Abstract
Researching the relationship between physical activity and academic performance is becoming an important research topic due to increasing evidence about the positive effect of physical activity on cognitive functioning. The present systematic review and meta-analysis (PROSPERO registration number: CDR132118) is a unique contribution to the recently published reviews since it only includes interventions longer than 6 weeks and acknowledges the influence of the qualifications of practitioners who deliver interventions. After identifying 14,245 records in five databases and selecting 247 full-text articles assessed for eligibility, 44 interventions passed all eligibility criteria. This meta-analysis uses validity generalization in a random effects model, which shows that academic performance itself is not solely caused by increased physical activity. The weighted mean population effect of all included interventions was rw = 0.181. Most of the studies had serious limitations since they did not report physical activity intensity, which is an essential component to achieving positive exercise effects on cognition. In addition, the qualifications of the staff who administer the interventions were largely ignored in existing literature. It was found that 13 out of 20 physical activity interventions with significant positive effects on academic performance were performed by practitioners who held higher qualifications in the field of physical education and exercise science, who could mediate higher physical activity intensities of the given interventions. The population effect in studies where interventions were administered by practitioners with lower qualifications in the field (rw = 0.14) was lower compared to interventions performed by staff with higher qualifications (rw = 0.22). There was also a significant difference in academic performance with regard to staff qualification level (χ = 4.464; p = 0.035). In addition to activity duration, future physical activity intervention studies including those investigating academic performance should focus on the importance of physical activity intensity and include measures of physical fitness as objective indicators to enable more reliable analyses to establish physical activity influence on academic performance.
Keywords: physical activity, academic performance, teaching qualifications, children, adolescents, intervention
Introduction
Regular physical activity (PA) at an adequate intensity and duration is indispensable for maintaining a healthy lifestyle due to its continued positive impact on skeletal (1), metabolic (2), cardiovascular (3) and psychosocial functioning of the human body (4). Low levels of PA, on the other hand, lead to low cardiorespiratory fitness and are associated with a decline in academic performance (AP) (5), possibly due to the deterioration of brain structure, and thus, cognitive abilities and brain function (6–8). PA increases oxygen saturation (9) and angiogenesis (10) in brain areas responsible for task performance. The positive effects of PA on the prefrontal cortex and the hippocampus have been emphasized in many studies (11–13). Furthermore, the molecular architecture and behavior of the basal ganglia may also be directly influenced by PA (8).
Some studies have found positive results between PA and academic performance (AP) (14–17), whereas other studies have found no difference, or even negative correlations between the two factors (18, 19). Review articles using effect size (ES) suggest that PA itself has positive effects on AP (6). For example, one of the earliest meta-analyses in which 1,260 ES were calculated, reported an overall ES of 0.25 and suggested that PA has a small, yet positive effect on cognition (20). ES has been shown to be the largest in studies investigating cognition as a function of fitness level (ES = 0.53). For example, Sibley and Etnier (6) calculated ES for each study that met their study eligibility criteria. They concluded that there is a statistically-significant positive effect of PA on cognition in children, reporting an overall ES of 0.32.
Contemporary children often experience a lack of physical exercise and an abundance of sedentariness in their school and domestic environments, which inevitably leads to deterioration of PA. School-based PA as a part of the normal physical education classes (PE) and extracurricular activities are often the only stable access to PA for many children, and have shown to provide a significant impact on classroom behavior (21), self-esteem (4), self-image (22), and cognitive function (5–7). This is why the competencies of PE teachers and other specialists who deliver school-based PA programmes are so critical, and can affect student outcomes considerably. Formally gained knowledge and professional experiences that assure the professional competencies of the practitioners are key elements for the safe and effective implementation of any PA-enhancing intervention (23, 24). However, the professional competencies of the practitioners who deliver such interventions (and really, any school-based PA programme), are often ignored or not represented in the literature. This meta-analysis does differentiates professional qualifications of staff who perform interventions and measurements in the physical activity classes.
In this regard, our meta-analysis provides a unique contribution to the recently published reviews (25–29), since it exclusively includes interventions longer than 6 weeks (30) and also considers the influence of the qualifications of the PE teachers and practitioners administering the interventions.
Thus, the main purpose of this review is to examine whether primary and secondary school children who were involved in PA-enhancing school-based interventions demonstrated higher AP than their peers who were not involved in regular PA, and whether the improvements observed in AP were influenced by practitioners with higher professional competencies compared to less qualified practitioners. This review examines only those interventions completed in studies with an experimental design, and which reportclear and reliable measures and indicators of both PA and AP. The studies also had to be implemented in school settings.
Methods
The meta-analysis was performed and reported in accordance with the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA) guidelines (31, 32). The present work was registered at the International Prospective Register for Systematic Reviews, identification code CDR132118.
Literature Search
Two review authors (VS and GS) independently searched literature from databases PubMed, Scopus and ScienceDirect, which were accessed between January 2017 and July 2017 and searches were re-run from February 2019 and June 2019. Gray literature of published interventions and systematic reviews was searched through Google Scholar and Dart electronic databases from February 2019 to June 2019. Unpublished studies won't be sought. The international prospective register of systematic reviews (PROSPERO database) was also searched to identify any unpublished and/or important ongoing meta-analyses. Titles and abstracts of studies retrieved using search string and those from additional sources were screened independently by two review authors to identify potential studies whereas the third review author (GJ) mediated any disagreement until a consensus was reached. Abstracts were screened using following search string: children AND intervention AND school AND (physical activity OR physical education OR extracurricular activity) AND (cognition OR academic performance OR academic achievement). All articles generated from the initial search were stored on Mendeley reference management software & researcher network (Elsevier, Amsterdam, Netherlands) which removed duplicate references.
Inclusion Criteria
Since the primary objective of the current study was to determine any impact and/or consequences of increased PA on children's behavior and AP, the reviewed interventions had to be published as articles in peer-reviewed scientific journals, as proceedings, or as doctoral dissertations with a focus on healthy school-aged children between 5 and 16 years of age, without disadvantages regarding socio economic standard and with evenly distributed sample in both genders.
The articles also needed to report an obvious measure of increased PA (expressed in minutes per week), including one or more of extracurricular or morning PA, PE, school sports, excluding professional sport activity, including standardized measures of AP (e.g., grade point average, standardized national tests, cognition and intelligence tests, validated tests from algebra, reading and writing).
Only interventions longer than 6 weeks, with control and experimental groups, and with more than 25 participants were included in this review. Results with p < 0.05 were considered statistically significant. All AP outcomes have been recalculated according to the same scale—ES, considering one of the two criteria: (1) ES = 0, no effect; or (2) ES > 0.01, intervention group academically performing better than the control group. Interventions meeting all eligibility criteria are presented in Table 1.
Table 1.
Author (year) | N | Age of children (years or school grade) | Intervention duration | Experimental design | Type of activity and AP assessment | Staff implementing intervention and measurements (HQ or LQ) | Intervention min/week | Effects on AP (ES) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Ahamed et al. (18) | 288 | 9–11 years old** (4th and 5th grade) | 16 months, 5 days/week; 15 min/day | CG and IG | Increased PA time; Canadian achievement test | Classroom teachers (LQ) | 47 | No effect (0.00) |
Alesi et al. (33) | 44 | 8–10 years old | 6 months; 2 days/week; 75 min/day | CG and IG | Increased PA time; Corsi block test, Forward digit span test, The backward digit span test | PE teacher (HQ) | 150 | Significantly better executive functions in IG (1.62) |
Ardoy et al. (34) | 67 | 12–14 years old (II–V Tanner Grade) | 17 weeks, 2 days/week; 55 min/day | CG and 2 IG. (1st IG had only increased PE time, 2nd IG had increased PE time, and intensity of PE lessons) | Enhanced PE and increased PA time; IGF-M Intelligence Test, and school grades (mathematics, language, natural sciences, English) | PE teachers (HQ) | 110 | Significantly better results in mathematics (0.47) and in GPA (2.60) |
Beck et al. (35) | 165 | 7.5 (±0.02) years old | 6 weeks; 3 days/week; 60 min/day | CG and 2 IG (CG: non-motor enriched mathematical teaching; IG1: fine motor math group; IG2: gross motor math group) | Increased PA time (during academic narrated lessons); standardized mathematical test, modified Eriksen Flanker test, CANTAB | Classroom teachers (LQ) | 180 | No effect (0) after the last testing. Right after the intervention only normal math subgroup in gross motor math IG benefited compared to CG and fine motor IG |
Bunketorp Käll et al. (36) | 545 | 12 years old (5th grade) | 3 years; 2 days/week; 30–45 min/day | CG and IG | Increased PA time; Academic performance grades of mathematics, English, and Swedish language | Not reported (LQ) | 75*** | No effect (0.00) |
Chaddock-Heyman et al. (37) | 32 | 8–9 years old | 9 months; 5 days/week; 2 school periods/day | CG and IG | Increased PA time; fMRI | At university by research staff (HQ) | 425*** | No effect (0.00) |
Coe et al. (38) | 214 | 10–11 years old** (6th grade) | 1 semester, 5 days/week, On average 19 min of MVPA/day | CG and IG. 1st group was assigned in PE in 1st semester; 2nd group was assigned to PE during 2nd semester | One semester without PE, other semester increased PE time; Terra Nova Test and individual grades | Not reported (LQ) | 95 | No effect (0.00) |
Costigan et al. (39) | 65 | 15.8 (±0.6) years old | 8 weeks; 1(3)****days/week; 30(120)****min/day | CG and 2 IG (CG had normal PE-2 school hours/week; IG had high-interval PE) | Increased PA and enhanced PE; The trail making test (TMT) | PE teachers (HQ) | 30 | No effect (0.00) |
Davis et al. (40) | 94 | 7–11 (M = 9.2) years old | 15 weeks, 5 days/week, 40 min/day | CG (no-exercise) and 2 IG (low-dose exercise group, high-dose exercise group) | Aerobic PA; average heart rate > 150 bpm; CAS | PE teachers, researchers (HQ) | 200 | Significantly better AP in high-dose exercise group (2.24) |
Greeff et al. (41) | 499 | 8.1 years old (2nd and 3rd grade) | 2 years; 22 weeks/year; 3 days/week; 20–30 min/day | CG and IG | Increased PA time (during academic narrated lessons); Golden stroop test, Digit Span backward, and Visual span backward test, M-WCST | Primary and classroom teachers (LQ) | 75*** | No effect (0.00) |
Dwyer et al. (14) | ~500 in 1st phase (1978); 216 in 2nd phase (1980) | 10 years old | 14 weeks, 5 days/week, 60 min/day | CG and 2 IG (fitness group and skill programme group) | Enhanced PE time and increased intensity of PA in skill programme group; arithmetic and reading test | Researchers (HQ) | 225 | No effect (0.00) |
Ericsson (42) | 251 | 6–9 years old; (1st, 2nd, and 3rd grades) | 3 years, 3 days/week; 45 min/day | CG and 2 IG | Increased PE time (in CG normal curriculum-−2 h per week, in IG 5 h per week); LUS, national tests in Swedish and mathematics, word, and reading test | PE teachers (HQ) | 135 | Significantly better AP in both intervention groups (in national test in mathematics) (0.21) |
Ericsson and Karlsson (43) | 220 | 6–9 years old; (1st, 2nd, and 3rd grades) at baseline—follow-up till the 16 years of age | 7–9 years, 3 days/week; 45 min/day | CG and IG | Increased PE time (in CG normal curriculum-−2 h per week, in IG 5 h per week); LUS, national tests in Swedish and mathematics, word, and reading test | PE teachers (HQ) | 135 | Significantly better AP in boys IG (1.5) and no effect in IG in girls (0.0) |
Erwin et al. (44) | 29 | 8–9 years old (M = 8.87) (3rd grade) | 20 weeks; 5 days/week; 20 min/day | CG and IG | Increased PA time; reading and mathematics fluency, school grades, standardized test scores | Classroom teachers (LQ) | 100 | Significantly better AP on CBM scores (1, 24) and no effect on standardized tests (0.00) and teachers' grades (0.00) |
Fedewa and Davis (45) | 460 | 8–11 years old** (3rd-−5th grade) | 8 months; 5 days/week; 20 min/day | CG and IG | Increased PA time (breaks); Fluid intelligence (SPM), academic performance grades (mathematics and reading) | Classroom teachers (LQ) | 100 | No effect (0.00) |
Fisher et al. (46) | 64 | 5–7 years old | 10 weeks, 120 min/week | CG and IG | Aerobic PA; CAS, CANTAB, ANT, Conner's Behavior Rating Scale | Researchers, PE teachers and classroom teachers (HQ) | 90 | Significantly better AP in intervention group (in ANT and CANTAB and Conner's Behavioral Rating Scale) (0.14) |
Gao et al. (47) | 208 | 10–12 years old (M = 10, 3 years) (3rd−6th grade) | 2 years; 3 days/week; 30 min/day | CG and IG | Increased PA time; reading and math scores for Utah Criterion Referenced Test | Classroom teachers (LQ) | 90 | No effect (0.00); nevertheless greater improvement on math scores of intervention children in Year 1 and 2, the difference was not statistically significant (0.00) |
Hedges and Hardin (48) | 152 | 6–7 years old (1st grade) | 5 months, 5 days/week, 20 min/week | CG and IG | Increased PA time; S.A.A.T | Classroom teachers (LQ) | 100 | No effect (0.00) |
Hillman et al. (49) | 221 | 7–9 years old | 9 months; 5 days/week; 2 school periods/day | CG and IG | Increased PA time; Flanker task, RT, Switch task | At university by research staff (HQ) | 425*** | Significantly better results in IG in inhibition (0.27), cognitive flexibility (0.35) |
Hollar et al. (50) | 2,494 | 6–13 years old | 2 years, 5 days/week, 10 min/day | CG and IG | Increased PA and lessons about healthy lifestyle, integrated and replicable nutrition; FCAT (mathematics and reading) | Researchers (HQ) | 50 | Significantly better AP in FCAT math scores (0.21) |
Ismail (51) | 142 | 10–12 years old | Academic year, 5 days/week, 60 min/day | CG and IG | Enhanced PE and increased PA with an emphasis on coordination and balance; S.A.A.T and Otis test | PE teachers (HQ) | 225 | Intervention group performed significantly better in AP (0.35) |
Kamijo et al. (52) | 36 | 7–9 years old | 9 months; 5 days/week; 90 min/day | CG and IG | Increased PA time; Sternberg cognitive task, EEG activity | At university by research staff (HQ) | 425*** | Response accuracy was better in IG (0.73), three letter condition was significantly better in IG (0.65) |
Katz et al. (53) | 352 | 7–9 years old** (2nd−4th grade) | 8 months; 5 days/week; 30 min/day | CG and IG | Increased PA time; MAP, academic performance grades from communication, mathematics, and arts | Classroom teachers (LQ) | 150 | No overall significant change was seen in math AP scores (0.00). |
Koutsandreou et al. (54) | 71 | 9–10 years old (3rd and 4th grade) | 10 weeks; 3 days/week; 45 min/day | CG and 2IG (the motor-demanding exercise program and cardiovascular exercise program) | Increased PA time; Letter Digit Span test | Experienced exercise instructor (HQ) | 135 | No effect (0.00) |
Kvalø et al. (55) | 449 | 10–11 years old (5th grade) | 10 months; 5/2 days/week; 20/45 min/day | CG and IG | Increased PA time (during academic narrated lessons, breaks and active homework); Stroop test, verbal fluency test, digit span, and Trail Making test | Classroom teachers (LQ) | 188 | Significantly increased executive function in intervention group (0.21) |
Ludyga et al. (56) | 36 | 12–15 years old | 8 weeks; 5 days/week; 20 min/day | CG and IG | Increased PA time; Sternberg cognitive task, EEG activity | Experienced instructors (HQ) | 100 | No effect (0.00) in accuracy rates and significant impact on reaction time (0.79) |
Mahar et al. (57) | 342 | 5–11 years old | 15 weeks, 5 days/week, 10 min/day | CG and IG | Increased PA (during academic narrated lessons); knowledge test | Classroom teachers (LQ) | 50 | No effect (0.00) |
Mcclelland et al. (58) | 348 | 7–13 years old | 12 weeks; 5 days/week; 20 min/day | CG and IG | Increased PA time; National examinations in mathematics, reading, and writing. | Classroom teachers (LQ) | 100 | IG performed significantly better than CG (0.86) for national exams and (1.24) for progress through National Curriculum levels in reading, maths and writing. |
Mullender-Wijnsma et al. (59) | 81 | 8.1 years old (2nd and 3rd grade) | 22 weeks; 3 days/week; 20–30 min/day | CG and IG | Increased PA; Tempo test Rekenen (speed test, arithmetic), Een-Minut Test, Reading | Classroom teachers (LQ) | 75 | Mathematics (4.97) and reading (3.38) grades of 3nd grade children were significantly higher and mathematics grade of 2nd grade children were significantly lower (−5.17) |
Mullender-Wijnsma et al. (60) | 499 | 8.1 years old (2nd and 3rd grade) | 44 weeks, 3 days/week, 45 min/day | CG and IG | Increased PA (during academic narrated lessons); mathematic speed test and general mathematics skills test, reading, and spelling | Qualified primary teachers at the beginning of the study, later classroom teachers (LQ) | 135 | Intervention group perform significantly better in AP: mathematic speed, general mathematics, and spelling (0.43) |
Murray et al. (61) | 893 (193) | 8–11 years old | 1.5 years; 5–20 min/day | CG and IG | Increased PA (during academic narrated lessons); Stanford 10 reading comprehension and math problem-solving achievement tests | Classroom teachers (LQ) | 50*** | Intervention group perform significantly better in math score and reading (0.31) |
Peternelj et al. (62) | 134 | 7–15 years old (1st−8th grade) | 8 years; 2 (1st−6th grade) or 1 time/week (7th and 8th grade); 45 min/day | CG and IG | Increased PE time; Academic performance grades (mathematics and language), GPA | PE teachers (HQ) | 78, 75*** | Significant effect only in boys on the language (0.83) and GPA (0.54), whereas no effect in girls (0.00) |
Reed et al. (63) | 155 | 7–10 years old | 4 months, 3 days/week, 30 min/day | CG and IG | Aerobic PA; PACT, SPM, Fluid Intelligence Tests | Researchers and classroom teachers (HQ) | 90 | Intervention group performed better in Fluid intelligence testing and in PACT tests (0.31) |
Resaland et al. (64) | 1,129 | (M = 10.2 years) | 8 months;30 min/day (3 times/week); 15 min/day (every day) | CG and IG | Increased PA time (during academic narrated lessons, homework and breaks); Standard Norwegian national tests (mathematics, reading, English) | Classroom teachers (LQ) | 165 | No effect (0.00), some significant effects in subgroups (poorest in the baseline). |
Riley et al. (65) | 240 | 10–12 years old (M = 11.13) (5th and 6th grade) | 6 weeks; 3 days/week; 60 min/day | CG and IG | Increased PA time (during academic narrated lessons); math performance | Classroom teachers (LQ) | 180 | No effect (0.00) |
Sallis et al. (19) | 655 | 10–11 years old** (5th and 6th grades) | 2 years, 27–42 min/day | CG and 2 IG (group taught by professional PE teachers and group taught by untrained classroom teachers) | Enhanced PE; Metropolitan achievement tests | PE teachers and classroom teachers (HQ) | 35.5 | No effect (0.00) |
Shephard et al. (17) | 546 | 6–12 years old** (1st grade-−6th grade) | 6 years, 5 days/week, 60 min/day | CG and IG | Increased PA; Standard provincial tests, teacher grades, WISC test | PE teachers (HQ) | 225 | No effect (0.00); intervention group showed some insignificant improvements in AP grades and performed significantly better in Math Provincial Test Scores but significantly worse in overall score and English score. |
Sjöwall et al. (66) | 470 | 6–13 years old (1st−6th grade) | 2 years; 3 days/week; 60 min/day | CG and IG | Increased PA time; working memory, arithmetic test | Activity leader (LQ) | 120 | No effect (0.00) |
Spitzer and Hollmann (67) | 44 | 12–13 years old [M = 12,5(IG) and 13 years (CG)] (6th grade) | 4 months; 3 days/week; 45 min/day | CG and IG | Increased PA time; academic performance grades from mathematics, German, and English language | Classroom teacher (LQ) | 135 | No effect (0.00) |
Tarp et al. (68) | 855 | 12–14 years old (6th and 7th grade) | 20 weeks; 5 days/week; 60 min/day | CG and IG | Increased PA time; Eriksen flanker test for cognitive control, mathematics skills test (algebra, arithmetic, problem-solving, geometry) | Researchers—external collaborator (also responsible for inviting schools) (HQ) | 300 | No effect (0.00) |
Tuckman and Hinkle (69) | 154 | 9–12 years old | 12 weeks, 3 days/week, 30 min/day | CG and IG | Aerobic PA (running); TDT | Two experimenters and two undergraduate students (LQ) | 90 | No effect (0.00) |
Niet et al. (70) | 112 | 8–12 years old | 22 weeks; 2 days/week; 30 min/day | CG and IG | Increased PA time (break time); Stroop test, VMS, TMT, ToL | PE teachers (HQ) | 60 | No effect (0.00); when taking baseline scores into account, intervention children showed small improvement in Stroop test but no significant differences were found on other executive functioning measures. |
Vazou et al. (71) | 284 | (4th and 5th grade) | 8 weeks; 5 days/week; 25–50 min/day | CG and IG | Increased PA time (during academic narrated lessons); Academic performance grades (mathematics) | Classroom teachers (LQ) | 185 | Intervention group performed significantly better in math (0.68) |
Zervas et al. (72) | 26 | 11–14 years old | 25 weeks, 3 days/week, 75 min/day | CG and IG | Aerobic PA; Cognitrone test | PE teachers (HQ) | 225 | Intervention group performed significantly better in Cognitrone test (2.01) |
Bpa, beats per minute; MVPA, moderate to vigorous PA; CG, control group; IG, intervention group; HQ, staff with higher professional qualifications; LQ, staff with lower professional qualifications; IGF-M, Medium version of the Spanish Overall Factorial Intelligence Test; CAS, Cognitive Assessment Treatment; M-WCST, modified Wisconsin card sorting test; WIAT-II, Wechsler Individual Achievement Test-2nd Edition; LUS, Reading development test; CANTAB, Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Battery; ANT, Attention Network Test; FCAT, Florida Comprehensive Achievement Test; S.A.A.T., Stanford Academic Achievement Test; MAP, Missouri Academic Performance; PACT, Palmetto Achievement Challenge Tests; SPM, Standard Progressive Matrices; WISC, Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children; TDT, Test of divergent thinking; VMS, Visual Memory Span test; TMT, Trailmakig test; ToT, The tower of London test.
age was calculated using national primary and lower secondary school enrolment information;
time was calculated using average minutes;
overall intervention time.
Data Extraction and Statistical Analysis
One review author extracted data (VS), while the second (GS) and third (GJ) reviewer checked the entered data. Two review authors (VS and GS) extracted outcome data in duplicate, discussing and resolving discrepancies between them and consulted third (GJ) reviewer if necessary. Data supplied for included meta-analysis were checked for missing data and internal data consistency. Summary tables of entered data were checked with the trial protocol and latest trial report or publication. Any discrepancies or unusual patterns were checked with the study investigator. Hunter-Schmidt estimate was used for reducing the amount of bias and Fisher's z transformation was applied to samples' ES to display publication bias (31, 32). We also assessed publication bias with Egger bias test (73).
Chi-square statistical analysis were performed to establish the difference in the effects of interventions on AP in regards to the staff qualifications in PE teaching and exercise science. In the review, we differentiated between staff groups with higher professional qualifications, including exercise science researchers and PE teachers, and staff groups with lower professional qualifications, such as classroom teachers and students, who performed interventions and measurements.
The review includes only studies with enough data to calculate the standardized mean difference (ES) between the intervention and the control group's AP score. For this calculation, the Cohen's (74) and Rosenthal and Rosnow's (75) formulas were used: ES = M1 − M2/SDpooled (where SDpooled = √[( + )/2]) and ES = 2t/√df), where M1 represents the intervention group, M2 represents the control group, SDpooled is the pooled standard deviation of both groups, SD1 represents the standard deviation of the intervention group, and SD2 represents standard deviation of the control group.
In studies that enabled the calculation of more ES, the average ES was used for further analysis. The ES of each intervention was converted to correlation (rw) determined by Hunter-Schmidt approach (76), which suggests using pooled within-group SD, because it has less sampling error than the control group. In other words, the aforementioned method corrects ES for measurement error under the condition of equal ES. Rw was multiplied by the sample size of each study (rw × N), which represents the numerator and sum of sample sizes represents the denominator of the equation to calculate population effect (rp). The generalizability of rp was corrected using an artifact correction and variance sample ES, where the sampling error variance (Vobs) was based on the population correlation estimate (ri) and the average sample size N. The variance due to sampling error was conducted using the equation: Vs = (1-)2/(N-1), where N is the sample size across studies. Estimates of the variance in ES have been calculated using the equation: Vp = Vobs-Vs. For weighted mean (rw), 95% credibility interval: CIw = rw + 1.96√Vp and I2and Q statistics to measure heterogeneity of ES were calculated. Descriptors for the used magnitudes of ES were suggested by Cohen (74) and expanded by Sawilowsky (77).
Assessment of Bias Risk
The assessment of bias risk in the final sample (n = 44 studies) was conducted using the criteria previously used by Sneck et al. (78), including the criterion of power calculations. Each study received “0” (does not meet the criterion) or “1” (meets the criterion) based on an analysis of the reporting described in the original article. The Grades Research, Assessment, Development and Evaluation (GRADE) approach was applied to the meta-analysis to determine the quality of evidence; this involved grading the evidence based on a criteria for risk of bias, imprecision, inconsistency, indirectness and publication bias, in conformation with studies reported elsewhere (79, 80).
Results
The flow of the review process is shown in Figure 1. Altogether 14,245 records were identified. After removing duplicates from three different search engines, 591 abstracts with matching key words were identified, and 247 full-text papers were further reviewed. Among them, 68 were intervention studies with the control group. These were thoroughly reviewed, which left 44 intervention studies that met the final criteria for inclusion in the final review.
Altogether, 13,681 children participated in the interventions (n = 44) meeting the eligibility criteria. Present review covers over five decades of research studies, ranging from 1967 (51) to 2018 (56). All interventions, meeting all predefined criteria are presented in Table 1.
The mean of intervention time was 60.6 weeks and 144.5 min/week per intervention (Table 2). Duration of intervened time/week in studies with positive effect on AP was 170.9 min/week and 118.3 min/week in studies with no significant effect on AP. Duration of the average intervention time/week was 44.6% longer in studies with positive effect, also, the average study duration (weeks) was 4.9% longer in studies reporting a positive effect compared to studies with none or negative effect of increased PA on AP. Positive results of PA were evidenced in 20 interventions; of this, in 13 studies (65%) the interventions were performed by staff with higher professional qualifications. Negative or null effects on AP were reported in 24 interventions; of these in 9 studies (38%) the intervention(s) were performed by staff with higher professional qualifications (Table 1).
Table 2.
Number of participants | Duration of all interventions (week) | Intervention time (min/week) | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
PA effect on AP | all | + | – | all | + | – | all | + | – |
Min | 26 | 26 | 29 | 6 | 8 | 6 | 30 | 50 | 30 |
Max | 2,494 | 2,494 | 1,129 | 411 | 411 | 411 | 425 | 425 | 300 |
Median | 211 | 142 | 215 | 25 | 28.5 | 22 | 115 | 135 | 100 |
Mean | 240.5 | 281.8 | 294.2 | 60.6 | 73.2 | 69.8 | 144.5 | 170.9 | 118.3 |
All, presents all studies included in the meta-analysis; +, studies with positive effect on AP; –, studies with null or negative effect on AP.
The Hunter-Schmidt method (76) for isolation and correction of sources of error, such as sampling error and reliability of measurement variables, was used. Unweighted mean effect of population is ru = 0.351, whereas weighted mean effect or population effect size is rw = 0.181. This means that groups with increased PA experienced a positive weak effect on AP compared to control groups. Since the variance across sample ES consists of the variance of ES and the sampling error, sampling error variance (Vobs) for every intervention and variance due to sampling error, using population effect (rp), were calculated.
Variance due to sampling error (Vs = 0.003) and variance of population correlations (Vp = 0.107) were estimated. An average sample size of N = 240 yields a population effect size of rp = 0.181with 95% confidence interval ranging from 0.083 and 0.279 and 80% credibility interval ranging from −0.237 and 0.600. Such reliable differences between studies that give rise to varying effects could be due to publication bias of included studies (31). The Egger bias test (73) provides significant evidence for publication bias (bias = 4.137, 95% CI: 0.805–7.469, p = 0.019). There were considerable differences between the ES's from all individual studies (I2 = 97%; Q = 1598.2; p = 0.000), in studies where staff with higher professional qualifications performed intervention (I2 = 97%; Q = 970.5; p = 0.000) and in studies where staff with lower professional qualifications performed intervention (I2 = 96%; Q = 622.8; p = 0.000). Distribution of ES in all eligible interventions and publication bias (Figure 2), and a forest plot of all included studies (Figure 3) are presented.
The positive effect of a PA intervention on AP was estimated in 13 out of 20 significant interventions in which staff with higher professional qualifications performed the intervention and measurements. The weighted mean effect of the population effect size in interventions, performed by staff with higher professional qualifications, is 0.22 and in interventions performed by staff with lower professional qualifications is 0.14. Chi-square statistics were calculated and showed a significant difference (χ2 = 4.464; p = 0.035) on AP between studies in which the intervention and measurement were performed by practitioners with higher professional qualifications compared to those conducted by staff with lower professional qualifications, or in studies that lacked this information. Cramer's V value (0.319) showed there is a strong association between staff qualification and its effect on AP (p = 0.035).
The results of the risk-of bias assessment analysis are shown in Table 3. Of the 44 studies, 29 were rated as having a low risk of bias (> 67% of total score) with average of 0.79 of total score and 15 were rated as having moderate risk of bias (between 33 and 67% of the total score) with average of 0.50 of total score. None of the studies was rated as having a high risk of bias. Only 11 studies (25 %) reported power calculations to determine sufficient sample size, of those reporting positive effects of PA on academic performance/cognition, power calculations were provided in five studies (34, 46, 49, 55, 60). The quality of evidence (GRADE) where staff with higher and lower qualifications performed intervention and measurements was moderate (Table 4).
Table 3.
Author (year) | 1. Randomization | 2. Baseline comparable | 3. Baseline values accounted for in analyses | 4. Timing | 5. Blinding of measuring | 6. Validated outcome measures | 7. Dropout analysis | 8. Reporting of results | 9. Power calculation | Total score of the risk of bias (decimal format) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Ahamed et al. (18) | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 7/9 (0.78) |
Alesi et al. (33) | 0 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 4/9 (0.44) |
Ardoy et al. (34) | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 9/9 (1.00) |
Beck et al. (35) | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 0 | 6/9 (0.67) |
Bunketorp Käll et al. (36) | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 3/9 (0.33) |
Chaddock-Heyman et al. (37) | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 8/9 (0.89) |
Coe et al. (38) | 1 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 5/9 (0.56) |
Costigan et al. (39) | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 8/9 (0.89) |
Davis et al. (40) | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 0 | 7/9 (0.78) |
Greeff et al. (41) | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 8/9 (0.89) |
Dwyer et al. (14) | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 7/9 (0.78) |
Ericsson (42) | 0 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 5/9 (0.56) |
Ericsson and Karlsson (43) | 0 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 6/9 (0.67) |
Erwin et al. (44) | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 5/9 (0.56) |
Fedewa and Davis (45) | 1 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 5/9 (0.56) |
Fisher et al. (46) | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 8/9 (0.89) |
Gao et al. (47) | 0 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 5/9 (0.56) |
Hedges and Hardin (48) | 0 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 4/9 (0.44) |
Hillman et al. (49) | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 8/9 (0.89) |
Hollar et al. (50) | 0 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 6/9 (0.67) |
Ismail (51) | 0 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 4/9 (0.44) |
Kamijo et al. (52) | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 6/9 (0.67) |
Katz et al. (53) | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 7/9 (0.78) |
Koutsandreou et al. (54) | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 8/9 (0.89) |
Kvalø et al. (55) | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 9/9 (1.00) |
Ludyga et al. (56) | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 7/9 (0.78) |
Mahar et al. (57) | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 7/9 (0.78) |
Mcclelland et al. (58) | 0 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 4/9 (0.44) |
Mullender-Wijnsma et al. (59) | 1 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 6/9 (0.67) |
Mullender-Wijnsma et al. (60) | 1 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 7/9 (0.78) |
Murray et al. (61) | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 6/9 (0.67) |
Peternelj et al. (62) | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 6/9 (0.67) |
Reed et al. (63) | 1 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 7/9 (0.78) |
Resaland et al. (64) | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 8/9 (0.89) |
Riley et al. (65) | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 9/9 (1.00) |
Sallis et al. (19) | 1 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 5/9 (0.56) |
Shephard et al. (17) | 0 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 4/9 (0.44) |
Sjöwall et al. (66) | 0 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 5/9 (0.56) |
Spitzer and Hollmann (67) | 0 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 4/9 (0.44) |
Tarp et al. (68) | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 7/9 (0.78) |
Tuckman and Hinkle (69) | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 6/9 (0.67) |
Niet et al. (70) | 0 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 5/9 (0.56) |
Vazou et al. (71) | 0 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 7/9 (0.78) |
Zervas et al. (72) | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 6/9 (0.67) |
Average of all studies | 0.68 | 0.84 | 0.93 | 0.98 | 0.34 | 0.82 | 0.45 | 0.95 | 0.25 | 0.69 |
Table 4.
Quality assessment | Effect | Quality of evidence—GRADE | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Outcome | Study design | No. of studies (no. of participants) | Risk of bias | Inconsistency | Indirectness | Imprecision | Other considerations | Rw (95 % CI) | |
Positive effect on AP where staff with higher professional qualifications performed intervention | 15 randomized, 7 non-randomized | 22 (6,536) | No serious risk of bias (15 low risk of bias, 4 moderate risk of bias). |
No serious inconsistency I2 = 97.8% | No serious indirectness. | No serious limitations. | None | 0.22 (0.07–0.37) | ⊕⊕⊕□ MODERATE (6 high, 13 moderate, 3 low) |
Positive effect on AP where staff with lower professional qualifications performed intervention | 15 randomized, 7 non-randomized | 22 (7,145) | No serious risk of bias (2 moderate risk of bias, 5 high risk of bias). |
No serious inconsistency I2 = 96.6% | No serious indirectness. | No serious limitations. | None | 0.14 (0.02–0.27) | ⊕⊕⊕□ MODERATE (4 high, 14 moderate, 4 low) |
GRADE, Grades of Research, Assessment, Development and Evaluation (GRADE Working Group).
⊕⊕⊕⊕ (high): We have a lot of confidence that the true effect is similar to the estimated effect.
⊕⊕⊕□ (moderate): We believe that the true effect is probably close to the estimated effect.
⊕⊕□□ (low): We believe that the true effect might be markedly different from the estimated effect
⊕□□□ (very low): We believe that the true effect is probably markedly different from the estimated effect.
Discussion
This systematic review and meta-analysis investigated the impact of school-based PA interventions on the AP of primary and lower- secondary schoolchildren; it considered the amount of PA in the interventions and the qualifications of staff administering interventions in its analysis. The main findings of the study are that changes in AP itself is not caused solely by an increase in frequency and/or duration of PA, but studies must also take into consideration the intensity of PA administered. Secondly, and of equal import, is the significant positive effect observed when PA interventions are delivered by practitioners with higher professional qualifications, who were able to mediate higher PA intensity and focus of interventions.
Despite some promising results in the reviewed interventions (31, 33, 34, 37, 40, 42, 46, 49, 50, 52–55, 57, 58, 60, 61, 63, 71, 72), it is essential to emphasize that future research on the relationships between AP and PA should consider more qualitative aspects of PA, including intensity and types of activities. Namely, positive effects of AP may only accrue when of moderate-to-vigorous PA is increased (41, 42). Although the effects of PA on AP are seemingly well-documented in the literature most research and review studies have focused on the behavioral aspects of PA, such as frequency and duration, whereas PA intensity was hardly addressed, even though it is essential for properly elucidating the effects of exercise on cognition (43).
To avoid reporting sometimes misleading results of short interventions that are often affected by an initial increase in motivation of study participants (i.e., researchers, teachers, parents and children), only interventions longer than 6 weeks' duration were included for this analysis. The analysis showed that in the longer-lasting interventions there was a greater decline in moderate-to vigorous PA across the intervention time.
Samples in interventions which reported positive PA impacts were larger than the ones in the interventions with negative or no impact; this should be taken into consideration when interpreting the results. The population effect (rp) was shown to be small, which means that increased PA in experimental groups had only a small effect on AP compared to control groups. Since the connection between both variables was small, PA alone may not be the best predictor of children's AP. When researching the relationships between cognition and activity, researchers should start thinking about utilizing and reporting more stable measures, like the level of physical fitness, which is a marker outcome of habitual physical activity. For example, it has been shown that after-school PA improves cardiovascular endurance in children, which then mediates improvements in AP (81). A meta-analysis of published evidence on the relationship between physical fitness and AP between 2005 and 2015 asserts that cardiorespiratory fitness, speed-agility, motor coordination, and perceptual-motor skills are highly associated with AP (45), but the findings on the relationship between AP to strength and flexibility remain unclear in this regard. Indeed, it can be argued that physical fitness may be a better predictor of AP than PA. Several studies have identified a positive relationship between cardiorespiratory fitness, weight and AP (46–49) or with overall physical fitness (82). For example, researchers from Portugal have shown that cardiorespiratory fitness is independently related to AP; moreover, students with normal weight tended to have the best academic performance (52). The influence of moderators and mediators to PA, such as socioeconomic status, parental education (83), concentration (84) gender (85), personality (86), motivation, body-image, self-esteem (87), have also been shown to have an impact on AP.
In almost all analyzed interventions, the higher qualifications of the involved staff were shown to positively influence changes observed in AP. We know that PE lessons led by trained PE teachers provides more activity to students than ones led by generalized classroom teachers (25, 88–90). In general, the pedagogical qualifications of classroom teachers for delivering PE classes are far lower than those for of specialist PE teachers. The classroom teachers are of course qualified teachers, but with a rather limited training in PE teaching and exercise science, which requires specialist training to perform well. They often experience insufficient expertise and ability to organize PE with its distinctive content in an effective way (91). Breslin et al. (92) showed that the PE specialists show higher levels of self-determination toward exercise, are more autonomous in their decisions to be active, are more physically active and have a higher level of perceived qualification in delivering a PE lesson than the generalist teachers do. In addition, McKenzie et al. (89) reported that children taught by PE specialists spend 57% more time in moderate-to vigorous PA, with a concurrently increased emphasis on the promotion of physical fitness. This also has the important advantage of maintaining positive health-related behaviors that may last beyond childhood (93, 94).
Study Strengths and Limitations
The present review covers more than five decades worth of research studies, but analyses only PA interventions that have met the predefined criteria. Therefore, only interventions published in peer-reviewed scientific journals with a focus on primary and lower secondary school-children, evenly represented genders, exceeding 6 weeks and with reported ES or with data that enables the calculation of ES were included in the meta-analysis. The analysis did not: (i) distinguish between the published results that used moderate-to vigorous PA as a significant predictor that might affect AP and the ones that used low or vigorous physical activity; (ii) it did not take into consideration the differences between the results deriving from subjective or objective measures of PA; (iii) it did not take into consideration different instruments for assessing AP that were used in various studies, thus results cannot be generalized to different measures of AP; (iv) it did not take into consideration the differences between studies with large sample size that were able to detect even small differences and the ones with smaller sample sizes that could not, which can lead to potential bias; and it should always be noted that (v) statistically non-significant results are less-likely to be published, resulting in upwardly biased meta-analytically derived effect sizes for any analysis of this kind.
Conclusion
Parents are often concerned that time allocated to PA and sport may negatively affect children's AP. The present analysis shows that PA itself does not negatively affect AP; moreover, there are positive, (although relatively small) relationships between the two, and that changes in AP itself is not caused solely by an increase in frequency and/or duration of PA, but studies must also take into consideration the intensity of PA administered. Secondly, the significant positive effect of PA interventions are most observed when delivered by practitioners with higher professional qualifications who are able to mediate higher PA intensity in the interventions. Finally, in interventions with long durations, there are greater declines in moderate-to vigorous PA, suggesting a challenge to maintaining interventions across the intervention time-span. Finally, when reporting the monitoring, surveillance and evaluation of PA interventions, using physical fitness as criteria measure of PA is much more effective in terms of both the economic and organizational sense.
Data Availability Statement
All datasets generated for this study are included in the article/supplementary material.
Author Contributions
All authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
Prof. Jose Carlos Ribeiro is kindly acknowledged for reading the manuscript and giving helpful suggestions.
Footnotes
Funding. This research was funded by the Slovenian Research Agency within the Research programme P5-0142 Bio-psycho-social context of kinesiology.
References
- 1.Gunter KB, Almstedt HC, Janz KF. Physical activity in childhood may be the key to optimizing lifespan skeletal health. Exerc Sport Sci Rev. (2012) 40:13–21. 10.1097/JES.0b013e318236e5ee [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Janssen I, LeBlanc AG. Systematic review of the health benefits of physical activity and fitness in school-aged children and youth. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. (2010) 7:40. 10.1186/1479-5868-7-40 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Fernhall B, Agiovlasitis S. Arterial function in youth: window into cardiovascular risk. J Appl Physiol. (2008) 105:325–33. 10.1152/japplphysiol.00001.2008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Biddle SJH, Asare M. Physical activity and mental health in children and adolescents: a review of reviews. Br J Sports Med. (2011) 45:886–95. 10.1136/bjsports-2011-090185 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Chaddock L, Pontifex MB, Hillman CH, Kramer AF. A review of the relation of aerobic fitness and physical activity to brain structure and function in children. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2011) 17: 975–85. 10.1017/S1355617711000567 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Sibley BA, Etnier JL. The relationship between physical activity and cognition in children: a meta-analysis. Pediatr Exerc Sci. (2003) 15:243–56. 10.1123/pes.15.3.243 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Castelli DM, Hillman CH, Buck SM, Erwin HE. Physical fitness and academic achievement in third-and fifth-grade students. J Sport Exerc Psychol. (2007) 29:239–52. 10.1123/jsep.29.2.239 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Chaddock L, Erickson KI, Prakash RS, VanPatter M, Voss MW, Pontifex MB, et al. Basal ganglia volume is associated with aerobic fitness in preadolescent children. Dev Neurosci. (2010) 32:249–56. 10.1159/000316648 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Kramer AF, Hahn S, Cohen NJ, Banich MT, McAuley E, Harrison CR, et al. Ageing, fitness and neurocognitive function. Nature. (1999) 400:418–9. 10.1038/22682 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Kleim JA, Cooper NR, VandenBerg PM. Exercise induces angiogenesis but does not alter movement representations within rat motor cortex. Brain Res. (2002) 934:1–6. 10.1016/S0006-8993(02)02239-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Kramer AF, Erickson KI. Capitalizing on cortical plasticity: influence of physical activity on cognition and brain function. Trends Cogn Sci. (2007) 11:342–8. 10.1016/j.tics.2007.06.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Hillman CH, Erickson KI, Kramer AF. Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2008) 9:58–65. 10.1038/nrn2298 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Bherer L, Erickson KI, Liu-Ambrose T. A review of the effects of physical activity and exercise on cognitive and brain functions in older adults. J Aging Res. (2013) 9:657508. 10.1155/2013/657508 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Dwyer T, Coonan WE, Leitch DR, Hetzel BS, Baghurst RA. An investigation of the effects of daily physical activity on the health of primary school students in South Australia. Int J Epidemiol. (1983) 12:308–13. 10.1093/ije/12.3.308 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Hollar D, Messiah SE, Lopez-Mitnik G, Hollar TL, Almon M, Agatston AS. Healthier options for public schoolchildren program improves weight and blood pressure in 6-to 13-year-olds. J Am Diet Assoc. (2010) 110:261–7. 10.1016/j.jada.2009.10.029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Donnelly JE, Greene JL, Gibson CA, Smith BK, Washburn RA, Sullivan DK, et al. Physical activity across the curriculum (PAAC): a randomized controlled trial to promote physical activity and diminish overweight and obesity in elementary school children. Prev Med. (2009) 49:336–41. 10.1016/j.ypmed.2009.07.022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Shephard RJ, Volle M, Lavallee H, LaBarre R, Jequier JC, Rajic M. Required physical activity and academic grades: a controlled study. In: Ilmarinen J, Välimäki I, editors. Children and Sport. Berlin: Springer; (1984). p. 58–63. 10.1007/978-3-642-69465-3_8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Ahamed Y, MacDonald H, Reed K, Naylor PJ, Liu-Ambrose T, Mckay H. School-based physical activity does not compromise children's academic performance. Med Sci Sports Exerc. (2007) 39:371–6 10.1249/01.mss.0000241654.45500.8e [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Sallis JF, McKenzie TL, Alcaraz JE, Kolody B, Faucette N, Hovell MF. The effects of a 2-year physical education program (SPARK) on physical activity and fitness in elementary school students. Sports, Play and Active Recreation for Kids. Am J Public Health. (1997) 87:1328–34. 10.2105/AJPH.87.8.1328 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Etnier JL, Salazar W, Landers DM, Petruzzello SJ, Han M, Nowell P. The influence of physical fitness and exercise upon cognitive functioning: a meta-analysis. J Sport Exerc Psychol. (1997) 19:249–77. 10.1123/jsep.19.3.249 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Trudeau F, Shephard RJ. Physical education, school physical activity, school sports and academic performance. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. (2008) 5:10. 10.1186/1479-5868-5-10 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Verstraete SJM, Cardon GM, De Clercq DLR, De Bourdeaudhuij IMM. Increasing children's physical activity levels during recess periods in elementary schools: the effects of providing game equipment. Eur J Public Health. (2006) 16:415–9. 10.1093/eurpub/ckl008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Darling-Hammond L, Berry B. Highly qualified teachers for all. Educ Leadersh. (2006) 64:14–20. [Google Scholar]
- 24.Pantics N. The meaning of teacher competence in contexts of change: in search of missing elements of a knowledge base for teacher education–moral purposes and change agentry (doctoral dissertation), Utrecht, Netherlands, University of Utrecht; (2011). [Google Scholar]
- 25.Daly-smith AJ, Zwolinsky S, Mckenna J, Tomporowski PD, Defeyter MA, Manley A. Systematic review of acute physically active learning and classroom movement breaks on children's physical activity, cognition, academic performance and classroom behaviour: understanding critical design features. BMJ Open Sport Exerc Med. (2018) 4:e000341. 10.1136/bmjsem-2018-000341 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Donnelly JE, Hillman CH, Castelli D, Etnier JL, Lee S, Tomporowski P, et al. Physical activity, fitness, cognitive function, and academic achievement in children: a systematic review. Med Sci Sports Exerc. (2016) 48:1197–222. 10.1249/MSS.0000000000000901 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Singh AS, Saliasi E, Van Den Berg V, Uijtdewilligen L, De Groot RHM, Jolles J, et al. Effects of physical activity interventions on cognitive and academic performance in children and adolescents: a novel combination of a systematic review and recommendations from an expert panel. Br J Sports Med. (2018) 53:640–7. 10.1136/bjsports-2017-098136 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Xue Y, Yang Y, Huang T. Effects of chronic exercise interventions on executive function among children and adolescents: a systematic review with meta-analysis. Br J Sport Med. (2019) 53:1397–404. 10.1136/bjsports-2018-099825 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Watson A, Timperio A, Brown H, Best K, Hesketh KD. Effect of classroom-based physical activity interventions on academic and physical activity outcomes: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. (2017) 14:114. 10.1186/s12966-017-0569-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Braaksma P, Stuive I, Garst RME, Wesselink CF, van der Sluis CK, Dekker R, et al. Characteristics of physical activity interventions and effects on cardiorespiratory fitness in children aged 6–12 years—A systematic review. J Sci Med Sport. (2018) 21:296–306. 10.1016/j.jsams.2017.07.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG, PRISMA Group. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Med. (2010) 6:e1000097 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Moher D, Shamseer L, Clarke M, Ghersi D, Liberati A, Petticrew M, et al. Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015 statement. Ann Intern Med. (2009) 151:264–9. 10.7326/0003-4819-151-4-200908180-00135 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Alesi M, Bianco A, Luppina G, Palma A. Improving children's coordinative skills and executive functions: the effects of a football exercise program. perceptual and motor skills. Percept Mot Skills. (2016) 122:27–46. 10.1177/0031512515627527 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Ardoy DN, Fernández-Rodríguez JM, Jiménez-Pavón D, Castillo R, Ruiz JR, Ortega FB. A physical education trial improves adolescents' cognitive performance and academic achievement: the EDUFIT study. Scand J Med Sci Sports. (2014) 24:e52–61. 10.1111/sms.12093 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Beck MM, Lind RR, Geertsen SS, Ritz C, Lundbye-Jensen J, Wienecke J. Motor-enriched learning activities can improve mathematical performance in preadolescent children. Front Hum Neurosci. (2016) 10:645. 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00645 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Bunketorp Käll L, Malmgren H, Olsson E, Lindén T, Nilsson M. Effects of a curricular physical activity intervention on children's school performance, wellness, and brain development. J Sch Health. (2015) 85:704–13. 10.1111/josh.12303 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Chaddock-Heyman L, Erickson KI, Voss M, Knecht A, Pontifex MB, Castelli D, et al. The effects of physical activity on functional MRI activation associated with cognitive control in children: a randomized controlled intervention. Front Hum Neurosci. (2013) 12:72. 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00072 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Coe DP, Pivarnik JM, Womack CJ, Reeves MJ, Malina RM. Effect of physical education and activity levels on academic achievement in children. Med Sci Sport Exerc. (2006) 38:1515–9. 10.1249/01.mss.0000227537.13175.1b [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Costigan SA, Eather N, Plotnikoff RC, Hillman CH, Lubans DR. High-intensity interval training on cognitive and mental health in adolescents. Med Sci Sports Exerc. (2016) 48:1985–93. 10.1249/MSS.0000000000000993 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Davis CL, Tomporowski PD, Boyle CA, Waller JL, Miller PH, Naglieri JA, et al. Effects of aerobic exercise on overweight children's cognitive functioning: a randomized controlled trial. Res Q Exerc Sport. (2007) 78:510–9. 10.1080/02701367.2007.10599450 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.De Greeff JW, Hartman E, Bosker RJ, Doolaard S, Visscher C. Long-term effects of physically active academic lessons on physical fitness and executive functions in primary school children. Health Educ Res. (2016) 31:185–94. 10.1093/her/cyv102 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Ericsson I. Motor skills, attention and academic achievements. An intervention study in school years 1–3. Br Educ Res J. (2008) 34:301–13. 10.1080/01411920701609299 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Ericsson I, Karlsson MK. Motor skills and school performance in children with daily physical education in school–a 9-year intervention study. Scand J Med Sci Sports. (2014) 24:273–8. 10.1111/j.1600-0838.2012.01458.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Erwin H, Fedewa A, Ahn S. Student academic performance outcomes of a classroom physical activity intervention : a pilot study. Int Electron J Elem Educ. (2013) 5:473–87. [Google Scholar]
- 45.Fedewa AL, Davis MC. A randomized controlled design investigating the effects of classroom- based physical activity on children's fluid intelligence and achievement. School Psychol Int. (2015) 36:135–53. 10.1177/0143034314565424 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Fisher A, Boyle JME, Paton JY, Tomporowski P, Watson C, McColl JH, et al. Effects of a physical education intervention on cognitive function in young children: randomized controlled pilot study. BMC Pediatr. (2011) 11:97. 10.1186/1471-2431-11-97 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Gao Z, Hannan P, Xiang P, Stodden DF, Valdez VE. Video game-based exercise, Latino children's physical health, and academic achievement. Am J Prev Med. (2013) 44:S240–6. 10.1016/j.amepre.2012.11.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Hedges WD, Hardin VB. Effects of a perceptual motor program on achievement of first graders. Educ Leadersh Res Suppl. (1972) 6:5231–2. [Google Scholar]
- 49.Hillman C, Pontifex MB, Castelli D, Khan NA. Effects of the FITKids randomized controlled trial on executive control and brain function. Pediatrics. (2014) 134:e1063–71. 10.1542/peds.2013-3219 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Hollar D, Lombardo M, Lopez-Mitnik G, Hollar TL, Almon M, Agatston AS, et al. Effective multi-level, multi-sector, school-based obesity prevention programming improves weight, blood pressure, and academic performance, especially among low-income, minority children. J Health Care Poor Underserved. (2010) 21:93–108. 10.1353/hpu.0.0304 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Ismail AH. The effects of a well-organized physical education programme on intellectual performance. Research in Phys Educ. (1967) 1:31–8. [Google Scholar]
- 52.Kamijo K, Pontifex MB, Leary KCO, Scudder MR, Wu C, Castelli DM, et al. The effects of an afterschool physical activity program on working memory in preadolescent children. Dev Sci. (2011) 14:1046–58. 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01054.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Katz DL, Cushman D, Reynolds J, Njike V, Treu JA, Walker J, et al. Putting physical activity where it fits in the school day: preliminary results of the abc (activity bursts in the classroom) for fitness program. Prev Chronic Dis. (2010) 7:A82. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.KoutsandrÉou F, Wegner M, Niemann C, Budde H. Effects of motor versus cardiovascular exercise training on children's working memory. Med Sci Sport Exerc. (2016) 48:1144–52. 10.1249/MSS.0000000000000869 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Kvalø SE, Bru E, Brønnick K, Dyrstad SM. Does increased physical activity in school affect children's executive function and aerobic fitness? Scand J Med Sci Sports. (2017) 27:1833–41. 10.1111/sms.12856 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Ludyga S, Gerber M, Kamijo K, Brand S, Pühse U. The effects of a school-based exercise program on neurophysiological indices of working memory operations in adolescents. J Sci Med Sport. (2018) 21:833–8. 10.1111/josh.12303 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Mahar MT, Rowe DA, Kenny RK, Fesperman DN. Evaluation of the TAKE 10 classroom-based physical activity program. Med Sci Sport Exerc. (2003) 35:S135 10.1097/00005768-200305001-00742 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Mcclelland E, Pitt A, Stein J. Enhanced academic performance using a novel classroom physical activity intervention to increase awareness, attention and self- control: putting embodied cognition into practice. Improv Schools. (2015) 18:83–100. 10.1177/1365480214562125 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Mullender-Wijnsma MJ, Hartman E, de Greeff JW, Bosker RJ, Doolaard S, Visscher C. Improving academic performance of school-age children by physical activity in the classroom: 1-year program evaluation. J Sch Health. (2015) 85:365–71. 10.1111/josh.12259 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Mullender-Wijnsma MJ, Hartman E, de Greeff JW, Doolaard S, Bosker RJ, Visscher C. Physically active math and language lessons improve academic achievement: a cluster randomized controlled trial. Pediatrics. (2016) 1:e20152743. 10.1542/peds.2015-2743 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Murray NG, Garza JC, Diamond PM, Hoelscher DM, Kelder S, Ward JL. Fitness and academic achievement among 3rd and 4th grade students in texas: 932. Med Sci Sport Exerc. (2008) 40:S96 10.1249/01.mss.0000321867.49584.bb [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Peternelj B, Škof B, Strel J. Academic achievement of pupils in sport classes: pupils attending sport classes have higher final grades, but… Kinesiologia Slovenica. (2009) 15:5–16. [Google Scholar]
- 63.Reed JA, Einstein G, Hahn E, Hooker SP, Gross VP, Kravitz J. Examining the impact of integrating physical activity on fluid intelligence and academic performance in an elementary school setting: a preliminary investigation. J Phys Act Heal. (2010) 7:343–51. 10.1123/jpah.7.3.343 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Resaland GK, Aadland E, Moe VF, Aadland KN, Skrede T, Stavnsbo M, et al. Effects of physical activity on schoolchildren's academic performance: the Active Smarter Kids (ASK) cluster-randomized controlled trial. Prev Med. (2016) 91:322–8. 10.1016/j.ypmed.2016.09.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Riley N, Lubans DR, Holmes K, Morgan PJ. Findings from the EASY minds cluster randomized controlled trial : evaluation of a physical activity integration program for mathematics in primary schools. J Phys Act Heal. (2016) 13:198–206. 10.1123/jpah.2015-0046 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Sjöwall D, Hertz M, Klingberg T. No long-term effect of physical activity intervention on working memory or arithmetic in preadolescents. Front Psychol. (2017) 8:1–10. 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01342 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Spitzer US, Hollmann W. Trends in neuroscience and education experimental observations of the effects of physical exercise on attention, academic and prosocial performance in school settings. Trends Neurosci Educ. (2013) 2:1–6. 10.1016/j.tine.2013.03.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Tarp J, Domazet SL, Froberg K, Hillman CH, Andersen LB, Bugge A. Effectiveness of a school-based physical activity intervention on cognitive performance in danish adolescents: lcomotion — learning, cognition and motion – a cluster randomized controlled trial. PLoS ONE. (2016) 11:e0158087. 10.1371/journal.pone.0158087 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Tuckman BW, Hinkle JS. An experimental study of the physical and psychological effects of aerobic exercise on schoolchildren. Heal Psychol. (1986) 5:197–207. 10.1037/0278-6133.5.3.197 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Niet AG, Van Der Smith J, Scherder EJA, Hartman E, Visscher C. Effects of a cognitively demanding aerobic intervention during recess on children's physical fitness and executive functioning. Pediatr Exerc Sci. (2016) 28:64–70. 10.1123/pes.2015-0084 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Vazou S. Intervention integrating physical activity with math : math performance, perceived competence, and need satisfaction. J Sport Exercise Psy. (2017) 15:508–22. 10.1080/1612197X.2016.1164226 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Zervas Y, Danis A, Klissouras V. Influence of physical exertion on mental performance with reference to training. Percept Mot Skills. (1991) 72:1215–21. 10.2466/pms.1991.72.3c.1215 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Egger M, Smith GD, Schneider M, Minder C. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ. (1997) 15:629–34. 10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; (1988). [Google Scholar]
- 75.Rosenthal R, Rosnow RL. Essentials of Behavioral Research: Methods and Data Analysis. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill; (1991). p. 45. [Google Scholar]
- 76.Morris SB. Book Review: Hunter, JE, & Schmidt, FL (2004). Methods of Meta-Analysis: Correcting Error and Bias in Research Findings. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Organ Res Methods. (2008) 11:184–7. 10.1177/1094428106295494 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Sawilowsky SS. New effect size rules of thumb. J Mod Appl Stat Methods. (2009) 8:597–9. 10.22237/jmasm/1257035100 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Sneck S, Viholainen H, Syväoja H, Kankaapää A, Hakonen H, Poikkeus AM, et al. Effects of school-based physical activity on mathematics performance in children: a systematic review. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. (2019) 16:109. 10.1186/s12966-019-0866-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Cumpston M, Li T, Page MJ, Chandler J, Welch VA, Higgins JP, et al. Updated guidance for trusted systematic reviews: a new edition of the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. (2019) 3:ED000142. 10.1002/14651858.ED000142 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Guyatt GH, Oxman AD, Kunz R, Atkins D, Brozek J, Vist G, et al. GRADE guidelines: 2. Framing the question and deciding on important outcomes. J Clin Epidemiol. (2011) 64:395–400. 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2010.09.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Fredericks CR, Kokot SJ, Krog S. Using a developmental movement programme to enhance academic skills in grade 1 learners. South African J Res Sport Phys Educ Recreat. (2006) 28:29–42. 10.4314/sajrs.v28i1.25929 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Rauner RR, Walters RW, Avery M, Wanser TJ. Evidence that aerobic fitness is more salient than weight status in predicting standardized math and reading outcomes in fourth-through eighth-grade students. J Pediatr. (2013) 163:344–8. 10.1016/j.jpeds.2013.01.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Yang XL, Telama R, Laakso L. Parents' physical activity, socioeconomic status and education as predictors of physical activity and sport among children and youths-A 12-year follow-up study. Int Rev Sociol Sport. (1996) 31:273–91. 10.1177/101269029603100304 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Caterino MC, Polak ED. Effects of two types of activity on the performance of second-, third-, and fourth-grade students on a test of concentration. Percept Mot Skills. (1999) 89:245–8. 10.2466/pms.1999.89.1.245 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Fedewa AL, Ahn S. The effects of physical activity and physical fitness on children's achievement and cognitive outcomes: a meta-analysis. Res Q Exerc Sport. (2011) 82:521–35. 10.1080/02701367.2011.10599785 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Chamorro-Premuzic T, Furnham A. Neuroticism and “special treatment” in university examinations. Soc Behav Personal an Int J. (2002) 30:807–11. 10.2224/sbp.2002.30.8.807 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Nelson MC, Gordon-Larsen P. Physical activity and sedentary behavior patterns are associated with selected adolescent health risk behaviors. Pediatrics. (2006) 117:1281–90. 10.1542/peds.2005-1692 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Jurak G, Cooper A, Leskosek B, Kovac M. Long-term effects of 4-year longitudinal school-based physical activity intervention on the physical fitness of children and youth during 7-year follow-up assessment. Cent Eur J Public Health. (2013) 21:190–4. 10.21101/cejph.a3823 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.McKenzie TL, Sallis JF, Faucette N, Roby JJ, Kolody B. Effects of a curriculum and inservice program on the quantity and quality of elementary physical education classes. Res Q Exerc Sport. (1993) 64:178–87. 10.1080/02701367.1993.10608795 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Starc G, Strel J, Kovač M. Telesni in Gibalni Razvoj Slovenskih Otrok in Mladine v Številkah: Šolsko Leto 2007/2008. Ljubljana: Fakulteta za šport; (2010). [Google Scholar]
- 91.Talbot M. Ways forward for primary physical education. Phys Educ Matters. (2008) 3:6–8. [Google Scholar]
- 92.Breslin G, Hanna D, Lowry RG, McKee D, Haughey T, Moore N. An exploratory study of specialist and generalist teachers: predicting self efficacy in delivering primary physical education. Work Pap Heal Sci. (2012) 1:1–9. [Google Scholar]
- 93.Manios Y, Kafatos I, Kafatos A, Team NCR. Ten-year follow-up of the Cretan Health and Nutrition Education Program on children's physical activity levels. Prev Med. (2006) 43:442–6. 10.1016/j.ypmed.2006.06.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Whitehead M. Physical Literacy: Throughout the Lifecourse. New York, NY: Routledge; (2010). 10.4324/9780203881903 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
All datasets generated for this study are included in the article/supplementary material.