Abstract
Background:
Older patients with Huntington’s disease (HD) are often thought to have a slower progressing disease course with less behavioral symptoms than younger patients. However, phenotypic differences based on age of onset have not been well characterized in a large HD population. This study will determine the difference in manifestations and disease progression between patients with young, typical, and late onset adult HD at different stages of disease.
Methods:
Data obtained from Enroll-HD. Adults with manifest HD were included. Age groups were defined as young onset (YO: 20-29 years), typical onset (TO: 30–59 years), and late onset (LO: 60+ years). Subjects were categorized by TFC score, from Stage I (least severe) to Stage V (most severe). Motor, cognitive, and behavioral symptoms were analyzed. Descriptive statistics and Bonferroni p-value correction for pairwise comparison were calculated.
Results:
7,311 manifest HD participants were included (612 YO, 5,776 TO, and 923 LO). The average decline in TFC score from baseline to second visit (1.5–2.5 years) was significantly faster for YO (–1.75 points) compared to TO (–1.23 points, p = 0.0105) or LO (–0.97 points, p = 0.0017). Motor deficits were worse for LO participants at early stages of HD, and worse for YO participants at advanced stages. YO and TO participants had greater burden of behavioral symptoms at early stages of disease compared to LO.
Discussion:
YO is predictive of a faster functional decline for adults with HD when compared to those with TO and LO. Motor and behavioral manifestations differ based on age of onset.
Highlights:
This study compares HD manifestations while controlling for disease severity, detailing robust phenotypic differences by age of onset alone. These findings have implications for the clinical management of HD symptoms and have the possibility to improve prognostic and treatment precision.
Keywords: Huntington Disease (HD), Enroll-HD, phenotype, UHDRS, TFC
Introduction
Huntington’s Disease (HD) is an inherited neurodegenerative disorder that is characterized by progressive motor, cognitive, and behavioral decline. It is inherited in an autosomal dominant pattern, with the number of CAG repeats in the HTT gene being the strongest determinant of age of onset [1]. Average age of onset is about 40 years old, with a range from childhood to the eighth decade [2,3]. It has been suggested that older onset patients are more likely to present with motor manifestations, and younger patients with psychiatric/behavioral manifestations [4]. However, phenotypic differences by age of onset have not been well-characterized in the HD literature, likely due to sample size limitations of most studies. Furthermore, whether phenotypic differences persist or vary by disease stage has not been previously described. There remains a lack of clarity on the relationship between age of onset and HD progression. In a 1995 study, Feigin et al. reported no significant connection between rate of functional decline and age of onset [5]. A more recent publication described a significant association between younger age of onset and faster decline in UHDRS motor score and Independence Scale, but did not find an association with Total Functional Capacity (TFC) [6]. Using the large database of HD patients from Enroll-HD, we characterized HD phenotype and functional decline by age group across the spectrum of disease severity. Knowledge of the phenotypic profile and rate of progression by age of onset will inform standard of care recommendations and aim to improve the quality of care for HD patients.
Methods
This study is a cross-sectional analysis of prospectively collected data from manifest HD subjects in the Enroll-HD database. Enroll-HD began in 2012 as part of an observational, multi-national, multi-center study and contains longitudinal clinical information on over 15,000 participants as of December 2018. This dataset includes 3,802 participants from the European REGISTRY study that preceded Enroll-HD [7]. Study participants are recruited from specialty clinics and enrolled from 125 sites located in 13 countries across four continents. Each study site is evaluated by an IRB or equivalent ethics committee. Enroll-HD collects deidentified data from participants at annual study visits and utilizes a risk-based monitoring strategy to maintain data quality [8]. The data for this study was from the dataset cutoff made available in December 2018.
Participants with manifest/motor-manifest HD at enrollment were included. Participants younger than 20 years of age at symptom onset were excluded to remove those with juvenile-onset Huntington’s disease, which differs significantly in phenotype from adult-onset HD and has already been well-characterized in the literature as a distinct clinical presentation [9,10]. Age groups were defined as follows: young onset (20–29 years), typical onset (30–59 years), and late onset (60+ years). Typical and late onset age group cutoffs have been used and validated by previous studies [11,12,13,14,15]. Age of onset was determined by Enroll-HD rater’s estimate of symptom onset. Subjects in each group were categorized into TFC total score bins, from Stage I (least severe) to Stage V (most severe). TFC Total Score 11–13 is characterized as Stage I, 7–10 is Stage II, 3–6 is Stage III, 1–2 is Stage IV, and 0 is Stage V. For the purposes of analysis, TFC scores were grouped into 3 previously validated categories: early (stage I and II), moderate (stage III), and advanced (stages IV and V) [16,17]. These TFC score stages are utilized in clinical practice and in contemporary research studies [13,18,19].
Demographic data and motor, cognitive, and behavioral measures were analyzed. Motor variables included the Unified Huntington’s Disease Rating Scale (UHDRS) motor score and Timed “Up and Go” test. Cognitive variables included Symbol Digit Modality Test, Verbal Fluency Test (Category), Verbal Fluency Test (Letter), Stroop Color Naming Test, Stroop Word Reading Test, and Stroop Interference Test. For the above measures, percentage correct was calculated by dividing total correct answers by total answers. Percentage correct was used for analyses. Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) score was also included as a measure of cognitive function.
Behavioral measures included quantification of drug and alcohol abuse; Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale – Snaith Irritability Scale (HADS-SIS) anxiety, depression, irritability, outward irritability, and inward irritability subscores, total number of suicide attempts (from Columbia-Suicide Rating Scale baseline and follow-up); and Problem Behaviours Assessment – Short (PBA-s) Depression, Irritability aggression, Psychosis, Apathy, Executive function, Depressed mood frequency, Suicidal ideation frequency, Anxiety frequency, Irritability frequency, Angry or aggressive behavior frequency, Lack of initiative (apathy) frequency, Perseverative thinking or behavior frequency, Obsessive-Compulsive Behaviors frequency, Delusions frequency, Hallucinations frequency, Disoriented Behavior frequency. For all PBA-s frequency variables, responses were converted to and analyzed as binary variables: not present (coded as 0-absent) vs. present (includes 1-slight, questionable, 2-mild, 3-moderate, and 4-severe).
Data Analysis
Descriptive statistics were calculated for all variables of interest. Categorical variables were summarized with counts and percentages, and continuous variables with means and standard deviations or median and interquartile range, as appropriate. The drug and alcohol abuse variables were binary variables (yes/no). TFC score at baseline was utilized to sort participants into early, moderate, and advanced disease stage. Chi-square test, Fisher’s exact test, and Kruskal-Wallis test, as appropriate, were used to determine p-values. P-values less than 0.05 were considered significant for overall comparison. As we aimed to identify any factors potentially associated with age of onset, we did not consider p-value correction on potential factors. However, Bonferroni p-value correction was used to do pairwise comparison for post-hoc analysis. Bonferroni corrected p-values less than 0.01667 were considered significant when we compared 3 combinations of age of onset groups. In this context, we viewed control over type II error more important than control over type I error. In addition, the analyses were deemed exploratory overall and the primary focus was not to generate a precise, predictive model.
Sensitivity analysis was conducted to determine the average change in TFC score between age of onset groups, excluding participants whose first visit was outside 6 months to 1.5 years after baseline visit, participants whose second visit was outside 1.5 year to 2.5 years after baseline, and participants whose third visit was outside 2.5 years to 3.5 years after baseline. Follow-up visits for Enroll-HD are to be conducted annually, and these exclusions remove variation from that timeline. Additionally, only participants who had TFC scores for their baseline, first visit, second visit, and third visit were included. ANOVA was used to compare mean TFC score change from baseline between age of onset groups. The same parameters were used for a sensitivity analysis of average change in UHDRS Functional Assessment Independence Scale.
Results
7,311 manifest HD subjects were included in the analysis of which 612 were young onset, 5,776 typical onset, and 923 late onset. There were 15,301 total participants in the Enroll-HD database, of which 8,043 had manifest HD. 176 participants were excluded due to age <20 and 556 were excluded due to missing age of onset value. Participants were 51.5% female, 94.3% white/Caucasian, and average CAG repeats in the HTT gene was 43.8. The average age of clinical HD diagnosis in our adult study population was 49.2 years (Table 1).
Table 1.
Demographic Characteristics.
Age of Onset Groups | ALL | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Young onset (20–29 years) |
Typical onset (30–59 years) |
Late onset (60+ years) |
||||||
N | % | N | % | N | % | N | % | |
TFC stage | ||||||||
Stage I (11–13) | 191 | 31.31 | 1864 | 32.30 | 278 | 30.18 | 2333 | 31.95 |
Stage II (7–10) | 198 | 32.46 | 2025 | 35.09 | 347 | 37.68 | 2570 | 35.20 |
Stage III (3–6) | 139 | 22.79 | 1265 | 21.92 | 215 | 23.34 | 1619 | 22.17 |
Stage IV (1–2) | 54 | 8.85 | 445 | 7.71 | 67 | 7.27 | 566 | 7.75 |
Stage V (0) | 28 | 4.59 | 172 | 2.98 | 14 | 1.52 | 214 | 2.93 |
TFC stage | ||||||||
Early (Stage I & II) | 389 | 63.77 | 3889 | 67.39 | 625 | 67.86 | 4903 | 67.15 |
Moderate (Stage III) | 139 | 22.79 | 1265 | 21.92 | 215 | 23.34 | 1619 | 22.17 |
Advanced (Stage IV & V) | 82 | 13.44 | 617 | 10.69 | 81 | 8.79 | 780 | 10.68 |
Sex | ||||||||
Female | 319 | 52.12 | 2998 | 51.90 | 445 | 48.21 | 3762 | 51.46 |
Male | 293 | 47.88 | 2778 | 48.10 | 478 | 51.79 | 3549 | 48.54 |
Race | ||||||||
Other | 41 | 6.70 | 335 | 5.80 | 39 | 4.23 | 415 | 5.68 |
White/Caucasian | 571 | 93.30 | 5441 | 94.20 | 884 | 95.77 | 6896 | 94.32 |
Has Mother Affected | 279 | 46.66 | 2660 | 47.75 | 365 | 45.23 | 3304 | 47.36 |
Has Father Affected | 308 | 51.85 | 2602 | 47.12 | 278 | 34.71 | 3188 | 46.09 |
Has Family History | 526 | 85.95 | 4866 | 84.25 | 788 | 85.37 | 6180 | 84.53 |
Marital Status | ||||||||
Married/Partnership | 199 | 32.52 | 3782 | 65.56 | 691 | 74.95 | 4672 | 63.97 |
Single/Divorced/Widowed/Legally Separated | 413 | 67.48 | 1987 | 34.44 | 231 | 25.05 | 2631 | 36.03 |
ISCED Education Level | ||||||||
Less than or equal to 12th grade | 374 | 61.41 | 3217 | 55.96 | 541 | 58.87 | 4132 | 56.78 |
Higher than 12th grade | 235 | 38.59 | 2532 | 44.04 | 378 | 41.13 | 3145 | 43.22 |
Employment Status | ||||||||
Employed | 129 | 21.11 | 1302 | 22.59 | 79 | 8.60 | 1510 | 20.70 |
Not Employed | 482 | 78.89 | 4461 | 77.41 | 840 | 91.40 | 5783 | 79.30 |
Rater’s Judgement of Initial Major Symptom | ||||||||
Motor | 269 | 44.39 | 2966 | 51.51 | 631 | 68.36 | 3866 | 53.05 |
Cognitive | 46 | 7.59 | 489 | 8.49 | 42 | 4.55 | 577 | 7.92 |
Psychiatric | 165 | 27.23 | 1197 | 20.79 | 104 | 11.27 | 1466 | 20.12 |
Oculomotor/Other/Mixed | 126 | 20.79 | 1106 | 19.21 | 146 | 15.82 | 1378 | 18.91 |
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | Mean | SD | Mean | SD | |
BMI | 24.32 | 5.43 | 25.03 | 5.03 | 25.02 | 4.31 | 24.97 | 4.99 |
Age of clinical HD diagnosis | 29.83 | 6.25 | 48.27 | 8.72 | 67.92 | 5.57 | 49.22 | 11.99 |
Larger research CAG allele determined from DNA | 49.81 | 4.67 | 43.59 | 2.49 | 40.75 | 1.19 | 43.75 | 3.34 |
Missing values were encountered in 9 (0.12%) for TFC score, 335 (4.58%) for mother affected, 394 (5.39%) for father affected, 8 (0.11%) for marital status, 34 (0.47%) for education level, 18 (0.25%) for employment status, 24 (0.33%) for rater’s judgement of initial major symptom, 235 (3.21%) in BMI, and 140 (1.91%) in age of clinical HD diagnosis.
Motor Variables
At early (TFC I-II) and advanced (TFC IV-V) stages of disease, motor function varied based on age of onset. At early stages of disease, the late age of onset group had worse motor function compared to the young and typical age of onset groups, with significantly worse UHDRS Motor scores in late onset (median [Q1–Q3] = 30.00 [20.00–39.00]) compared to young onset (25.00 [15.00–38.00], p-value = 0.00007) and typical onset participants (27.00 [18.00–38.00], p = 0.0003).
At advanced stages of disease, all the age groups had significantly different motor function. The young onset group had worse UHDRS motor scores (83.00 [70.00–91.50]) compared to the typical onset (75.00 [63.00–87.00], p = 0.003) and late onset participants (65.00 [53.00–77.00], p < 0.001). The typical onset group also had significantly worse motor scores compared to the late onset group (p < 0.001) (Table 2).
Table 2.
Motor and Cognitive Variables by Age of Onset Groups and TFC Stages.
TFC Score Category | Young | Typical | Late | P-value | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N | Median (Q1–Q3) or N (%) | N | Median (Q1–Q3) or N (%) | N | Median (Q1–Q3) or N (%) | |||
Motor Symptoms | ||||||||
UHDRS Motor score (TMS) | Early | 387 | 25.00 (15.00, 38.00) | 3871 | 27.00 (18.00, 38.00) | 619 | 30.00 (20.00, 39.00) | * <0.01 |
Moderate | 139 | 56.00 (39.00, 67.00) | 1248 | 51.00 (38.50, 63.00) | 214 | 51.00 (39.00, 60.00) | 0.13 | |
Advanced | 80 | 83.00 (70.00, 91.50) | 609 | 75.00 (63.00, 87.00) | 79 | 65.00 (53.00, 77.00) | * <0.01 | |
Timed “Up and Go” Total time | Early | 190 | 8.75 (7.00, 11.00) | 1661 | 9.00 (7.40, 11.00) | 252 | 10.00 (8.80, 13.00) | * <0.01 |
Moderate | 45 | 11.00 (9.00, 15.00) | 462 | 12.00 (9.50, 16.00) | 63 | 14.00 (11.00, 20.00) | * 0.02 | |
Advanced | 10 | 14.00 (12.00, 21.00) | 72 | 15.50 (10.00, 23.00) | 12 | 17.00 (15.00, 26.50) | 0.47 | |
Cognitive Symptoms | ||||||||
Symbol Digit Modality Test Total correct % | Early | 384 | 100.00 (93.75, 100.00) | 3819 | 100.00 (95.00, 100.00) | 609 | 100.00 (93.33, 100.00) | * 0.02 |
Moderate | 118 | 100.00 (86.67, 100.00) | 1104 | 96.23 (86.36, 100.00) | 187 | 95.00 (75.00, 100.00) | 0.13 | |
Advanced | 52 | 0.00 (0.00, 87.78) | 398 | 0.00 (0.00, 94.74) | 45 | 50.00 (0.00, 100.00) | 0.16 | |
Verbal Fluency Test Total correct % Category | Early | 384 | 100.00 (93.22, 100.00) | 3847 | 100.00 (90.91, 100.00) | 622 | 100.00 (90.91, 100.00) | * 0.01 |
Moderate | 134 | 100.00 (87.50, 100.00) | 1227 | 100.00 (85.71, 100.00) | 210 | 100.00 (83.33, 100.00) | 0.41 | |
Advanced | 65 | 85.71 (0.00, 100.00) | 491 | 85.71 (40.00, 100.00) | 67 | 100.00 (75.00, 100.00) | * 0.03 | |
Verbal Fluency Test Total correct % Letter | Early | 300 | 95.45 (90.12, 100.00) | 3037 | 95.00 (88.89, 100.00) | 456 | 94.12 (87.50, 100.00) | 0.18 |
Moderate | 92 | 94.28 (85.96, 100.00) | 854 | 92.98 (82.61, 100.00) | 155 | 90.00 (76.00, 100.00) | * <0.01 | |
Advanced | 41 | 80.95 (0.00, 100.00) | 277 | 76.47 (50.00, 100.00) | 41 | 85.71 (61.54, 100.00) | 0.44 | |
Stroop Color Naming Test Total correct % | Early | 381 | 100.00 (100.00, 100.00) | 3833 | 100.00 (100.00, 100.00) | 614 | 100.00 (100.00, 100.00) | 0.84 |
Moderate | 133 | 100.00 (98.63, 100.00) | 1206 | 100.00 (98.08, 100.00) | 202 | 100.00 (97.50, 100.00) | 0.78 | |
Advanced | 64 | 98.49 (0.00, 100.00) | 468 | 97.26 (0.00, 100.00) | 60 | 98.53 (64.38, 100.00) | 0.82 | |
Stroop Word Reading test Total correct % | Early | 381 | 100.00 (100.00, 100.00) | 3831 | 100.00 (100.00, 100.00) | 614 | 100.00 (100.00, 100.00) | 0.18 |
Moderate | 129 | 100.00 (100.00, 100.00) | 1187 | 100.00 (100.00, 100.00) | 200 | 100.00 (100.00, 100.00) | 0.65 | |
Advanced | 63 | 96.67 (0.00, 100.00) | 457 | 100.00 (0.00, 100.00) | 60 | 100.00 (0.00, 100.00) | 0.19 | |
Stroop Interference Test Total correct % | Early | 356 | 100.00 (96.08, 100.00) | 3511 | 100.00 (96.15, 100.00) | 548 | 100.00 (95.00, 100.00) | 0.24 |
Moderate | 106 | 100.00 (88.89, 100.00) | 956 | 100.00 (87.50, 100.00) | 155 | 94.74 (76.92, 100.00) | * 0.02 | |
Advanced | 38 | 95.00 (36.84, 100.00) | 281 | 88.24 (16.67, 100.00) | 33 | 84.62 (30.00, 100.00) | 0.53 | |
Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) score | Early | 270 | 26.00 (24.00, 29.00) | 2562 | 27.00 (25.00, 29.00) | 393 | 27.00 (25.00, 28.00) | 0.13 |
Moderate | 80 | 24.00 (20.00, 26.50) | 725 | 23.00 (20.00, 26.00) | 128 | 23.00 (19.00, 25.00) | 0.08 | |
Advanced | 34 | 17.00 (11.00, 22.00) | 252 | 17.00 (13.00, 21.00) | 37 | 18.00 (15.00, 22.00) | 0.68 |
* Significant at level p < 0.05.
Cognitive Variables
The cognitive variables analyzed yielded no clinically significant trends when compared between groups. At early stages of HD, some intergroup differences were seen for Symbol Digit Modality Test (p = 0.02) and Verbal Fluency Test Category (p = 0.01). For instance, the young age of onset group scored slightly better (median [Q1–Q3] = 100.00 [93.22–100.00]) compared to typical (100.00 [90.91–100.00], p = 0.003) and late onset group (100.00 [90.91–100.00], p = 0.013) on the Verbal Fluency Test Category. At moderate stages of disease, late onset participants had significantly worse scores compared to typical onset participants compared on the Verbal Fluency Test Letter (90.00 [76.00–100.00] vs. 92.98 [82.61–100.00], p = 0.003) and Stroop Interference Test (94.74 [76.92–100.00] vs. 100.00 [87.50, 100.00], p = 0.011) (Table 2).
Behavioral Variables
At early stages of disease, the young onset group tended to have significantly worse behavioral symptoms, including drug and alcohol abuse, anxiety, depression, irritability, aggression, apathy, lack of initiative, obsessive-compulsive behaviors, and delusions when compared to the late onset group (Table 3). At moderate stages of disease, the late onset group scored significantly worse on HADS-SIS depression and HADS-SIS inward irritability when compared to the young onset group. Young onset participants were more likely to abuse drugs compared to both the typical and late onset groups at early, moderate, and advanced stages of disease (all p < 0.01) (Appendix A). At advanced stages of disease, young onset participants were more likely to have delusions (29.27% vs. 16.02%, p < 0.009) and hallucinations (20.73% vs. 7.41%, p = 0.015) when compared to the late onset group (Table 3).
Table 3.
Behavioral Variables by Age of Onset Groups and TFC Stages.
TFC Score Category | Young | Typical | Late | P-value | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N | Median (Q1–Q3) or N (%) | N | Median (Q1–Q3) or N (%) | N | Median (Q1–Q3) or N (%) | |||
Behavioral Symptoms | ||||||||
Has the participant ever abused drugs? Yes |
Early | 389 | 77 (19.79) | 3883 | 385 (9.92) | 622 | 23 (3.70) | * <0.01 |
Moderate | 139 | 27 (19.42) | 1262 | 84 (6.66) | 214 | 4 (1.87) | * <0.01 | |
Advanced | 80 | 10 (12.50) | 613 | 23 (3.75) | 81 | 0 (0.00) | * <0.01 | |
Has the participant had alcohol problems in the past? Yes |
Early | 389 | 50 (12.85) | 3881 | 358 (9.22) | 622 | 28 (4.50) | * <0.01 |
Moderate | 139 | 17 (12.23) | 1261 | 146 (11.58) | 214 | 15 (7.01) | 0.13 | |
Advanced | 81 | 8 (9.88) | 612 | 64 (10.46) | 81 | 5 (6.17) | 0.55 | |
HADS-SIS anxiety subscore | Early | 264 | 6.00 (3.50, 9.00) | 2417 | 6.00 (3.00, 9.00) | 365 | 4.00 (2.00, 7.00) | * <0.01 |
Moderate | 62 | 4.00 (2.00, 8.00) | 604 | 5.00 (3.00, 9.00) | 100 | 5.00 (3.00, 8.00) | 0.28 | |
Advanced | 24 | 4.00 (2.50, 8.50) | 172 | 5.00 (3.00, 9.00) | 25 | 4.00 (2.00, 6.00) | 0.40 | |
HADS-SIS depression subscore | Early | 264 | 5.00 (3.00, 8.00) | 2416 | 5.00 (3.00, 8.00) | 365 | 4.00 (2.00, 7.00) | * <0.01 |
Moderate | 62 | 5.00 (3.00, 8.00) | 602 | 7.00 (4.00, 10.00) | 99 | 8.00 (4.00, 11.00) | * <0.01 | |
Advanced | 24 | 7.00 (3.00, 11.00) | 176 | 8.00 (5.00, 12.00) | 25 | 7.00 (6.00, 9.00) | 0.39 | |
HADS-SIS irritability subscore | Early | 263 | 6.00 (3.00, 10.00) | 2413 | 6.00 (2.00, 9.00) | 365 | 5.00 (2.00, 7.00) | * <0.01 |
Moderate | 62 | 5.00 (2.00, 8.00) | 601 | 5.00 (2.00, 9.00) | 100 | 5.00 (3.00, 8.00) | 0.52 | |
Advanced | 25 | 4.00 (2.00, 8.00) | 174 | 5.00 (2.00, 8.00) | 25 | 4.00 (2.00, 7.00) | 0.88 | |
HADS-SIS outward irritability subscore | Early | 263 | 4.00 (2.00, 7.00) | 2426 | 3.00 (1.00, 6.00) | 368 | 3.00 (1.00, 5.00) | * <0.01 |
Moderate | 62 | 3.00 (1.00, 6.00) | 604 | 3.00 (1.00, 6.00) | 101 | 3.00 (1.00, 5.00) | 0.23 | |
Advanced | 25 | 3.00 (1.00, 5.00) | 175 | 3.00 (1.00, 6.00) | 25 | 2.00 (1.00, 5.00) | 0.65 | |
HADS-SIS inward irritability subscore | Early | 264 | 2.00 (0.00, 4.00) | 2421 | 2.00 (0.00, 4.00) | 365 | 2.00 (0.00, 3.00) | * <0.01 |
Moderate | 62 | 1.00 (0.00, 3.00) | 603 | 2.00 (0.00, 4.00) | 100 | 2.50 (1.00, 4.00) | * 0.01 | |
Advanced | 25 | 1.00 (0.00, 3.00) | 174 | 1.00 (0.00, 3.00) | 25 | 1.00 (0.00, 3.00) | 0.86 | |
Total # of suicide attempts (C-SSRS BL and FUP) | Early | 27 | 1.00 (1.00, 2.00) | 142 | 1.00 (1.00, 2.00) | 13 | 1.00 (1.00, 1.00) | 0.55 |
Moderate | 19 | 1.00 (1.00, 2.00) | 75 | 1.00 (1.00, 2.00) | 7 | 1.00 (1.00, 2.00) | 0.99 | |
Advanced | 5 | 2.00 (1.00, 2.00) | 32 | 1.00 (1.00, 2.50) | 3 | 1.00 (1.00, 3.00) | 0.75 | |
PBA-s Depression | Early | 388 | 4.00 (0.00, 9.50) | 3883 | 4.00 (0.00, 8.00) | 624 | 2.00 (0.00, 6.00) | * <0.01 |
Moderate | 138 | 2.50 (0.00, 7.00) | 1257 | 3.00 (0.00, 8.00) | 215 | 4.00 (0.00, 8.00) | 0.50 | |
Advanced | 71 | 2.00 (0.00, 8.00) | 545 | 2.00 (0.00, 8.00) | 78 | 2.50 (0.00, 8.00) | 0.88 | |
PBA-s Irritability aggression | Early | 386 | 2.00 (0.00, 6.00) | 3885 | 1.00 (0.00, 4.00) | 624 | 0.50 (0.00, 4.00) | * <0.01 |
Moderate | 138 | 1.00 (0.00, 6.00) | 1261 | 1.00 (0.00, 6.00) | 215 | 1.00 (0.00, 4.00) | 0.13 | |
Advanced | 80 | 1.00 (0.00, 7.00) | 599 | 2.00 (0.00, 8.00) | 79 | 1.00 (0.00, 4.00) | 0.32 | |
PBA-s Psychosis | Early | 386 | 0.00 (0.00, 0.00) | 3878 | 0.00 (0.00, 0.00) | 625 | 0.00 (0.00, 0.00) | 0.17 |
Moderate | 137 | 0.00 (0.00, 0.00) | 1257 | 0.00 (0.00, 0.00) | 215 | 0.00 (0.00, 0.00) | 0.06 | |
Advanced | 69 | 0.00 (0.00, 0.00) | 547 | 0.00 (0.00, 0.00) | 77 | 0.00 (0.00, 0.00) | 0.22 | |
PBA-s Apathy | Early | 387 | 1.00 (0.00, 6.00) | 3884 | 0.00 (0.00, 4.00) | 624 | 0.00 (0.00, 4.00) | * <0.01 |
Moderate | 138 | 3.00 (0.00, 8.00) | 1258 | 4.00 (0.00, 9.00) | 215 | 6.00 (0.00, 9.00) | 0.10 | |
Advanced | 71 | 8.00 (0.00, 12.00) | 555 | 8.00 (0.00, 12.00) | 77 | 8.00 (1.00, 12.00) | 0.97 | |
PBA-s Executive function | Early | 386 | 0.00 (0.00, 4.00) | 3874 | 0.00 (0.00, 4.00) | 623 | 0.00 (0.00, 4.00) | * <0.01 |
Moderate | 137 | 4.00 (0.00, 9.00) | 1256 | 3.00 (0.00, 9.00) | 215 | 2.00 (0.00, 6.00) | 0.16 | |
Advanced | 71 | 6.00 (0.00, 12.00) | 541 | 6.00 (0.00, 10.00) | 76 | 4.00 (0.00, 9.00) | 0.34 | |
PBA-s Depressed mood frequency Present vs. Not Present | Early | 389 | 224 (57.58) | 3889 | 2064 (53.07) | 625 | 267 (42.72) | * <0.01 |
Moderate | 139 | 60 (43.17) | 1265 | 623 (49.25) | 215 | 98 (45.58) | 0.28 | |
Advanced | 82 | 45 (54.88) | 617 | 323 (52.35) | 81 | 45 (55.56) | 0.81 | |
PBA-s Suicidal ideation frequency Present vs. Not Present | Early | 389 | 40 (10.28) | 3889 | 363 (9.33) | 625 | 39 (6.24) | * 0.03 |
Moderate | 139 | 13 (9.35) | 1265 | 110 (8.70) | 215 | 21 (9.77) | 0.86 | |
Advanced | 82 | 15 (18.29) | 617 | 100 (16.21) | 81 | 14 (17.28) | 0.88 | |
PBA-s Anxiety frequency Present vs. Not Present | Early | 389 | 235 (60.41) | 3889 | 2225 (57.21) | 625 | 316 (50.56) | * <0.01 |
Moderate | 139 | 69 (49.64) | 1265 | 691 (54.62) | 215 | 125 (58.14) | 0.29 | |
Advanced | 82 | 48 (58.54) | 617 | 333 (53.97) | 81 | 36 (44.44) | 0.17 | |
PBA-s Irritability frequency Present vs. Not Present | Early | 389 | 228 (58.61) | 3889 | 2124 (54.62) | 625 | 303 (48.48) | * <0.01 |
Moderate | 139 | 72 (51.80) | 1265 | 690 (54.55) | 215 | 106 (49.30) | 0.33 | |
Advanced | 82 | 49 (59.76) | 617 | 353 (57.21) | 81 | 46 (56.79) | 0.90 | |
PBA-s Angry or aggressive behavior frequency Present vs. Not Present | Early | 389 | 129 (33.16) | 3889 | 1091 (28.05) | 625 | 135 (21.60) | * <0.01 |
Moderate | 139 | 41 (29.50) | 1265 | 417 (32.96) | 215 | 49 (22.79) | * 0.01 | |
Advanced | 82 | 29 (35.37) | 617 | 238 (38.57) | 81 | 24 (29.63) | 0.27 | |
PBA-s Lack of initiative (apathy) frequency Present vs. Not Present | Early | 389 | 209 (53.73) | 3889 | 1944 (49.99) | 625 | 264 (42.24) | * <0.01 |
Moderate | 139 | 88 (63.31) | 1265 | 884 (69.88) | 215 | 147 (68.37) | 0.27 | |
Advanced | 82 | 63 (76.83) | 617 | 469 (76.01) | 81 | 62 (76.54) | 0.98 | |
PBA-s Perseverative thinking or behavior frequency Present vs. Not Present | Early | 389 | 128 (32.90) | 3889 | 1493 (38.39) | 625 | 210 (33.60) | * 0.01 |
Moderate | 139 | 76 (54.68) | 1265 | 759 (60.00) | 215 | 119 (55.35) | 0.25 | |
Advanced | 82 | 53 (64.63) | 617 | 408 (66.13) | 81 | 50 (61.73) | 0.72 | |
PBA-s Obsessive-Compulsive Behaviors frequency Present vs. Not Present | Early | 389 | 100 (25.71) | 3889 | 800 (20.57) | 625 | 96 (15.36) | * <0.01 |
Moderate | 139 | 42 (30.22) | 1265 | 353 (27.91) | 215 | 49 (22.79) | 0.22 | |
Advanced | 82 | 33 (40.24) | 617 | 207 (33.55) | 81 | 20 (24.69) | 0.11 | |
PBA-s Delusions frequency Present vs. Not Present | Early | 389 | 26 (6.68) | 3889 | 198 (5.09) | 625 | 21 (3.36) | * 0.05 |
Moderate | 139 | 13 (9.35) | 1265 | 124 (9.80) | 215 | 12 (5.58) | 0.14 | |
Advanced | 82 | 24 (29.27) | 617 | 120 (19.45) | 81 | 10 (12.35) | * 0.02 | |
PBA-s Hallucinations frequency Present vs. Not Present | Early | 389 | 6 (1.54) | 3889 | 47 (1.21) | 625 | 7 (1.12) | 0.82 |
Moderate | 139 | 7 (5.04) | 1265 | 36 (2.85) | 215 | 1 (0.47) | * 0.02 | |
Advanced | 82 | 17 (20.73) | 617 | 105 (17.02) | 81 | 6 (7.41) | * 0.05 | |
PBA-s Disoriented Behavior frequency Present vs. Not Present | Early | 389 | 78 (20.05) | 3889 | 666 (17.13) | 625 | 104 (16.64) | 0.31 |
Moderate | 139 | 54 (38.85) | 1265 | 534 (42.21) | 215 | 102 (47.44) | 0.23 | |
Advanced | 82 | 49 (59.76) | 617 | 436 (70.66) | 81 | 58 (71.60) | 0.12 |
* Significant at level p < 0.05.
PBA-s = Problem Behaviours Assessment – Short, UHDRS = Unified Huntington’s Disease Rating Scale, HADS-SIS = Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale – Snaith Irritability Scale.
At moderate stages of disease, late onset participants were significantly more likely than young onset participants to have worse HADS-SIS depression (8.00 [4.00–11.00] vs. 5.00 [3.00–8.00], p < 0.001) and HADS-SIS inward irritability (2.50 [1.00–4.00] vs. 1.00 [0.00–3.00], p = 0.006) (Table 3, Appendix A). HADS-SIS depression and anxiety scores are normal 0-7, borderline 8–10, and abnormal 11–21 [20]. HADS-SIS inward irritability scores are normal <4 and outward irritability scores are normal <5 [21].
Behavioral symptom profiles also differed significantly between the typical onset group and the late onset group at early stages of disease. At early stages of disease, typical onset participants were significantly more likely than late onset participants to abuse drugs and alcohol, have worse anxiety, depression, irritability, apathy, executive function, suicidal ideation frequency, angry or aggressive behavior, lack of initiative, and obsessive-compulsive behaviors (Table 3, Appendix A).
At moderate stages of HD, typical onset participants scored significantly worse compared to late onset participants on drug abuse (6.66% vs. 1.87%, p = 0.004) and PBA-s Angry or aggressive behavior frequency (32.96% vs. 22.79%, p = 0.003) (Table 3, Appendix A).
Disease Progression
With exclusions applied to remove variability in time of follow-up assessments, 1,467 manifest HD participants were included in the sensitivity analysis determining average change in TFC score. The average decline in mean TFC score from baseline to second visit (1.5 to 2.5 years) was significantly faster in the young onset participants (–1.75 points) compared to the typical (–1.23 points, p = 0.0105) or late onset (–0.97 points, p = 0.0017) participants. From baseline to third visit (2.5 to 3.5 years), the young onset participants again declined significantly faster (–2.27 points) when compared to the late onset group (–1.28 points, p = 0.0002), and the typical group declined faster than the late onset group (–1.81 points, p = 0.005) (Figure A).
Figure A.
Average change in TFC score by age-of-onset groups. Young age of onset group shows markedly increased rate of decline compared to typical and late onset groups. Δ = difference in mean TFC score, BL = baseline.
1,406 manifest HD participants were included in the sensitivity analysis determining average change in UHDRS Functional Assessment Independence Scale. Results reinforced the changes seen in TFC score: the average decline in mean Independence Scale score from baseline to second visit was significantly faster in the young onset participants (–3.53 points) compared to the typical (–2.35 points, p = 0.002) or late onset participants (–2.01 points, p = 0.001). From baseline to third visit, the young onset group again declined significantly faster (–4.62 points) compared to the typical (–3.52 points, p = 0.013) and late onset (–2.92 points, p = 0.001) groups (Figure B).
Figure B.
Average change in UHDRS Functional Assessment Independence Scale. Young age of onset shows markedly increased rate of decline compared to typical and late onset groups from baseline to second visit and third visit. Δ = difference in mean FA score, BL = baseline.
Discussion
This analysis corroborates previous findings that symptom burden profile and HD progression differ significantly by age of onset [4]. Knowledge of the expected symptoms profile for different age of onset groups throughout disease progression will help to inform appropriate care of HD patients. Further, a strength of this study is that it clearly defines statistically significant phenotypic differences between the given age groups. Young adult onset (20–29 years) is a novel age group proposed by this study, and our analysis validates the separation of this cohort from the typical onset group based on distinct differences in symptom profile and most clearly in terms of rate of disease progression (Figure A and B). This study distinguishes young adult onset as a clinically unique phenotype, similar to the distinction of juvenile Huntington’s disease, which is defined by features of increased behavioral symptoms and faster progressors in functional decline [10,12,22]. While we recognize these differences likely exist on a continuum rather than stop at our defined age of onset cutoff point, our study shows that younger skewing adult onset participants warrant special considerations in management of their HD.
Regarding behavioral symptoms, at early stages of disease, young onset participants were more likely than late onset participants to suffer from worse behavioral symptoms, including drug and alcohol abuse, anxiety, depression, irritability, aggression, apathy, lack of initiative, obsessive-compulsive behaviors, and delusions. At moderate stages of disease, this preponderance of behavioral symptom burden diminishes, and late onset participants were found to have more depression and inward irritability when compared to the young onset group. At advanced stages of disease, the young onset group was again found to have a greater prevalence of drug abuse, hallucinations, and delusions than the late onset group.
A similar trend was also found in early and moderate stages of disease when comparing the typical onset group to the late onset group. At early stages of disease, the typical onset participants were more likely than late onset participants to abuse drugs and alcohol, have anxiety, depression, irritability, apathy, decreased executive function, increased suicidal ideation, angry or aggressive behavior, lack of initiative, and obsessive-compulsive behaviors. At moderate stages of disease, typical onset participants were more likely than late onset participants to abuse drugs and have angry or aggressive behavior.
These findings support previous suggestions that HD patients with young age of onset have a greater behavioral symptom burden at early disease stage, while late age-of-onset patients have greater motor burden at early stages [4]. Additionally, these findings reinforce our clinical observations, in which younger onset patients are more psychiatrically affected than older onset patients with the same degree of functional impairment. This study builds upon those findings by showing that typical onset participants have a significantly greater burden of behavioral symptoms profiles when compared to late onset participants at both early and moderate stages of disease. Further, although it varies by disease state evaluated, HADS-SIS score has been shown to have a minimal clinically important difference of about 1.5; thus even the small intergroup differences described in this study can affect symptom presentation and management [23].
Additionally, our study makes the important distinction that after adjusting for TFC score, young and typical age of onset participants still had a greater burden of behavioral symptoms compared to the late onset group. Traditionally, expanded CAG allele repeats associated with more aggressive disease have been used to explain the increased burden of behavioral symptoms in people with younger age of onset [1]. However, differences in symptom profiles were observed between the age of onset groups when compared at the same stage of disease severity (Table 3). These findings are corroborated by a previous study, which showed no correlation between CAG repeat length and psychiatric symptoms in HD [24]. Thus, other factors may explain the observed behavioral differences.
Social factors and anxiety about disease progression likely play into the behavioral differences observed. Young people in families with HD endure considerable anxiety and have a lack of support in relation to their HD risk [25]. This burden of anxiety and lack of social support could become magnified once HD manifests. Specifically, lack of social support is associated with decreased quality of life for patients with neurodegenerative diseases [26]. Additionally, given their young age of onset and the genetic nature of HD, these participants are more likely to be caretakers for living family members affected by HD, which has been shown to cause high rates of family dysfunction, psychological stress, and significantly affect quality of life [27,28,29]. Even when controlling for CAG repeat length, HD patients with a family history of HD have been shown to have earlier depression onset and are more likely to have behavioral manifestations as their initial major symptom compared to de novo HD patients [30]. Thus, the young age of onset group may have to deal with the emotional stress of their HD, which is potentially compounded by the emotional stress of caring for ailing HD family members [31].
Besides caring for affected relatives, young age of onset HD patients may also have to manage functional decline with the social expectations of young adult life, such as family planning and employment [31]. Notably, 78.89% of the young onset participants were unemployed, compared to 77.41% in typical onset and 91.40% in late onset (Table 1). While the unemployment rate is not significantly higher in the young onset group, loss of employment at a younger age could be an exacerbating factor for functional decline in the young onset population. There is compelling evidence that the cognitive and functional decline due to HD could cause loss of employment and contribute to exacerbation of behavioral symptoms. A previous study utilizing Enroll-HD found that the functional declines associated with HD contributed to HD patients leaving the workforce earlier [32]. Leaving the labor force has been found to be associated with higher risk of poor physical and mental health in adults [33]. This increased risk for mental illness is likely exacerbated in the young onset group, given they have less opportunity to participate in the workforce compared to late onset participants.
With respect to motor symptoms, this analysis shows that at early disease stages, the late onset group had worse motor function compared to the young onset group. At advanced stages of disease, young onset participants tended to have worse motor function than their late onset counterparts. Therefore, this study indicates that the greater burden of motor symptoms in the late onset group does not persist into more severe disease. At early disease stages, participants with late onset HD potentially have more motor deficits due to natural loss of function from aging. At later stages, HD patients are likely all similarly affected by injury to striatal networks. Further studies are warranted to elucidate why the young onset group had worse motor function with more advanced HD.
Our study found no significant trends regarding differences in cognitive variables between age groups and across stages of disease. This is somewhat surprising, given that age is a risk factor for dementia and cognitive decline [34]. Cognitive decline may be difficult to predict given the number of factors that influence cognitive reserve, such as exercise levels, intelligence level, occupational status, and years of education [34,35,36]. However, the Enroll-HD study participants analyzed in our study were relatively matched in education level and employment status (Table 1); therefore, we can expect all groups would have relatively equal levels of cognitive reserve.
The evidence relating cognitive decline in HD to age of onset prior to this study have been mixed. A previous study found cognitive status was better preserved in younger onset HD patients when compared to later onset patients [37]. Other studies suggested found that CAG repeats, which are correlated with earlier onset, were strongly associated with striatal atrophy and that subcortical atrophy, specifically atrophy of the head of the caudate nucleus, were positively associated with cognitive deficits [1,38,39]. More aggressive disease in the young onset group could potentially be equal to the cognitive deficits caused by aging in the late onset group, thus leading to our result of no significant intergroup differences in cognitive function.
Finally, young age of onset is predictive of a faster functional decline for adults with HD when compared to those with typical and late age of onset. This finding is consistent with the fact that younger age of onset is associated with expanded CAG repeats, which have been associated with faster and more widespread basal ganglia atrophy [1,40,41]. This result directly contradicts the highly cited 1995 study by Feigin et al. which reported that there was no correlation found between functional decline and age of onset in 129 manifest HD patients studied [5]. A 2003 study published by Mahant et al. had results more consistent with ours: in 1,026 patients, the rate of decline in UHDRS total motor score and Independence Scale was significantly faster with a younger age of onset [6]. However, their study included patients with juvenile onset HD and found no association with total functional capacity.
This study makes the important distinction that the association between faster functional decline and younger age of onset persists in the context of adult HD. Additionally, this study was able to show a novel correlation between younger age of onset and a global measure of functional capacity, TFC score. This study shows these robust associations in larger and more diverse HD population followed over the course of 3 years. These findings, paired with the finding that younger age of onset participants had a greater burden of behavioral symptoms, suggest that behavioral symptoms may contribute more significantly to functional decline in this younger population.
Our findings have important implications for the treatment and prognosis of HD. Particularly, patients with a young and typical age of onset should be screened for behavioral symptoms and directed towards resources which will help them more effectively manage these symptoms. This study can help family members and caretakers become more aware that behavioral issues are likely to manifest in patients with earlier HD onset. Given that motor symptoms are worse for young onset patients at later stages of disease, preventative and early interventions with modalities such as physical therapy can be implemented into their care plans [42]. Similar adaptations of our findings can be implemented to tailor treatments and interventions for typical and late onset HD patients.
Limitations
Since this study was cross-sectional and observational, this precludes us from making causative explanations between the variables studied and limits our conclusions to associations. Although we used rater determination of symptom onset, this estimation of age of onset could still be imprecise and amount to some discrepancy in the actual age of onset of manifest HD and the group assigned in this study. Study participants were categorized based on their TFC score at baseline and the motor, cognitive, and behavioral variables analyzed were determined from baseline visits. This cross-sectional design meant we could not account for interpersonal differences in the participant population. We are limited by the amount of follow-up data currently available in Enroll-HD. Sufficient TFC data points to analyze functional decline were only available up to visit 3, and these participants represented 20.88% of manifest HD patients in the Enroll-HD database. Insufficient powering also limited analyses at advanced stages of disease, since there were less than 1,000 participants at TFC Stages IV and V.
Future directions
Future longitudinal analyses are necessary to expand on and elucidate the findings of this study. Particularly, a multifactorial cause for the greater burden of behavioral symptoms in younger onset HD patients should be investigated. As the Enroll-HD database continues to grow, future studies may be able to better examine the disease course and phenotype of HD, particularly in its advanced stages.
Conclusions
Young age of onset is predictive of a faster functional decline for adults with HD when compared to those with typical and late age of onset. HD patients with a young age of onset have a greater burden of behavioral symptoms at early stages of disease, suggesting that behavioral symptoms may contribute more significantly to functional decline in this younger population. Motor deficits are comparatively worse for late onset participants at early stages of HD, and worse at advanced stages for young onset participants.
Additional File
The additional file for this article can be found as follows:
Pairwise Comparison after Bonferroni P-Value Correction of Motor, Cognitive, and Behavioral Variables.
Acknowledgements
Enroll-HD would not be possible without the vital contribution of the research participants and their families. We also acknowledge all the lead sites and investigators of Enroll-HD.
Funding Statement
LK has no conflicts of interest or funding sources relevant to this work. DL has received grant support from the Parkinson Foundation and the Huntington Disease Society of America. CY no conflicts of interest or funding sources relevant to this work. DB serves as a speaker for Acorda Therapeutics, Teva Pharmaceuticals, Adamas Pharmaceuticals, and Neurocrine Biosciences. He has served as a consultant for Acadia Pharmaceuticals, Genentech, Amneal Pharmaceuticals, Biogen Pharmaceuticals, Gerson Lehrman Group, Guidepoint, and L.E.K. Consulting. He is on the editorial board for Annals of Clinical & Translational Neurology. He has received grant support from the Parkinson Foundation and the Huntington Disease Society of America.
Ethics and Consent
This study did not require institutional review board (IRB) approval because it utilized pre-collected, deidentified data from the Enroll-HD database. Data collected at each study site was evaluated by its own IRB or equivalent [8]. Informed patient consent was not necessary for this work. We confirm that we have read the Journal’s position on issues involved in ethical publication and affirm that this work is consistent with those guidelines.
Funding Information
LK has no conflicts of interest or funding sources relevant to this work.
DL has received grant support from the Parkinson Foundation and the Huntington Disease Society of America.
CY no conflicts of interest or funding sources relevant to this work.
DB serves as a speaker for Acorda Therapeutics, Teva Pharmaceuticals, Adamas Pharmaceuticals, and Neurocrine Biosciences. He has served as a consultant for Acadia Pharmaceuticals, Genentech, Amneal Pharmaceuticals, Biogen Pharmaceuticals, Gerson Lehrman Group, Guidepoint, and L.E.K. Consulting. He is on the editorial board for Annals of Clinical & Translational Neurology. He has received grant support from the Parkinson Foundation and the Huntington Disease Society of America.
Competing Interests
The authors have no competing interests to declare.
Author Contribution
Research project: A. Conception, B. Organization, C. Execution
Statistical Analysis: A. Design, B. Execution, C. Review and Critique
Manuscript Preparation: A. Writing of the first draft, B. Editing the Manuscript, C. Review and Critique
LK: 1A, 1B, 1C, 2A, 2B, 2C, 3A, 3B
DL: 1A, 1B, 1C, 2A, 2B, 2C, 3B, 3C
CY: 2A, 2C, 3C
DB: 1A, 1B, 1C, 2A, 2B, 2C, 3B, 3C
References
- 1.Lee J-M, Ramos E, Lee J-H, et al. CAG repeat expansion in Huntington disease determines age at onset in a fully dominant fashion. Neurology. 2012; 78(10): 690–95. DOI: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318249f683 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Myers RH. Huntington’s disease genetics. NeuroRx. 2004; 1(2): 255–62. DOI: 10.1602/neurorx.1.2.255 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Duyao M, Ambrose C, Myers R, et al. Trinucleotide repeat length instability and age of onset in Huntington’s disease. Nature Genetics. 1993; 4(4): 387 DOI: 10.1038/ng0893-387 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Ranganathan M, Race JA, Allain DC, et al. Presentation of Behavioral Symptoms Varies with Age of Disease Onset in Huntington Disease. HSG 2018: Unlocking HD Houston, TX: Neurotherapeutics, 2018: 1168–214. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Feigin A, Kieburtz K, Bordwell K, et al. Functional decline in Huntington’s disease. Movement Disorders. 1995; 10(2): 211–14. DOI: 10.1002/mds.870100213 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Mahant N, McCusker E, Byth K, et al. Huntington’s disease: clinical correlates of disability and progression. Neurology. 2003; 61(8): 1085–92. DOI: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000086373.32347.16 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Access Data, Biosamples and Clinical Rater Training enroll-hd.org: CHDI Foundation, Inc.; 2020. Available from: https://www.enroll-hd.org/for-researchers/access-data/ accessed 5/25/20 2020.
- 8.Landwehrmeyer GB, Fitzer-Attas CJ, Giuliano JD, et al. Data analytics from Enroll-HD, a global clinical research platform for Huntington’s disease. Movement Disorders Clinical Practice. 2017; 4(2): 212–24. DOI: 10.1002/mdc3.12388 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Squitieri F, Frati L, Ciarmiello A, et al. Juvenile Huntington’s disease: does a dosage-effect pathogenic mechanism differ from the classical adult disease? Mechanisms of ageing and development. 2006; 127(2): 208–12. DOI: 10.1016/j.mad.2005.09.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Quarrell O, O’Donovan KL, Bandmann O, et al. The prevalence of juvenile Huntington’s disease: a review of the literature and meta-analysis. 2012; 4 DOI: 10.1371/4f8606b742ef3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Oosterloo M, Bijlsma EK, Van Kuijk SM, et al. Clinical and genetic characteristics of late-onset Huntington’s disease. 2019; 61: 101–05. DOI: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2018.11.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Roos RAJ. Huntington’s disease: a clinical review. Ojord. 2010; 5(1): 40 DOI: 10.1186/1750-1172-5-40 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Koutsis G, Karadima G, Kladi A, et al. Late-onset Huntington’s disease: diagnostic and prognostic considerations. 2014; 20(7): 726–30. DOI: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2014.03.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Cornejo-Olivas MR, Inca-Martinez MA, Espinoza-Huertas K, et al. Clinical and molecular features of late onset Huntington disease in a Peruvian cohort. 2015; 4(1): 99–105. DOI: 10.3233/JHD-140119 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Chaganti SS, McCusker EA, Loy CT. What do we know about late onset Huntington’s disease? JJoHsd. 2017; 6(2): 95–103. DOI: 10.3233/JHD-170247 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Wild E, Tabrizi S. Premanifest and early Huntington’s disease Huntington’s Disease (Oxford Monographs on Medical Genetics). Oxford University Press; 2014. DOI: 10.1093/med/9780199929146.003.0005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Shoulson I, Fahn S. Huntington disease. Clinical care and evaluation. 1979; 29(1): 1–1. DOI: 10.1212/WNL.29.1.1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Novak MJ, Tabrizi SJ. Huntington’s disease: clinical presentation and treatment. International review of neurobiology: Elsevier. 2011: 297–323. DOI: 10.1016/B978-0-12-381328-2.00013-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Winder JY, Achterberg WP, Marinus J, et al. Assessment Scales for Patients with Advanced Huntington’s Disease: Comparison of the UHDRS and UHDRS-FAP. 2018; 5(5): 527–33. DOI: 10.1002/mdc3.12646 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Rishi P, Rishi E, Maitray A, et al. Hospital anxiety and depression scale assessment of 100 patients before and after using low vision care: A prospective study in a tertiary eye-care setting. 2017; 65(11): 1203 DOI: 10.4103/ijo.IJO_436_17 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Snaith R, Constantopoulos A, Jardine M, et al. A clinical scale for the self-assessment of irritability. 1978; 132(2): 164–71. DOI: 10.1192/bjp.132.2.164 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Quarrell OW, Nance MA, Nopoulos P, et al. Managing juvenile Huntington’s disease. 2013; 3(3): 267–76. DOI: 10.2217/nmt.13.18 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Puhan MA, Frey M, Büchi S, et al. The minimal important difference of the hospital anxiety and depression scale in patients with chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. 2008; 6(1): 46 DOI: 10.1186/1477-7525-6-46 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Zappacosta B, Monza D, Meoni C, et al. Psychiatric symptoms do not correlate with cognitive decline, motor symptoms, or CAG repeat length in Huntington’s disease. Archives of Neurology. 1996; 53(6): 493–97. DOI: 10.1001/archneur.1996.00550060035012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Lewit-Mendes MF, Lowe GC, Lewis S, et al. Young People Living at Risk of Huntington’s Disease: The Lived Experience. Journal of Huntington’s disease. 2018(Preprint): 1–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Bužgová R, Kozáková R. Informing patients with progressive neurological disease of their health status, and their adaptation to the disease. BMC neurology. 2019; 19(1): 250 DOI: 10.1186/s12883-019-1488-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Anderson KE, Griffin J, Kinel A, et al. Quality of Care for Huntington’s Disease in the United States: Findings from a National Survey of Patients and Caregivers. Journal of Huntington’s disease. 2019(Preprint): 1–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Vamos M, Hambridge J, Edwards M, et al. The impact of Huntington’s disease on family life. Psychosomatics. 2007; 48(5): 400–04. DOI: 10.1176/appi.psy.48.5.400 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Aubeeluck A, Moskowitz CB. Huntington’s disease. Part 3: family aspects of HD. British Journal of Nursing. 2008; 17(5): 328–31. DOI: 10.12968/bjon.2008.17.5.28830 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Kringlen G, Kinsley L, Aufox S, et al. The Impact of Family History on the Clinical Features of Huntington’s Disease. Journal of Huntington’s disease. 2017; 6(4): 327–35. DOI: 10.3233/JHD-170256 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Carlozzi NE, Tulsky DS. Identification of health-related quality of life (HRQOL) issues relevant to individuals with Huntington disease. Journal of health psychology. 2013; 18(2): 212–25. DOI: 10.1177/1359105312438109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Watkins K, Purks J, Kumar A, et al. Huntington’s Disease and Employment: The Relative Contributions of Cognitive and Motor Decline to the Decision to Leave Work. Journal of Huntington’s disease. 2018(Preprint): 1–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Whitley E, Popham F. Leaving the labour market later in life: how does it impact on mechanisms for health? Occup Environ Med. 2017; 74(12): 877–86. DOI: 10.1136/oemed-2016-104258 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.DeCarli C. Mild cognitive impairment: prevalence, prognosis, aetiology, and treatment. The Lancet Neurology. 2003; 2(1): 15–21. DOI: 10.1016/S1474-4422(03)00262-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Radák Z, Kaneko T, Tahara S, et al. Regular exercise improves cognitive function and decreases oxidative damage in rat brain. Neurochemistry international. 2001; 38(1): 17–23. DOI: 10.1016/S0197-0186(00)00063-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Bonner-Jackson A, Long JD, Westervelt H, et al. Cognitive reserve and brain reserve in prodromal Huntington’s disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2013; 19(7): 739–50. DOI: 10.1017/S1355617713000507 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Gómez-Tortosa E, del Barrio A, Ruiz PJG, et al. Severity of cognitive impairment in juvenile and late-onset Huntington disease. Archives of neurology. 1998; 55(6): 835–43. DOI: 10.1001/archneur.55.6.835 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Starkstein SE, Brandt J, Bylsma F, et al. Neuropsychological correlates of brain atrophy in Huntington’s disease: a magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuroradiology. 1992; 34(6): 487–89. DOI: 10.1007/BF00598956 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Ruocco H, Lopes-Cendes I, Li L, et al. Striatal and extrastriatal atrophy in Huntington’s disease and its relationship with length of the CAG repeat. Brazilian journal of medical and biological research. 2006; 39(8): 1129–36. DOI: 10.1590/S0100-879X2006000800016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Snell RG, MacMillan JC, Cheadle JP, et al. Relationship between trinucleotide repeat expansion and phenotypic variation in Huntington’s disease. Nature genetics. 1993; 4(4): 393 DOI: 10.1038/ng0893-393 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Ruocco HH, Bonilha L, Li LM, et al. Longitudinal analysis of regional grey matter loss in Huntington disease: effects of the length of the expanded CAG repeat. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry. 2008; 79(2): 130–35. DOI: 10.1136/jnnp.2007.116244 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Fritz NE, Rao AK, Kegelmeyer D, et al. Physical therapy and exercise interventions in Huntington’s disease: a mixed methods systematic review. Journal of Huntington’s disease. 2017; 6(3): 217–35. DOI: 10.3233/JHD-170260 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Pairwise Comparison after Bonferroni P-Value Correction of Motor, Cognitive, and Behavioral Variables.