Abstract
Prior research on the sibling relationship in the context of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) has included only one sibling per family. We used multi-level modeling to examine sibling relationships in 207 adults who have a brother or sister with ASD from 125 families, investigating variability in sibling relationships within and between families. We found that there was greater variability in the sibling relationship with the brother or sister with ASD within families than between families. Sibling individual-level factors were associated with positive affect in the sibling relationship, while family-level factors were associated with the sibling’s pessimism about their brother or sister’s future. The findings illustrate the unique experiences of siblings within families.
Introduction
The sibling relationship has unique salience in adulthood in the context of illness or disability. Researchers have examined sibling relationships when one sibling has an intellectual or developmental disability (Davys, Mitchell, & Haigh, 2015; Greenberg, Seltzer, Orsmond, & Krauss, 1999; Orsmond & Seltzer, 2000; Orsmond & Seltzer, 2007a), but relatively less research has focused on sibling relationships during adulthood when one sibling has autism spectrum disorder (ASD) (Orsmond & Seltzer, 2007a). Autism is a relatively recently identified disorder, with the first cohort diagnosed in the 1940’s now reaching middle and later adulthood. Autism is also unique in that social impairments are a core feature and may specifically affect sibling relationships. Understanding adult sibling relationships in the context of ASD is important because of the long-term caregiving implications. Siblings often become the guardians or primary caregivers for their brother or sister with a disability once their parents are no longer able to do so (Bigby, 1997; Heller & Arnold, 2010). Thus, research findings will also have relevance to the development of supports and policies affecting these families (Arnold, Heller, & Kramer, 2012).
Sibling relationships in adulthood have been characterized along a number of dimensions, including warmth, conflict, and rivalry (Stocker, Lanthier, & Furman, 1997). Most research with the general population of adults has focused on feelings of warmth and closeness and has reported that while adult siblings may have less contact due to geographic distance or life circumstances, many adults report increased feelings of closeness to their siblings throughout adulthood (Bedford, 1989; Cicirelli, 1991; Dew, Llewellyn, & Balandin, 2004; Gold, 1989). In times of family crisis, especially in the context of declining parental health and eventual death, the sibling relationship is often reactivated and becomes a source of support (Goetting, 1986). When one sibling has a disability such mutual support may be less available, and the parental decline and death likely has caregiving implications for the sibling without a disability.
The research on siblings of individuals with ASD has focused on siblings in childhood, and primarily on behavioral adjustment in the sibling without a disability, rather than on the qualities of the sibling relationship (Orsmond & Seltzer, 2007a). Limited research on sibling relationships in adulthood when one sibling has ASD has indicated that although siblings may have less direct contact with their brother or sister with ASD, they generally rate the sibling relationship relatively high in positive affect (Hodapp & Urbano, 2007; Orsmond, Kuo, & Seltzer, 2009). However, siblings with a brother or sister with ASD appear to experience less emotional closeness in the sibling relationship and have less direct contact with their brother or sister than adult siblings who have a brother or sister with other types of developmental disabilities, such as Down syndrome (Hodapp & Urbano, 2007; Orsmond & Seltzer, 2007b; Tozer, Atkin, & Wenham, 2013). Qualitative research suggests that the sibling relationship is conditioned by past experiences, that siblings feel a sense of responsibility and commitment to their brother or sister and value the sibling relationship, yet sometimes express frustration with maintaining a reciprocal relationship (Tozer et al., 2013).
Research on siblings of individuals with disability has been methodologically challenged by the selection of one sibling per family so that data points are independent (Hodapp, Glidden, & Kaiser, 2005; Krull, 2007). Prior research examining siblings who have a brother or sister with a developmental disability has examined one sibling dyad per family (one sibling and the brother or sister with disability). Recently, researchers have used multilevel modeling statistical techniques to examine the within and between family processes that affect sibling relationships in the general population (Gilligan, Suitor, & Nam, 2015; Jenkins, Rasbash, Leckie, Gass, & Dunn, 2012; Jenkins, Dunn, O'Connor, Rasbash, & Behnke, 2005; Marciniak, 2017) and to understand family processes in parent-child relationships (O'Connor, Dunn, Jenkins, & Rasbash, 2006). No prior research has used such methods to examine sibling relationships in the context of disability. In the current study, we used multi-level modeling to examine sibling relationships in adults who have a brother or sister with ASD, investigating variability in sibling relationships within and between families. Both individual-level and family-level covariates were examined as predictors of the sibling relationship from the perspective of the adult sibling without a disability.
Because of the limited research examining correlates of sibling relationships when one sibling has ASD, we drew upon the broader literature on sibling relationships in the context of intellectual and developmental disability (IDD) to inform our inquiry. At the sibling individual level, we examined sibling age. The age of the brother or sister with ASD also may condition the sibling relationship. Hodapp and Urbano (2007) found that siblings whose brother or sister with ASD was age 45 or older reported less close and affectionate sibling relationships than siblings whose brother or sister was in young adulthood. Similarly, Orsmond and colleagues (Orsmond et al., 2009) reported that siblings engaged in more shared activities if their brother or sister with ASD was younger in age. Thus, we also included age of the brother or sister with ASD as a family-level covariate. Although age difference may impact sibling relationships in the general population, it is less important in the context of disability. In childhood, both younger and older siblings who have a brother or sister with a disability assume caregiving roles; it is this role, rather than birth order, that results in role asymmetry (Stoneman, 2005). Thus, we did not focus on birth order or age difference in the current analyses.
Prior research examining adult sibling relationships in the context of IDD has shown that the sibling relationship might be conditioned by the gender of each sibling, as well as the gender combination of the sibling dyad. Sisters tend to report more warmth and closer relationships with their brother or sister with IDD than do brothers (Begum & Blacher, 2011; Hodapp, Urbano, & Burke, 2010). Moreover, Orsmond and Seltzer (2000) found that sisters who had a sister with IDD reported the closest sibling relationship and brother who had a sister with IDD reported the least close sibling relationship. Although the small number of studies examining adult sibling relationships in ASD have reported that gender of the sibling or gender composition of the sibling dyad did not condition the sibling relationship (Hodapp & Urbano, 2007; Orsmond & Seltzer, 2007b; Orsmond et al., 2009), we included adult sibling gender and gender composition of the sibling dyad in the analyses. We also included sibling education in our analyses, as higher sibling education was found to be associated closer sibling relationships in the context of ASD (Orsmond & Seltzer, 2007b) and in the general population of adults (Connidis & Campbell, 1995).
In addition to age and gender, adult sibling relationships have been associated with coping skills and mental health of the sibling without the disability. Siblings of adults with ASD who used more problem-focused coping strategies reported closer sibling relationships (Orsmond & Seltzer, 2007b; Orsmond et al., 2009). Similarly, Tomeny and colleagues (Tomeny, Ellis, Rankin, & Barry, 2017) found that siblings who expressed more positive attitudes about their relationship with their adult brother or sister with ASD or ID had fewer depressive symptoms. The association between sibling well-being and sibling relationships is likely bidirectional. College age individuals who report supportive sibling relationships were less lonely and reported fewer depressive symptoms (Milevsky, 2005). Similarly, in later adulthood in the general population, having a closer sibling relationship is associated with fewer reported depressive symptoms (Cicirelli, 1989).
The sibling relationship is likely conditioned not only by the sibling individual characteristics, but also by the characteristics of the brother or sister with a disability. Challenging behaviors in the brother or sister with ASD are consistently reported to negatively affect the sibling relationship, both in childhood (Hastings & Petalas, 2014; Orsmond & Seltzer, 2009; Orsmond et al., 2009) as well as in adulthood (Hodapp & Urbano, 2007; Orsmond et al., 2009). We also examined whether the presence of intellectual disability, in addition to ASD, was associated with the sibling relationship. Researchers have found that if the brother or sister with ASD had higher levels of functional independence, the siblings engaged in more shared activities together (Orsmond & Seltzer, 2007b; Orsmond et al., 2009; Taylor & Hodapp, 2012).
The sibling’s experience of family relationships also appears to influence sibling relationships in adulthood. In the general population, the frequency of contact between siblings is associated with closer relationships (Connidis & Campbell, 1995). Similarly, a closer relationship has been observed when siblings have more contact with their brother or sister with ASD in adulthood (Hodapp & Urbano, 2007). The closeness of the sibling relationship in adolescence also appears to be an important correlate of adult sibling relationships in the context of IDD or mental illness (Greenberg et al., 1999). Moreover, the relationship that the sibling has with his or her parent(s) is associated with sibling relationship quality (Brody, 1998; McHale & Crouter, 1996; Orsmond et al., 2009; Portner & Riggs, 2016).
There is some indication that family size may be associated with more positive sibling relationships, although the findings are equivocal. Siblings from larger families tend to report closer relationships in middle and later adulthood in the general population (Connidis & Campbell, 1995) and in adolescence when one sibling has ASD (Orsmond et al., 2009). In one study, adult siblings reported having a closer relationship with their brother or sister with IDD if they came from a larger family (Meadan, Stoner, & Angell, 2010). However, other researchers have reported that adult siblings from larger families in the general population reported less close relationships with one another (Milevsky, 2005) and Riggio (2006) found no association between family size and sibling relationships.
An important limitation of the findings with respect to adult siblings who have a brother or sister with ASD or IDD is that only one sibling per family participated; thus, the findings reflect differences in these factors across families. In these studies, adult siblings who participated represented the sibling closest in age to the brother or sister with ASD (Begum & Blacher, 2011; Orsmond et al., 2009), the “most involved sibling” from the perspective of the mother (Orsmond & Seltzer, 2007b), a sibling randomly selected from the family (Floyd, Purcell, Richardson, & Kupersmidt, 2009), or a sibling who responded to a national online survey (Hodapp & Urbano, 2007). No prior research has examined multiple siblings within a family, and whether there is variability within families with respect to the conditioning effects of these factors on sibling relationships.
In the current analysis, we utilized data from 207 adult siblings from 125 families who reported on the quality of their relationship with their brother or sister with ASD. In some families, only one other sibling was available for participation in the study, but in approximately one-third of the families, data were available from two or more siblings in the family. Thus, the aims of the current study were to: (1) Examine the variation in sibling relationships between and within families that include an individual with ‘ASD; and (2) Examine the individual-level and family-level correlates of sibling relationships between an adult sibling and his or her brother or sister with ASD. We focused on the sibling relationship dimensions of positive affect and pessimism about the future of the brother or sister with ASD. We examined sibling individual- level correlates (age, gender, gender composition of the sibling dyad, education, frequency of contact, sibling relationship in adolescence, parental support, sibling depressive symptoms, and problem-focused coping skills) and family-level correlates (family size; and age, cognitive limitations, and challenging behaviors in the brother or sister with ASD).
Method
Participants
Data for this study were collected during a 12-year longitudinal study of families of adolescents and adults with an ASD (Seltzer et al., 2003, 2011). Families participating in this study were recruited via agencies, schools, diagnostic clinics, and the media. The families met three criteria when initially recruited: (a) the family member with an ASD was age 10 or older; (b) he or she had received a diagnosis on the autism spectrum from a medical, psychological, or educational professional, as reported by the parents; and (c) administration of the Autism Diagnostic Interview – Revised (ADI-R; Lord, Rutter, & LeCouteur, 1994) confirmed the parental report of an autism spectrum disorder. Adult siblings (ages 18 years or older) with a brother or sister with an ASD were invited to participate in a mailed survey during the second wave of data collection.
A total of 243 of 351 available siblings participated in the mailed survey at the second round of data collection, for a response rate of 69.5%. For this set of analyses we were interested in examining maternal data in coordination with sibling variables, so we excluded 12 cases where the father was the primary respondent in the interview. These eliminations resulted in a total sample size of 232 siblings with maternal data. We eliminated an additional 25 siblings because of unique circumstances we thought might affect our outcomes: the sibling had more than one brother or sister with ASD (n = 1); half siblings (n = 12); step-siblings (n = 4); and the sibling or brother or sister with an ASD was adopted (n = 8). The sample final sample consisted of 207 siblings from 125 families.
Table 1 presents descriptive information on the background characteristics of siblings and their family members (mothers and brother or sister with ASD). Siblings ranged in age from 18 to 59 years old. Almost all (94.7%) were Caucasian. Just over half of the participating siblings were female. Almost half of the siblings had a college or graduate degree. Most siblings were employed (83.6%) and 28% had a household income of $70,000 or more a year. Just under one half were married and a similar percentage had children. A few siblings reported they had a child with a disability (7.7%). About two-thirds were older than their brother or sister with an ASD. Just over 10% lived with their parent(s) and 6.3% of siblings lived with their brother or sister with ASD.
Table 1.
Total | |
---|---|
Adult Siblings | N = 207 |
Age in years (M, SD) | 35.6 (10.8) |
Female | 55.1% |
Education | |
High school or less | 18.4% |
Some college or Associate’s degree | 32.4% |
College graduate | 20.3% |
Some graduate school or graduate degree | 29.0% |
Household income | |
<$35,000/year | 3.9% |
$35,000-$69,000 | 30.4% |
$70,000+ | 28.0% |
Employed | 83.6% |
Married | 49.8% |
Parents | 45.4% |
Older than Brother/Sister with ASD | 68.1% |
Mothers | N = 125 |
Age in years (M, SD) | 59.0 (11.5) |
Married | 70.4% |
Health | |
Poor | 1.6% |
Fair | 17.6% |
Good | 39.2% |
Excellent | 56% |
Household income | |
<$35,000/year | 31.2% |
$35,000-$69,999/year | 21.6% |
$70,000+ | 33.6% |
Brother/Sister with ASD | N = 125 |
Age in years (M, SD) | 29.7 (10.8) |
Male | 70.4% |
Live with parent(s) | 46.4% |
Intellectual disability | 84.0% |
The mothers in these families ranged in age from 38 to 83. Most mothers were in good or excellent health. Over two-thirds were married. Family size ranged from 2 children (sibling and brother/sister with ASD; 31% of the sample) to 8 children (inclusive of brother/sister with ASD). The siblings’ brother or sister with ASD ranged in age from 12 to 53 and over two-thirds were male. Just under half lived with the parent(s). Over 80% had intellectual disability and over two-thirds (68.8%) had at least phrase speech.
Measures
Sibling Relationship Quality.
Siblings completed written measures that assessed two aspects of the sibling relationship: Positive Affect and Pessimism. Siblings completed the Positive Affect Index (PAI; Bengtson & Black, 1973), which includes 10 items measuring positive affect in the relationship. Five questions addressed the adult sibling’s feelings toward their brother or sister with an ASD (e.g., “How much affection do you have toward your brother/sister?”), and five questions assessed the sibling’s perception of the positive affect from their brother or sister with an ASD (e.g., “How much affection do you feel that your brother/sister has for you?”). The questions reflect the dimensions of affection, understanding, trust, fairness, and respect in the relationship. Each item is rated on a scale from 1 (not at all) to 6 (extremely), with higher scores indicating more positive affect. The test-retest reliability, internal consistency and construct validity of PAI were high as reported by the test authors (Bengtson & Black, 1973). Alpha reliability for this sample was .91 for the total scale, .88 for the 5 items of positive affect perceived from the brother or sister with ASD, and .85 for the 5 items of positive affect felt towards the brother or sister with ASD.
Sibling pessimism about the brother or sister with ASD’s future was measured with 10 items from the Pessimism scale of the Questionnaire on Resources and Stress (QRS-F; Friedrich, Greenberg, & Crnic, 1983). This scale assesses worries and pessimism about the future of the family member with the disability (e.g., “I worry about what will happen to my brother/sister when my mother can no longer take care of him/her”). Siblings rated each item as 0 (false) or 1 (true) with resulting scores ranging from 0 to 10 (alpha =.68).
Sibling Individual-Level Covariates.
Siblings provided background information, such as birthdate, gender, birth order, level of education, income, employment status, marital status, and whether or not they had children (see Table 1).
Siblings completed eight subscales of the Multidimensional Coping Inventory (Carver, Sheier, & Weintraub, 1989). Four subscales (each comprised of four items) measured problem-focused coping: active coping, planning, suppression of competing activities, and positive reinterpretation and growth. Four subscales measured emotion-focused coping: denial, focusing on and venting of emotions, behavioral disengagement, and mental disengagement. Siblings rated each item on a 4-point scale according to how often they used the strategy from 1 ‘not at all’ to 4 ‘a lot’. Alpha reliability coefficients were .92 and .75 for problem-focused and emotion- focused coping skills, respectively.
Siblings reported depressive symptoms on the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D; Radloff, 1977). The CES-D is a well-validated and reliable measure of depressive affect in the general population (Radloff, 1991). Twenty items assess the frequency of depressive symptoms during the preceding week, ranging from 0 ‘rarely’ to 3 ‘most of the time’. A higher total score indicates more depressive symptoms. Test-retest reliability, internal consistency and concurrent validity are good (Radloff, 1977). The alpha reliability coefficient for this sample of siblings was .91.
Siblings reported on the frequency of contact with their brother or sister with ASD using two questions that asked siblings how often they saw their brother or sister in person and spoke with them on the phone (both scored 0 ‘never’ to 7 ‘daily’). Siblings completed one item rating the emotional closeness of their sibling relationship during adolescence, on a scale from 0 ‘not at all close’ to 3 ‘very close’.
We used the Perceived Social Support Scales (Procidano & Heller, 1983) to measure siblings’ perceived social support from parents. Siblings responded to this 20-item measure for the support that they receive separately from their mother and father (e.g., “My mother/father gives me the moral support I need”). Siblings responded whether each statement was true or false. Scores on each scale range from 0 to 20, with higher scores indicating that siblings perceived greater support. Test-retest reliability, internal consistency and construct validity of the measure are reported to be high (Procidano & Heller, 1983). We averaged the scores reflecting support from mothers and fathers for each sibling.
Family-Level Covariates.
During the first wave of data collection, mothers provided background information about their age, the number of children in the family, and the age and gender of her son or daughter with ASD. Mothers completed measures at the second wave of data collection, close in time to the adult siblings’ participation. They provided updated information about their marital status, income, health, and whether or not the son or daughter with ASD lived at home (see Table 1).
At the second wave of data collection, mothers completed the behavior problems subscale of the Scales of Independent Behaviors – Revised (SIB-R; Bruininks, Woodcock, Weatherman, & Hill, 1996). This measure consists of eight challenging behaviors: behavior that is hurtful to self, unusual or repetitive, withdrawn or inattentive, socially offensive, uncooperative, hurtful to others, disruptive, and destructive of property. The mother was asked whether her son or daughter manifested each of these eight challenging behaviors within the last 6 months, and, if so, to rate the frequency and severity of the behavior, each measured on a 6-point scale. Standardized algorithms (Bruininks et al., 1996) were used to translate frequency and severity ratings into a general summary score where higher scores indicated more severe challenging behaviors. Challenging behavior scores ranged from 100 to 134.
At the second wave of data collection, we also characterized the individuals with ASD as to whether or not they had an intellectual disability, using a variety of sources of information. When possible, we administered the Wide Range Intelligence Test (WRIT; Glutting, Adams & Sheslow, 2000) to the individual with an ASD. In addition, mothers reported on adaptive behavior via the Vineland Screener (Sparrow, Carter & Cicchetti, 1987). Individuals with a standard scores of 70 or below on each instrument were classified as having an intellectual disability, whereas those with scores above 75 on either measure were deemed not to have an intellectual disability. For the remaining cases, independent review of records (including psychological testing records when available, adaptive behavior information, parental report of a prior diagnosis of intellectual disability) by three psychologists, combined with a clinical consensus procedure, was used to determine whether or not an individual had an intellectual disability.
Data Analysis
The first level of analysis examined variation in sibling relationships between and within families using bivariate correlations. Variables were represented on two levels: the family-level (e.g., family size, challenging behaviors of brother/sister with ASD) and sibling-specific level (e.g. depressive symptoms, perception of parental support). Siblings within a family experienced the same family-level factors, while data at the sibling-specific level pertained directly to each sibling.
Multilevel models were used to examine associations between the outcome measures and selected sibling- and family-level covariates. Dependent variables were total positive affect (PA total), sibling positive affect toward their brother or sister with ASD (PA toward), sibling perceived positive affect from their brother or sister with ASD (PA from), and sibling pessimism about the future of their brother or sister with ASD (Pessimism). Intraclass Correlation Coefficients (ICC) were calculated to assess the variability of the outcome measures within and between families.
We selected covariates for inclusion in the multivariate models based on bivariate correlations and theoretical interest. Sibling-level covariates included own education, perceived parental support, relationship with sibling with ASD during adolescence, sibling age, current frequency of in-person contact with brother or sister with ASD, and own depressive symptoms. Family-level covariates included intellectual disability in the brother or sister with ASD, challenging behaviors in the brother or sister with ASD, and total number of siblings in the family.
Results
Bivariate correlations
Several sibling- and family-level variables were associated with positive affect in sibling relationships (see Table 2). Relationship in adolescence, frequency of current in-person contact, and parental support were sibling-specific variables that were positively associated with PA total, PA from, and PA toward scores. The sibling’s own education had a small but significant negative association with PA total and PA from scores. Intellectual disability in the brother or sister with ASD was a family-level variable that was significantly associated with PA total and PA from scores. No family-level variables were associated with PA toward scores.
Table 2.
Sibling-level variables | ||||||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. | 2. | 3. | 4. | 5. | 6. | 7. | 8. | 9. | 10. | 11. | 12. | 13. | 14. | 15. | 16. | |
1. Positive affect - total | ||||||||||||||||
2. Positive affect - toward | .931** | |||||||||||||||
3. Positive affect - from | .984** | .766** | ||||||||||||||
4. Pessimism | −.163* | −.194* | −.119 | |||||||||||||
5. Adult sibling age | −.117 | −.091 | −.121 | −.187** | ||||||||||||
6. Adult sibling gender | .084 | .102 | .064 | .040 | −.083 | |||||||||||
7. Gender composition | .070 | .081 | .057 | .022 | −.060 | .917** | ||||||||||
8. Adult sibling education | −.167* | −.134 | −.169* | .020 | .124 | .027 | .018 | |||||||||
9. Contact | .321** | .336** | .275** | .099 | −.216** | .003 | .009 | −.194* | ||||||||
10. Relationship in adolescence | .562** | .523** | .536** | −.061 | −.113 | .058 | .047 | −.134 | .282** | |||||||
11. Parental support | .218** | .178* | .230** | .015 | −.020 | .064 | .024 | −.255** | .192** | .156* | ||||||
12. Adult sibling depressive symptoms | −.133 | −.127 | −.117 | .199** | −.036 | −.006 | .004 | −.105 | −.063 | −.057 | −.061 | |||||
13. Adult sibling problem-focused coping | .087 | .103 | .055 | −.098 | .165* | .052 | .110 | .190** | −.074 | .009 | −.112 | −.378** | ||||
Family-level variables | ||||||||||||||||
14. Age of b/s with ASD | −.129 | −.122 | −.114 | −.175* | .788** | −.109 | −.090 | .164* | −.205** | −.231** | −.020 | .005 | .060 | |||
15. Family size | −.110 | −.088 | −.100 | −.202** | .453** | −.092 | −.138* | −.057 | −.117 | .007 | −.114 | −.034 | .024 | .234** | ||
16. Challenging behaviors in b/s | .020 | .004 | .027 | .219** | −.450** | .010 | −.008 | −.072 | .167* | .049 | −.068 | .043 | −.055 | −.360** | −.279** | |
17. Intellectual disability in b/s | −.180* | −.113 | −.214** | .138* | −.050 | −.080 | −.093 | −.158* | .027 | .035 | .009 | −.039 | −.080 | −.024 | .059 | .059 |
p < .05
p < .01
Sibling pessimism about the future of their brother or sister with ASD was negatively associated with their own age and positively associated with their own depressive symptoms. All four family-level variables were associated with pessimism: age of brother or sister with ASD and number of siblings in the family were negatively associated with pessimism, while intellectual disability and challenging behaviors in the brother or sister with ASD were positively associated with pessimism.
Multilevel models
For total PA, 25% of the variance occurred between-families and 75% within-families (ICC = 0.246). Sixteen percent of the variance in a sibling’s PA toward their brother or sister with ASD was between families, and 84% within families (ICC = 0.157), while 22% of the variance in a sibling’s PA from their brother or sister with ASD was between and 78% within families (ICC = 0.221). For pessimism, 36% of the variance occurred between-families and 64% within-families (ICC = 0.364).
Multilevel models focused on total PA in the sibling relationship, PA toward their brother or sister with ASD, PA from their brother or sister with ASD, and pessimism about their brother or sister with ASD’s future as dependent variables (see Table 3). Siblings who reported a closer relationship in adolescence had higher total PA scores compared to siblings who were less close during adolescence. Siblings with higher depressive symptoms reported lower levels of positive affect in the sibling relationships. Intellectual disability in the brother or sister with ASD was negatively associated with overall positive affect in the sibling relationship.
Table 3.
Total Positive Affect in the Sibling Relationship |
Sibling Positive Affect toward Brother or Sister with ASD |
Sibling Positive Affect from Brother or Sister with ASD |
Sibling Pessimism about Brother or Sister with ASD’s future |
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Coefficient (SE) |
p | Coefficient (SE) |
p | Coefficient (SE) |
p | Coefficient (SE) |
p | |
Sibling Level | ||||||||
Own education | −1.003 (0.598) | 0.096 | −0.444 (0.306) | 0.149 | −0.428 (0.342) | 0.213 | 0.103 (0.183) | 0.572 |
Parental support | 0.103 (0.079) | 0.193 | 0.032 (0.042) | 0.450 | 0.076 (0.045) | 0.092 | 0.037 (0.027) | 0.164 |
Adolescent relationship | 5.980 (0.872) | <0.001 | 2.706 (0.465) | <0.001 | 3.330 (0.510) | <0.001 | −0.094 (0.257) | 0.716 |
Own age | −0.061 (0.070) | 0.384 | −0.001 (0.036) | 0.970 | −0.055 (0.041) | 0.180 | −0.016 (0.021) | 0.470 |
Contact with b/s with ASD | 0.603 (0.419) | 0.153 | 0.455 (0.223) | 0.043 | 0.166 (0.240) | 0.489 | 0.140 (0.134) | 0.300 |
Own depressive symptoms | −0.188 (0.076) | 0.015 | −0.090 (0.039) | 0.023 | −0.076 (0.042) | 0.092 | 0.026 (0.020) | 0.188 |
Family Level | ||||||||
Intellectual disability in b/s with ASD | −4.464 (1.732) | 0.009 | −1.682 (0.887) | 0.060 | −2.884 (1.013) | 0.006 | 1.571 (0.529) | 0.004 |
Challenging behaviors in b/s with ASD | −0.009 (0.072) | 0.902 | −0.001 (0.037) | 0.970 | −0.008 (0.043) | 0.858 | 0.047 (0.023) | 0.048 |
Number of siblings in family | −0.269 (0.461) | 0.560 | −0.146 (0.231) | 0.528 | −0.055 (0.268) | 0.838 | −0.322 (0.130) | 0.015 |
Sibling positive affect toward the brother or sister with ASD was predicted solely by sibling-level variables (though there was a trend for intellectual disability in the brother or sister with ASD being associated with lower PA toward): closeness in adolescence, frequency of in-person contact, and own depressive symptoms. Siblings who reported a closer relationship in adolescence and more frequent in-person contact reported more positive affect towards their brother or sister with ASD, while those with higher depressive symptoms reported lower positive affect toward their brother or sister with ASD.
The sibling’s perception of positive affect from the brother or sister with ASD was predicted by one sibling-level and one family-level variable. Siblings who reported a closer relationship in adolescence reported more positive affect from their brother or sister with ASD compared to siblings who were less close during adolescence. Intellectual disability in the brother or sister with ASD was negatively associated with positive affect from that sibling.
Sibling pessimism about the brother or sister with ASD’s future was associated with no sibling-level variables and all family-level variables. Siblings who came from smaller families, those whose brother or sister with ASD had more challenging behaviors, and those whose brother or sister had intellectual disability were more pessimistic about their brother or sister’s future.
Discussion
In this manuscript, we utilized newer approaches to data analysis that add to our understanding of how the sibling relationship is conditioned by having a brother or sister with ASD. Although research is emerging that informs our understanding of the sibling relationship in this specific context, the research to date has been limited by the selection of only one sibling per family. By using multi-level modeling, we were able to examine the extent to which prior research findings hold when we examine the perspectives of multiple siblings per family. Moreover, we were able to examine within-family variability in the sibling relationship, as reported by different siblings, as well as variability across families. Interestingly, we found that there was greater variability in the sibling relationship with the brother or sister with ASD within families than between families. This finding suggests that the sibling relationship may be conditioned more by the sibling’s own experience and characteristics than the characteristics of the brother or sister with ASD. This finding challenges prior views of the sibling experience in the context of ASD, which have generally assumed that it is the characteristics of the family member with ASD that “impacts” other family members, including siblings and parents. This finding also reminds us that each person’s experience within the family is unique and that, to the extent possible, we should examine family experiences from the perspective of each family member, and not assume that the experience is uniform or similar across family members.
Prior research on adult sibling relationships in the general population has also found considerable within family variation in sibling relationships. In fact, researchers have documented that the relationship each sibling reports within a family is more different than the relationships siblings report in different families (Branje, van Aken, Marcel A. G., & van Lieshout, Cornelis F. M., 2002; Cook & Kenny, 2004; Daniels & Plomin, 1985). For example, Branje et al. (2002) found that adolescent siblings’ perceived support from parents was reflective of their own characteristics, rather than the characteristics of their parent. Thus, the findings of sibling relationships in the context of ASD parallel those in the general population. The sibling’s self-report of the sibling relationship likely reflects their own subjective experience in the relationship (Cook & Kenny, 2004) and thus it is not surprising that it is influenced by the sibling’s own characteristics. A more objective measure, such as an observational measure, may be less reflective of the sibling’s own characteristics.
When we examined the factors associated with the sibling’s report of the relationship with their brother or sister with ASD, and their feelings of pessimism about the future, we observed that both sibling-level and family-level factors had significant associations. With respect to positive affect reported in the sibling relationship, primarily sibling-level factors were associated (feelings of closeness in adolescence and siblings’ own depressive symptoms). The sibling’s pessimistic feelings about the future, however, were predicted primarily by family-level characteristics, including challenging behaviors in the brother or sister with ASD and having fewer other siblings. The co-occurrence of intellectual disability in the brother or sister with ASD was a significant factor associated with both the sibling relationships and sibling pessimism about their brother or sister’s future. If the brother or sister with ASD also had intellectual disability, the sibling reported lower levels of positive affect in the sibling relationship and was more pessimistic about his or her brother or sister’s future. This association has previously been reported in the literature (Doody, Hastings, O’Neill, & Grey, 2010; Hodapp & Urbano, 2007). It is not surprising that siblings would be more pessimistic about their brother or sister’s future if their brother or sister has more limited cognitive abilities and more significant challenging behaviors, as well as if they have fewer other siblings to share responsibility or to assist when needed. But, the finding that the sibling relationship was primarily associated with the sibling’s own characteristics, again reinforces the notion that siblings have different experiences within a family.
The findings from this analysis generally support, with a few exceptions, prior research on sibling relationships in the context of ASD and IDD that included only one sibling per family. In these prior studies, the selected sibling was either randomly selected, the closest in age, or viewed by the parent as the “most involved.” Similar to prior studies, we found that the sibling’s recollection of their sibling relationship during adolescence and the sibling’s own mental health were associated with the quality of adult sibling relationships. Prior research has found that the sibling relationship in adolescence is important. For example, Cuskelly (2016) used longitudinal data to show that having a good relationship in childhood was associated with more warmth expressed by adult siblings towards their brother or sister with Down syndrome. Importantly, Greenberg and colleagues (Greenberg et al., 1999) reported that a closer relationship reported in adolescence with a brother or sister with intellectual disability was predictive of emotional support during adulthood and expectations of future caregiving responsibilities later in adulthood.
Also similar to prior research, we found that siblings who endorsed more depressive symptoms themselves reported a less close sibling relationship (Tomeny et al., 2017). This finding was observed primarily with respect to how much positive affect the sibling reported feeling towards their brother or sister with ASD; not the positive affect they perceived as reciprocated. It is interesting to note that in prior analysis of this data, using only the sibling closest in age to the brother or sister with ASD, we did not observe an association between depressive symptoms and positive affect in the sibling relationship (Orsmond et al., 2009). This dissimilarity in findings suggests that methodological differences in studies may account for differences in findings; selecting the sibling closest in age for analysis may have limited the variability of some measures analyzed. It is important to note that it is likely, as observed with parents of youth with IDD (Orsmond, Seltzer, Krauss, & Hong, 2003), that the association between depressive symptoms and perceptions of the relationship are bidirectional.
Adult siblings’ worries about the future of their brother or sister with IDD and/or ASD is a prominent theme in the literature, especially in qualitative studies (Davys et al., 2016; Rawson, 2010). Adult siblings express concern about their brother or sister’s access to appropriate services, the type of care they might need in the future, and their health (Davys et al., 2016). The current analysis helps to understand the factors that may contribute to concern or pessimism about the future and what it holds for the sibling and their relationship with their brother or sister with ASD. Not surprisingly, greater pessimism about the brother or sister’s future was observed when the brother or sister had more limited cognitive abilities and more impactful challenging behaviors. Siblings were also more pessimistic if they had fewer other siblings, presumably who could help if needed in the future. These findings underscore the need to develop supports for families that include siblings in the future planning process (Arnold et al., 2012; Burke, Fish, & Lawton, 2015; Davys et al., 2016).
Similar to the findings in this study, prior researchers have reported that sibling constellation and demographic characteristics have relatively little influence on the sibling relationship when one sibling had ASD and/or IDD (Floyd, Costigan, & Richardson, 2016; Hodapp & Urbano, 2007). For example, using the same data set, but analyzing only the sibling closest in age to the brother or sister with ASD, we found that family size was not associated with positive affect in the sibling relationship (Orsmond et al., 2009).
The fact that we did not find some associations previously reported in some prior studies may reflect the differences in sampling strategies, and the possibility that the sample used in the current analysis was less biased because we did not only include one sibling per family. In the multi-level models we did not find significant associations between the sibling relationship and sibling contact, parental support, and the gender of each and both siblings. Prior researchers have reported that sibling contact was associated with perceptions of the sibling relationship (Hodapp & Urbano, 2007). It could be the sibling selection factors in prior studies were associated with less variability in some measures; more involved siblings or those closest in age to the brother or sister with IDD/ASD might be more likely to be sisters, have more contact, and report a closer relationship by default. Those nominated by their parents for participation in a study may have a closer relationship with their parent.
Some differences with prior research may also reflect the sibling relationship constructs examined. For example, caregiving expectations and the sibling relationship may have different predictors. Burke and colleagues (Burke, Taylor, Urbano, & Hodapp, 2012) found that siblings who reported a closer relationship with their brother or sister with IDD had higher expectations for future caregiving. But, it is also possible that stress or expectation of future caregiving could negatively impact the sibling relationship. Future caregiving expectations and felt obligations may not correspond to emotional aspects of the sibling relationship.
Although the use of multi-level modeling in the current analysis allowed us to reduce selection bias and examine variability within and between families, this approach is not without its limitations. Not all siblings from all families participated. This aspect of selection bias may affect the variability of the data as well (Marciniak, 2017). In addition, family size is not a random variable, but may be influenced by parental and family factors not measured (Krull, 2007). Moreover, the siblings who participated in this study were from families participating in a longitudinal study on family caregiving and autism in adolescence and adulthood; these families were not especially ethnically or racially diverse and had the ability to participate in an ongoing study. Finally, this secondary analysis of existing data did not allow for a broad examination of sibling relationship variables. We were limited with the measured constructs and the fact that we did not seek the perspective of the brother or sister with ASD when possible.
Nonetheless, this set of analyses provides new insights into the sibling relationship when one sibling has ASD and cautions sibling researchers to carefully consider methodological issues such as sample selection. Perhaps the most important contribution of this manuscript is the finding of greater variability in the sibling relationship within families than across families; what the sibling brings to the relationship may in fact be more important than the limitations or characteristics of the brother or sister with ASD. Thus, as the research literature on sibling relationships in the context of ASD continues to emerge, researchers are encouraged to consider methods such as multilevel modeling that allow for multiple perspectives within the family.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported in part by a grant from the National Institute on Aging (R01 AG08768) to Marsha Mailick and support from the Waisman Center Core Grant (U54 HD090256).
References
- Arnold CK, Heller T, & Kramer J (2012). Support needs of siblings of people with developmental disabilities. Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 50(5), 373–383. doi: 10.1352/1934-9556-50.5.373 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bedford VH (1989). A comparison of thematic apperceptions of sibling affiliation, conflict, and separation at two periods of adulthood. The International Journal of Aging & Human Development, 28(1), 53–66. doi: 10.2190/L5XM-HNGJ-4YTT-8KUT [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Begum G, & Blacher J (2011). The siblings relationship of adolescents with and without intellectual disabilities. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32(5), 1580–1588. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bengston VL, & Black KD (1973). Intergenerational relations and continuities in socialization In Baltes P & Schaie W (Eds.), Life-span development psychology: Personality and socialization (pp. 207–234). New York: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Bigby C (1997). Parental substitutes? The role of siblings in the lives of older people with intellectual disability. Journal of Gerontological Social Work, 29(1), 3–21. doi: 10.1300/J083v29n01_02 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Branje SJT, van Aken Marcel A. G., & van Lieshout Cornelis F. M. (2002). Relational support in families with adolescents. Journal of Family Psychology, 16(3), 351–362. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.16.3.351 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brody GH (1998). Sibling relationship quality: Its causes and consequences. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 1–24. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.49.1.1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bruininks RH, Woodcock RW, Weatherman RF, & Hill BK (1996). SIB-R. Scales of Independent Behavior-Revised. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- Burke MM, Fish T, & Lawton K (2015). A comparative analysis of adult siblings' perceptions toward caregiving. Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 53(2), 143–157. doi: 10.1352/1934-9556-53.2.143 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burke MM, Taylor JL, Urbano R, & Hodapp RM (2012). Predictors of future caregiving by adult siblings of individuals with intellectual and developmental disabilities. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 117(1), 33–47. doi: 10.1352/1944-7558-117.1.33 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carver CS, Sheier MF, & Weintraub JK (1989). Assessing coping strategies: A theoretically based approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 267–283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cicirelli VG (1989). Feelings of attachment to siblings and well-being in later life. Psychology and Aging, 4(2), 211–216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cicirelli VG (1991). Sibling relationships in adulthood. Marriage & Family Review, 16(3-4), 291–310. [Google Scholar]
- Connidis IA, & Campbell LD (1995). Closeness, confiding, and contact among siblings in middle and late adulthood. Journal of Family Issues, 16(6), 722–745. [Google Scholar]
- Cook WL, & Kenny DA (2004). Application of the social relations model to family assessment. Journal of Family Psychology, 18(2), 361–371. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.18.2.361 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cuskelly M (2016). Contributors to adult sibling relationships and intention to care of siblings of individuals with down syndrome. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 121(3), 204–218. doi: 10.1352/1944-7558-121.3.204 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daniels D, & Plomin R (1985). Differential experience of siblings in the same family. Developmental Psychology, 21(5), 747–760. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.21.5.747 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Davys D, Mitchell D, & Haigh C (2011). Adult sibling experience, roles, relationships and future concerns—A review of the literature in learning disabilities. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 20(19-20), 2837–2853. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2702.2010.03530.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davys D, Mitchell D, & Haigh C (2015). Futures planning—Adult sibling perspectives. British Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43(3), 219–226. doi: 10.1111/bld.12099 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Davys D, Mitchell D, & Haigh C (2016). Adult siblings consider the future: Emergent themes. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 29(3), 220–230. doi: 10.1111/jar.12172 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dew A, Llewellyn G, & Balandin S (2004). Post-parental care: A new generation of sibling-carers. Journal of Intellectual and Developmental Disability, 29(2), 176–179. doi: 10.1080/13668250410001709520 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Doody MA, Hastings RP, O’Neill S, & Grey IM (2010). Sibling relationships in adults who have siblings with or without intellectual disabilities. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 31(1), 224–231. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2009.09.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Floyd FJ, Costigan CL, & Richardson SS (2016). Sibling relationships in adolescence and early adulthood with people who have intellectual disability. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 121(5), 383–397. doi: 10.1352/1944-7558-121.5.383 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Floyd FJ, Purcell SE, Richardson SS, & Kupersmidt JB (2009). Sibling relationship quality and social functioning of children and adolescents with intellectual disability. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 114(2), 110–127. doi: 10.1352/2009.114.110-127 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friedrich WN, Greenberg MT, & Crnic K (1983). A short-form of the Questionnaire on Resources and Stress. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 88, 41–48. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilligan M, Suitor JJ, & Nam S (2015). Maternal differential treatment in later life families and within-family variations in adult sibling closeness. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 70B(1), 167–177. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbu148 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glutting J, Adams W, & Sheslow D (2000). Wide Range Intelligence Test. Wilmington, DE: Wide Range, Inc. [Google Scholar]
- Goetting A (1986). The developmental tasks of siblingship over the life cycle. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 48(4), 703–714. doi: 10.2307/352563 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Gold DT (1989). Generational solidarity: Conceptual antecedents and consequences. American Behavioral Scientist, 33(1), 19–32. doi: 10.1177/0002764289033001003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Greenberg JS, Seltzer MM, Orsmond GI, & Krauss MW (1999). Siblings of adults with mental illness or mental retardation: Current involvement and expectation of future caregiving. Psychiatric Services, 50(9), 1214–1219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hastings RP, & Petalas MA (2014). Self-reported behaviour problems and sibling relationship quality by siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder. Child: Care, Health and Development, 40(6), 833–839. doi: 10.1111/cch.12131 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heller T, & Arnold CK (2010). Siblings of adults with developmental disabilities: Psychosocial outcomes, relationships, and future planning. Journal of Policy and Practice in Intellectual Disabilities, 7(1), 16–25. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-1130.2010.00243.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hodapp RM, & Urbano RC (2007). Adult siblings of individuals with down syndrome versus with autism: Findings from a large-scale us survey. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 51(12), 1018–1029. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2007.00994.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hodapp RM, Glidden LM, & Kaiser AP (2005). Siblings of persons with disabilities: Toward a research agenda. Mental Retardation, 43(5), 334–338. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hodapp RM, Urbano RC, & Burke MM (2010). Adult female and male siblings of persons with disabilities: Findings from a national survey. Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 48(1), 52–62. doi: 10.1352/1934-9556-48.1.52 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jenkins JM, Dunn J, O'Connor TG, Rasbash J, & Behnke P (2005). Change in maternal perception of sibling negativity: Within- and between-family influences. Journal of Family Psychology, 19(4), 533–541. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.19.4.533 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jenkins J, Rasbash J, Leckie G, Gass K, & Dunn J (2012). The role of maternal factors in sibling relationship quality: A multilevel study of multiple dyads per family. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53(6), 622–629. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02484.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krull JL (2007). Using multilevel analyses with sibling data to increase analytic power: An illustration and simulation study. Developmental Psychology, 43(3), 602–619. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.3.602 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lord C, Rutter M, & Le Couteur A (1994). Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marciniak K (2017). Variance distribution in sibling relationships: Advantages of multilevel modeling using full sibling groups. Family Process, 56, 189–202. doi: 10.1111/famp.12157 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McHale SM, & Crouter AC (1996). The family contexts of children's sibling relationships. In Brody GH (Ed.), (pp. 173–195). Westport, CT, US: Ablex Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- Meadan H, Stoner JB, & Angell ME (2010). Review of literature related to the social, emotional, and behavioral adjustment of siblings of individuals with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 22(1), 83–100. doi: 10.1007/s10882-009-9171-7 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Milevsky A (2005). Compensatory patterns of sibling support in emerging adulthood: Variations in loneliness, self-esteem, depression and life satisfaction. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 22(6), 743–755. doi: 10.1177/0265407505056447 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- O'Connor TG, Dunn J, Jenkins JM, & Rasbash J (2006). Predictors of between-family and within-family variation in parent-child relationships. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(5), 498–510. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01527.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orsmond GI, Kuo H, & Seltzer MM (2009). Siblings of individuals with an autism spectrum disorder: Sibling relationships and wellbeing in adolescence and adulthood. Autism, 13(1), 59–80. doi: 10.1177/1362361308097119 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orsmond GI, & Seltzer MM (2000). Brothers and sisters of adults with mental retardation: Gendered nature of the sibling relationship. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 105(6), 486–508. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2000)1052.0.CO;2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orsmond GI, & Seltzer MM (2007a). Siblings of individuals with autism spectrum disorders across the life course. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 13(4), 313–320. doi: 10.1002/mrdd.20171 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orsmond GI, & Seltzer MM (2007b). Siblings of individuals with autism or Down syndrome: Effects on adult lives. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 51(9), 682–696. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2007.00954.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orsmond GI, & Seltzer MM (2009). Adolescent siblings of individuals with an autism spectrum disorder: Testing a diathesis-stress model of sibling well-being. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(7), 1053–1065. doi: 10.1007/s10803-009-0722-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orsmond GI, Seltzer MM, Krauss MW, & Hong J (2003). Behavior problems in adults with mental retardation and maternal well-being: Examination of the direction of effects. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 108(4), 257–271. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2003)1082.0.CO;2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Portner LC, & Riggs SA (2016). Sibling relationships in emerging adulthood: Associations with parent–child relationship. Journal of Child and Family Studies, doi: 10.1007/s10826-015-0358-5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Procidano ME, & Heller K (1983). Measures of perceived social support from friend and from family: Three validation studies. American Journal of Community Psychology, 11, 1–24. Doi: 10.1007/BF00898416. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Radloff LS (1991). The use of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale in adolescents and young adults. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 20, 149–166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rawson H (2010). 'I'm going to be here long after you've gone—Sibling perspectives of the future. British Journal of Learning Disabilities, 38(3), 225–231. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-3156.2009.00599.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Riggio HR (2006). Structural features of sibling dyads and attitudes toward sibling relationships in young adulthood. Journal of Family Issues, 27(9), 1233–1254. doi: 10.1177/0192513X06289103 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Seltzer MM, Greenberg JS, Taylor JL, Smith L, Orsmond GI, Esbensen A, & Hong J (2011). Adolescents and adults with autism spectrum disorders In Amaral DG, Dawson G & Geschwind DH (Eds.), Autism spectrum disorders (pp. 241–252). New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Seltzer MM, Krauss MW, Shattuck P, Orsmond GI, Swe A, & Lord C (2003). The symptoms of autism spectrum disorder in adolescence and adulthood. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, 565–581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sparrow SS, Carter AS, & Cicchetti DV (1993). Vineland Screener: Overview, reliability, validity, administration, and scoring. New Haven, CT: Yale University Child Study Center. [Google Scholar]
- Stocker CM, Lanthier RP, & Furman W (1997). Sibling relationships in early adulthood. Journal of Family Psychology, 11(2), 210–221. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.11.2.210 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Stoneman Z (2005). Siblings of children with disabilities: Research themes. Mental Retardation, 43(5), 339–350. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor JL, & Hodapp RM (2012). Doing nothing: Adults with disabilities with no daily activities and their siblings. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 117(1), 67–79. doi: 10.1352/1944-7558-117.1.67 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tomeny TS, Ellis BM, Rankin JA, & Barry TD (2017). Sibling relationship quality and psychosocial outcomes among adult siblings of individuals with autism spectrum disorder and individuals with intellectual disability without autism. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 62, 104–114. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2017.01.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tozer R, Atkin K, & Wenham A (2013). Continuity, commitment and context: Adult siblings of people with autism plus learning disability. Health & Social Care in the Community, 21(5), 480–488. doi: 10.1111/hsc.12034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]