Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2020 Jul 29;10:12677. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-69507-9

Stronger resting-state neural oscillations associated with wiser advising from the 2nd- but not the 3rd-person perspective

Chengli Huang 1,2,3, Haotian Zhang 1,2,3, Jinhao Huang 1,2,3, Cuiwen Duan 4, Juensung J Kim 5, Michel Ferrari 5, Chao S Hu 1,2,3,
PMCID: PMC7391636  PMID: 32728108

Abstract

This is the first electroencephalogram study exploring the personal perspective effect on wise advising. Participants advised hypothetical protagonists in life dilemmas from both the 2nd- and 3rd-person perspective. Their advice for each dilemma was rated by two independent raters on wisdom criteria, i.e., metacognitive humility, metacognitive flexibility, and perspective taking. The results revealed that participants felt a significantly shorter psychological distance from protagonists when advising from the 2nd- (vs. the 3rd-) person perspective, p < 0.001. However, there was no significant effect of perspective condition on the wisdom score. Nevertheless, stronger resting-state absolute EEG powers in the frontal lobe were associated with wiser advising from the 2nd-, but not the 3rd-person perspective. Moreover, Z tests revealed that the correlations between the resting-state absolute EEG powers and wisdom scores were significantly stronger during advising from the 2nd- than the 3rd-person perspective. These results suggest that advising from the 2nd-person perspective was more self-related, and mental activities during rest contributed to advising from the 2nd- but not the 3rd-person perspective.

Subject terms: Neuroscience, Psychology

Introduction

What makes one wise may lie in one’s resting mental activity. In fact, "resting-state" may not be resting at all. The brain is relatively active during resting-state, with an ongoing high level of neural network dynamics ready to allocate neural resources to potential brain activities1. When not engaged in processing external information, the resting-state brain is still undertaking internally-oriented self-related cognitive activities such as self-referential processing24, mind-wandering5,6, autobiographical memory retrieval and reasoning7, which are essential processes for wisdom derived from life narratives8,9. In particular, the "rest-self overlap" (i.e., the phenomenon that resting-state brain activities are always related to the processes of the internal self) may be a manifestation of wisdom from the 2nd-person perspective, which is likely to induce self-involvement. Therefore, resting-state brain activity may contribute to wisdom about life, which manifests in wise advising on others’ problems.

Wise advising from the 2nd- versus the 3rd-person perspective

Wisdom has fascinated philosophers, theologians, and ethicists, East and West, for thousands of years10,11. The Berlin Wisdom Paradigm (BWP) is a well-known and well-researched open-ended performance measure of wisdom that requires participants to think aloud about brief vignettes describing difficult fictitious life problems12. For instance, "Somebody gets a phone call from a good friend who says that he/she can't go on any more, that she/he has decided to commit suicide. What should the person/one do and consider?"13. Giving wise advice (i.e., advising) for others in life dilemmas is regarded as an essential performance embodiment of "wisdom"1114 in both experts and lay people’s definitions of wisdom1417. Through the external behavior of advising, it is possible to gain insight into one’s wisdom. More specifically, affording flexible, context-sensitive advice reflects metacognitive humility, metacognitive flexibility, and perspective taking—three hallmarks of wisdom consistently defined by researches as "morally-grounded application of metacognition to reasoning and problem-solving"18.

Wisdom is not merely theoretical knowledge, but a profoundly personal phenomenon, rooted in individual experiences and the insights gained from them19. Therefore, self-related mental activities such as self-involvement and self-reflection should be necessary antecedents to transforming knowledge19 and life experience9,20 into wisdom. In the process of wise advising, person perspective is a subtle but important manifestation of self-involvement and self-reflection.

Most BWP studies focus on advising from the 3rd-person perspective12; little is known about wise advising from a 2nd-person perspective, which is common in daily life. We propose that advising from the 2nd- (vs. the 3rd-) person perspective might be more self-related. First, the 2nd- (vs. the 3rd-) person perspective should involve a smaller psychological distance from the protagonist in the hypothetical vignette, making advisers feel closer and more self-involved during advising. Psychological distance is a subjective experience that something is close or far away from the self, here, and now21. Calling someone "YOU" assumes that the advice-receiver is right in front of us, or at least in direct contact, whereas calling someone "HE/SHE" implies the advice-receiver is far away. Secondly, "YOU" may provide an opportunity for self-reflection. Previous studies on "self-talk" (wherein people typically refer to themselves as "You") suggest that "YOU" is expected to arise in contexts requiring explicit self-control or conscious self-guidance22, and plays a crucial role in introspection23. Moreover, generic-you, another kind of the 2nd-person perspective, is used to express more general generalizations that are deeply self-relevant and allows individuals to derive broader meanings from negative experience24,25. This process of meaning construction conforms to advising as expressing wisdom—through meanings derived from life narratives26.

Additionally, the 2nd-person perspective is also related to internally-oriented cognition, such as experience simulation27 and mind-reading28,29. While the 1st-person perspective is subjective and the 3rd-person perspective is objective, the 2nd-person perspective is intersubjective because it invokes a relation between an epistemic subject and another sentient being’s mental states27,30,31. Such an intersubjective perspective requires people to simulate or imagine certain experiences, as well as beliefs, desires, and emotions, to access another’s mental states27. Besides, unlike merely observing others from the 3rd-person perspective, the 2nd-person perspective implies interacting with others, which provides a unique kind of access to other minds28,29. In fact, wisdom might be a phenomenon that is more likely to be observed when multiple minds are interacting13,32,33. Such an interact-mind activity is well embodied in the dialogue. A previous study found that interpersonal dialogue, even imagined (internal dialogical activity), improved the level of wisdom-related performance by almost one standard deviation compared to the group which reflected on the solutions alone13.

To sum up, advising from the 2nd-person perspective should induce more self-related mental activities and internally-oriented cognitions, which may contribute to wise advising.

Resting-state brain activity in the frontal lobe and its relationship with self

Spontaneous thought processes during resting-state share common brain structures with a variety of higher cognitive functions7,34,35. For example, the resting brain activities have been linked to the anterior prefrontal cortex34, which is activated during reasoning (e.g., Raven’s Progressive Matrices)36, and memory retrieval 37. The anterior prefrontal cortex is also thought to be specifically involved in the evaluation of internally generated information, or information that cannot be readily perceived from the external environment but has to be inferred or self-generated38. In terms of neural oscillations, resting-state electroencephalogram (EEG) reflects the electrophysiological predisposition of human behavior. For example, stronger resting-state EEG power was found to be significantly positively correlated with IQ1,39,40, problem-solving (e.g., anagram)41.

Resting-state brain activities are also related to the processes of the internal self. Previous studies found that resting-state neural activity was related to conflict-related activity in internally-guided decision making (IDM) (e.g., choosing color participants preferred), but not in externally-guided decision making (EDM) (e.g., choosing a color more frequently displayed in the experiment)42,43. Moreover, previous studies have demonstrated a robust neural overlap between self-related activities (e.g., label stimuli as self-referential, self-awareness, autobiographical memory) and resting-state activities within cortical midline structures (CMS)2,3,44, especially the anterior cortical midline structures, such as medial orbital prefrontal cortex (MOFC), medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC), ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC), pregenual anterior cingulate cortex (PACC), supragenual anterior cingulate cortex (SACC), and dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC)4,4547. Such "rest-self overlap" suggests that the brain’s resting-state activity is closely related to our sense of self, self-referential, or self-consciousness4850. Further, the self-related network is also recruited during others' mind-reading, memory recall, reasoning7, and self-introspection51.

Although most "self-rest overlap" findings come from neuroimaging studies (e.g., fMRI, PET), it should be noted that hemodynamic change is significantly positively correlated with electrophysiological oscillations52, e.g., mean frequency of the EEG was significantly correlated with the cerebral blood flow53. Consistently, a growing body of EEG studies supports the interesting "self-rest overlap". For example, temporary measures of resting-state EEG as featured by temporal nestedness, temporal continuity, and temporal integration can predict self-consciousness temporally50. Moreover, resting-state alpha power at frontal regions before stimulus presentation or perception was significantly positively correlated with the perception of stimuli judged to be highly self-related54. In a cross-cultural study, higher self-referential thought (SRT) scores were associated with resting-state higher alpha activity in the anterior default mode network (ADMN) hub (i.e., superior frontal gyrus and the anterior cingulate cortex) among Taiwanese participants (but not Russian participants)55.

Consider together, this evidence suggests that resting-state neural activities, especially those in the frontal lobe, are associated with cognitive activities related to the inner self. Importantly, self-related cognitive activities (e.g., self-reflection and self-introspection), more easily induced from the 2nd-person perspective, are considered essential subcomponents of wisdom29, or for developing wisdom17. Thus, it can cautiously propose that resting-state brain activation should be correlated with wise advising from the 2nd-person perspective.

Hypotheses

  • The 2nd- (vs. the 3rd-) person perspective should involve a smaller psychological distance from the protagonist in the hypothetic vignette.

  • Resting-state neural oscillations should be significantly positively correlated with wise advising from the 2nd-person perspective. Besides, the correlations between resting-state neural oscillations and wise advising should be stronger during advising from the 2nd- than the 3rd-person perspective.

Materials and methods

All procedures used in the current study were approved by the ethics committee at the sponsoring University. The current study used the software program G*Power3.1 to conduct a power analysis. Our goal was to obtain 0.80 power to detect a hypothetical effect size based on the smallest effect size (R2 = 0.15) reported in previous studies on EEG correlates of self-related processing55,56 at the standard 0.05 alpha error probability. All methods were performed in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations.

Participants

A total of 63 graduate students (28 males, 35 females; mean age 23.78 ± 1.55 years, range 19–28 years), who had never involved in BWP studies before, were recruited via campus advertisement. Twelve participants were excluded due to low voltage EEG signal or behavioral measures failure in the data analyses. Thus, the final results were based on the data of 51 participants (18 females, 33 males; mean age 23.59 ± 1.58 years, range 19–28 years). All participants were provided informed consent before the participation, and each received 80 RMB as compensation.

Materials

Wisdom vignettes

Two Berlin-style fundamental life scenarios: (1) the meaning of education: "One university student is doing well in all aspects of family and study, but suddenly begins to think about the meaning of life, and then feels that it is meaningless to go to college and wants to drop out of school."; and (2) the choice between living and dying: "There is a student who grew up in a family lacked love. He does not get along well with people and has no friends. Recently, he was rejected for pursuing a female classmate and wanted to commit suicide."

Based on the traditional Berlin-style wisdom vignettes, the current study leveraged a well-adapted 2nd-person perspective wise advising paradigm in which participants imagined the lens of a camera as the eyes of hypothetical protagonists in specific life dilemmas and talked to "him"16,57. Previous studies showed that the paradigm had acceptable inter-rater and inter-item reliabilities, and minimized social desirability bias57. Different from previous research, both the 2nd- and 3rd-person perspective wise advising were included in the current study.

Psychological distance

The psychological distance was assessed by the Inclusion of Other in the Self (IOS) Scale58, which used the degree of overlap between two circles to reflect the psychological distance. Participants indicated on a 7-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (no overlap at all) to 7 (almost complete overlap), how far do they feel from the student they helped. The higher the score, the more overlap, the smaller the psychological distance between the participant and the hypothetical protagonist.

Procedure

The participants completed the tasks individually in a quiet laboratory room, under the guidance of a program in E-prime 2.059 run on a Windows system computer. The computer screen was about 50 cm away from their faces. They seated in an armchair at a comfortable sitting position in front of a camera. A chin rest was used to help participants maintain the head position during recording.

First, the participants underwent a 6-min EEG resting-state data acquisition with eyes closed. Subsequently, the participants completed practice tasks to ensure proper use of personal pronouns and to be accustomed to the unusual situation of talking to a camera. Then, during the formal task, the hypothetical vignette and specified personal pronoun, i.e., "YOU" (the 2nd-person perspective) or "HE" (the 3rd-person perspective), were presented on the computer screen. The participants were required to close their eyes and reflect upon the vignette thoroughly with specified personal pronoun for 6 min. Afterwards, the participants were instructed to videotape 3-min advising for the hypothetical vignette by using the specified personal pronoun to address the protagonists in the vignettes. During advising, the experimenter left the room to make sure the participants felt less constrained.

The order of person perspective was counterbalanced between the participants, i.e., for each vignette, half of the participants advised from the 2nd-person perspective, and then advised again from the 3rd-person perspective; the other half participants advised from the 3rd-, and then the 2nd-person perspective. Altogether, each participant advised from the 2nd-person perspective for twice, and the 3rd- for twice (for details, see Supplementary initial instructions). After each wise advising, the participants indicated the psychological distance between themselves and the protagonist in the vignette. Finally, the participants were debriefed, compensated, and dismissed.

Data collection and analyses

Wisdom ratings

Participants’ videotaped advice was transcribed. Two strictly trained undergraduate students rated participants’ advice on three wisdom criteria: metacognitive humility (MH) (i.e., recognize uncertainty and one’s limitation of knowledge), metacognitive flexibility (MF) (i.e., consider a multitude of life contexts and influencing factors, then use different rules to solve the problem/provide different advice), and perspective taking (PT) (i.e., reason about the life dilemma from others’ perspectives), which are well-acknowledged criteria for measuring wisdom12,14,18,60. A recent study found that the wise advising score on these three criteria could predict clinical psychologists' assessment of the potential for suicide prevention61. In the current study, the inter-rater reliabilities were acceptable for all wisdom criteria (Cronbach’s Alpha > 0.82). Therefore, the ratings were added up to get the rating on each wisdom criterion. Moreover, the inter-criteria reliabilities were acceptable for both perspectives (both Cronbach’s Alpha > 0.71). Therefore, the ratings on different criteria were added up to get the total wisdom score for each perspective (i.e., 2nd- or 3rd-).

EEG collection and processing

Throughout the whole experiment, EEG was recorded from 64 electrodes positioned according to the international 10–20 system and referenced to linked mastoids with a forehead ground at AFz. In the current study, we focused on the frontal lobe based on a range of sites across the scalp: Fpz, Fp1, Fp2, AF3, AF4, AF7, AF8, F1, F2, F3, F4, F5, F6, F7, F8, Fz (see Fig. 1). Electrode impedances were kept below 10 kΩ, and signals were amplified with a portable wireless EEG amplifier (NeuSen.W64, Neuracle, China) at a sampling rate of 1,000 Hz.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

The electrocap layout and the focused area. F stands for the focused frontal lobe.

Data of a 6-min resting-state EEG was analyzed. During the period, the participants’ heads were fixed on a chin rest with eyes closed. EEG data were preprocessed using EEGLAB62, an open-source toolbox running in the MATLAB environment. Continuous EEG data were band-pass filtered (low pass: 0.01 Hz, high pass: 30 Hz). For each participant, one EEG epoch was extracted using a window-analysis time of 350,000 ms (the first 10,000 ms were not analyzed, avoiding potential distraction at the beginning of the resting or thinking stage). Trials contaminated by eye blinks and artificial movements were corrected using an Independent Component Analysis (ICA) algorithm62 and ADJUST, a completely automatic algorithm for artifact identification and removal in EEG data which largely matches a manual one by experts (agreement on 95.2% of the data variance)63. Finally, fast Fourier transformed (FFT) was applied to the processed EEG data to calculate power for different frequency bands: theta (4–7 Hz), alpha (7.5–12.5 Hz), and beta (13–30 Hz). Since the distribution of EEG power was skewed, the log transformation (log10) was applied, which produces normal distributions, according to the normality tests in SPSS 24.0.

Data analyses

Statistical analyses were carried out in SPSS 24.0 software for Windows (version 10). Paired-sample t tests were conducted to test the difference in psychological distance and ratings on different wisdom criteria between the 2nd- and the 3rd-person perspective. Pearson correlational analyses (two-tail) and Spearman correlational analyses (two-tail) were conducted to analyze the correlations between individual resting-state neural oscillations (i.e., resting-state EEG powers) and wise advising from different person perspectives (i.e., scores on different wisdom criteria), depending on the distribution of data. Z-tests (one-tail) were conducted to test whether the correlations between the resting-state neural oscillation and wise advising were significantly stronger during the advising from the 2nd- than the 3rd-person perspective. Data were presented by means ± SD. Unless otherwise specified, significant levels were set at p < 0.05.

Results

Psychological distance

Psychological distance from the 2nd-person perspective (M = 4.29, SD = 1.48) was significantly smaller than that from the 3rd-person perspective (M = 3.05, SD = 1.36), t (50) = 5.90, p < 0.001, Cohens’d = 0.83, n = 51.

Correlations between resting-state EEG powers and wisdom scores

Table 1 shows the descriptive statistics of resting-state absolute EEG powers in the frontal lobe. For the descriptive statistics of thinking-states counterpart, see Supplementary Table S1 online.

Table 1.

Descriptive statistics of resting-state absolute EEG powers (transformed by log10) in the frontal lobe on the theta, alpha and beta band.

Resting-state absolute EEG powers
Band Theta
M (SD)
Alpha
M (SD)
Beta
M (SD)
Power 6.97 (0.15) 7.32 (0.21) 7.43 (0.15)

Shapiro–Wilk tests indicated that most wisdom scores were normally distributed, except for metacognitive humility (MH) and Metacognitive Flexibility (MF) from the 2nd-person perspective, respectively: p = 0.035, n = 51; p = 0.026, n = 51. Therefore, Spearman correlational analyses (two-tail) were conducted to analyze the correlations between resting-state absolute EEG powers and 2nd-person-perspective MH and MF, and Pearson correlational analyses (two-tail) were conducted to analyze the correlations between resting-state absolute EEG powers and other wisdom scores. Figures 2 and 3 shows the across-participant correlations between resting-state absolute EEG powers and wisdom scores from different person perspectives. When advising from the 2nd-person perspective, the wisdom scores were significantly positively correlated with both absolute theta and beta power, respectively: r = 0.433, p = 0.001, n = 51; r = 0.370, p = 0.007, n = 51; and marginally significantly with absolute alpha power, r = 0.249, p = 0.078, n = 51. However, there was no significant correlation between any resting-state absolute EEG power and wisdom score when advising from the 3rd-person perspective, all ps > 0.05, n = 51.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Across-participant correlations between wisdom scores from the 2nd-person perspective and resting-state absolute EEG powers in the frontal lobe. p < 0.1, *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Across-participant correlations between wisdom scores from the 3rd-person perspective and resting-state absolute EEG powers in the frontal lobe. p < 0.1, *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

The same Spearman correlational analyses (two-tail) and Pearson correlational analyses (two-tail) were also conducted to analyze the correlations between resting-state relative EEG powers (ratio of power in a frequency band to total power in the channel64) and wisdom scores from the 2nd- and 3rd-person perspective. When advising from the 2nd-person perspective, only the metacognitive humility scores was marginally significantly positively correlated with relative theta power, r = 0.266, p = 0.059, n = 51. When advising from the 3rd-person perspective, the metacognitive humility scores was significantly negatively correlated with relative alpha power, r = − 0.282, p = 0.045, n = 51; and the wisdom scores was marginally significantly negatively correlated with relative alpha power, r = − 0.254, p = 0.083, n = 51 (for the details, see Table 2).

Table 2.

Across-participant correlations between resting-state relative EEG powers in the frontal lobe and wisdom scores from the 2nd- and 3rd-person perspective.

The 2nd-person perspective The 3rd-person perspective
Metacognitive humility Metacognitive flexibility Perspective taking Wisdom Metacognitive humility Metacognitive flexibility Perspective taking Wisdom
Theta 0.266 − 0.017 0.111 0.158 0.223 0.048 0.141 0.174
Alpha − 0.009 − 0.063 0.099 0.001 − 0.282* − 0.194 − 0.087 − 0.254
Beta − 0.018 0.174 − 0.064 0.008 0.167 0.198 0.031 0.176

p < 0.1, *p < 0.05.

Z tests (one-tail) revealed that the correlations of resting-state absolute EEG powers with wise advising from the 2nd-person perspective were significantly stronger than these with wise advising from the 3rd-person perspective, especially on the theta and beta band. The same Z tests (one-tail) revealed that the correlations of resting-state relative alpha power with wise advising from the 2nd-person perspective was significantly stronger than this with wise advising from the 3rd-person perspective. To avoid an inflated Type I error rate, the alpha level was adjusted to 0.017 (0.05/3) (for the details, see Tables 3 and 4).

Table 3.

Z scores for the difference in the correlations between the resting-state absolute EEG powers and wisdom scores from different perspectives.

Theta Alpha Beta
Metacognitive humility 3.175** 2.666* 1.729
Metacognitive flexibility 1.132 1.491 1.281
Perspective taking 2.216* 2.084 1.897
Wisdom 2.980** 3.161** 2.613*

p < 0.033, *p < 0.017, **p < 0.003.

Table 4.

Z scores for the difference in the correlations between the resting-state relative EEG powers and wisdom scores from different perspectives.

Theta Alpha Beta
Metacognitive humility 0.334 2.081 0.105
Metacognitive flexibility − 0.500 1.019 − 0.188
Perspective taking − 0.210 1.295 − 0.660
Wisdom − 0.140 2.154* − 1.461

p < 0.033, *p < 0.017, **p < 0.003.

We also analyzed the thinking-state neural oscillations: first, paired-sample t-tests revealed no significant effect of person perspective on thinking-state absolute EEG powers (see Supplementary Table S2 online); second, correlation analyses showed that thinking-state absolute EEG powers were significantly correlated with the wisdom scores when advising from the 2nd-, but not from the 3rd-person perspective (see Supplementary Table S3 online); finally, Z tests showed that correlations between the thinking-state absolute EEG powers and wisdom scores were significantly stronger during advising from the 2nd- than the 3rd-person perspective (see Supplementary Table S4 online). We also conducted Paired-sample t-tests to explore the rest-task absolute difference (subtract resting-state absolute power from the thinking-state absolute power) in the power of the respective frequencies between the 2nd-person perspective advising and the 3rd-person perspective advising. However, there was no significant difference between the two different person perspectives (see Supplementary Table S5 online).

Besides the narrow-band, rhythmic neural oscillations, the current study also explored the relationship between wisdom scores and the broadband, arrhythmic activity, indexed by the power-law exponent (PLE)65. The PLE was calculated using MATLAB script according to the method of a previous EEG study50 (the detailed script was available on: osf.io/dhpey). Unlike the narrow-band, rhythmic neural oscillations, the PLEs (M = 1.195, SD = 0.196) were not related to wisdom scores from any person perspective (for details, see Supplementary Table S6 online).

Wisdom

Table 5 shows the descriptive statistics of wisdom scores. There was no significant difference in total wisdom scores between the 2nd- and 3rd-person perspectives, ps > 0.013, n = 51. To avoid an inflated Type I error rate, the alpha level was adjusted to 0.013 (0.05/4).

Table 5.

Descriptive statistics of wisdom scores for advising from the 2nd- and 3rd-person perspective.

Criteria The 2nd-person perspective
M (SD)
The 3rd-person perspective
M (SD)
Metacognitive humility 1.52 (0.87) 1.40 (0.78)
Metacognitive flexibility 2.06 (0.91) 1.75 (0.91)
Perspective taking 1.79 (0.99) 2.04 (0.80)
Total wisdom 5.38 (2.17) 5.19 (1.91)

Discussion

The results of the present study partially supported our hypotheses. Participants reported a significantly less psychological distance from the 2nd- than the 3rd-person perspective, supporting hypothesis A. Moreover, resting-state neural oscillations were associated with wise advising from the 2nd- (not the 3rd-) person perspective, supporting hypothesis B.

The psychological distances were significantly smaller when advising from the 2nd- than 3rd-person perspective. Construal level theory (CLT) claims that the farther removed from the direct experience, the higher the level of construal (i.e., more abstract)21. Consequently, taking a 2nd- (vs. 3rd-) person perspective is likely to induce the participants to construct the experience of the protagonist more concretely and detailed, which is consistent with Ardelt’s (2004) interpretation of wisdom: wisdom should be personal, concrete, applied, and self-implicating rather than theoretical, abstract, and self-alienated.

There were significant positive correlations between wisdom scores from the 2nd- (not the 3rd-) person perspective and resting-state absolute EEG powers, which is widely used to explore the task-related brain activity64,66. As shown in Figs. 2 and 3, the resting-state absolute EEG powers in the frontal lobe were significantly positively correlated with the wisdom scores when advising from the 2nd-person perspective, but not the 3rd-person perspective. Moreover, the correlations between resting-state absolute theta and beta powers and total wisdom score were significantly stronger when advising from the 2nd-person perspective than the 3rd-person perspective. Previous studies found that skull-thickness may affect the magnitude of EEG activity67,68. However, the skull-thickness was not measured in the current study, and thus we cannot test this possibility.

Previous studies found that spontaneous activities of the resting-state brain share common brain structures with a variety of higher cognitive functions7,34,35. In particular, resting-state brain activities are relevant to self-related networks2, 69. Recently, a growing body of EEG studies also supports the interesting "self-rest overlap"50,54. Consistently, the current study found that resting-state neural oscillations were only associated with wise advising from the 2nd-person perspective when the participants felt the advice-receiver were closer to self. Previous studies on "self-talk" find that "YOU" is related to self-related mental activities22,23 (e.g., introspection or self-reflection). Similarly, generic-"YOU" allows people to craft a deeply self-relevant generalization, reflecting one's own negative experiences and deriving broad meanings25. It seems that "YOU" is a way to reflect on self and induce self-related mental activity, which is in accord with "self-rest overlap".

The "YOU"-induced self-involved mental activities probably contributed to wise advising (e.g., exploratory self-reflection), accounting for the positive correlation between neural oscillations and wisdom scores. Exploratory self-reflection is regarded as a necessary antecedent to wisdom19,20. Some researcher empirically examined the relationship between modes of self-reflective processing and wisdom derived from difficult life experiences8. They found that wisdom was positively associated with exploratory modes of processing (meaning-making, personal growth), suggesting that the development of wisdom may be partially determined by how individuals reflect upon their own significant life experiences. In terms of neurophysiology, some speculated medial prefrontal cingulate (MPFC) might be associated with instrumental or agentic self-reflection70. Therefore, wise advising from the 2nd-person perspective is more likely to be self-involved and may even induce exploratory self-reflection. Consistently, some participants in our study reported that advising from the 2nd-person perspective was actually like dialoguing with self. Some participants reported: "I'm also confused with the same question", "It is indeed well worthwhile to ponder the meaningless of college". These results together suggested that neural activities during rest were probably related to self-reflections that contributed to advising from the 2nd-person perspective.

Limitation

In previous studies on "Solomon Paradox", participants reasoned more wisely from the 3rd- than the 1st-person perspective60. However, advising from the 1st-person perspective (advising to self) was not assessed in this study, which should be addressed in future studies.

Conclusions

Wisdom is difficult to probe using cognitive tasks typical of cognitive neuroscience research71; the complicated cognitive processing associated with wisdom, therefore, increases the difficulty of fine-grained neuroscientific analyses. Nevertheless, the current study revealed that participants felt closer to the protagonist when advising about existential dilemmas from the 2nd- than the 3rd-person perspective. Such self-involvement may be the reason for the significantly stronger associations between the resting-state neural oscillations in the frontal lobe and wise advising from the 2nd- than the 3rd-person perspective. Mental activities during rest may contribute to wise advising from the 2nd- but not the 3rd-person perspective.

Supplementary information

Acknowledgements

We wish to thank Prf. Xiaoyi Fang, Beijing Normal University for advice on the design of the experimental material, Dr Igor Grossmann, the University of Waterloo for advice on the writing of this report, and Jianfeng Zhang, Zhejiang University for the help on the data analysis! This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China under Grant 31800905; and the Natural Science Foundation of Zhejiang Province under Grant LQ18C090004; and the Talent Cultivation Plan of Hangzhou under Grant 2018RC2X08. Huang, C. was supported by the Provincial Advantage Discipline Construction Project from School of Education, Hangzhou Normal University.

Author contributions

Hu, C. S. and Huang, C. contributed to the concept and study design; Huang, C., Zhang, H., and Duan, C. collected the data; Huang, C. and Huang, J. analyzed the data; Huang, C. wrote the main manuscript text; Kim, J., Ferrari, M., and Hu, C. S. polished the language; All authors reviewed the manuscript.

Data availability

The original EEG data was accessible on: https://pan.baidu.com/s/1d67peEkUTcvzl5Q0Plq_mQ; access code: adsj. The processed EEG power data, PLE script, wisdom scores, psychological distance, and wise advising transcripts (in Chinese) were accessible on: https://osf.io/dhpey/.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher's note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

is available for this paper at 10.1038/s41598-020-69507-9.

References

  • 1.Thatcher RW, North D, Biver C. EEG and intelligence: relations between EEG coherence, EEG phase delay and power. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2005;116:2129–2141. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2005.04.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Northoff G, Bermpohl F. Cortical midline structures and the self. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2004;8:102–107. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Northoff G, et al. Self-referential processing in our brain—a meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self. NeuroImage. 2006;31:440–457. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.D’argembeau A, et al. Self-referential reflective activity and its relationship with rest: a PET study. Neuroimage. 2005;25:616–624. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.11.048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Stawarczyk D. Phenomenological properties of mind-wandering and daydreaming: a historical overview and functional. In: Fox KCR, Christoff K, editors. The Oxford Handbook of Spontaneous Thought: Mind-Wandering, Creativity, and Dreaming. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2018. pp. 193–214. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Zabelina DL, Andrews-Hanna JR. Dynamic network interactions supporting internally-oriented cognition. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2016;40:86–93. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2016.06.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Legrand D, Ruby P. What is self-specific? Theoretical investigation and critical review of neuroimaging results. Psychol. Rev. 2009;116:252–282. doi: 10.1037/a0014172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Weststrate NM, Glück J. Wiser but not sadder, blissful but not ignorant: exploring the co-development of wisdom and well-being over time. In: Robinson MD, Eid M, editors. The Happy Mind: Cognitive Contributions to Well-Being. Cham: Springer International Publishing; 2017. pp. 459–480. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Glück J, Bluck S. The MORE life experience model: a theory of the development of personal wisdom. In: Ferrari M, Weststrate N, editors. The Scientific Study of Personal Wisdom. Dordrecht: Springer, Netherlands; 2013. pp. 75–97. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Yang, S. The meaning and course of wisdom. Indig. Psychol. Res. 185–238 (2008).
  • 11.Baltes PB, Smith J. The fascination of wisdom: its nature, ontogeny, and function. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2008;3:56–64. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2008.00062.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Baltes PB, Staudinger UM. Wisdom: a metaheuristic (pragmatic) to orchestrate mind and virtue toward excellence. Am. Psychol. 2000;55:122–136. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.55.1.122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Staudinger UM, Baltes PB. Interactive minds: a facilitative setting for wisdom-related performance? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1996;71:746–762. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Glück J. Measuring wisdom: existing approaches, continuing challenges, and new developments. J. Gerontol. Ser. B. 2017;73:1393–1403. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbx140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Bluck S, Glück J. From the inside out: people’s implicit theories of wisdom. In: Sternberg RJ, Jordan J, editors. A Handbook of Wisdom: Psychological Perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2005. pp. 84–109. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Hu CS, et al. Sadder but wiser: emotional reactions and wisdom in a simulated suicide intervention. Int. J. Psychol. 2018;54:791–799. doi: 10.1002/ijop.12536. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Weststrate NM, Glück J. Hard-earned wisdom: exploratory processing of difficult life experience is positively associated with wisdom. Dev. Psychol. 2017;53:800–814. doi: 10.1037/dev0000286. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Grossmann I, et al. The science of wisdom in a polarized world: knowns and unknowns. Psychol. Inq. 2020;31:103–133. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Ardelt M. Wisdom as expert knowledge system: a critical review of a contemporary operationalization of an ancient concept. Hum. Dev. 2004;47:257–285. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Weststrate NM, Ferrari M, Fournier MA, McLean KC. “It was the best worst day of my life”: narrative content, structure, and process in wisdom-fostering life event memories. J. Gerontol. Ser. B. 2018;73:1359–1373. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gby005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Trope Y, Liberman N. Construal-level theory of psychological distance. Psychol. Rev. 2010;117:440–463. doi: 10.1037/a0018963. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Zell E, Warriner AB, Albarracín D. Splitting of the mind: when the you I talk to is me and needs commands. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 2012;3:549–555. doi: 10.1177/1948550611430164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Kross E, et al. Self-talk as a regulatory mechanism: how you do it matters. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2014;106:304–324. doi: 10.1037/a0035173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Orvell A, Ayduk Ö, Moser JS, Gelman SA, Kross E. Linguistic shifts: a relatively effortless route to emotion regulation? Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2019;28:567–573. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Orvell A, Kross E, Gelman SA. How, “you” makes meaning. Science. 2017;355:1299–1302. doi: 10.1126/science.aaj2014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Ferrari M, Weststrate NM, Petro A. Stories of wisdom to live by: developing wisdom in a narrative mode. In: Ferrari M, Weststrate NM, editors. The Scientific Study of Personal Wisdom: From Contemplative Traditions to Neuroscience. New York: Springer; 2012. pp. 137–164. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Pauen M. The second-person perspective. Inquiry. 2012;55:33–49. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Schilbach L. Towards a second-person neuropsychiatry. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2016;371:20150081. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0081. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Steuber K. Social cognition and the allure of the second-person perspective: in defense of empathy and simulation. In: Seemann A, editor. Joint Attention. Cambridge: The MIT Press; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.De Quincey C. Intersubjectivity: exploring consciousness from the second-person perspective. J. Transpers. Psychol. 2000;32:135–156. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Galbusera L, Fellin L. The intersubjective endeavor of psychopathology research: methodological reflections on a second-person perspective approach. Front. Psychol. 2014;5:1150. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01150. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Staudinger UM. Wisdom and the social-interactive foundation of the mind. In: Baltes PB, Staudinger UM, editors. Interactive Minds: Life-Span Perspectives on the Social Foundation of Cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1996. pp. 276–315. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Borawski D. Supporting the development of wisdom: the dialogical perspective. Rocz. Psychol. 2017;20:563–578. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Christoff K, Ream JM, Gabrieli JDE. Neural basis of spontaneous thought processes. Cortex. 2004;40:623–630. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70158-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Cole MW, Ito T, Bassett DS, Schultz DH. Activity flow over resting-state networks shapes cognitive task activations. Nat. Neurosci. 2016;19:1718–1726. doi: 10.1038/nn.4406. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Christoff K, et al. Rostrolateral prefrontal cortex involvement in relational integration during reasoning. NeuroImage. 2001;14:1136–1149. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0922. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Christoff K, Gabrieli JDE. The frontopolar cortex and human cognition: evidence for a rostrocaudal hierarchical organization within the human prefrontal cortex. Psychobiology. 2000;28:168–186. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Christoff K, Ream JM, Geddes LPT, Gabrieli JDE. Evaluating self-generated information: anterior prefrontal contributions to human cognition. Behav. Neurosci. 2003;117:1161–1168. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.117.6.1161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Marosi E, et al. Broad band spectral EEG parameters correlated with different IQ measurements. Int. J. Neurosci. 1999;97:17–27. doi: 10.3109/00207459908994300. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Schmid R. Correlation between spectral EEG parameters and intelligence test variables in school-age children. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2002;113:1647–1656. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(02)00212-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Kounios J, et al. The origins of insight in resting-state brain activity. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46:281–291. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.07.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Nakao T, Bai Y, Nashiwa H, Northoff G. Resting-state EEG power predicts conflict-related brain activity in internally guided but not in externally guided decision-making. NeuroImage. 2013;66:9–21. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.10.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Nakao T, et al. From neuronal to psychological noise—long-range temporal correlations in EEG intrinsic activity reduce noise in internally-guided decision making. NeuroImage. 2019;201:116015. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Qin P, Northoff G. How is our self related to midline regions and the default-mode network? NeuroImage. 2011;57:1221–1233. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Knyazev GG. EEG correlates of self-referential processing. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013;7:264. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00264. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Lipsman N, et al. Neural overlap between resting state and self-relevant activity in human subcallosal cingulate cortex—single unit recording in an intracranial study. Cortex. 2014;60:139–144. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.09.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.van der Meer L, Costafreda S, Aleman A, David AS. Self-reflection and the brain: a theoretical review and meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies with implications for schizophrenia. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2010;34:935–946. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Huang Z, Obara N, Davis HH, IV, Pokorny J, Northoff G. The temporal structure of resting-state brain activity in the medial prefrontal cortex predicts self-consciousness. Neuropsychologia. 2016;82:161–170. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.01.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Northoff G. Is the self a higher-order or fundamental function of the brain? The “basis model of self-specificity” and its encoding by the brain’s spontaneous activity. Cogn. Neurosci. 2016;7:203–222. doi: 10.1080/17588928.2015.1111868. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Wolff A, et al. The temporal signature of self: temporal measures of resting-state EEG predict self-consciousness. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2019;40:789–803. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24412. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Goldberg II, Harel M, Malach R. When the brain loses its self: prefrontal inactivation during sensorimotor processing. Neuron. 2006;50:329–339. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.03.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Mantini D, Perrucci MG, Del Gratta C, Romani GL, Corbetta M. Electrophysiological signatures of resting state networks in the human brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2007;104:13170–13175. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0700668104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Ingvar DH. “Hyperfrontal” distribution of the cerebral grey matter flow in resting wakefulness; on the functional anatomy of the conscious state. Acta Neurol. Scand. 2009;60:12–25. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0404.1979.tb02947.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Bai Y, et al. Resting state glutamate predicts elevated pre-stimulus alpha during self-relatedness: a combined EEG-MRS study on “rest-self overlap”. Soc. Neurosci. 2016;11:249–263. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2015.1072582. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Knyazev GG, Savostyanov AN, Volf NV, Liou M, Bocharov AV. EEG correlates of spontaneous self-referential thoughts: a cross-cultural study. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2012;86:173–181. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Nakao T, Ohira H, Northoff G. Distinction between externally vs. internally guided decision-making: operational differences, meta-analytical comparisons and their theoretical implications. Front. Neurosci. 2012;6:31. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2012.00031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Hu CS, Ferrari M, Wang Q, Woodruff E. Thin-slice measurement of wisdom. Front. Psychol. 2017;8:1378. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01378. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Aron A, Aron EN, Smollan D. Inclusion of other in the self scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1992;63:596–612. [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Schneider W, Eschman A, Zuccolotto A. E-Prime: User’s Guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Incorporated; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Grossmann I, Kross E. Exploring Solomon’s paradox: self-distancing eliminates the self-other asymmetry in wise reasoning about close relationships in younger and older adults. Psychol. Sci. 2014;25:1571–1580. doi: 10.1177/0956797614535400. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Hu, C. et al. Wiser reasoning and less disgust have the potential to better achieve suicide prevention. Crisis J. Crisis Interv. Suicide Prev.(in press). [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 62.Delorme A, Makeig S. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2004;134:9–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Mognon A, Jovicich J, Bruzzone L, Buiatti M. ADJUST: an automatic EEG artifact detector based on the joint use of spatial and temporal features. Psychophysiology. 2011;48:229–240. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01061.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Oken BS, Chiappa KH. Short-term variability in EEG frequency analysis. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1988;69:191–198. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(88)90128-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.He BJ. Scale-free brain activity: past, present, and future. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2014;18:480–487. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.04.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Corsi-Cabrera M, Arce C, Ramos J, Lorenzo I, Guevara MA. Time course of reaction time and eeg while performing a vigilance task during total sleep deprivation. Sleep. 1996;19:563–569. doi: 10.1093/sleep/19.7.563. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Cuffin BN. Effects of local variations in skull and scalp thickness on EEG’s and MEG’s. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 1993;40:42–48. doi: 10.1109/10.204770. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Leissner P, Lindholm L-E, Petersén I. Alpha amplitude dependence on skull thickness as measured by ultrasound technique. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1970;29:392–399. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(70)90047-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Wicker B, Ruby P, Royet J-P, Fonlupt P. A relation between rest and the self in the brain? Brain Res. Rev. 2003;43:224–230. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2003.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Johnson MK, et al. Dissociating medial frontal and posterior cingulate activity during self-reflection. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2006;1:56–64. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsl004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Thomas ML, et al. Individual differences in level of wisdom are associated with brain activation during a moral decision-making task. Brain Behav. 2019;9:e01302. doi: 10.1002/brb3.1302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Data Availability Statement

The original EEG data was accessible on: https://pan.baidu.com/s/1d67peEkUTcvzl5Q0Plq_mQ; access code: adsj. The processed EEG power data, PLE script, wisdom scores, psychological distance, and wise advising transcripts (in Chinese) were accessible on: https://osf.io/dhpey/.


Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES