important to note that—due to the cross-sectional study design—the reported correlations are not causal in nature. Hence, it would also be possible, for instance, that creativity leads to more positive affect65 and higher levels of positive affect result in more everyday bodily movement44. Due to the moderate (and even low) reliability of the measures of bodily movement, PAA, and creativity, results should be treated with caution. However, physical activity was assessed on five different days including the weekend. Thus, a moderate internal consistency is well in line with the assumed variations of bodily movement throughout a week66. Similarly, total creative performance was assessed by two measures in two domains (i.e., figural and verbal), further decreasing internal consistency, which was in the reported range of various scoring methods in this field59. Although, the total creative performance score showed the expected association with bodily movements, it has also been criticized57–59,67, and a differentiation between quality (i.e., originality) and quantity (i.e., fluency) of ideas has been suggested. In accordance with the total creative performance score, originality and fluency were positively associated with bodily movement, however, only fluency was associated with PAA. This is in line with many laboratory studies investigating the association with physical activity by means of creativity measures combining fluency and originality of ideas19,28,30. Finally, the sample size of the present study might be too small to detect a small effect of PAA on the association between creativity and physical activity by means of a frequentist approach. However, the use of informative priors in a Bayesian mediation analysis increases the power of analyses and the precision of estimates56. Nevertheless, and not at least due to the high number of correlations in this study, a replication of the findings, also by means of a momentary ecological assessment approach is recommended before far-reaching conclusions are warrantable.
To sum up, the present findings are in accordance with the common notion that physical activity has many beneficial effects. Besides the well-known effects on physical health, a greater degree of everyday bodily movement is associated with higher levels of PAA (and subjective well-being40) and better creative cognition12,19. This study represents a first step to investigate this complex relationship in everyday life. The findings argue for an association between everyday bodily movement and creativity that is not merely attributed to the PAA. Further studies are certainly warranted to elucidate other pathways.
Methods
Participants
Seventy-nine participants (42 women) with an age range from 18 to 33 years (M = 22.95; SD = 3.34) took part in the study. Their Body Mass Index (BMI) varied between 17.29 kg/m2 and 39.18 kg/m2 (M = 22.81 kg/m2, SD = 3.43 kg/m2). All participants were university students. The highest degree of completed education ranged from job that requires training (1.3%) to academic degree (26.6%; grammar school, 56.9%; vocational school, 15.2%). Participants with chronic or acute musculoskeletal impairment were not included in the sample. The study was approved by the ethics committee of the University of Graz (approval number GZ. 39/21/63 ex 2015/16), methods were carried out in accordance with this, and written informed consent was obtained.
Creativity tasks
Verbal creativity
In the Alternate Uses (AU) task22 participants had to produce as many original and creative uses of conventional everyday objects (e.g., umbrella, car tire) as possible within 1 min each. In total participants worked on five items. This procedure resulted in 1,164 separable ideas in total. The originality of each idea was rated by three instructed and independent raters on a four-point rating scale ranging from 1 (not original at all) to 4 (highly original; cf. Consensual Assessment Technique68; see, e.g., Rominger et al.69). The inter-rater agreement was ICC (2, k) = 0.75. Participants’ verbal creative performance was indexed by the sum of all originality scores of the produced ideas (M = 44.36, SD = 12.05; similar procedure see20,26,70). Since studies reported a high correlation between this additive scoring procedure of creative performance and the fluency of ideas57,58,71, the quantity (i.e., fluency, mean number of answers per item constituted the individual fluency score, M = 4.91, SD = 1.27) and quality (i.e., originality, average of all originality scores of a participant; M = 1.82, SD = 0.26) of ideas were calculated for additional analyses.
Figural creativity
In the Picture Completion Task of the Torrance Test of Creative Thinking (TTCT26) participants have to extend abstract lines and figures in an original and creative way. Similar to the procedure of the AU task, the originality of each picture was evaluated by three independent raters on a four-point scale ranging from 1 to 4 (ICC (2, k) = 0.79)72. The raters were instructed to rate the originality of the idea only, and not the drawing skills or the title of the drawing. Participant’s figural creative performance score was calculated as the sum of all produced originality scores, that is a weighted sum (M = 18.03, SD = 3.79; similar procedure see 20,26,70). Fluency was determined as the number of completed pictures within the given time-limit of 10 min (734 pictures in total; M = 9.29, SD = 1.21). Originality was indicated as the average of all originality scores of a participant (M = 1.95, SD = 0.35).
Total creative performance
In order to indicate participant’s total creative performance, the z-transformed verbal and figural creative performance scores were aggregated (α = 0.45). For additional analyses the fluency and originality scores were calculated by the sum of the z-transformed scores. Cronbach’s alpha for fluency was α = 0.63 and α = 0.59 for originality.
Objective measurement of everyday bodily movement
Bodily movement was recorded by means of tri-axial acceleration sensors (ActiGraph®, Model GT3X + , weight of 19 g). All participants carried the sensor on the right side of their hip, attached with an elastic belt66,73,74. This approach provides a measure of metabolically relevant whole-body movements and their intensity levels66. The acceleration data were sampled with 30 Hz and activity counts were analyzed using 1 min epochs (i.e., counts/min, CPM; see Schwerdtfeger et al.44 for a similar approach). The activity counts were the composite vector magnitude of the three axes75,76. The CPM as the main output of the ActiGraph77 was validated in a high number of studies (e.g.,78; for details how ActiGraph counts are generated, see Brønd, Andersen, & Arvidsson79). All data were analyzed with the software ActiLife (Version 6.13.2).
The acceleration data were recorded on five consecutive days during waking hours, in order to reliably and representatively measure the degree of participants’ everyday bodily movement66,80. The recordings started on Wednesday 00:00 and ended on Sunday at 23:59. Since all participants were instructed to note the sensors’ non-wearing times in a calendar template (e.g., during bathing, or sleeping), we were able to exclude invalid periods of sensor data (i.e., counts of zero) from CPM estimation. The minimum of wearing time per day was set to 600 min (M = 839.89 min, SD = 79.69 min)38. Non-activities longer than 90 min during the self-rated wearing time were treated as invalid non-activities and all counts of zero during this time were excluded from the CPM estimation81. The resulting data were finally quality checked for spurious counts (i.e., CPMs above 20,000 were inspected in more detail)80,82. This procedure resulted in M = 4.99 (SD = 0.01) valid days per participant (max = 5, min = 4) showing a mean CPM of 601.38 (SD = 168.54). There was no significant difference of CPM between the five days of measurement (F(4,74) = 1.367; p = 0.245). The reliability and the validity of the applied method are acceptable66,83 with a Cronbach’s α of 0.61 in the present study (for all five days of recording).
In addition to CPM as an index of general everyday bodily movement, the proportion of minutes (relative to the total wearing time) spent in no to light (< 2,690 CPMs; M = 93.60%, SD = 2.84%), moderate (2,690–6,166 CPMs; M = 5.40%, SD = 2.43%), vigorous (6,167–9,642 CPMs; M = 0.76%, SD = 0.77%), and very vigorous activity (> 9,642 CPMs; M = 0.26%, SD = 0.50%) was analyzed. These measures are estimates of the probability with which participants show movement in a specified intensity level. The classification cut-points were in accordance with former studies76.
Positive-activated affect (PAA)
Positive-activated affect as a trait was assessed by four items of the German version of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS)84,85. Participants were requested to indicate how they feel in general on all 20 adjectives of the PANAS on a five-point Likert scale (from 1 = not at all to 5 = very much so). Similar to Schwerdtfeger and Gerteis86, PAA was indexed by the mean of the four items87 (“active”, “alert”, “attentive”, “inspired”; M = 3.47, SD = 0.55, Cronbach’s α = 0.61).
Procedure
All participants were tested separately. Firstly, they signed the informed consent, followed by the PANAS and the divergent thinking tests (AUT, TTCT). Participants were familiarized with the study protocol and the technical equipment (acceleration sensor, belt). Following the instruction, the accelerometer was initialized and attached. Finally, participants were instructed to report the sensor wearing times in a template. After the objective measurement of habitual bodily movement, participants returned the sensors and finally rated the typicality of the recording days on a four-point rating scale (1 = absolutely typical, 2 = rather typical, 3 = rather untypical, 4 = totally untypical). No participant rated these days as totally untypical (17.72% absolutely typical, 69.62% rather typical, 12.66% rather untypical).
Statistical analysis
In order to answer the first study question, Pearson correlation was used to test the relationship between everyday bodily movement and total creative performance. Furthermore, as an important prerequisite for a mediation, the inter-correlations of all three variables of interest (i.e., CPM, PAA, and total creative performance) were calculated88.
Bayesian mediation analysis was applied to evaluate if the association between everyday bodily movement and creativity was due to the influence of PAA. The statistical program R (version 3.4.3)89 with the package brms (version 2.7.0)90 was used. A Bayesian approach instead of a common frequentist approach was applied, because of (1) the moderate sample size of 79 participants56,91 and (2) the a priori knowledge about the intercorrelations. The use of prior information in Bayesian statistics increases the power of the applied mediation analysis56. Firstly, physical activity is associated with increased creativity19,21. Secondly, physical activity is positively associated with PAA44, and, thirdly, PAA is linked with creativity47,55. Based on relevant literature, normally distributed informative priors for the regression coefficients of bodily movement and creativity (µ = 0.12, σ2 = 0.10; Etnier et al.12 reporting a d of 0.25), bodily movement and PAA (µ = 0.27, σ2 = 0.10; Reed & Buck40 reporting a d of 0.57), and creativity and PAA (µ = 0.08, σ2 = 0.10)55 were used56.
Samples were derived by Markov Chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) algorithm, 4 chains with 5,000 iterations (1,000 warm-up samples for each chain) and 16,000 post-warmup samples were used. Unstandardized effect estimates (b), 95% credible intervals (CI), and the potential scale reduction factor on split chains (Rhat) are reported. For Rhat, values close to 1 indicate that the chains converged (i.e., the number of iterations was sufficient). To evaluate the mediating role of PAA on the association between bodily movement and creativity the direct, indirect, and total effect, and the effect proportion of the mediation are reported. This was done with the R package mediation (version 4.4.6)92. The mediating effect of PAA is considered as present, when the credible intervals (CIs) of the indirect effect are positive and do not include zero.
In additional analyses the association between the intensity levels of physical activity and total creative performance with the proportion of time spent with physical activities in different intensity levels were calculated. Equivalent correlations were calculated for PAA. Since the total creative performance score has been criticized to be mainly driven by the fluency of ideas58,93, additional analyses were calculated to investigate the association of originality and fluency with bodily movements, by means of a regression analysis. This approach allows to investigate the unique variance quality and quantity of ideas share with physical activity. A similar regression analysis was calculated for PAA.
Acknowledgements
The authors thank Martina Schein, Martina Kern, and Sonja Knieling for their valuable help in data collection and preparation. The authors acknowledge the financial support by the University of Graz.
Author contributions
C.R., A.S., B.W., and A.F. developed the study concept and the study design. Data collection was performed by B.W. All authors contributed to the data analysis and interpretation. C.R. and A.S. drafted the manuscript, to which B.W., I.P., and A.F. provided critical revisions.
Data availability
The data will be available from the first author.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Miles L. Physical activity and health. Nutr. Bull. 2007;32:314–363. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-3010.2007.00668.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Benelam B. Physical activity in pregnancy. Nutr. Bull. 2011;36:370–372. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-3010.2011.01915.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 3.WHO . Global Recommendations on Physical Activity for Health. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2010. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Lee I-M, et al. Effect of physical inactivity on major non-communicable diseases worldwide: an analysis of burden of disease and life expectancy. The Lancet. 2012;380:219–229. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)61031-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Lemez S, Baker J. Do elite athletes live longer? A systematic review of mortality and longevity in elite athletes. Sports Med. Open. 2015;1(16):2015. doi: 10.1186/s40798-015-0024-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Pinto Pereira SM, Geoffroy M-C, Power C. Depressive symptoms and physical activity during 3 decades in adult life: bidirectional associations in a prospective cohort study. JAMA Psychiatry. 2014;71:1373–1380. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.1240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Peluso MA, Andrade LHDGD. Physical activity and mental health: the association between exercise and mood. Clinics. 2005;60:61–70. doi: 10.1590/S1807-59322005000100012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Lubans D, et al. Physical activity for cognitive and mental health in youth: a systematic review of mechanisms. Pediatrics. 2016 doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-1642. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Marker AM, Steele RG, Noser AE. Physical activity and health-related quality of life in children and adolescents: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Health Psychol. 2018;37:893–903. doi: 10.1037/hea0000653. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Forcier K, et al. Links between physical fitness and cardiovascular reactivity and recovery to psychological stressors. A meta-analysis. Health Psychol. 2006;25:723–739. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.25.6.723. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Folkins CH, Sime WE. Physical fitness training and mental health. Am. Psychol. 1981;36:373–389. doi: 10.1037//0003-066X.36.4.373. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Etnier JL, et al. The influence of physical fitness and exercise upon cognitive functioning. A meta-analysis. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 1997;19(249–277):1997. doi: 10.1123/jsep.19.3.249. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Etnier JL, Chang Y-K. The effect of physical activity on executive function. A brief commentary on definitions, measurement issues, and the current state of the literature. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2009;31:469–483. doi: 10.1123/jsep.31.4.469. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Etnier JL, Nowell PM, Landers DM, Sibley BA. A meta-regression to examine the relationship between aerobic fitness and cognitive performance. Brain Res. Rev. 2006;52:119–130. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2006.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Colcombe S, Kramer AF. Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults A meta-analytic study. Psychol. Sci. 2003;14(125–130):2003. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.t01-1-01430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Sibley BA, Etnier JL. The relationship between physical activity and cognition in children. A meta-analysis. Pediatric Exerc. Sci. 2003;15:243–256. doi: 10.1123/pes.15.3.243. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Lambourne K, Tomporowski P. The effect of exercise-induced arousal on cognitive task performance. A meta-regression analysis. Brain Res. 2010;1341:12–24. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.03.091. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Young J, Angevaren M, Rusted J, Tabet N. Aerobic exercise to improve cognitive function in older people without known cognitive impairment. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2015 doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD005381.pub4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Oppezzo M, Schwartz DL. Give your ideas some legs: the positive effect of walking on creative thinking. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cognit. 2014;40:1142–1152. doi: 10.1037/a0036577. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Steinberg H, et al. Exercise enhances creativity independently of mood. Br. J. Sports Med. 1997;31:240–245. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.31.3.240. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Gondola JC, Tuckman BW. Effects of a systematic program of exercise on selected measures of creativity. Percept. Mot. Skills. 1985;60:53–54. doi: 10.2466/pms.1985.60.1.53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Guilford JP. The Nature of Human Intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1967. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Tuckman BW, Hinkle JS. An experimental study of the physical and psychological effects of aerobic exercise on schoolchildren. Health Psychol. 1986;5:197–207. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.5.3.197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Gondola JC. The enhancement of creativity through long and short term exercise programs. J. Soc. Behav. Personal. 1986;1:77–82. [Google Scholar]
- 25.Cavallera GM, Boari G, Labbrozzi D, Bello ED. Morningness-eveningness personality and creative thinking among young people who play recreational sport. Soc. Behav. Personal. 2011;39:503–518. doi: 10.2224/sbp.2011.39.4.503. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Torrance PE. Torrance Test of Creative Thinking. Norms-Technical Manual Research Edition-Verbal Tests, Forms A and B-Figural tests, Forms A and B. Princeton, New Jersey: Personnel Press; 1966. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Ramocki SP. Creativity interacts with fitness and exercise. Phys. Educ. 2002;59:8. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Latorre Román PÁ, Pinillos FG, Pantoja Vallejo A, Berrios Aguayo B. Creativity and physical fitness in primary school-aged children. Pediatrics Int. 2017;59:1194–1199. doi: 10.1111/ped.13391. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Caspersen CJ, Powell KE, Christenson GM. Physical activity, exercise, and physical fitness: definitions and distinctions for health-related research. Public Health Rep. 1985;100:126–131. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Blanchette DM, Ramocki SP, O’del JN, Casey MS. Aerobic exercise and creative potential. Immediate and residual effects. Creat. Res. J. 2005;17:257–264. doi: 10.1080/10400419.2005.9651483. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Gondola JC. The effects of a single bout of aerobic dancing on selected tests of creativity. J. Soc. Behav. Personal. 1987;2:275–278. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Colzato LS, Szapora A, Pannekoek JN, Hommel B. The impact of physical exercise on convergent and divergent thinking. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013;7:824. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00824. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Donnegan KF, Setti A, Allen AP. Exercise and creativity: can one bout of yoga improve convergent and divergent Thinking? J. Cognit. Enhanc. 2018;2:193–199. doi: 10.1007/s41465-018-0082-3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Patterson R, Frith E, Loprinzi P. The experimental effects of acute walking on cognitive creativity performance. J. Behav. Health. 2018 doi: 10.5455/jbh.20180415053930. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Isen AM, Daubman KA, Nowicki GP. Positive affect facilitates creative problem solving. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1987;52:1122–1131. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.52.6.1122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Frith E, Loprinzi PD. Experimental effects of acute exercise and music listening on cognitive creativity. Physiol. Behav. 2018;191:21–28. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.03.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Fahrenberg J, Myrtek M, Pawlik K, Perrez M. Ambulatory assessment—monitoring behavior in daily life settings. A behavioral-scientific challenge for psychology. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 2007;23:206–213. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759.23.4.206. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Matthews CE, Hagströmer M, Pober DM, Bowles HR. Best practices for using physical activity monitors in population-based research. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2012;44:S68–S76. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0b013e3182399e5b. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Curnow KE, Turner ET. The effect of exercise and music on the creativity of college students. J. Creative Behav. 1992;26:50–52. doi: 10.1002/j.2162-6057.1992.tb01156.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Reed J, Buck S. The effect of regular aerobic exercise on positive-activated affect: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 2009;10:581–594. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2009.05.009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Reed J, Ones DS. The effect of acute aerobic exercise on positive activated affect: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 2006;7:477–514. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2005.11.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Pasco JA, et al. Don’t worry, be active: positive affect and habitual physical activity. Aust. N.Z. J. Psychiatry. 2011;45:1047–1052. doi: 10.3109/00048674.2011.621063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Schwerdtfeger AR, Eberhardt R, Chmitorz A. Gibt es einen Zusammenhang zwischen Bewegungsaktivität und psychischem Befinden im Alltag? Z. Gesundheitspsychol. 2008;16:2–11. doi: 10.5282/ubm/epub.8679. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Schwerdtfeger AR, Eberhardt R, Chmitorz A, Schaller E. Momentary affect predicts bodily movement in daily life: an ambulatory monitoring study. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2010;32:674–693. doi: 10.1123/jsep.32.5.674. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Ekkekakis P, Hall EE, VanLanduyt LM, Petruzzello SJ. Walking in (affective) circles: can short walks enhance affect? J. Behav. Med. 2000;23:245–275. doi: 10.1023/A:1005558025163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Thayer RE, Peters DP, Takahashi PJ, Birkhead-Flight AM. Mood and behavior (smoking and sugar snacking) following moderate exercise: a partial test of self-regulation theory. Personal. Individ. Differ. 1993;14:97–104. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(93)90178-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 47.de Dreu CKW, Baas M, Nijstad BA. Hedonic tone and activation level in the mood-creativity link: toward a dual pathway to creativity model. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2008;94:739–756. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.94.5.739. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Fink A, Schwab D, Papousek I. Sensitivity of EEG upper alpha activity to cognitive and affective creativity interventions. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2011;82:233–239. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Ashby FG, Isen AM, Turken AU. A neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its influence on cognition. Psychol. Rev. 1999;106:529–550. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.106.3.529. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Conner TS, Silvia PJ. Creative days. A daily diary study of emotion, personality, and everyday creativity. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts. 2015;9:463–470. doi: 10.1037/aca0000022. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Silvia PJ, et al. Everyday creativity in daily life. An experience-sampling study of “little c” creativity. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts. 2014;8:183–188. doi: 10.1037/a0035722. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Karwowski M, Lebuda I, Szumski G, Firkowska-Mankiewicz A. From moment-to-moment to day-to-day. Experience sampling and diary investigations in adults’ everyday creativity. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts. 2017;11:309–324. doi: 10.1037/aca0000127. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Benedek M, Jauk E, Kerschenbauer K, Anderwald R, Grond L. Creating art. An experience sampling study in the domain of moving image art. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts. 2017;10:15–20. doi: 10.1037/aca0000102. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 54.To ML, Fisher CD, Ashkanasy NM, Rowe PA. Within-person relationships between mood and creativity. J. Appl. Psychol. 2012;97:599–612. doi: 10.1037/a0026097. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Baas M, De Dreu CKW, Nijstad BA. A meta-analysis of 25 years of mood-creativity research: hedonic tone, activation, or regulatory focus? Psychol. Bull. 2008;134:779–806. doi: 10.1037/a0012815. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Yuan Y, MacKinnon DP. Bayesian mediation analysis. Psychol. Methods. 2009;14:301–322. doi: 10.1037/a0016972. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Forthmann B, Szardenings C, Holling H. Understanding the confounding effect of fluency in divergent thinking scores: revisiting average scores to quantify artifactual correlation. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts. 2020;14:94–112. doi: 10.1037/aca0000196. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Benedek M, Mühlmann C, Jauk E, Neubauer AC. Assessment of divergent thinking by means of the subjective top-scoring method: effects of the number of top-ideas and time-on-task on reliability and validity. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts. 2013;7:341–349. doi: 10.1037/a0033644. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Plucker JA, Qian M, Wang S. Is originality in the eye of the beholder? Comparison of scoring techniques in the assessment of divergent thinking. J. Creat. Behav. 2011;45:1–22. doi: 10.1002/j.2162-6057.2011.tb01081.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Rominger C, Papousek I, Fink A, Weiss EM. Enhancement of figural creativity by motor activation: effects of unilateral hand contractions on creativity are moderated by positive schizotypy. Laterality. 2014;19:424–438. doi: 10.1080/1357650X.2013.858725. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Goldstein A, Revivo K, Kreitler M, Metuki N. Unilateral muscle contractions enhance creative thinking. Psychon. Bull. & Rev. 2010;17:895–899. doi: 10.3758/PBR.17.6.895. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Turner CE, Hahn ME, Kellogg RT. Semantic processing in the left versus right cerebral hemispheres following unilateral hand contractions. Laterality. 2017;22:219–232. doi: 10.1080/1357650X.2016.1154861. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Jauk EV, Benedek M, Neubauer AC. The road to creative achievement: a latent variable model of ability and personality predictors. Eur. J. Personal. 2014;28:95–105. doi: 10.1002/per.1941. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Stephan Y, Sutin AR, Luchetti M, Bosselut G, Terracciano A. Physical activity and personality development over twenty years: evidence from three longitudinal samples. J. Res. Personal. 2018;73:173–179. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2018.02.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Conner TS, DeYoung CG, Silvia PJ. Everyday creative activity as a path to flourishing. J. Posit. Psychol. 2018;13:181–189. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2016.1257049. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Trost SG, Mciver KL, Pate RR. Conducting accelerometer-based activity assessments in field-based research. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2005;37:S531–S543. doi: 10.1249/01.mss.0000185657.86065.98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Forthmann B, Szardenings C, Dumas D. On the conceptual overlap between the fluency contamination effect in divergent thinking scores and the chance view on scientific creativity. J. Creat. Behav. 2020 doi: 10.1002/jocb.445. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Amabile TM. Social psychology of creativity: a consensual assessment technique. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1982;43:997–1013. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.43.5.997. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Rominger C, Fink A, Weiss EM, Bosch J, Papousek I. Allusive thinking (remote associations) and auditory top-down inhibition skills differentially predict creativity and positive schizotypy. Cognit. Neuropsychiatry. 2017;22:108–121. doi: 10.1080/13546805.2016.1278361. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Palmiero M, Nori R, Piccardi L. Verbal and visual divergent thinking in aging. Exp. Brain Res. 2017;235:1021–1029. doi: 10.1007/s00221-016-4857-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Mouchiroud C, Lubart T. Children’s original thinking: an empirical examination of alternative measures derived from divergent thinking tasks. J. Genet. Psychol. 2001;162:382–401. doi: 10.1080/00221320109597491. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Rominger C, et al. The creative brain in the figural domain. Distinct patterns of EEG alpha power during idea generation and idea elaboration. Neuropsychologia. 2018;118:13–19. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.02.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Troiano RP, et al. Physical activity in the United States measured by accelerometer. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2008;40:181–188. doi: 10.1249/mss.0b013e31815a51b3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Fleig L, et al. Environmental and psychosocial correlates of objectively measured physical activity among older adults. Health Psychol. 2016;35:1364–1372. doi: 10.1037/hea0000403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Peeters G, van Gellecum Y, Ryde G, Farías NA, Brown WJ. Is the pain of activity log-books worth the gain in precision when distinguishing wear and non-wear time for tri-axial accelerometers? J. Sci. Med. Sport. 2013;16:515–519. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2012.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Sasaki JE, John D, Freedson PS. Validation and comparison of ActiGraph activity monitors. J. Sci. Med. Sport. 2011;14:411–416. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2011.04.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Tryon WW, Williams R. Fully proportional actigraphy: a new instrument. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 1996;28:392–403. doi: 10.3758/BF03200519. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Aadland E, Ylvisåker E. Reliability of the actigraph GT3X+ accelerometer in adults under free-living conditions. PLoS ONE. 2015;10:e0134606. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0134606. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Brønd JC, Andersen LB, Arvidsson D. Generating ActiGraph counts from raw acceleration recorded by an alternative monitor. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2017;49:2351–2360. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000001344. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Mâsse LC, et al. Accelerometer data reduction: a comparison of four reduction algorithms on select outcome variables. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2005;37:S544–S554. doi: 10.1249/01.mss.0000185674.09066.8a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Choi L, Liu Z, Matthews CE, Buchowski MS. Validation of accelerometer wear and nonwear time classification algorithm. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2011;43:357–364. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0b013e3181ed61a3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Colley R, Connor Gorber S, Tremblay MS. Quality control and data reduction procedures for accelerometry-derived measures of physical activity. Health Rep. 2010;21:63. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Matthews CE, Ainsworth BE, Thompson RW, Bassett DR. Sources of variance in daily physical activity levels as measured by an accelerometer. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2002;34:1376–1381. doi: 10.1097/00005768-200208000-00021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Krohne HW, Egloff B, Kohlmann CW, Tausch A. Untersuchungen mit einer deutschen Version der , Positive and Negative Affect Schedule‘ (PANAS) Diagnostica. 1996;42:139–156. [Google Scholar]
- 85.Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1988;54:1063–1070. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Schwerdtfeger AR, Gerteis AKS. The manifold effects of positive affect on heart rate variability in everyday life. Distinguishing within-person and between-person associations. Health Psychol. 2014;33:1065–1073. doi: 10.1037/hea0000079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Egloff B, Schmukle SC, Burns LR, Kohlmann C-W, Hock M. Facets of dynamic positive affect: differentiating joy, interest, and activation in the positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS) J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2003;85:528–540. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.3.528. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS. Using Multivariate Statistics. 6. Harlow, Essex: Pearson Education; 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 89.R Development Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation; 2017. [Google Scholar]
- 90.Bürkner P-C. brm: an R package for Bayesian multilevel models using Stan. J. Stat. Softw. 2017 doi: 10.18637/jss.v080.i01. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 91.van de Schoot R, Broere JJ, Perryck KH, Zondervan-Zwijnenburg M, van Loey NE. Analyzing small data sets using Bayesian estimation: The case of posttraumatic stress symptoms following mechanical ventilation in burn survivors. Eur. J. Psychotraumatol. 2015;6:25216. doi: 10.3402/ejpt.v6.25216. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Tingley, D., Yamamoto, T., Hirose, K., Keele, L. & Imai, K. Mediation: Causal Mediation Analysis (2017).
- 93.Silvia PJ, et al. Assessing creativity with divergent thinking tasks. Exploring the reliability and validity of new subjective scoring methods. Psych. Aesthet. Creat. Arts. 2008;2:68–85. doi: 10.1037/1931-3896.2.2.68. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The data will be available from the first author.