Abstract
Objective:
To examine the contribution of anesthesia exposure during treatment for childhood medulloblastoma to neurocognitive outcomes 3 years after tumor diagnosis.
Study design:
In this retrospective study, anesthesia data were abstracted from medical records for 111 patients treated with risk-adapted protocol therapy at St. Jude Children’s Research Hospital. Neurocognitive testing data were obtained for 90.9% of patients.
Results:
For the 101 patients (62.4% male) who completed testing, mean age at diagnosis was 10.1 years and 74.3% were staged to have average-risk disease. Anesthesia exposure during treatment ranged from 1–52 events (mean = 19.9); mean cumulative duration per patient was 21.1 hours (range 0.7–59.7). Compared with normative expectations (16%), the group had a significantly higher frequency of at-risk scores (<1 SD) on measures of intelligence (28.7%), attention (35.2%), working memory (26.6%), processing speed (46.7%), and reading (25.8%). Including anesthesia exposure duration to linear regression models accounting for age at diagnosis, treatment intensity, and baseline IQ significantly increased the predicted variance for intelligence (r2=.59), attention (r2 =.29), working memory (r2=.31), processing speed (r2=.44), and reading (r2=.25; all P values <.001).
Conclusion:
In survivors of childhood medulloblastoma, a neurodevelopmentally vulnerable population, greater exposure to anesthesia significantly and independently predicts deficits in neurocognitive and academic functioning. When feasible, anesthesia exposure during treatment should be reduced.
Medulloblastoma is the most common malignant childhood brain tumor. Five-year survival rates are 70–85% with contemporary multi-modal therapy that includes surgery, risk-adapted craniospinal irradiation, and adjuvant chemotherapy. (1) Survivors are at risk for treatment-related cognitive and academic declines. (2–4) Disease and treatment-related risk factors, including higher intensity of central nervous system-directed therapy, younger age at diagnosis, and treatment-related sequelae (5–9), do not fully account for variance in outcomes.
The US Food and Drug Administration has issued a warning about repeated anesthesia exposure in young children (10), citing animal and human studies that suggest a potential deleterious impact on neurodevelopment, learning, and cognition. (11–18) Findings from preclinical studies show a relationship between anesthesia dosage and the extent of neuronal apoptosis (19, 20) and subsequent deficits in learning and memory (21–23), particularly for animals treated during critically sensitive neurodevelopmental periods. Data from retrospective studies in humans suggest that anesthesia exposure during neurodevelopmentally vulnerable periods of development may be associated with decreased academic outcomes (16), particularly with longer duration or multiple exposures (12, 13, 15, 17). Two prospective studies found no association between single anesthesia exposure of a short duration and subsequent academic achievement (24) or frequency of autism spectrum diagnosis. (25)
These findings suggest the potential for adverse consequences of anesthesia exposure in early childhood. However, limitations including birth cohort design, lack of clinical details about participants, poorly specified outcomes, and lack of variability in anesthesia exposure (ie, single exposure, relatively short duration) make interpretation challenging. Many studies also have focused on drugs that are no longer in frequent use, and thus findings are limited by treatment era effects.
Children treated for medulloblastoma receive anesthesia for procedures during therapy, including surgery, radiation therapy, and neuroimaging. The association between anesthesia exposure and neurocognitive outcomes in survivors of childhood medulloblastoma has not been previously investigated. Understanding the contribution of anesthesia to neurocognitive outcomes in this vulnerable population is critical, given their young age as well as disease and treatment-related impacts on central nervous system development. We examined the effect of anesthesia exposure during protocol therapy to neurocognitive outcomes in early survivorship.
Methods
A total of 327 patients 3–21 years old with histologically confirmed medulloblastoma were consecutively enrolled in an IRB-approved, multisite clinical trial for patients with newly diagnosed embryonal brain tumor between 9/2003 and 3/2013 (SJMB03; ClinicalTrials.gov: NCT00085202). The current study includes only the subgroup of patients enrolled and treated at St. Jude Children’s Research Hospital (n =155). We excluded 2 patients due to a medical course that included prolonged sedation in the context of intubation and mechanical ventilation. Anesthesia exposure data were abstracted for a total of 153 patients. Of these, 42 were not eligible for protocol-directed neurocognitive testing at 3 years post diagnosis (ie, did not consent, n=3; inadequate English proficiency or psychological or sensorimotor condition that precluded participation, n=12; off study or off treatment due to death or progressive disease, n=27). Of the 111 eligible patients, neurocognitive data were obtained for 101 patients (90.1%). Data were missing due to missed appointments due to medical status or patient refusal (n =10).
Patients underwent surgical resection and were classified as having average-risk medulloblastoma (≤1.5 cm2 residual tumor and no metastatic disease) or high-risk medulloblastoma (>1.5 cm2 residual disease and/or metastatic disease localized to the neuraxis), according to a modified Chang staging system. (26) Following enrollment on SJMB03, risk-adapted radiation therapy was initiated within 31 days of surgery. Radiation therapy for patients with high-risk disease included craniospinal irradiation (CSI; [M0–1, 36 Gy; M2–3, 36–39.6 Gy]) and supplemental photon irradiation to the tumor bed using conformal treatment methods (total dosage, 55.8 Gy). When appropriate, local sites of metastasis received supplemental photon irradiation (total dosage, 50.4–54 Gy). Patients with average-risk disease were treated with lower CSI (23.4 Gy) and supplemental conformal photon irradiation to the tumor bed (total dose, 55.8 Gy). The clinical target volume to the tumor bed was 1.0 cm for all patients. Following radiation therapy, at approximately 12 weeks post-treatment initiation, patients were treated with four cycles of high-dose chemotherapy (cyclophosphamide, cisplatin, and vincristine) with peripheral blood stem cell support.
Neurocognitive assessments were scheduled at baseline (after surgical resection and within 2 weeks of initiating radiation therapy), upon completion of radiation therapy, and annually for 5 years post-diagnosis. This study reports on outcomes obtained at 3 years post-diagnosis. Assessments were administered by psychological examiners or clinical research assistants under supervision of a licensed psychologist.
Neurocognitive outcomes included global intelligence (General Intellectual Ability), Broad Attention, Working Memory, and Processing Speed cluster scores from the Woodcock Johnson Tests of Cognitive Abilities, Third Edition, and the Broad Reading cluster score from the Woodcock Johnson Tests of Academic Achievement, Third Edition.(27, 28) All measures are normed on nationally representative data. Scores are age standardized with a population mean of 100 and standard deviation (SD) of 15. Lower scores indicate poorer performance. Standardized scores (SS) below the 16th percentile (SS ≤ 84) are considered at-risk. The selection of these outcomes from the broader neurocognitive assessment battery was empirically and theoretically driven. Specifically, we restricted the current analyses to include only those neurocognitive outcomes with group means and at-risk frequencies that significantly differed from normative expectations at p ≤ .01 (Table I; available at www.jpeds.com). These neurocognitive domains have been shown to particularly vulnerable to in studies of survivors of childhood medulloblastoma (5, 6, 8, 29).
Table 1 - Online.
N | Mean | SD | Mean difference | pa | % At Riskb | pc | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Woodcock Johnson Tests of Cognitive Abilities, 3rd Edition (SS) | |||||||
General Intellectual Ability | 94 | 94.9 | 17.9 | 5.0 | 0.0080 | 28.7 | 0.0005 |
Broad Attention | 88 | 89.1 | 17.9 | 10.9 | < 0.0001 | 35.2 | < 0.0001 |
Working Memory | 94 | 93.6 | 16.9 | 6.4 | 0.0004 | 26.6 | 0.0027 |
Processing Speed | 92 | 82.2 | 22.2 | 17.8 | < 0.0001 | 46.7 | < 0.0001 |
Woodcock Johnson Tests of Achievement, 3rd Edition (SS) | |||||||
Broad Reading | 89 | 93.7 | 14.8 | 6.3 | < 0.0001 | 25.8 | 0.0064 |
Broad Math | 93 | 93.9 | 15.8 | 6.0 | 0.0004 | 21.5 | 0.1017 |
Continuous Performance Test, 2nd Edition (T) | |||||||
Omissions | 79 | 53.3 | 17.9 | 3.3 | 0.1083 | 15.2 | 0.7850 |
Hit Reaction Time | 79 | 53.4 | 12.9 | 3.4 | 0.0244 | 25.3 | 0.0141 |
Variability | 79 | 52.6 | 9.5 | 2.6 | 0.0164 | 21.5 | 0.1017 |
Discriminability | 79 | 53.5 | 9.6 | 3.5 | 0.0017 | 25.3 | 0.0141 |
Response Style | 79 | 50.9 | 9.9 | 0.9 | 0.4339 | 13.9 | 0.5854 |
Child Behavior Checklist (T) | |||||||
Attention Problems | 81 | 56.0 | 7.8 | 6.0 | < 0.0001 | 24.7 | 0.0141 |
Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (T) | |||||||
Global Executive Composite | 82 | 51.8 | 12.4 | 1.8 | 0.1949 | 26.8 | 0.0027 |
Abbreviations: SS = Standard Score, T = T-score; SD = standard deviation. Standard Score population mean = 100, SD = 15; T-score population mean = 50, SD = 10. Bold font = statistically significant at p ≤ 0.001.
two-tailed p-value from a one-sample t-test comparing the group mean to normative expectations.
frequency of scores 1 SD outside the normative mean.
two-sided p-value from frequency comparison (chi-Square) of the distribution of at-risk and not at-risk scores (percentage).
Medical record abstraction was performed to capture data for all anesthesia exposure events occurring between the date of study enrollment and one-year follow-up. Variables included the indication (procedure), agents, and cumulative dosages and total anesthesia duration. Records and coding were reviewed by a board-certified pediatric anesthesiologist and occurred between October 2015 and May 2016.
Statistical Analysis
Descriptive statistics were performed to characterize the overall group on relevant demographic and clinical variables. Frequency or mean comparisons (ie, Fisher exact test or independent samples t-test) were used to compare groups with and without neurocognitive data on relevant demographic and clinical variables to establish representativeness. Descriptive statistics were used to characterize anesthesia exposure and neurocognitive outcomes. Group mean neurocognitive scores were compared with normative data using a one-sample t-test. Chi-square was used to compare the frequency of at-risk scores in the group to the expected frequency in the normative population. Univariable methods were used to assess for the association between anesthesia exposure and established risk factors for neurocognitive effects. Bivariate correlation was used to examine the association between age at diagnosis and anesthesia exposure. One-way ANOVA was used to check if anesthesia exposure varied by treatment risk arm.
Linear regression was used to examine the factors predictive of neurocognitive outcomes. For the overall group (n=101), two models were tested for each neurocognitive outcome. First, we examined the association of known risk factors with neurocognitive outcomes (y =age at diagnosis + treatment risk arm). Anesthesia exposure was added to the second model (y=age at diagnosis + treatment risk arm + cumulative duration of anesthesia exposure). The variable of baseline intellectual functioning (ie, pre-treatment General Intellectual Ability score) was added to models that included the subset of patients who completed neurocognitive testing at study baseline (y=age at diagnosis + treatment risk arm + baseline IQ; y=age at diagnosis + treatment risk arm + baseline IQ + cumulative duration of anesthesia exposure). For both sets of models, we used ANOVA to compare the variance between the two models, to test the hypothesis that the full model adds explanatory value over the reduced model.
To explore the impact of surgery and complexity of medical course on our findings, we performed a secondary case-control analysis comparing neurocognitive outcomes between patients with posterior fossa syndrome (PFS) and a sample matched for sex, age at diagnosis, and treatment risk arm (1 case: 4 controls). Unless otherwise specified, all tests of statistical significance were two-sided. Data were analyzed using SAS® software, Version 9.4.
Results
Patient Demographic and Clinical Characteristics
Demographic and clinical characteristics for the overall group of patients eligible for neurocognitive testing (N=111) are presented in Table 2. The mean age at diagnosis was 10.0 years, 60.4% of patients were male, and 74.8% were treated for average-risk disease.
Table 2.
Overall Group N = 111 | Completed Testing n = 101 | No Testing n = 10 | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
n | % | n | % | n | % | pa | ||
Sex | Male | 67 | 60.4 | 63 | 62.4 | 4 | 40.0 | 0.168 |
Female | 44 | 39.6 | 38 | 37.6 | 6 | 60.0 | ||
Race | White | 83 | 74.8 | 77 | 76.2 | 6 | 60.0 | |
Black | 15 | 13.5 | 13 | 12.9 | 2 | 20.0 | ||
Asian | 5 | 4.5 | 4 | 4 | 1 | 10.0 | ||
Mixed Race, Other | 8 | 7.2 | 7 | 6.9 | 1 | 10.0 | ||
Risk Arm | Average | 83 | 74.8 | 75 | 74.3 | 8 | 80.0 | 0.690 |
High | 28 | 25.2 | 26 | 25.7 | 2 | 20.0 | ||
Posterior Fossa Syndrome | Yes | 22 | 19.8 | 20 | 19.8 | 2 | 20.0 | 0.988 |
No | 89 | 80.2 | 81 | 80.2 | 8 | 80.0 | ||
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | Mean | SD | |||
Age at Diagnosis | 10.0 | 4.5 | 10.1 | 4.5 | 9.0 | 4.6 | 0.452 | |
Anesthesia Exposure | Cumulative Frequency | 19.9 | 16.0 | 19.8 | 16.1 | 20.4 | 15.7 | 0.775 |
Cumulative Duration | 21.3 | 15.8 | 21.1 | 15.9 | 22.6 | 15.9 | 0.914 |
Abbreviations: SD = standard deviation. Age is reported in years and months. Anesthesia exposure duration reported in hours.
Notes:
two-sided p-values from statistical comparisons between groups with and without neurocognitive testing (Chi-square or independent samples t-test).
A total of 101 out of 111 eligible patients completed neurocognitive testing at 3 years post-diagnosis. On average, patients were 10.1 years old at diagnosis (SD=4.5) and 13.1 years old at assessment (SD=4.5). In those who completed testing, 62.4% were male and 74.3% were treated for average-risk disease. There were no significant differences between the groups with and without neurocognitive testing on the distribution of sex (male/female), treatment risk arm (average/high), or posterior fossa syndrome (yes/no). There were no significant differences between the group that completed testing and the group that did not complete testing on age at diagnosis or cumulative anesthesia exposure.
Of the 101 patients who completed neurocognitive testing at 3 years post diagnosis, 70 also had completed an assessment of global intellectual functioning prior to the initiation of radiation therapy (69.3%). Compared with the group who underwent baseline testing, patients without baseline testing data were significantly younger at diagnosis (mean[SD], 11.1[4.7] vs 7.9[3.1], p = .001) and had significantly greater cumulative anesthesia frequency (14.4[13.1] vs 31.9[15.7], p ≤.001) and duration (15.8[13.1] vs 33.1[15.2], p ≤ .001. There was no significant difference between the groups with and without baseline testing with regard to sex (P = .236) or risk-arm distribution (P = .992).
Anesthesia Exposure for the Group Completing Neurocognitive Testing
The mean cumulative frequency of anesthesia exposure per patient was 19.9 events (SD 16.1, range 1–52) and the mean cumulative duration was 21.1 hours (SD 15.9, range 0.7–59.7). Anesthesia exposure indications included procedures (eg, lumbar puncture, bone marrow harvest, and placement of central lines), imaging (MRI or CT), and radiation therapy. Radiation therapy was the most common indication for anesthesia (52.9% of all recorded events; 42.6% of patients completing at least one fraction with anesthesia), followed by imaging (23.9% of all recorded events, 86.1% of patients completing at least one scan with anesthesia). Anesthesia was administered via inhalation, intravenous, or mixed (inhalation and intravenous) routes, depending on procedure.
The frequency of patients receiving each anesthetic agent and the cumulative dose of each agent are shown in Table 3. The most commonly used inhaled anesthetic was sevoflurane, with 91.9% of patients receiving it at least once. For intravenous administration, the most common agents were propofol and fentanyl, with 100% of patients receiving these agents at least once. Over all procedures, 2 to 6 unique agents were used. For inhaled agents, the average number per patient was 1.56. For intravenous agents, the average number per patient was 3.36.
Table 3.
Patients receiving agent | Cumulative Exposure per Patient | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
n | % | Mean | SD | Median | Min | Max | ||
Inhaled Agents | Sevoflurane | 92 | 91.09 | 1.83 | 1.27 | 2.00 | 0.00 | 6.00 |
Nitrus Oxide | 47 | 46.53 | 0.62 | 0.85 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 4.00 | |
Isoflurane | 15 | 14.85 | 0.18 | 0.48 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 3.00 | |
Desflurane | 5 | 4.95 | 0.05 | 0.22 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 1.00 | |
Halothane | 1 | 0.99 | 0.01 | 0.10 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 1.00 | |
Patients receiving agent | Cumulative Dose per Patient | |||||||
Intravenous Agents | Propofol (mg/kg) | 101 | 100 | 6169.92 | 5345.35 | 4055.00 | 150.00 | 24846.00 |
Fentanyl (mcg/kg) | 101 | 100 | 327.11 | 297.28 | 200.00 | 50.00 | 1465.00 | |
Midazolam (mg/kg) | 61 | 60.4 | 5.87 | 11.37 | 1.00 | 0.00 | 70.00 | |
Morphine (mg/kg) | 27 | 26.73 | 2.95 | 11.64 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 85.00 | |
Pentobarbitol (mg/kg) | 18 | 17.82 | 53.32 | 141.56 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 910.00 | |
Meperidine (mg/kg) | 13 | 12.87 | 13.58 | 48.16 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 380.00 | |
Ketamine (mg/kg) | 9 | 8.91 | 30.55 | 130.87 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 769.61 | |
Lorazepam (mg/kg) | 5 | 4.95 | 0.07 | 0.38 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 3.00 | |
Hydromorphone (mg/kg) | 2 | 1.98 | 0.02 | 0.20 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 2.00 | |
Clonidine (mg/kg) | 1 | 0.99 | 0.00 | 0.01 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.10 | |
Dexmedetomidine (mg/kg) | 0 | 0 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.00 |
N = 101. Abbreviations: SD = standard deviation; Min = minimum; Max = Maximum; mg = milligram; mcg = microgram; kg = kilogram
Results from bivariate correlation analysis showed that anesthesia exposure varied significantly by age at diagnosis, with younger age predicting longer exposure duration (r =−0.65, p <.0001). Results from one-way ANOVA showed that anesthesia exposure varied significantly by treatment risk arm. Cumulative frequency was greater for patients treated for high-risk disease (mean =24.8 events, SD = 17.6) compared with the group treated for average-risk disease (mean events =18.1, SD = 15.3, p=.07), but this difference did not reach statistical significance. Cumulative duration was significantly greater for the high-risk group (mean = 27.7 hours, SD=18.8) compared with the average-risk group (mean = 18.9 hours, SD = 14.1, p =.014).
Neurocognitive Outcomes at Three Years Post Diagnosis
Descriptive statistics for neurocognitive outcomes are shown in Table 4. Group mean scores were within the average to low-average range for all measures. Results from one-sample t-tests showed that group means were significantly different from normative expectations at p < 0.01. Group means were significantly lower on measures of global intelligence (94.9, SD = 17.9, p =.008), attention (89.1, SD = 17.9, p <.0001), working memory (93.6, SD = 16.9, p =.0004), processing speed (82.2, SD = 22.2, p < 0.0001), and reading (93.7, SD = 14.8, p =.0001). Compared with normative expectations (16%), the group had a significantly greater frequency of at-risk scores on measures of global intelligence (at-risk =28.7%, p =.0005), attention (at-risk =35.2%, p <.0001), working memory (at-risk =26.6% p =.0027), processing speed (at-risk =46.7%, p <.0001), and reading (at-risk =25.8%, p =.0064).
Table 4.
N | Mean | SD | Mean difference | pa | % At Riskb | pc | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Woodcock Johnson Tests of Cognitive Abilities, 3rd edition (SS) | |||||||
General Intellectual Ability | 94 | 94.9 | 17.9 | 5.0 | 0.0080 | 28.7 | 0.0005 |
Broad Attention | 88 | 89.1 | 17.9 | 10.9 | < 0.0001 | 35.2 | < 0.0001 |
Working Memory | 94 | 93.6 | 16.9 | 6.4 | 0.0004 | 26.6 | 0.0027 |
Processing Speed | 92 | 82.2 | 22.2 | 17.8 | < 0.0001 | 46.7 | < 0.0001 |
Woodcock Johnson Tests of Achievement , 3rd edition (SS) | |||||||
Broad Reading | 89 | 93.7 | 14.8 | 6.3 | < 0.0001 | 25.8 | 0.0064 |
Abbreviations: SS = Standard Score, population mean = 100, SD = 15; Bold font = statistically significant at p ≤ 0.001.
two-tailed p-value from a one-sample t-test comparing the group mean to normative expectations
frequency of scores 1 SD outside the normative mean
two-sided p-value from frequency comparison (Chi-Square) of the distribution of at-risk and not at-risk scores (percentage).
Anesthesia exposure during therapy predicts neurocognitive outcomes three years post diagnosis
Compared with the model using predictors of age at diagnosis and treatment risk arm, the model using age at diagnosis, treatment risk arm, and cumulative duration of anesthesia exposure predicted a significantly greater amount of variance in global intelligence (r2 =0.05, 0.20; p<.0001), attention (r2 =0.04, 0.14, p=.0016), working memory (r2 =0.02, 0.17, p <.0001), processing speed (r2 =0.09, 0.19, p=.0009), and reading ability (r2 =0.01, 0.10, p=.0023; Table 5 [available at www.jpeds.com]). Compared with the model incorporating predictors of age at diagnosis, treatment risk arm, and baseline IQ, the model with age at diagnosis, treatment risk arm, baseline IQ, and cumulative duration of anesthesia exposure predicted a significantly greater amount of variance in global intelligence (r2 =0.05, 0.20; p<.0001), attention (r2 =0.04, 0.14, p=.0016), working memory (r2 =0.02, 0.17, p <.0001), processing speed (r2 =0.09, 0.19, p=.0009), and reading ability (r2 =0.01, 0.10, p=.0023; Table 6).
Table 5 – Online.
Outcome | Model | Variables | Parameter Estimate | SE | T | P (a) | R (2) Adjusted | R (2) Change | F-Value | p (b) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
General Intellectual Ability | 1 | Age at Diagnosis | 0.89 | 0.41 | 2.15 | 0.0345 | 0.05 | 0.15 | 18.96 | <.0001 |
Risk Arm | −4.93 | 4.14 | −1.19 | 0.2377 | ||||||
2 | Age at Diagnosis | −0.45 | 0.49 | −0.93 | 0.3566 | 0.20 | ||||
Risk Arm | −0.83 | 3.90 | −0.21 | 0.8314 | ||||||
Anesthesia Duration | −0.62 | 0.14 | −4.35 | < 0.0001 | ||||||
Broad Attention | 1 | Age at Diagnosis | 0.84 | 0.43 | 1.98 | 0.0507 | 0.04 | 0.10 | 10.61 | 0.0016 |
Risk Arm | −4.58 | 4.35 | −1.05 | 0.2963 | ||||||
2 | Age at Diagnosis | −0.34 | 0.54 | −0.62 | 0.5341 | 0.14 | ||||
Risk Arm | −1.27 | 4.03 | −0.30 | 0.7653 | ||||||
Anesthesia Duration | −0.54 | 0.17 | −3.26 | 0.0016 | ||||||
Working Memory | 1 | Age at Diagnosis | 0.66 | 0.40 | 1.65 | 0.102 | 0.02 | 0.15 | 18.09 | <.0001 |
Risk Arm | −0.32 | 3.93 | −0.82 | 0.417 | ||||||
2 | Age at Diagnosis | −0.61 | 0.47 | −1.29 | 0.199 | 0.17 | ||||
Risk Arm | 0.21 | 3.69 | 0.06 | 0.9542 | ||||||
Anesthesia Duration | −0.59 | 0.14 | −4.25 | <0.0001 | ||||||
Processing Speed | 1 | Age at Diagnosis | 1.66 | 0.51 | 3.26 | 0.0016 | 0.09 | 0.10 | 11.86 | 0.0009 |
Risk Arm | −0.06 | 5.21 | −0.01 | 0.9908 | ||||||
2 | Age at Diagnosis | 0.31 | 0.62 | 0.50 | 0.6189 | 0.19 | ||||
Risk Arm | 3.77 | 5.04 | 0.75 | 0.4565 | ||||||
Anesthesia Duration | −0.63 | 0.18 | −3.44 | 0.0009 | ||||||
Broad Reading | 1 | Age at Diagnosis | 0.32 | 0.36 | 0.89 | 0.3765 | 0.01 | 0.09 | 9.87 | 0.0023 |
Risk Arm | −4.57 | 3.59 | −1.27 | 0.2067 | ||||||
2 | Age at Diagnosis | −0.35 | 0.40 | −0.88 | 0.3829 | 0.10 | ||||
Risk Arm | −2.48 | 3.48 | −0.71 | 0.4773 | ||||||
Anesthesia Duration | −0.39 | 0.12 | −3.14 | 0.0023 |
Abbreviations: SE = Standard Error. Bold font = statistically significant at p ≤ 0.05.
2-sided p-value from comparison of parameter estimate to 0.
2-sided p-value from ANOVA comparison of regression models. Anesthesia duration is measured in hours.
Table 6.
Outcome | Model | Variables | Parameter Estimate | SE | T | p (a) | R (2) Adjusted | R (2) Change | F-value | p (b) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
General Intellectual Ability | 1 | Age at Diagnosis | 1.08 | 0.32 | 3.34 | 0.0014 | 0.50 | |||
Risk Arm | −12.04 | 3.44 | −3.51 | 0.0009 | ||||||
Baseline IQ | 0.78 | 0.10 | 7.81 | <.0001 | ||||||
2 | Age at Diagnosis | 0.04 | 0.39 | 0.11 | 0.9135 | 0.59 | 0.09 | 15.51 | 0.0002 | |
Risk Arm | −7.04 | 3.34 | −2.11 | 0.0390 | ||||||
Baseline IQ | 0.71 | 0.09 | 7.77 | <.0001 | ||||||
Anesthesia Duration | −0.55 | 0.14 | −3.94 | 0.0002 | ||||||
Broad Attention | 1 | Age at Diagnosis | 0.92 | 0.41 | 2.23 | 0.0293 | 0.21 | |||
Risk Arm | −9.60 | 4.66 | −2.06 | 0.0440 | ||||||
Baseline IQ | 0.54 | 0.13 | 4.12 | 0.0001 | ||||||
2 | Age at Diagnosis | −0.04 | 0.54 | −0.08 | 0.9367 | 0.29 | 0.09 | 6.93 | 0.0109 | |
Risk Arm | −5.91 | 4.66 | −1.27 | 0.2100 | ||||||
Baseline IQ | 0.51 | 0.13 | 4.08 | 0.0001 | ||||||
Anesthesia Duration | −0.53 | 0.20 | −2.63 | 0.0109 | ||||||
Working Memory | 1 | Age at Diagnosis | 0.64 | 0.39 | 1.63 | 0.1082 | 0.20 | |||
Risk Arm | −8.47 | 4.15 | −2.04 | 0.0459 | ||||||
Baseline IQ | 0.51 | 0.12 | 4.24 | <.0001 | ||||||
2 | Age at Diagnosis | −0.46 | 0.49 | −0.94 | 0.3512 | 0.31 | 0.11 | 11.11 | 0.0015 | |
Risk Arm | −3.53 | 4.12 | −0.86 | 0.3949 | ||||||
Baseline IQ | 0.43 | 0.11 | 3.84 | 0.0003 | ||||||
Anesthesia Duration | −0.59 | 0.18 | −3.33 | 0.0015 | ||||||
Processing Speed | 1 | Age at Diagnosis | 1.99 | 0.36 | 5.52 | <.0001 | 0.41 | |||
Risk Arm | −7.87 | 4.03 | −1.95 | 0.0554 | ||||||
Baseline IQ | 0.58 | 0.11 | 5.12 | <.0001 | ||||||
2 | Age at Diagnosis | 1.33 | 0.48 | 2.79 | 0.0071 | 0.44 | 0.03 | 4.08 | 0.0480 | |
Risk Arm | −5.19 | 4.15 | −1.25 | 0.2153 | ||||||
Baseline IQ | 0.55 | 0.11 | 4.98 | <.0001 | ||||||
Anesthesia Duration | −0.36 | 0.18 | −2.02 | 0.0480 | ||||||
Broad Reading | 1 | Age at Diagnosis | 0.48 | 0.31 | 1.54 | 0.1294 | 0.20 | |||
Risk Arm | −8.55 | 3.36 | −2.55 | 0.0133 | ||||||
Baseline IQ | 0.39 | 0.10 | 4.06 | 0.0001 | ||||||
2 | Age at Diagnosis | −0.11 | 0.41 | −0.27 | 0.7905 | 0.25 | 0.05 | 4.59 | 0.0362 | |
Risk Arm | −6.20 | 3.44 | −1.80 | 0.0765 | ||||||
Baseline IQ | 0.34 | 0.10 | 3.61 | 0.0006 | ||||||
Anesthesia Duration | −0.34 | 0.16 | −2.14 | 0.0362 |
Abbreviations: SE = Standard Error Bold font = statistically significant at p ≤ 0.05.
2-sided p-value from comparison of parameter estimate to 0.
2-sided p-value from ANOVA comparison of regression models. Anesthesia duration is measured in hours.
Models including cumulative duration of anesthesia exposure, baseline IQ, age at diagnosis, and treatment risk arm significantly predicted scores on measures of intelligence (r2 =0.50, 0.59, p =.0002), attention (r2 =0.21, 0.29, p =.0109), working memory (r2 =0.20, 0.31, p =.0015), processing speed (r2 =0.41, 0.44, p =.0480), and reading (r2 =0.20, 0.25. p =.0362).
Given preclinical data that suggests younger patients may be at greatest risk from anesthesia exposure, we examined the interaction between age at diagnosis and anesthesia exposure. After accounting for variability from age at diagnosis, risk arm, baseline IQ, and cumulative anesthesia duration, the interaction term was not statistically significant for any of the modeled outcomes (Table 7; available at www.jpeds.com).
Table 7 – Online.
Outcome | Variables | Parameter Estimate | SE | T | p | R (2) Adjusted |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
General Intellectual Ability | Age at Diagnosis | −0.08 | 0.49 | −0.16 | 0.8761 | 0.62 |
Risk Arm | −7.07 | 3.36 | −2.10 | 0.0396 | ||
Baseline IQ | 0.71 | 0.09 | 7.64 | <.0001 | ||
Anesthesia Duration | −0.65 | 0.29 | −2.29 | 0.0256 | ||
Anesthesia × Age at Diagnosis | 0.02 | 0.04 | 0.42 | 0.6786 | ||
Broad Attention | Age at Diagnosis | −0.09 | 0.67 | −0.13 | 0.8966 | 0.33 |
Risk Arm | −5.87 | 4.71 | −1.25 | 0.2181 | ||
Baseline IQ | 0.51 | 0.13 | 4.03 | 0.0002 | ||
Anesthesia Duration | −0.57 | 0.40 | −1.43 | 0.1578 | ||
Anesthesia Duration × Age at Diagnosis | 0.01 | 0.06 | 0.11 | 0.9096 | ||
Working Memory | Age at Diagnosis | −0.64 | 0.61 | −1.05 | 0.2996 | 0.36 |
Risk Arm | −3.62 | 4.15 | −0.87 | 0.3874 | ||
Baseline IQ | 0.44 | 0.12 | 3.84 | 0.0003 | ||
Anesthesia Duration | −0.74 | 0.35 | −2.08 | 0.0418 | ||
Anesthesia Duration × Age at Diagnosis | 0.02 | 0.05 | 0.49 | 0.6241 | ||
Processing Speed | Age at Diagnosis | 1.62 | 0.59 | 2.75 | 0.0080 | 0.48 |
Risk Arm | −5.42 | 4.17 | −1.30 | 0.1980 | ||
Baseline IQ | 0.54 | 0.11 | 4.85 | <.0001 | ||
Anesthesia Duration | −0.10 | 0.35 | −0.29 | 0.7731 | ||
Anesthesia Duration × Age at Diagnosis | −0.04 | 0.05 | −0.84 | 0.4050 | ||
Broad Reading | Age at Diagnosis | −0.30 | 0.52 | −0.58 | 0.5648 | 0.30 |
Risk Arm | −6.32 | 3.46 | −1.82 | 0.0731 | ||
Baseline IQ | 0.35 | 0.10 | 3.64 | 0.0006 | ||
Anesthesia Duration | −0.51 | 0.33 | −1.56 | 0.1252 | ||
Anesthesia Duration × Age at Diagnosis | 0.02 | 0.04 | 0.60 | 0.5506 |
Abbreviations: SE = Standard Error. Bold font = statistically significant at p ≤ 0.05. 2-sided p-value from comparison of parameter estimate to 0. Anesthesia duration is measured in hours
Exploratory analyses
Demographic and clinical characteristics for subgroups of patients with and without posterior fossa syndrome are shown in Table 8 (available at www.jpeds.com). The groups did not significantly differ with regard to distribution of sex, distribution of risk arm, mean age at diagnosis, or mean age at assessment. Compared with the group who had posterior fossa syndrome (n = 20), patients without posterior fossa syndrome (n = 81) had a significantly higher cumulative duration of anesthesia (p=<.0001). Results from a case-control analysis comparing patients with and without posterior fossa syndrome are shown in Table 9 (available at www.jpeds.com). Analyses are adjusted for anesthesia exposure (cumulative duration, measured in hours). Compared with the group having posterior fossa syndrome, patients without posterior fossa syndrome had significantly higher scores on measures of intelligence (p =0.0359) and processing speed (p =0.0219). For the group without posterior fossa syndrome, after adjusting for age at diagnosis, risk arm, and baseline IQ, increased duration of anesthesia significantly and independently predicted variability on all neurocognitive and academic outcomes (Table 10; available at www.jpeds.com). These results suggest that the adverse impact of anesthesia exposure on neurocognitive outcomes is evident in patients without posterior fossa syndrome, and account for a similar magnitude of variance in outcomes.
Table 8 – Online.
PFS-yes | PFS-no | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
n = 20 | n = 81 | |||||
n | % | n | % | P* | ||
Sex | Male | 13 | 65.0 | 50 | 61.7 | 0.787 |
Female | 7 | 35.0 | 31 | 38.3 | ||
Risk Arm | Average | 14 | 70.0 | 61 | 75.3 | 0.627 |
High | 6 | 30.0 | 20 | 24.7 | ||
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | p** | ||
Age at Diagnosis | 8.6 | 11.6 | 10.5 | 4.7 | 0.082 | |
Cumulative Anesthesia Duration | 33.7 | 16.1 | 18.0 | 14.3 | <.0001 | |
Age at Testing | 11.6 | 3.2 | 13.5 | 4.7 | 0.095 |
PFS = posterior fossa syndrome.
2-sided p-value from chi-square frequency comparison;
2-sided p-value from two-sample t-test. Bold font = statistically significant at p ≤ 0.05. Anesthesia duration is measured in hours.
Table 9 - Online.
PFS-yes | PFS-no | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | F | pa | |
General Cognitive Ability | 80.89 | 14.27 | 97.47 | 19.90 | 4.59 | 0.0359 |
Broad Attention | 75.50 | 16.89 | 92.00 | 20.87 | 2.56 | 0.1147 |
Working Memory | 85.17 | 14.90 | 96.57 | 19.50 | 1.18 | 0.2816 |
Processing Speed | 60.00 | 24.59 | 82.04 | 23.07 | 5.51 | 0.0219 |
Broad Reading | 86.29 | 13.89 | 95.40 | 18.41 | 1.20 | 0.2781 |
Abbreviations: PFS = posterior fossa syndrome; SD = standard deviation
2-tailed p-value from ANCOVA models controlling for duration of anesthesia exposure
Table 10 – Online.
Outcome | Model | Variables | Parameter Estimate | SE | T | P (a) | R (2) Adjusted | R (2)Change |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
General Intellectual Ability | 1 | Age at Diagnosis | 0.99 | 0.34 | 2.95 | 0.0046 | 0.49 | |
Risk Arm | −12.90 | 3.56 | −3.62 | 0.0006 | ||||
Baseline IQ | 0.76 | 0.10 | 7.51 | <.0001 | ||||
2 | Age at Diagnosis | −0.09 | 0.41 | −0.23 | 0.8201 | 0.59 | 0.10 | |
Risk Arm | −7.12 | 3.51 | −2.03 | 0.0472 | ||||
Baseline IQ | 0.68 | 0.09 | 7.36 | <.0001 | ||||
Anesthesia Duration | −0.57 | 0.15 | −3.92 | 0.0002 | ||||
Broad Attention | 1 | Age at Diagnosis | 0.80 | 0.43 | 1.86 | 0.0687 | 0.19 | |
Risk Arm | −10.34 | 4.86 | −2.13 | 0.0378 | ||||
Baseline IQ | 0.51 | 0.13 | 3.83 | 0.0003 | ||||
2 | Age at Diagnosis | −0.24 | 0.56 | −0.43 | 0.6687 | 0.28 | 0.09 | |
Risk Arm | −5.75 | 4.89 | −1.17 | 0.2452 | ||||
Baseline IQ | 0.47 | 0.13 | 3.72 | 0.0005 | ||||
Anesthesia Duration | −0.57 | 0.21 | −2.72 | 0.0089 | ||||
Working Memory | 1 | Age at Diagnosis | 0.52 | 0.41 | 1.27 | 0.2085 | 0.19 | |
Risk Arm | −9.74 | 4.27 | −2.28 | 0.0264 | ||||
2 | Baseline IQ Age at Diagnosis | 0.49 | 0.12 | 4.02 | 0.0002 | 0.32 | 0.12 | |
−0.64 | 0.51 | −1.25 | 0.2159 | |||||
Risk Arm | −4.04 | 4.29 | −0.94 | 0.3498 | ||||
Baseline IQ | 0.40 | 0.11 | 3.53 | 0.0008 | ||||
Anesthesia Duration | −0.60 | 0.18 | −3.34 | 0.0015 | ||||
Processing Speed | 1 | Age at Diagnosis | 1.91 | 0.38 | 5.05 | <.0001 | 0.38 | |
Risk Arm | −7.11 | 4.25 | −1.67 | 0.0999 | ||||
Baseline IQ | 0.57 | 0.12 | 4.83 | <.0001 | ||||
2 | Age at Diagnosis Risk Arm | 1.22 | 0.50 | 2.42 | 0.0187 | 0.41 | 0.03 | |
−3.85 | 4.44 | −0.87 | 0.3894 | |||||
Baseline IQ | 0.53 | 0.12 | 4.64 | <.0001 | ||||
Anesthesia Duration | −0.38 | 0.19 | −2.02 | 0.0482 | ||||
Broad Reading | 1 | Age at Diagnosis | 0.53 | 0.33 | 1.59 | 0.1181 | 0.19 | |
Risk Arm | −7.42 | 3.51 | −2.11 | 0.0392 | ||||
Baseline IQ | 0.39 | 0.10 | 3.96 | 0.0002 | ||||
2 | Age at Diagnosis Risk Arm | −0.22 | 0.43 | −0.51 | 0.6115 | 0.27 | 0.08 | |
−4.07 | 3.59 | −1.13 | 0.2618 | |||||
Baseline IQ | 0.33 | 0.10 | 3.39 | 0.0013 | ||||
Anesthesia Duration | −0.42 | 0.16 | −2.58 | 0.0125 |
N = 81. Abbreviations: SE = Standard Error Bold font = statistically significant at p ≤ 0.05.
2-sided p-value from comparison of parameter estimate to 0. Anesthesia duration is measured in hours.
Discussion
The current study examined the contribution of anesthesia exposure during protocol-directed treatment for pediatric medulloblastoma to neurocognitive outcomes obtained three years post diagnosis. Anesthesia data were well-characterized and reflective of current practice. Neurocognitive data were obtained prospectively, and the test battery includes measures with established reliability, validity, and clinical utility.
This study yielded novel data characterizing anesthesia exposure during protocol-directed treatment for pediatric medulloblastoma. Over a 12-month period, patients were exposed to general anesthesia an average of 19 times, for an average cumulative duration of over 21 hours. Younger patients received general anesthesia more frequently and for a longer cumulative duration. This finding is notable, as studies from the general childhood population suggest that younger children are at the highest risk for anesthesia-related problems with learning and cognition. Radiation therapy treatment accounted for the majority of cumulative anesthesia exposures across the entire group; however, nearly half of all patients completed all radiation therapy treatments without anesthesia, suggesting that radiation therapy may be a potential target for behavioral interventions focused on reducing the need for anesthesia exposure during treatment.
This study examines the contribution of anesthesia exposure during treatment in survivors of childhood brain tumor, a neurodevelopmentally vulnerable population at significant risk for disease and treatment-related neurocognitive deficits. Findings from a recently published retrospective study examining the association between anesthesia exposure during therapy for childhood acute leukemia and neurocognitive outcomes in survivorship are similar to the current study (30). Notably, patients in that prior study were significantly older at diagnosis (14 years old). Together, these findings from childhood cancer survivors suggest that the period of neurodevelopmental vulnerability may be significantly longer in pediatric medical populations (ie, the most likely to have higher exposure to anesthesia).
Our findings on neurocognitive outcomes three years post diagnosis are largely consistent with prior studies in survivors of pediatric medulloblastoma. (6–9) Three years after study enrollment, the overall group of survivors had an elevated frequency of at-risk scores on measures of neurocognitive and academic skills compared with normative expectations, with specific areas of vulnerability including attention, processing speed, and working memory. Younger age at diagnosis was a risk factor for lower scores and higher ratings of problem behavior in daily life across nearly all measured domains.
Our finding that neurocognitive outcomes three years after diagnosis did not significantly differ by treatment risk arm is somewhat unexpected, as higher intensity of CNS-directed therapy is a well-recognized risk factor for poorer neurocognitive outcomes. Prior studies of neurocognitive outcomes in survivors of medulloblastoma treated on SJMB03 have identified the intensity of treatment (ie, risk arm) as a significant contributor to the decline in neurocognitive and academic performance that is seen one to five years after diagnosis. (8, 9) It is possible that the impact of risk arm on these outcomes emerges over time, such that the differences by risk arm are more evident at later study time points.
Models including age at diagnosis, treatment risk arm, and anesthesia exposure significantly predicted variance in neurocognitive outcomes. In all instances the amount of variance accounted for by the three-factor model was notably greater than that predicted by a two-factor model. Findings were similar for models that included baseline IQ as an additional predictor. Results from exploratory analysis in a subgroup of patients without posterior fossa syndrome are consistent with the findings from primary models. These results strongly support our hypothesis about the contribution of anesthesia exposure to neurocognitive outcomes.
From a clinical perspective, we suggest that alternatives to anesthesia exposure be considered when feasible (ie, when alternative measures exist to provide analgesic effects or to limit motion and increase compliance). Programmatic efforts to reduce anesthesia use may improve patient experience, reduce associated costs, and increase efficiency. Evidence-based behavioral interventions for promoting nonsedated scans have been implemented successfully in pediatric populations. (31–38) A survey of 101 parents of patients treated for childhood cancer at our institution supports the perceived feasibility and acceptability of nonsedated MRI examinations. (39)
Our study is not without limitations. Anesthesia data prior to treatment were not available. Data on anesthesia exposure are restricted to the first twelve months after enrollment on a clinical treatment trial. All cancer-directed therapy, including radiation therapy, was completed during this period. Protocol-directed indications for anesthesia exposure substantially decrease during the follow-up period. Nevertheless, the total exposure is not known for patients in the sample, and may slightly exceed the reported duration.
We compared neurocognitive outcomes to nationally representative normative data; however, it would not have been feasible to recruit a control group with the same diagnosis that completed treatment (ie, radiation therapy, neuroimaging, and procedures) without exposure to anesthesia. In the context of limitations of a retrospective study, our findings contribute meaningful information about the additive risk anesthesia exposure during protocol-directed, risk-adapted treatment for pediatric medulloblastoma. Future studies should prospectively collect anesthesia exposure data to more precisely characterize risk.
Acknowledgements:
We thank the patients and their families who volunteered their time to participate in neurocognitive assessments and the psychological examiners (Maggi Dunavant, MS; Charlotte Fineberg-Buchner, MA; David Hopper, EdS; and Deborah Stewart, MEd) for their valuable contributions to this work. These individuals have no conflicts of interest to declare.
Funded by the National Cancer Institute (Cancer Center Support Grant P30-CA21765) and the American Lebanese Syrian Associated Charities (ALSAC).
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
The authors declare no conflicts of interest.
Portions of this study were presented at the 45th annual meeting of the International Neuropsychological Society, February 1–4, 2017, New Orleans LA.
REFERENCES
- 1.Gajjar A, Chintagumpala M, Ashley D, Kellie S, Kun LE, Merchant TE, et al. Risk-adapted craniospinal radiotherapy followed by high-dose chemotherapy and stem-cell rescue in children with newly diagnosed medulloblastoma (St Jude Medulloblastoma-96): long-term results from a prospective, multicentre trial. The Lancet Oncology. 2006;7: 813–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.de Ruiter MA, van Mourik R, Schouten-van Meeteren AY, Grootenhuis MA, & Oosterlaan J Neurocognitive consequences of a paediatric brain tumour and its treatments: a meta-analysis. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. 2013;55:408–17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Mulhern RK, Palmer SL, Merchant TE, Wallace D, Kocak M, Brouwers P, et al. Neurocognitive consequences of risk-adapted therapy for childhood medulloblastoma. Journal of Clinical Oncology. 2005;23:5511–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Robinson KE, Fraley CE, Pearson MM, Kuttesch JF, Jr., Compas BE. Neurocognitive late effects of pediatric brain tumors of the posterior fossa: a quantitative review. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society : JINS. 2013;19:44–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Knight SJ, Conklin HM, Palmer SL, Schreiber JE, Armstrong CL, Wallace D, et al. Working memory abilities among children treated for medulloblastoma: parent report and child performance. Journal of Pediatric Psychology. 2014;39:501–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Palmer SL, Armstrong C, Onar-Thomas A, Wu S, Wallace D, Bonner MJ, et al. Processing speed, attention, and working memory after treatment for medulloblastoma: an international, prospective, and longitudinal study. Journal of Clinical Oncology. 2013;31:3494–500. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Palmer SL, Hassall T, Evankovich K, Mabbott DJ, Bonner M, Deluca C, et al. Neurocognitive outcome 12 months following cerebellar mutism syndrome in pediatric patients with medulloblastoma. Neuro-Oncology. 2010;12:1311–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Schreiber JE, Gurney JG, Palmer SL, Bass JK, Wang M, Chen S, et al. Examination of risk factors for intellectual and academic outcomes following treatment for pediatric medulloblastoma. Neuro-Oncology. 2014;16:1129–36. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Schreiber JE, Palmer SL, Conklin HM, Mabbott DJ, Swain MA, Bonner MJ, et al. Posterior fossa syndrome and long-term neuropsychological outcomes among children treated for medulloblastoma on a multi-institutional, prospective study. Neuro-Oncology. 2017;19:1673–82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.FDA Drug Safety Communication: FDA review results in new warnings about using general anesthetics and sedation drugs in young children and pregnant women. Washington, DC: 2016. [Google Scholar]
- 11.Bong CL, Allen JC, Kim JT. The effects of exposure to general anesthesia in infancy on academic performance at age 12. Anesthesia and Analgesia. 2013;117:1419–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.DiMaggio C, Sun LS, Li G. Early childhood exposure to anesthesia and risk of developmental and behavioral disorders in a sibling birth cohort. Anesthesia and Analgesia. 2011;113:1143–51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Flick RP, Katusic SK, Colligan RC, Wilder RT, Voigt RG, Olson MD, et al. Cognitive and behavioral outcomes after early exposure to anesthesia and surgery. Pediatrics. 2011;128:e1053–61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Hansen TG, Pedersen JK, Henneberg SW, Pedersen DA, Murray JC, Morton NS, et al. Academic performance in adolescence after inguinal hernia repair in infancy: a nationwide cohort study. Anesthesiology. 2011;114:1076–85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Ing C, DiMaggio C, Whitehouse A, Hegarty MK, Brady J, von Ungern-Sternberg BS, et al. Long-term differences in language and cognitive function after childhood exposure to anesthesia. Pediatrics. 2012;130:e476–85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Kalkman CJ, Peelen L, Moons KG, Veenhuizen M, Bruens M, Sinnema G, et al. Behavior and development in children and age at the time of first anesthetic exposure. Anesthesiology. 2009;110:805–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Sprung J, Flick RP, Katusic SK, Colligan RC, Barbaresi WJ, Bojanic K, et al. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder after early exposure to procedures requiring general anesthesia. Mayo Clinic Proceedings. 2012;87:120–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Wilder RT, Flick RP, Sprung J, Katusic SK, Barbaresi WJ, Mickelson C, et al. Early exposure to anesthesia and learning disabilities in a population-based birth cohort. Anesthesiology. 2009;110:796–804. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Fredriksson A, Ponten E, Gordh T, Eriksson P. Neonatal exposure to a combination of N-methyl-D-aspartate and gamma-aminobutyric acid type A receptor anesthetic agents potentiates apoptotic neurodegeneration and persistent behavioral deficits. Anesthesiology. 2007;107:427–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Ikonomidou C, Bosch F, Miksa M, Bittigau P, Vockler J, Dikranian K, et al. Blockade of NMDA receptors and apoptotic neurodegeneration in the developing brain. Science. 1999;283:70–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Yu D, Jiang Y, Gao J, Liu B, Chen P. Repeated exposure to propofol potentiates neuroapoptosis and long-term behavioral deficits in neonatal rats. Neuroscience letters. 2013;534:41–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Sun L Early childhood general anaesthesia exposure and neurocognitive development. British journal of anaesthesia. 2010;105 Suppl 1:i61–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Jevtovic-Todorovic V, Hartman RE, Izumi Y, Benshoff ND, Dikranian K, Zorumski CF, et al. Early exposure to common anesthetic agents causes widespread neurodegeneration in the developing rat brain and persistent learning deficits. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2003;23:876–82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Bartels M, Althoff RR, Boomsma DI. Anesthesia and cognitive performance in children: no evidence for a causal relationship. Twin Research and Human Genetics. 2009;12:246–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Ko WR, Huang JY, Chiang YC, Nfor ON, Ko PC, Jan SR, et al. Risk of autistic disorder after exposure to general anaesthesia and surgery: a nationwide, retrospective matched cohort study. European Journal of Anaesthesiology. 2015;32(5):303–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Chang CH, Housepian EM, Herbert C, Jr. An operative staging system and a megavoltage radiotherapeutic technic for cerebellar medulloblastomas. Radiology. 1969;93:1351–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Woodcock R, McGraw K, & Mather N Woodcock-Johnson Third Edition, Tests of Cognitive Abilities. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Woodcock R, McGraw K, & Mather N Woodcock-Johnson Third Edition, Tests of Academic Achievement Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Zou P, Conklin HM, Scoggins MA, Li Y, Li X, Jones MM, et al. Functional MRI in medulloblastoma survivors supports prophylactic reading intervention during tumor treatment. Brain Imaging and Behavior. 2016;10:258–71. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Banerjee P, Rossi MG, Anghelescu DL, Liu W, Breazeale AM, Reddick WE, et al. Association Between Anesthesia Exposure and Neurocognitive and Neuroimaging Outcomes in Long-term Survivors of Childhood Acute Lymphoblastic Leukemia. JAMA Oncol. 2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Theys C, Wouters J, Ghesquiere P. Diffusion tensor imaging and resting-state functional MRI-scanning in 5- and 6-year-old children: training protocol and motion assessment. PloS one. 2014;e94019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Schlund MW, Cataldo MF, Siegle GJ, Ladouceur CD, Silk JS, Forbes EE, et al. Pediatric functional magnetic resonance neuroimaging: tactics for encouraging task compliance. Behav Brain Funct. 2011;7:10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Rosenberg DR, Sweeney JA, Gillen JS, Kim J, Varanelli MJ, O’Hearn KM, et al. Magnetic resonance imaging of children without sedation: preparation with simulation. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1997;36:853–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Gale C, Jeffries S, Logan KM, Chappell KE, Uthaya SN, Modi N. Avoiding sedation in research MRI and spectroscopy in infants: our approach, success rate and prevalence of incidental findings. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2013;98:F267–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Dean DC 3rd, Dirks H, O’Muircheartaigh J, Walker L, Jerskey BA, Lehman K, et al. Pediatric neuroimaging using magnetic resonance imaging during non-sedated sleep. Pediatric Radiology. 2014;44:64–72. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.de Bie HM, Boersma M, Wattjes MP, Adriaanse S, Vermeulen RJ, Oostrom KJ, et al. Preparing children with a mock scanner training protocol results in high quality structural and functional MRI scans. European Journal of Pediatrics. 2010;169:1079–85. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Byars AW, Holland SK, Strawsburg RH, Bommer W, Dunn RS, Schmithorst VJ, et al. Practical aspects of conducting large-scale functional magnetic resonance imaging studies in children. Journal of Child Neurology. 2002;17:885–90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Bharti B, Malhi P, Khandelwal N. MRI Customized Play Therapy in Children Reduces the Need for Sedation--A Randomized Controlled Trial. Indian J Pediatr. 2016;83:209–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Walker B, Conklin HM, Anghelescu DL, Hall LP, Reddick WE, Ogg R, et al. Parent perspectives and preferences for strategies regarding nonsedated MRI scans in a pediatric oncology population. Supportive Care in Cancer, 2018;26:1815–24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]