Skip to main content
Springer logoLink to Springer
. 2020 May 29;20(4):816–841. doi: 10.3758/s13415-020-00804-6

Characterizing anhedonia: A systematic review of neuroimaging across the subtypes of reward processing deficits in depression

Alessandra Borsini 1,✉,#, Amelia St John Wallis 2,#, Patricia Zunszain 1, Carmine Maria Pariante 1, Matthew J Kempton 3
PMCID: PMC7395022  PMID: 32472419

Abstract

Anhedonia is a key symptom of major depressive disorder (MDD) and comprises behavioural deficits in three reward processing subtypes: reward liking, reward wanting, and reward learning. However, neuroimaging findings regarding the neural abnormalities underpinning these deficits are complex. We have conducted a systematic review to update, reframe and summarize neuroimaging findings across the three subtypes of anhedonia in MDD. Using PubMed, The Cochrane Library, PsycINFO, and Web of Science databases, we identified 59 fMRI studies comparing participants with current or remitted MDD with controls, using reward processing tasks. For reward liking and wanting, striatal hypoactivation was observed, alongside hypoactivation and hyperactivation across frontal regions. For reward learning, blunted frontostriatal sensitivity to positive feedback was observed. These findings highlight the importance of studying anhedonia not only as a clinical manifestation but also as a neurobiological mechanism underlying depressive disorder and other broader psychiatric conditions.

Keywords: Anhedonia, Depression, Neuroimaging, fMRI, Reward processing


Major depressive disorder (MDD) is both common, with a lifetime prevalence of 16.6% in the USA (Kessler, Petukhova, Sampson, Zaslavsky, & Wittchen, 2012), and consequential, being the second leading contributor to global years lived with disability (YLDs) worldwide (Ferrari et al., 2013). Anhedonia is one of two key symptoms required for a diagnosis of MDD in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association, 2013), and is defined as ‘markedly diminished interest or pleasure in all, or almost all, activities most of the day, nearly every day’ (American Psychiatric Association, 2013), and so represents a deficit in reward processing. In a study examining the factor structure for DSM-IV, MDD symptoms in a sample 2,615 army recruits, the best fit for the data indicated that MDD consisted of both a somatic and nonsomatic component (Elhai et al., 2012), and anhedonia had the second highest factor weighting (Beta = 0.76) for the nonsomatic component (after depressed mood), as well as the second highest factor weighting of all symptoms (Elhai et al., 2012). This suggests that anhedonia is a core feature of depression. Anhedonia is a symptom which warrants attention; indeed, reward processing deficits are associated with increased risk of new onset MDD (Rawal, Collishaw, Thapar, & Rice, 2013), anhedonia may precede illness onset, and, moreover, it can often persist past the remission of other depressive symptoms (Schrader, 1997), as do deficits on reward processing tasks (Pechtel, Dutra, Goetz, & Pizzagalli, 2013).

Three subtypes of anhedonia

In DSM-5, anhedonia comprises deficits in hedonic experience of rewards and motivation for rewards (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). However, reviews have called for research to conceptualize anhedonia as comprising deficits across three partially separable subtypes of reward processing: reward liking, reward wanting, and reward learning (Admon & Pizzagalli, 2015; Rømer Thomsen, Whybrow, & Kringelbach, 2015; Treadway & Zald, 2011). Reward liking refers to the experience of pleasure from rewards; reward wanting refers to motivation driving individuals towards rewards; and reward learning refers to guiding behaviour based on previous rewards and punishments using prediction errors (PE), which signal differences between expected outcomes and what actually happens in order to support learning (Berridge & Robinson, 2003; Rømer Thomsen et al., 2015). These three subtypes of reward processing are understood to have partially separable neurobiological underpinnings (Berridge & Robinson, 2003; Rømer Thomsen et al., 2015), and behavioural deficits in each of the three subtypes make up anhedonia in MDD (Rømer Thomsen, 2015).

A more recent review also provides a more comprehensive model of anhedonia (Husain & Roiser, 2018); this involves self-cued or environmentally cued option generation; evaluation and selection between options; anticipation and preparation for action; motor mechanisms to initiative and sustain approach behaviour; a consummatory phase with positive or negative impact; and, finally, learning from the outcomes to optimize future decision-making (Husain & Roiser, 2018).

In this review, anhedonia will be conceptualized as comprising three reward processing subtypes (reward liking, reward wanting, reward learning) as it has been validated and used in several other studies (Admon & Pizzagalli, 2015; Rømer Thomsen et al., 2015; Treadway & Zald, 2011). However, these subtypes have been proposed to map onto a more comprehensive, transdiagnostic models of anhedonia (Husain & Roiser, 2018); for example, reward liking is related to the consummatory phase with positive or negative impact, reward wanting is related to selection between options and initiating and sustaining approach behaviour (incentive motivation) as well as the anticipation and preparation phase, and reward learning is related to learning from outcomes to optimize future decisions. Additionally, it should be noted that overlap does exist across these subtypes; indeed, reward wanting involves valuation and decision-making processes, and all three subtypes involve some representation of the hedonic value of the reward.

Poor outcomes of anhedonia

Research has increased our understanding of the partially dissociable behavioural deficits underlying anhedonia in MDD. However, common antidepressants, such as selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) do not ameliorate these behavioural deficits (Argyropoulos & Nutt, 2013; Price, Cole, & Goodwin, 2009), and, conversely, SSRIs have been shown to blunt neural responses to rewarding stimuli in healthy controls (McCabe, Mishor, Cowen, & Harmer, 2010). Therefore, those experiencing anhedonia may show a worse response to treatment. Indeed, anhedonia predicts a longer time to remission in adolescents treated with medication switch or medication switch with added cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT; McMakin et al., 2012), and poor antidepressant treatment response in adults (Uher et al., 2012); additionally, objectively measured impairments in reward learning are associated with poorer response to inpatient treatment (Vrieze et al., 2013). Furthermore, anhedonia is associated with both increased severity of depressive symptoms (Gong et al., 2017; Pelizza & Ferrari, 2009) and illness persistence (Spijker, Bijl, de Graaf, & Nolen, 2001). Therefore, to diminish the association between anhedonia, poorer treatment response, and worse illness outcomes, new targeted treatments are required to specifically address anhedonia. Indeed, in pharmacological treatment development, it is helpful to have objective neurobiological markers of successful treatment of the symptom (Krystal et al., 2018). Neuroimaging is a key tool for improving our understanding of the neurobiology of anhedonia in MDD, which is vital for developing these new targeted treatments and neurobiological treatment markers.

Importance of neuroimaging in anhedonia

Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) has been used extensively to investigate the neural abnormalities associated with anhedonia in MDD within the frontostriatal reward processing network, which comprises frontal areas such as the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), and orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), and midbrain limbic areas, including the ventral striatum (VS), insula, and thalamus (Haber & Knutson, 2010; Sescousse, Caldu, Segura, & Dreher, 2013). By using fMRI to compare activation and connectivity in the frontostriatal network between MDD patients and controls during reward processing tasks, researchers can assess the neural abnormalities underpinning the behavioural deficits across reward liking, reward wanting, and reward learning, which make up anhedonia in MDD. This empirical data are essential for the development of neurobiological models of anhedonia (Treadway & Zald, 2011).

Aim of the current study

Functional MRI studies demonstrate that MDD patients show abnormalities in frontostriatal functioning and connectivity during reward processing, associated with the behavioural reward processing deficits making up anhedonia (Admon & Pizzagalli, 2015). Indeed, striatal hypoactivation in response to rewards in MDD has been highlighted by meta-analyses (Keren et al., 2018; Zhang, Chang, Guo, Zhang, & Wang, 2013) and transdiagnostic reviews of the neural basis of reward liking, wanting, and learning deficits (Baskin-Sommers & Foti, 2015; Whitton, Treadway, & Pizzagalli, 2015). Striatal hypoactivation has also been reported for anticipation and receipt of rewards in a review focusing on adolescent MDD (O’Callaghan & Stringaris, 2019). However, complexity and inconsistency in the literature regarding the neurobiological changes associated with each of the three subtypes of anhedonia remains significant. This paper presents an updated and reframed review of neuroimaging studies in MDD across the three subtypes of anhedonia, with reference to more recent comprehensive anhedonia models (Husain & Roiser, 2018). The aim is to further clarify patterns in the data, detail inconsistencies in the literature, understand the limitations of the current evidence base, and make recommendations for future research. The results of this study will be important for improving our understanding of the common and dissociable neural underpinnings of the three subtypes of anhedonia in MDD and elucidating findings from the literature, which will aid in the development of neurobiological models of anhedonia. These, in turn, we hope will be important for developing targeted treatments and revealing neurobiological markers of successful treatment of anhedonia.

Methodology

English language studies dated from 1992 to August 2019 were included in this systematic review, as the first study using fMRI neuroimaging in humans was published in 1992. Studies were included if they used fMRI to compare participants with current or remitted major depressive disorder (MDD) with controls using reward processing tasks to probe reward liking, reward wanting, and/or reward learning.

Studies were identified using the keywords ‘reward processing’ or ‘reward’ or ‘anhedonia’; ‘depression’ or ‘major depression’ or ‘major depressive disorder’ or ‘MDD’; ‘neuroimaging’ or ‘fmri’ or ‘fMRI’. The following databases were searched to identify relevant studies: PubMed, The Cochrane Library, PsycINFO, Web of Science. Studies from the database searches were initially screened based on the title and abstract, and then the full text was reviewed for relevant studies. Studies identified by the searches were excluded for the following reasons: using healthy participants rather than participants with current or remitted MDD versus healthy controls, using resting state fMRI rather than fMRI with a reward processing task, observing behaviour rather than using fMRI, and/or using a different imaging technique such as EEG to measure event-related potentials. A PRISMA flow diagram is presented in Fig. 1. Extracted information included the author and date, the neuroimaging technique, the subtype of reward processing studied, the reward processing task used, the sample characteristics, the diagnostic criteria, and the neuroimaging abnormality observed in MDD.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

PRISMA flow diagram showing flow of information through the systematic review process, including records identified, screened, included, and excluded

A total of 59 studies using the following reward processing tasks were included: the monetary incentive delay task (MID), presentation of positive and negative stimuli (including pictorial, word, and oral stimuli, and pleasant music), reward guessing tasks involving choosing between stimuli and receiving a random reward or loss outcome, the Wheel of Fortune (WoF) task, the Effort Expenditure for Reward Task (EEfRT), an effort-based cost-benefit valuation task, Pavlovian, instrumental and reversal learning tasks, probabilistic reward tasks involving choosing between lotteries with varying values and probabilities, and the slot machine task involving receiving unexpected rewards based on the outcome of slot machine spins. Two of the 59 studies identified were meta-analyses. It was recorded whether the study interpreted neuroimaging results from their tasks as relating to reward liking, reward wanting, and/or reward learning processes, and studies were categorized into one or more of the three subtypes based on this. A summary of identified studies for each reward processing subtype is presented in Fig. 2.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Summary of the total number of studies included and the number of studies identified for each subtype of anhedonia. Six papers were included in both reward liking and reward wanting, and one paper was included in all three subtypes

Results

Reward liking

A total of 29 studies investigating neuroimaging abnormalities in MDD during the experience of reward or loss were identified. These studies used a variety of tasks, including the monetary incentive delay task (MID), presentation of positive and negative stimuli, reward guessing tasks, and the reward delivery phases of an instrumental loss-avoidance win-gain task, a probabilistic reward task, and the wheel of fortune task (WoF). Two meta-analyses were also identified. A summary of identified papers for reward liking is presented in Table 1.

Table 1.

Papers investigating neuroimaging in major depressive disorder (MDD) within the subtype of reward liking, in the order they appear in the results section

REWARD LIKING
Author Reward task Sample characteristics Mean depression and anhedonia scores Diagnostic criteria Neuroimaging abnormality
Knutson et al., 2008 Monetary incentive delay task (MID) 14 Major Depressive Disorder (MDD) v 12 healthy controls (HCs)

Beck Depression Inventory–II (BDI-II) 25.38

No anhedonia score provided

Met DSM-IV criteria Recruit ACC more during anticipation of increasing gains, opposite to controls
Pizzagalli et al., 2009 MID 30 unmedicated MDD v 31 HCs

Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HAM-D) 17.97

BDI-II 27.48

Anhedonia present and assessed using BDI-II subscale, but score not reported

Met DSM-IV criteria NAc and caudate hypoactivation for rewards, and caudate hypoactivation for losses
Sankar et al., 2019 MID 20 female MDD v 20 HCs

HAM-D 14.88

No anhedonia score provided

Met DSM-IV criteria Did not show activation in right anterior insula in response to gains and losses, unlike controls
Forbes et al., 2009 Monetary reward guessing task 15 adolescents with MDD v 28 adolescent HCs

No HAM-D or BDI score provided

No anhedonia score provided

Diagnosis assessed by K-SADS-PL, confirmed by interview with child psychiatrist Caudate hypoactivation, and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) and mPFC hyperactivation for rewards, correlating with lower positive affect
Redlich et al., 2015 Card guessing 33 MDD v 33 bipolar disorder v 34 HCs

HAM-D 24.56

BDI 27.88

Snaith-Hamilton Pleasure Scale (SHAPS) 6.26 (HCs 0.52), binary scoring system where higher scores indicated higher anhedonia

Met DSM-IV criteria NAc hypoactivation for rewards, and increased coupling with ventral tegmental area (VTA)
Foti et al., 2014 Monetary reward guessing task 24 MDD v 18 HCs MASQ depression subscale 38.09 MASQ anhedonia subscale 64.36 (HCs 40.00) Met DSM-IV criteria VS hypoactivation for reward, correlating with impaired mood reactivity
Satterthwaite et al., 2015 Card guessing 25 MDD v 27 bipolar v 37 HCs

BDI-II 21.75

No anhedonia score provided

Met DSM-IV criteria Hypoactivation in VS, cingulate and insula for rewards, correlating with depression severity
Steele et al., 2007 Card guessing 15 MDD v 14 HCs

HAM-D 27.5

BDI 36.9

SH 33.8 (HCs 51.9), where higher scores indicated lower anhedonia

Met DSM-IV criteria ACC hypoactivation for negative feedback, VS hypoactivation for positive feedback
Carl et al., 2016 MID 33 MDD v 20 HCs

BDI-II 25.27

Anhedonia assessed using BDI-II subscale score 4.91

Met DSM-IV criteria Faster NAc attenuation to rewards
Moses-Kolko et al., 2011 Card guessing 12 post-partum MDD v 12 HCs

HAM-D 21.3

FCPS 126.3 (HCs 139.9)

Met DSM-IV criteria, HAM-D score ≤15 in past month Faster VS attenuation to rewards
Epstein et al., 2006 Positive and negative word stimuli 10 MDD v 12 HCs

No HAM-D or BDI score provided

Anhedonia assessed with 1 question on HAM-D, score not provided

Met DSM-IV criteria Ventral striatum (VS) hypoactivation for positive stimuli, correlating with anhedonia
Connolly et al., 2015 Affective pictorial stimuli 51 female unmedicated MDD v 61 HCs

IDS-C score 25.43

Anhedonia score from average of two IDS-C items 1.71 (HCs 0.02)

Met DSM-IV criteria Striatal hypoactivation for affective stimuli, across caudate, putamen and nucleus accumbens (NAc)
Antonesei et al., 2018 Gustatory reward stimuli 26 MDD v 33 HCs No HAM-D, BDI or anhedonia score provided. Not reported Left caudate hypoactivation in response to targets predicting rewarding stimuli
Keedwell et al., 2005 Happy and sad emotional stimuli 12 MDD v 12 HCs

BDI 33.5

Fawcett-Clark Pleasure Scale (FCPS) 63.3 (HCs not provided)

Met ICD-10 criteria Ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) hyperactivation and VS hypoactivation for happy stimuli, correlating with anhedonia
Osuch et al., 2009 Listening to favourite music 16 MDD v 15 HCs

BDI 25.3

SHAPS 36.4 (significantly lower than HCs), where higher SHAPS scores indicated lower anhedonia

Met DSM-IV criteria Hypoactivation of orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) and VS for music
Jenkins et al., 2018 Listening to preferred music 12 MDD v 10 HCs

HAM-D 15.08

SHAPS 6.67 (HCs 0.00), binary scoring system where higher SHAPS scores indicated higher anhedonia

Met DSM-IV criteria Faster attenuation of NAc activation
Johnston et al., 2015 Instrumental loss-avoidance and win-gain task 20 treatment resistant MDD v 20 HCs

HAM-D 16.00

BDI-II 32.42

No anhedonia score provided

Clinical diagnosis in tertiary service for treatment resistant MDD Striatal hyperactivation for rewards, less hippocampal deactivation for losses
Forbes et al., 2006 Probabilistic reward task 14 MDD v 17 HCs, all aged 9–17

No HAM-D or BDI score provided

No anhedonia score provided

Diagnosis assessed using K-SADS-PL, met DSM-IV criteria Hypoactivation of ACC, caudate and OFC, and hyperactivation of amygdala
Keren et al., 2018 Meta-analysis 38 fMRI studies NA NA Striatal hypoactivation for rewards
Zhang et al., 2013 Meta-analysis 22 fMRI studies NA NA Caudate hypoactivation for rewards
McCabe et al., 2009 Sight and flavour of pleasant and aversive foods 13 remitted MDD v 14 HCs

HAM-D 2.3

BDI 5.5

FCPS 118 (HCs 118), no significant difference

SHAPS 23 (HCs 19.25), no significant difference

Met DSM-IV criteria for at least 1 past major depressive episode (MDE), no current Axis I psychopathology VS hypoactivation for pleasant stimuli, and caudate hypoactivation for unpleasant stimuli
Ubl, Kuehner, Kirsch, Ruttorf, Diener, et al., 2015b Probabilistic reward task 30 unmedicated MDD v 29 HCs

HAM-D 18.40

BDI-II 25.50

SHAPS 42.93 (HCs 49.29), where higher SHAPS scores indicated lower anhedonia

Met DSM-IV criteria No difference in striatal activation for rewards
Engelmann et al., 2017 Probabilistic reward task 19 unmedicated MDD v 23 HCs

No HAM-D or BDI score provided

No anhedonia score provided

Met DSM-IV criteria Increased coding of losses in anterior insula
Mitterschiffthaler et al., 2003 Positive and negative valenced images 7 females with MDD and high anhedonia v 7 HCs

BDI 33.6

FCPS 2.90 (HCs 4.14)

Met DSM-IV criteria, full criteria met over period of ≥2 years. Hypoactivation of mPFC, and hyperactivation of inferior frontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), thalamus, putamen and insula for positive images
Kumar et al., 2015 MID 12 MDD v 10 HCs

BDI-II 25.25

SHAPS 5.42 (HCs 0.40), binary scoring system where higher scores indicated higher anhedonia

Met DSM-IV criteria Hyperactivation of medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) for rewards under stress, greatest with previous adverse life events
Dichter et al., 2012 MID 19 remitted MDD v 19 HCs

BDI 2.63

No anhedonia score provided

Met DSM-IV criteria for remitted MDD no current Axis I psychopathology Hypoactivation in OFC, frontal pole, thalamus and insula for rewards
McCabe, 2016 Subjective ratings of oral stimuli 13 remitted MDD v 14 HCs

HAM-D 2.3

BDI 5.5

FCPS 118 (HCs 118), no significant difference

SHAPS 23 (HCs 19.25), no significant difference

Met DSM-IV criteria for at least 1 past Major Depressive Episode (MDE), recovery assessed through clinical interview and HAM-D score <8 Negative correlation of dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC) with liking of stimuli
Schiller et al., 2013 MID 19 remitted MDD v 19 HCs

BDI-II 2.6

No anhedonia score provided

Met DSM-IV criteria for remitted MDD, no current Axis I psychopathology Superior frontal and inferior frontal hypoactivation for losses
Morgan et al., 2016 Card guessing 43 boys with history of MDD v 68 with history of other psychiatric illnesses v 55 HCs

MAFQ 6.27

No anhedonia score provided

Diagnosis assessed using K-SADS at ages 8, 10, 11, 12, and using DSM-IV at age 20 Increased connectivity from the mPFC to striatal areas for rewards
Young et al., 2016 Listening to pleasant music 25 MDD v 25 HCs

HAM-D 26.57

Anhedonia subscale of Mood and Anxiety Symptom Questionnaire (MASQ), MASQ-AD 61.81 (HCs 39.27)

Met DSM-IV criteria Reduced connectivity from posterior vmPFC to other frontostriatal areas, including the OFC, insula, NAc, and VTA, during music, correlating with anhedonia

Using the MID, during which participants are presented with a cue indicating potential gains and losses and then perform a speeded button press to win the gain or avoid the loss (Knutson et al., 2000), studies have observed hypoactivation of the caudate and nucleus accumbens (NAc) hypoactivation in response to rewards in unmedicated MDD participants (Pizzagalli et al., 2009), and hypoactivation of the right anterior insula in response to gains and losses in females with MDD (Sankar et al., 2019).

Using reward guessing tasks, which involve participants choosing between stimuli and receiving a random reward or loss outcome (Delgado, Nystrom, Fissell, Noll, & Fiez, 2000), other studies have found similar patterns in MDD patients during response to rewards, including hypoactivation in the caudate (Forbes et al., 2009), NAc (Redlich et al., 2015), ventral striatum (VS; Foti, Carlson, Sauder, & Proudfit, 2014; Satterthwaite et al., 2015; Steele, Kumar, & Ebmeier, 2007), and insula (Satterthwaite et al., 2015). In one study, caudate hypoactivation correlated with lower positive affect (Forbes et al., 2009), and, in another, VS hypoactivation correlated with impaired mood reactivity (Foti et al., 2014). Moreover, faster attenuation of NAc activity has been reported during response to rewards in MDD using the MID (Carl et al., 2016), and a study in females with postpartum depression using a reward guessing task found faster attenuation of VS activation in response to rewards (Moses-Kolko et al., 2011).

Similar patterns have been found using passive responses to positive and negative stimuli. Studies have observed VS hypoactivation in response to positive word stimuli in MDD (Epstein et al., 2006); hypoactivation across the caudate, putamen, and NAc in response to affective pictures in unmedicated females with MDD (Connolly, Gollan, Cobia, & Wang, 2015); hypoactivation of the left caudate in response to targets predicting rewarding gustatory stimuli (Antonesei, Murayama, & McCabe, 2018); and hypoactivation of the VS in response to happy stimuli (Keedwell, Andrew, Williams, Brammer, & Phillips, 2005) and to favourite music (Osuch et al., 2009). Furthermore, similar to results using both the MID and guessing tasks, it has also been observed that MDD participants have faster attenuation of NAc activation when listening to preferred music (Jenkins et al., 2018). In one study, the reported striatal hypoactivation did not correlate with MDD or anhedonia severity (Connolly et al., 2015), but in other studies, hypoactivation of the VS did correlate with anhedonia levels (Epstein et al., 2006; Keedwell et al., 2005).

Other tasks have also been used to investigate reward liking in MDD, albeit more rarely. Using an instrumental loss-avoidance win-gain task, striatal hypoactivation during response to rewards in participants with treatment-resistant MDD (Johnston et al., 2015). Furthermore, using a probabilistic reward task involving choosing between options with differing reward values and probabilities (Rogers et al., 2003), hypoactivation of the caudate was found in children with MDD in response to rewards (Forbes et al., 2006).

In terms of meta-analyses, one reported striatal hypoactivation in MDD during reward feedback (Keren et al., 2018), and another found specifically caudate hypoactivation during response to rewards (Zhang et al., 2013), both across studies using a variety of reward processing tasks, such as the MID, card guessing, and presentation of positive and negative stimuli (Keren et al., 2018; Zhang et al., 2013). Furthermore, the striatal hypoactivation patterns in response to rewards reported in the above studies have been also been observed in remission, as one study reported those with remitted MDD showed VS hypoactivation for pleasant food stimuli and caudate hypoactivation for unpleasant food stimuli (McCabe, Cowen, & Harmer, 2009).

However, three identified studies observed different patterns of striatal activation in MDD in response to rewards: A study using a reinforcement learning task with unmedicated MDD participants found no striatal hypoactivation in response to rewards (Ubl, Kuehner, Kirsch, Ruttorf, Diener, et al., 2015b); another using a probabilistic reward task showed increased coding of losses in the anterior insula in unmedicated MDD (Engelmann, Berns, & Dunlop, 2017); and an early study actually found hyperactivation of lower limbic areas (including the thalamus, putamen, and insula) in response to positively valanced images (Mitterschiffthaler et al., 2003).

In terms of frontal areas, hyperactivation of the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) has been reported in MDD participants during response to rewards on the MID task under stress, and this effect was greatest when individuals had experienced previous adverse life events (Kumar et al., 2015). Similarly, using reward guessing tasks, studies have found vmPFC and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) hyperactivation in response to rewards in adolescents with MDD, correlating with lower positive affect (Forbes et al., 2009). This time using response to happy stimuli, another study also observed frontal hyperactivation in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), alongside lower limbic hypoactivation in the VS, in MDD (Keedwell et al., 2005), correlating with anhedonia levels (Keedwell et al., 2005).

In contrast, hypoactivation of the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) has been observed in MDD participants when listening to their favourite music (Osuch et al., 2009) and in children with MDD during a probabilistic reward task (Forbes et al., 2006). In the latter study, they also observed hypoactivation of the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) in response to rewards (Forbes et al., 2006). Moreover, cingulate cortex hypoactivation in response to rewards has been reported in MDD participants using reward guessing tasks (Satterthwaite et al., 2015), as well as lower anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) recruitment for unexpected losses (Steele et al., 2007).

During remission of MDD, frontal areas have been found to show a general pattern of hypoactivation (in the OFC and right frontal pole) during response to rewards on the MID (Dichter, Kozink, McClernon, & Smoski, 2012), and a negative correlation between dmPFC activity and liking of an oral stimulus, opposite to healthy controls (McCabe, 2016). Additionally, a study observed hypoactivation of the superior and inferior frontal gyri in response to losses in remitted MDD (Schiller, Minkel, Smoski, & Dichter, 2013).

Two other identified studies in the reward liking subtype assessed frontostriatal connectivity during response to rewards: one study reported that, in a group of male adolescents, those with a history of MDD showed increased frontostriatal connectivity during delivery of rewards on the reward guessing task (Morgan et al., 2016), whereas another found reduced connectivity between the posterior vmPFC and other frontostriatal regions in MDD while listening to pleasant music (Young et al., 2016), which correlated with higher anhedonia levels (Young et al., 2016).

Reward wanting

A total of 25 papers investigating neuroimaging abnormalities in MDD patients were identified for the reward wanting subtype. In line with recent models of anhedonia, these reward wanting studies were divided into those focusing on the anticipatory phase (reward anticipation) and those looking at selection between reward options and motor mechanisms initiating approach behaviour (incentive motivation; Husain & Roiser, 2018). These studies used a variety of tasks, including the EEfRT an effort-based cost-benefit valuation task; the WoF for incentive motivation; and the MID, reward guessing tasks, and a slot machine task for reward anticipation. One meta-analysis was also identified. A summary of identified papers for reward wanting is presented in Table 2.

Table 2.

Papers investigating neuroimaging in major depressive disorder (MDD) within the subtype of reward wanting, grouped by incentive motivation and reward anticipation, and in the order they appear in the results section

REWARD WANTING
Incentive motivation
Author Reward task Sample characteristics Mean depression score Diagnostic criteria Neuroimaging abnormality
Yang et al., 2016 Effort expenditure for rewards task (EEfRT) 25 MDD v 25 HCs

HAM-D 27.58

BDI 33.04

SHAPS 34.36 (HCs 21.56), where higher scores indicated higher anhedonia

TEPS 63.52 (HCs 91.00)

Met DSM-IV criteria Caudate hypoactivation during reward selection
Smoski et al., 2009 Wheel of Fortune (WoF) 16 MDD v 15 HCs

HAM-D 23.5

No anhedonia score provided

Met DSM-IV criteria OFC hyperactivation and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) hypoactivation during reward selection; Caudate hypoactivation, but no change in medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) during reward anticipation
Shad et al., 2011 WoF 22 adolescents with MDD v 22 adolescent HCs

No HAM-D or BDI score provided

No anhedonia score provided

Diagnosis assessed using K-SADS-PL OFC hypoactivation, and right ACC hyperactivation during reward selection
Forbes et al., 2006 Probabilistic reward task 14 MDD v 17 HCs, all aged 9–17

No HAM-D or BDI score provided

No anhedonia score provided

Diagnosis assessed using K-SADS-PL, met DSM-IV criteria OFC hyperactivation during reward selection
Park et al., 2017 Effort-based cost-benefit valuation task 22 MDD v 23 schizophrenia v 31 HCs

HAM-D 15.5

BDI 25.9

Apathy Evaluation Scale (AES) 43.4 (HCs 35.5)

Met DSM-IV criteria Reduced medial orbitofrontal cortex (OFC)-striatal functional connectivity
Reward anticipation
Author Reward task Sample characteristics Mean depression score Diagnostic criteria Neuroimaging abnormality
Arrondo et al., 2015 MID 24 MDD v 22 schizophrenia v 21 HCs

BDI median 32

SHAPS 36 (HCs 24), where higher score indicated higher anhedonia

Temporal Experience of Pleasure Scale (TEPS) 53.5 (HCs 80)

Met DSM-IV criteria, confirmed diagnosis using PANSS and Mini-International Psychiatric Inventory VS hypoactivation during reward anticipation, not correlating with anhedonia
Hagele et al., 2015 MID 24 MDD v 106 other psychiatric illness v 54 HCs

BDI 24.3

No anhedonia score provided

Met ICD-10 and DSM-IV criteria VS hypoactivation during reward anticipation
Takamura et al., 2017 MID 12 MDD v 12 HCs

HAM-D 20.1

BDI-II 30.8

No anhedonia score provided

Met DSM-IV criteria Decreased VS and putamen sensitivity to increasing rewards
Stringaris et al., 2015 MID At baseline, 22 MDD v 101 subthreshold MDD v 123 HCs.

Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ)

MDD 16.4

Subclinical MDD 13.9

Coded as having anhedonia is rated by self-report in screening questions of Development and Well-being Assessment (DAWBA)

Met DSM-IV criteria. VS hypoactivation during reward anticipation in those with current or future subthreshold and clinical MDD, associated with anhedonia scores
Ubl, Kuehner, Kirsch, Ruttorf, Diener, et al., 2015b Probabilistic reward task 30 unmedicated MDD v 29 HCs

HAM-D 18.40

BDI-II 25.50

SHAPS 42.93 (HCs 49.29), where higher scores indicated lower anhedonia

Met DSM-IV criteria VS, ACC and OFC hypoactivation during reward anticipation
Hamilton et al., 2018 MID 16 MDD v 14 HCs

HAM-D 13.6

BDI-II 26.27

SHAPS 49 (HCs 64), where higher scores indicated lower anhedonia

Met DSM-5 criteria Lower dopamine activity in the VS and right dorsal striatum, associated with lower connectivity to cortical targets
Misaki et al., 2016 MID 44 MDD v 45 HCs

HAM-D 17.3

SHAPS 28.9 (HCs 18.3), where higher scores indicated higher anhedonia

Met DSM-IV criteria Nucleus accumbens (NAc) hypoactivation during reward anticipation, correlating with anhedonia
Pizzagalli et al., 2009 MID 30 unmedicated MDD v 31 HCs

HAM-D 17.97

BDI-II 27.48

Anhedonia present and assessed using BDI-II subscale, but score not reported

Met DSM-IV criteria Putamen hypoactivation during reward anticipation
Olino et al., 2011 Card guessing 10 MDD v 16 HCs, aged 8–16 years

No HAM-D or BDI score provided

No anhedonia score provided

Diagnosis assessed using K-SADS Ventral striatum (VS) hypoactivation during reward anticipation
Insel et al., 2018 Card guessing 56 MDD v 56 HCs, females aged 15–20 years

No HAM-D or BDI score provided

No anhedonia score provided

Met DSM-IV criteria No increase in striatal recruitment for higher magnitude rewards
Forbes et al., 2009 Monetary reward guessing task 15 adolescents with MDD v 28 adolescent HCs

No HAM-D or BDI score provided

No anhedonia score provided

Diagnosis assessed by K-SADS-PL, confirmed by interview with child psychiatrist Striatal hypoactivation, and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) and mPFC hyperactivation during reward anticipation
Zhang et al., 2013 Meta-analysis 22 fMRI studies NA NA Caudate hypoactivation, and ACC and middle frontal gyrus hyperactivation during reward anticipation
Gorka et al., 2014 Slot machine task 9 MDD v 13 MDD with co-morbid panic disorder v 18 HCs

HAM-D 26.3 for MDD

HAM-D 28.2 for MDD with co-morbid panic disorder

No anhedonia score provided

Met DSM-IV criteria Hyperactivation of dorsal ACC during reward anticipation
Smoski et al., 2011 MID 9 MDD v 13 HCs

BDI-II 16.7

No anhedonia score provided

Met DSM-IV criteria OFC, subcallosal cingulate and paracingulate hypoactivation during reward anticipation
Chase et al., 2013 Card guessing 40 MDD v 23 bipolar disorder v 37 HCs

HAM-D 26.63

No anhedonia score provided

Met DSM-IV criteria Hyperactivation of ACC during reward anticipation
Dichter et al., 2012 MID 19 remitted MDD v 19 HCs

BDI 2.63

No anhedonia score provided

Met DSM-IV criteria for remitted MDD, no current Axis I psychopathology Hyperactivation of ACC, right midfrontal gyrus and cerebellum during reward anticipation
Ubl, Kuehner, Kirsch, Ruttorf, Flor, et al., 2015a MID 23 remitted MDD v 23 HCs

HAM-D 3.23

BDI-II 2.04

SHAPS 49.10 (HCs 49.87), no significant difference

Met DSM-IV criteria for 2 or more past MDEs, no current MDE or dysthymia Hyperactivation in frontostriatal regions during reward anticipation
Schiller et al., 2013 MID 19 remitted MDD v 19 HCs

BDI-II 2.6

No anhedonia score provided

Met DSM-IV criteria for remitted MDD, no current Axis I psychopathology Superior frontal gyrus hypoactivation during loss anticipation
Manelis et al., 2016 Card guessing 46 MDD v 36 bipolar v 42 HCs

HAM-D 26.97

No anhedonia score provided

Met DSM-IV criteria Increased frontostriatal connectivity during loss anticipation, and decreased connectivity during reward anticipation

Incentive motivation

Using the EEfRT (Treadway et al., 2009), which assesses neural responses during selection between reward options with varying reward values, probabilities, and physical effort requirements, one study found caudate and superior temporal gyrus hypoactivation in MDD participants for high reward and high probability choices, respectively (Yang et al., 2016).

In terms of more frontal areas, one study used the WoF task, during which participants select between two reward options with varying reward values and probability (Ernst et al., 2004) to assess neural responses during reward selection. Indeed, this study found OFC hyperactivation in MDD (Smoski et al., 2009), as did a study using a probabilistic reward task (Forbes et al., 2006). However, contrastingly, a later study found the opposite in adolescents with MDD, observing OFC hypoactivation during reward selection on this same task (Shad et al., 2011). Furthermore, this study also reported ACC hyperactivation during reward selection (Shad et al., 2011).

One identified study investigated connectivity during reward selection in MDD using an effort-based cost-benefit valuation task, involving pressing a button to turn off light bulbs a varying number of times to attain a reward (Park, Lee, Kim, Kim, & Koo, 2017); this study observed reduced functional connectivity between the medial OFC and the striatum during this task (Park et al., 2017).

Reward anticipation

Using the MID to assess neural responses during anticipation of rewards following cue presentation, three fMRI studies observed VS hypoactivation during reward anticipation in MDD (Arrondo et al., 2015; Hagele et al., 2015; Takamura et al., 2017). Additionally, VS hypoactivation during reward anticipation has been shown to be associated with both current and future subthreshold and clinical MDD (Stringaris et al., 2015) and was observed in a sample of unmedicated MDD participants (Ubl, Kuehner, Kirsch, Ruttorf, Diener, et al., 2015b). One identified study with MDD participants utilized simultaneous fMRI and positron emission tomography (PET) alongside the MID and observed lower VS and right dorsal striatum dopamine activity alongside lower connectivity with cortical targets (Hamilton et al., 2018). The MID involves components of various reward processes, but striatal dopamine activity is associated with reward coding during anticipation (Abler, Walter, Erk, Kammerer, Spitzer, 2006).

Other studies using the MID in MDD participants have also found patterns of striatal hypoactivation during reward anticipation, in both the NAc (Misaki, Suzuki, Savitz, Drevets, & Bodurka, 2016) and the putamen (Pizzagalli et al., 2009; Takamura et al., 2017), with one study using an unmedicated MDD sample (Pizzagalli et al., 2009). In agreement with studies using the MID, studies using the card guessing task have reported VS hypoactivation during reward anticipation in children with MDD (Olino et al., 2011) and lower striatal reactivity to rewards in adolescents (Insel, Glenn, Nock, & Somerville, 2018). Correlations between striatal hypoactivation during reward anticipation and anhedonia scores have been noted by two identified studies, in both the VS (Stringaris et al., 2015) and the NAc (Misaki et al., 2016). However, one study found no association between VS hypoactivation and anhedonia levels (Arrondo et al., 2015).

Other identified studies reported findings across frontostriatal areas. Firstly, striatal hypoactivation has been observed alongside dlPFC and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) hyperactivation during reward anticipation in adolescents with MDD on a monetary reward guessing task, correlating with lower positive affect (Forbes et al., 2009). Additionally, a meta-analysis identified a pattern of middle frontal gyrus and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) hyperactivation, and caudate hypoactivation, during reward anticipation in MDD (Zhang et al., 2013), on tasks including the MID, card guessing, and WoF (Zhang et al., 2013).

In agreement with the ACC hyperactivation noted by the above meta-analysis (Zhang et al., 2013), one identified study, using a passive slot machine task, observed dorsal ACC hyperactivation during reward anticipation in MDD (Gorka et al., 2014), and another, using the MID, found that MDD participants showed increasing ACC activation during anticipation of increasing gains, opposite to controls. However, another study using the MID found paracingulate and subcallosal cingulate hypoactivation during reward anticipation in MDD (Smoski, Rittenberg, & Dichter, 2011), and a study using the card guessing task observed ACC hypoactivation during the reward anticipation phase in MDD (Chase et al., 2013). Further to this, in a study using a sample of unmedicated MDD participants, VS hypoactivation was observed alongside hypoactivation of the OFC and ACC (Ubl, Kuehner, Kirsch, Ruttorf, Diener, et al., 2015b).

In studies using participants with remitted MDD, hyperactivation of the ACC and right midfrontal gyrus has been observed during anticipation of rewards on the card guessing task (Dichter et al., 2012), and hyperactivity across the frontostriatal network has also been observed during reward anticipation on the MID (Ubl, Kuehner, Kirsch, Ruttorf, Flor, et al., 2015a). However, during anticipation of losses rather than gains, another study found superior frontal gyrus hypoactivity in rMDD (Schiller et al., 2013).

The review identified one study investigating frontostriatal connectivity during reward anticipation in MDD using a card guessing paradigm (Manelis et al., 2016), and this study reported lower frontostriatal connectivity during reward anticipation and higher connectivity during loss anticipation in MDD (Manelis et al., 2016).

Reward learning

A total of 13 papers were identified investigating neuroimaging abnormalities in MDD during both reward learning tasks and receipt of unexpected rewards and losses. These studies used a variety of tasks, including reward learning tasks (Pavlovian, instrumental, reversal learning), the MID, reward guessing tasks, a probabilistic reward task, and the slot machine task. A summary of identified papers for reward learning is presented in Table 3.

Table 3.

Papers investigating neuroimaging in major depressive disorder (MDD) within the subtype of reward learning, in the order they appear in the results section

REWARD LEARNING
Author Reward task Sample characteristics Mean depression score Diagnostic criteria Neuroimaging abnormality
Kumar et al., 2008 Pavlovian reward learning task 15 MDD v 18 HCs

HAM-D 23.2

BDI 22.9

Snaith Hamilton Hedonia Scale (SH) 35.0 (HCs 51.7), where higher scores indicated lower anhedonia

DSM-IV diagnosis assessed by treating consultant and 1 author, symptom duration >3 months Reduced prediction error (PE) during learning in ventral striatum (VS), cingulate, midbrain, and hippocampus, correlating with illness ratings
Gradin et al., 2011 Instrumental reward learning task 15 MDD v 14 schizophrenia v 17 HCs

HAM-D 23.2

BDI 22.93

BDI anhedonia score from 4 questions 6.27 (HCs 0.71)

DSM-IV diagnosis assessed by clinical assessment and psychiatric interview by 1 author and consultant psychiatrist Reduced PE during learning in striatum, caudate, and nucleus accumbens (NAc), correlating with anhedonia
Kumar et al., 2018 Instrumental reward learning task 25 unmedicated MDD v 26 HCs

HAM-D 17.27

BDI 26.6

SHAPS 33.40 (HCs 18.6), where higher score indicated higher anhedonia

Met DSM-IV criteria Blunted PE in striatum and reduced ventral tegmental area (VTA)-striatal connectivity in response to feedback
Rothkirch et al., 2017 Instrumental reward learning task 28 unmedicated MDD v 30 HCs

HAM-D 22.5

BDI 33.0

SHAPS 5.60 (HCs 0.33), binary scoring system where higher score indicated higher anhedonia

Met DSM-IV criteria Reduced PE in medial orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), negative correlation between VS PE and anhedonia severity
Geugies et al., 2019 Pavlovian reward learning task 36 remitted MDD v 27 HCs

HAM-D 3 (median)

SHAPS 24 (HCs 17) median score, where higher scores indicated higher anhedonia

Met DSM-IV criteria for recurrent depression; stable remission defined as HAM-D score ≤7 for 8 subsequent weeks Reduced PE in ventral tegmental area (VTA), associated with higher anhedonia levels in remitted MDD.
Ubl, Kuehner, Kirsch, Ruttorf, Diener, et al., 2015b Probabilistic reward task 30 unmedicated MDD v 29 HCs

HAM-D 18.40

BDI-II 25.50

SHAPS 42.93 (HCs 49.29), where higher scores indicated lower anhedonia

Met DSM-IV criteria Reduced PE in anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and amygdala for rewards, increased PE in VS for losses
Greenberg et al., 2015 Card guessing 148 unmedicated MDD v 31 HCs

HAM-D 26.52

SHAPS 33.46 (HCs 20.52), where higher scores indicated higher anhedonia

Met DSM-IV criteria No inverse relationship between reward expectancy and PE in ventral striatum (VS)
Rutledge et al., 2017 Probabilistic reward task 32 MDD v 20 HCs

HAM-D 16.6

No anhedonia score provided

Patients receiving treatment based on primary MDD diagnosis No difference in PE signals in VS
Segarra et al., 2016 Slot machine task 24 MDD v 21 schizophrenia, v 21 HCs

BDI 32.62

SHAPS 33.42 (HCs 23.38), where higher scores indicated higher anhedonia

Met DSM-IV criteria OFC, VS, insula, and thalamus hypoactivation for unexpected rewards (interpreted as positive reward prediction error)
Steele et al., 2004 Card guessing 15 MDD with 15 HCs

HAM-D 27.3

BDI 36.9

No anhedonia score provided

Met DSM-IV criteria Increased cingulate and parahippocampus PE signalling for unexpected losses, correlating with depression scores
Robinson et al., 2012 Reversal learning 13 unmedicated MDD v 14 HCs

HAM-D 20

No anhedonia score provided

Met DSM-IV criteria Reduced PE in VS for reward feedback, but not punishment feedback
Hall et al., 2014 Reversal learning 29 MDD v 25 HCs

HAM-D 17.93 first episode of depression

HAM-D 9.4 multiple episodes of depression

No anhedonia score provided

Met DSM-IV criteria NAc and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) hypoactivity during reversal learning feedback
Liu et al., 2017 Instrumental reward learning task 24 unmedicated MDD v 21 HCs

HAM-D 24.05

SHAPS 28.5 (HCs 23.6), where higher scores indicated higher anhedonia

TEPS 72.6 (HCs 81.9)

Met DSM-IV criteria Trend towards left habenula hypoactivation in response to losses

The majority of identified studies in the reward learning subtype calculated prediction errors in response to reward feedback, and four of these studies calculated prediction errors during learning tasks, including Pavlovian, instrumental, and reversal learning. Using a Pavlovian learning task, a study found blunted prediction error signalling in the VS in MDD during reward learning, correlating with illness ratings (Kumar et al., 2008). Additionally, using instrumental learning tasks, fMRI studies have found similar blunting of prediction error signalling, this time in the caudate and NAc (Gradin et al., 2011), as well as in the striatum in unmedicated MDD (Kumar et al., 2018). Furthermore, again using an instrumental learning task, another fMRI study observed a negative correlation between VS prediction error signalling and anhedonia severity (Rothkirch, Tonn, Köhler, & Sterzer, 2017). This pattern has also been observed in remission, as a study using a Pavlovian learning task found reduced prediction errors in the ventral tegmental area (VTA), associated with higher anhedonia levels (Geugies et al., 2019).

Similar findings have been observed across other tasks; using the MID to assess neural activity as a function of prediction error signals for unexpected rewards and losses, one study found blunted reward-related prediction error signalling and potentiated loss-related prediction error signalling in the VS in unmedicated MDD participants (Ubl, Kuehner, Kirsch, Ruttorf, Diener, et al., 2015b). Furthermore, a study using a card guessing task observed that those with unmedicated MDD did not show the normal inverse association between reward expectancy and VS prediction error signals (Greenberg et al., 2015). However, a study using a probabilistic reward task, involving choosing between lotteries with varying monetary values and probabilities to assess neural responses during value-based decision-making, observed no difference in VS prediction error signals in MDD participants (Rutledge et al., 2017).

In terms of other brain regions, one study calculating prediction errors using an instrumental learning task found reduced reward prediction errors in the medial OFC during in unmedicated MDD (Rothkirch et al., 2017), alongside reduced VS prediction error signals (Rothkirch et al., 2017). Another study used a slot machine task, where participants are presented with a slot machine with two reels and, when the images on the reels in the centre of view match, the participant wins a financial reward; on 50% of trials, participants can choose the centre image of the left wheel, and in 50% of trials the computer chooses (Segarra et al., 2016). This study observed hyposensitivity of prediction errors for unexpected rewards in the frontostriatal network, including the OFC, VS, insula, and thalamus in MDD (Segarra et al., 2016). Lastly, for unexpected losses rather than gains, an early study using a card guessing paradigm found increased prediction error signalling for unexpected losses in the ACC in MDD, correlating with MDD severity (Steele, Meyer, & Ebmeier, 2004).

Three other identified studies did not calculate prediction errors, but instead investigated neural responses to feedback during learning tasks. An fMRI study using a reversal learning paradigm found VS hypoactivity in response to positive feedback in unmedicated MDD, but did not find abnormal VS activation for negative feedback (Robinson, Cools, Carlisi, Sahakian, & Drevets, 2012). Furthermore, another study using reversal learning observed hypoactivation of frontostriatal regions, including the NAc and vmPFC, during reinforcement contingency changes, correlating with lifetime disease burden (Hall, Milne, & Macqueen, 2014). Finally, one study observed a trend towards left habenula hypoactivation in response to losses during an instrumental learning task in MDD, but this did not reach significance (Liu, Valton, Zhu, & Roiser, 2017).

Discussion

The aim of this review was to better characterize the neurobiology of anhedonia in MDD; anhedonia was conceptualized as comprising deficits across three partially separable subtypes of reward processing: reward liking, reward wanting, and reward learning (Admon & Pizzagalli, 2015; Rømer Thomsen et al., 2015; Treadway & Zald, 2011), with consideration to how these map onto recent, transdiagnostic models of anhedonia (Husain & Roiser, 2018). The identified studies showed both common and dissociable neural underpinnings for each subtype. For reward liking and reward wanting (including reward anticipation and incentive motivation), studies observed striatal hypoactivation, alongside hypoactivation and hyperactivation across various frontal regions, and, for reward learning, studies observed blunted frontostriatal network sensitivity in response to unexpected rewards and positive feedback, but no abnormality in response to unexpected losses or negative feedback.

Reward liking

Reward liking represents the consummatory stage of reward processing, involving positive or negative hedonic impact (Berridge & Robinson, 2003; Husain & Roiser, 2018; Rømer Thomsen et al., 2015; Treadway & Zald, 2011). The most consistent neuroimaging abnormality associated with reward liking was striatal hypoactivation, reported to be observed in the VS/NAc (Carl et al., 2016; Connolly et al., 2015; Epstein et al., 2006; Foti et al., 2014; Jenkins et al., 2018; Keedwell et al., 2005; Moses-Kolko et al., 2011; Osuch et al., 2009; Pizzagalli et al., 2009; Redlich et al., 2015; Satterthwaite et al., 2015; Steele et al., 2007), caudate (Antonesei et al., 2018; Connolly et al., 2015; Forbes et al., 2006; Forbes et al., 2009; Pizzagalli et al., 2009; Zhang et al., 2013), putamen (Connolly et al., 2015), and right anterior insula (Sankar et al., 2019).

Six of these studies reported no anhedonia levels in the participants, but 11 identified higher anhedonia levels in the MDD participants using a variety of anhedonia measures, indicating their ability to inform our understanding of anhedonia. Additionally, striatal hypoactivation has been shown to correlate with anhedonia (Epstein et al., 2006; Keedwell et al., 2005), although one study did not find this association (Connolly et al., 2015). These three studies all used different methods of assessing anhedonia; the study finding no correlation used the average of two items of the Inventory of Depressive Symptomology–Clinician-Rated (IDS-C; Connolly et al., 2015), and the studies finding correlations used one item from the Hamilton Anxiety and Depression Scale (HAM-D; Epstein et al., 2006) and the Fawcett-Clark Pleasure Scale (FCPS; Keedwell et al., 2005). Since two studies supported a correlation, and one used the FCPS—which has more items and is validated for measuring anhedonia (Rizvi, Pizzagalli, Sproule, & Kennedy, 2016)—it seems the weight of evidence supports a correlation between striatal hypoactivation and anhedonia severity in MDD.

Indeed, striatal hypoactivation during reward liking is a robust finding, as it has also been observed across heterogeneous tasks (MID, presentation of positive stimuli, card guessing). It must be noted that the MID contrast for specific reward processing phases is difficult to isolate completely, meaning these results may also include some signals related to other phases e.g. prediction error for unexpected rewards. However, similar findings also being observed with the use of passive tasks (presentation of positive stimuli) is important, as these tasks could be argued to have less interference from other reward processing phases (such as reward selection and reward learning).

Striatal hypoactivation associated with reward liking deficits in MDD is consistent with the neurobiology of reward processing, as the striatum plays a key role in hedonic processing (Admon & Pizzagalli, 2015; Berridge and Kringelbach, 2013). Furthermore, striatal hypoactivation during reward liking may be associated with abnormal opioid signalling, as opioid signalling in the striatum mediates our core liking responses (Berridge & Kringelbach, 2008, 2013; Kelley et al., 2002), and is dysfunctional in MDD (Kennedy, Koeppe, Young, & Zubieta, 2006).

However, striatal hypoactivation could be caused by antidepressants, as the SSRI citalopram has been shown to blunt VS activation for pleasant gustatory stimuli in healthy participants (McCabe et al., 2010), and paroxetine blunts striatal activation for erotic stimuli in MDD (Abler, Gron, Hartmann, Metzger, & Walter, 2012). Concurring with these medication effects, one study found no striatal hypoactivation during reward liking when using an unmedicated MDD sample (Ubl, Kuehner, Kirsch, Ruttorf, Diener, et al., 2015b). In contrast, though, two others did observe striatal hypoactivation in unmedicated samples (Connolly et al., 2015; Pizzagalli et al., 2009). Interestingly, the study finding no evidence of striatal hypoactivation (Ubl, Kuehner, Kirsch, Ruttorf, Diener, et al., 2015b) and the one presented by Pizzagalli and colleagues—which did report striatal hypoactivation—both had a sample size of 30 unmedicated individuals with MDD, and the HAM-D average scores were similar (Pizzagalli et al., 2009; Ubl, Kuehner, Kirsch, Ruttorf, Diener, et al., 2015b). Furthermore, this striatal hypoactivation has been shown to persist into unmedicated remission (McCabe et al., 2009), together suggesting medication is unlikely to be the primary cause of striatal hypoactivation during reward liking in MDD.

In the frontal cortex, hyperactivation in the vmPFC (Keedwell et al., 2005), mPFC (Forbes et al., 2009; Kumar et al., 2015), and dlPFC (Forbes et al., 2009) has been observed during reward liking in MDD across three heterogeneous reward processing tasks (MID, card guessing, and presentation of positive and negative stimuli). Although sample sizes were limited to only 12 MDD participants (Keedwell et al., 2005; Kumar et al., 2015) and 15 MDD participants (Forbes et al., 2009), the studies do span an age range from adolescents (Forbes et al., 2009) to adults (Keedwell et al., 2005; Kumar et al., 2015). One of the three studies reported anhedonia scores, indicating significantly higher anhedonia scores in MDD than controls using the SHAPS (Kumar et al., 2015), thus providing preliminary evidence for the relationship between this activation pattern and anhedonia in MDD. Additionally, a pattern of mPFC hyperactivation and striatal hypoactivation in the reward liking subtype of anhedonia is consistent with a recent optogenetics study, which found that stimulating mPFC hyperactivation inhibits striatal responses to rewards, and thus induces an anhedonic phenotype in rats (Ferenczi et al., 2016).

However, distinct frontal regions have dissociable functions in reward processing (Der-Avakian & Markou, 2012), and studies have also found frontal hypoactivation during reward liking, in both the cingulate cortex (Satterthwaite et al., 2015) and OFC (Forbes et al., 2006; Osuch et al., 2009). Again, studies used a variety of reward processing tasks (passive listening to favourite music and active card guessing and probabilistic reward tasks) and an age range including children/adolescents aged 9–17 (Forbes et al., 2006) and adults (Osuch et al., 2009; Satterthwaite et al., 2015). Additionally, one study reported significantly higher anhedonia levels in MDD versus healthy controls using the SHAPS (Osuch et al., 2009), and the probabilistic reward task used by Forbes and colleagues allowed separation of the consummatory reward liking phase from other phases e.g. selection between reward options (Forbes et al., 2006), indicating the usefulness of these results for understanding the reward liking component of anhedonia. This pattern of OFC hypoactivation also continues into remission (Dichter et al., 2012) and is consistent with the role of this area in mediating our conscious experience of rewards (Berridge & Kringelbach, 2008; Kringelbach, 2005; Kringelbach & Berridge, 2010). Therefore, taken together, OFC hypoactivation, alongside striatal hypoactivation and mPFC hyperactivation, is likely to be a neural underpinning of reward liking deficits in MDD.

Reward wanting

In the anticipatory phase of reward processing, striatal hypoactivation is the most consistent neuroimaging abnormality observed in the VS (Arrondo et al., 2015; Hagele et al., 2015; Insel et al., 2018; Olino et al., 2011; Takamura et al., 2017; Ubl, Kuehner, Kirsch, Ruttorf, Diener, et al., 2015b), NAc (Misaki et al., 2016), caudate (Smoski et al., 2009; Zhang et al., 2013), and putamen (Pizzagalli et al., 2009; Takamura et al., 2017). This appears to be a relatively robust neural basis of reward anticipation deficits in MDD, as it is observed across tasks (WoF, MID, card guessing). Additionally, five out of the 10 studies identified higher anhedonia levels in the MDD participants versus controls (Arrondo et al., 2015; Misaki et al., 2016; Pizzagalli et al., 2009; Ubl, Kuehner, Kirsch, Ruttorf, Diener, et al., 2015b). In addition, this pattern has been shown to correlate with anhedonia (Misaki et al., 2016); however, another study using the same task and also measuring anhedonia using the SHAPS (albeit with a smaller sample size of 24 MDD participants compared with 44 in the former study) did not find this correlation (Arrondo et al., 2015).

There were only two reports of striatal abnormalities during selection between reward options and initiating approach behaviour (incentive motivation), but hypoactivation was observed in the caudate during the EEfRT in MDD in a study identifying significantly higher levels of anhedonia in MDD versus controls with the SHAPS (Yang et al., 2016) and reduced medial OFC to striatal connectivity was observed during an effort-based reward task (Park et al., 2017). Therefore, striatal hypoactivation appears to underpin reward wanting deficits across both reward anticipation and selection.

Striatal hypoactivation in reward wanting deficits may be associated with abnormal dopamine signalling in this region in MDD, as modulating dopamine transmission using amisulpride in healthy individuals is associated with striatal alterations—for example, putamen, NAc (Metzger, Wiegers, Walter, Abler, & Graf, 2015), and dopaminergic activity in the striatum is involved in coding reward expectancy during anticipation (Abler et al., 2006), and driving incentive motivation (Bardgett, Depenbrock, Downs, Points, & Green, 2009; Denk et al., 2005; Salamone, Correa, Farrar, & Mingote, 2007; Salamone, Correa, Nunes, Randall, & Pardo, 2012). Indeed, midbrain dopamine function is dysfunctional in MDD (Dailly, Chenu, Renard, & Bourin, 2004; Dunlop & Nemeroff, 2007; Nestler & Carlezon, 2006), and amisulpride enhancement of dopamine transmission in MDD normalizes striatal hypoactivation during reward processing (Admon et al., 2017). Additionally, the identified study using fMRI and PET observed reduced VS dopamine activity in MDD during the MID (Hamilton et al., 2018). The MID does involve various reward processing components (e.g., reward liking, reward anticipation), and these aspects are closely related in time during this task. However, taken together, the evidence suggests a link between dysfunctional dopamine signalling in MDD and reward wanting deficits.

In the frontal cortex during reward anticipation, mPFC and dlPFC hyperactivation was observed in one study (Forbes et al., 2009), as well as middle frontal gyrus hyperactivation in a meta-analysis (Zhang et al., 2013). Aa further study found no abnormality in mPFC activation (Smoski et al., 2009); however, this study used a sample of 16 participants compared with the 341 MDD participants identified in the meta-analysis (Zhang et al., 2013). Since a limited number of studies present varying abnormalities across frontal regions, it is difficult to form a clear picture of frontal abnormalities during reward anticipation, but there does appear to be a trend towards hyperactivation in certain frontal regions—for example, middle frontal gyrus (Zhang et al., 2013). In terms of the OFC, hypoactivation has been reported in both MDD (Smoski et al., 2011; Ubl, Kuehner, Kirsch, Ruttorf, Diener, et al., 2015b) and remitted MDD (Ubl, Kuehner, Kirsch, Ruttorf, Flor, et al., 2015a). It should be noted that one study used a sample size of only none MDD participants with a relatively low average BDI-II score of 16.7 (Smoski et al., 2011), but the evidence is strengthened by the use of unmedicated participants with significantly higher anhedonia levels than controls (Ubl, Kuehner, Kirsch, Ruttorf, Diener, et al., 2015b) and remitted participants (Ubl, Kuehner, Kirsch, Ruttorf, Flor, et al., 2015a) in the other studies detecting this abnormality. Therefore, there is evidence suggesting OFC hypoactivation is a neural underpinning of reward anticipation deficits in MDD.

In terms of incentive motivation, one study using the WoF observed OFC hyperactivation (Smoski et al., 2009), whereas another using the WoF observed hypoactivation (Shad et al., 2011), and one study using a probabilistic reward task also observed OFC hypoactivation (Forbes et al., 2006). It should be noted that the two studies observing OFC hypoactivation both used child and adolescent populations (Forbes et al., 2006; Shad et al., 2011), whereas the study observing hyperactivation used an adult population (Smoski et al., 2009). This could have had an impact on the inconsistencies observed here, since adolescents and adults show differential neural engagement patterns during reward processing (Eshel, Nelson, Blair, Pine, & Ernst, 2007; Geier et al., 2010; Silverman, Jedd, & Luciana, 2015). Overall, the findings suggest that some form of OFC abnormality (potentially hypoactivation in adolescents and hyperactivation in adults) is associated with reward selection deficits in MDD, although none of these studies provided anhedonia scores, making it difficult to ascertain how these activation patterns relate to anhedonia. However, an OFC abnormality is consistent with the neurobiology of reward processing, as the WoF task consistently activates the OFC (Smith et al., 2009), and the OFC codes the relative values of reward options during decision-making (Der-Avakian & Markou, 2012; Grabenhorst & Rolls, 2011; Hornak et al., 2004). In future, further replication is required to assess the potentially separable OFC abnormalities underpinning adolescent and adult incentive motivation deficits in MDD.

In terms of cingulate cortex abnormalities during reward anticipation, both ACC hypoactivation (Chase et al., 2013), and paracingulate hypoactivation (Smoski et al., 2011) have been observed using the card guessing task and MID, respectively, whereas ACC hyperactivation has been reported by two other studies (Gorka et al., 2014; Knutson, Bhanji, Cooney, Atlas, & Gotlib, 2008). One of these studies also used the MID (Knutson et al., 2008), but the other used a more passive slot machine task (Gorka et al., 2014). The use of this passive rather than active task (slot machine versus card guessing and MID) by Gorka and colleagues could have impacted on the inconsistency in results, as the task did not demand a selection and choice behaviour from the participant prior to anticipating the reward (Gorka et al., 2014), thus potentially better isolating the reward anticipation component. Overall, the weight of evidence appears to support ACC hyperactivation associated with reward anticipation in MDD, especially as it has also been supported by a meta-analysis (Zhang et al., 2013) and has been found to continue into remission (Dichter et al., 2012). However, it is evident that these cingulate abnormalities require further investigation to overcome inconsistency in results.

Inconsistent results have been identified for cingulate abnormalities during incentive motivation, ACC hyperactivation has been identified in MDD (Shad et al., 2011), as has dorsal ACC hypoactivation (Smoski et al., 2009), with both studies using the WoF task (Shad et al., 2011; Smoski et al., 2009). Both also used a similar number of trials—four runs of 46 trials (Smoski et al., 2009) and four runs of 39 trials (Shad et al., 2011)—and similar monetary compensation following the task, but reported different run lengths of 12 minutes (Smoski et al., 2009) and 7.8 minutes (Shad et al., 2011). Inconsistency may have arisen in part through minor methodological differences, but also because the former used an adolescent population (Shad et al., 2011); adults and adolescents have different neural engagement patterns during reward processing, as mentioned above (Geier et al., 2010; Silverman et al., 2015), and, importantly, show different patterns of dorsal ACC recruitment on this task (Eshel et al., 2007). Overall, there are ACC abnormalities associated with incentive motivation deficits in MDD, but these may be specific to adult versus adolescent populations and require further replication to be more comprehensively understood.

Reward learning

For learning from reward outcomes , there was a pattern of blunted striatal prediction error signalling for positive feedback on learning tasks (Geugies et al., 2019; Gradin et al., 2011; Kumar et al., 2018; Kumar et al., 2008), and lack of inverse relationship between unexpected rewards and prediction error signalling in the VS on a card guessing task (Greenberg et al., 2015). All studies here identified higher anhedonia scores in MDD participants than in healthy controls, using the SHAPS (Geugies et al., 2019; Greenberg et al., 2015; Kumar et al., 2018; Kumar et al., 2008) and a subscale of the BDI (Gradin et al., 2011), thus supporting the relationship between blunted striatal prediction error signalling and anhedonia. Furthermore, blunted VS prediction errors have been shown to correlate with increased anhedonia severity in unmedicated MDD participants (Rothkirch et al., 2017). Although blunted striatal prediction errors were not observed during a probabilistic reward task (Rutledge et al., 2017), patients in this study had a lower average HAM-D score than in those studies finding blunted striatal prediction errors.

Striatal hypoactivation may represent blunted dopamine signalling, because midbrain dopamine neurons projecting to the striatum code prediction errors when reward feedback is better or worse than expected (Abler et al., 2006; Bayer & Glimcher, 2005; Schultz, 1997, 1998), which is essential for reinforcement learning (Glimcher, 2011). Two other studies did not compute prediction error signals, but did report striatal hyposensitivity for unexpected positive feedback (Robinson et al., 2012; Segarra et al., 2016). Therefore, despite heterogeneity in terms of calculating striatal prediction errors, the evidence across both learning tasks and tasks delivering unexpected rewards suggests blunted striatal sensitivity to reward feedback may be a neural associate of reward learning deficits in MDD. However, this observed heterogeneity between studies may indicate that that the findings may not be demonstrating blunting in the same striatal process.

In terms of striatal responses to negative feedback during learning, some studies reported normal VS sensitivity to negative feedback on learning tasks (Hall et al., 2014; Robinson et al., 2012); conversely, Ubl and colleagues found enhanced VS prediction error signalling for unexpected losses on a probabilistic reward task (Ubl, Kuehner, Kirsch, Ruttorf, Diener, et al., 2015b). Antidepressant use may have caused this heterogeneity, as antidepressants normalize behavioural sensitivity to negative feedback (Herzallah et al., 2013), and unmedicated MDD patients tend to be hyperresponsive to negative feedback (Eshel & Roiser, 2010; Herzallah et al., 2013). One study, here, using a medicated sample, observed normal striatal sensitivity to negative feedback (Hall et al., 2014), whereas another, using an unmedicated sample, observed hyperresponsive prediction error signalling to negative feedback (Ubl, Kuehner, Kirsch, Ruttorf, Diener, et al., 2015b). However, one identified study also using unmedicated MDD participants did not report any striatal abnormalities on a reversal learning task (Robinson et al., 2012). The two identified studies using unmedicated participants may have differed because the former used an algorithm to calculate prediction errors (Ubl, Kuehner, Kirsch, Ruttorf, Diener, et al., 2015b), whereas the latter did not calculate prediction errors (Robinson et al., 2012); this may have contributed to the latter study not identifying any striatal changes to negative feedback (Robinson et al., 2012). Taken together, striatal hypersensitivity to negative feedback during learning may be a neural underpinning of only unmedicated MDD, but it could also be argued that striatal hypersensitivity to losses in MDD could be related specifically to hyperactive prediction error processing for losses during reward learning.

In terms of frontal areas, both vmPFC hypoactivation in response to negative feedback (Hall et al., 2014), and OFC hypoactivation during response to positive feedback (Rothkirch et al., 2017; Segarra et al., 2016) has been reported. This suggests a consistent pattern of frontal hypoactivity in the OFC and vmPFC regions alongside reward learning deficits in MDD, especially since two of the three studies identified higher anhedonia scores in MDD participants versus controls using the SHAPS (Rothkirch et al., 2017; Segarra et al., 2016). Interestingly, these patterns were observed across different task types, including reversal learning (Hall et al., 2014), the slot machine task (Segarra et al., 2016), and an instrumental reward learning task (Rothkirch et al., 2017), and assessing responses to positive (Rothkirch et al., 2017; Segarra et al., 2016) versus negative feedback (Hall et al., 2014). In future, further investigations of frontal abnormalities in response to positive and negative feedback during reward learning would be useful to enhance support for this conclusion.

In terms of cingulate abnormalities, a recent finding indicated blunted ACC prediction error signalling for unexpected rewards (Ubl, Kuehner, Kirsch, Ruttorf, Diener, et al., 2015b). Although no study has yet replicated this finding, we believe it is relatively robust, as they calculated prediction errors, and the result is consistent with the lack of inverse relationship between reward expectancy and striatal prediction error signalling, mentioned above (Greenberg et al., 2015). Furthermore, blunted ACC prediction error signalling underpinning reward learning deficits in MDD concurs with the role of this area in reward processing, as the ACC codes prediction error signals for unexpected rewards (Hayden, Heilbronner, Pearson, & Platt, 2011), to guide reward-related behaviour based on previous reinforcement (Der-Avakian & Markou, 2012; Kennerley, Walton, Behrens, Buckley, & Rushworth, 2006; Rushworth & Behrens, 2008).

Summary of findings

Here, a tentative integration is provided based on the most robust neuroimaging findings within each component of anhedonia, in order to elucidate findings from the literature.

Reward liking (consummatory phase)

Striatal hypoactivation was observed in the VS, NAc, caudate, putamen and right anterior insula, which could be associated with abnormal opioid signalling. Frontal hyperactivation was observed in areas including the vmPFC, mPFC, and dlPFC, as well as the frontal hypoactivation in the OFC.

Reward wanting (anticipatory phase)

Striatal hypoactivation was observed in the VS/NAc, caudate, and putamen, which could be associated with abnormal dopamine signalling. Frontal hypoactivation was observed in the OFC. There was a trend towards hyperactivation in other frontal areas, including the middle frontal gyrus, mPFC, and dlPFC, as well as a trend towards ACC hyperactivation.

Reward wanting (incentive motivation)

Striatal hypoactivation was observed, which could be associated with abnormal dopamine signalling. Abnormal activation in the OFC and ACC was also observed, but requires further replication to be comprehensively understood

Reward learning

Blunted striatal prediction error signalling was observed in the VS as well as striatal hypoactivity for unexpected rewards, which could be associated with abnormal striatal dopamine signalling. Frontal hypoactivation in the vmPFC and OFC was observed in response to feedback in reward learning, as well as blunted ACC prediction errors in response to unexpected rewards.

Common and dissociable neural underpinnings

There are common frontostriatal abnormalities underpinning deficits across the three subtypes of anhedonia in MDD. However, on closer inspection, these common neural patterns are partially dissociable across the subtypes, in line with previous reviews suggesting the three reward processing subtypes are partially dissociable in the healthy brain (Rømer Thomsen et al., 2015), and deficits across the distinguishable components of anhedonia may have partially dissociable neural bases (Admon & Pizzagalli, 2015; Husain & Roiser, 2018). Indeed, a recent review suggests individuals with anhedonia could have different, particular combinations of dissociable neural underpinnings contributing to their anhedonic phenotype (Husain & Roiser, 2018).

Striatal hypoactivation is common across the components of anhedonia in MDD, but may be associated with distinct neurotransmitter systems. For example, striatal hypoactivation for reward liking deficits may be associated with dysfunctional opioid signalling, as striatal opioids code our core liking responses (Berridge & Kringelbach, 2008, 2013; Kelley et al., 2002), and there are abnormalities in opioid signalling both in MDD (Kennedy et al., 2006), and in suicide victims (Zalsman et al., 2005). In contrast, striatal hypoactivation for reward wanting deficits (anticipation and reward selection and approach components of anhedonia) may be associated with dysfunctional dopamine signalling, because dopamine in the striatum is involved in both coding reward expectancy (Abler et al., 2006), and in driving behaviour towards the highest reward outcome (Bardgett et al., 2009; Denk et al., 2005; Salamone et al., 2007; Salamone et al., 2012). Finally, for reward learning, studies have calculated blunted prediction error signalling in the striatum (Gradin et al., 2011; Greenberg et al., 2015; Kumar et al., 2008), and this may be associated with dysfunctional midbrain dopamine signalling, as midbrain dopamine neurons projecting to the striatum code prediction errors (Abler et al., 2006; Bayer & Glimcher, 2005; Schultz, 1997, 1998), to guide reward-related behaviour (Glimcher, 2011). Additionally, there are abnormalities in dopamine signalling in MDD (Dailly et al., 2004; Dunlop & Nemeroff, 2007; Nestler & Carlezon, 2006), and these striatal abnormalities have been observed during the reward processing MID task in MDD (Hamilton et al., 2018). Therefore, striatal hypoactivation associated with deficits in reward liking, wanting and learning, potentially has differential associations with abnormalities in striatal opioid and dopamine function, making it a partially dissociable neural underpinning of the components of anhedonia.

The results of this review suggest reward liking and wanting deficits in MDD are associated with frontal hyperactivation, in areas such as the mPFC and dlPFC, whereas OFC hypoactivation is observed across all three subtypes. Different OFC regions have dissociable functions in reward processing, so OFC hypoactivation itself may be also a partially dissociable neural underpinning of the components of anhedonia. Indeed, midanterior OFC regions integrate reward valence with state for conscious hedonic experience (Berridge & Kringelbach, 2008), so midanterior dysfunction may underpin reward liking and anticipation deficits. However, medial OFC regions are involved in reward monitoring (Berridge & Kringelbach, 2008), allowing the OFC to hold and update stimulus-reinforcement representations (O’Doherty, 2004; Pizzagalli, 2014), so medial OFC dysfunction may conversely be associated with reward learning deficits.

Limitations

Reward processing tasks

A strength of the variety of tasks used in the identified studies is that they vary in the type of reward stimuli presented to assess hedonic response, how participants gain these rewards, and the type of learning feedback, so similar findings across tasks within a reward processing subtype appear robust.

However, similar reward processing tasks have been used across neuroimaging studies investigating reward liking, wanting, and learning. This overlap in task use across the subtypes of anhedonia is a limitation of the current evidence base, because it makes it unclear which partially dissociable subtype of reward processing the neural abnormalities are associated with.

Firstly, the delivery phases of the MID and card guessing task have been used to assess neural abnormalities associated with reward liking, and the anticipation phases of these tasks have also been used to assess the neural abnormalities associated with reward wanting. In these tasks, the reward delivery and anticipation phases are temporally close together, so the studies may have experienced interference between the two reward processes in their neuroimaging analysis, meaning it would be more difficult to detect the specific neural abnormalities underpinning these two reward processes. Additionally, dissociability in the neural abnormalities for reward liking and reward wanting is difficult to distinguish.

Secondly, similar reward processing tasks have been used by certain studies, but these studies have interpreted the results as relating to different reward processing subtypes. Indeed, some reward learning studies have assessed neural abnormalities during delivery of unexpected rewards, and interpreted their results in terms of neural reward learning signals in MDD (Segarra et al., 2016), whereas reward liking studies have used similar tasks and the same task phase to assess neural responses to the experience of rewards. The activation abnormalities observed in response to unexpected rewards in these tasks could either represent abnormal prediction error signals, associated with reward learning, or abnormal neural signals for hedonic response to rewards, associated with reward liking (Segarra et al., 2016). Therefore, in future, reward learning studies using learning tasks or calculating prediction errors may be useful for clarifying the separable neural underpinnings of reward learning deficits in MDD.

Sample characteristics

Another limitation is that the characteristics of the patient samples vary across all identified studies, in terms of the sample size, age of their populations, whether the MDD patients were taking medication, whether the participants had remitted or current MDD, and the average depression rating scale scores. Furthermore, although the majority of studies used the DSM-IV as their diagnostic criteria, assessed by the structured clinical interview (SCID), some studies assessed DSM-IV diagnosis using clinical assessment rather than the SCID (Gradin et al., 2011; Johnston et al., 2015; Kumar et al., 2008), one study used an ICD-10 diagnosis of MDD (Keedwell et al., 2005), and studies in children and adolescents assessed diagnosis using the K-SADS-PL (Forbes et al., 2006; Forbes et al., 2009; Morgan et al., 2016; Shad et al., 2011).

Another limitation of the sample characteristics is that studies varied as to whether they measured and reported the anhedonia scores of MDD participants, what scales were used to assess anhedonia levels, and how certain scales were scored (e.g., the SHAPS). This variability makes it difficult to ascertain the extent to which certain findings relate specifically to the experience of anhedonia in the MDD participants rather than to MDD in general, and to assess the relationship between severity of anhedonia levels in individuals and the observed neural changes. However, as indicated above, certain findings were strengthened by studies that measured higher anhedonia levels in MDD participants than controls, and, importantly, by studies additionally investigating correlations between anhedonia severity and the observed neural underpinnings.

Neuroimaging

There was also heterogeneity in terms of the fMRI analysis methodology. For example, some studies used ROIs analysis, whereas others conducted whole brain analysis and, across the studies using ROIs, heterogeneous brain regions were used. Therefore, the neural activations observed in MDD may have been made heterogeneous by the varying sample and analysis characteristics, thus obscuring the neural abnormalities detected in MDD patients when looking across studies within each subtype of anhedonia.

Furthermore, although neuroimaging is an essential tool for understanding the activation patterns associated with the three subtypes of anhedonia, a limitation of the neuroimaging evidence base is that only one identified study simultaneously assayed neurotransmitter function using PET (Hamilton et al., 2018). As mentioned above, striatal hypoactivation across the three subtypes of anhedonia may have partially dissociable associations with neurotransmitter functions. However, currently, it is difficult to discern whether striatal hypoactivation is a partially dissociable neural underpinning of the three subtypes of anhedonia, based on neurotransmitter dysfunction, because we cannot determine whether the predicted neurotransmitter dysfunction patterns are present. Therefore, further studies using simultaneous fMRI and PET would be useful to improve our understanding of the dissociability of neural abnormalities across the three subtypes of anhedonia.

Clinical implications

The results of this review, through contributing to enhancing our understanding of the neurobiology of anhedonia in MDD, have future clinical implications in terms of developing treatments better addressing anhedonia and identifying markers of treatment response. Indeed, although two identified studies did not find an association with anhedonia (Arrondo et al., 2015; Connolly et al., 2015), five others did report a relationship between the neural patterns related to reward processing deficits and anhedonia levels in MDD (Epstein et al., 2006; Keedwell et al., 2005; Misaki et al., 2016; Rothkirch et al., 2017; Stringaris et al., 2015; Young et al., 2016) and remitted MDD (Geugies et al., 2019). This tentatively suggests that the observed neurophysiological processes underpinning reward processing do have a relationship with the experience of anhedonia in MDD.

With this in mind, better understanding the neural abnormalities underpinning reward processing deficits in MDD should be able to contribute the development of pharmacotherapies targeting these neural abnormalities and, therefore, anhedonia. For example, new treatments could be developed to remediate striatal hypoactivation, such as drugs modulating dopamine function. Indeed, as mentioned previously, striatal hypoactivation in reward wanting may be associated with dopamine signalling dysfunction, and, furthermore, there are abnormalities in MDD in striatal dopamine function (Dailly et al., 2004; Nestler & Carlezon, 2006), which are associated with anhedonia (Argyropoulos & Nutt, 2013). Preliminary evidence shows that dopamine-targeting drugs such as bupropion (Tomarken, Dichter, Freid, Addington, & Shelton, 2004) and aripiprazole are effective at reducing anhedonia in MDD (Reimherr et al., 2010), and, furthermore, increasing dopamine transmission using amisulpride ameliorates reward-related striatal hypoactivation in MDD (Admon et al., 2017). Opioid function is also abnormal in MDD (Kennedy et al., 2006; Zalsman et al., 2005), and, as mentioned previously, may be associated with striatal hypoactivation in reward liking, but there are only preliminary results thus far suggesting opioid-targeting drugs could have antidepressant properties (Ehrich et al., 2015; Karp et al., 2014). In future, further clinical studies investigating these novel, targeted pharmacotherapies will be useful to assess their efficacy for improving anhedonia and remediating its neural underpinnings.

Dopamine-targeting and opioid-targeting drugs, however, are not the only options for new pharmacotherapies targeting the neural abnormalities of anhedonia. Indeed, various abnormalities in the frontostriatal reward processing network are associated with anhedonia, but are also associated with inflammatory activation (Swardfager, Rosenblat, Benlamri, & McIntyre, 2016). For example, endotoxin-induced inflammation is associated with striatal hypoactivation for rewards (Eisenberger et al., 2010), and increased C-reactive protein levels are associated with decreased VS to vmPFC connectivity, which in turn correlates with anhedonia (Felger et al., 2016). Therefore, an alternative treatment targeting the neural abnormalities associated with anhedonia could be anti-inflammatory drugs (Swardfager et al., 2016). So far, evidence relating anti-inflammatory treatments to anhedonia is sparse, but the anti-inflammatory antibiotic minocycline has been shown to attenuate lipopolysaccharide-induced inflammatory activation, and thus prevent anhedonic-like behaviour in mice (Henry et al., 2008). Therefore, future studies could build on this result to determine whether anti-inflammatory treatments could be developed to remediate both anhedonia and its neural underpinnings.

Importantly, the presence of partially dissociable neural underpinnings across the components of anhedonia could indicate the potential for variability across individuals with an anhedonic phenotype in terms of their particular combination of underlying, disrupted neurobiological mechanisms (Husain & Roiser, 2018). The treatment options mentioned above may be useful for addressing these disrupted mechanisms, but it would be useful to identify the individual combination of disrupted mechanisms in order to develop personalized interventions for anhedonia (Husain & Roiser, 2018), such as the patient’s individual neurotransmitter alterations affecting striatal regions and/or changes in activity in different frontal regions.

Finally, enhancing our neurobiological models of reward processing deficits in depression will aid in treatment development, by allowing the assessment of how new treatments impact on the frontostriatal abnormalities associated with reward processing deficits and, therefore, anhedonia. For example, behavioural activation therapy results in functional changes in frontostriatal network activity during the WoF task (Dichter et al., 2009), suggesting this therapy modality may be particularly useful for patients with anhedonia, through altering neural activity during reward processing. This type of study can also be attempted in other treatment modalities, such as dopamine-targeting and opioid-targeting drugs and anti-inflammatory drugs, to determine whether targeted treatments could be used to remediate the neural abnormalities underpinning the components of reward processing deficits in anhedonia. For example, a proof-of-mechanism trial observed that 8-week use of a κ-opioid receptor agonist increased VS activation during reward anticipation versus placebo in patients with anhedonia and a mood or anxiety disorder (Krystal et al., 2020), indicating this treatment does have specific effects on the functioning of reward circuitry in these patients. Indeed, using biomarkers of neural effects which have potential therapeutic benefit will be very useful in future development of pharmacological and psychological treatments for anhedonia as important outcome measures for proof of mechanism studies such as this (Krystal et al., 2018), as this reduces the risk of failure in later trials by showing whether therapeutic effects are due to certain neural reward circuitry alterations rather than other factors (Krystal et al., 2018).

Conclusions

In conclusion, this systematic review summarized both common and dissociable neural underpinnings for deficits across the three subtypes of anhedonia in MDD: reward liking, reward wanting, and reward learning. For reward liking and reward wanting, studies showed consistent striatal hypoactivation, alongside hypoactivation and hyperactivation across dissociable frontal regions. Conversely, for reward learning, studies showed blunted frontostriatal sensitivity in response to positive feedback, but no neural abnormalities for negative feedback. These findings suggest the importance of studying anhedonia not only as a clinical manifestation but also as a neurobiological mechanism underlying depressive disorder and other psychiatric conditions. Developing more comprehensive neurobiological models of anhedonia will have clinical implications, as, firstly, it will aid the development of novel pharmacotherapies targeting the neural abnormalities underpinning anhedonia, and, secondly, it will allow us to determine whether targeted treatments do indeed remediate address these neural abnormalities better than current antidepressant treatments.

Acknowledgments

Dr Borsini and Dr Zunszain have received research funding by the Medical Research Council (UK) (Grant MR/N029488/1) and by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King’s College London. Professor Pariante has received funding from the UK National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre at the South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King’s College London, the UK Medical Research Council (Grants MR/L014815/1, MR/J002739/1, and MR/N029488/1) and the Psychiatry Research Trust. Dr Matthew Kempton has received funding from a Medical Research Council Fellowship (Grant MR/J008915/1) and by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King’s College London.

Footnotes

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Alessandra Borsini and Amelia St John Wallis contributed equally to this work.

References

  1. Abler B, Gron G, Hartmann A, Metzger C, Walter M. Modulation of frontostriatal interaction aligns with reduced primary reward processing under serotonergic drugs. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32(4):1329–1335. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.5826-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Abler B, Walter H, Erk S, Kammerer H, Spitzer M. Prediction error as a linear function of reward probability is coded in human nucleus accumbens. NeuroImage. 2006;31:790–795. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Admon R, Kaiser RH, Dillon DG, Beltzer M, Goer F, Olson DP, et al. Dopaminergic enhancement of striatal response to reward in major depression. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2017;174(4):378–386. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.16010111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Admon R, Pizzagalli DA. Dysfunctional reward processing in depression. Current Opinion in Psychology. 2015;4:114–118. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2014.12.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
  6. Antonesei A, Murayama K, McCabe C. Reduced neural response to reward in major depression disorder using a fMRI reinforcement learning task. Biological Psychiatry. 2018;83:S162–S163. [Google Scholar]
  7. Argyropoulos SV, Nutt DJ. Anhedonia revisited: Is there a role for dopamine-targeting drugs for depression? Journal of Psychopharmacology. 2013;27:869–877. doi: 10.1177/0269881113494104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Arrondo G, Segarra N, Metastasio A, Ziauddeen H, Spencer J, Reinders NR, et al. Reduction in ventral striatal activity when anticipating a reward in depression and schizophrenia: A replicated cross-diagnostic finding. Frontiers in Psychology. 2015;6:1280. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01280. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bardgett ME, Depenbrock M, Downs N, Points M, Green L. Dopamine modulates effort-based decision making in rats. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2009;123:242–251. doi: 10.1037/a0014625. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Baskin-Sommers AR, Foti D. Abnormal reward functioning across substance use disorders and major depressive disorder: Considering reward as a transdiagnotic mechanism. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2015;98:227–239. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.01.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bayer HM, Glimcher PW. Midbrain dopamine neurons encode a quantitative reward prediction error signal. Neuron. 2005;47(1):129–141. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.05.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Berridge KC, Kringelbach ML. Affective neuroscience of pleasure: Reward in humans and animals. Psychopharmacology. 2008;199:457–480. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1099-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Berridge KC, Kringelbach ML. Neuroscience of affect: brain mechanisms of pleasure and displeasure. Current Opinions in Neurobiology. 2013;23:294–303. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2013.01.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Berridge KC, Robinson TE. Parsing reward. Trends in Neurosciences. 2003;26:507–513. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(03)00233-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Carl H, Walsh E, Eisenlohr-Moul T, Minkel J, Crowther A, Moore T, et al. Sustained anterior cingulate cortex activation during reward processing predicts response to psychotherapy in major depressive disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2016;203:204–212. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.06.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Chase HW, Nusslock R, Almeida JR, Forbes EE, LaBarbara EJ, Phillips ML. Dissociable patterns of abnormal frontal cortical activation during anticipation of an uncertain reward or loss in bipolar versus major depression. Bipolar Dorders. 2013;15:839–854. doi: 10.1111/bdi.12132. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Connolly ME, Gollan JK, Cobia D, Wang X. Reduced striatal activation in females with major depression during the processing of affective stimuli. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2015;68:384–391. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2015.04.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Dailly E, Chenu F, Renard CE, Bourin M. Dopamine, depression and antidepressants. Fundamental & Clinical Pharmacology. 2004;18:601–607. doi: 10.1111/j.1472-8206.2004.00287.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Delgado MR, Nystrom LE, Fissell C, Noll DC, Fiez JA. Tracking the hemodynamic response to reward and punishment in the striatum. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2000;84:3072–3077. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.84.6.3072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Denk F, Walton ME, Jennings KA, Sharp T, Rushworth MF, Bannerman DM. Differential involvement of serotonin and dopamine systems in cost-benefit decisions about delay or effort. Psychopharmacology. 2005;179:587–596. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-2059-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Der-Avakian A, Markou A. The neurobiology of anhedonia and other reward-related deficits. Trends in Neurosciences. 2012;35:68–77. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2011.11.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Dichter GS, Felder JN, Petty C, Bizzell J, Ernst M, Smoski MJ. The effects of psychotherapy on neural responses to rewards in major depression. Biological Psychiatry. 2009;66:886–897. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.06.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Dichter GS, Kozink RV, McClernon FJ, Smoski MJ. Remitted major depression is characterized by reward network hyperactivation during reward anticipation and hypoactivation during reward outcomes. Journal of affective disorders. 2012;136:1126–1134. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.09.048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Dunlop BW, Nemeroff CB. The role of dopamine in the pathophysiology of depression. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2007;64(3):327–337. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.3.327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Ehrich E, Turncliff R, Du Y, Leigh-Pemberton R, Fernandez E, Jones R, Fava M. Evaluation of opioid modulation in major depressive disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology: Official Publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40:1448–1455. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.330. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Eisenberger NI, Berkman ET, Inagaki TK, Rameson LT, Mashal NM, Irwin MR. Inflammation-induced anhedonia: Endotoxin reduces ventral striatum responses to reward. Biological Psychiatry. 2010;68:748–754. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.06.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Elhai JD, Contractor AA, Tamburrino M, Fine TH, Prescott MR, Shirley E, et al. The factor structure of major depression symptoms: A test of four competing models using the Patient Health Questionnaire–9. Psychiatry Research. 2012;3:169–173. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2012.05.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Engelmann JB, Berns GS, Dunlop BW. Hyperresponsivity to losses in the anterior insula during economic choice scales with depression severity. Psychological Medicine. 2017;47:2879–2891. doi: 10.1017/S0033291717001428. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Epstein J, Pan H, Kocsis JH, Yang Y, Butler T, Chusid J, et al. Lack of ventral striatal response to positive stimuli in depressed versus normal subjects. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 2006;163:1784–1790. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2006.163.10.1784. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Ernst M, Nelson EE, McClure EB, Monk CS, Munson S, Eshel N, et al. Choice selection and reward anticipation: An fMRI study. Neuropsychologia. 2004;42:1585–1597. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.05.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Eshel N, Nelson EE, Blair RJ, Pine DS, Ernst M. Neural substrates of choice selection in adults and adolescents: Development of the ventrolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortices. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:1270–1279. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.10.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Eshel N, Roiser JP. Reward and punishment processing in depression. Biological Psychiatry. 2010;68:118–124. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.01.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Felger JC, Li Z, Haroon E, Woolwine BJ, Jung MY, Hu X, Miller AH. Inflammation is associated with decreased functional connectivity within corticostriatal reward circuitry in depression. Molecular Psychiatry. 2016;21:1358–1365. doi: 10.1038/mp.2015.168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Ferenczi EA, Zalocusky KA, Liston C, Grosenick L, Warden MR, Amatya D, et al. Prefrontal cortical regulation of brainwide circuit dynamics and reward-related behavior. Science. 2016;351:aac9698. doi: 10.1126/science.aac9698. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Ferrari AJ, Charlson FJ, Norman RE, Patten SB, Freedman G, Murray CJ, et al. Burden of depressive disorders by country, sex, age, and year: Findings from the global burden of disease study 2010. PLOS Medicine. 2013;10:e1001547. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1001547. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Forbes EE, Christopher May J, Siegle GJ, Ladouceur CD, Ryan ND, et al. Reward-related decision-making in pediatric major depressive disorder: An fMRI study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 2006;47:1031–1040. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01673.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Forbes EE, Hariri AR, Martin SL, Silk JS, Moyles DL, Fisher PM, et al. Altered striatal activation predicting real-world positive affect in adolescent major depressive disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 2009;166:64–73. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07081336. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Foti D, Carlson JM, Sauder CL, Proudfit GH. Reward dysfunction in major depression: Multimodal neuroimaging evidence for refining the melancholic phenotype. NeuroImage. 2014;101:50–58. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.06.058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Geier, C.F., Terwilliger, R., Teslovich, T., Velanova, K., Luna, B. (2010). Immaturities in reward processing and its influence on inhibitory control in adolescence. Cerebral Cortex, 20(7), 1613–1629. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  40. Geugies H, Mocking RJT, Figueroa CA, Groot PFC, Marsman JC, Servaas MN, et al. Impaired reward-related learning signals in remitted unmediated patients with recurrent depression. Brain. 2019;142:2510–2522. doi: 10.1093/brain/awz167. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Glimcher PW. Understanding dopamine and reinforcement learning: The dopamine reward prediction error hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2011;108(Suppl 3):15647–15654. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1014269108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Gong L, Yin YY, He CC, Ye Q, Bai F, Yuan YG, et al. Disrupted reward circuits is associated with cognitive deficits and depression severity in major depressive disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2017;84:9–17. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2016.09.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Gorka SM, Huggins AA, Fitzgerald DA, Nelson BD, Phan KL, Shankman SA. Neural response to reward anticipation in those with depression with and without panic disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2014;164:50–56. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.04.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Grabenhorst F, Rolls ET. Value, pleasure and choice in the ventral prefrontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Science. 2011;15:56–67. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Gradin VB, Kumar P, Waiter G, Ahearn T, Stickle C, Milders M, et al. Expected value and prediction error abnormalities in depression and schizophrenia. Brain: A Journal of Neurology. 2011;134:1751–1764. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Greenberg T, Chase HW, Almeida JR, Stiffler R, Zevallos CR, Aslam HA, et al. Moderation of the relationship between reward expectancy and prediction error-related ventral striatal reactivity by anhedonia in unmedicated major depressive disorder: Findings from the EMBARC study. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 2015;172:881–891. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.14050594. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Haber SN, Knutson B. The reward circuit: Linking primate anatomy and human imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology: Official Publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:4–26. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Hagele C, Schlagenhauf F, Rapp M, Sterzer P, Beck A, Bermpohl F, et al. Dimensional psychiatry: Reward dysfunction and depressive mood across psychiatric disorders. Psychopharmacology. 2015;232:331–341. doi: 10.1007/s00213-014-3662-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Hall GB, Milne AM, Macqueen GM. An fMRI study of reward circuitry in patients with minimal or extensive history of major depression. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience. 2014;264:187–198. doi: 10.1007/s00406-013-0437-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Hamilton JP, Sacchet MD, Hjørnevik T, Chin FT, Shen B, Kämpe R, et al. Striatal dopamine deficits predict reductions in striatal functional connectivity in major depression: A concurrent 11C-raclopride positron emission tomography and functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation. Translational Psychiatry. 2018;8:264. doi: 10.1038/s41398-018-0316-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Hayden BY, Heilbronner SR, Pearson JM, Platt ML. Surprise signals in anterior cingulate cortex: Neuronal encoding of unsigned reward prediction errors driving adjustment in behavior. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:4178–4187. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4652-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Henry CJ, Huang Y, Wynne A, Hanke M, Himler J, Bailey MT, et al. Minocycline attenuates lipopolysaccharide (LPS)-induced neuroinflammation, sickness behavior, and anhedonia. Journal of Neuroinflammation. 2008;5:15–15. doi: 10.1186/1742-2094-5-15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Herzallah MM, Moustafa AA, Natsheh JY, Abdellatif SM, Taha MB, Tayem YI, et al. Learning from negative feedback in patients with major depressive disorder is attenuated by SSRI antidepressants. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience. 2013;7:67. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2013.00067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Hornak J, O’Doherty J, Bramham J, Rolls ET, Morris RG, Bullock PR, Polkey CE. Reward-related reversal learning after surgical excisions in orbito-frontal or dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2004;16:463–478. doi: 10.1162/089892904322926791. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Insel C, Glenn CR, Nock MK, Somerville LH. Aberrant striatal tracking of reward magnitude in youth with current or past-year depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2018;128(1):44–56. doi: 10.1037/abn0000389. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Husain M, Roiser JP. Neuroscience of apathy and anhedonia: A transdiagnostic approach. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2018;19:470–484. doi: 10.1038/s41583-018-0029-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Jenkins LM, Skerrett KA, DelDonno SR, Patrón VG, Meyers KK, Peltier S, et al. Individuals with more severe depression fail to sustain nucleus accumbens activity to preferred music over time: Psychiatry research. NeuroImaging. 2018;275:21–27. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2018.03.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Johnston BA, Tolomeo S, Gradin V, Christmas D, Matthews K, Steele JD. Failure of hippocampal deactivation during loss events in treatment-resistant depression. Brain: A Journal of Neurology. 2015;138:2766–2776. doi: 10.1093/brain/awv177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Karp JF, Butters MA, Begley AE, Miller MD, Lenze EJ, Blumberger DM, et al. Safety, tolerability, and clinical effect of low-dose buprenorphine for treatment-resistant depression in midlife and older adults. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2014;75:e785–e793. doi: 10.4088/JCP.13m08725. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Keedwell PA, Andrew C, Williams SC, Brammer MJ, Phillips ML. The neural correlates of anhedonia in major depressive disorder. Biological Psychiatry. 2005;58:843–853. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.05.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Kelley AE, Bakshi VP, Haber SN, Steininger TL, Will MJ, Zhang M. Opioid modulation of taste hedonics within the ventral striatum. Physiology & Behavior. 2002;76:365–377. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(02)00751-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Kennedy SE, Koeppe RA, Young EA, Zubieta J. Dysregulation of endogenous opioid emotion regulation circuitry in major depression in women. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2006;63:1199–1208. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.11.1199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Kennerley SW, Walton ME, Behrens TEJ, Buckley MJ, Rushworth MFS. Optimal decision making and the anterior cingulate cortex. Nature Neuroscience. 2006;9:940–947. doi: 10.1038/nn1724. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Keren, H., O’Callaghan, G., Vidal-Ribas, P., Buzzell, G. A., Brotman, M. A., Leibenluft, E., … Stringaris, A. (2018). Reward processsing in depression: A conceptual and meta-analytic review across fMRI and EEG studies. American Journal of Psychiatry. Advance online publication. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2018.17101124 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  65. Kessler RC, Petukhova M, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky AM, Wittchen HU. Twelve-month and lifetime prevalence and lifetime morbid risk of anxiety and mood disorders in the United States. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research. 2012;21:169–184. doi: 10.1002/mpr.1359. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Knutson, B., Westdorp, A., Kaiser, E., Hommer, D. (2000). FMRI visualization of brain activity during a monetary incentive delay task. Neuroimage,12, 20–27. [DOI] [PubMed]
  67. Knutson B, Bhanji JP, Cooney RE, Atlas LY, Gotlib IH. Neural responses to monetary incentives in major depression. Biological Psychiatry. 2008;63:686–692. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.07.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Kringelbach ML. The human orbitofrontal cortex: Linking reward to hedonic experience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2005;6:691–702. doi: 10.1038/nrn1747. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Kringelbach ML, Berridge KC. The functional neuroanatomy of pleasure and happiness. Discovery Medicine. 2010;9:579–587. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Krystal AD, Pizzagalli DA, Mathew SJ, Sanacora G, Keefe R, Song A, et al. The first implementation of the NIMH FAST-FAIL approach to psychiatric drug development. Nature Reviews Drug Discovery. 2018;18:82–84. doi: 10.1038/nrd.2018.222. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Krystal, A. D., Pizzagalli, D. A., Smoski, M., Mathew, S. J., Nurnberger, J., Jr., Lisanby, S. H., … Potter, W. Z. (2020). A randomised proof-of-mechanism trial applying the ‘fast-fail’ approach to evaluating κ- opioid antagonism as a treatment for anhedonia. Nature Medicine.10.1038/s41591-020-0806-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  72. Kumar P, Goer F, Murray L, Dillon DG, Beltzer ML, Cohen AL, et al. Impaired reward prediction error encoding and striatal-midbrain connectivity in depression. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2018;43:1581–1588. doi: 10.1038/s41386-018-0032-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Kumar P, Slavich GM, Berghorst LH, Treadway MT, Brooks NH, Dutra SJ, et al. Perceived life stress exposure modulates reward-related medial prefrontal cortex responses to acute stress in depression. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2015;180:104–111. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.03.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Kumar P, Waiter G, Ahearn T, Milders M, Reid I, Steele JD. Abnormal temporal difference reward-learning signals in major depression. Brain: A Journal of Neurology. 2008;131:2084–2093. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Liu WH, Valton V, Zhu YH, Roiser JP. Association between habenula dysfunction and motiational symptoms in unmedicated major depressive disorder. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2017;12:1520–1533. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsx074. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Manelis A, Almeida JR, Stiffler R, Lockovich JC, Aslam HA, Phillips ML. Anticipation-related brain connectivity in bipolar and unipolar depression: A graph theory approach. Brain: A Journal of Neurology. 2016;139:2554–2566. doi: 10.1093/brain/aww157. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. McCabe C. Neural signals of ‘intensity’ but not ‘wanting’ or ‘liking’ of rewards may be trait markers for depression. Journal of Psychopharmacology (Oxford, England) 2016;30:1020–1027. doi: 10.1177/0269881116653079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. McCabe C, Cowen PJ, Harmer CJ. Neural representation of reward in recovered depressed patients. Psychopharmacology. 2009;205:667–677. doi: 10.1007/s00213-009-1573-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. McCabe C, Mishor Z, Cowen PJ, Harmer CJ. Diminished neural processing of aversive and rewarding stimuli during selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor treatment. Biological Psychiatry. 2010;67:439–445. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. McMakin DL, Olino TM, Porta G, Dietz LJ, Emslie G, Clarke G, et al. Anhedonia predicts poorer recovery among youth with selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor treatment—Resistant depression. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2012;51:404–411. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.01.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Misaki M, Suzuki H, Savitz J, Drevets WC, Bodurka J. Individual variations in nucleus accumbens responses associated with major depressive disorder symptoms. Scientific Reports. 2016;6:21227. doi: 10.1038/srep21227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Metzger CD, Wiegers M, Walter M, Abler B, Graf H. Local and global resting state activity in noradrenergic and dopaminergic pathway modulated by reboxetine and amisulpride in healthy subjects. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;19:pvy080. doi: 10.1093/ijnp/pyv080. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Mitterschiffthaler MT, Kumari V, Malhi GS, Brown RG, Giampietro VP, Brammer MJ, et al. Neural response to pleasant stimuli in anhedonia: An fMRI study. NeuroReport. 2003;14:177–182. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200302100-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Morgan JK, Shaw DS, Olino TM, Musselman SC, Kurapati NT, Forbes EE. History of depression and frontostriatal connectivity during reward processing in late adolescent boys. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology. 2016;45:59–68. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2015.1030753. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Moses-Kolko EL, Fraser D, Wisner KL, James JA, Saul AT, et al. Rapid habituation of ventral striatal response to reward receipt in postpartum depression. Biological Psychiatry. 2011;70:395–399. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.02.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Nestler EJ, Carlezon WA. The mesolimbic dopamine reward circuit in depression. Biological Psychiatry. 2006;59(12):1151–1159. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.09.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. O’Callaghan G, Stringaris A. Reward processing in adolescent depression across neuroimaging modalities. Z Kinder Jugendpsychiatr Psychoth. 2019;8:1–7. doi: 10.1024/1422-4917/a000663. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. O’Doherty JP. Reward representations and reward-related learning in the human brain: Insights from neuroimaging. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2004;14:769–776. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2004.10.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Olino TM, McMakin DL, Dahl RE, Ryan ND, Silk JS, Birmaher B, et al. “I won, but I’m not getting my hopes up”: Depression moderates the relationship of outcomes and reward anticipation. Psychiatry Research. 2011;194:393–395. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.04.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Osuch EA, Bluhm RL, Williamson PC, Theberge J, Densmore M, Neufeld RWJ. Brain activation to favorite music in healthy controls and depressed patients. NeuroReport. 2009;20:1204–1208. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e32832f4da3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Park IH, Lee BC, Kim J, Kim JI, Koo M. Effort-based reinforcement processing and functional connectiivty underlying amotivation in medicated patients with depression and schizophrenia. Journal of Neuroscience. 2017;37:4370–4380. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2524-16.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Pechtel P, Dutra SJ, Goetz EL, Pizzagalli DA. Blunted reward responsiveness in remitted depression. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2013;47:1864–1869. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.08.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Pelizza L, Ferrari A. Anhedonia in schizophrenia and major depression: State or trait? Annals of General Psychiatry. 2009;8:22. doi: 10.1186/1744-859X-8-22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Pizzagalli DA. Depression, stress, and anhedonia: Toward a synthesis and integrated model. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2014;10:393–423. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185606. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Pizzagalli DA, Holmes AJ, Dillon DG, Goetz EL, Birk JL, Bogdan R, et al. Reduced caudate and nucleus accumbens response to rewards in unmedicated individuals with major depressive disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 2009;166:702–710. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08081201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Price J, Cole V, Goodwin GM. Emotional side-effects of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors: Qualitative study. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2009;195:211–217. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.108.051110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Rawal A, Collishaw S, Thapar A, Rice F. ‘The risks of playing it safe’: A prospective longitudinal study of response to reward in the adolescent offspring of depressed parents. Psychological Medicine. 2013;43:27–38. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712001158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Redlich R, Dohm K, Grotegerd D, Opel N, Zwitserlood P, Heindel W, et al. Reward processing in unipolar and bipolar depression: A functional MRI study. Neuropsychopharmacology: Official Publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40:2623–2631. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Reimherr FW, Martin ML, Eudicone JM, Marchant BK, Tran QV, Pikalov A, et al. A pooled MADRS/IDS cross-correlation analysis: Clinician and patient self-report assessment of improvement in core depressive symptoms with adjunctive aripiprazole. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2010;30:300–305. doi: 10.1097/JCP.0b013e3181db320f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Rizvi SJ, Pizzagalli DA, Sproule BA, Kennedy SH. Assessing anhedonia in depression: Potentials and pitfalls. Neuroscience & Biobehavioural Reviews. 2016;65:21–35. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.03.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Robinson OJ, Cools R, Carlisi CO, Sahakian BJ, Drevets WC. Ventral striatum response during reward and punishment reversal learning in unmedicated major depressive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2012;169:152–159. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11010137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Rogers RD, Tunbridge EM, Bhagwagar Z, Drevets WC, Sahakian BJ, Carter CS. Tryptophan depletion alters the decision-making of healthy volunteers through altered processing of reward cues. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2003;28:153–162. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Rothkirch M, Tonn J, Köhler S, Sterzer P. Neural mechanisms of reinforcement learning in unmedicated patients with major depressive disorder. Brain. 2017;140:1147–1157. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Rømer Thomsen K. Measuring anhedonia: Impaired ability to pursue, experience, and learn about reward. Frontiers in Psychology. 2015;6:1409. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01409. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Rømer Thomsen K, Whybrow PC, Kringelbach ML. Reconceptualizing anhedonia: Novel perspectives on balancing the pleasure networks in the human brain. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2015;9:49. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Rushworth MFS, Behrens TEJ. Choice, uncertainty and value in prefrontal and cingulate cortex. Nature Neuroscience. 2008;11:389–397. doi: 10.1038/nn2066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Rutledge RB, Moutoussis M, Smittenaar P, Zeidman P, Taylor T, Hrynkiewicz L, et al. Association of neural and emotional impacts of reward prediction errors with major depression. JAMA Psychiatry. 2017;74:790–797. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.1713. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Salamone JD, Correa M, Farrar A, Mingote SM. Effort-related functions of nucleus accumbens dopamine and associated forebrain circuits. Psychopharmacology. 2007;191:461–482. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0668-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Salamone JD, Correa M, Nunes EJ, Randall PA, Pardo M. The behavioral pharmacology of effort-related choice behavior: Dopamine, adenosine and beyond. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 2012;97:125–146. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2012.97-125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Sankar A, Yttredahl AA, Fourcade EW, Mickey BJ, Love TM, Langenecker SA, Hsu DT. Dissociable neural responses to monetary and social gain and loss in women with major depressive disorder. Frontiers in Behavioural Neuroscience. 2019;13:149. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00149. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Satterthwaite TD, Kable JW, Vandekar L, Katchmar N, Bassett DS, Baldassano CF, et al. Common and dissociable dysfunction of the reward system in bipolar and unipolar depression. Neuropsychopharmacology: Official Publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40:2258–2268. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.75. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Schiller CE, Minkel J, Smoski MJ, Dichter GS. Remitted major depression is characterized by reduced prefrontal cortex reactivity to reward loss. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2013;151:756–762. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2013.06.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Schrader GD. Does anhedonia correlate with depression severity in chronic depression? Comprehensive Psychiatry. 1997;38:260–263. doi: 10.1016/s0010-440x(97)90057-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Schultz W. The phasic reward signal of primate dopamine neurons. Advances in Pharmacology. 1997;42:686–690. doi: 10.1016/s1054-3589(08)60841-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Schultz W. Predictive reward signal of dopamine neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1998;80:1–27. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.80.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Segarra N, Metastasio A, Ziauddeen H, Spencer J, Reinders NR, Dudas RB, et al. Abnormal frontostriatal activity during unexpected reward receipt in depression and schizophrenia: Relationship to anhedonia. Neuropsychopharmacology: Official Publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology. 2016;41:2001–2010. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.370. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Sescousse G, Caldu X, Segura B, Dreher JC. Processing of primary and secondary rewards: A quantitative meta-analysis and review of human functional neuroimaging studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2013;37:681–696. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.02.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Shad, M. U., Bidesi, A. P., Chen, L. A., Ernst, M., & Rao, U. (2011). Neurobiology of decision making in depressed adolescents: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 50, 612–621.e612. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  119. Silverman MH, Jedd K, Luciana M. Neural networks involved in adolescent reward processing: An activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. NeuroImage. 2015;122:427–439. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.083. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Smith BW, Mitchell DGV, Hardin MG, Jazbec S, Fridberg D, Blair RJR, Ernst M. Neural substrates of reward magnitude, probability, and risk during a wheel of fortune decision-making task. NeuroImage. 2009;44:600–609. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.08.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Smoski MJ, Felder J, Bizzell J, Green SR, Ernst M, Lynch TR, Dichter GS. fMRI of alterations in reward selection, anticipation, and feedback in major depressive disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2009;118:69–78. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2009.01.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Smoski MJ, Rittenberg A, Dichter GS. Major depressive disorder is characterized by greater reward network activation to monetary than pleasant image rewards. Psychiatry Research. 2011;194:263–270. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.06.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Spijker J, Bijl RV, de Graaf R, Nolen WA. Determinants of poor 1-year outcome of DSM-III-R major depression in the general population: Results of the Netherlands Mental Health Survey and Incidence Study (NEMESIS) Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2001;103:122–130. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0447.2001.103002122.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Steele JD, Kumar P, Ebmeier KP. Blunted response to feedback information in depressive illness. Brain: A Journal of Neurology. 2007;130(Pt. 9):2367–2374. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Steele JD, Meyer M, Ebmeier KP. Neural predictive error signal correlates with depressive illness severity in a game paradigm. NeuroImage. 2004;23(1):269–280. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.04.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Stringaris A, Vidal-RIbas Belil P, Artiges E, Lemaitre H, Gollier-Briant F, Wolke S, et al. The brain’s response to reward anticipation and depression in adolescence: Dimensionality, specificity, and longitudinal predictions in a community-based sample. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 2015;172:1215–1223. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.14101298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Swardfager W, Rosenblat JD, Benlamri M, McIntyre RS. Mapping inflammation onto mood: Inflammatory mediators of anhedonia. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2016;64:148–66. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.02.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Takamura M, Okamoto Y, Okada G, Toki S, Yamamoto T, Ichikawa N, et al. Patients with major depressive disorder exhibit reduced reward size coding in the striatum. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry. 2017;79:317–323. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2017.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Tomarken AJ, Dichter GS, Freid C, Addington S, Shelton RC. Assessing the effects of bupropion SR on mood dimensions of depression. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2004;78:235–241. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(02)00306-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Treadway MT, Buckholtz JW, Schwartzman AN, Lambert WE, Zald DH. Worth the “EEfRT”? The effort expenditure for rewards task as an objective measure of motivation and anhedonia. PLOS ONE. 2009;4:e6598. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006598. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Treadway MT, Zald DH. Reconsidering anhedonia in depression: Lessons from translational neuroscience. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2011;35:537–555. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.06.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Ubl B, Kuehner C, Kirsch P, Ruttorf M, Diener C, Flor H. Altered neural reward and loss processing and prediction error signalling in depression. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2015;10:1102–1112. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu158. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Ubl B, Kuehner C, Kirsch P, Ruttorf M, Flor H, Diener C. Neural reward processing in individuals remitted from major depression. Psychological Medicine. 2015;45:3549–3558. doi: 10.1017/S0033291715001452. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Uher R, Perlis RH, Henigsberg N, Zobel A, Rietschel M, Mors O, et al. Depression symptom dimensions as predictors of antidepressant treatment outcome: Replicable evidence for interest-activity symptoms. Psychological Medicine. 2012;42:967–980. doi: 10.1017/S0033291711001905. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Vrieze E, Pizzagalli DA, Demyttenaere K, Hompes T, Sienaert P, de Boer P, et al. Reduced reward learning predicts outcome in major depressive disorder. Biological Psychiatry. 2013;73:639–645. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.10.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Whitton AE, Treadway MT, Pizzagalli DA. Reward processing dysfunction in major depression, bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Current Opinion in Psychiatry. 2015;28:7–12. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0000000000000122. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Yang XH, Huang J, Lan Y, Zhu CY, Liu XQ, Wang YF, et al. Diminished caudate and superior temporal gyrus responses to effort-based decision making in patients with first-episode major depressive disorder. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry. 2016;64:52–59. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2015.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Young CB, Chen T, Nusslock R, Keller J, Schatzberg AF, Menon V. Anhedonia and general distress show dissociable ventromedial prefrontal cortex connectivity in major depressive disorder. Translational Psychiatry. 2016;6:e810. doi: 10.1038/tp.2016.80. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Zalsman G, Molcho A, Huang Y, Dwork A, Li S, Mann JJ. Postmortem mu-opioid receptor binding in suicide victims and controls. Journal of Neural Transmission (Vienna, Austria: 1996) 2005;112:949–954. doi: 10.1007/s00702-004-0239-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Zhang WN, Chang SH, Guo LY, Zhang KL, Wang J. The neural correlates of reward-related processing in major depressive disorder: A meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2013;151:531–539. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2013.06.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Springer

RESOURCES