Abstract
During the recent COVID‐19 outbreak in Spain, we explored the individual and relational well‐being of people confined together with their partners and/or children during the first 3 weeks of state‐regulated lockdown. Adults 18 years or older (N = 407) completed an online survey that included demographic, household, and employment information along with standardized measures of psychological distress (State‐Trait Anxiety and Beck Depression) and relationship functioning—either the Dyadic Adjustment Scale if there were no children in the household or a Basic Family Relations Evaluation Questionnaire (CERFB) measuring conjugal, parental, and coparental functions. Qualitative analyses of responses to an open‐ended question about perceived changes in couple or family dynamics during lockdown revealed nine specific themes comprising two overarching categories: relational improvement and deterioration. The overall prevalence of improvement themes (61.7%) exceeded deterioration themes (41.0%), with increased (re)connection and conflict atmosphere cited most often. Quantitative analyses found elevated levels of state anxiety but not trait anxiety or depression during lockdown. Consistent with the qualitative results, couples having no children at home reported high levels of dyadic adjustment, but with children present CERFB parental functioning exceeded conjugal functioning, a pattern sometimes associated with child triangulation into adult conflicts. Although correlates of psychological distress (e.g., unemployment, perceived economic risk) were relatively stable across subgroups, predictors of relationship functioning varied substantially with household/parental status (e.g., telecommuting and employment facilitated conjugal functioning only for couples with children).
Keywords: Couple, Family, Lockdown, Quarantine, COVID‐19, Coronavirus
Resumen
Durante el reciente brote de la COVID‐19 en España, analizamos el bienestar individual y relacional de las personas confinadas con sus parejas o hijos durante las primeras tres semanas de confinamiento regulado por el estado. Un grupo de adultos mayores de 18 años (N=407) completó una encuesta con datos demográficos, información sobre la vivienda y el empleo, evaluaciones estandarizadas de distrés psicológico (ansiedad‐rasgo y ansiedad‐estado, depresión de Beck) y funcionamiento familiar (la Escala de ajuste diádico si no había niños en la vivienda o un Cuestionario básico de evaluación de las relaciones familiares (CERFB) que miden las funciones conyugales, parentales y coparentales. Los análisis cualitativos de las respuestas a una pregunta abierta acerca de los cambios percibidos en la dinámica de pareja o familiar durante el confinamiento revelaron nueve temas específicos que comprenden dos categorías dominantes: la mejora y el deterioro relacional. La prevalencia general de los temas de mejora (61.7 %) excedió los temas de deterioro (41.0 %), y se mencionó con más frecuencia una mayor (re)conexión y un ambiente de conflicto. Los análisis cuantitativos indicaron niveles elevados de ansiedad‐estado pero no de ansiedad‐rasgo ni de depresión durante el confinamiento. De acuerdo con los resultados cualitativos, las parejas que no tienen hijos en la casa informaron niveles altos de ajuste diádico, pero con los niños presentes, el funcionamiento parental del CERFB excedió el funcionamiento conyugal, un patrón asociado a veces con la triangulación de los niños en los conflictos de los adultos. Aunque las relaciones de distrés psicológico (p. ej.: desempleo, riesgo económico percibido) fueron relativamente estables entre los subgrupos, los predictores del funcionamiento relacional variaron considerablemente con la situación habitacional/parental (p. ej: el teletrabajo y el empleo facilitaron el funcionamiento conyugal solo en el caso de las parejas con niños).
摘要
最近在COVID‐19新冠肺炎爆发期间的西班牙,我们探索了在国家管控封城禁足的头三个星期里,在人们与其伴侣和/或儿童一起被关闭起来的情况下,个体的健康状况和关系的健康情况。407位18岁或以上的成年人(N=407) 完成了一项在线调查,内容包括人口、家庭和就业信息,还有标准化的心理困扰量表(状态‐特质焦虑、贝克抑郁)以及关系功能‐‐‐‐如果家庭中没有儿童,则采用二分调整量表,或者基本家庭关系评估问卷(CERFB)测量夫妻、父母和共同父母的功能。本研究对一个开放式问题的回答作了定性分析,结果揭示了9个特定的主题,两个大的类别即关系得到改善和关系走向恶化两种。使用的问题是关于伴侣关系方面或家庭互动方面发生的可觉察变化。关系有改善的主题占了主导,达测量对象的61.7%,超过了关系有所恶化的主题(占41.0%),其中最常提到的是相互之间的(重新)连接增强了 ,还有更多的冲突情况。定量分析研究发现,在管制禁闭期间,情绪状态型的焦虑水平升高,但生理特质性焦虑或生理性抑郁的水平没有升高。与定性研究结果相一致的是,没有孩子的家庭报告了高水平的二元适应,但有孩子的情况下,问卷中归纳出的为人父母的功能超过了配偶的功能,这种模式有时和儿童构成的三角关系演变成成人冲突。虽然心理压力的相关因素(如失业、感知到的经济风险)在各个亚群体中相对稳定,但关系功能的预测因素因家庭/父母现状而显著不同(例如,远程办公和就业促进的婚姻功能仅适用于有孩子的夫妇)。
According to the World Health Organization (WHO, 2020a), a pneumonia of unknown cause was first detected in Wuhan, China, in December 2019. After rapid escalation, the WHO (2020b) declared the novel coronavirus disease (COVID‐19) a global pandemic. In the context of this outbreak, governments around the world have taken measures to prevent and control the COVID‐19 infection. Starting in China, one such measure has been to place entire cities under mass quarantine.
Spain has been one of the worst affected European countries. The virus spread to all Spanish regions, with the communities of Madrid and Catalonia suffering the highest number of cases. On March 14, 2020, the Spanish government formally declared a State of Alarm over COVID‐19 and ordered a state‐regulated lockdown defined as home confinement, in which everyone, including those who were asymptomatic, had to stay confined at home for the next 2 weeks. The only exceptions were for basic activities like buying food or medicine, attending medical centers, or commuting to work (BOE, 2020). Schools, universities, and other education institutions were also on lockdown, going online in most cases. One week into the State of Alarm, as COVID‐19 taxed the country’s oversaturated healthcare system, Spain’s government tightened quarantine even further, ordering all nonessential workers to stay home for two additional weeks with the possibility of extending the emergency measures until outbreak remission.
In addition to the biomedical and epidemiological benefits of mandatory mass quarantine, it is prudent to consider possible psychological and behavioral impacts (Rubin & Wessely, 2020). In a prompt and clarifying article, Brooks, Webster, Smith, Woodland, et al. (2020) have reviewed scientific literature relevant to effects of quarantine on individual mental health, aiming to facilitate decision making in the current global crisis. Based on 24 quantitative and qualitative studies across 10 countries where quarantines involved direct or potential exposure to SARS, Ebola, H1N1 influenza pandemic, Middle East respiratory syndrome, or equine influenza, Brooks et al. (2020) conclude that quarantine often has negative psychological effects, including anxiety, depressed mood, irritability, insomnia, symptoms of post‐traumatic stress, and emotional exhaustion. Indeed, a recent large‐scale survey during the COVID‐19 pandemic in China found that well over a third of the general population experienced significant psychological distress, though it is unclear how much of this was related to lockdown (Qiu, Shen, Zhao, Wang, et al., 2020). In May of this year, a similar proportion of American adults reported clinical anxiety or depression according to the U.S. Census Bureau (Fowers & Wan, 2020).
The literature cites many factors that could have negative mental health consequences during or after quarantine including fear of infection (Bai, Lin, Lin, Chen, et al., 2004; Desclaux, Badji, Ndione, & Sow, 2017), boredom and isolation (Cava, Fay, Beanlands, McCay, & Wignall, 2005; DiGiovanni, Conley, Chiu, & Zaborski, 2004), financial insecurity (Jeong, Yim, Song, Min, et al., 2016; Mihashi, Ostubo, Yinjuan, Nagatomi, et al., 2009), limited access to basic supplies (Jeong et al., 2016), and confusing public information (Blendon, Benson, DesRoches, Raleigh & Taylor‐Clark, 2004; Jeong et al., 2016). Another factor particularly relevant to the present study is quarantine duration (Hawryluck et al., 2004; Marjanovic, Greenglass, & Coffey, 2007; Reynolds, Garay, Damond, Moran, et al., 2008). For example, Hawryluck et al. (2004) found significantly more symptoms of post‐traumatic stress among people locked down for more than 10 days compared with those in quarantine less than 10 days. In Spain, we collected data through the first 3 weeks of intensive COVID‐19 lockdown.
Although the literature emphasizes psychological effects of quarantine on individuals, there are good reasons to consider implications for couple and family relationships as well (Sprang & Silman, 2013). For example, a Global Times (2020) newspaper article reported unprecedented divorce rates in some districts of Xi’an, the capital of Northwest China’s Shaanxi province, as a direct repercussion of COVID‐19. Similarly, in qualitative studies, participants described Ebola containment measures in Liberia as creating mutual distrust, even between family members (Pellecchia, Crestani, Decroo, Van den Bergh, & Al‐Kourdi, 2015), and Toronto healthcare workers quarantined following exposure to SARS reported disruptions in parental roles and routines, “creating stress for the entire family” (Robertson, Hershenfield, Grace, & Steward, 2004, p.404).
From our interpersonal‐systems perspective, a more general reason to widen the lockdown lens is that individual and family functioning are inextricably interwoven, especially for children and adolescents but also for adults. In fact, an enormous body of research links family conflict and dysfunction to psychological distress, physical health symptoms, and a wide variety of behavior problems (e.g., Cummings, Koss & Davies, 2015; Repetti, Taylor & Seeman, 2002). Similarly, cohesive and supportive family processes not only protect individuals from negative effects of life stress (Hobfoll & Spielberger, 1992) but also generate a variety of positive outcomes (e.g., Conger & Conger, 2002; Joel Wong, Uhm & Li, 2012). Relevant to the COVID‐19 pandemic, disruptions of family functioning in the wake of widespread socioeconomic stress such as the Great Recession of 2007–2009 (Forbes & Krueger, 2019; Margerison‐Zilko et al., 2016), as well as natural disasters such as floods and earthquakes (Cao, Jiang, Li, Lo & Li, 2013; McDermott & Cobham, 2012), have had multiple negative impacts on survivors’ behavioral health just as stable and cohesive family relations protect against these.
While the quarantine literature emphasizes mainly deleterious effects, it is possible that positive and negative repercussions of lockdown could occur at the level of intimate relationships. On the one hand, home confinement can easily create conditions for conflict or estrangement as household members readjust work, school, and recreational activities; face possible contagion and financial strain; and spend virtually all of their time together in limited physical space. On the other hand, such proximity might also create opportunities for increased closeness, communal problem solving, and deeper personal relationships. The title of a May 24th New York Times article—“The Virus Has Wrecked Some Families. It Has Brought Others Closer” (Wilson, 2020)—essentially captures this mixed picture. As couples and families face the demands of a new (crisis) situation, interactional discontinuities may sometimes lead to more resilient as well as deteriorated functioning (Patterson, 2002; Walsh, 2007).
Soon after COVID‐19 disrupted Spain, we were able to organize a sizable online study of repercussions for individuals, couples, and families. Although the sample of convenience did not rigorously represent the Spanish population, we hoped to gain preliminary information about the individual and relational well‐being of people confined together with their partners and/or children during the first 3 weeks of state‐regulated lockdown. In addition to demographic, household, and COVID‐related employment information, the survey included standardized Spanish‐language measures of psychological distress and relationship functioning: The former were the State‐Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI; Buela‐Casal, Guillen‐Riquelme & Seisdedos‐Cubero, 2015; Spielberger, Gorsuch & Lushene, 2008) and the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck, Steer & Brown, 1996; Sanz & Vázquez, 2011). Relationship assessment entailed either the Dyadic Adjustment Scale (DAS; Spanier, 1976, 2017), if there were no children in the household, or a Basic Family Relations Evaluation Questionnaire (CERFB; Ibáñez et al., 2012) that measures conjugal and parental functioning. Finally, an important qualitative (mixed‐method) component of the study is that participants also responded to an open‐ended question about perceived changes in couple or family dynamics since the beginning of home confinement.
The relationship aspects of the study were of special interest to us as couple and family therapists, and including the CERFB followed naturally from our involvement in a research and development project (“Family Relational Diagnosis in Mental Health”) funded by the Spanish government (Ministerio de Economía & Industria y Competitividad, 2017). The CERFB attempts to operationalize central constructs in Linares’ (1996, 2002, 2012) basic family relations theory, where partially orthogonal conjugal functions and parenting functions converge to create optimal (or suboptimal) conditions for relational nurturing, a crucial determinant of child mental health. According to the theory, combining the bipolar conjugal (harmony–disharmony) and parenting (preservation–deterioration) dimensions yields four prognostically significant quadrants: Functionality (both dimensions high) allows for mature and balanced child development; triangulation (parenting high, conjugal low) facilitates child involvement in couple conflicts; deprivation (conjugal high, parenting low) sustains satisfactory couple relations at the expense of child nurturing; while chaotization (both dimensions low) maximizes conditions for child psychopathology (Linares, 2002). Because the CERFB scales apply only with children present, we used the DAS to assess conjugal relationship quality when parents had no children (couple only) or when children were no longer at home (empty nest).
More specific aims of the study were to (a) compare lockdown responses from the pandemic convenience sample to benchmarks for established measures of individual, couple and parental functioning; (b) describe via qualitative analysis the ways in which participants felt their couple and family relationships had improved and/or deteriorated during the first few weeks of lockdown; (c) identify demographic, household, and employment‐related correlates of pandemic relationship functioning and psychological distress with special attention to variations across couples with children at home, couples with no children, and couples with empty nests; and (d) explore possible changes in relationship functioning over time during the first 3 weeks of lockdown.
Method
Participants
A total of 407 participants recruited through Facebook and other social media platforms completed our online survey between March 24 and April 7, 2020, weeks 2 and 3 of the state‐regulated home confinement. Inclusion criteria were as follows: (a) aged 18 or higher, (b) currently living in Spain, and (c) living with one’s romantic partner and/or one’s children (including divorced parents currently living with children in a shared custody arrangement).
The participants were predominantly female (77.0%) and ranged in age from 22 to 77 years (M = 42.7; SD = 12.7). Most were also well educated (76.7% had university degrees) and resided in cities (67.2%) rather than smaller urbanized towns (27.5%) or rural areas (5.1%). The crucial variable of household–parental status governing which relationship measure(s) they would complete distributed as follows: partnered parents living with children (47.4%), partners in couples without children (37.3%), partners in couples whose children were not at home (9.6%), and divorced parents (5.7%). Children’s ages ranged from 5 months to 51 years, and 49.7% of parents had more than one child at home.
A majority of participants (69.9%) were at least partly employed at the time of the survey, with 54% telecommuting, 7.3% working entirely on site, and 8.8% doing both. While only 7.1% had experienced COVID‐related job loss, 17.0% of the sample was currently unemployed and 5.9% had retired. Occupations varied widely, with 33.4% of the sample in some way affiliated with the health professions, 11.3% working in an educational capacity, and 47.2% in general commerce or self‐employed.
Some participants (14.0%) reported that a member of their household was experiencing a health problem at the time of the survey, and 11.8% indicated they were currently receiving psychological or psychiatric treatment for problems such as anxiety, depression, substance abuse, ADHD, relationship issues, or wanting “personal growth.”
Procedure
The project received ethics approval from the Ethics Research Committee of the School of Psychology, Education, and Sports Sciences, Blanquerna, Ramon Llull University (certificate # 1920005P). Before beginning the online survey, each participant reviewed information about the study’s purpose and procedures, including assurance of confidentiality, and provided her or his informed consent.
Responses to an initial question about household and parental status distributed participants according to which standardized relationship measures they would complete later in the survey. While all 407 participants completed the STAI and BDI, those in couples with no children or all children away from home completed the DAS (n = 191); partnered parents with a child in the household did the full CERFB (n = 193); and divorced parents completed the CERFB parenting and co‐parenting scales but not the conjugal function scale (n = 23). Although a common measure of couple functioning for participants with and without children at home would have been ideal, allocating scales as we did made the online survey more time efficient. Fortunately, previous validation research has found high correlations between CERFB conjugal functioning and the DAS (e.g., r = .74 in Ibañez, 2016).
The sequence of survey questions proceeded from demographic, household, and current employment information (including perceived economic risk) to the standardized measures of psychological distress and relationship functioning. Instructions throughout the survey reminded participants to focus on the lockdown period in considering their responses.
A final, open‐ended question eliciting data for qualitative analysis asked, “What changes have you perceived in your couple or family dynamics since the beginning of home confinement (March 14th 2020)?” Accompanying this was an apology for not conducting a face‐to‐face interview and a request to answer in as much written detail as possible: “The more information you provide, the better.”
We discontinued the survey on April 8, 2020, when state‐regulated restrictions first began to ease.
Measures
State and trait anxiety
The State‐Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI; Spielberger et al., 2008), validated for use in Spain by Buela‐Casal et al. (2015), is a 40‐item self‐report instrument that assesses anxiety as both a state (20 items) and a trait (20 items). With items in a 0–3 response format, state and trait anxiety scores range from 0 to 60, and benchmark cut points for adult Spanish men and women provide a basis for classifying subscale scores from “very high” to “very low” (Buela‐Casal et al., 2015). Internal consistency coefficients for the current lockdown sample were α = .93 and .84 for state and trait anxiety, respectively.
Depression
The Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck et al., 1996), validated in Spanish by Sanz and Vázquez (2011), consists of 21 self‐report items measuring the presence and severity of depression. As with the STAI, benchmark cut points for the Spanish population permit classifying BDI scores as reflecting minimal, mild, moderate, or severe depression (Sanz & Vázquez, 2011). Reliability for the lockdown sample was α = .85.
Dyadic adjustment
Participants with no children at home completed the Spanish version of the Dyadic Adjustment Scale (DAS; Spanier, 1976, 2017), a 32‐item questionnaire measuring general couple relationship quality. The DAS also has consensus, satisfaction, affectional expression, and cohesion subscales, but because these were highly intercorrelated we used only the DAS total score (α = .94) in the main analyses. Although clinical cut points for the DAS are not available, descriptive statistics from the Spanish validation studies (Martín‐Lanas et al., 2017; Roca et al., 2020) provide an approximate benchmark for evaluating couple relationship quality in the lockdown sample.
Conjugal, parental, and coparental functioning
Participants with children at home completed the Basic Family Relations Evaluation Questionnaire (CERFB; Ibáñez et al., 2012; Vilaregut et al., 2019), a 25‐item parent‐report instrument inspired by Linares’ (2002, 2012) theoretical ideas about relational nurturing. The original CERFB includes a 14‐item parenting function scale (α = .92), measuring the quality of parent–child relations (e.g., “I feel that my children return my affection”), and an 11‐item conjugal function scale (α = .91) reflecting the quality of how parents relate to each other as a couple (e.g., “My partner knows how to treat me”). Responses are on 5‐point Likert scales ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (always). To further assess collaboration between the two parents with regard to childrearing, we included 16 additional items from a preliminary co‐parenting scale (α = .85) currently undergoing validation (e.g., “We make a good team as parents”; Mollà‐Cusí et al., 2019).
Clinical norms for CERFB scales are not yet available but, as with the Spanish DAS, descriptive statistics from validation studies provide tentative benchmarks for evaluating levels of parental and conjugal functioning in the lockdown sample (Campreciós, 2015; Ibáñez et al., 2012; Roca et al., 2020; Vilaregut et al., 2019). Interpretation may be complicated, however, because identifying Linares’ patterns of functional and dysfunctional parenting requires taking both dimensions (and ideally the views of both parents) into account.
Data Analysis
Given the exploratory nature of the study, we approached the research aims with the basic premise that combining qualitative and quantitative forms of evidence provides a better understanding than either method does by itself (Creswell & Plano Clark, 2011). Indeed, both qualitative and quantitative data figure prominently in the results—and despite homage to validated quantitative measurement methods (above), our most direct evidence of actual lockdown “effects” on couple and family functioning came from participants’ qualitative (written) descriptions of what had changed. In the spirit of mixed‐method research, we then used dichotomous variables representing the presence or absence of specific qualitative themes in participants’ responses to explore quantitative associations with other study variables.
After dropping 23 written responses reporting no couple/family change (e.g., “Everything continues as usual”) and 53 blank responses, we used Braun and Clarke’s (2006) method of thematic analysis, assisted by ATLAS.ti software for Mac (v. 8), to code 329 descriptions of change. The total qualitative data set consisted of 13,226 words, with individual responses ranging from 2 words (e.g., “Closer now”) to 353 words. The thematic analysis involved identifying interesting data features, or codes; clustering codes and searching for potential themes; and, finally, naming and defining the themes. To facilitate accuracy and trustworthiness, two authors served as cocoders in an ongoing consensual review process, and the full team reviewed emerging results to reach on the final thematic configuration.
On the quantitative side (using SPSS Statistics, v. 20), preliminary analyses justified creating two composite variables that would simplify later examination of psychological distress and couple relationship functioning: composite distress was a z‐score combination of STAI state, STAI trait, and BDI scores, which intercorrelated highly with all r s > .66. Similarly, the composite measure of couple functioning combined z‐transformations of DAS total scores and CERFB conjugal function scores, which were not available for the same participants here but had correlated highly in previous research. As dependent variables, the two composites helped clarify multivariate and moderated influences on central study constructs.
We approached the main study aims by first examining descriptive statistics for psychological distress (STAI and BDI) and relationship functioning (DAS and CERFB) in the lockdown sample with an eye toward areas of possible discrepancy and/or alignment with benchmarks for the broader population. In light of sampling limitations both here and in the standardization studies, however, such comparisons can only be approximate, with conclusions about lockdown effects necessarily tentative. Next, having defined qualitative change themes (as described above) and coded their presence/absence across participant responses, simple tabulations and cross‐tabulations illuminated the prevalence of various improvement and deterioration themes in the full sample and across the four main participant groupings: partnered parents with children at home, partners in couples with no children, parents with all children away from home, and divorced parents.
Finally, we employed a variety of univariate, multivariate, and moderation analyses to identify demographic, household, and employment‐related correlates of pandemic relationship functioning and psychological distress, again with attention to variations across household/parental subgroups. In addition to CERFB scores, DAS scores, and the two composites, these correlational explorations included the qualitative change themes and a rough approximation of the Linares CERFB parenting styles (functionality, triangulation, etc.). Also of interest were possible changes in relationship functioning over time as the lockdown progressed, including system–symptom links between relationship quality and individual distress.
Results
Levels of Functioning During Lockdown
Compared to STAI and BDI norms for the general Spanish population, participants in lockdown reported relatively high levels of situational (state) anxiety but unremarkable trait anxiety or depression. State anxiety means in the lockdown sample were clearly higher than standardization scores for both women (M = 23.1, SD = 10.4 vs. M = 18.3, SD = 11.3) and men (M = 20.5, SD = 10.0 vs. M = 16.3, SD = 10.4). For STAI trait anxiety and BDI depression, however, means were in the opposite direction. Based on clinical cut points, 49.2% of the lockdown sample reported high or very high levels of state anxiety while the comparable figure for trait anxiety was 22.7%. Similarly, BDI depression reached at least mild clinical levels for 13.4% of lockdown participants but only 4.6% were in the moderate or severe range. When asked about their perception of economic risk, just over half of the respondents were at least moderately concerned with economic risk during the pandemic (53.5%) but less than a quarter were very concerned (15.7%) or extremely concerned (6.6%). In general, the lockdown experience appeared to generate moderate to high levels of situational anxiety and uncertainty but not much chronic distress among the adults who completed the online survey.
In the relationship realm, lockdown participants who completed the DAS (those with no children in the household) reported average levels of dyadic adjustment at least comparable to the Spanish standardization sample (M = 126.1, SD = 16.3 vs. M = 114.9, SD = 17.5) and far above the clinical range (M = 86.5, SD = 19.2). With children in the household, however, the picture is somewhat different: Whereas CERFB parental functioning in lockdown was roughly comparable to the standardization group (M = 45.2, SD = 5.0 vs. M = 43.3, SD = 5.9), lockdown scores for conjugal functioning were decidedly worse (M = 42.8, SD = 5.1 vs. M = 56.0, SD = 8.4). Results for coparental functioning have no basis for standardized comparison because this scale is currently undergoing validation.
From the perspective of Linares’ (2002) relational nurturing model, the discrepancy between parental and conjugal functions with children in lockdown is not ideal. Although CERFB norms provide no firm basis for locating parenting styles in the Linares’ quadrants, a rough comparison of lockdown means with standardization data suggests that at least half of the reports in our sample would be most consistent with the triangulation style, where parents tend to maintain harmony by involving children in their conflicts. Such classification is approximate at best, however, especially without reports from both parents or confirmation from a child.
To summarize, with no children in the household, the quality of couple relationships during the COVID‐19 lockdown appeared no worse and possibly better than would have been the case without lockdown. With children present, however, our data raise the possibility that preservation of family harmony may have sometimes occurred at the expense of relational nurturing.
Perceptions of Family Change During Lockdown
Two overarching thematic categories—perceived improvement and perceived deterioration—emerged from our qualitative analysis of participants’ free‐form descriptions of how family and couple dynamics had changed during the lockdown. Table 1 describes the specific themes that comprise these categories with definitions, component codes, and representative quotations for each. Improvement themes include family (re)connection and acknowledgement, better communication, emotional expressiveness, teamwork spirit, and balance between individual and shared needs; the deterioration themes were experiencing loneliness and couple/family distance, conflict atmosphere, negative expectations, and imbalance between individual and shared needs.
Table 1.
Themes | Codes | Representative Quotations |
---|---|---|
A. Perceived improvement | ||
Family (re)connection and acknowledgement through: | Enjoying time together | We share more during our meals, which allows us to enjoy some good moments. [P85] |
Experiencing hope and identifying opportunities for change | Due to the lockdown, maybe because we have more time to think, the relationship dynamics are more obvious to me, and gives an opportunity to talk about it and change something if necessary. [P210] | |
Slowing down and pausing | The positive side of all of this is that I am enjoying my family, since we didn’t have time for that with the ‘life pace’ that we had. [P286] | |
Appreciating what we have/are | I now see how lucky we are to be able to be free and stay in touch with each other. We are not so aware when we are in our regular daily activities [P122] | |
Having more couple’s intimacy | We have intimate moments for the couple (we’ve improved the frequency of when we have sex) [P246] | |
Improving extended family relations | We have family video calls almost daily. Before the lockdown we could spend an entire week without talking with any of my brothers (who don’t live in the same city as me). I am excited to spend more time with them overall. [P242] | |
Better communication dynamics by: | Having more conversations | When we sit down to eat we have long talks with our children, without rushing to finish eating and leave the table. I think that our communication has improved very much. [P110] |
Communicating needs | I tell my partner what bothers me more often and that I’d like to make more of an effort to change things. [P9] | |
Improving conflict resolution | Since we see each other more than usual, we communicate better and the conflicts are resolved more easily. [P26] | |
Emotional expressiveness with: | Physical gestures of affection | The entire family is more united than ever, we are constantly hugging and kissing each other! [P261] |
Openness and feeling‐sharing | We are getting to know parts of ourselves we used to ignore. The members of our family that were open to express their feelings are still that way, while those who weren’t seem to be opening up more even though it may be difficult for them. [P389] | |
Teamwork spirit driven by: | Feeling mutual support | Anytime there is something that worries us we find support in each other to overcome those moments of higher anxiety together. [P4] |
Growing empathy and tolerance | Patience, tolerance, and respect are all values that are arising more these days. [P121] | |
Creativity and adaptability | There is a better ability to adapt. [P271] | |
Task distribution | We distribute the housework. We even decided to include the kids in it, which we didn’t even think of before, or did so very little. [P256] | |
Shared goals | Since day one we decided that we were going to take the opportunity of this lockdown to improve on our family relationships. [P160] | |
Balance between individual and shared needs by: | Respecting each other’s space | At home we’ve found ways for each of us to have our own space. [P211] |
Respecting individual and shared time | Overall, we have more quality time together and that allows us to spend more time on our personal interests without it meaning that we are mistreating the relationship. [P179] | |
B. Perceived deterioration | ||
Experiencing loneliness and couple/family distance through: | Isolation between family members | I’ve noticed more communication and emotional distance. I notice that we look for things to do to distract and evade ourselves a lot more than we used to. We are together, but in some way we are isolated, not only from the outside world. [P173] |
Less couple time | Little time to communicate with my partner without the kids present since we use that time to look at the news. [P166] | |
Missing the contact with the extended family | I miss the conversations, the kisses, the hugs, and get‐togethers with our kids and grandkids!!!!! [P141] | |
Conflict atmosphere due to: | Increased tension | My partner and I are more short‐tempered. I am less patient with things I used to be very patient with. [P34] |
More arguments | It’s harder for us to stay calm, and something that used to be meaningless now turns into a big problem and an argument. [P125] | |
Resurfacing of old issues | Living 24/7 with my partner has brought old issues back. [P218] | |
Negative expectations because of: | Fear of the disease | Uncertainty of losing a loved one or a very loved one. Cleaning intensity, like trying to prevent the coronavirus from entering the house. Security measures inside and outside the house. Fear when people get too close. [P195] |
Economic uncertainty | My husband had an “ERTE” (Temporary Workforce Reduction Plan) and this will directly affect our economic security, and I think this has affected me pretty much. [P287] | |
Information overload | We now “depend” on social media a lot more so that we can stay informed about the situation at all times, and constantly seeing the news (which are always bad) creates even more tension. [P109] | |
Unbalanced individual and shared needs through: | Uneven task distribution and no family collaboration | I feel like I “can’t manage to do” a lot of what I’d like to do. But housework and childrearing of a few‐months‐old child are more demanding. I also noticed gender inequality. My partner works the 8 hours that he usually works on‐site. I have to adjust to his schedule and juggle to telecommute. [P218] |
Overwhelming childrearing | There’s more tension in the air. We have 3 little kids, all of them are very demanding, and the feeling is of being completely overwhelmed. [P86] | |
No personal space | We need to spend time apart and it is difficult now. [P344] |
P = Participant.
Original responses were translated from Spanish or Catalan.
Table 2 shows the prevalence of these themes in the subsample of 329 respondents who indicated that some degree of lockdown‐related family change had occurred. (Note that a given response could include more than one theme; in fact, 24 cited both improvement and deterioration.) Interestingly, the overall prevalence of improvement themes (61.7%) exceeded the prevalence of deterioration themes (41.0%), with increased connection/cohesion (44.7%) and conflict (21.9%) cited most often. Relative to the DAS and CERFB data above, this appears more consistent with the dyadic adjustment of couples having no children at home than with the CERFB parenting picture.
Table 2.
Themes | All Participants (N = 329) | Child At Home (N = 167) | Couple Only (N = 113) | Children Away (N = 30 | Divorced Parent (N = 19) | Chi‐square Significance |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Any improvement | 61.7% | 64.1% | 68.1% | 40.0% | 36.8% | .004 |
Family (re)connection | 44.7 | 46.7 | 49.6 | 30.0 | 21.1 | .041 |
Teamwork spirit | 20.4 | 21.0 | 22.1 | 13.3 | 15.8 | ns |
Better communication | 19.1 | 17.4 | 25.7 | 6.7 | 15.8 | .086 |
More expressiveness | 8.8 | 9.0 | 9.7 | 10.0 | 0.0 | ns |
Balanced needs | 5.2 | 6.6 | 4.4 | 3.3 | 0.0 | ns |
Any deterioration | 41.0% | 42.5% | 33.6% | 50.0% | 57.9% | ns |
Conflict atmosphere | 21.9 | 25.1 | 19.5 | 3.3 | 36.8 | .019 |
Couple/family distance | 12.2 | 5.4 | 11.5 | 46.7 | 21.1 | .000 |
Unbalanced needs | 9.7 | 16.2 | 4.4 | 0.0 | 0.0 | .001 |
Negative expectations | 7.9 | 7.8 | 8.0 | 6.7 | 10.5 | ns |
Table 2 also indicates (via chi‐square comparisons) that some themes distributed less evenly than others across subgroups defined by household composition and parental status. For example, the theme of family (re)connection was more prevalent for parents with no children and those with children at home than for parents who were divorced or had children not at home. The most striking differences, however, involved deterioration themes such as couple/family distance (most common with children away and negligible with children at home), conflict atmosphere (less frequent with emancipated children), and unbalanced needs (most likely with children with children at home).
Overall, participants tended to use more words when describing deterioration themes than improvement themes, with unbalanced needs (r = .24, p < .01), negative expectations (r = .21, p < .001), and conflict atmosphere (r = .14, p = .01) entailing the thickest descriptions. On the improvement side, longer responses were associated with balanced needs (r = .26, p < .001) and teamwork spirit (r = .16, p = .003).
Correlates of Individual, Couple, and Parental Functioning
As noted above, we created two composite‐dependent variables representing psychological distress and couple functioning to facilitate identifying correlates of individual and relational well‐being during lockdown. The distress composite is available for the full sample while the latter includes all participants except 19 who had divorced. The couple composite is useful because the survey entailed different (though highly correlated) measures of couple functioning depending on whether or not the respondent had a child at home. Thus, although it was not possible to compare these subgroups directly, the z‐score composite permits examining differential (moderated) prediction of conjugal functioning.
Table 3 provides an overview of predictor variables associated with various measures of individual, couple, and parental functioning. Point biserial correlations in the first few rows, where dichotomous dummy variables represent household/parental subgroups, reveal few group differences in composite individual or couple functioning apart from the small group of divorced parents and parents with a preschool‐age child reporting more psychological distress.
Table 3.
Variables | % or M (SD) | Full Sample z‐score Composites | Child Not At Home (DAS) | Child At Home (CERFB) | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Psychological Distress a (N = 407) | Couple Functioning b (N = 384) | Dyadic Adjustment c (N = 191) | Conjugal Function d (N = 193) | Parental Function d (N = 216) | Coparental Function d (N = 216) | ||
A. Demographics and lockdown duration | |||||||
Parental/household status | |||||||
Child at home (0/1) | .48 | .01 | .00 | ||||
No children (0/1) | .37 | −.04 | .03 | ||||
Empty nest (0/1) | .10 | −.03 | −.04 | ||||
Divorced parent (0/1) | .06 | .11* | |||||
Preschool child (0/1) | .13 | .14** | .09† | .14* | .08 | .03 | |
Employment situation | |||||||
Employed (0/1) | .63 | −.10† | .08 | −.02 | .18* | .06 | .08 |
Telecommute (0/1) | .54 | −.09† | .09† | −.03 | .22** | .12† | .09 |
COVID job loss (0/1) | .07 | .12* | −.12* | −.09 | −.16* | −.07 | −.19** |
Unemployed (0/1) | .17 | .11* | .06 | −.05 | −.20** | −.09 | −.15* |
Perceived economic risk (1–5) | 2.7 (1.1) | .18*** | .01 | .01 | .00 | −.12† | −.11 |
Age | 42.7 (12.7) | −.04 | −.11* | −.08 | −.17* | −.11 | −.14* |
Female (0/1) | .77 | .16** | −.10† | −.15* | −.04 | −.13† | −.17* |
Urban residence (0/1) | .67 | .00 | .05 | −.02 | .12† | .12† | .07 |
University graduate (0/1) | .77 | −.08† | −.01 | −.06 | .05 | .24*** | .12† |
Health professional (0/1) | .33 | −.18*** | −.02 | −.08 | .11 | .11† | .05 |
Educator (0/1) | .11 | −.12 | −.05 | .04 | .04 | .15* | .07 |
Commercial, self−employed (0/1) | .47 | .20*** | .07 | .04 | .10 | −.15* | −.05 |
Household health problem (0/1) | .14 | .15** | .01 | .05 | −.04 | −.15* | −.10 |
Psychological treatment (0/1) | .12 | .24*** | −.03 | −.02 | −.04 | −.23** | −.15* |
Psychological distress a | — | 1.00 | −.25*** | −.45*** | −.06 | −.30*** | −.31*** |
Lockdown days | 15.9 (4.2) | .04 | −.08 | .09 | .25*** | −.03 | .07 |
Variables | % | Full Sample z‐score Composites | Child Not At Home (DAS) | Child At Home (CERFB) | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Psychological Distress a (N = 329) | Couple Functioning b (N = 310) | Dyadic Adjustment c (N = 143) | Conjugal Function d (N = 167) | Parental Function d (N = 186) | Coparental Function d (N = 186) | ||
B. Perceived relationship changes during lockdown | |||||||
Any improvement (0/1) | .62 | −.31*** | .21*** | .39*** | .04 | .11 | .24** |
Family (re)connection | .45 | −.25*** | .14** | .28*** | .01 | .06 | .17* |
Teamwork spirit | .20 | −.03 | .08 | .22** | −.05 | .09 | .15* |
Better communication | .19 | −.13* | .08 | .16† | .00 | .02 | .02 |
More expressiveness | .09 | −.06 | .02 | .08 | .04 | .01 | .11 |
Balanced needs | .05 | −.06 | .01 | .11 | −.07 | −.02 | .10 |
Any deterioration (0/1) | .41 | .28*** | −.16** | −.38*** | .05 | −.15* | −.19** |
Conflict atmosphere | .22 | .23*** | −.13* | −.35*** | .05 | −.12 | −.18* |
Couple/family distance | .12 | .20*** | −.24*** | −.37*** | −.07 | −.02 | −.18* |
Unbalanced needs | .10 | .09* | −.13* | −.28** | −.07 | −.02 | −.05 |
Negative expectations | .08 | .07 | −.01 | .04 | −.04 | −.09 | −.01 |
Table entries are Pearson’s or point‐biserial correlation coefficients reflecting associations between predictor variables and measures of individual, couple, and parental functioning. Left‐most column includes prevalence proportions for dichotomous predictors and qualitative themes.
Two‐tailed significance levels: ***p < .001, **p < .01, *p < .05, † p < .10.
Z‐score composite of STAI state and trait anxiety measures and BDI depression available for all participants.
Z‐score composite of total dyadic adjustment (DAS) and CERFB marital functioning scores, excluding divorced parents.
Dyadic Adjustment Scale (DAS) total score.
Family relations (CERFB) scores for parents with at least on child at home.
A strikingly different picture of couple functioning emerges in relation to pandemic‐related employment, where stronger associations appear when respondents have children at home (CERFB conjugal function) compared to when they do not (DAS total score). Of particular interest is how couple adjustment relates to telecommuting and (un)employment. General linear model (GLM) moderation analyses examining these association across three groups of couples (no children, child at home, and empty nest) found significant Group × Telecommute (F = 5.23, p = .006) and Group × Employed (F = 4.44, p = .012) interaction terms, with group means suggesting that telecommuting and employment were positively related to couple functioning when respondents were parents (regardless of whether children were at home or emancipated) but in the opposite direction when they were not.
Table 3 also highlights correlates of individual distress and relationship functioning that did not vary appreciably (interact) with household/parental status: Psychological distress was generally higher among women, recipients of psychological treatment, unemployed respondents, those in business professions or perceiving economic risk, and those with a health problem at home—but lower among health professionals. Among the CERFB family relationship measures, parental functioning showed significant correlations with some of the same predictors and was positively related to education; and if anything, marital and coparental functioning was better among younger couples.
The last two rows in the top panel of Table 3 show potentially important associations involving lockdown duration, couple functioning, and psychological distress. First, marital functioning for couples with children at home systematically improved with days in lockdown, which was not the case for parenting functions, psychological distress, or for couples without children at home. This cross‐sectional temporal trend remained after statistically controlling for parallel changes over days in other demographics such as urban location, employment status, or having a preschool child at home. Note also that effective marital functioning with a child at home correlated positively with telecommuting and having a job but not with psychological distress. In this way, couple functioning in the context of active parenting during lockdown was unique among the relationship measures.
The strong associations between relationship quality and individual distress in Table 3 are consistent with a large couple and family relations literature, but we do not know if they are in any way unique to quarantine. An indirect way to approach this is to ask whether the linkage between relationship quality and individual functioning systematically changed over the course of quarantine, as would be indicated by statistical interactions involving lockdown days. GLM analyses along these lines, with psychological distress as the dependent variable, did in fact show suggestive relationship–quality × lockdown–duration interaction terms for DAS total dyadic adjustment (F = 5.37, p = .022) and CERFB coparenting (F = 4.50, p = .035) in the direction of system–symptom linkages strengthening as the lockdown progressed. The lockdown study period was relatively short, however, so this intriguing evidence is tentative at best.
Finally, because the CERFB results in Table 3 do not speak directly to the Linares model of relational nurturing that guided development of the instrument, we were interested in how parenting styles representing functionality, triangulation, deprivation, and chaotization might have operated during the lockdown. This seemed important because the apparent parental–conjugal discrepancy in our sample suggests that some degree of triangulation may have been in play. Although the CERFB standardization data offer no cut points for defining Linares quadrants with any precision, we attempted to approximate the quadrants in a relative manner by splitting the conjugal and parental distributions at their medians. Interestingly, participants in the high‐parenting/low‐conjugal (triangulation) quadrant reported less distress and fewer deterioration themes than other participants (Table S1). Triangulation represented in this manner was also more common among university graduates and health professionals.
Patterns of Relational Improvement and Deterioration
Because the most direct evidence of lockdown effects on family relationships comes from participants’ free‐form reports of improvement and deterioration, we were interested in which qualitative themes were most and least likely to come from which participants. The bottom panel of Table 3 shows strong associations between perceptions of relational improvement/deterioration and some but not all of the standardized individual, couple, and parental functioning measures. In particular, the qualitative reports aligned most solidly with psychological distress and with the dyadic adjustment of partners with no children in the household. With children at home, there were no significant correlations for CERFB conjugal functioning and only marginal ones for parenting and coparenting. Of the specific qualitative themes, family (re)connection dominated criterion connections on the improvement side while conflict and distance did so on the deterioration side.
Significant phi coefficients (Table S2) indicate that respondents reporting improvement were more often employed and had no emancipated children or preschoolers at home. Deterioration themes, on the other hand, were associated with receiving psychological treatment, the presence of preschoolers, and coping with health problems in the household. As for markers of specific qualitative theme categories, improvements in (re)connection were more prevalent among health professionals and respondents with no preschoolers; education and employment predicted improved communication; expressiveness themes correlated with telecommuting and low perceived economic risk; city dwellers were high on teamwork; younger participants described more conflict and fewer distance themes; females reported more conflict; health professionals described fewer negative expectations; an empty nest predicted more distance and less conflict; health problems in the household portended distance themes; and having children at home meant less distance and more unbalanced family member needs as noted above.
Discussion
This mixed‐method exploration of the recent COVID‐19 lockdown in Spain adds depth and complexity to the quarantine literature by highlighting repercussions for relationships as well as individuals in the direction of improved as well as compromised functioning. Although survey respondents experienced moderate to high levels of situational anxiety during the first 3 weeks of state‐regulated lockdown, they also reported high dyadic (couple) adjustment relative to a norm group and cited more instances in which their couple and family dynamics had improved rather than deteriorated. This contrasts with previous studies that have emphasized predominantly negative psychological effects of mass quarantine on individuals (Brooks et al., 2020), and occasionally also on couple and family relations.
The results also suggest that quarantine ramifications for couples and families are more complex than those for individual children and adults. One aspect of this complexity is that positive and negative forms of change appear more likely at the level of close relationships (with opportunities for collective coping, interpersonal need satisfaction, etc.) than at the level of individual adjustment. Positive individual change (e.g., post‐traumatic growth) is certainly possible, of course, but the quarantine literature has not emphasized this. Another indication of greater complexity is that measures of couple and parental functioning varied with household composition and parental status in ways that measures of individual well‐being (psychological distress) did not. For example, conjugal relations during lockdown appeared more harmonious when there were no children in the household, and moderation analyses indicated that COVID‐related employment variables (e.g., telecommuting) predicted successful couple functioning in different ways depending on parental status.
In general, the relationship implications of lockdown were clearer for participants with no children at home than for those actively engaged in parenting. For the former, dyadic adjustment scores were higher than those from a benchmark comparison group and correlated highly with both the absence of psychological distress and the presence of improvement themes in participants’ descriptions of how relationship dynamics had changed. For active parents, on the other hand, benchmark CERFB comparisons were more ambiguous, with parenting quality tending to exceed conjugal quality, and qualitative improvement/deterioration themes did not map so clearly onto relationship functioning.
Although tentative, the apparent prevalence in our sample of a parenting style Linares (2002) associates with triangulation raises the possibility of a subtle downside to family relations during lockdown. If greater appreciation of parent–child than parent–parent (conjugal) bonds does in fact invite triangulation of children into adult conflicts, this could undermine child development in ways that would not be apparent without direct assessment of child functioning. The fact that provisional triangulation correlated with less psychological distress and fewer family conflict themes seems also to underscore the subtlety of this ultimately dysfunctional pattern. As noted earlier, however, the CERFB norms as yet provide no firm basis for identifying Linares parenting styles, so without converging reliability and validity evidence we must offer this interpretation cautiously. If nothing else, the possibility of increased risk for triangulation during lockdown provides a hypothesis for future research.
What might it mean that participants without children at home more often reported increased couple cohesion during lockdown? From an interpersonal‐systems perspective, the relative absence of third‐party involvements with children, relatives, friends, or colleagues, coupled with sustained proximity and more time for shared conjugal activities, provides a plausible explanation. A more psychological explanation, from the author of a recent survey of U.S. couples conducted around the same time as ours, is that people simply want more closeness in their important relationships and turn to partners for support under COVID‐19 stress—so in effect they are getting what they want (G. Lewandowski, as cited in Bonos, 2020).
The current data are cross‐sectional and can only indirectly address any changes that may have occurred over time during the relatively brief (two‐week) study period. Such change was of interest because at least one prior study found increased symptoms of psychological distress among individuals quarantined at least 10 days (Hawryluck et al., 2004). This was not the case in the present study, where STAI, BDI, and composite measures of individual distress did not change—yet the CERFB measure of conjugal functioning actually showed an opposite trend of improvement over lockdown days. In other words, participants challenged by managing children at home, whose conjugal relations were probably not as good on average as those of other participants, nonetheless tended to report more couple‐level resilience in the third week of lockdown than in the second. Our rough indicator of triangulation decreased over days as well, implying the possibility of positive spillover for children. Intriguingly, moderation results also suggest that the correlation or linkage between individual distress and couple relationship functioning may have strengthened as the lockdown progressed. The direction of influence in this would be ambiguous, however.
In addition to supplementing quantitative analyses, the qualitative themes add texture and detail to how participants experienced relational improvement and deterioration during lockdown. On the deterioration side, some of the themes echo aspects of previously identified stressors linked to quarantine at the individual level. For example, experiencing loneliness and couple/family distance connects to boredom and isolation (Cava et al., 2005; DiGiovanni et al., 2004), and negative expectations incorporate fear of the disease (Bai et al., 2004; Desclaux et al., 2017), economic uncertainty (Jeong et al., 2016; Mihashi et al., 2009), and information overload (Blendon et al., 2004).
Although a few participants cited instances of both improvement and deterioration, most followed one of the two paths in ways that sometimes resonated with our clinical experiences during the first few months of the pandemic. As if to illustrate the improvement pathway, a stay‐at‐home mother who had come to therapy distressed by a disobedient, disrespectful and occasionally violent child reported that she, her husband, and the difficult son had been having “so much fun together” (for a change) since the lockdown began, adding that “while we were playing, several picture frames fell of the wall and broke without even bothering me!” More often, however, we encountered deterioration themes in the form of a lockdown family crisis. Here, a useful strategy has been to contextualize the current problem situation by exploring how family members had (successfully) handled similar situations or had more satisfying relations before the lockdown began. The qualitative improvement themes suggest possible areas to explore.
Our study has many limitations, with sample representativeness foremost among them. The self‐selected sample of convenience was predominantly female, well educated, employed, interested in the topic at hand (family relationships), and probably more functional and economically advantaged than the adult Spanish population at large. It is entirely possible that different results (e.g., more evidence of individual distress or relational deterioration) would obtain in other sectors of the population or in other countries or cultures. Another limitation is that time‐limited, cross‐sectional survey data shed little light on enduring effects of quarantine, on how adaptations to lockdown changed or evolved over time, or on what happened during re‐opening, when home‐confinement restrictions began to ease. We plan to address these and other questions through follow‐up interviews with study participants. Finally, it is difficult to obtain reliable and valid representations of family functioning from the self‐reports of only one family member, and shared method variance (e.g., positive or negative response set) could account for some of the quantitative association between individual and couple/family variables.
Despite these limitations, our exploratory study of the recent state‐regulated COVID‐19 lockdown in Spain helps to break new ground by looking beyond the individual to understand psychosocial impacts of quarantine experiences. The ramifications of these experiences for couples and families are clearly important and probably more complex than for individuals.
Supporting information
The authors appreciate the assistance of Dr. Michael Rohrbaugh in preparing the manuscript for publication.
References
- Bai, Y. , Lin, C.‐C. , Lin, C.‐Y. , Chen, J.‐Y. , Chue, C.‐M. , & Chou, P. (2004). Survey of stress reactions among health care workers involved with the SARS outbreak. Psychiatric Services, 55(9), 1055–1057. 10.1176/appi.ps.55.9.1055 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beck, A. T. , Steer, R. A. , & Brown, G. K. (1996). BDI‐II. Beck depression inventory‐second edition. Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
- Blendon, R. J. , Benson, J. M. , DesRoches, C. M. , Raleigh, E. , & Taylor‐Clark, K. (2004). The public’s response to severe acute respiratory syndrome in Toronto and the United States. Clinical Infectious Diseases, 38, 925–931. 10.1086/382355 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- BOE, Boletín Oficial del Estado (2020, March 14). Real Decreto 463/2020, de 14 de marzo, por el que se declara el estado de alarma para la gestión de la situación de crisis sanitaria ocasionada por el COVID‐19. Retrieved April 10, 2020, from https://www.boe.es/eli/es/rd/2020/03/14/463/dof/spa/pdf [Google Scholar]
- Bonos, L. (2020. May 22). Our relationships are relationships are actually doing well during the pandemic, study finds. Washington Post. Retrieved June 2, 2020, from https://www.washingtonpost.com/lifestyle/2020/05/22/marriage-relationships-coronavirus-arguments-sex-couples/ [Google Scholar]
- Braun, V. , & Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qualitative Research in Psychology, 3(2), 77–101. 10.1191/1478088706qp063oa [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Brooks, S. K. , Webster, R. K. , Smith, L. E. , Woodland, L. , Wessely, S. , Greenberg, N. et al. (2020). The psychological impact of quarantine and how to reduce it: rapid review of the evidence. Lancet, 395, 912–920. 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30460-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buela‐Casal, G. , Guillén‐Riquelme, A. , & Seisdedos Cubero, N. (2015). Cuestionario de ansiedad estado‐rasgo. Madrid, Spain: TEA, Ediciones. [Google Scholar]
- Campreciós, M. (2015). Validación y aplicabilidad clínica del Cuestionario de Evaluación de las Relaciones Familiares Básicas (CERFB) en los trastornos de la conducta alimentaria (Doctoral Dissertation). Retrieved Retrieved June 15, 2020, from https://www.tdx.cat/handle/10803/352474#page=1 [Google Scholar]
- Cao, X. , Jiang, X. , Li, X. , Lo, M. J. , & Li, R. (2013). Family functioning and its predictors among disaster bereaved individuals in china: eighteen months after the Wenchuan earthquake. PLoS One, 8, e60738. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cava, M. A. , Fay, K. E. , Beanlands, H. J. , McCay, E. A. , & Wignall, R. (2005). The experience of quarantine for individuals affected by SARS in Toronto. Public Health Nursing, 22(5), 398–406. 10.1111/j.0737-1209.2005.220504.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conger, R. , & Conger, K. (2002). Resilience in Midwestern families: Selected findings from the first decade of a prospective, longitudinal study. Journal of Marriage and Family, 64(2), 361–373. 0.1111/j.1741-3737.2002.00361.x [Google Scholar]
- Creswell, J. W. , & Plano Clark, V. L. (2011). Designing and conducting mixed methods research (2a ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. [Google Scholar]
- Cummings, M. E. , Koss, K. J. , & Davies, P. T. (2015). Prospective relations between family conflict and adolescent maladjustment: Security in the family system as a mediating process. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 43(3), 503–515. 10.1007/s10802-014-9926-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Desclaux, A. , Badji, D. , Gautier Ndione, A. , & Sow, K. (2017). Accepted monitoring or endured quarantine? Ebola contacts’ perceptions in Senegal. Social Science & Medicine, 178, 38–45. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.02.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DiGiovanni, C. , Conley, J. , Chiu, D. , & Zaborski, J. (2004). Factors influencing compliance with quarantine in Toronto during the 2003 SARS outbreak. Biosecurity and Bioterrorism, 2(4), 265–272. 10.1089/bsp.2004.2.265 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Forbes, M. K. , & Kreuger, R. F. (2019). The great recession and mental health in the United States. Clinical Psychology Science, 7(5), 900–913. 10.1177/2167702619859337 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fowers, A. , & Wan, W. (2020). A third of Americans now show signs of clinical anxiety or depression, Census Bureau finds amid coronavirus pandemic. The Washington Post. Retrieved June 2, 2020, from https://www.washingtonpost.com/health/2020/05/26/americans-with-depression-anxiety-pandemic/?arc404=true [Google Scholar]
- Global Times (2020, March 7). Chinese city experiencing a divorce peak as a repercussion of COVID‐19. Global Times. Retrieved April 20, 2020, from https://www.globaltimes.cn/content/1181829.shtml [Google Scholar]
- Hawryluck, L. , Gold, W. L. , Robinson, S. , Pogorski, S. , Galea, S. , & Styra, R. (2004). SARS control and psychological effects of quarantine, Toronto, Canada. Emerging Infectious Diseases, 10(7), 1206–1212. 10.3201/eid1007.030703 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hobfoll, S. , & Spielberger, C. (1992). Family stress: Integrating theory and measurement. Journal of Family Psychology, 6(2), 99–112. 10.1037/0893-3200.6.2.99 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ibañez, N. (2016). Validación del Cuestionario de Evaluación de las Relaciones Familiares Básicas (Doctoral Dissertation). Retrieved June 15, 2020, from: https://www.tdx.cat/handle/10803/350314 [Google Scholar]
- Ibáñez, N. , Linares, J. L. , Vilaregut, A. , Virgili, C. , & Campreciós, M. (2012). Propiedades psicométricas del cuestionario de evaluación de las relaciones familiares Básicas (CERFB). Psicothema, 24, 489–494. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jeong, H. , Yim, H. W. , Song, Y.‐J. , Ki, M. , Min, J.‐A. , Cho, J. et al. (2016). Mental health status of people isolated due to Middle East respiratory syndrome. Epidemiology and Health, 38, e2016048. 10.4178/epih.e2016048 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joel Wong, Y. , Uhm, S. Y. , & Li, P. (2012). Asian Americans' family cohesion and suicide ideation: Moderating and mediating effects. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 82(3), 309–318. 10.1111/j.1939-0025.2012.01170.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Linares, J. L. (1996). Identidad y narrativa. La terapia familiar en la práctica clínica. Barcelona, Spain: Paidós. [Google Scholar]
- Linares, J. L. (2002). Del abuso y otros desmanes. El maltrato familiar, entre la terapia y el control. Barcelona, Spain: Paidós. [Google Scholar]
- Linares, J. L. (2012). Terapia familiar ultramoderna. La inteligencia terapéutica. Barcelona, Spain: Herder. [Google Scholar]
- Margerison‐Zilko, C. , Goldman‐Mellor, S. , Falconi, A. , & Downing, J. (2016). Health impacts of the great recession: A critical review. Current Epidemiology Reports, 3(1), 81–91. 10.1007/s40471-016-0068-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marjanovic, Z. , Greenglass, E. R. , & Coffey, S. (2007). The relevance of psychosocial variables and working conditions in predicting nurses’ coping strategies during the SARS crisis: an online questionnaire survey. International Journal of Nursing Studies, 44, 991–998. 10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2006.02.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martín‐Lanas, R. , Cano‐Prous, A. , & Beunza‐Nuin, M. I. (2017). DAS. Escala de Ajuste Diádico. Adaptación Española. Madrid, Spain: TEA Ediciones. [Google Scholar]
- McDermott, B. M. , & Cobham, V. E. (2012). Family functioning in the aftermath of a natural disaster. BMC Psychiatry, 12(1), 12–55. 10.1186/1471-244X-12-55 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mihashi, M. , Otsubo, Y. , Yinjuan, X. , Nagatomi, K. , Hoshiko, M. , & Ishitake, T. (2009). Predictive factors of psychological disorder development during recovery following SARS outbreak. Health Psychology, 28(1), 91–100. 10.1037/a0013674 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ministerio de Economía, Industria y Competitividad (2017, June). Propuesta de Resolución Provisional del Procedimiento de Concesión de Ayudas a Proyectos de I+D+I. Retrieved June 15, 2020, from https://sede.micinn.gob.es/stfls/eSede/Ficheros/2017/Propuesta_Resolucion_Provisional_Proyectos_Retos_2017.pdf [Google Scholar]
- Mollà‐Cusí, L. , Vilaregut, A. , Günther‐Bel, C. , Campreciós, M. , Roca, M. , & Matalí Costa, J. L. (2019, June). Construcción del Cuestionario de Evaluación de Relaciones Familiares Básicas y Coparentalidad (CERFB‐CoP). Poster session presented at XIX Jornades ANPIR. Oviedo, Spain. [Google Scholar]
- Patterson, J. M. (2002). Understanding family resilience. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 58(3), 233–246. 10.1002/jclp.10019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pellecchia, U. , Crestani, R. , Decroo, T. , Van den Bergh, R. , & Al‐Kourdi, Y. (2015). Social consequences of Ebola containment measures in Liberia. PLoS One, 10(12), e0143036. 10.1371/journal.pone.0143036 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Qiu, J. , Shen, B. , Zhao, M. , Wang, Z. , Xie, B. , & Xu, Y. (2020). A nationwide survey of psychological distress among Chinese people in the COVID‐19 epidemic: implications and policy recommendations. General Psychiatry, 33(2), e100213. 10.1136/gpsych-2020-100213 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Repetti, R. L. , Taylor, S. E. , & Seeman, T. E. (2002). Risky families: Family social environments and the mental and physical health of offspring. Psychological Bulletin, 128(2), 330–366. 10.1037/0033-2909.128.2.330 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reynolds, D. L. , Garay, J. R. , Deamond, S. L. , Moran, M. K. , Gold, W. , & Styra, R. (2008). Understanding, compliance and psychological impact of the SARS quarantine experience. Epidemiology & Infection, 136(7), 997–1007. 10.1017/S0950268807009156 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robertson, E. , Hershenfield, K. , Grace, S. L. , & Stewart, D. E. (2004). The psychosocial effects of being quarantined following exposure to SARS: A qualitative study of Toronto health care workers. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 49(6), 403–407. 10.1177/070674370404900612 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roca, M. , Vilaregut, A. , Palma, C. , Barón, F. J. , Campreciós, M. , & Mercadal, L. (2020). Basic family relations, parental bonding, and dyadic adjustment in families with a member with psychosis. Community Mental Health Journal, 403–407. 10.1007/s10597-020-00581-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rubin, G. , & Wessely, S. (2020). The psychological effects of quarantining a city. BMJ: British Medical Journal, 368, m313. 10.1136/bmj.m313 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanz, J. , & Vázquez, C. (2011). Adaptación española del Inventario para de Depresión de Beck‐II(BDI‐II) [Spanish adaptation of the Beck Depression Inventory II]. Madrid, Spain: Pearson Education. [Google Scholar]
- Spanier, G. B. (1976). Measuring dyadic adjustment: New scales for assessing the quality of marriage and similar dyads. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 38, 15–28. [Google Scholar]
- Spanier, G. B. (2017). DAS. Escala de Ajuste Diádico (adapted by R. Martín‐Lanas, A. Cano‐Prous, & M. I. Beunza‐Nuin). Madrid, Spain: TEA Ediciones. [Google Scholar]
- Spielberger, C. D. , Gorsuch, R. L. , & Lushene, R. E. (2008). STAI. Manual for the state‐trait anxiety inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologist Press. [Google Scholar]
- Sprang, G. , & Silman, M. (2013). Posttraumatic stress disorder in parents and youth after health‐related disasters. Disaster Medicine and Public Health Preparedness, 7, 105–110. 10.1017/dmp.2013.22 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vilaregut, A. , Callea, A. , Campreciós, M. , Coletti, M. , Mercadal, L. , & Mateu, C. (2019). Measuring family relations: Italian validation of the “cuestionario de evaluación de las relaciones familiares básicas”. Clinical Neuropsychiatry, 16(2), 116–123. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walsh, F. (2007). Traumatic loss and major disasters: Strengthening family and community resilience. Family Process, 46(2), 207–227. 10.1111/j.1545-5300.2007.00205.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson, M. (2020, May 24). The Virus Has Wrecked Some Families. It Has Brought Others Closer. The New York Times. Retrieved June 2, 2020, from https://www.nytimes.com/2020/05/24/nyregion/coronavirus-nyc-families.html?referringSource=articleShare [Google Scholar]
- World Health Organization (2020a, January 5). Pneumonia of unknown cause‐China. World Health Organization. Retrieved April 10, 2020, from https://www.who.int/csr/don/05-january-2020-pneumonia-of-unkown-cause-china/en/ [Google Scholar]
- World Health Organization (2020b, March 12). WHO announces COVID‐19 outbreak a pandemic. World Health Organization. Retrieved April 10, 2020, from http://www.euro.who.int/en/health-topics/health-emergencies/coronavirus-covid-19/news/news/2020/3/who-announces-covid-19-outbreak-a-pandemic [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.