Abstract
Although growing up in stressful conditions can undermine mental abilities, people in harsh environments may develop intact, or even enhanced, social and cognitive abilities for solving problems in high‐adversity contexts (i.e. ‘hidden talents’). We examine whether childhood and current exposure to violence are associated with memory (number of learning rounds needed to memorize relations between items) and reasoning performance (accuracy in deducing a novel relation) on transitive inference tasks involving both violence‐relevant and violence‐neutral social information (social dominance vs. chronological age). We hypothesized that individuals who had more exposure to violence would perform better than individuals with less exposure on the social dominance task. We tested this hypothesis in a preregistered study in 100 Dutch college students and 99 Dutch community participants. We found that more exposure to violence was associated with lower overall memory performance, but not with reasoning performance. However, the main effects of current (but not childhood) exposure to violence on memory were qualified by significant interaction effects. More current exposure to neighborhood violence was associated with worse memory for age relations, but not with memory for dominance relations. By contrast, more current personal involvement in violence was associated with better memory for dominance relations, but not with memory for age relations. These results suggest incomplete transfer of learning and memory abilities across contents. This pattern of results, which supports a combination of deficits and ‘hidden talents,’ is striking in relation to the broader developmental literature, which has nearly exclusively reported deficits in people from harsh conditions. A video abstract of this article can be viewed at: https://youtu.be/e4ePmSzZsuc.
People who grow up in stressful conditions may develop ‘hidden talents’: intact, or even enhanced, abilities for solving problems in high‐adversity contexts. We find partial support for this hypothesis. In our study, more current exposure to violence predicts intact or even enhanced memory for social dominance relationships, but more childhood exposure to violence predicts impaired memory for social dominance relationships.
Research Highlights
People who grow up in stressful conditions may develop ‘hidden talents’: intact, or even enhanced, abilities for solving problems in high‐adversity contexts.
Contrary to this hypothesis, in our study childhood exposure to violence was negatively or not associated with memory and inference for social dominance content.
However, current exposure to violence was associated with equal, or even better, memory performance (but not reasoning) for social dominance content.
Our data thus provide some support for the existence of a ‘hidden talent’, in addition to evidence for associations between adversity and impairment.
1. INTRODUCTION
It is well‐established that growing up under conditions of social and economic adversity can undermine children's development, cognitive abilities, and health (Duncan, Magnuson, & Votruba‐Drzal, 2017; Nelson, Fox, & Zeanah, 2014). An extensive body of research has demonstrated these harmful effects, producing valuable knowledge that has informed educational and economic policies and interventions designed to prevent and repair deficits. Far less recognized is an emerging body of research showing that people from high‐adversity backgrounds may develop enhanced social and cognitive abilities that are adapted to stressful conditions (known as the ‘specialization hypothesis’; Ellis, Bianchi, Griskevicius, & Frankenhuis, 2017; Frankenhuis & de Weerth, 2013). Documenting these ‘hidden talents’—and then redesigning teaching, learning, and assessment processes to capitalize on these abilities in school—is a potential tool for improving the academic performance of students from socially and economically disadvantaged backgrounds. Using these abilities as building blocks for success may boost confidence, motivation, and hence performance in people who suffer from stigma and hardship. Thus, to complement the prevailing focus on deficits, we need to identify the abilities that are enhanced by chronic exposures to stress. In the future, we envision an assessment battery that measures these hidden talents, which can be used to benefit high‐adversity youth in education, jobs, and civic life (Ellis et al., 2017).
This ‘hidden talents’ approach is based on an evolutionary‐developmental perspective focusing on adaptation in context (Ellis et al., 2017; Frankenhuis & de Weerth, 2013). It dovetails with research in cultural psychology, which regards differences in performance between individuals and social groups—including different socioeconomic groups—as not necessarily resulting from deficits, but as potentially resulting from people using their intelligence to solve locally significant challenges within the constraints posed by their environments (Banerjee, Bhattacharjee, Chattopadhyay, & Ganimian, 2017; Greenfield, 2014; Grossmann & Varnum, 2011; Kraus, Piff, Mendoza‐Denton, Rheinschmidt, & Keltner, 2012; Rogoff et al., 2017; Schliemann & Carraher, 2002; Sternberg, 2014; Varnum & Kitayama, 2017). Social and economic adversity can be regarded as one context that gives rise to specific knowledge, skills, and abilities. Such stress‐adapted skills, however, may be less likely to manifest in evolutionarily novel contexts, such as formal classroom settings, than in the ecological contexts in which they developed, such as neighborhood and family settings. Further, there are multiple dimensions of childhood stress (e.g. neglect, violence, unpredictability), each of which poses unique challenges that may promote the development of particular skills and abilities. For this reason, empirical studies of ‘hidden talents’ may benefit from measuring exposures to specific dimensions of stress, rather than general social or economic adversity (e.g. cumulative stress scores), which do not capture more fine‐grained individual variation.
1.1. Memory and reasoning about social dominance versus chronological age
The current study focuses on the challenge of navigating social hierarchies in violent environments. Social status is a crucial factor that determines access to resources in both humans and non‐human animals (Hawley, 1999). In order to respond effectively to potential rivals, allies, and mates, individuals need to determine their own and others’ social status (Penn, Holyoak, & Povinelli, 2008). Inferring social rank from observations of interactions is a vital skill, as direct confrontations can be time‐consuming and entail the risk of injury or death (Grosenick, Clement, & Fernald, 2007; Nakamaru & Sasaki, 2003; Paz‐y‐Miño, Bond, Kamil, & Balda, 2004; Wynne, 1995). Humans infer social dominance even from thin slices of behavior (Ambady, Bernieri, & Richeson, 2000).
Social hierarchies are often linear (Fiske, 1992; Gazes, Hampton, & Lourenco, 2015; Hawley, 1999; Hsu, Earley, & Wolf, 2006; De Soto, 1960; Zitek & Tiedens, 2012). Therefore, dominance relations may be inferred using transitive inference (TI): the ability to infer unknown relations between items (e.g. A > C), based on known relations of those items (e.g. A > B; B > C). TI involves memory of premises (e.g., A > B; B > C) and inference of a conclusion (e.g. A > C). Variation in performance may result from differences in memory, inference, or both. Hence, it is crucial to study the separate contributions of these processes (Bryant & Trabasso, 1971; Chalmers & McGonigle, 1984; Wright, 2012).
We theorized that inferring social dominance—in particular, forms of social dominance that involve the ability to win in a physical fight—is a critical task for people from harsh‐dangerous environments. In such contexts, the costs of incorrect memories and inferences about dominance are likely to be greater than in safer environments. For instance, in violent street gangs, challenging a dominant individual could trigger a beating or deadly assault (Keiser, 1979; Shakur, 2007; Venkatesh, 2008). Therefore, we hypothesized that people who have had greater exposure to violence develop enhanced memory and reasoning for dominance relations, compared with people who have had less exposure to violence. Our task stimuli specifically focused on dominance relations among young adult men. We operationalized exposure to violence both in terms of the amount of violence in one's lived environment (e.g. perceived neighborhood violence, witnessing fights) and one's direct involvement in physical fighting (e.g. frequency of involvement, severity of injuries).
To test our hypothesis, we examined memory and reasoning performance across two different relational domains: social dominance and chronological age. We included age relations, which are commonly included in conventional TI problems (Burt, 1919; Markovits & Dumas, 1999; Piaget, 1955), as a comparison variable. We expected small age differences to be equally (ir)relevant to all participants, regardless of exposure to violence. Although age can provide cues to rates of violence (e.g. young men fight more), age cues are not informative if age differences are small, as they were in our stimuli (i.e. all young adult men). We conceptualized minor age variations as exemplars of the kind of abstract or irrelevant information that tends to undermine performance on cognitive tasks among people from high‐adversity backgrounds (see Ellis et al., 2017). Because performance on conventional TI tasks is positively correlated with IQ (Burt, 1919; Sternberg, 1983), and because people exposed to social and economic adversity tend to score lower on IQ tests (APA Task Force on Socioeconomic Status, 2007), exposure to harsh environments may be associated with lower performance on conventional TI problems involving abstract or irrelevant relations. Accordingly, we hypothesized that people who had more exposure to violence would perform equal (based on equal relevance of age) or worse (based on previously documented lower performance on conventional transitive inference tasks) at memorizing and inferring age relations relative to people who had less exposure to violence.
We separately examined perceived neighborhood violence experienced as a bystander and direct involvement in violence, because their associations with cognitive abilities and performance could plausibly differ in weight. However, we had no a priori predictions about which kind of exposure to violence would show the highest association with our dependent variables. On the one hand, living in a dangerous neighborhood means frequently encountering potential dangers, and this could enhance memory and inference about dangers. On the other hand, actually being involved in violence (e.g. out of need, or due to higher levels of antisociality) increases the relevance of potential dangers, and this could enhance memory and inference about danger. Likewise, we examined childhood and current exposures to violence separately, again without a priori predictions regarding their relative weights. If cognitive abilities develop gradually, the association with childhood experiences may be higher. If, however, abilities adjust dynamically in response to ongoing contextual factors throughout adulthood, the association with current exposures may be higher (Frankenhuis, Panchanathan, & Nettle, 2016).
1.2. Training and transfer of hidden talents
If people with greater exposure to violence are indeed better able to memorize and reason about dominance relations than about abstract relations (e.g. symbols, numbers), or about less relevant, violence‐neutral social relations (e.g. minor age differences), then these enhanced abilities could potentially be leveraged in education and interventions. For instance, people could be taught mental operations (e.g. mathematics, transitive inference, syllogisms) in the context of dominance problems, which they may find more relevant and engaging than problems with more abstract or mundane content. Once they have mastered these operations in this more ecologically relevant context, they may learn to generalize them to other contents (‘near transfer’) and even to other kinds of contexts (‘far transfer’; Barnett & Ceci, 2002). By utilizing skills and social information content that are important in harsh‐dangerous environments, this instructional method works with, rather than against, social and cognitive adaptations to stress.
If this method—starting from a place of motivation and competence—succeeds, then personalized learning tools could be developed that leverage content domains (e.g. social dominance), styles of social interaction (e.g. heightened collaboration; Rogoff et al., 2017), and even executive function skills (e.g. attention shifting, working memory updating; Mittal, Griskevicius, Simpson, Sung, & Young, 2015; Young, Griskevicius, Simpson, Waters, & Mittal, 2018), that are potentially enhanced in students from high‐adversity backgrounds. Such learning tools could also enhance performance in cultural groups where formal schooling is less normative and which live in more unpredictable environments (Pope, Fagot, Meguerditchian, Washburn, & Hopkins, 2019). For example, social dominance content could serve as a starting point for adaptive learning, from which students can ‘fan out’ to other types of contents (i.e. near transfer), improving the scope of their performance.
The explosion of online learning, including at the middle and high school levels, greatly increases the opportunities for computer‐assisted instruction to be customized for specific students (Ellis et al., 2017). Two students from different backgrounds could take the same algebra course, but the course materials could, at least initially, be presented to each student in different ways. For students from high‐adversity contexts, instructional methods could utilize skills and content areas that reflect developmental adaptations to stress. If this method works, training and transfer based on hidden talents may offer a new tool to help reduce educational gaps, mitigating the pernicious cycle through which economic disadvantage translates into lasting educational disadvantage. In this way, our approach supports educational ‘equity—providing individuals with the means to thrive—rather than equality—treating everyone uniformly regardless of their specific needs’ (Moreau, Macnamara, & Hambrick, 2018, p. 4).
2. METHODS
The current research included two populations: an adult community sample and a college student sample. The community sample comprised people living in disadvantaged conditions for Dutch standards, some of whom have experienced chronic (i.e. prolonged, intense) stress, such as diverse exposures to violence while growing up, and others whom are currently facing an acute stressor (e.g. risk of eviction), but who have not necessarily experienced chronic stress. Members of the community sample on average had attained lower levels of education and were more likely to need governmental support in meeting their basic needs. We recruited the community sample via several organizations that help people who live in disadvantaged conditions in the Netherlands, facing such stressors as debt relief, previous incarceration, unemployment, homelessness, and neighborhood and family violence. We recruited the college student sample inside the university building via flyers and personal communication. Jointly, these samples capture a substantial range of variation in exposure to violence, from childhood experiences to present conditions.
We first conducted a pilot study in the United States with 46 participants (21 from a community sample [recruited from an employment development center in a large city] and 25 college students [from the same city]; age range 16–25), which served to refine our study design (e.g. instructions, exclusion criteria) and to obtain effect size estimates (for a recent critique of this approach, see Albers & Lakens, 2018).
We initially determined a sample size of 200 for this confirmatory study based on a power analysis via G × Power (Erdfielder, Faul, & Buchner, 1996) using a medium effect size (f = 0.1), 5% alpha‐level, 80% power, assuming a repeated‐measures ANOVA with a within‐between interaction (see preregistration link below). We later realized it would be better to use generalized linear mixed models. We thus switched to this analytic method before having looked at the data; therefore, this switch did not introduce bias.
We preregistered our sample size, hypotheses, and statistical analyses for the confirmatory study at the Open Science Framework: https://osf.io/c8fne/ (under Social Dominance Study). Our materials and data are accessible at: https://osf.io/3vbky/. Our study was approved by the Ethics Committee, Faculty of Social Sciences, Radboud University; CSW2014‐1310‐250.
2.1. Participants
Our goal was to test 200 participants: 100 students and 100 from the community sample. We tested 268 participants: 130 students and 138 community participants. We excluded 16 community participants who did not have the capacity to do the assessment (e.g. major drug use, head trauma, indicated not understanding the instructions), four who completed the dominance task but not the age task (no one completed the age task, but not the dominance task), and 17 who did not complete either task, leaving us with 101 community participants. After applying our other exclusion criteria, our final sample size comprised 199 participants: 100 students (60 females, age 18–61; M = 23.13, SD = 4.78) and 99 community participants (60 females, age 18–65; M = 40.14, SD = 11.96).
As sensitivity analyses, we also conducted our main analyses without removing the participants we over‐tested and without removing outliers (231 participants) and with all 3‐SD outliers removed (191 participants). We made all of the above decisions based on principled grounds, without knowing their implications for confirmatory testing. All participants received financial compensation.
2.2. Materials
2.2.1. Stimuli
In order to measure both memory and inference, we developed a novel version of the five‐item problem, which has been widely used in TI research with humans and non‐human animals (Grosenick et al., 2007; Nakamaru & Sasaki, 2003; Paz‐y‐Miño et al., 2004; Vasconcelos, 2008; Wynne, 1995). In the five‐item problem, individuals first learn a series of four overlapping pairs (A > B; B > C; C > D; D > E) and are then tested on a novel pair (B–D). We used five faces for the dominance task and five different faces for the age task; that is, a total of 10 faces. Dominance or age relations were represented using symbols: dominance by a fist, age by a ruler. The dominant or older face was always presented above the subordinate or younger face, with a symbol in between them. Figure 1 depicts a flow chart of the task structure.
The instructions for the memory phase were (original in Dutch): ‘During this task you will see two people. For each pair, we will tell you which person is more dominant [or older, in the age task]. The person placed higher on the screen is more dominant [older] than the person placed lower on the screen. Try to remember this information.’ The instructions for the inference phase stated: ‘You will now see a NEW PAIR of people. Please indicate which person is more dominant [older].’
We obtained male face stimuli and symbols through an online image search. In a pilot study, we had 14 students rate a much larger set of faces on the level of dominance for each face. We did not ask them to rate the perceived age of each face (in retrospect, it would have been good to do this). For our final stimulus set, we selected a subset of faces for which responses were mixed; that is, average ratings of these faces were in the moderate range. If people do not have strong priors about the level of dominance of a face, it will be easier to learn new information about relative dominance about this face. None of the pilot participants completed the actual study. The dimensions of the pictures were 150 (width) by 210 (height) pixels. We implemented the study in Inquisit, 2015 (4.0.8.0).
All participants completed one and the same dominance TI task and age TI task, with task order counterbalanced between participants. Whether the correct answer for the inference item on age or dominance was presented on the left or right (B–D or D–B) was also counterbalanced between participants. For all participants, the correct answer to the inference item was once presented on the left side of the screen (e.g. for dominance) and once on the right side (e.g. for age) in order to avoid simple response bias (e.g. always selecting the face on the left side) producing correct or incorrect answers to both items.
2.2.2. Neighborhood violence
We measured childhood (seven items; e.g. ‘In the neighborhood where I grew up, shootings or stabbings occurred’) and current (seven items; e.g. ‘In the neighborhood where I live, physical fights are common’) exposure to neighborhood violence as a bystander using the Neighborhood Violence Scale (see Frankenhuis, Roelofs, & de Vries, 2018, for the development of this scale). The subscales are identical except in referring to childhood, that is, up to 18 years (α = 0.84), or current experiences (α = 0.87). Participants rated items on each scale from 1 to 7 (completely agree‐disagree). We computed an average score for each participant for each subscale, with higher scores indicating greater neighborhood violence. For both samples, the distribution of scores on both subscales was positively skewed: most scores were low, fewer were medium, and very few were high (for details, see Results section 3.2.1 section).
2.2.3. Involvement in violence
We measured direct involvement in violence using a subset of four items (see preregistration) from the Youth Risk Behavior Survey (Eaton et al., 2012). One item asked about the frequency of childhood (adolescent) involvement (14–17 years) and a second item asked about the frequency of current involvement (in the last year) in a physical fight that required treatment for injuries. Participants rated these on a scale from 1 to 5 (0–6+ times). The other two items asked about the frequency of childhood and current involvement in a physical fight more generally, irrespective of injuries. Participants rated these on a scale from 1 to 8 (0–12+ times). These two response scales were identical to the ones used in the original Youth Risk Behavior Survey. To compute averages across scales with different numbers of response options (i.e. five or eight), we truncated 10 scores that were higher than five on the eight‐option items, assigning them a value of five (so, a score of 5 on both item types means 6+ times). Then we computed a childhood measure based on the average of the two childhood items and a current measure based on the average of the two current items.
2.2.4. Exploratory instruments
We measured four constructs for exploratory analyses (for details, see preregistration). We measured parent–child relationship quality (warmth and coercion) using an abbreviated version of the Parenting Questionnaire developed by Ellis, Schlomer, Tilley, and Butler (2012). This scale included 20 items (e.g. ‘My mother pushed, grabbed, or slapped me’), and asked about the first 16 years of life, as well as current experience. We measured childhood and current basic material needs (adequacy of resources to make ends meet) using a version of the Material Needs Scale developed by Conger and Ge (1994). This scale included eight items (e.g. ‘Your family had enough money to afford the kind of clothing you all needed’), and asked about the first 16 years of life, as well as current experience. We measured perceived life expectancy (the age until which a person expects to live), using a scale of eight items that we developed (e.g. ‘Do you think you will reach the age of 70’), in the majority of community participants and about half of the student sample (we added this scale later). We analyzed these four constructs using the same kind of statistical models that we used in our confirmatory analyses.
2.3. Procedure
The community sample completed the study in a room of the building of the community organization from which they were recruited; students completed the study in a test‐cubicle at the university. All participants completed the questionnaires and tasks by themselves, in Dutch, and were invited to ask clarification questions to the experimenter. The community sample was tested on a 17‐inch laptop; students on a 24‐inch desktop. Stimuli were shown in the midpoint of the screen.
In the learning phase, four pairs (A > B; B > C; C > D; D > E) appeared, sequentially, on a computer screen. After each pair was shown once, for a fixed window of 5 s, participants recalled from memory, also sequentially, for each face pair, which individual is dominant or older. This learning‐memory phase repeated until participants reached the predefined threshold of correctly recalling the entire sequence twice in a row, evidencing memorization; thus, perfect performance resulted in a memory score of two rounds. The probability of reaching this threshold by chance—that is, guessing correctly eight times in a row—is very low. The number of sequences participants needed to reach this threshold indicated memory accuracy. Having reached the memory threshold, the inference phase would start. In this phase, participants answered who is dominant or older in the novel B–D pair, revealing inference accuracy (0 = incorrect; 1 = correct). After the response to a memory or inference question, the next stimulus would appear without delay. Response windows for answering the memory or inference questions were indefinite. We did not design the study to examine reaction times; hence we do not report these.
3. RESULTS
3.1. Analytic approach
We calculated generalized linear mixed models (GLMMs) with the number of memory rounds serving as the dependent variable in the first set of analyses and inference performance (correct or incorrect) serving as the dependent variable in the second set. In all analyses, (a) the target adversity measure was entered as a between‐subjects variable and (b) information type (age vs. dominance) was entered as a within‐subjects factor. We evaluated main effects and within‐between interaction effects (to allow us to formally test our main hypothesis: that the effects of adversity on the cognitive outcome variables differ as a function of information type). For memory performance, we also included task order (completed first or second) as a within‐subjects factor (as explained below). We operationalized adversity in terms of sample (community vs. student) in the group‐level analyses and in terms of exposure to or involvement in violence in the individual‐level analyses. Each model included a per‐participant intercept and no random slopes. For example, we analyzed memory using the following kinds of models:
where the symbol * denotes both main effects and interactions. We used R to compute GLMMs with a Poisson link function to analyze memory performance (Version 3.5.0; R Development Core Team, 2018). This function matched the positively skewed distribution of the memory variables; that is, integers with many low scores and few high scores. We obtained p‐values based on parametric bootstrapping with type 3 sums of squares (for explanation see Luke, 2017) using the mixed () function from the afex package (Singmann, Bolker, Westfall, & Aust, 2018). For each effect, this analysis compares the full model with a model that restricts that effect to zero using a χ2 distribution. As inference performance was binary (correct, incorrect), we used GLMMs with a binomial link function. For example, we analyzed inference using the following kinds of models:
3.1.1. Error control
We evaluated two hypotheses: one about memory, and one about inference. For each, we planned to conduct five tests: one comparing the community and the student sample, and four for the continuous predictors. To control for multiple testing, we applied the sequential Holm–Bonferroni correction (Holm, 1979). We applied this correction per hypothesis, as recommended by Lakens (2016), for these five tests. We list the adjusted alpha with every p‐value lower than 0.05. We calculated simple slopes only for interactions with a p‐value lower than 0.05.
We also applied Holm–Bonferroni corrections to our four exploratory analyses, and again separately for each variable (parent–child relationship quality; childhood and current basic material needs; perceived life expectancy). For these analyses, we report only the effects that were significant after applying the correction.
3.2. Statistical analyses
3.2.1. Descriptive statistics
We provide only descriptive, not inferential, statistics for the United States pilot study (Table 1). Although the two samples were about the same age in this pilot study, the community participants scored higher across all measures of violence exposure. The descriptive statistics further suggest that the memory and inference performance of the community participants, more than that of students, was facilitated by dominance (vs. age) content. Students generally performed better than community participants on inference and on memory for age, but not on memory for dominance. Although it appears that the community participants displayed better memory performance (Table 1), two of the three community participants that we dropped (because they did not complete both tasks) needed many rounds to complete one task. Including them would have resulted in a higher mean and larger range for this group.
Table 1.
Measures | United States sample | |
---|---|---|
Community (n = 18) | Students (n = 22) | |
Mean (range) | Mean (range) | |
Childhood neighborhood violence | 3.94 (1.00–7.00) | 1.77 (1.00–4.71) |
Current neighborhood violence | 3.36 (1.00–5.86) | 2.81 (1.29–6.00) |
Childhood involvement in violence | 1.78 (1.00–3.50) | 1.09 (1.00–2.00) |
Current involvement in violence | 1.53 (1.00–3.00) | 1.05 (1.00–1.50) |
Memory dominancea | 3.22 (2–6) | 3.64 (2–13) |
Memory agea | 3.33 (2–6) | 3.64 (2–9) |
Inference dominanceb | 0.78 | 0.82 |
Inference ageb | 0.44 | 0.59 |
Number of memory rounds;
correct = 1, incorrect = 0, with the mean score indicating the percentage of participants (across the full sample) who answered correctly.
In the confirmatory Dutch study, the community participants also scored higher than student participants on all measures of violence exposure (Table 2). We compared the two samples using Welch's t test (p‐values) and Bayesian Independent Samples T‐Tests (Bayes Factors; BF). We computed BFs using JASP software (2018). BFs quantify the likelihood of the data conditional on Model 1 (representing H1), divided by the likelihood of the data conditional on Model 0 (representing H0). For three out of four exposure variables, BFs indicate that the data is much more likely under H1 than H0.
Table 2.
Measures | Dutch sample | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Community | Students | Tests | ||
Mean (range) | Mean (range) | T | 1‐sided BF10 | |
Childhood neighborhood violence | 3.74 (1.14–6.14) | 2.64 (1.00–5.71) | 7.11*** | 730,300,000 |
Current neighborhood violence | 2.92 (1.14–6.43) | 2.18 (1.14–6.00) | 4.78*** | 9,070 |
Childhood involvement in violence | 1.63 (1.00–4.00) | 1.25 (1.00–3.00) | 4.01*** | 490 |
Current involvement in violence | 1.16 (1.00–3.00) | 1.06 (1.00–2.00) | 2.03* | 2.08 |
Memory dominancea | 5.34 (2–25) | 3.22 (2–16) | n.a. | n.a. |
Memory agea | 5.88 (2–23) | 3.13 (2–16) | n.a. | n.a. |
Inference dominancea | 0.59 | 0.82 | n.a. | n.a. |
Inference ageb | 0.59 | 0.77 | n.a. | n.a. |
Abbreviations: BF, Bayes Factors; n.a., not applicable.
Number of memory rounds;
correct = 1, incorrect = 0, with the mean score indicating the percentage of participants (across the full sample) who answered correctly.
p < 0.05,
p < 0.001.
3.3. Memory
3.3.1. Order effect
A GLMM with memory rounds as the dependent variable, and information type (age or dominance) and task order (first or second task) as within‐subject variables, revealed a significant main effect of task order (χ 2(1) = 58.93, p = 0.001). Participants needed fewer rounds to memorize the pairs in the second task than in the first task. Therefore, we controlled for order (task number) in all analyses of memory performance by including it as a main effect. We also included the interaction between the adversity measures and order because in the case of an impairment effect, those with higher adversity scores would have had more room to improve from one task to the next than those with lower adversity scores. There was no main effect of information type (χ 2(1) = 0.79, p = 0.38), and no interaction between information type and task number (χ 2(1) = 0.47, p = 0.51).
3.3.2. Main analyses
The main effect of task order on memory performance was significant in all of the models (all χ 2(1) 28.69–60.10, all p = 0.001). There was no significant main effect of information type in any of the models (χ 2(1) = 0.008–1.86, p = 0.17–0.94). Students memorized both dominance and age relations better than community participants (Table 3). At an individual level, more exposure to violence (across all four indicators) was associated with lower overall memory performance. Contrary to our predictions, the main effects of childhood exposure to violence on memory were not qualified by significant interaction effects. However, consistent with our predictions, the main effects of current exposure to violence on memory were qualified by significant interaction effects.
Table 3.
Between‐subject independent variable | Main effect | Interaction with information type | Interaction with order | Figure |
---|---|---|---|---|
Sample: community or student |
χ 2(1) = 57.61 p = 0.001 αHolm = 0.0167 |
χ 2(1) = 0.58 p = 0.42 |
χ 2(1) = 25.44 p = 0.001 αHolm = 0.0125 |
See Table 2 |
Childhood neighborhood violence |
χ 2(1) = 22.46 p = 0.001 αHolm = 0.0125 |
χ 2(1) = 0.59 p = 0.45 |
χ 2(1) = 11.66 p = 0.002 αHolm = 0.0167 |
Figure 2a |
Current neighborhood violence |
χ 2(1) = 8.30 p = 0.003 αHolm = 0.025 |
χ 2(1) = 18.20 p = 0.002 αHolm = 0.01 |
χ 2(1) = 4.03 p = 0.044 αHolm = 0.05 |
Figure 2b |
Childhood involvement in violence |
χ 2(1) = 18.34 p = 0.001 αHolm = 0.01 |
χ 2(1) = 0.19 p = 0.65 |
χ 2(1) = 14.87 p = 0.001 αHolm = 0.01 |
Figure 2c |
Current involvement in violence |
χ 2(1) = 6.90 p = 0.007 αHolm = 0.05 |
χ 2(1) = 7.56 p = 0.012 αHolm = 0.0125 |
χ 2(1) = 6.10 p = 0.016 αHolm = 0.025 |
Figure 2d |
Simple slopes (Figure 2) revealed that current neighborhood violence positively correlated with the number of rounds needed to memorize age relations (τ = 0.217, p < 0.001, BF 10 = 2,852), but not with dominance relations (τ = 0.020, p = 0.71, BF 01 = 9.94). Current involvement in violence was negatively correlated with the number of rounds needed to memorize dominance relations (τ = −0.12, p = 0.048, BF 10 = 2.69), but not with age relations (τ = 0.06, p = 0.37, BF 01 = 5.49).
3.4. Inference
3.4.1. Order effect
A GLMM with inference performance (correct or incorrect) as the dependent variable, and information type (age or dominance) and task order (first or second task) as within‐subject variables, revealed no significant main effects of task order (χ 2(1) = 0.18, p = 0.66) or information type (χ 2(1) = 0.45, p = 0.51), and no interaction between information type and task order (χ 2(1) = 0.33, p = 0.57). Therefore, we did not control for order in analyses of inference.
3.4.2. Main analyses
There was no significant main effect of information type in any of the models (χ 2(1) = 0.35 – 0.50, p = 0.47 – 0.54). Students inferred both dominance and age relations better than community participants (Table 4). At an individual level, across all four indicators, there were no statistically significant associations between exposure to violence and overall reasoning performance. Contrary to our predictions, the main effects of childhood and current exposure to violence on inference were not qualified by significant interaction effects.
Table 4.
Between‐subject independent variable | Main effect | Interaction with information type |
---|---|---|
Sample: community or student |
χ 2(1) = 18.55 p = 0.001 αHolm = 0.01 |
χ 2(1) = 0.50 p = 0.48 |
Childhood neighborhood violence |
χ 2(1) = 0.52 p = 0.50 |
χ 2(1) = 0.03 p = 0.86 |
Current neighborhood violence |
χ 2(1) = 0.00 p = 0.97 |
χ 2(1) = 0.15 p = 0.70 |
Childhood involvement in violence |
χ 2(1) = 2.46 p = 0.13 |
χ 2(1) = 1.46 p = 0.22 |
Current involvement in violence |
χ 2(1) = 0.24 p = 0.64 |
χ 2(1) = 0.25 p = 0.62 |
3.5. Sensitivity analyses
Participant age was positively associated with the number of rounds needed to memorize age (τ = 0.21, p < 0.001, BF 10 = 27,076) and dominance relations (τ = 0.24, p < 0.001, BF 10 = 908). Including the main effect of age in the analyses on memory performance did not change qualitative results from the main analyses. Welch's t tests showed no significant gender difference on age memory (t = −1.01, p = 0.31) or dominance memory (t = −0.28, p = 0.78).
3.5.1. More liberal inclusion criteria (231 participants)
Results did not differ qualitatively from our main analyses.
3.5.2. More conservative inclusion criteria (191 participants)
Results did not differ qualitatively from our main analyses in most cases. However, there was no significant main effect of current involvement in violence on memory rounds (χ 2(1) = 1.70, p = 0.20). Additionally, the interaction between sample and information type on memory, though still non‐significant, resulted in a lower p‐value (χ 2(1) = 3.60, p = 0.060).
3.5.3. Correcting for sample
We did not include sample in our preregistered analyses of the continuous predictors, because we expected substantial overlap between variation in sample and these predictors. Including the main effect of sample in the questionnaire analyses did not change most results. Notably, theoretically relevant interaction effects remained significant. However, the main effects of current neighborhood violence (χ 2(1) = 4.00, p = 0.052) and current involvement in violence (χ 2(1) = 5.41, p = 0.0251, αHolm = 0.025) became non‐significant.
3.6. Auxiliary hypotheses
There was a significant main effect of childhood basic financial needs on overall memory performance (χ 2(1) = 9.08, p = 0.006, αHolm = 0.0167). Those whose families were better able to pay for their basic financial needs needed fewer rounds to memorize the relations. The same was true for current basic financial needs (χ 2(1) = 24.15, p = 0.001, αHolm = 0.0125). None of the exploratory questionnaires had significant main or interaction effects on inference performance.
3.7. Exploring the cognitive measures
A GLM with a Poisson link function predicting one type of memory performance from the other and controlling for task order showed either a non‐significant or a significant relation, depending on whether age (Estimate = 0.019, SE = 0.009, p = 0.039) or dominance (Estimate = 0.016, SE = 0.008, p = 0.053) was the predictor.
Participants who correctly answered the age inference question were more likely to correctly answer the dominance inference question (χ 2(1) = 10.02, p = 0.002, BF 10 = 38.33).
Logistic regressions indicated that the number of rounds needed to memorize dominance relations was not associated with dominance inference performance (Estimate = −0.05, p = 0.20). However, the number of rounds needed to memorize age relations was significantly associated with age inference performance (Estimate = −0.09, p = 0.03).
4. DISCUSSION
Students memorized and inferred both dominance and age relations better than community participants. Further, more childhood and current neighborhood violence, and involvement in violence, were associated with lower memory performance, but not with reasoning performance. However, the main effects of current neighborhood violence and involvement in violence on memory performance were qualified by significant interaction effects. Specifically, more current neighborhood violence was associated with worse memory for age relations, but not with memory for dominance relations. More current involvement in violence was associated with better memory for dominance relations, but not with memory for age relations. This pattern of equal or enhanced performance despite greater exposure to adversity is striking when considering the developmental literature, which has nearly exclusively revealed deficits in the social and cognitive abilities of people who grow up in harsh conditions (but see Ellis et al., 2017; Frankenhuis & de Weerth, 2013).
Our main effects are consistent with the deficit model. However, our interaction effects on current exposure to violence are consistent with the specialization hypothesis. Only the specialization hypothesis predicts that people from harsh environments show equal or even enhanced performance on tasks matching recurrent problems in those environments (Ellis et al., 2017; Frankenhuis & de Weerth, 2013). We speculate that these interactions reflect a blend of impairment and specialization processes operating in concert, where impairment generally lowers performance, but specialization enhances it, specifically for relevant contents, such as social dominance. Notably, we find interaction effects only for current experiences, not for childhood experiences; there, we find only impairment. This difference suggests that, in our study, intact or enhanced performance despite greater adversity is more likely to result from a dynamic adjustment of cognition in response to current contextual factors than from gradual tailoring of cognitive abilities over the course of childhood.
4.1. Implications for training and transfer
An open and interesting question is what mechanisms explain performance in our study. We speculate that the relevance of social dominance content increased the level of interest, hence the attention and motivation, more so for participants who currently live in hostile conditions. Emotion research shows that military veterans reasoned better about combat‐related syllogisms than formally identical syllogisms with neutral contents (Blanchette & Caparos, 2013). This advantage could not be attributed to variation in expertise about combat. The authors theorize: ‘When the affective reaction is relevant to the semantic contents reasoned about, emotions may have a positive impact on reasoning and this effect may be mediated by utility’ (p. 412).
Although we do not find enhancement of reasoning, social dominance content may elicit stronger emotions in people who are exposed to hostile conditions (given the greater relevance and higher utility of this content), thus enhancing their memory performance. Such a process would be consistent with studies showing enhanced performance in individuals from unpredictable environments only under conditions of primed economic hardship or uncertainty (Dang et al., 2016; Mittal et al., 2015; Young et al., 2018). In addition to enhancing attention and motivation, research on ‘everyday’ (or ‘street’) mathematics shows that people also employ different cognitive strategies in‐context when solving problems with ecologically relevant content, compared with more abstract problems in formal school settings (Banerjee et al., 2017; Schliemann & Carraher, 2002). Future research could examine whether participants who are exposed to high levels of violence use different strategies when solving memory and inference problems involving social dominance versus age content. As noted above, understanding what contents and formats improve cognitive abilities in individuals who have been exposed to significant social and economic adversity provides a potential lever for enhancing their educational outcomes (Ellis et al., 2017). Our findings suggest that people who are currently exposed to or involved in violence learn differences between young men in dominance position more readily than differences between young men in age. This finding is important because it suggests that individuals living under harsh conditions may be able to hold their own, or even excel, when solving problems in which the content is adaptively relevant to their lives. The next critical research question is the transfer of such abilities across contexts and contents. For instance, we could first ask students to memorize relations that involve social dominance content, then less‐relevant yet social content (e.g. age), then non‐social content (e.g. objects), and finally abstract content (e.g. symbols). Similarly, the format of tasks can be gradually modified (e.g. from pictures, to words, to formal symbols). The importance of ecologically valid content and testing conditions has been demonstrated in past research on the mathematical ability of children from high‐adversity backgrounds (Banerjee et al., 2017; Schliemann & Carraher, 2002; Sternberg, Lipka, Newman, Wildfeuer, & Grigorenko, 2006).
Such an approach could not only benefit students who struggle in traditional school settings, but also gifted students who excel relative to their peers. A recent review of effective curriculum interventions concludes that gifted students from low‐income backgrounds have ‘a pragmatic outlook that encourages their preference for concreteness in learning, for practical applications of knowledge in their world and for examples that both come from and harken back to their world’ (VanTassel‐Baska, 2018, p. 69). Using students’ stress‐adapted skills as a starting point to gain mastery in a given domain, and then transferring these capacities by guiding them towards other kinds of contents and formats, could be a promising avenue for improving academic outcomes.
More generally, we need to know how to optimize educational settings for different populations, especially for marginalized groups, and for different age groups (Ellis et al., 2017; Markant, Ackerman, Nussenbaum, & Amso, 2016). We can examine variations in curricular content (e.g. abstract vs. social), format (e.g. pictures vs. symbols), information delivery (e.g. static books vs. dynamic touch screens), and instructional practices (e.g. sitting vs. moving around in the classroom). Such an approach fits with work in other populations that struggle in conventional school settings, such as children with attention‐deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Recent studies show that the performance of these children is enhanced, more than that of controls, if they are allowed to learn while moving around (Hartanto, Krafft, Iosif, & Schweitzer, 2016; Sarver, Rapport, Kofler, Raiker, & Friedman, 2015). What is critically needed is more research that delineates the contexts that maximize performance of stress‐adapted children and youth (Ellis et al., 2017; Frankenhuis & de Weerth, 2013; Goudeau & Croizet, 2017; Richardson, Castellano, Stone, & Sanning, 2016). In the words of Barbara Rogoff et al.: ‘A challenge for future research is looking for strengths in all populations and designing learning situations and assessments in ways that build on and build toward the strengths of all’ (2017, p. 885). We thus view stress‐adapted memory for ecologically relevant information, such as social dominance, as a starting point for further learning. Working in concert with these abilities can help to improve learning outcomes and hence reduce educational inequalities and its sequelae for socioeconomic position and health. But before we design interventions, we need solid scientific foundations.
4.2. Limitations
Our preregistered study evaluates a diverse group of participants; however, it also has limitations. First, the test setting differed, inevitably, between our student and community sample. Second, testing community participants in a computerized setting, rather than in a more hands‐on, real‐world, practical setting, could have lowered their performance (Ellis et al., 2017). Third, we obtained only two memory and inference scores per participant; more measurements would have allowed for more precise estimates of ability. This point applies more so to our dichotomous measure of inference than to our continuous measure of memory. Fourth, we measured involvement in violence using a scale that captures only serious involvement in violence, resulting in many low scores. Future work should include a scale that is more sensitive to individual differences in less serious involvement in violence, such as verbal threats (e.g. a version of the Aggression Scale; Orpinas & Frankowski, 2001). Fifth, the correlation between age and dominance memory performance was lower than we expected. This could reflect a measurement issue or genuine differences in the ability to solve our task depending on its content. Richardson (1991) found similarly low correlations between socially more or less meaningful versions of the Raven Progressive Matrices task. Sixth, our research design was correlational. Therefore, we are not able to rule out the possibility that genetic variation has contributed to the empirical relations we have documented. Despite these limitations, we consider our study more breakthrough than incremental, because it is one of the first studies to systematically investigate, and offer tentative evidence for, enhanced skills and abilities in people who are exposed to higher levels of adversity.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
We thank Jonathan Butner, Bill Burke, and Jesse Fenneman for statistical advice, and Desi Beckers, Daan van Rijswijk, two anonymous reviewers, and the editor for their thoughtful and constructive comments on previous versions of this manuscript.
Frankenhuis WE, de Vries SA, Bianchi J, Ellis BJ. Hidden talents in harsh conditions? A preregistered study of memory and reasoning about social dominance. Dev Sci. 2020;23:e12835 10.1111/desc.12835
Funding information
This research was supported by grants from the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (016.155.195), the James S. McDonnell Foundation (220020502), and the Jacobs Foundation (2017 1261 02) to WEF; by a grant from the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation (73657) to BJE and WEF; and by grants from the Sorenson Legacy Foundation and the Consortium for Families and Health Research at the University of Utah to BJE.
Data Availability Statement: The materials and data of this work are accessible at: https://osf.io/3vbky/.
ENDNOTES
We removed three community participants and three students who did not complete both tasks. The final pilot sample thus included 40 participants: 18 community participants and 22 students. The pilot study made us aware that we would need to over‐test in order to achieve the sample size we needed for our confirmatory study.
As only four community participants completed one task and not the other, we did not conduct an MCAR test in order to examine whether these participants were missing at random. All students completed both tasks.
We checked outliers for memory performance on both tasks. We removed two 3‐SD outliers in the student sample. Next, we removed the 28 students who we tested most recently, based on their date and time of participation (without having seen their data), in order to arrive at the preregistered sample size (100). There were 10 3‐SD outliers in the community sample. Removing them would leave 91 in this sample, which is lower than our preregistered sample size (so we did not). We noticed two extreme outliers in the community sample (±7.5 SDs; the next outliers were ±4 SDs), which we removed.
For both samples, the distribution of scores on both subscales is positively skewed. Therefore, we used a non‐parametric statistic, Kendall's tau (rather than Cronbach's alpha), to compute correlations between the two childhood items (τ = 0.36, p < 0.001) and the two current items (τ = 0.28, p < 0.001). These coefficients are moderate to low, and perhaps lower than is desirable. However, components of composites need not be highly correlated. For instance, it is common practice to compute an overall adversity index for individuals by ‘summing’ over their adverse childhood experiences, which may or may not be correlated with each other; for instance, depending on how rare certain adverse experiences are (e.g. fights resulting in hospitalization).
For each set of five tests, we ordered the p‐values from smallest to largest. For the smallest p‐value, we used an alpha level of 0.05 divided by the number of tests (0.05/5 = 0.01). For the second smallest p‐value, we used an alpha level of 0.05 divided by the number of tests minus one (0.05/4 = 0.0125), and so on. If one p‐value was larger than the adjusted alpha (i.e. non‐significant), all subsequent p‐values were also non‐significant.
Based on a reviewer's request, we explored gender differences in the relation between exposure to violence and performance on memory and inference, separately for age and dominance (we had no power to explore a three‐way interaction). As after correcting for multiple comparisons per dependent variable (i.e. for four tests at a time), only one of these eight interaction effects was significant. We interpret these results as indicating no support for gender differences in the slopes of exposure to violence on performance.
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
The materials and data of this work are accessible at: https://osf.io/3vbky/.
REFERENCES
- Albers, C. , & Lakens, D. (2018). When power analyses based on pilot data are biased: Inaccurate effect size estimators and follow‐up bias. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 74, 187–195. 10.1016/j.jesp.2017.09.004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ambady, N. , Bernieri, F. J. , & Richeson, J. A. (2000). Toward a histology of social behavior: Judgmental accuracy from thin slices of the behavioral stream. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 32, 201–271. 10.1016/S0065-2601(00)80006-4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- American Psychological Association , Task Force on Socioeconomic Status . (2007). Report of the APA Task Force on socioeconomic status. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. [Google Scholar]
- Banerjee, A. V. , Bhattacharjee, S. , Chattopadhyay, R. , & Ganimian, A. J. (2017). The Untapped math skills of working children in India: Evidence, possible explanations, and implications. Retrieved fromhttps://static1.squarespace.com/static/5990cfd52994ca797742fae9/t/59a896aee6f2e11b76983238/1504220847338/Banerjee+et+al.+2017+-+2017-08-17.pdf .
- Barnett, S. M. , & Ceci, S. J. (2002). When and where do we apply what we learn? A taxonomy for far transfer. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 612–637. 10.1037/0033-2909.128.4.612 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blanchette, I. , & Caparos, S. (2013). When emotions improve reasoning: The possible roles of relevance and utility. Thinking & Reasoning, 19, 399–413. 10.1080/l3546783.2013.791642 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bryant, P. E. , & Trabasso, T. (1971). Transitive inferences and memory in young children. Nature, 232, 456–458. 10.1038/232456a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burt, C. (1919). The development of reasoning in school children. Journal of Experimental Pedagogy, 5, 68–77. [Google Scholar]
- Chalmers, M. , & McGonigle, B. (1984). Are children any more logical than monkeys on the five term series problem? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 37, 355–377. 10.1016/0022-0965(84)90009-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conger, R. D. , Ge, X. , Elder, G. H. , Lorenz, F. O. , & Simons, R. L. (1994). Economic stress, coercive family process, and developmental problems of adolescents. Child Development, 65, 541–561. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1994.tb00768.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dang, J. , Xiao, S. , Zhang, T. , Liu, Y. , Jiang, B. , & Mao, L. (2016). When the poor excel: Poverty facilitates procedural learning. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 57, 288–291. 10.1111/sjop.12292 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Soto, C. B. (1960). Learning a social structure. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 60, 417–421. 10.1037/h0047511 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duncan, G. J. , Magnuson, K. , & Votruba‐Drzal, E. (2017). Moving beyond correlations in assessing the consequences of poverty. Annual Review of Psychology, 68, 413–434. 10.1146/annurev-psych-010416-044224 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eaton, D. K. , Kann, L. , Kinchen, S. , Shanklin, S. , Flint, K. H. , Hawkins, J. , Lim, C. , et al. (2012). Youth risk behavior surveillance – United States, 2011. MMWR Surveillance Summary, 61, 1–162. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ellis, B. J. , Bianchi, J. , Griskevicius, V. , & Frankenhuis, W. E. (2017). Beyond risk and protective factors: An adaptation‐based approach to resilience. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12, 561–587. 10.1177/1745691617693054 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ellis, B. J. , Schlomer, G. L. , Tilley, E. H. , & Butler, E. A. (2012). Impact of fathers on risky sexual behavior in daughters: A genetically and environmentally controlled sibling study. Development and Psychopathology, 24, 317–332. 10.1017/S095457941100085X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Erdfielder, E. , Faul, F. , & Buchner, A. (1996). GPOWER: A general power analysis program. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 28, 1–11. 10.3758/BF03203630 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Fiske, A. P. (1992). The four elementary forms of sociality: Framework for a unified theory of social relations. Psychological Review, 99, 689–723. 10.1037/0033-295X.99.4.689 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frankenhuis, W. E. , & de Weerth, C. (2013). Does early‐life exposure to stress shape or impair cognition? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 407–412. 10.1177/0963721413484324 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Frankenhuis, W. E. , Panchanathan, K. , & Nettle, D. (2016). Cognition in harsh and unpredictable environments. Current Opinion in Psychology, 7, 76–80. 10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.08.011 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Frankenhuis, W. E. , Roelofs, M. , & de Vries, S. A. (2018). Does exposure to social adversity enhance deception detection ability? Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 12, 218–229. 10.1037/ebs0000103 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Gazes, R. P. , Hampton, R. R. , & Lourenco, S. F. (2015). Transitive inference of social dominance by human infants. Developmental Science, 20, e12367 10.1111/desc.12367 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goudeau, S. , & Croizet, J. C. (2017). Hidden advantages and disadvantages of social class: How classroom settings reproduce social inequality by staging unfair comparison. Psychological Science, 28, 162–170. 10.1177/0956797616676600 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greenfield, P. M. (2014). Cross‐cultural roots of minority child development. New York, NY: Psychology Press; 10.4324/9781315746555 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Grosenick, L. , Clement, T. S. , & Fernald, R. D. (2007). Fish can infer social rank by observation alone. Nature, 445, 429–432. 10.1038/nature05511 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grossmann, I. , & Varnum, M. E. (2011). Social class, culture, and cognition. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2, 81–89. 10.1177/1948550610377119 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hartanto, T. A. , Krafft, C. E. , Iosif, A. M. , & Schweitzer, J. B. (2016). A trial‐by‐trial analysis reveals more intense physical activity is associated with better cognitive control performance in attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Child Neuropsychology, 22, 618–626. 10.1080/09297049.2015.1044511 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hawley, P. H. (1999). The ontogenesis of social dominance: A strategy‐based evolutionary perspective. Developmental Review, 19, 97–132. 10.1006/drev.1998.0470 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Holm, S. (1979). A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scandinavian Journal of Statistics, 6, 65–70. www.jstor.org/stable/4615733 [Google Scholar]
- Hsu, Y. , Earley, R. L. , & Wolf, L. L. (2006). Modulation of aggressive behaviour by fighting experience: Mechanisms and contest outcomes. Biological Reviews, 81, 33–74. 10.1017/S146479310500686X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Inquisit 4 . (2015). Inquisit 4 [Computer software]. Retrieved from http://www.millisecond.com
- JASP Team . (2018). JASP (Version 0.9.1) [Computer software]. Retrieved from https://jasp-stats.org/
- Keiser, R. L. (1979). The vice lords: Warriors of the streets. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. [Google Scholar]
- Kraus, M. W. , Piff, P. K. , Mendoza‐Denton, R. , Rheinschmidt, M. L. , & Keltner, D. (2012). Social class, solipsism, and contextualism: How the rich are different from the poor. Psychological Review, 119, 546–572. 10.1037/a0028756 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lakens, D. (2016). Why you don’t need to adjust your alpha level for all tests you’ll do in your lifetime. The 20% Statistician. Retrieved from http://daniellakens.blogspot.ch/2016/02/why-you-dont-need-toadjust-you-alpha.html
- Luke, S. G. (2017). Evaluating significance in linear mixed‐effects models in R. Behavior Research Methods, 49, 1494–1502. 10.3758/s13428-016-0809-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Markant, J. , Ackerman, L. K. , Nussenbaum, K. , & Amso, D. (2016). Selective attention neutralizes the adverse effects of low socioeconomic status on memory in 9‐month‐old infants. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 26–33. 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.10.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Markovits, H. , & Dumas, C. (1999). Developmental patterns in the understanding of social and physical transitivity. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 73, 95–114. 10.1006/jecp.1999.2496 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mittal, C. , Griskevicius, V. , Simpson, J. A. , Sung, S. , & Young, E. S. (2015). Cognitive adaptations to stressful environments: When childhood adversity enhances adult executive function. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109, 604–621. 10.1037/pspi0000028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moreau, D. , Macnamara, B. N. , & Hambrick, D. Z. (2018). Overstating the role of environmental factors in success: A cautionary note. Current Directions in Psychological Science. Online first publication. 0963721418797300.
- Nakamaru, M. , & Sasaki, A. (2003). Can transitive inference evolve in animals playing the hawk–dove game? Journal of Theoretical Biology, 222, 461–470. 10.1016/S0022-5193(03)00059-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nelson, C. A. , Fox, N. A. , & Zeanah, C. H. (2014). Romania’s abandoned children. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Orpinas, P. , & Frankowski, R. (2001). The aggression scale: A self‐report measure of aggressive behavior for young adolescents. Journal of Early Adolescence, 21, 50–67. 10.1177/0272431601021001003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Paz‐y‐Miño, C. G. , Bond, A. B. , Kamil, A. C. , & Balda, R. P. (2004). Pinyon jays use transitive inference to predict social dominance. Nature, 430, 778–781. 10.1038/nature02723 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Penn, D. C. , Holyoak, K. J. , & Povinelli, D. J. (2008). Darwin's mistake: Explaining the discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 109–129. 10.1017/S0140525X08003543P [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Piaget, J. (1955). The language and thought of the child. New York: Meridian Books. [Google Scholar]
- Pope, S. M. , Fagot, J. , Meguerditchian, A. , Washburn, D. A. , & Hopkins, W. D. (2019). Enhanced cognitive flexibility in the semi‐nomadic Himba. Journal of Cross‐Cultural Psychology, 50, 47–62. 10.1177/0022022118806581 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- R Development Core Team . (2018). R: A language and environment for statistical computing (Version 3.5.0) [Computer software]. Retrieved from https://www.r-project.org/index.html
- Richardson, G. B. , Castellano, M. E. , Stone, J. R. , & Sanning, B. K. (2016). Ecological and evolutionary principles for secondary education: Analyzing career and tech ed. Evolutionary Psychological Science, 2, 58–69. 10.1007/s40806-015-0034-4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Richardson, K. (1991). Reasoning with Raven—in and out of context. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 61, 129–138. 10.1111/j.2044-8279.1991.tb00969.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Rogoff, B. , Coppens, A. D. , Alcalá, L. , Aceves‐Azuara, I. , Ruvalcaba, O. , López, A. , & Dayton, A. (2017). Noticing learners’ strengths through cultural research. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12, 876–888. 10.1177/1745691617718355 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sarver, D. E. , Rapport, M. D. , Kofler, M. J. , Raiker, J. S. , & Friedman, L. M. (2015). Hyperactivity in attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Impairing deficit or compensatory behavior? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 43, 1219–1232. 10.1007/s10802-015-0011-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schliemann, A. D. , & Carraher, D. W. (2002). The evolution of mathematical reasoning: Everyday versus idealized understandings. Developmental Review, 22, 242–266. 10.1006/drev.2002.0547 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Shakur, S. (2007). Monster: The autobiography of an LA gang member. New York, NY: Grove Press. [Google Scholar]
- Singmann, H. , Bolker, B. , Westfall, J. , & Aust, F. (2018). afex: Analysis of Factorial Experiments. R package version 0.20‐2. Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=afex
- Sternberg, R. J. (1983). Components of human intelligence. Cognition, 15, 1–48. 10.1016/0010-0277(83)90032-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sternberg, R. J. (2014). The development of adaptive competence: Why cultural psychology is necessary and not just nice. Developmental Review, 34, 208–224. 10.1016/j.dr.2014.05.004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Sternberg, R. J. , Lipka, J. , Newman, T. , Wildfeuer, S. , & Grigorenko, E. L. (2006). Triarchically‐based instruction and assessment of sixth‐grade mathematics in a Yup’ik cultural setting in Alaska. Gifted and Talented International, 21, 9–19. 10.1080/15332276.2006.11673471 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- VanTassel‐Baska, J. (2018). Achievement unlocked: Effective curriculum interventions with low‐income students. Gifted Child Quarterly, 62, 68–82. 10.1177/0016986217738565 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Varnum, M. E. , & Kitayama, S. (2017). The neuroscience of social class. Current Opinion in Psychology, 18, 147–151. 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.07.032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vasconcelos, M. (2008). Transitive inference in non‐human animals: An empirical and theoretical analysis. Behavioral Processes, 78, 313–334. 10.1016/j.beproc.2008.02.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Venkatesh, S. (2008). Gang leader for a day: A rogue sociologist takes to the streets. New York, NY: The Penguin Press. [Google Scholar]
- Wright, B. C. (2012). The case for a dual‐process theory of transitive reasoning. Developmental Review, 32, 89–124. 10.1016/j.dr.2012.04.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Wynne, C. D. L. (1995). Reinforcement accounts for transitive inference performance. Animal Learning & Behavior, 23, 207–217. 10.3758/BF03199936 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Young, E. S. , Griskevicius, V. , Simpson, J. A. , Waters, T. E. , & Mittal, C. (2018). Can an unpredictable childhood environment enhance working memory? Testing the sensitized‐specialization hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 114, 891–908. 10.1037/pspi0000124 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zitek, E. M. , & Tiedens, L. Z. (2012). The fluency of social hierarchy: The ease with which hierarchical relationships are seen, remembered, learned and liked. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 98–115. 10.1037/a0025345 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The materials and data of this work are accessible at: https://osf.io/3vbky/.