Abstract
Cancer-related cognitive impairment (CRCI) has not been objectively assessed in chronic lymphocytic leukemia (CLL). It is currently unclear how much of CRCI is attributable to disease, treatment, or both. We used CLL as a novel model to study the differential roles of disease and treatment in CRCI. 150 CLL patients (100 treatment-naïve and 50 chemotherapy-treated) including 84 patients with higher-risk of CLL progression completed objective neuropsychological tests. Sociodemographic-adjusted linear regression models examined cognitive outcomes in relation to risk and treatment. Higher-risk patients recalled 2 fewer words on a memory task (β=−1.8, 95%CI-3.3,−0.3) and took 15 seconds longer on an executive function task (β=15.4, 95%CI 3.1,27.6) than lower-risk patients, independent of treatment. Treated patients reported greater cognitive difficulties than treatment-naive patients (β=−6.1, 95%CI-10.1,−2.2) but did not perform worse on objective measures. Higher-risk patients experienced impairments in executive function and memory suggesting that disease biology contributes to CRCI independent of treatment.
Keywords: Chronic lymphocytic leukemia, cognitive impairment, chemotherapy, treatment naïve
Introduction
Cancer-related cognitive impairment (CRCI) affects up to 10 million cancer survivors and is associated with decreases in quality of life, social engagement, and occupational and educational success.[1,2] CRCI is also associated with decreased treatment adherence and poorer overall survivors in older hematological malignancy patients.[3,4] Most studies of CRCI have been conducted in cancer patients treated with chemotherapy which is believed to be an important cause of this condition. In contrast, the literature is not conclusive about the role of other factors including cancer biology in the etiology of CRCI.[5] Longitudinal studies have reported that cancer survivors decline on tests of attention, memory, executive function, and processing speed during chemotherapy, implicating these agents in CRCI.[6,7] In these studies, 30% of survivors were impaired prior to adjuvant chemotherapy treatment, suggesting that their malignancy could be associated with CRCI which is then exacerbated by chemotherapy. Possible mechanisms for CRCI include pro-inflammatory or oxidative stress pathways stimulated either by disease processes or treatment that lead to neurotoxicity and cognitive impairment.[8,9] Understanding the etiology and mechanisms of CRCI has been limited by the paucity of data from treatment naïve populations.
We chose chronic lymphocytic leukemia/small lymphocytic lymphoma (CLL) as a novel approach to study the role of disease effects and treatment in the etiology of CRCI. CLL is a common lymphoid malignancy with well-characterized biology and has effective and tolerable but non-curative treatment, and a relatively long median survival. Little data on the effect of disease on cognitive function exists and no data regarding cognitive function assessed via objective cognitive testing has been published to date specifically in CLL. Less than 10% of patients with this non-curable indolent B-cell malignancy present with constitutional symptoms or cytopenias that require immediate treatment. The remainder are monitored closely and only treated upon disease progression, with a median time from diagnosis to first treatment between 5 and 7 years, resulting in a large treatment naïve population of CLL patients. [10,11] The use of flow cytometry, fluorescence in situ hybridization (FISH) and gene sequencing has identified several molecular prognostic markers including interstitial chromosomal deletions of chromosomes 17 (del17p13) and 11 (del11q22.3), absence of IGHV somatic hypermutation, dysfunction mutations in genes including TP53, NOTCH1, and SF3B1 and increased expression of ZAP70 or CD38. These prognostic biomarkers reflect underlying disease biology, allow for more accurate risk stratification, and can inform surveillance, treatment practices, and risk of progression.[11,12] Patients with CLL thus provide a unique opportunity to understand the impact of a systemic malignancy in treatment naïve patients.
We examined the effects of CLL and its treatment on cognitive function by dichotomizing patients for biological risk (higher-risk vs. lower-risk disease) and treatment (treatment-naïve vs. treated). To our knowledge, this is the first objective cognitive assessment study of CRCI in CLL. We hypothesized that patients with higher-risk disease would perform worse on neurocognitive tests than those with lower-risk disease and that patients previously treated with chemotherapy would perform worse than treatment naïve patients.
Methods
Patients and Study Procedures
We recruited eligible CLL patients enrolled in the University of Rochester (UR) Wilmot Cancer Institute (WCI) CLL Registry. This study was approved by the UR Institutional Review Board, conformed to the standards set by the Declaration of Helsinki, and all participants provided written informed consent. Eligibility criteria included: diagnosis of CLL based on standard criteria[11], age ≥21 years, no neurodegenerative disorders or major psychiatric illness (e.g. hospitalization within the past five years), and English fluency. Treatment naïve CLL patients had to be diagnosed with CLL for ≥3 months and have had no prior systemic treatment (e.g. chemotherapy, immunotherapy, etc.) for any malignancy. Previously-treated patients had completed ≥1 chemotherapy regimen ≥3 months before study entry and achieved a sustained complete or partial remission. Patients previously treated with a chemotherapy agent but currently on the BTK inhibitor ibrutinib ≥1 year and in a stable remission were also eligible for the treated group.
Patients were further classified as at higher- or lower-risk of disease progression requiring treatment using validated standard clinical biomarkers abstracted from patient medical records.[11,12] If a patient had one or more of the following prognostic markers they were considered higher-risk: somatically unmutated IGHV (<2%), deletion 17p13, deletion 11q22.3, ZAP70 positivity, CD38 positivity, or deleterious NOTCH1, SF3B1, or TP53 mutation. Patients without any of these markers including those whose markers were not measured (based on clinical judgment of necessity) were defined as lower-risk for the purposes of this study. Target enrollment of 50 higher- and 50 lower-risk treatment naïve patients was met.
Study Measures and Outcomes
Cognitive Function
Participants completed a 1.5 hour comprehensive cognitive function test battery on the same day as their regularly scheduled apppointment in the lymphoma clinic. The neuropsychological battery assessed domains commonly reported to be impaired in cancer survivors including executive function, attention, episodic memory, language, working memory, processing speed, and verbal fluency.[13] Specific validated tests used in this study were the NIH Toolbox for the Assessment of Neurological and Behavior Function Cognition (NIH-TB[14]), Comprehensive Trail Making Test (TMT A/B; representing Trails 1 and 5 respectively[15]), Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised (HVLT-R[16]), and the Controlled Word Association Test (COWA[17]).
The NIH-TB is an iPad-administered battery of seven individual neuropsychological tests of executive function, attention, memory, language, and processing speed.[14] The NIH-TB yields fully-adjusted scores (age, gender, race, and education) for the total composite score of global cognition as well as for each of the seven Individual tests. In addition, we utilized the following paper-based tests: HVLT-R immediate recall summed over the first three trials (range 0–36), HVLT-R delayed recall (trial 4, range 0–12), TMT-A and TMT-B time to completion in seconds, and COWA total number of correct words averaged across three trials. Finally, self-reported cognitive function was measured using the perceived cognitive impairment subscale of the Functional Assessment after Cancer Therapy Cognition questionnaire (FACT-Cog version 3).[18] Scores of one standard deviation (SD) below the age-adjusted normative mean on the NIH Toolbox outcomes are considered impaired.29,[19] In addition to reporting mean age-adjusted scores, we applied this criterion to all cognitive outcomes based on the T-score conversion to derive the percent impaired compared to population norms, as 1 SD is within the range of what is considered clinically meaningful in CRCI research.[14,20]
Other Patient-Reported Outcomes
Within one-week of neurocognitive testing, participants also completed questionnaires for fatigue[21], anxiety[22], and depression[23] that are frequently co-occurring symptoms of CRCI.[24]
Statistical Analysis
All analyses were completed using SAS 9.4 (SAS Institute, Cary, N.C.). Statistical significance was assessed at the two-sided 0.05 level. Descriptive statistics for covariates were calculated overall and according to risk status and treatment history. We evaluated our hypotheses using linear regression models that included risk (model 1), treatment (model 2), and a model that included both risk and treatment (model 3), for each cognitive outcome, adjusting for a priori defined covariates age, gender, race, and education. A priori, we planned to run an additional model with an interaction term between treatment and risk for any cognitive outcome that showed an association (p<0.10) with treatment or risk. In additional exploratory analyses, anxiety, depression, and fatigue were included as covariates.[24] All models were assessed for appropriate model fit. Several potentially influential outliers were identified through residual plots (conservatively, a cook’s distance of >0.5 indicated an influential outlier) but all data points were deemed to be valid. Thus, results including these data points are presented here and estimates omitting these data points are noted in table footnotes. Given the large number of hypothesis tests, p-values were adjusted for the false discovery rate (FDR)[25].
Sample Size and Power
The sample size for this study was determined to estimate a clinically meaningful difference on the NIH Toolbox Total Composite Score (15 points) between higher- and lower-risk patients and between treated and treatment-naive patients (Models 1 and 2). An alpha of 0.025 was used to calculate the required sample size to account for these two primary hypotheses. All other analyses are considered exploratory and were corrected for multiple comparisons using the false discovery rate as described above. With a 1:1 ratio of higher- and lower-risk patients in the treatment naïve sample and a 2:1 ratio of treatment naïve to treated patients, and a power of 80% we estimated at least 90 treatment naïve (45 higher- and 45 lower-risk) and 45 previously treated patients were required. We planned to recruit 10% more patients in each group to account for potential missing data and non-compliance.
Results
Participant Characteristics
Of the 196 patients contacted, 150 agreed to participate in the study (76.5%). Primary reasons participants declined were lack of time (n=28, 61%), and lack of interest (n=7, 15%). %). Among patients who declined, 36% had been previously treated and 52% were high risk, similar to the overall distribution in the final sample (33% and 56% respectively). Patients who refused were, on average, older (median age 72 vs. 65 in the study sample), however, the age of our study sample is consistent with the age of patients routinely seen in our CLL clinic. [26] Demographic and clinical characteristics of the participants are presented in Table I.
Table I:
Patient characteristics | Total | Treatment Naive | Treated | p-value | Lower-risk | Higher-risk | p-value |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N | 150 | 100 | 50 | 66 | 84 | ||
Age (median (min, max)) | 65 (33,91) | 64 (45,91) | 68 (33,85) | 0.139 | 64.5 (47,89) | 65 (33,91) | 0.829 |
Male | 88 (58.7) | 58 (58.0) | 30 (60.0) | 0.815 | 38 (57.6) | 50 (59.5) | 0.810 |
Race | |||||||
White | 144 (96.0) | 96 (96.0) | 48 (96.0) | 1.00 | 65 (98.5) | 79 (94.0) | 0.169 |
Black | 6 (4.0) | 4 (4.0) | 2 (4.0) | 1 (1.5) | 5 (6.0) | ||
Non-Hispanic | 149 (99.3) | 100 (100.0) | 49 (98.0) | 0.333 | 66 (100.0) | 83 (98.8) | 0.374 |
Education | |||||||
≤High School Graduate/GED | 29 (19.3) | 17 (17.0) | 12 (24.0) | 0.313 | 9 (13.6) | 20 (23.8) | 0.308 |
Some College | 57 (38.0) | 37 (37.0) | 20 (40.0) | 24 (36.4) | 33 (39.3) | ||
College Graduate | 30 (20.0) | 19 (19.0) | 11 (22.0) | 15 (22.7) | 15 (17.9) | ||
Graduate/Professional Degree | 34 (22.7) | 27 (27.0) | 7 (14.0) | 18 (27.3) | 16 (19.0) | ||
ECOG PS | |||||||
0 | 98 (65.3) | 72 (72.0) | 26 (52.0) | 0.021 | 47 (71.2) | 51 (60.7) | 0.180 |
1 | 51 (34.0) | 27 (27.0) | 24 (48.0) | 19 (28.8) | 32 (38.1) | ||
2 | 1 (0.7) | 1 (1.0) | 0 (0.0) | 0 (0.0) | 1 (1.2) | ||
Rai stagea | |||||||
0 | 51 (51.0) | N/A | 27 (54.0) | 24 (48.0) | 0.757 | ||
I | 36 (36.0) | 17 (34.0) | 19 (38.0) | ||||
II | 9 (9.0) | 5 (10.0) | 4 (8.00) | ||||
III | 2 (2.0) | 0 (0.0) | 2 (4.00) | ||||
V | 2 (2.0) | 1 (2.00) | 1 (2.00) | ||||
Time since diagnosis (years) | |||||||
Median (min, max) | 3.18 (0.25,19.74) | 2.04 (0.25,19.74) | 7.89 (0.85,17.64) | <0.001 | 3.86 (0.27,13.73) | 2.75 (0.25,19.74) | 0.669 |
Disease Risk Group (overall) | |||||||
Low-Risk Disease | 66 (44.0) | 50 (50.0) | 16 (32.0) | 0.036 | |||
High-Risk Disease | 84 (56.0) | 50 (50.0) | 34 (68.0) |
Treated patients were in a stable partial or complete remission and were unstaged.
Overall Cognitive Impairment
The percent of patients impaired on each neurocognitive assessment based on NIH-TB impairment criteria of 1 SD is reported in Table II. Overall, 8.7% of patients were impaired on the NIH-TB total composite score. Of note, approximately 20% of patients were impaired on tests of executive function, attention, and processing speed (NIH Flanker Inhibitory Control & Attention, NIH Pattern Comparison). Twenty-eight and 36% of patients were impaired on immediate and delayed memory recall, respectively (HVLT-R).
Table II:
Test | N | Direction Of Better Score | Age Adjusted Scorea (mean (SD)) | % Impairedb |
---|---|---|---|---|
NIH-TB Total Composite Score (global cognition) | 150 | Higher | 106.81 (14.13) | 8.7 |
Dimensional Change Card Sort (executive function) | 150 | Higher | 108.81 (15.84) | 3.3 |
Oral Reading Recognition Test (reading decoding) | 150 | Higher | 110.35 (17.37) | 8.0 |
Picture Vocabulary Test (vocabulary comprehension) | 150 | Higher | 108.43 (12.56) | 3.3 |
Flanker Inhibitory Control & Attention (attention & executive function) | 150 | Higher | 93.59 (10.67) | 20.7 |
List Sorting Working Memory (memory) | 150 | Higher | 104.74 (13.78) | 8.0 |
Pattern Comparison (processing speed) | 150 | Higher | 98.65 (17.74) | 23.3 |
Picture Sequence Matching (memory) | 150 | Higher | 100.82 (14.85) | 12.0 |
COWA (verbal fluency) | 148e | Higher | 42.61 (10.49)f | 13.3 |
HVLT-R Total Correct Immediate Recall (memory) | 150 | Higher | 46.19 (11.68) | 28.0 |
HVLT-R Total Correct Delayed Recall (memory) | 150 | Higher | 43.91 (12.34) | 36.7 |
TMT-A (attention) | 148e | Lower | 51.70 (8.89) | 7.3 |
TMT-B (executive function) | 148e | Lower | 51.22 (9.96) | 11.3 |
FACT-Cog Perceived Cognitive Impairment Subscale (self-report) | 149c | Higher | 58.21 (11.68) | N/Ad |
NIH-TB: National Institutes of Health Toolbox Cognition Battery; FACT-Cog: Functional Assessment After Cancer Treatment Cognition; COWA: Controlled Word Association Test; HVLT-R: Hopkins Verbal Learning Test Revised; TMT-A/B: Trail Making Test A or B.
NIH Toolbox outcomes are age adjusted scaled scores (μ=100), paper tests are age adjusted T-scores (μ=50),
impairment defined as an age-adjusted score >1 SD below the normative mean,
one participant did not return study questionnaires,
normative mean unavailable,
two participants did not complete these tests due to time constraints,
mean COWA score adjusted for age and education
Cognitive Function in Higher- vs. Lower-Risk Patients
Higher-risk patients, on average, performed worse on tests of memory, attention, and executive function (Model 1, Table III) compared to those with lower-risk disease. Executive function and memory associations remained statistically significant after adjustment for treatment (Model 3, Table III). Higher-risk patients recalled almost two fewer words than lower-risk patients on the HVLT-R immediate recall (β=−1.79, 95%CI −3.30 to −0.28) and performed 15 seconds slower than lower-risk patients on the TMT-B, a measure of executive function (β=15.37, 95%CI 3.10 to 27.65). Higher-risk patients performed 6.5 seconds slower on the TMT-A, a test of attention/processing speed, compared to lower-risk patients (β=6.54, 95%CI −0.20 to 13.28). After adjustment for multiple comparisons (FDR), these associations lost statistical significance at the two-sided 0.05 level (both p=0.145, Supplemental Table I). There were no statistically significant interactions between treatment and risk status for the FACT-Cog, HVLT-R immediate recall, TMT-A, and TMT-B (Supplemental Table II). Anxiety, depression, and fatigue did not significantly predict cognitive outcomes other than the FACT-Cog.
Table III:
Model 1a | Model 2a | Model 3a, b | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Higher-risk vs. Lower-risk | Treated vs. Treatment Naïve | Higher-risk vs. Lower-risk | Treated vs. Treatment Naïve | ||
Direction Of Better Score | β (95%CI) | β (95%CI) | β (95%CI) | β (95%CI) | |
NIH-TB Total Composite Scorec (primary outcome) | Higher | −1.96 (−4.94,1.01) | −0.38 (−3.53,2.77) | −1.96 (−4.99,1.07) | −0.03 (−3.22,3.16) |
Dimensional Change Card Sort | Higher | −2.05 (−5.34,1.23) | −0.58 (−4.06,2.90) | −2.01 (−5.36,1.33) | −0.22 (−3.74,3.31) |
Oral Reading Recognition | Higher | −3.22 (−6.78,0.34)* | 0.82 (−2.97,4.61) | −3.45 (−7.08,0.17)* | 1.44 (−2.37,5.26) |
Picture Vocabulary Test | Higher | −2.20 (−4.76,0.36)* | −0.41 (−3.13,2.31) | −2.19 (−4.80,0.41)* | −0.01 (−2.76,2.73) |
Flanker Inhibitory Control & Attention | Higher | −0.17 (−2.46,2.13) | −0.81 (−3.23,1.61) | −0.04 (−2.37,2.30) | −0.80 (−3.26,1.66) |
List Sorting Working Memory | Higher | 0.41 (−2.46,3.27) | −0.48 (−3.50,2.54) | 0.50 (−2.42,3.42) | −0.57 (−3.64,2.50) |
Pattern Comparison | Higher | 0.35 (−3.49,4.19) | −0.80 (−4.84,3.24) | 0.50 (−3.41,4.41) | −0.89 (−5.01,3.23) |
Picture Sequence Matching | Higher | 0.66 (−2.68,4.01) | −0.69 (−4.21,2.83) | 0.80 (−2.61,4.20) | −0.83 (−4.42,2.75) |
HVLT-R Total Correct Immediate Recall | Higher | -1.81 (−3.29,−0.32)e | −0.44 (−2.04,1.15) | -1.79 (−3.30,−0.28)e | −0.14 (−1.73,1.45) |
HVLT-R Total Correct Delayed Recall | Higher | −0.38 (−1.19,0.44) | −0.10 (−0.96,0.76) | −0.37 (−1.20,0.46) | −0.04 (−0.91,0.84) |
TMT-A time in secondsd | Lower | 6.89 (0.24,13.54)e | 3.46 (−3.59,10.51) | 6.54 (−0.20,13.28)* | 2.43 (−4.63,9.50) |
TMT-B time in secondsd | Lower | 15.41 (3.32,27.51)e | 2.69 (−10.26,15.65) | 15.37 (3.10,27.65)e | 0.28 (−12.59,13.15) |
COWA total correct words | Higher | 0.21 (−0.96,1.38) | 0.66 (−0.57,1.89) | 0.11 (−1.07,1.30) | 0.64 (−0.61,1.89) |
FACT-Cog | Higher | −0.08 (−3.96,3.80) | -6.12 (−10.07,−2.17)f | 0.84 (−2.98,4.66) | -6.26 (−10.27,2.25)f |
NIH-TB: National Institutes of Health Toolbox Cognition Battery; FACT-Cog: Functional Assessment After Cancer Treatment Cognition; COWA: Controlled Word Association Test; HVLT-R: Hopkins Verbal Learning Test Revised; TMT-A/B: Trail Making Test A or B.
Boldindicates statistical significance p<0.05,
indicates p<0.10, prior to adjustment for multiple comparisons. False discovery rate adjusted p-values are available in Supplemental Table 1.
Model adjusted for age, gender, race, and education,
model mutually assessed both treatment and risk status,
fully adjusted NIH toolbox scores are t –scores,
these models had three potential influential outliers, estimates upon removal: TMT-A Model 1 β =4.16 95%CI −0.21 to 8.52; TMT-B Model 1 β =11.63 95%CI 2.82 to 20.45; TMT-A Model 3 β =4.07 95%CI −0.37 to 8.52; TMT-B Model 3 β =13.20 95%CI 1.70 to 24.69,
after adjustment for multiple comparisons trends remained (P<0.2) but were no longer significant,
after adjustment for multiple comparisons effect remains significant (p=0.030).
Cognitive Function in Treated vs Treatment Naïve Patients
Treated patients, on average, performed worse than treatment-naïve patients on all tests across all domains with the exception of the COWA. However, the differences were small and not statistically significant (Model 2, Table III). These findings did not change even after controlling for anxiety, depression, and fatigue. However, treated patients self-reported significantly more cognitive impairment, scoring six points worse on the FACT-Cog perceived impairment scale than treatment naïve patients (β= −6.12, 95%CI −10.07 to −2.17). This difference remained statistically significant after adjustment for multiple comparisons (p=0.033, Supplemental Table I) and additional adjustment for anxiety, depression, and fatigue scores (β= −5.06, 95%CI −5.58 to −1.54).
Discussion
In this the first study of objective CRCI in CLL patients, we report that up to 20 to 30% of patients experience cognitive impairment in the domains of memory, processing speed, attention, and executive function. In this study, we have to the best of knowledge determined objectively for the first time that higher-risk CLL patients performed significantly worse on objective tests of executive function and memory compared to low-risk patients, independent of treatment status. Overall, our findings suggest that disease biology may play a role in the etiology of CRCI in CLL. These associations remained after adjustment for demographic and psychosocial factors. However, when we adjusted for multiple comparisons our findings were no longer significant. Further research is needed to confirm these findings and clarify if high-risk patients should be monitored for cognitive impairment prior to treatment. . Additionally, treated CLL patients self-reported significantly more cognitive impairments than treatment naïve CLL patients despite little difference in objective tests, potentially due to the contribution of disease on cognitive functioning. These results remained statistically significant after adjustment for multiple comparisons, anxiety, depression and demographic factors increasing our confidence that CRCI remains an important issue for those treated with chemotherapy.
To our knowledge, only one other study has examined associations between tumor characteristics (other than stage) and CRCI. Using a population of breast cancer survivors, Koleck and colleagues report patients who were HER2 positive performed worse on visual and verbal memory than HER2 negative patients.[27] In our study, higher-risk patients performed worse than lower-risk patients on tests of executive function and memory, independent of treatment. Both studies suggest a role for tumor biology in CRCI. It is also possible, given the chronic and systemic nature of CLL, that the biological processes of disease play a more important role than previous chemotherapy treatment. Among the higher-risk markers used in this study, there is significant variability in associations with treatment response and overall survival, therefore further research is needed, in larger prospective cohorts, to determine which of these higher-risk prognostic markers may best predict CRCI.[12]
Similar to a longitudinal study of CLL patients randomized to varying chemotherapy regimens[28], we report that treated patients self-report more cognitive impairment than treatment-naïve patients. Recent work suggests that the difference presented here (5 points) is clinically meaningful.[29] Nonetheless, only small differences were observed on objective measures comparing those treated to treatment naïve. It is possible our study was not powered to find a treatment association given the heterogeneity in treatment history. In sensitivity analyses of treated patients (n=50), we did not see any indication that type of treatment (bendamustine-rituximab vs fludarabine-cyclophosphamide-rituximab) or time since treatment was predictive of CRCI. This suggests that heterogeneity in the treated group is unlikely to explain the small effect sizes in our study. In sensitivity analyses excluding patients currently on ibrutinib (n=18), associations remained unchanged with the exception of TMT-B which inverted (β=−5.50, 95%CI −15.50, 4.50). These patients were the most heavily previously treated patients (>3 regimens) suggesting that our original findings may be attributable to cumulative chemotherapy exposures. Unfortunately, only 18 participants were on ibrutinib at the time of the study and all were previously treated, limiting our ability to examine the individual effects of ibrutinib. As the treatment for CLL moves away from chemoimmunotherapy in the U.S. it will be important to see what, if any, effect ibrutinib and other targeted agents have on cognition, particularly in previously treatment naïve populations. However, chemoimmunotherapy remains important in the treatment of many B cell malignancies and the role of these regimens in the etiology of CRCI requires further evaluation because of patient self-report of CRCI and evidence suggesting that non-Hodgkin lymphoma patients experience CRCI after bendamustine-rituximab.[30]
In addition to the novelty of this study, strengths include an objective analysis of CLL biology, a comprehensive cognitive battery of both objective and subjective assessments, covering a range of cognitive domains, and analyses that accounted for anxiety, depression, and fatigue. We were also able to provide novel data to inform on cognitive function in aging cancer patients, larger studies are needed to examine these effects across the full age distribution of CLL (e.g. 50 to 85+) while accounting for comorbidities. This study is cross-sectional; therefore, we cannot determine the true trajectory of CRCI CLL patients. Inherent limitations to neuropsychological testing may have restricted our ability to detect associations. Neurocognitive tests were originally designed to detect overt neurological injury in specific locations. A priori, this study was powered to find a large association (e.g. 15 point difference on the NIH toolbox), and it is becoming clear that in CLL, like much of the other cancer types, CRCI is more often a mild to moderate condition. For example, despite high proportions of those with CLL classified as impaired on domain-specific tasks of executive function and memory (20.7% to 36.7%), the frequency of global impairment was similar to population based studies of elderly populations (8.7% impaired on the NIH-TB Composite Score) (Table II). [31] It should be noted that that our sample was highly educated (43% college degree or more); however, our results also suggests a domain-specific tasks of CRCI in those with CLL which should be examined further. Even small changes or deficits in cognitive function can impact quality of life and should not be disregarded. Future research using assessments based in cognitive neuroscience focused on executive function, attention, processing speed, and memory may be warranted to enhance our ability to detect mild to moderate effects.[32] Additionally, future research should be expanded to assess the risks of dementia in this older population using clinical screening measures (e.g. MMSE, MOCA, Blessed).[33,34] Lastly, only 19/66 (29%) of patients classified as lower-risk CLL had all biomarkers tested (Supplemental Table III). Most of the missing markers were IGHV mutation status and TP53, SF3B1, and NOTCH1 mutation analysis which have only more recently become routine clinical tests in determining CLL prognosis at diagnosis. Lower-risk patients without complete testing were older (median age 67 vs 62, p<0.035) but did not differ on other demographic variables (Supplemental Table IV). The median time from diagnosis to study entry for lower-risk patients with incomplete testing was >4 years suggesting that fewer of these patients had higher-risk disease compared to published data.[35] It is likely that clinicians did not complete full testing because the patient was diagnosed several years prior to the tests becoming available which diminishes the prognostic value. To test for the potential impact of any misclassification, regression models were run that omitted any lower-risk patient without complete biomarker testing which produced attenuated but similar results (Supplemental Table V). Collectively, these analyses indicate that misclassification in this study likely had minimal effect on our overall findings. Additionally, the statistical significance of these higher-risk associations was lost after adjustment for multiple comparisons. Therefore, further research in larger longitudinal cohorts of treatment-naïve patients is warranted to confirm the higher-risk association and determine which clinical biomarkers may best predict CRCI (e.g. deletion 11q22).
In conclusion, our findings suggest that disease biology contributes to CRCI, independent of treatment. Larger longitudinal studies with complete prognostic biomarker data are needed to fully examine the risk of CRCI associated with higher-risk disease, characterize the trajectory of CRCI in CLL in both chemoimmunotherapy and targeted agent treated patients, and explore any potential interactions between treatment and risk. Understanding the trajectory of CRCI in CLL and its predictors will allow us to design targeted interventions to preserve cognitive function and mitigate any further dysfunction in an elderly population of cancer patients that may already be experiencing cognitive decline due to normal aging and aging-related conditions.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank all of the patients who participated in our study. We would also like to thank members of the Cancer Control Psychoneuroimmunology Laboratory including Kassandra Doyle and Sara Alberti, and Sharon Lewinski, and Rina Yarosh for assisting with data collection.
Funding
This work was supported by the National Cancer Institute at the National Institutes of Health under grant number F99CA222742 (to A.M.W.); under grant number DP2CA195765 and K07CA168886 (to M.C.J.).; and by the Michael Napoleone Foundation (to A.M.W.); and the Cadregari Endowment Fund (to C.S.Z.).
Footnotes
Disclosure of Interest
The authors report no conflicts of interest.
References
- 1.Reid-Arndt SA, Yee A, Perry MC, Hsieh C. Cognitive and psychological factors associated with early posttreatment functional outcomes in breast cancer survivors. J Psychosoc Oncol 2009;27:415–434. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Jacola LM, Edelstein K, Liu W, et al. Cognitive, behaviour, and academic functioning in adolescent and young adult survivors of childhood acute lymphoblastic leukaemia: a report from the Childhood Cancer Survivor Study. Lancet Psychiatry 2016;3:965–972. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Hshieh TT, Jung WF, Grande LJ, et al. Prevalence of Cognitive Impairment and Association With Survival Among Older Patients With Hematologic Cancers. JAMA Oncol 2018;4:686–693. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Dos Santos M, Lange M, Gervais R, et al. Impact of anxio-depressive symptoms and cognitive function on oral anticancer therapies adherence. Support Care Cancer 2019;27:3573–3581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Joly F, Giffard B, Rigal O, et al. Impact of Cancer and Its Treatments on Cognitive Function: Advances in Research From the Paris International Cognition and Cancer Task Force Symposium and Update Since 2012. J Pain Symptom Manage 2015;50:830–841. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Janelsins MC, Heckler CE, Peppone LJ, et al. Longitudinal Trajectory and Characterization of Cancer-Related Cognitive Impairment in a Nationwide Cohort Study. J Clin Oncol 2018:Jco2018786624. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Janelsins MC, Heckler CE, Peppone LJ, et al. Cognitive Complaints in Survivors of Breast Cancer After Chemotherapy Compared With Age-Matched Controls: An Analysis From a Nationwide, Multicenter, Prospective Longitudinal Study. J Clin Oncol 2017;35:506–514. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Williams AM, Shah R, Shayne M, et al. Associations between inflammatory markers and cognitive function in breast cancer patients receiving chemotherapy. J Neuroimmunol 2018;314:17–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Ahles TA, Saykin AJ. Candidate mechanisms for chemotherapy-induced cognitive changes. Nat Rev Cancer 2007;7:192–201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Zent CS, Kay NE. Management of patients with chronic lymphocytic leukemia with a high risk of adverse outcome: the Mayo Clinic approach. Leuk Lymphoma 2011;52:1425–1434. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Hallek M, Cheson BD, Catovsky D, et al. iwCLL guidelines for diagnosis, indications for treatment, response assessment, and supportive management of CLL. Blood 2018;131:2745–2760. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Dohner H, Stilgenbauer S, Benner A, et al. Genomic aberrations and survival in chronic lymphocytic leukemia. N Engl J Med 2000;343:1910–1916. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Wefel JS, Vardy J, Ahles T, Schagen SB. International Cognition and Cancer Task Force recommendations to harmonise studies of cognitive function in patients with cancer. Lancet Oncol 2011;12:703–708. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Weintraub S, Dikmen SS, Heaton RK, et al. Cognition assessment using the NIH Toolbox. Neurology 2013;80:S54–64. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Reynolds CR. Comprehensive Trail-Making Test professional manual. Pro-ed. 2002. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Brandt JBR. Hopkins Verbal Learning Test Revised professional manual. Psychological Assessment Resources. 2001. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Ruff RM, Light RH, Parker SB, Levin HS. Benton Controlled Oral Word Association Test: reliability and updated norms. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 1996;11:329–338. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Von Ah D, Tallman EF. Perceived cognitive function in breast cancer survivors: evaluating relationships with objective cognitive performance and other symptoms using the functional assessment of cancer therapy-cognitive function instrument. J Pain Symptom Manage 2015;49:697–706. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.NIH. NIH Toolbox Scoring and Interpretation Guide for the iPad.: National Institutes of Health and Northwestern University; 2016. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Wefel JS, Kesler SR, Noll KR, Schagen SB. Clinical characteristics, pathophysiology, and management of noncentral nervous system cancer-related cognitive impairment in adults. CA Cancer J Clin 2015;65:123–138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Smets EM, Garssen B, Bonke B, De Haes JC. The Multidimensional Fatigue Inventory (MFI) psychometric qualities of an instrument to assess fatigue. J Psychosom Res 1995;39:315–325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Spielberger CD S SJ, Owen AE, Marsh BJ The use of psychological testing for treatment planning and outcomes assessment:Measuring anxiety and anger with the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) and the State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory (STAXI), in Maruish ME (ed): The use of psychological testing for treatment planning and outcomes assessment. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- 23.McCallum J, Mackinnon A, Simons L, Simons J. Measurement properties of the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale: an Australian community study of aged persons. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 1995;50:S182–189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Jacobs SR, Jacobsen PB, Booth-Jones M, Wagner LI, Anasetti C. Evaluation of the functional assessment of cancer therapy cognitive scale with hematopoietic stem cell transplant patients. J Pain Symptom Manage 2007;33:13–23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the False Discovery Rate: A Practical and Powerful Approach to Multiple Testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. Series B (Methodological) 1995;57:289–300. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Williams AM, Baran AM, Meacham PJ, et al. Analysis of the risk of infection in patients with chronic lymphocytic leukemia in the era of novel therapies. Leuk Lymphoma 2018;59:625–632. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Koleck TA, Bender CM, Sereika SM, et al. Associations between pathologic tumor features and preadjuvant therapy cognitive performance in women diagnosed with breast cancer. Cancer Medicine 2017;6:339–348. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Else M, Cocks K, Crofts S, et al. Quality of life in chronic lymphocytic leukemia: 5-year results from the multicenter randomized LRF CLL4 trial. Leuk Lymphoma 2012;53:1289–1298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Bell ML, Dhillon HM, Bray VJ, Vardy JL. Important differences and meaningful changes for the Functional Assessment of Cancer Therapy-Cognitive Function (FACT-Cog). Journal of Patient-Reported Outcomes 2018;2:48. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Zimmer P, Mierau A, Bloch W, et al. Post-chemotherapy cognitive impairment in patients with B-cell non-Hodgkin lymphoma: a first comprehensive approach to determine cognitive impairments after treatment with rituximab, cyclophosphamide, doxorubicin, vincristine and prednisone or rituximab and bendamustine. Leuk Lymphoma 2015;56:347–352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Petersen RC, Lopez O, Armstrong MJ, et al. Practice guideline update summary: Mild cognitive impairment: Report of the Guideline Development, Dissemination, and Implementation Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology. Neurology 2018;90:126–135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Boykoff N, Moieni M, Subramanian SK. Confronting chemobrain: an in-depth look at survivors’ reports of impact on work, social networks, and health care response. Journal of cancer survivorship : research and practice 2009;3:223–232. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bédirian V, et al. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 2005;53:695–699. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Carpenter CR, Bassett ER, Fischer GM, Shirshekan J, Galvin JE, Morris JC. Four sensitive screening tools to detect cognitive dysfunction in geriatric emergency department patients: brief Alzheimer’s Screen, Short Blessed Test, Ottawa 3DY, and the caregiver-completed AD8. Academic emergency medicine : official journal of the Society for Academic Emergency Medicine 2011;18:374–384. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Jain N, O’Brien S. Initial treatment of CLL: integrating biology and functional status. Blood 2015;126:463–470. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.