Abstract
Interactions between individuals and the environment occur within the peri-personal space (PPS). The encoding of this space plastically adapts to bodily constraints and stimuli features. However, these remapping effects have not been demonstrated on an adaptive time-scale, trial-to-trial. Here, we test this idea first via a visuo-tactile reaction time (RT) paradigm in augmented reality where participants are asked to respond as fast as possible to touch, as visual objects approach them. Results demonstrate that RTs to touch are facilitated as a function of visual proximity, and the sigmoidal function describing this facilitation shifts closer to the body if the immediately precedent trial had indexed a smaller visuo-tactile disparity. Next, we derive the electroencephalographic correlates of PPS and demonstrate that this multisensory measure is equally shaped by recent sensory history. Finally, we demonstrate that a validated neural network model of PPS is able to account for the present results via a simple Hebbian plasticity rule. The present findings suggest that PPS encoding remaps on a very rapid time-scale and, more generally, that it is sensitive to sensory history, a key feature for any process contextualizing subsequent incoming sensory information (e.g., a Bayesian prior).
Keywords: adaptation, body, multisensory, plasticity, touch, vision
Introduction
Physical interactions between an agent and the environment happen by the mediation of the body and occur within the peri-personal space (PPS; Rizzolatti et al. 1981)—the space immediately adjacent to and surrounding one’s body (di Pellegrino et al. 1997; Rizzolatti et al. 1997). This space is encoded by a dedicated fronto-parietal neural network, in which neurons possess visuo-tactile receptive fields anchored on particular body parts—most notably the face and hand, but also the trunk (Graziano et al. 1994, 1997, 1999; Duhamel et al. 1998; Schlack et al. 2005; see Serino et al. 2015b for a behavioral demonstration in humans, and Cléry et al. 2017, 2018, for brain-wide networks supporting PPS). These multisensory neurons are most responsive to dynamic looming stimuli (Fogassi et al. 1996; Noel et al. 2018b) and are part of a network that can evoke complex and stereotyped defensive movements upon microstimulation (Cooke et al. 2003; Cooke and Graziano 2004). In turn, PPS is conceived as an adaptive multisensory space mediating both bodily protection and goal-directed action (Cooke and Graziano 2004; Brozzoli et al. 2014; Serino 2019).
Studies show that the shape and size of PPS are not fixed but instead adapt as a function of interaction with the environment both dynamically and plastically. For instance, visual-tactile coding of the peri-hand space modifies online—in a dynamic fashion—during planning (Patane et al. 2018) and execution (Brozzoli et al. 2009, 2010) of grasping actions. Furthermore, PPS extends in space after few minutes of using a tool to reach far locations (Iriki et al. 1996; Farne and Ladavas 2000; Maravita and Iriki 2004; Canzoneri et al. 2013), a remapping that has inclusively been shown to alter the conscious perception of the space near the body (Guterstam et al. 2018). Conversely, PPS contracts after prolonged immobilization (Bassolino et al. 2010) or shifts to reflect the altered self-location (Noel et al. 2015a, 2019; Salomon et al. 2017). Arguably, the largest portion of the effort in studying PPS today centers around delineating its remapping as a function of a number of external manipulations on the order of days, hours, or minutes (see references above and Cléry et al. 2015 for a review specifically detailed known and unknowns in the plastic and dynamic remapping of PPS).
However, if the encoding of the space immediately surrounding one’s body is truly fundamental in mediating sensorimotor affordances and interaction with potential threats, this encoding must update as the environment and sensory history change. That is, beyond dynamic updating within trials (e.g., Fogassi et al. 1996; Noel et al. 2018) and slow plastic updating between trials (e.g., Canzoneri et al. 2013), PPS must be subject to well-established rapid serial dependencies (Fernberger 1920; Yu and Cohen 2008; Fischer and Whitney 2014). Within the multisensory literature, classic adaptation-recalibration studies in temporal acuity demonstrated early on that upon extensive sensory exposure, perceptual judgments shifted as to reflect the statistics of the presented stimuli (Fujisaki et al. 2004; Vroomen et al. 2004; Noel et al. 2015c). More recently, these effects have been shown to occur on a trial-by-trial basis (positive serial dependency; Van der Burg et al. 2013, 2015; Noel et al. 2016a, 2016b). In analogy to these recent findings within the multisensory temporal domain, we asked whether rapid recalibration applies to the multisensory mechanisms underlying PPS. This putative occurrence would suggest that PPS can be regulated online to adapt to changes in the environment.
We present psychophysical (Experiment 1) and electrophysiological (Experiment 2) evidence for the rapid recalibration of PPS, as well as a neural network model implementing a neurophysiologically plausible mechanism for this recalibration (Experiment 3). In Experiment 1, we index rapid recalibration of PPS in a naturalistic context via a behavioral paradigm implemented in augmented-reality (Serino et al. 2017). Participants were presented with looming visual stimuli and at a given distance between the body and the visual stimuli, target tactile stimulation was delivered. Indexing PPS, reaction times (RTs) to touch are expected to decrease as a function of decreasing visuo-tactile spatial disparities, when visual stimuli were presented closer to the body (Serino 2019). Critically, if rapid recalibration occurs, here, we expect the facilitation to touch as a function of visuo-tactile distance to be more pronounced (occur at larger spatial disparities) when the immediately precedent trial probed a larger, as opposed to smaller, visuo-tactile distance disparity. In Experiment 2, we extend an intracortical local field measure of PPS that specifically probes for the integration of sensory signals (Bernasconi et al. 2018, see also Noel et al. 2018c, 2019) to scalp electrophysiology (EEG), and demonstrate a multisensory PPS rapid recalibration correlate akin to that observed in multisensory temporal judgments (Simon et al. 2017). Finally, to suggest a mechanistic account, we demonstrate that a validated and biologically plausible neural network model of PPS (Magosso et al. 2010a, 2010b) can in principle account for the rapid recalibration of PPS given Hebbian learning (see Serino et al. 2015a).
Materials and Methods
Experiment 1: Psychophysics
Participants
A total of 38 (mean age = 22.9 ± 0.86, range 19–44) right-handed students from the Ecole Polytechnique Federale de Lausanne took part in this experiment. This sample size is approximately twice as large as most behavioral studies of PPS (e.g., 20 participants in Noel et al. 2018a, 2018b, 2018e), given that we anticipated needing to discard a large portion of subjects due to the multiple data fitting procedures (three separate sigmoidal fits as opposed to a singular one, see below). All participants reported normal touch and had normal or corrected-to-normal eyesight. All participants gave their written informed consent to take part in this study, which was approved by the local ethics committee, the Brain Mind Institute Ethics Committee for Human Behavioral Research at EPFL, and were reimbursed for their participation.
Materials and Apparatus
A mixed/augmented-reality technology (Fig. 1A Reality Substitution Machine—RealiSM; http://lnco.epfl.ch/realism) was utilized in order to render a prerecorded panoramic physical scene (a looming ball originating at approximately 2 m and traveling at a velocity of approximately 75 cm/s; recorded with 14 GoPro Hero4 cameras placed in a spherical rig—3D 360hero 3DH3PRO14H) alongside the veritable surrounding environment (the experimental room) in which the participants were placed. Real-time stereoscopic images of the participant’s body were captured via a DuoCam (Duo3D MLX, 752 × 480 at 56 Hz) and participant’s limbs were rendered online (delay <5 ms). The visual stimulation was displayed on an Oculus Rift DK2 head-mounted display (HMD; 900 × 1080 per eye, 105° FOV). Tactile stimulation (duration; 6-ms up/down state) was administered via a solenoid (MSTC-3 tappers, M & E Solve).
Procedure
Participants were seated in a dimly lit room (see Fig. 1A) in which they performed a tactile RT task to a stimulation administered on their right cheek. Each trial began with a white fixation cross presented in the center of the virtual environment for 1.2 s. Critically, in experimental trials (72% of all trials), they concurrently viewed a 3D ball loom toward them in augmented reality (see Serino et al. 2015b, 2017 for a similar approach). This visual stimulus started approaching participants 300 ms following fixation cross disappearance, and the onset of tactile stimulation was staggered with regard trial start by either T1 = 1.83 s, T2 = 2.15 s, T3 = 2.47 s, T4 = 2.79 s, T5 = 3.11 s, or T6 = 3.43 s. These temporal offsets map onto the spatial dimension linearly and negatively when stimuli approached the body at a constant velocity, and thus, we denominate in the spatial dimension D1 = T6, D2 = T5, and so on. Therefore, the visuo-tactile disparities tested were D1 = 55.25 cm, D2 = 79.25 cm, D3 = 103.25 cm, D4 = 127.25 cm, D5 = 151.25 cm, and D6 = 175.25 cm. Distances were chosen to a priori probe visuo-tactile distances both within and outside PPS (estimated at ~80 cm with exteroceptive stimuli incoming at 75 cm/s, see Noel et al. 2018b, Fig. 7) and correspond to the following time-to-contact durations: D1 = 736.7 ms, D2 = 1056.7 ms, D3 = 1376.7 ms, D4 = 1696.7 ms, D5 = 2016.7 ms, and D6 = 2336.7 ms.
In addition to the experimental trials, we also included unisensory tactile trials (16% of all trials). These trials acted as a baseline condition in which tactile stimulation was administered at the equivalent temporal offsets as in experimental trials (T1–T6), but no visual stimulus was presented. These trials are fundamental in ascertaining that putative effects in the experimental trials are veritably due to multisensory interactions, and not simply due to an expectancy effect (which would be time-dependent and thus revealed in the baseline trials; see Kandula et al. 2017 and Hobeika et al. 2019). Finally, catch trials in which visual stimuli were presented but no tactile stimulation was administered (12% of total trials) were undertaken in order to monitor task-compliance. All trial types were randomized within- and between-subjects and the intertrial interval was set to 500 ms. Every subject performed a total of 300 trials (36 repetitions × [6 experimental conditions + catch] + 8 repetitions × 6 baseline conditions).
Analysis
In a first step, participant’s RTs were collected and averaged as a function of condition (and regardless of the nature of the precedent trial). As a preliminary analysis, we performed a 2 (Condition: Experimental vs. Baseline) × 6 (Distance: D1 through D6) within-subject ANOVA in order to confirm that: 1) multisensory visuo-tactile trials are faster than unisensory tactile trials; 2) visuo-tactile trials exhibit a space-dependence; but 3) tactile trials do not. Next, returning to the raw data, we split experimental and baseline trials (t) on whether they had been preceded (t − 1) by either a smaller or a larger visuo-tactile/tactile distance. That is, say trial t administered tactile stimulation when the visual stimulus was placed at D3. This trial was sorted into “D3, T − 1 smaller” if t − 1 was a D1 or D2 trial, or “D3, T − 1 larger” if t − 1 was a D4, D5, or D6 trial. Trials that were preceded by either the same distance (D3 preceded by D3) or a catch trial was omitted, as it were D1 and D6 trials as, respectively, there was no smaller/bigger condition for these. Subsequently to this conditional sorting of trials by the nature of the immediately prior trial, on a subject-per-subject basis, we fit the average RTs to a sigmoidal function (eq. 1) from which we extract the central point of the sigmoidal (, in eq. 1, representing the boundary of PPS) and a parameter proportional to its slope at the central point ( in eq. 1, characterizing the gradient of PPS representation; see Canzoneri et al. 2012 for a similar approach). Participants were discarded from further analysis if one of their fits (i.e., regardless of sensory history, T − 1 smaller, or T − 1 larger than current) was dissatisfactory (a priori set to R2 < 0.50, 17 subjects removed. About 21 subjects had R2 > 0.75 for all three experimental conditions). Central point and slope were compared as a function of T − 1 smaller or larger by means of a paired-samples t-test as follows:
(1) |
Experiment 2: Electroencephalography
Participants
A total of 27 (15 females, mean age = 21.3 ± 0.79, range 18–31) right-handed students from Vanderbilt University took part in this experiment. All participants self-reported normal touch and had normal or corrected-to-normal eyesight. Six participants were excluded from analysis due to excessive motion and/or blink artifacts in the EEG recordings resulting in >50% of trials being rejected (3 subjects), large degrees of electrical noise (1 subject), technical problems (1 subject), and/or poor psychometric fits that precluded behavioral analysis (1 subject). Therefore, in total, data from 21 participants (12 females) were analyzed. All participants gave their written informed consent to take part in this study, which was approved by the Behavioral Sciences Committee at Vanderbilt University.
Materials and Apparatus
The augmented reality setup from Experiment 1 was not used, due to the difficulty in recording high-density EEG while concurrently wearing an HMD. Furthermore, we decided to utilize static stimuli, as opposed to dynamically looming stimuli in Experiment 1 to render both visual and tactile stimuli “evoked” (i.e., with sharp on–off transients), as opposed to having the tactile component be evoked, and the visual induced (i.e., prolonged presence and no transient visual on–off during tactile stimulation). These modifications render Experiments 1 and 2 quite different. Whereas looming stimuli (Experiment 1) have been frequently used in the study of PPS in humans (see Serino 2019 for a review) by using static stimuli in Experiment 2—where all stimuli only have transient on and off temporal dynamics—we are able to fully eliminate the possibility for expectancy effects, as there is no consistent mapping between space and time (see Kandula et al. 2017 and Hobeika et al. 2019 for interesting reports warning that much of the tactile facilitation during looming exteroceptive stimuli may be due to temporal expectancies). Visual and tactile stimuli were driven via a microcontroller (SparkFun Electronics) and direct serial port communication under the control of purpose written MATLAB (MathWorks) and Arduino (Arduino) scripts. Visual stimuli were a flash of light presented by means for a red LED (3-mm diameter, 640-nm wavelength), while tactile stimuli consisted of vibrotactile stimulation administered via a mini motor disc (10-mm diameter, 2.7-mm thick, 0.9 g, 5 V, 11 000 RPM). These stimuli were 50 ms in duration (square-wave, onset and offset <1 ms, as measured via oscilloscope). The LEDs and vibrotactile motor were mounted in an opaque enclosure where 30 LEDs sequentially protruded above the enclosure every 3.3 cm (in depth) and counted with a hand rest immediately adjacent to the first LED (Fig. 2A, see Noel et al. 2018a for a similar apparatus). In the current study, LEDs number 2, 5, 8, 11, 14, 17, and 20 were utilized, corresponding to visuotactile depth distances of 3.3, 13.2, 23.1, 33.0, 42.9, 52.8, and 62.9 cm. Visuotactile stimuli consisted of the synchronous presentation of the visual and tactile stimuli described above. At difference from Experiment 1, here, we cannot equate distance with time-to-impact, as the stimuli have no temporal dynamics.
Procedure
Participants were seated in a room with lights off (ambient light allowed for visibility) and performed a tactile RT task to stimulation administered on their left index finger. Participants were required to gaze toward a fixation point present at eye-level and placed at the end of the apparatus enclosing the LEDs (Fig. 2A). Here, tactile stimuli were given on the index finger as opposed to the cheek—as in Experiment 1—in order to minimize the potential for eye-blink artifact. Furthermore, this change from Experiment 1 afforded us the possibility of exploring whether the rapid recalibration of PPS applies generally to both peri-hand and peri-face space (and is present in both a veridical and augmented reality setting). Trials could be visuotactile (i.e., experimental trials; VT), tactile (i.e., baseline; T), or visual (i.e., catch; V) trials. Similar to Experiment 1, visual trials were “catch trials” in that they did not require a motor response, while tactile trials were “baseline trials” as these permitted us to gauge tactile RTs in the absence of visual inputs, and hence determine whether a multisensory effect was present as a function of visuo-tactile distance disparity. Visuotactile trials were presented at seven different distances (D1 through D7 = 3.3, 13.2, 23.1, 33.0, 42.9, 52.8, and 62.9 cm; Fig. 2A), while visual only trials were presented solely at D2, D3, D4, D5, and D6 due to time constraints and the fact that analyses heavily relied on distances between D2 and D6, as there were no smaller distances than D1 or larger distances than D7. Within each block, 360 trials were presented: 40 VT trials at each of the seven distances, 10 V trials at each of the five distances at which these were presented, and 30 T only trials. Trial type was randomized within blocks, and intertrial interval was random between 1250 and 2250 ms (uniform distribution). Participants completed 10–12 blocks (~10 min per block and ~2.30 h of total experimental time for a grand total of 3600–4320 trials; ~2800 VT trials, 500 V trials, and 300 T trials) according to time constraints and were allowed to take brief breaks between blocks.
Behavioral Analysis
Behavioral analyses mimicked that described in Experiment 1, with the following exceptions. First, seven distances were utilized as opposed to six, and thus, the distance factor counts with an extra level. Furthermore, due to the EEG recordings and the chosen approach to contrast evoked responses (i.e., with rapid on–off transients) (vs. induced; e.g., looming visual stimuli are turned-on rapidly but then sustained for a long duration), static and transient stimuli (vs. dynamic and continuous) were used in this experiment. In turn, tactile-only conditions could not be mapped onto particular visuotactile distances, and hence, the initial ANOVA performed in Experiment 1 (contrasting VT and T trials as a function of distance) was not possible. Instead, we averaged VT RTs across all disparities and performed a paired-samples t-test between VT and T trials. Note that this approach is extremely conservative statistically, as in principle, there should be no multisensory facilitation when visual and tactile stimuli are presented far from each other (e.g., Noel et al. 2015a, 2015b; Salomon et al. 2017).
EEG Recording and Preprocessing
Continuous EEG was recorded from 128 electrodes with a sampling rate of 1000 Hz (Net Amps 400 amplifier, Hydrocel GSN 128 EEG cap, EGI systems Inc.) and referenced to the vertex (Cz). Electrode impedances were maintained below 40 kΩ throughout the recording procedure. Data were acquired with NetStation 5.1.2 and further preprocessed using MATLAB and EEGLAB (Delorme and Makeig 2004). Continuous EEG data were notch filtered at 60 Hz and bandpass filtered from 0.1 to 40 Hz using an eighth-order bi-directional zero-phase infinite impulse response filter. Epochs from 200 ms before to 800 ms after stimuli onset were extracted and split according to experimental condition. Artifact contaminated trials and bad channels were identified and removed through a combination of automated rejection of trials in which any channel exceeded ±100 mV and rigorous visual inspection (e.g., Simon et al. 2017; Noel et al. 2019). A mean of 334.9 (SEM = 9.8) or 83.7% of trials were retained per VT condition, while 2.73% (SEM = 1.71%) of channels were removed per participant. Data were then recalculated to the average reference and bad channels were reconstructed using spherical spline interpolation. Finally, data were baseline corrected for the prestimuli period (−200 to 0 ms poststimuli onset).
EEG Analyses
Within the current study, we adopt the so-called electrical neuroimaging framework (Brunet et al. 2011) for EEG analyses. Within this framework, we leverage the fact that EEG recordings are performed from a full-montage of electrodes covering the entire skull and utilize data-reduction techniques to overcome the inherent multiple comparisons problem in EEG studies. Furthermore, we avoid indexing particular components, which are both reference-dependent and potentially subject to experimenter bias. In turn, the global electric field strength present throughout the recording montage was quantified using global field power (GFP; Lehmann and Skrandies 1980; Lehmann 1987). This measure corresponds to the standard deviation of the trial-averaged voltage values across the entire electrode montage at a given time point and represents a reference- and topographic-independent measure of evoked potential magnitude (Murray et al. 2008). Furthermore, GFP is used as a data-reduction technique by summarizing 128 distinct time-series (i.e., electrodes) into a singular one.
In a first pass, we calculated average GFPs for each subject, as well as for the entire sample of participants (i.e., grand average) for tactile, visual, and visuotactile conditions separately while collapsing across distances. Time-resolved t-tests against zero were performed at each time-point from 200-ms prestimuli presentation to 800 poststimuli onset in order to ascertain whether reliable evoked potentials were generated (to V, T, and VT stimuli). To account for the inherent autocorrelation problem in EEG, we set alpha at <0.01 for at least 10 consecutive time points (Guthrie and Buchwald 1991; see Noel et al. 2018c, 2018d, and Simon et al. 2017, for a similar approach. Note that given the emphasis on GFP, a single time-course, we do not have a multiple comparisons problem requiring permutation testing).
Next, to ascertain whether a veritable multisensory effect existed (i.e., nonlinearity between the copresentation of V and T information vis-à-vis their presentation in isolation), we created visuo-tactile summed responses (hereafter, “summed” or “sum”) by adding the subject-level average responses to V and T. GFP was then calculated for this summed response and contrasted to the GFP of the multisensory visuotactile condition (or “paired” response; see Cappe et al. 2010 and Noel et al. 2018c for a similar approach). Indeed, as GFP is by definition positive, an advantage of utilizing this method within a multisensory framework is that supra- and subadditivity indices may be measured (Sperdin et al. 2010; Murray and Wallace 2012). The contrast between multisensory visuo-tactile discrepancies is undertaken solely for distances D2–D6, as the main interest here is in describing the neural correlates of PPS rapid recalibration and, by definition, there are no smaller visuo-tactile discrepancies than D1 and no larger discrepancies that D7. Having first established that the copresentation of visual and tactile information resulted in a multisensory effect, we queried via a time-resolved one-way ANOVA whether VT GFPs differed as a function of visuo-tactile distance.
Finally, having identified a time-period of interest (demonstrating both multisensory supraadditivity and space-dependent modulation of its multisensory response, see below), we examined whether this metric of PPS was altered as a function of the nature of the previous trial (i.e., trial t − 1 being larger or smaller than t) and highlights the electrodes that are contributing to this effect.
Experiment 3: Neural Network Modeling
To suggest a putative mechanistic underpinning the observed rapid recalibration of PPS, we employed a nonspiking biologically inspired neural network model that has previously been demonstrated to account for a number of PPS phenomena (Magosso et al. 2010a, 2010b; Serino et al. 2015a; Noel et al. 2018b). Importantly, we did not attempt to build a new model from scratch to explain the rapid recalibration of PPS; contrarily, we simply took the most recent version of the model (Noel et al. 2018b) and imbued this model with Hebbian learning (as in Magosso et al. 2010a; Serino et al. 2015a) given the conceptual hypothesis that this form of learning ought in principle to account for rapid recalibration. This approach was taken as we considered it more powerful (conceptually) to demonstrate that a model already shown to account for a number of PPS phenomena can also incorporate the newly described rapid recalibration effect, than it is important to exactly fit behavioral results. Previous iterations of this model can account for sigmoidal facilitation functions (Serino et al. 2015a), the fact that PPS has different sizes for different body parts (Noel et al. 2018b), as well as its enlargement after tool-use (Magosso et al. 2010a, 2010b) and as a function of increasing exteroceptive signal velocities (Noel et al. 2018b). In the latest version of the model (Noel et al. 2018b) and here, we present exteroceptive looming stimuli by appropriately shifting the center of the bidimensional Gaussian that simulates the sensory input. The location of stimuli, parameters of the model, and activity of neurons are updated at each time-step (1 ms). The model simply inherited previous parameters (see Table 1 in Noel et al. 2018a), with exception of those ruling Hebbian learning (for more detail regarding the model parameters and the robustness of the its PPS encoding to parameter selection, see Noel et al. 2018b. For detail regarding Hebbian learning within PPS, see Serino et al. 2015a). In turn, here, we briefly explain the neural network implementation (for detail see Magosso et al. 2010a, 2010b) and only briefly explore results from the simulation—focusing on the conceptual contribution, rather than the peculiarities of the model and its parameters.
The neural network simulates the peri-face space, although it could equally simulate PPS around any other body part (see Magosso et al. 2010a; Noel et al. 2018b). It includes two areas of unisensory neurons (tactile and visual) and a third area composed of a multisensory visuo-tactile neuron (see Fig. 6A). Both the tactile and visual stimuli are mimicked by bidimensional Gaussian functions with small standard deviations (i.e., high precision in space) to simulate localized stimuli. Dynamic looming visual stimuli are simulated in the model, thus resembling experimental conditions as in Experiment 1. Specifically, the visual stimuli are iteratively displaced closer and closer to the location of tactile receptive fields, to engender an approaching stimulus with equivalent velocity as that used in Experiment 1 (i.e., 75 cm/s for ~200 cm). Each unisensory area is composed by a matrix of N × N (N = 41) unisensory neurons. Unisensory neurons have a receptive field with a bidimensional Gaussian shape, through which the approaching stimulus is convolved (i.e., filtered), and are topologically aligned (i.e., proximal neurons respond to proximal spatial stimuli, shaded area in Fig. 6A visual area). The fact that they are topologically aligned simply means that the tactile unisensory neurons respond to touch on the body and that tactile responses may occur in conjunction with visual responses near the body, but not with visual responses far from the body (as the tactile receptive fields code for an area of space that is different from the receptive fields covered by visual neurons far from the body). The receptive fields of tactile neurons are 0.5 cm apart from one another along each dimension of the face, thus mapping a surface of 20 × 20 cm (approximate size of the face). The visual neurons respond to visual inputs in a planar space that extends sideways and in front of the body. Visual neurons’ receptive fields are 10 cm from one another along each dimension, mapping a space of 400 × 400 cm. Furthermore, unisensory neurons within each area are reciprocally connected via lateral synapses (L) having a Mexican-hat pattern (near excitation and far inhibition).
Neurons within the two unisensory areas send excitatory feedforward synapses (W) to the downstream visuotactile area. This area mimics multisensory regions in the fronto-parietal cortex (e.g., ventral premotor cortex, ventral intraparietal area, area 7b), which devoted to the representation of body-part specific PPS (e.g., peri-face). As electrophysiological data stress the existence of multisensory neurons having large receptive fields covering an entire body part, for parsimony only, one multisensory neuron is included (see Magosso et al. 2010a, 2010b; Serino et al. 2015a; Noel et al. 2018b, for a similar approach). The feedforward synapses from the tactile neurons to the multisensory one have a uniform value (). As such, the multisensory neuron has a tactile receptive field covering the whole face. The strength of the feedforward synapses from the visual neurons depends on the distance of the visual neurons’ receptive field from the body part. These synapses assume a maximum value () for visual neurons coding for the space covering and bordering the body part; then, their value decreases exponentially as the distance of the visual neurons’ receptive field from the face increases. Finally, the multisensory neuron sends excitatory feedback synapses () to the tactile and visual unisensory neurons; the feedback synapses have the same pattern as the feedforward ones (see Noel et al. 2018b, eqs 5–7 for more detail). Importantly, in the current work, the strength of the feedforward excitatory connections from the unisensory neurons to the multisensory one is not set in stone but is modified according to the following Hebbian rule:
(2) |
(3) |
In equations (2) and (3), ij denotes the topographical position of a generic presynaptic unisensory neuron (t = tactile, v = visual) within the corresponding map, is the strength of the feedforward synapse connecting the presynaptic unisensory neuron at position ij with the postsynaptic multisensory neuron, denotes the neural activity of the presynaptic unisensory neuron, denotes the neural activity of the postsynaptic multisensory neuron, and indicates the positive part of the function. in equation (2) is the temporal step of synapses updating ( = 1 ms in our simulations), and t simple refers to a particular moment in the interval of the simulation. The Hebbian rule contains a reinforcing component (i.e., the first term in the right-hand member of eq. 3) and a forgetting factor (i.e., the second term in the right-hand member of eq. 3). According to the reinforcing factor, the strength of the feedforward synapse () increases when both the presynaptic (unisensory) neuron and the postsynaptic (multisensory) neuron are active. In particular, the postsynaptic multisensory activity is compared with a small threshold (5% of the maximum activation), in order to avoid reinforcement in case of very small activity of the postsynaptic neuron. denotes the reinforcement learning factor, which is time-dependent (see below). The forgetting factor is constantly acting on the synapses regardless of neural activity and is effectively inducing an exponential decay of the synaptic weight toward the fixed “basal” level with a time constant (in simulation steps). This time constant is a key parameter in the simulations as it determines the approximate timescale at which the network “remembers” the past multisensory events. To avoid that the strength of excitatory connections increases unlimitedly, we imposed a saturation constraint for synapsis value; the reinforcement learning factor () progressively reduces to zero as synapses approach their maximum value. We have
(4) |
where (s = t, v) is the maximum value allowed for the tactile and visual synapses and is assumed equal to the preexisting (basal) value of the synapses on the face, that is, and . Therefore, tactile feedforward synapses (as well as visual feedforward synapses on or close to the face) are effectively not subject to further strengthening, as their weights are already maximal (in practice all feedforward synapses are nonetheless subject to learning). Value of parameter was assigned so that synapses reinforce gradually during stimulus presentation (i.e., several stimulation trials are required so that the reinforcing factor in eq. 3 leads synapses close to saturation). The key parameter has been subjected to a sensitivity analysis to test how its value may affect a rapid trial-by-trial recalibration. As an extension, we also simulate the rapid recalibration of PPS under incoming exteroceptive stimuli of velocities other than that used during the behavior experiment (namely, 25, 50, and 100 cm/s, in addition to the experimental velocity of 75 cm/s; see Results; see Noel et al. 2018b for sensitivity analyses of other parameters).
As in our previous models (Magosso et al. 2010a; Serino et al. 2015b), only feedforward synapses were subjected to training, while lateral synapses within unisensory areas and feedback synapses are kept fixed. This computational choice was made as in principle, the training of the lateral and feedback synapses ought not to significantly affect our results (that would still mainly depend on feedforward synapses training, which are subject to training). Within the current model feedback, synapses are not subject to alterations as from a practical standpoint, they can only be modified very seldomly, since their potentiation can easily produce phantom effects (i.e., activation in one unisensory area following stimulation in the other unisensory modality). The training of a large multitude of lateral synapses, on the other hand, requires an extremely delicate balance between overall excitation and inhibition to prevent network instability that can frequently occur during training. Most importantly, including a training of lateral synapses that maintains network stability (and spatial sensitivity) would result in a slight modification of the single bubble of activation in the unisensory areas, inducing yet even smaller effects on multimodal neuron activation and thus on PPS recalibration. In turn, in line with our previous studies (Magosso et al. 2010a; Serino et al. 2015b), we avoided to include these further mechanisms. Biologically, the divide between synapses that are, here, subject to reinforcements or not (or more realistically, subject to small modifications or only on longer time-scales) may easily be implemented by different synaptic properties (e.g., the nature of voltage-gated channels)—unfortunately, the anatomical and molecular features of PPS neurons’ dendrites (feedforward from unisensory to PPS neurons) and axons (feedback from PPS neurons to unisensory areas) are very poorly understood. Recent reports have, however, started showing differences in near versus far space processing in simpler animal models and can represent the starting point for future anatomical and molecular work (see Dürr and Schilling 2018, and La Chioma et al. 2019, respectively, for interesting reports mapping PPS in insects and suggesting a selectivity for near space in the visual rostro-lateral area of the mouse; see Hihara et al. 2006, for anterograde and retrograde tracer experiments establishing the anatomical connections between PPS neurons and others).
The overall input (say ) to a generic neuron (unisensory or multisensory ) in the network is processed via a first-order temporal dynamics (eq. 5, mimicking the postsynaptic membrane time constant) and a sigmoidal function (eq. 6, mimicking the neuron’s activation function), generating the neuron’s output activity [say ]
(5) |
(6) |
Equations (5) and (6) hold for both unisensory and multisensory neuron; in case of the multisensory neuron (), the equations hold without the subscripts as a single multisensory neuron is used in the network. Equation (5) describes the first-order dynamics, where is the state variable, is the input to the neuron, and is the time constant. Equation (6) describes the sigmoidal activation function; and represent the lower and upper saturation of the sigmoidal function, respectively, establishes the central value of the sigmoidal function (i.e., the input value at which the output is midway between and ), and defines the slope. In turn, the output of each neuron is a continuous variable representing the particular neuron’s firing rate.
The overall input to the unisensory neurons s = t, v] is made up of the external input coming from outside the network (i.e., the stimulus filtered by the neurons’ receptive field s = t, v), plus the lateral input coming from other neurons in the same area (via weights defined by lateral synapses ), and feedback input from the multisensory neuron (via weight defined by the feedback synapses , see eq. 7). The overall input to the multisensory neuron is made up of the feedforward inputs from the two unisensory areas (via weights defined by the feedforward synapses , see eq. 8)
(7) |
(8) |
where in equation (7) denotes the strength of the lateral synapse from the presynaptic neuron at position to the postsynaptic neuron at position within the same unisensory (tactile t or visual v) area, and is the strength of the feedback synapse from the multisensory neuron to the unisensory neuron at position ij within the tactile (t) or visual (v) area. The sums of equations (7) and (8) extend to all neurons within each unisensory area.
Finally, since behavioral data are expressed in terms of tactile RTs, we decoded such a measure from the network. This network tactile RT was computed as the time necessary for the overall tactile activity (the sum of all tactile neurons’ activity) to reach a given threshold = 4 starting from the tactile stimulus onset. Since neuron activity ranges between 0 and 1, this means that an ensemble of a few tactile neurons needed to be active for the stimulus to be detected. In the multisensory condition, the activation in the tactile area can be speeded up compared with the unisensory condition, and thus, the network RT decreased, when the visual stimulus is able to trigger the multisensory neuron. For each condition (unisensory, and multisensory at each of the visuo-tactile disparities and), 20 trials were simulated.
Results
Augmented Reality Psychophysics: Experiment 1
Overall participants were very accurate at withholding responses during unisensory visual catch trials (<1% false positives). The initial 2 (Condition: Experimental vs. Baseline) × 6 (Distance: D1 through D6) within-subjects ANOVA demonstrated a significant main effect of Condition [F(1, 37) = 10.92, P = 0.003, η2 = 0.313; Experimental, M = 0.289 s, SEM = 0.010 s; Baseline, M = 0.333 s, SEM = 0.011 s] and a significant main effect of Distance [F(5, 190) = 2.324, P = 0.047, η2 = 0.088]. Importantly, and as illustrated in Figure 1B, the results revealed a significant interaction between these factors [F(5, 190) = 3.463, P = 0.005]. This interaction is further explained by the lack of a main effect of Distance in the Control condition [F(5, 190) = 0.668, P = 0.649) and the presence of the same effect in the Experimental condition [F(5, 190) = 46.97, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.559]. Thus, participant’s RT to tactile stimulation became faster the closer a task-irrelevant visual stimulus was to their body, and this result cannot be explained merely by an expectancy effect (see Kandula et al. 2017). The central point of the sigmoidal function best describing multisensory RTs as a function of visuo-tactile distance was at D = 3.74 (see Fig. 1B). The complete lack of an expectancy effect (Kandula et al. 2017; Hobeika et al. 2019) is not as unusual for the visuo-tactile pairing as it is for the audio-tactile pairing (e.g., Pfeiffer et al. 2018; Serino et al. 2015b), and in fact, the prior reports using a visuo-tactile virtual and the augmented reality setups have also shown no strong expectancy effect (Pellencin et al. 2017; Noel et al. 2018). It is possible that the considerably higher number of repetitions per distance (36 here vs. ~20 in most reports) or the more ecologically valid setup here contributed to the lack of an expectancy effect.
With regard the fitting procedure, when trials were divided given the nature of their precedent trial, goodness of fit measures demonstrated that for both conditions (T − 1 smaller and T − 1 larger), the sigmoidal fitting described the data equally well (R2T − 1 smaller = 0.84, R2T − 1 larger = 0.81, paired-samples t-test P = 0.62). The central point, describing the spatial extension of PPS representation, was significantly modulated as a consequence of the nature of the immediately preceding trial, as demonstrated by a significant difference between T − 1 smaller (M = 3.42, SEM = 0.11) and T − 1 larger [M = 4.04, SEM = 0.13; t(27) = 3.6707, P < 0.001; Fig. 1C]. We did not statistically compare the central point of T − 1 smaller and T − 1 larger with that of the central point when trials were not split depending on sensory history, as this comparison would be confounded by a significantly different amount of repetition in each condition. However, it must be noted that numerically, the extension of PPS when not split as a conditional of T − 1 falls in between the values reported for T − 1 smaller and T − 1 larger. Although there was a trend, statistically, there was no significant difference in the value of the slope (P = 0.17), describing visuo-tactile RTs as a function of whether the precedent trial had been larger or smaller. As a precaution, given that a number of participants was excluded from this analysis given poor fits in one of the conditions, we confirmed the abovementioned results by within-subject averaging RTs as a function of condition and then contrasting unisensory and multisensory responses (regardless of distance) in the population as a whole via a paired t-test, as well as a 2 (T − 1 smaller vs. T − 1 larger) × 4 (distance; D2–D5) within-subjects ANOVA including all 38 subjects. These latter analyses confirmed that multisensory RTs were faster than unisensory ones (t = 6.71, P = 7.03 × 10−11), confirming a facilitation in tactile RT during concurrent visual presentation. Furthermore, visual proximity facilitated tactile RTs, as demonstrated by a significant effect of the one-way ANOVA (F = 5.04, P = 0.02). Most importantly, generally RTs after T − 1 larger were quicker (F = 7.69, P = 0.009, main effect) than after T − 1 smaller trial, and there was a significant visuo-tactile disparity by sensory history (T − 1 larger vs. T − 1 smaller) interaction (F = 3.47, P = 0.04; see Supplementary Fig. 1), suggesting a larger spatial extent over which tactile RTs are facilitated by vision after a larger visuo-tactile disparity at T − 1.
FInally, to provide an estimate of the effect size (i.e., impact of trial t vs. t − 1 on RTs) and an estimate regarding how many (n-back) trials directly impact RTs, we fit a linear general model using 10 trials (t to t − 9) to predict RTs (canonical link function expecting a gamma distribution). As shown in Supplementary Figure 2, this analysis suggested that trial t (P = 1.9 × 10−19), t − 1 (P = 9.9 × 10−5), and t − 2 (P = 0.026) significantly impact RTs at trial t. The magnitude of the effect is ~27% that of trial t at trial t − 1, and ~15% at trial t − 2. Of note, this does not mean that trials before t − 2 do not impact PPS encoding but, instead, that they probably do so via an accumulated effect (i.e., requiring the interaction of terms) and not directly.
Given the rapid recalibration effect, we defined the degree at which each participant rapidly recalibrated his/her representation of PPS as the difference between their central point value for T − 1 larger and T − 1 smaller trials (PPS rapid recalibration = T − 1 larger—T − 1 smaller), and then correlated this value with the particular participant’s b value (that contributes to define the raw PPS slope, see eq. 1). This analysis was motivated by the audio-visual rapid recalibration literature (Van der Burg et al. 2013; Noel et al. 2016a), which demonstrates a strong relation between the amount a particular participant rapidly incorporates sensory history and their a priori sensitivity to the task at hand. Similar to the audio-visual studies, our analyses (R2 = 0.22, P = 0.03; Fig. 1D) indicated that the shallower, or more gradual, a participant’s gradient between “near” and “far” space representation (i.e., the less well defined a participant’s PPS boundary is, corresponding to flatter slopes), the more he/she will recalibrate his/her PPS representation. Please note that the relationship is positive here, and not negative, as in the sigmoidal function a larger b value (see eq. 1), indicates a shallower slope (parameter b contributing to defining the steepness/shallowness of the sigmoidal function).
Psychophysics During EEG: Experiment 2
As for Experiment 1, overall participants were very accurate at withholding responses during catch trials (false alarm on 3.75% of trials, SEM = 1.20%), and thus, behavioral results are analyzed solely in light of RTs (see Noel et al. 2015a, 2015b). The paired-samples t-test between visuotactile multisensory presentations and tactile alone presentations was significant (t = 6.76, P = 1.4e-06), indicating that responses to the former condition were quicker (M = 296 ms, SEM = 9.8 ms) than to the latter (M = 317 ms, SEM = 7.3 ms). Namely, the copresentation of visual stimuli with tactile stimulation resulted in multisensory facilitation.
Next, we fit visuo-tactile RTs to a sigmoidal function (see Materials and Methods and Experiment 1) on an individual subject level (goodness of fit; r2 = 0.76) and extract the central point and slope of the function at the central point. This procedure indicated that on average, the inflection point where visual stimuli facilitated tactile RTs was D = 1.73 (see Fig. 2B). Having divided trials given the nature of the immediately precedent trial (i.e., whether a smaller or larger visuo-tactile disparity has been presented on trial T − 1), we contrast both the central point and slope of the function describing tactile RTs given visuo-tactile distance as a function of trial history via a paired samples t-test. This analysis demonstrated that when the precedent trial had been one in which a smaller visuo-tactile spatial disparity was indexed, PPS was smaller than when on the previous trial a larger visuo-tactile disparity had been probed (central point when T − 1 smaller, M = 2.83, SEM = 0.11; central point when T − 1 larger, M = 4.42, SEM = 0.20; t-test T − 1 smaller vs. larger, t = 7.76, P = 1.83e-7; Fig. 2C). Thus, the same effect found as in Experiment 1 for face, PPS was replicated behaviorally in Experiment 2 for hand PPS. There was no difference in the b parameter (eq. 1, contributing to slope), although again a trend existed (P = 0.07) for the b value becoming larger after a large visuo-tactile disparity (T − 1 larger, b = 9.19 ± 1.97, T − 1 smaller, b = 4.57 ± 1.59). Qualitatively similar to the general linear model in Experiment 1, the same analysis here (Supplementary Fig. 2) suggested that trials t (P = 1.2 × 10−169) and t − 1 (P = 4.4 × 10−4) significantly impact RTs. The effect at trial t − 1 is estimated to be ~26% that of trial t (~27% in Experiment 1). Finally, we correlated the amount a particular subject’s PPS shifted due to rapid recalibration (i.e., central point when T − 1 larger minus central point when T − 1 smaller) and the parameter contributing to defining the slope (b). Contrary to Experiment 1, in this case, we did not find a linear relationship between these variables (r = −0.22, P = 0.32).
Electroencephalography Results
Multisensory Responses
Time-resolved t-tests of GFP values to zero (i.e., baseline) indicated significant evoked responses to T (69–93 ms poststimuli onset and 167-ms poststimuli onset and onward), V (75–89 ms poststimuli onset as well as 96-ms poststimuli onset and onward), and VT (78-ms poststimuli onset and onward) stimuli (see Fig. 3 left column). Hence, in a subsequent step, we directly contrasted the paired multisensory response with an artificially created summed visuo-tactile condition that counted with the same energies presented (e.g., V + T), but were not concurrently presented (Fig. 3, right column). This analysis revealed two transient time-periods demonstrating supra-additivity (i.e., VT > V + T; t1, 124–158 ms poststimuli onset; t2, 204–223 ms poststimuli onset), as well as a more sustained epoch of subadditivity (i.e., VT < V + T; 315-ms poststimuli onset onward). As an additional analysis, to control for the fact that the experimental design included a greater number of VT than V or T trials—leading to putative differences in signal-to-noise ratios—we subsampled individual subject data to match trial numbers and repeated this analysis. The results were virtually identical, showing supra-additivity from 123–160 ms to 204–226 ms poststimuli onset (see Supplementary Fig. 3). The different number of VT, V, and T, trials could have also led to the less frequent unisensory stimuli being processed as oddballs (Squires et al. 1975), yet PPS processing has been shown to be independent of attention (Salomon et al. 2017), and somehow one would have to explain why the oddball condition is not modulated by distance, while the standard (VT) condition is.
Space-Dependent Multisensory Responses
Given that GFP analyses contrasting multisensory and unisensory responses revealed the instances of true multisensory integration (i.e., nonlinearity), we next examined whether multisensory responses were modulated by visuo-tactile distance. A one-way ANOVA contrasting visuo-tactile distances D2, D3, D4, D5, and D6 showed a significant effect of distance between 70 and 191 ms poststimuli onset. Interestingly, a comparison of GFPs averages across the 130–150 ms poststimuli onset interval (an interval demonstrating the significant one-way ANOVA effect and exhibiting a clear peak in GFP) revealed a monotonic effect where GFP was largest for the nearest distance (D2, M = 2.52, SD = 0.30) and then in sequence D3 (M = 2.25, SD = 0.30), D4 (M = 2.10, SD = 0.28), D5 (M = 2.04, SD = 0.26), and D6 (M = 1.79, SD = 0.28). All pairwise comparison were significant from one another (P < 0.001), except for the contrast between D4 and D5 (P = 0.068; Fig. 4). Importantly, the same analysis contrasting differences for the visual condition alone did not show a significant main effect (F = 1.17, P = 0.32, see Supplementary Fig. 4). At a sensor and voltage level, the electrodes driving this GFP spatial effect were clustered over the occipital cortex and frontal cortex bilaterally (see Fig. 4). This GFP landscape, which is largely driven by a posterior positivity (see Fig. 5A), is consistent with a visual P1 (Di Russo et al. 2002). That is, seemingly, the modulation as a function of distance is reliant on the presence of a tactile signal, but the major driver of EEG signals around 100–150 ms post–visuo-tactile stimulus onset is visual.
Rapid Recalibration of Space-Dependent Multisensory Responses
Having established that supra-additive multisensory effects were present within the interval ~130–150 ms poststimuli onset and that this interval equally demonstrated a space dependency, we next examined whether we could discern a neural correlate to the rapid recalibration effect. Figure 5A illustrates the topography of voltages across the entire montage during the time-period of interest, suggesting a visually driven dipole (positivity in occipital sensors will lead to a negative pole in frontal sensors given a full montage). Next, the data were categorized as a function of whether the previous trial had been one in which a larger or smaller visuo-tactile discrepancy than the current was presented. A 2 (T − 1 smaller vs. T − 1 larger) × 5 (distance; D2–D6) ANOVA on the mean evoked GFP present between 130 and 150 ms poststimuli onset in the VT condition demonstrated the main effects of distance [F(4, 104) = 151.56, P < 0.001] and the nature of previous trials [F(1, 26) = 70.42, P < 0.001], as well as an interaction between these variables [F(4, 104) = 117.73, P < 0.001]. As indicated above, the main effect of distance was due to a monotonic reduction in GFP as a function of distance, while the main effect of trial history was due to larger GFPs when the precedent trial had been a large spatial disparity (M = 2.22, SEM = 0.051) than when the precedent trial had been one with a smaller spatial disparity (M = 2.05, SEM = 0.053). Most importantly, the interaction was driven by significant difference between T − 1 smaller and T − 1 larger at D3, D4, and D6 (all P < 0.001), but the lack thereof at D2 and D5 (all P > 0.10). The difference at D3 (T − 1 larger, M = 2.60, SEM = 0.35; T − 1 smaller, M = 1.90, SEM = 0.29) and D4 (T − 1 larger, M = 2.48, SEM = 0.30; T − 1 smaller, M = 1.82, SEM = 0.32) were due to stronger evoked responses in the T − 1 larger condition (vs. T − 1 smaller), while the opposite was true at D6 (T − 1 larger, M = 1.38, SEM = 0.26; T − 1 smaller, M = 1.98, SEM = 0.30). Figure 5B illustrates the time-course of the rapid-recalibration effect. Importantly, Figure 5C highlights the electrodes driving the difference in GFP (difference between T − 1 smaller vs. T − 1 larger) within the time period between 130 and 150 ms poststimuli interval. Interestingly, while the GFP is dominated during this time-period by a visual response (Fig. 5A, P1, see above), the modulation of this response as a function of immediately precedent visuo-tactile disparity is seemingly majorly driven by central electrodes, putatively indexing a somatosensory response. The distinct contrast topography (T − 1 smaller—T − 1 larger) for D6 (vs. D3 and D4) appears to be driven by the lack of a consistent response when T − 1 was larger on the previous trial (see blue curve in Fig. 5B). At D6, there are few trials where the previous disparity was larger (solely D7), and this could contribute to the lack of robust response. The same is true for smaller disparities than D2 (solely D1), but at this distance, the VT responses were generally stronger (given the main effect described above).
Neural Network Model
In a naturalistic and ecologically valid augmented-reality setup, we demonstrate that PPS recalibrates on a trial-by-trial basis, and then, we replicate and extend this behavioral effect with static as opposed to dynamic stimuli, with a different body part, and while participants’ electroencephalogram is monitored. The neural results suggest that visuo-tactile responses are supra-additive and that these responses, but not visual-only responses (see Supplementary Fig. 4), are graded as a function of distance during the time-period spanning 130 and 150 ms poststimulus onset. This suggests that the multisensory EEG response indexed is not related to the absolute distance of the observer to visual stimuli but indexes the spatial disparity between touch and vision. Furthermore, the fact that tactile stimulation and importantly its history modulate the same visually driven response (see Fig. 5A) suggests a local, perhaps microcircuit level, mechanism. That is, the short-term hysteresis demonstrated does not appear to depend on, say, a long-range feedback connection from higher order areas—as this would have been evidenced as T − 1 smaller versus T − 1 larger differences occurring earlier than 130 ms and modulating the latter visually driven response. In turn, we hypothesized that rapid recalibration of PPS may be driven by a change in the strength or pattern by which visual and tactile information converge.
We employed a well-established neural network model of PPS (see Fig. 6A) and asked whether—in principle—Hebbian learning (Hebb 1949) could account for the rapid recalibration of PPS. In particular, the strength of synapses was not set in stone but was allowed to alter within the cadre of Hebbian learning, given sensory stimuli mimicking those presented in Experiment 1 (i.e., looming stimuli at 75 cm/s and with 2000 ms between the onset of each approach). In the model, the strength of the feedforward connections between the unisensory neurons and the multisensory node increased when both the presynaptic (unisensory) neuron and the postsynaptic (multisensory) neuron were concurrently active. Furthermore, these feedforward synapses were subject to exponential forgetting (to avoid a scenario where all synapses are maximal; see Materials and Methods for further detail). As a first attempt to determine whether our inherited neural network model of PPS (Magosso et al. 2010a; Noel et al. 2018b) could account for the behavioral results presented here, we simulated a psychophysical experiment with the time constant of the network forgetting rate set to This parameter was set based on the intuition that, in order for the recalibration of PPS to be effective, the time constant of the forgetting rate must be of the same order of magnitude as the time that separates two consecutive responses. Since the meaningful learning takes place at the moment of tactile stimulation, if the time constant of the network is much smaller than the time between stimuli, the network goes back to its basal state and synapses before the following trial and no recalibration can be observed. If the time constant is much larger, instead, the network will retain information from several preceding trials; therefore, synapses will continue to reinforce until they are all saturated, effectively eliminating the PPS altogether (i.e., no space-dependent effect). As illustrated in Figure 6B, the model with =4 s could indeed replicate the abovementioned psychophysical effects, with a difference in the central point of the sigmoid of 4.5 cm between T − 1 larger and T − 1 smaller trials. Interestingly, and similarly to what observed behaviorally, most of the differences are observed in the far space. In the simulations, this happens because when the stimulus is close, the multisensory neuron is already close to maximally active solely because of the visual stimulus. Therefore, the tuning of visual synapses that are close to the body cannot be further enhanced given sensory history. Behaviorally, this is reflected by a floor effect on RTs, which is already maximally fast when the current stimulus is close, regardless of previous history.
To further test our hypothesis regarding the role of the forgetting rate—and provide an analysis regarding the sensitivity of the network to changes in this parameter—we performed further simulations in which we set to a set of values spanning between 1 and 10 s. To be able to compare the results across values of , the simulations were performed on the same set of permuted stimulation distances, as the order of trials influences the results. For the majority of values tested, , the differences we observe in the RT curves are in the expected direction (equal to that reported for ). Also in line with our predictions, there was little to no change as a function of sensory history when ; this is because the network forgets quickly. Finally, when, the model yields a flat PPS, that is, no real PPS. This last result suggests that the rapid recalibration of PPS is not solely an interesting oddity, but within this Hebbian framework, it may be that the rapid recalibration of PPS may be a necessary component to having one: the time constant ruling the forgetting factor of synaptic plasticity must be within a defined range to allow for a PPS that is sensitive to environmental factors. In other words, for the vast majority of temporal constants allowing for PPS (), rapid recalibration occurs (solely when , there is no rapid recalibration).
Finally, we run a simulation with a fixed time constant, , and different velocities for the incoming exteroceptive sensory signal. As shown in Supplementary Figure 5, this sensitivity analyses confirmed that PPS enlarges with increasing exteroceptive velocities (Noel et al. 2018) and suggested that while a rapid recalibration effect was apparent at 75 cm/s, it was not at the other very disparate velocities. At a very large velocity (i.e., 100 cm/s), the RTs are at floor for near distances, and there is only a different between T − 1 smaller and T − 1 larger for the very far distances. At smaller velocities (i.e., 25 and 50 cm/s), there is also no rapid recalibration. Interestingly, this suggests an interplay between the plastic and dynamic aspects of PPS. Namely, as we suggest in Noel et al. 2018, at a small velocity, stimuli are within a given receptive field for a prolonged period of time, which may cause neural adaptation to account for much of the neural response, and thus, the impact of Hebbian learning is not apparent. Altogether, this last simulation suggests that the rapid recalibration of PPS may be observed behaviorally in a regime (speculatively, ~60–90 cm/s) where incoming sensory evidence is fast enough overcome the neural adaptation that occurs when stimuli are within a receptive field for a prolonged period of time and slow enough not to engender floor effects in RTs.
Discussion
We noted that PPS is routinely argued to remap adaptively, given the state of the sensory environment. PPS has been shown to resize or reshape after training lasting a few minutes (Farne and Ladavas 2000; Maravita et al. 2002; Canzoneri et al. 2013; Guterstam et al. 2018), a few hours (Bassolino et al. 2010), months or years. Importantly, however, the timelines of this recalibration—from a few minutes to years—do not match an adaptive time scale. Here, we questioned whether similarly to a whole host of other phenomena, PPS is subject to serial dependency and remaps on a trial-by-trial basis as a function of short-term sensory input and history.
In Experiment 1, we measured in an ecologically valid augmented reality setup (Serino et al. 2017) the distance at which a dynamic external (visual) stimulus affected tactile processing of touch administered on the cheek. This space-dependent multisensory facilitation of tactile processing is taken to be an index of PPS (Serino 2019). We then analyzed tactile responses based on the type of multisensory stimulation received on the immediately preceding trial, that is, whether touch was given when the visual stimulus was either closer or farther from the participant with respect to the current trial. PPS was, respectively, larger or smaller when the preceding trial implied a multisensory interaction at a farther or at a closer distance, suggesting that PPS rapidly recalibrates based on prior sensory statistics. Experiment 2 successfully replicated the same rapid recalibration for the peri-hand space (in an independent set of participants, a different experimental setup, and static as opposed to dynamically looming stimuli and thus eliminating the possibility for an expectancy effect) and demonstrated the generalizability of the findings from Experiment 1, by showing trial-to-trial recalibration, while static (vs. dynamic) and physical (vs. virtual) visual stimuli were presented concurrently to tactile stimulation on the hand (vs. face). Furthermore, while a considerable number of participants showed poor sigmoidal fits in Experiment 1 and thus were excluded from that analysis (although included in confirmatory analyses of variance and the general linear model exploring time-course and effect sizes), in Experiment 2, behavioral fits were good for all but one subject (likely due to the greater number of repetitions) and thus provides strong support for the replicability of the behavioral finding. Altogether, the psychophysical results argue that PPS may not only remap dynamically—that is, within a trial (Brozzoli et al. 2009, 2010; Patane et al. 2018)—or plastically on a slower time scale (Iriki et al. 1996; Farne and Ladavas 2000; Maravita and Iriki 2004; Canzoneri et al. 2013) but is also subject to serial dependency.
Electroencephalographic monitoring of the visuo-tactile task (Experiment 2) showed that the GFP approximately 130-ms post–visuo-tactile presentation is supra-additive (i.e., demonstrates multisensory integration) and scales with visuo-tactile proximity (i.e., demonstrates a PPS effect). Thus, these physiological results add to the behavioral findings by specifying that the PPS effect is rooted in a true process of multisensory integration where the sum of unisensory responses does not predict the paired response (see Stein and Stanford 2008). Interestingly, the amplitude of the same multisensory and space-dependent response (~130-ms poststimuli onset) was altered as a function of whether the previous trial had been one with greater or smaller visuo-tactile spatial disparity. The timing of this effect is in line with the descriptions of the EEG components present during rapid recalibration of audio-visual temporal acuity (Simon et al. 2017). The voltage response at this time period was seemingly primarily driven by the visual response, while the topography of its modulation (i.e., difference wave) suggested an additional central origin, likely sensorimotor (yet this is speculative given the inverse problem). The conjecture that the history-dependent modulation in visual P1 amplitude was driven (at least in part) by sensorimotor signals is further supported by the fact that this modulation was most evident at distances showing a PPS remapping (from D2/D3 to D4/D5) as defined by tactile RTs, and is in line with panoply of recent rodent studies demonstrating modulations in neural responses as early as V1 given somatosensory and proprioceptive inputs (see Chaplin and Margrie 2020, for a recent review). The rapid recalibration topography also suggests potential voltage amplitude differences at occipital sensors, which would be in line with a more recent emphasis on bottom-up and early multisensory integration in classically considered unisensory areas (see Ghazanfar and Schroeder 2006 and Schroeder and Foxe 2005 for reviews).
Overall, these results add to the general neuroimaging literature on PPS and the growing study of this multisensory space in a time-resolved manner. Sambo and Foster (2009) showed modulations in the amplitude of the visual P1 and N140 with tactile proximity, but they only indexed two distances and did not ascertain the multisensory—integrative—nature of this response, nor if this response mimicked spatial remapping. In recent studies, Bernasconi et al. (2018), as well as Noel et al. (2019), showed a modulation of local field potentials evoked by touch on the trunk/hand as a function of the proximity of auditory stimuli presented at different distances. Results demonstrated PPS processing (i.e., different response for near vs. father stimuli) as early as 50-ms poststimuli onset, from insular cortex, but most commonly around 200-ms poststimuli onset, mainly from pre- and postcentral gyri (Bernasconi et al. 2018; Noel et al. 2019). These studies, however, did not include a condition wherein PPS was remapped, and thus, there has been no description of the EEG correlates of PPS remapping. Finally, Noel et al. (2018b) showed that audio-visual responses are facilitated near the boundary of PPS and only evident ~300-ms poststimulus onset. Altogether, this pattern of results is evocative of a temporal cascade, wherein visuo-tactile near space is differentiated from the far space first (~130 ms), then audio-tactile space is (~200 ms), and finally, perhaps, a space-selective audio-visual processing (Van der Stoep et al. 2016; Noel et al. 2018d) is scaffolded upon this encoding of PPS. This speculation, however, needs to be confirmed by further studies directly comparing visuo-tactile, audio-tactile, and audio-visual integration as a function of distance.
The neuroimaging results hint at two interesting possibilities. First, the P1 is considered to index fidelity of visual encoding (Di Russo et al. 2002), and hence, the fact that, when paired with tactile stimuli, this component is moderated by visuo-tactile distance implies that visual perception itself may be different within and outside the PPS. Indeed, initial studies in this domain have suggested that shape discrimination is better within the PPS—even after accounting for relative size (Blini et al. 2019). Second, the fact that the index of rapid recalibration of PPS was not prior to the component graded by visuo-tactile disparity (~130 ms) arguably suggests that this remapping is driven by a local reweighting of the convergence between visual and tactile signals or the co-activation of both somatosensory and visual responsive units. In this line, we have recently suggested that dynamic remapping, at least that provoked by stimuli velocity, may be instantiated by firing-dependent adaptation of multisensory neurons (Noel et al. 2018b). Speculatively, the mechanism behind the slower timescale plasticity of PPS most often indexed in the PPS literature may originate from distinct neural areas (e.g., feedback connections from arousal or valence centers during value-based remapping; Bufacchi and Iannetti 2018). That is, we hypothesize that the faster timescale remapping of PPS (dynamically within a trial, or rapidly on a trial-by-trial basis) may be intrinsic to the multisensory PPS circuitry (i.e., respectively, neural adaptation and Hebbian reweighting of synapses), while slower and longer lasting recalibration may be driven by factors extrinsic to the PPS circuitry itself (Bufacchi and Iannetti 2018; see Noel and Serino 2019).
To further ascertain whether Hebbian plasticity is in principle capable of accounting for rapid recalibration, we employed a well-established neural network model of PPS (Magosso et al. 2010a, 2010b). The model instantiates a biological-plausible network computing PPS, at least to the extent that the microcircuitry and cellular/molecular composition of PPS computation is known (an area of investigation understudied and that will undoubtedly be fruitful in further constraining future models). The network is biologically plausible in that it is composed of unisensory areas projecting onto a multisensory node, which in turn projects back. This architecture has been demonstrated in the brain via anatomical tracer studies where PPS neurons in posterior parietal cortex (e.g., ventral intra-parietal area) project directly (1 synapse) to and from early and later somatosensory areas (BA3, 1, and 2, as well as the secondary somatosensory area and insular cortex), as well as intermediate and late visual areas (lateral intra-parietal area, 7a and b, and MSTd; Hihara et al. 2006). Furthermore, the model is built with aligned visual and somatosensory receptive fields, mimicking the canonical organization of multisensory neurons whose unisensory responses are typically aligned in space (best studied in superior colliculus for audio, visual, and audio-visual stimuli but largely true broadly; Meredith and Stein 1996; Murray and Wallace 2012). Finally, the time constants driving Hebbian learning and the rest of neural dynamics (e.g., simulation time step) are also consistent with known biology (Arnsten et al. 2010; Dayan and Abbot 2001, respectively). Nevertheless, a more direct mapping of the model principles and parameters to their biological implementation would require a significant increase in the amount of information currently available and/or a combination of modeling and empirical neurophysiological studies, whereby experiments will be designed to test specific parameters from the model.
We added to the model presented in Noel et al. 2018b—which is capable of dynamic resizing due to neural adaptation—the flexibility of strengthening or weakening the feedforward synapses, i.e., the synapses from unisensory to multisensory areas. The modulation of the strength between synapses was performed given the co-occurrence of pre- and postsynaptic firing (i.e., Hebbian learning). The neural network was capable of accounting for the rapid recalibration of PPS over a wide range of time constant values (from 2 to ~7.5), while PPS was not plastic (on a rapid time scale) in a smaller range (from 0 to 2), and vanished entirely for values beyond ~7.5 (see Noel et al. 2018b, for a demonstration that the network’s capacity to engender a PPS effect is immune to changes in other parameter values). This pattern suggests that to the extent that the rapid recalibration of PPS is scaffolded on a Hebbian mechanism, its rapid recalibration is not a peculiarity, but a piece carefully regulated to allow for plasticity and concurrent gradation between the near and far space. That is, a time constant of integration large enough that would not allow for rapid recalibration of PPS would also eliminate the representation of PPS altogether.
Importantly, we do not mean to suggest that Hebbian learning is the sole mechanism that could potentially explain the rapid recalibration of PPS. For example, history-dependent modifications in the dynamical state of the network (i.e., a kind of working memory) could play an equally important role. For parsimony, however, we maintained the same mechanism used to model longer term forms of PPS plasticity (i.e., tool use, Serino et al. 2015a, 2015b) and show that it can also account for rapid recalibration of PPS, when implemented in this more extreme, yet physiologically plausible form (synaptic plasticity has been observed to be effective even over timescales shorter than those modeled in our work; Arnsten et al. 2010). Finally, it is worth noticing that the time constant of the forgetting factor able to show recalibration was found to be the same order of magnitude of the time separating two consecutive multisensory stimulations. Future studies, both experimental and theoretical, could be performed to investigate the effect of increasing or decreasing the frequency of stimuli presentation. Moreover, additional models can be implemented that combine the short-term plasticity of the current model with the long-term synaptic maturation rules used in previous works (Magosso et al. 2010a; Serino et al. 2015a), to investigate the interplay of different timescale in PPS plasticity.
We consider that in addition to demonstrating within the cadre of PPS a well-established effect seemingly applied throughout the nervous system (i.e., serial dependency), the current findings may also offer important insight into the functional role of PPS. Namely, given the current description of PPS as involved in defensive behaviors (Graziano and Cooke 2006), one could have postulated that after near visuo-tactile stimulations (a potential threat), PPS would enlarge as to increase the “safety zone.” This did not happen. Conversely, after near visuo-tactile stimulation, PPS seems to shrink. This result may hint toward a more general probabilistic framework within which PPS ought to be understood; it is to be expected that higher integration between vision and touch is predicted in a given portion of space if the two stimuli are more likely to be frequently associated in that space. The current findings suggest that with positive serial dependence, PPS keeps track of the history of multisensory stimuli, heavily weighting the most recent trial or two, but also beyond that. This feature is a key property of Bayesian priors (a running average of past experiences) and thus suggests that PPS may be a prior for coupling stimuli on the body with stimuli from the near environment (see Noel et al. 2018e, for a direct demonstration that PPS acts as a visuo-proprioceptive coupling prior). Importantly, positive serial dependency is adaptive in that it places the boundary of PPS—there where the dynamical range is, most sharply differentiating between the near and far space—at a distance where it is most likely to encounter exteroceptive stimuli given recent environmental statistics.
To conclude, here, we show that at the behavioral, electrophysiological, and computational level, a recalibration of the extent of PPS based on the type of multisensory interaction just experienced, on a trial-by-trial basis. This almost on-line regulation of the PPS boundary fits well with the current accounts of PPS (Brozzoli et al. 2014; Cléry et al. 2015; Bufacchi and Iannetti 2018; Serino 2019), which agree in describing it as multisensory-motor system mediating defensive and approaching interactions between the body and external stimuli. This space, therefore, needs to rapidly adapt to sudden changes in the environment to allow for adaptive responses.
Supplementary Material
Author Contributions
J.P.N. designed research, performed research, analyzed data, and wrote the paper. T.B. performed research, analyzed data, and wrote the paper. E.T. performed research. E.P. performed research. B.H. contributed tools. C.C. contributed tools. E.M. performed research, contributed tools, and wrote the paper. O.B. contributed tools and wrote the paper. M.W. contributed tools and wrote the paper. A.S. designed research, contributed tools, and wrote the paper.
Funding
Swiss National Foundation (PP00P3_163951/1); Fondation Biaggi (Lausanne, Switzerland) (to A.S.); Bertarelli Foundation and the Swiss National Science Foundation (grant number 320030_166643 to O.B.); NIH (CA183492, HD083211 to M.T.W.); NIH/NIMH (grant number 1F31MH112336 to J.P.N.).
Notes
The authors thank Jacob Feldman for help with figures. Conflict of Interest: The authors declare no competing financial interests.
References
- Arnsten AF, Paspalas CD, Gamo NJ, Yang Y, Wang M. 2010. Dynamic network connectivity: a new form of neuroplasticity. Trends Cogn Sci. 14(8):365–375. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.05.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bassolino M, Serino A, Ubaldi S, Làdavas E. 2010. Everyday use of the computer mouse extends peripersonal space representation. Neuropsychologia. 48:803–811. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.11.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bernasconi F, Noel J-P, Park H, Faivre N, Seeck M, Spinelli L et al. . 2018, 2018. Audio-tactile and peripersonal space processing around the trunk in human parietal and temporal cortex: an intracranial EEG study. Cereb Cortex. 28(9):3385–3397. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy156. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blini E, Desoche C, Salemme R, Kabil A, Hadj-Bouziane F, Farnè A. 2019. Mind the depth: visual perception of shapes is better in peripersonal space. Psychol Sci. 29:1868–1877. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brozzoli C, Cardinali L, Pavani F, Farnè A. 2010. Action-specific remapping of peripersonal space. Neuropsychologia. 48(3):796–802. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brozzoli C, Ehrsson H, Farné A. 2014. Multisensory representation of the space near the hand: from perception to action and interindividual interactions. Neuroscientist. 20(2):122–135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brozzoli C, Pavani F, Urquizar C, Cardinali L, Farnè A. 2009. Grasping actions remap peripersonal space. Neuroreport. 20(10):913–917. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brunet D, Murray MM, Michel CM. 2011. Spatiotemporal analysis of multichannel EEG: CARTOOL. Comput Intell Neurosci. 2011(813870):15. doi: 10.1155/2011/813870. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bufacchi R, Iannetti G. 2018. An action field theory of peripersonal space. Trends Cogn Sci. 22:1076–1090. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Canzoneri E, Magosso E, Serino A. 2012. Dynamic sounds capture the boundaries of peripersonal space representation in humans. PLoS One. 7:e44306. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0044306. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Canzoneri E, Marzolla M, Amoresano A, Verni G, Serino A. 2013. Amputation and prosthesis implantation shape body and peripersonal space representations. Sci Rep. 3:2844. doi: 10.1038/srep02844. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cappe C, Thut G, Romei V, Murray MM. 2010. Auditory-visual multisensory interactions in humans: timing, topography, directionality, and sources. J Neurosci. 30:12572–12580. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chaplin TA, Margrie TW. 2020. Cortical circuits for integration of self-motion and visual motion cues. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 60:122–128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cléry J, Guipponi O, Odouard S, Pinède S, Wardak C, Hamed SB. 2017. The prediction of impact of a looming stimulus onto the body is subserved by multisensory integration mechanisms. J Neurosci. 37:10656–10670. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cléry J, Guipponi O, Odouard S, Wardak C, Ben Hamed S. 2015. Impact prediction by looming visual stimuli enhances tactile detection. J Neurosci. 35:4179–4189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cléry J, Guipponi O, Odouard S, Wardak C, Ben Hamed S. 2018. Cortical networks for encoding near and far space in the non-human primate. NeuroImage. 176:164–178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooke DF, Graziano MSA. 2004. Sensorimotor integration in the precentral gyrus: polysensory neurons and defensive movements. J Neurophysiol. 91:1648–1660. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooke DF, Taylor CSR, Moore T, Graziano MSA. 2003. Complex movements evoked by microstimulation of the ventral intraparietal area. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 100:6163–6168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dayan P, Abbot LF. 2001. Theoretical neuroscience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Delorme A, Makeig S. 2004. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J Neurosci Methods. 134:9–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- di Pellegrino G, Làdavas E, Farnè A. 1997. Seeing where your hands are. Nature. 21:730. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Di Russo F, Martínez A, Sereno MI, Pitzalis S, Hillyard SA. 2002. Cortical sources of the early components of the visualevoked potential. Hum Brain Mapp. 15:95–111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duhamel JR, Colby CL, Goldberg ME. 1998. Ventral intraparietal of the macaque: congruent visual and somatic response properties. J Neurophysiol. 79:126–136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dürr V, Schilling M. 2018. Transfer of spatial contact information among limbs and the notion of peripersonal space in insects. Front Comput Neurosci. 12:101. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2018.00101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farne A, Ladavas E. 2000. Dynamic size-change of hand peripersonal space following tool use. Neuroreport. 11:1645–1649. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fernberger SW. 1920. Interdependence of judgments within the series for the method of constant stimuli. J Exp Psychol. 3:126–150. [Google Scholar]
- Fischer J, Whitney D. 2014. Serial dependency in visual perception. Nat Neurosci. 17:738. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fogassi L, Gallese V, Fadiga L, Luppino G, Matelli M, Rizzolatti G. 1996. Coding of peripersonal space in inferior premotor cortex (area F4). J Neurophysiol. 76(1):141–157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fujisaki W, Shimojo S, Kashino M, Nishida S. 2004. Recalibration of audiovisual simultaneity. Nat Neurosci. 7:773–778. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ghazanfar AA, Schroeder CE. 2006. Is neocortex essentially multisensory? Trends Cogn Sci. 10:278–285. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.04.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Graziano MSA, Cooke DF. 2006. Parieto-frontal interactions, personal space, and defensive behavior. Neuropsychologia. 44:845–859. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Graziano MSA, Hu XT, Gross CG. 1997. Visuospatial properties of ventral premotor cortex. J Neurophysiol. 77:2268–2292. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Graziano MSA, Reiss LAJ, Gross CG. 1999. A neuronal representation of the location of nearby sounds. Nature. 397:428–430. doi: 10.1038/17115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Graziano MSA, Yap GS, Gross CG. 1994. Coding of visual space by premotor neurons. Science. 266:1054–1057. doi: 10.1126/science.7973661. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guterstam A, Szczotka J, Zeberg H, Ehrsson HH. 2018. Tool use changes the spatial extension of the magnetic touch illusion. J Exp Psychol Gen. 147:298–303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guthrie D, Buchwald JS. 1991. Significance testing of difference potentials. Psychophysiology. 28:240–244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hebb DO. 1949. The organization of behavior. New York: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
- Hihara S, Notoya T, Tanaka M, Ichinose S, Ojima H, Obayashi S. 2006. Extension of corticocortical afferents into the anterior bank of the intraparietal sulcus by tool-use training in adult monkeys. Neuropsychologia. 44:2636–2646. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hobeika L, Taffou M, Carpentier T, Warusfel O, Viaud-Delmon I. 2019, 2019. Capturing the dynamics of peripersonal space by integrating expectancy effects and sound propagation properties. J Neurosci Methods. 332:108534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iriki A, Tanaka M, Iwamura Y. 1996. Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurones. Neuroreport. 7:2325–2330. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199610020-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kandula M, Van der Stoep N, Hofman D, Dijkerman HC. 2017. On the contribution of overt tactile expectations to visuo-tactile interactions within the peripersonal space. Exp Brain Res. 235:2511–2522. doi: 10.1007/s00221-017-4965-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- La Chioma A, Bonhoeffer T, Hübener M. 2019. Area-specific mapping of binocular disparity across mouse visual cortex. Curr Biol. 29:1–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lehmann D. 1987. Principles of spatial analysis In: Gevins AS, Remond A, editors. Handbook of electroencephalography and clinical logy. Vol 1 Amsterdam: Methods of Analysis of Brain Electrical and Magnetic Signals, Elsevier, pp. 309–354. [Google Scholar]
- Lehmann D, Skrandies W. 1980. Reference-free identification of components of checkerboard evoked multichannel potential fields. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 48:609–621. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Magosso E, Ursino M, di Pellegrino G, Ladavas E, Serino A. 2010a. Neural bases of peri-hand space plasticity through tool-use: insights from a combined computational-experimental approach. Neuropsychologia. 48:812–830. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.09.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Magosso E, Zavaglia M, Serino A, di Pellegrino G, Ursino M. 2010b. Visuotactile representation of peripersonal space: a neural network study. Neural Comput. 22:190–243. doi: 10.1162/neco.2009.01-08-694. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maravita A, Clarke K, Husain M, Driver J. 2002. Active tool-use with contralesional hand can reduce crossmodal extinction of touch on that hand. Neurocase. 8:411–416. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maravita A, Iriki A. 2004. Tools for the body (schema). Trends Cogn Sci. 8:79–86. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2003.12.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meredith M, Stein B. 1996. Spatial determinants of multisensory integration in cat superior colliculus neurons. J Neurophysiol. 75(5):1843–1857. doi: 10.1152/jn.1996.75.5.1843. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murray MM, Wallace MT. 2012. The neural bases of multisensory processes. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murray MM, Brunet D, Michel CM. 2008. Topographic ERP analyses: a step-by-step tutorial review. Brain Topogr. 20:249–264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noel J-P, De Niear M, Van der Burg E, Wallace MT. 2016a. Audiovisual simultaneity judgment and rapid recalibration throughout the lifespan. PLoS One. 11(8):e0161698. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0161698. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noel J-P, Chatelle C, Perdikis S, Jöhr J, Lopes Da Silva M, Ryvlin P, De Lucia M, del R, Millán J, Diserens K et al. . 2019. Peri-personal space encoding in patients with disorders of consciousness and cognitive-motor dissociation. NeuroImage: Clinical. 24:101940. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noel JP, Samad M, Doxon A, Clark J, Keller S, Di Luca M. 2018e. Peri-personal space as a prior in coupling visual and proprioceptive signals. Sci Rep. 8:15819. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-33961-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noel J-P, De Niear MA, Stevenson R, Alais D, Wallace MT. 2016b. Atypical rapid audio-visual temporal recalibration in autism spectrum disorders. Autism Res. 110:121–129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noel J-P, Pfeiffer C, Blanke O, Serino A. 2015a. Peri-personal space as the space of the bodily self. Cognition. 114:49–57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noel JP, Serino A. 2019. High action values occur near our body. Trends Cogn Sci. 23(4):269–270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noel JP, Grivaz P, Marmaroli P, Lissek H, Blanke O, Serino A. 2015b. Full body action remapping of peripersonal space: the case of walking. Neuropsychologia. 70:375–384. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noel JP, Modi K, Wallace M, Van der Stoep N. 2018a. Audiovisual integration in depth: multisensory binding and gain as a function of distance. Exp Brain Res. 236(7):1939–1951. doi: 10.1007/s00221-018-5274-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noel JP, Blanke O, Magosso E, Serino A. 2018b. Neural adaptation accounts for the dynamic resizing of peri-personal space: evidence from a psychophysical–computational approach. J Neurophysiol. 119(6):2307–2333. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noel JP, Park HD, Pasqualini I, Lissek H, Wallace M, Blanke O, Serino A. 2018. Audio-visual sensory deprivation degrades visuo-tactile peri-personal space. Conscious Cogn. 61:61–75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noel JP, Serino A, Wallace M. 2018c. Increased neural strenght and reliability at the boundary of peri-personal space. J Cogn Neurosci. 31(8):1155–1172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noel JP, Simon D, Thelen A, Maier A, Blake R, Wallace M. 2018d. Probing electrophysiological indices of perceptual awareness across unisensory and multisensory modalities. J Cogn Neurosci. 30(6):814–828. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noel JP, Wallace MT, Orchard-Mills E, Alais D, Burg V. 2015c. True and perceived synchrony are preferentially associated with particular sensory pairings. Sci Rep. 5:17467. doi: 10.1038/srep17467. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patane I, Cardinali L, Salemme R, Pavani F, Farne A, Brozzoli C. 2018. Action planning modulates peripersonal space. J Cogn Neurosci. 30:1–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pellencin E, Paladino P, Herbelin B, Serino A. 2017. Social perception of others spaces one’s own multisensory peripersonal space. Cortex. S0010-9452:30290–30293. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.08.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pfeiffer C, Noel J-P, Serino A, Blanke O. 2018. Vestibular modulation of peripersonal space boundaries. Eur J Neurosci. 47:800–811. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rizzolatti G, Fadiga L, Fogassi L, Gallese V. 1997. The space around us. Science. 277:190–191. doi: 10.1126/science.277.5323.190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rizzolatti G, Scandolara C, Matelli M, Gentilucci M. 1981. Afferent prop- erties of periarcuate neurons in macque monkeys. II. Visual responses. Behav Brain Res. 2:147–163. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(81)90053-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salomon R, Noel JP, Lukowska M et al. . 2017. Unconscious integration of multisensory bodily inputs in the peripersonal space shapes bodily self-consciousness. Cognition. 166:174–183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sambo CF, Forster B. 2009. An ERP investigation on visuotactile interactions in peripersonal and extrapersonal space: evidence for the spatial rule. J Cognitive Neurosci. 21:1550–1559. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schlack A, Sterbing S, Hartung K, Hoffmann KP, Bremmer F. 2005. Multisensory space representations in the macaque ventral intraparietal area. J Neurosci. 25:4616–4625. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0455-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schroeder CE, Foxe J. 2005. Multisensory contributionsto low-level, ‘unisensory’ processing. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 15:454–458. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Serino A, Canzoneri E, Marzolla M, Pellegrino G, Magosso E. 2015a. Extending peripersonal space representation without tool-use: evidence from a combined behavioral-computational approach. Front Behav Neurosci. 9:4. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Serino A. 2019. Peripersonal space (PPS) as a multisensory interface between the individual and the environment, defining the space of the self. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 99:138–159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Serino A, Noel J-P, Mange R et al. . 2017. Peripersonal space: an index of multisensory body–environment interactions in real, virtual, and mixed realities. Front ICT. 4:31. doi: 10.3389/fict.2017.00031. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Serino A, Noel JP, Galli G, Canzoneri E, Marmaroli P, Lissek H, Blanke O. 2015b. Body part-centered and full body-centered peripersonal space representations. Sci Rep. 5:18603. doi: 10.1038/srep18603. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stein BE, Stanford TR. 2008. Multisensory integration: current issues from the perspective of the single neuron. Nat Rev Neurosci. 9:255–266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simon DM, Noel J-P, Wallace MT. 2017. Event related potentials index rapid recalibration to audiovisual temporal asynchrony. Front Integr Neurosci. 11:8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sperdin HF, Cappe C, Murray MM. 2010. The behavioral relevance of multisensory neural response interactions. Front Neurosci. 4:9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Squires NK, Squires KC, Hillyard SA. 1975. Two varieties of long-latency positive waves evoked by unpredictable auditory stimuli in man. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 38(4):387–401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van der Burg E, Alais D, Cass J. 2013. Rapid recalibration to audiovisual asynchrony. J Neurosci. 33(37):14633–14637. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van der Burg E, Alais D, Cass J. 2015. Audiovisual temporal recalibration occurs independently at two different time scales. Scientific reports, 5, article number: 14526. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Van der Stoep N, Van der Stigchel S, Nijboer TCW, Van der Smagt MJ. 2016. Audiovisual integration in near and far space: effects of changes in distance and stimulus effectiveness. Exp Brain Res. 234(5):1175–1188. doi: 10.1007/s00221-015-4248-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vroomen J, Keetels M, Gelder B, Bertelson P. 2004. Recalibration of temporal order perception by exposure to audio-visual asynchrony. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 22(1):32–35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yu AJ, Cohen JD. 2008. Sequential effects: superstition or rational behavior. Adv Neural Inf Proces Syst. 21:1873–1880. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.