Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2020 Aug 5;15(8):e0236521. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0236521

Electrophysiological correlates of focused attention on low- and high-distressed tinnitus

Rafał Milner 1,*,#, Monika Lewandowska 2,#, Małgorzata Ganc 1, Jan Nikadon 3, Iwona Niedziałek 4, Wiesław Wiktor Jędrzejczak 1, Henryk Skarżyński 5
Editor: Manuel S Malmierca6
PMCID: PMC7406215  PMID: 32756593

Abstract

Objectives

The study aimed at determining the EEG correlates of concentration on either low or high-distressed tinnitus.

Methods

Sixty-seven patients (36 women, mean age = 50.34 ± 12.94 years) with chronic tinnitus were assigned to either a high (HD) or low (LD) tinnitus-related distress group based on THI results. All participants took part in the EEG study comprising two 3–4 min blocks of focusing on either tinnitus (Tinnitus Focus Condition, TFC) or the sensations from one’s own body (Body Focus Condition, BFC). The absolute power and current density of 8 frequency bands in 7 clusters were compared between conditions and groups.

Results

The most pronounced differences were found in the HD patients in the TFC, relative to the BFC, i.e. reduced power of frontally distributed low alpha (8–10 Hz) and posterior high alpha (10–12 Hz) as well as lower current density of 8–10 Hz rhythm over the right frontal/anterior cingulate cortex and higher middle beta (15–18 Hz) density in the precuneus. The HD, relative to LD patients, in both conditions, exhibited increased low beta (12–15 Hz) power over the left middle area and greater higher beta (15–25 Hz) power in the left posterior region.

Conclusions

The present study contrasted bioelectrical activity, acquired when concentrating on tinnitus with EEG data collected whilst patients focused on their body. Decreased alpha power and current density in the frontal/cingulate cortex when listening to bothersome tinnitus might reflect greater cortical arousal whereas increased beta power and density in the precuneus/posterior cingulate activity in this condition could be indicative for elevated tension or augmented cognitive/emotional processing of tinnitus sound. Enhanced beta rhythm in patients with high versus low tinnitus distress, observed independently of the study condition, may be due to greater self-focused attention or more active processing of sensations derived from the own body.

Introduction

Tinnitus, an auditory percept appearing in the absence of any external or internal sound source, is a serious medical and social problem affecting 10–15% of the global population [1,2]. If tinnitus becomes chronic, it can contribute to depression, anxiety, sleep disturbances, and concentration difficulties that dramatically reduce the quality of life [37]. Tinnitus varies in its perceptual characteristics (e.g. its laterality, type of sound, pitch, loudness), the distress it evokes (emotional reaction to its presence), comorbid conditions (e.g. hearing loss, hyperacusis, vertigo, affective disorder) and response to therapy [8]. The heterogeneity of tinnitus may explain why there is no one effective treatment for all patients and the outcomes of a given therapeutic intervention are highly variable. Patients might use various non-specific methods to cope with tinnitus including sound or music therapies [911], cochlear implantation [12], psychological therapies [1319], brain stimulation [2022], or neurofeedback training [23,24]. No effective pharmacological treatment for this condition is yet available.

Tinnitus etiologies encompass noise exposure, age-related hearing loss (presbycusis), cardiovascular and cerebrovascular disease, medications, head/neck injury, and hyper- or hypothyroidism [25]. Although the cause of tinnitus might be located outside the central nervous system, central brain structures are thought to be responsible for turning it into a chronic sensation [26]. Tinnitus has been considered as the acoustic analog of phantom pain [27,28] caused by deafferentation (incomplete afferent connection with the central nervous system) [2932] leading to possible increases in neural network gain [33], neural plasticity [34,35], neural synchrony [36], sensitization [37] or sensory uncertainty [38]. The complementary theory proposes that deafferentation is accompanied by dysfunction of the “noise-cancellation system” involving reciprocal connections between primary auditory cortex, ventromedial prefrontal area, thalamus, and amygdala [3941]. In the current theories, developed to integrate the abovementioned models, tinnitus has been considered as a property of multiple, parallel and partially overlapping neural networks [42] or a result of a prediction error in the auditory system [43].

Chronic tinnitus is associated with an abnormal resting-state MEG/EEG signal recorded in the auditory cortex, which could be seen as an increased activity in the range of gamma (> 40 Hz) [4446], delta, theta, alpha, and beta bands [47] as well as reduced low (8–10 Hz) but not high (10–12 Hz) alpha power in the temporal area [48]. Chronic tinnitus could also manifest in resting-state EEG by lowered alpha (8–12 Hz) power, accompanied by enhanced delta activity [49]. A possible theory accounting for reduced slow-wave activity together with increased gamma over the temporal areas in tinnitus patients is the thalamo–cortical dysrhythmia model [29,45] in which tinnitus relates to persistent gamma activity in the auditory cortex due to auditory deafferentation. In the deafferented state the dominant resting-state alpha rhythm (8–12 Hz) decreases to a theta rhythm (4–7 Hz). As a result, GABA-mediated lateral inhibition is reduced, inducing gamma band activity in regions adjacent to the deafferented theta (called an edge effect, 40). Notably, hearing loss is rarely controlled in these studies [50] but if it was, the obtained results only partially support this model.

The majority of patients are able to get used to their tinnitus percept, however, in some patients the reaction to this novel sound (tinnitus) does not reduce over time. Low-distress and high-distress tinnitus have different neural representation, as can be observed in EEG and fMRI studies on resting brain activity [5060], when, in the absence of any external stimuli, patients are asked to relax and not think of anything in particular [61,62]. Specifically, these studies show that troublesome tinnitus is associated with increased upper beta activity in the frontal area, whereas less bothersome tinnitus is associated with enhanced delta, alpha, and lower gamma (30–40 Hz) in the bilateral temporal area [58]. In comparison to healthy controls (from a normative database), patients with bothersome tinnitus showed higher current source density in the alpha (8–12 Hz) and beta (21–32.5 Hz) bands and less synchronized delta (2–3.5 Hz) and theta (4–7.5 Hz) in the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex [59]. In the same study, high-distress tinnitus (relative to low-distress tinnitus) was associated with greater current source density in the low alpha band (8–10 Hz) in the posterior subcallosal anterior cingulate, para-hippocampal gyrus, and amygdala. Bothersome tinnitus also co-occurred with increased synchronization in the high alpha (10.5–12.5 Hz) band in the para-hippocampal gyrus, amygdala, and middle temporal gyrus; there was also decreased current source density in this band in the posterior cingulate and precuneus [59]. Previous studies have demonstrated that distress caused by tinnitus involves the global fronto-parieto-cingulate network [63,64] and is related to the alpha and beta bands in the anterior cingulate and ventromedial prefrontal/orbitofrontal cortices [54,56,59,65] or gamma activity in the auditory cortex or insula [46,66,67]. The subcallosal/ventromedial prefrontal area and the amygdala are considered as the structures forming the “noise-cancellation” system thought to underlie long-term habituation to tinnitus [40]. Most of the above structures (the anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortices, the insula) are thought to be parts of the “pain neuromatrix” [68] suggesting a common “distress network” for tinnitus and pain.

It has been also demonstrated that tinnitus-related distress (a temporary feeling) and depression (a more prolonged state) are represented in different neural networks [56,58]. Joos et al. [56] found a lateralization effect when looking for differences in spontaneous bioelectrical activity in these two states. Specifically, the authors found that distress caused by tinnitus was associated with the alpha (10.5–12.5 Hz) and beta (13–21 Hz) bands in the right frontal areas as well as beta activity (18.5–21 Hz) in the anterior cingulate cortex. In the same study, the depression level in tinnitus patients correlated with an elevated alpha (10.5–12.5 Hz) current source density in the left frontal cortex. Another brain structure (the para-hippocampal gyrus) was considered as a node connecting the areas involved in tinnitus-related distress and depression. As this study suggested, in tinnitus patients both these emotional states are processed by specific neural circuits which are part of a greater common network.

Further evidence for specific alterations in neural networks associated with bothersome tinnitus comes from resting-state fMRI studies in which functional connectivity (FC)–i.e. temporal correlations of low-frequency fluctuations in the BOLD response [69]–are calculated. High-distress tinnitus was found to be associated with negative FC between the sensory cortices (auditory and visual)–i.e. when the signal decreases in one of these areas, it increases in the other [51]. On the other hand, non-bothersome tinnitus is considered to not significantly affect the FC pattern [60]. Tinnitus-related distress is thought to be positively associated with increased FC between the posterior cingulate cortex and right medial prefrontal cortex belonging to the default mode network (DMN) [52] that is typically engaged at rest and suppressed during performance of a mental task [62], or with enhances connectivity of the superior temporal gyrus with the amygdala within one hemisphere (right or left), which suggests a role for interaction between the auditory and limbic networks in emotional reaction to tinnitus [53]. There is also evidence of a positive correlation between tinnitus-related distress and FC of the right superior temporal gyrus and the cerebellum, which might reflect a dysfunctional filtering of unpleasant sounds [55]. In a recent study on a large tinnitus patient group (n = 135), highly bothersome tinnitus was associated with increased FC within the right executive control network encompassing the frontal and parietal regions, as well as greater connectivity between this network and the left executive control, default mode, auditory, and salience networks [57]. Since the executive control network is considered to be involved in various attentional processes (e.g. allocating top–down attentional resources, cognitive control, externally oriented awareness [70,71], the authors suggested that high-distress tinnitus could be related to attention to the sound.

Brain activity of high- and low-distress tinnitus patients has also been investigated during fMRI cognitive/emotional tasks. Patients with high distress (compared to low-distress) tinnitus showed increased activation in the left mid frontal gyrus in response to tinnitus-related sounds versus neutral sentences [72], or in the right insula and orbitofrontal cortex in the tinnitus-related sounds versus neutral words contrast [73]. Decreased activation of the frontal areas in patients with more bothersome tinnitus, relative to those who do not suffer from it so much, was found when they listening to affective sounds not associated with tinnitus [74]. Overall, these studies suggest a role for the frontal cortex and insula in the processing of emotional auditory stimuli in patients with troublesome tinnitus.

In the present study the EEG power and current density from signal recorded when focusing on tinnitus was contrasted with the signal acquired when just concentrating on other internal sensations from one’s own body. Such comparisons should allow one to identify the neural correlates of emotional reaction to tinnitus and the amount of attentional resources allocated to it. The literature in this area is scarce. The findings to date are that there is a lack of significant difference in global spectral power or source of the EEG signal when a comparison is made between active listening to tinnitus and the resting state [75]. We expect that the most pronounced differences between the above conditions to be found in the alpha band over the auditory areas in patients with high tinnitus-related distress. The rationale for this statement comes from a study suggesting that augmented alpha rhythm in sensory cortices reflects inhibition of sensory information processing in these regions [76]. Since in the present study the participants are supposed to be concentrated and attentive, increased alpha activity might be also observed over the areas that form the networks subserving attention, mainly, the prefrontal and parietal cortices [77]. In the face of evidence for reduced alpha power in response to emotional stimuli [78] we also assume that concentrating on bothersome tinnitus, i.e. an unpleasant and/or annoying sound, might be associated with more active information processing and, hence, lower alpha activity, compared to the focusing on one’s own body (a neutral stimulus). The above effect might be, therefore, seen over the regions involved in a relaxation and concentration on inner world [62] and/or unpleasant sensory experience [79,80].

In the current study it is also hypothesized that in comparison to focusing on something without any special meaning (e.g. one’s own body), the state of concentrating on bothersome tinnitus produces higher beta activity. Increased high-frequency oscillations have been found in response to salient or unpleasant stimuli [81,82]. Moreover, beta rhythm is thought to be associated with stress and anxiety [83] as well as concentration and arousal [84,85]. Therefore, high-distress tinnitus patients might be more occupied by their own tinnitus sound which contributes to increased beta activity over the brain representations regions of attentional and/or emotional processing.

Materials and methods

Participants

Eighty-seven participants with chronic (≥ 6 months) subjective tinnitus were recruited from patients of the Institute of Physiology and Pathology of Hearing (IPPH), Warsaw/Kajetany, Poland. Twenty of them were excluded due to excessive motion or muscle artifacts in the EEG signal and/or missing data in the questionnaires assessing tinnitus distress, depression, or anxiety. The remaining 67 patients (36 women and 31 men, mean age = 50.3 ± 12.9 years, age 17.7–70.6 years) did not suffer from any serious illness (hyperacusis, Meniere’s disease, neurological/psychiatric disorder, or major medical disease such as cancer or thyroid dysfunction), had no history of head trauma, seizure, stroke, smoking, or alcoholism and had not taken any medications affecting the central nervous system in the past 6 months. All subjects had not used any form of meditation (e.g. mindfulness, yoga) prior to participation in the study.

Each subject gave written informed consent to take part in the study after all procedures had been fully explained. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the IPPH and was congruent with the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki.

Participants were split into two groups according to the median value obtained in the Polish version of the Tinnitus Handicap Inventory (THI) [86]. As a result, there was a high tinnitus-related distress group (HD = 34) and a low tinnitus-related distress group (LD = 33). The detailed characteristics of LD and HD patients are presented in Table 1.

Table 1. Statistical comparison of patients with low tinnitus-related distress (LD) and high tinnitus-related distress (HD).

LD (n = 33) HD (n = 34) t, z, or χ2 value
Tinnitus characteristics
THI (M ± SD) 27.37 ± 7.41 58.26 ± 14.6 7.04***
Tinnitus duration (months, M ± SD) 57.52 ± 55.78 66.35 ± 64.1 .736
Tinnitus location (left/right/bilateral/head) 6/5/19/3 4/3/22/5 1.61
Demographic data
Age (years, M ± SD) 51.94 ± 11.77 47.97 ± 13.98 .99
Sex (women/men) 19/14 17/17 .387
Depression and anxiety
BDI-II (M ± SD) 9.03 ± 8.33 13.29 ± 8.02 2.59*
STAI X-1 (M ± SD) 37.61 ± 8.37 42.18 ± 10.75 1.81
STAI X-2 (M ± SD) 41.58 ± 8.11 48.03 ± 8.73 3.16**
Hearing loss (dB HL)#
Right ear 17.41 ± 13.07 16.51 ± 10.12 .063
Left ear 17.41 ± 10.65 17.56 ± 10.59 .213

* p < 0.05

** p < 0.01

*** p < 0.001

THI—Tinnitus Handicap Inventory; BDI—Beck Depression Inventory; STAI–State Trait Anxiety Inventory.

# Mean values of hearing loss both for the right and left ear were calculated for 0.125, 0.25, 0.5, 0.75, 1, 1.5, 2, 3, 4, 6, and 8 kHz

Medical interview and audiological examination

All procedures were carried out at IPPH. Each patient participated in a medical interview followed by otolaryngological examination conducted by an ENT specialist. Audiometric thresholds were measured for 0.125–8 kHz in accordance with standard procedures provided by the British Society of Audiology [87]. Average thresholds for 0.125, 0.25, 0.5, 0.75, 1, 1.5, 2, 3, 4, 6, and 8 kHz were calculated separately for the right and left ear. The outcomes of the pure tone audiometry in LD and HD groups are presented in Fig 1.

Fig 1. Audiograms: Average left and right ear hearing thresholds in the high tinnitus-related (HD) and low tinnitus-related (LD) distress groups.

Fig 1

Errors bars show standard deviations of the individual means.

Questionnaires

Patients were requested to fill in the THI [88], Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II) [89], and State Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) [90] (the order of administration was randomized across participants). Tinnitus distress was determined by means of a Polish version of THI, a reliable tool widely used to assess how tinnitus affects daily living. THI consists of 25 questions concerning limitations of social/occupational, physical, and emotional functioning as well as catastrophic reactions to the presence of tinnitus. A yes response to a question is awarded 4 points, sometimes 2 points, and no 0 points, with higher scores representing greater tinnitus distress. Subjective depression was assessed using BDI-II which is a 21-item tool asking for responses on a 3-point scale, with higher scores reflecting higher intensity of symptoms. STAI includes two 20-item scales, one of which (X-1) is referred to anxiety as a state and the other (X-2) measures anxiety as a personal trait. Each patient responded on a 4-point scale (scores 1–4) where higher scores indicate higher level of anxiety.

EEG data acquisition and pre-processing

The EEG signal was recorded in an acoustic-shielded room. Each participant was seated in a comfortable chair in an upright relaxed position. Prior to data recording, he or she was instructed to refrain from any movements (especially those involving the face, e.g. eyes, forehead, neck, and jaw). The EEG session consisted of two 3–4 min blocks during which patients were asked to focus attention (while keeping the eyes closed) and analyze sensations associated either with: 1) tinnitus sound (the tinnitus-focus condition, TFC) or 2) their own feet (the body-focus condition, BFC). The order of these two blocks was counterbalanced across the participants. The TFC and BFC in each patient were separated by a short (ca. 1-min) break, mainly conversation with the experimenter.

The EEG data were acquired from 19 channels (ElectroCap International, Inc., Eaton, USA), placed according to the 10–20 system [91] with the use of a 21-channel apparatus (Mitsar, St. Petersburg, Russia). The signal was referenced to linked earlobes and Fpz electrode was the ground electrode. The impedance was kept below 10 kΩ during the whole data registration. Sampling rate was 250 Hz and the signal was digitally filtered in the 0.3–70 Hz band.

The EEG signal from each condition was pre-processed using Neuroguide 2.9.4 software (Applied Neuroscience, St. Petersburg, FL). The data pre-processing steps were as follows: 1) the signal was filtered between 0.5 and 50 Hz; 2) artifacts were removed with the use of a semi-automatic algorithm implemented in the program (in each patient a manually selected 10-sec artifact-free epoch of the whole acquired EEG signal served as a template for automatic selection of segments without artifacts) [92]; 3) statistical analysis of the selected EEG segments using the split-half and test–retest reliability methods [93,94]. Only artifact-free EEG segments with a split-half reliability greater than 95% and test–retest reliability greater than 90% as well as a length ≥ 1 min were accepted and included to further analysis [92].

All data used in this study is available on figShare database with the following DOI: 10.6084/m9.figshare.12643403.v1.

QEEG data analysis

QEEG data analysis was also performed with the use of the Neuroguide 2.9.4 software. First, a fast Fourier transform (FFT) was performed on the EEG signals to determine the absolute power in the following bands: delta (1–4 Hz), theta (4–8 Hz), low alpha (8–10 Hz), high alpha (10–12 Hz), as well as low (12–15 Hz), middle (15–18 Hz), and high (18–25 Hz) beta and gamma (30–40 Hz), for both the TFC and BFC. A 75% sliding window was used to compute the FFT in consecutive 2-s epochs (i.e. 256 points were overlapped in 500-ms steps (64 points) in order to minimize the effects of FFT windowing [95]). The EEG absolute power in each frequency band was averaged separately for 7 clusters (Fig 2).

Fig 2. A scheme of electrode montage.

Fig 2

Clusters were presented from anterior (left, cluster LA orange and right, cluster RA yellow), left middle (cluster LM green), central (cluster CE gray), right middle (cluster RM red) and posterior (left, cluster LP purple and right, cluster RP blue).

The EEG data were checked for departures from normality with the use of a Shapiro–Wilk test. Since most distributions showed significant abnormality (basically due to right skewness), our data were normalized by means of square, cube square, or Lg10 transforms prior to statistical analysis.

A repeated measures ANOVA with the “frequency band” (delta, theta, low and high alpha, low, middle, high beta and gamma), “condition” (TFC and BFC), and “cluster” (LA, RA, LM, RM, CE, LP, RP) as repeated factors, as well as the “tinnitus” (LD and HD) as a between-subject factor was performed and only p values based on permutation methods were reported. Using the formula notation in R, the following model: POWER ~ TINNITUS*FREQUENCY BAND*CONDITION * CLUSTER + Error (SUB/(FREQUENCY BAND*CONDITION*CLUSTER)) + TINNITUS was fitted using aovperm() function implemented in R/permuco package [96] (100000 permutations). Post‐hoc pairwise t‐tests calculated with the use of R/emmeans package [97] were bootstrapped (Boot() function implemented in R/car package, 100000 bootstrap samples) in order to deconstruct significant interaction effects, and the p‐values from these bootstrap comparisons were adjusted using the false discovery rate (FDR) approach [98].

sLORETA analysis

Standardized low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (sLORETA) was applied to determine the intracerebral sources of bioelectrical brain activity recorded at the scalp. To prepare EEG signal for sLORETA analysis the Neuroguide ver. 2.9.4 software was used. Pre-selected artifact-free segments of EEG signals in each frequency band were exported to special ASCI files containing successive 256 points time series of signals recorded in all 19 channels. Overlapping windows method, implemented in the Neuroguide, was applied during export time series to minimize the FFT windowing effects by overlapping 256 point × 19 channel EEG segments by 75% in ASCII format [99]. Next, the time to frequency domain conversion was applied to pre-prepared ASCI files. The cross-spectral files for every frequency band were determined for 19 electrodes, 256 time points and the sampling rate of 128 Hz with the use of the EEG cross-spectral maker implemented in sLORETA utilities. Finally, the cross-spectra prepared in previous step, were transformed into sLORETA files containing information about the current density vector field power specific for each subject.

A standardization used by sLORETA algorithm allows to solve the inverse solution with zero localization error [100] and the point EEG sources can be find without any previous assumptions as to the numbers of modelled sources and their localizations [100,101]. As a result of sLORETA the distribution of current density (μA/mm2) is determined within solution space formed by 6239 voxels (voxel size: 5 × 5 × 5 mm) and restricted to cortical gray matter, hippocampus and amygdala, defined by the digitized Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) brain template [102]. The sLORETA algorithm solves the inverse problem by assuming related orientations and strengths of neighboring neuronal sources (represented by adjacent voxels). The method has been proved as an efficient functional brain activity mapping tool with a high temporal resolution and relatively high spatial resolution [100]. The localization properties of sLORETA have been previously validated (e.g. [103,104]).

In our study the current density distributions between the conditions (TFC versus BFC) and groups (HD versus LD), were compared with the use of non-parametric permutation tests (SnPM) of sLORETA. The computations were performed on a whole brain by sLORETA statistical contrast maps using multiple voxel-by-voxel comparisons method with 5000 permutations. As a result, the maps of log t-ratio statistics for each frequency band were generated. The statistical thresholds, corrected for multiple testing, were applied (p < 0.05 and p < 0.1, 2-tailed) [105] and the voxels with t-values that exceeded the pre-defined statistical threshold were used to identify the Brodmann areas (BA) where brain activity was significantly different between conditions or groups.

Results

QEEG analysis

The ANOVA results are shown in Table 2. We found a substantial 3-way interaction effect: “frequency band” × “condition” × “cluster” (F42, 2730 = 1.54, p = 0.0141) and a 4-way interaction: “frequency band” × “condition” × “cluster” × “tinnitus distress” (F42, 2730 = 1.71, p = 0.0037). The results of post-hoc analysis were presented in S1S6 Tables.

Table 2. The results of repeated-measures ANOVA.

P values based on permutation tests for repeated measures ANOVA.

dfNum dfDen F permutation p
Tinnitus distress 1 65 2.34 0.1284
Frequency 7 455 94.75 0.0001***
Frequency × tinnitus distress 7 455 1.22 0.2924
Condition 1 65 0.92 0.3364
Condition × tinnitus distress 1 65 0.58 0.4499
Cluster 6 390 50.62 0.0001***
Cluster × tinnitus distress 6 390 2.14 0.0494*
Frequency × condition 7 455 1.27 0.2642
Frequency × condition × tinnitus distress 7 455 0.44 0.8848
Frequency × cluster 42 2730 46.74 0.0001***
Frequency × cluster × tinnitus distress 42 2730 0.35 1.0000
Condition × cluster 6 390 0.96 0.4521
Condition × cluster × tinnitus distress 6 390 0.81 0.5608
Frequency ×condition × cluster 42 2730 1.54 0.0141*
Frequency ×condition × cluster × tinnitus distress 42 2730 1.71 0.0037**

dfNum indicates degrees of freedom numerator, dfDen indicates degrees of freedom denominator, p values were computed for permutations in repeated-measures ANOVA.

*p < 0.05

**p < 0.01

***p < 0.001.

Tinnitus versus body focus condition

In the whole study sample delta power in the LA cluster was significantly (p = 0.02, FDR-adjusted) higher in the TFC (M = 0.972, SE = 0.072 μV) than in the BFC (M = 0.943, SE = 0.020 μV). Furthermore, a power in the gamma frequency band over the RA region in the TFC was increased relative to the BFC (p = 0.013, FDR-corrected, for TFC: M = 0.269, SE = 0.034 μV and for BFC: M = 0.223, SE = 0.033 μV).

The results of comparisons between the bioelectrical activity recorded in the TFC and BFC in the HD and LD groups are shown in Fig 3 and included in S3 and S4 Tables.

Fig 3. Comparisons of particular frequency band (delta, theta, low and high alpha, low, middle and high beta, gamma) power between experimental conditions (tinnitus focus condition, TFC, and body focus condition, BFC) in selected clusters (LA–left anterior, RA—right anterior, LM—left middle, RM—right middle, CE–central, LP—left posterior, RP–right posterior).

Fig 3

The significant differences are marked with “stars” (* p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01).

For the HD group in the TFC, compared to BFC, there was significantly (p = 0.034) decreased low alpha power over the LA (M = 0.686, SE = 0.068 μV for the TFC; and M = 0.734, SE = 0.065 μV for the BFC) and RA (p = 0.028, M = 0.709, SE = 0.07 μV for the TFC and M = 0.752, SE = 0.068 μV for the BFC) areas. These patients also demonstrated reduced high alpha band power over the RA (p = 0.05, for the TFC: M = 0.570, SE = 0.06 μV and for the BFC: M = 0.613, SE = 0.064 μV), CE (p = 0.026, for the TFC: M = 0.872, SE = 0.069 and for the BFC: M = 0.935, SE = 0.074), LP (p = 0.006, for the TFC: M = 1.001, SE = 0.082 μV and for the BFC: M = 1.07, SE = 0.086 μV) and RP (p = 0.003, for the TFC: M = 0.993, SE = 0.086 μV and for the BFC: M = 1.073, SE = 0.091 μV) regions. The HD patients in the TFC, relative to the BFC, produced enhanced gamma power in the RA cluster (p = 0.020, for the TFC: M = 0.245, SE = 0.048 and for the BFC: M = 0.198, SE = 0.047).

In the LD group only one significant effect was observed, namely, increased delta power over the LA area in the TFC compared to the BFC (p = 0.0320, for the TFC: M = 0.976, SE = 0.031 and for the BFC: M = 0.954, SE = 0.026).

High versus low tinnitus distress

The differences in frequency band power between the HD and LD groups in the BFC and TFC are shown in Fig 4 and S5 and S6 Tables.

Fig 4. Comparisons of particular frequency band power (key as in previous figure) between patients with high (HD) and low (LD) tinnitus-related distress in selected clusters (key as in previous figure).

Fig 4

The significant differences are marked with “stars” (* p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01).

During the BFC in the LM area low beta power was significantly (p = 0.026) higher in the HD group (M = 0.538, SE = 0.045 μV) than in the LD group (M = 0.395, SE = 0.046 μV). In the TFC, there was also increased (p = 0.019) low beta power in the same region in the HD (M = 0.539, SE = 0.044 μV) versus LD (M = 0.392, SE = 0.044 μV) group. Finally, only in the BFC enhanced power in the low-frequency beta band (p = 0.045) was found in the LP cluster for the HD group (M = 0.707, SE = 0.055 μV), relative to the LD patients (M = 0.549, SE = 0.056 μV).

In the BFC middle beta power over the CE and LP regions was significantly increased in the HD (for the CE: p = 0.048, M = 0.501, SE = 0.046 μV and for the LP: p = 0.022, M = 0.484, SE = 0.047 μV) compared to the LD group (for the CE: M = 0.371, SE = 0.047 μV and for the LP: M = 0.329, SE = 0.048 μV). In the TFC, significantly (p = 0.025) higher middle beta power in the HD (M = 0.476, SE = 0.047 μV) compared to the LD patients (M = 0.326, SE = 0.047 μV) was observed only in the LP area.

The HD group in the BFC showed increased high beta power compared to the LD patients in the CE area (p = 0.029, M = 0.691, SE = 0.038 μV for HD patients and M = 0.576, SE = 0.039 μV for LD patients) and the LP area (p = 0.02, for HD: M = 0.656, SE = 0.039 μV and for LD: M = 0.528, SE = 0.04 μV), whereas in the TFC this effect was only found to be significant (p = 0.005) in the LP region (for HD: M = 0.647, SE = 0.037 μV and for LD: M = 0.5, SE = 0.038 μV).

sLORETA

Only in the HD group sLORETA analysis showed significant between-condition differences for low alpha (8–10 Hz) and middle beta (15–18 Hz) frequency bands (Fig 5, Table 3). Specifically, in these patients during the TFC, compared to BFC, there was a decrease of current density in the low alpha band in the right frontal area (medial, superior, middle and inferior frontal gyrus) and in the right anterior cingulate (threshold log t-statistics = 4.150 for p < 0.05; and t = 3.889 for p < 0.1). Conversely, middle beta current density in the HD group in the TFC, relative to the BFC, was increased in bilateral posterior cingulate and left precuneus (Fig 5, Table 3) (threshold log t-statistics = 3.889 for p < 0.1). sLORETA analysis did not reveal any significant between-group (HD versus LD) differences.

Fig 5.

Fig 5

Significant results for current density amplitude analysis in the low alpha (8–10 Hz) band (A) and middle beta (15–18 Hz) band (B). In the high distress tinnitus patients sLORETA current source density in the low alpha band was lower in the tinnitus focus condition compared to the body focus condition in the right frontal/cingulate cortex and, in the same contrast, the current density in the middle beta (15–18 Hz) was higher in the posterior cingulate/precuneus area. T-statistic that are positive are displayed in red (the mean current density in the body focus condition is greater than the mean in the tinnitus focus condition) and those that are negative in blue (the mean current density in the body focus condition is lower than the mean in the tinnitus focus condition). The horizontal (left) sagittal (middle), and coronal (right) sections through the voxel with maximal t-statistic are presented. The maximal t-value and MNI coordinates of the voxel with this value are shown in the yellow frame above a sagittal section. The image shows significant outcomes only.

Table 3. Brain structures with significant differences in current density (μA/mm2) between the tinnitus focus condition and body focus condition in patients with high distress tinnitus.

The regions were identified by the voxel-by-voxel sLORETA non-parametric permutation test. The table includes the Brodmann’s areas (BA) and brain regions, (Montreal Neurological Institute) MNI coordinates of voxels with the highest t-values and total number of activated voxels (p-values are marked with “stars”).

Band BA Region Side MNI coordinates Max t-value Total number of voxels
X Y Z
Low Alpha 10 Medial Frontal Gyrus Right 20 45 -5 -4.34** 5
10,11 Superior Frontal Gyrus Right 20 50 0 -4.34** 40
10,11 Middle Frontal Gyrus Right 25 50 -10 -4,27** 19
32 Anterior Cingulate Right 15 45 -5 -4.14* 4
11 Inferior Frontal Gyrus Right 20 40 -20 -3.94* 1
Middle Beta 23,29,30,31 Posterior Cingulate L, L/R, R -5 -60 15 4.03* 16
23,31 Precuneus L,L/R 0 -60 20 3,89* 3

p < 0.05**

p < 0.1*

Discussion

To our knowledge this is the first study demonstrating that active processing of bothersome tinnitus sensations coexist with alterations in the alpha (seen both in EEG power and current density analyses), middle beta and gamma rhythms (Figs 3, 5A, Table 3, S3 and S4 Tables). Furthermore, we found that patients with high and low tinnitus-related distress were different in terms of beta band power regardless of the experimental condition, i.e. while patients concentrating on their tinnitus and on sensations in another part of their body (Fig 4, S5 and S6 Tables).

Focusing on highly distressing tinnitus alters the alpha (8–12 Hz), but also beta (15–18 Hz) and gamma (30–40 Hz) rhythms

Congruent with our expectations, differences between the EEG signal recorded in each condition were most pronounced in the patients with high tinnitus-related distress in the alpha and beta frequency bands. Specifically, only in this group, when the TFC was compared to the BFC, there was a significant decrease of the low-frequency alpha (8–10 Hz) power over the anterior area, and reduced activity within the high alpha band (10–12 Hz) over right anterior, left and right posterior and central regions (Fig 3, S3 and S4 Tables). In the TFC, relative to the BFC, the HD patients also showed increased gamma power but only at the electrodes positioned over the right frontal cortex. Additionally, for the above contrast, sLORETA analysis revealed reduced low alpha current density amplitude in right frontal area and right anterior cingulate and enhanced density in the middle beta band in the precuneus (Fig 5, Table 3).

The alpha rhythm (8–12 Hz) is thought to reflect the balance between excitatory and inhibitory neuronal processes: reduced activity in this frequency band is associated with excitation or more active cognitive processing whereas enhanced activity is associated with inhibition (the alpha-inhibition hypothesis, [74,75]). By interpreting our outcomes according to this hypothesis we claim that reduced alpha power under the TFC compared to the BFC in the HD patients, may reflect higher cortical excitability and/or a more detailed analysis of tinnitus relative to the sensations derived from a part of one’s body. The strength of this explanation is enhanced when we take note that a relaxed state of mind is more easily accomplished when, as in the present study, subjects are asked to keep their eyes closed. In addition, focusing on one’s own body is an important part of mindfulness therapy [106] and the presented outcomes, showing enhanced alpha power over the anterior and posterior cortices when focusing on one’s own body, are consistent with the evidence on augmented alpha activity in the frontal and/or parieto-temporo-occipital cortex during various meditation states [107112].

In our study reduced high alpha power (10–12 Hz) over the central and posterior areas in the HD patients, when the TFC was compared to the BFC, might reflect greater semantic processing demands in the TFC conditions [113]. In other words, decreased high alpha power in the TFC might indicate that tinnitus was perceived as a meaningful sound. This interpretation of our findings is in agreement with previous evidence of a strong decrease in alpha activity (8–12 Hz) in the auditory cortex when a salient sound is heard [114]. Furthermore, when we take into account that affective stimuli, in comparison to neutral ones, induce more reduced posterior alpha rhythm [115], diminished power within this frequency band in the TFC in our study might also reflect an emotional reaction to tinnitus.

The analogy between phantom sound (tinnitus) and phantom pain are both thought to be due to thalamo-cortical dysrhythmia [30], and our results are in accordance with the evidence on reduced alpha and enhanced gamma activity when attention is devoted to the sensation of pain [116]. Decreased alpha might reflect the degree to which the thalamo-cortical “gate” is opened and therefore how relevant information reaches the cortex and how it is then processed. In our study, concentrating on bothersome tinnitus keeps the gate open, resulting in weak inhibition of tinnitus-related information that might manifest as decreased power and/or current density within the alpha band accompanied by increased gamma activity.

The similarities between tinnitus and chronic pain (i.e. both are variable subjective sensations caused by peripheral lesions, the patients also share a psychological profile characterized by increased tendency to depression and anxiety [41]) implicate a common neural foundation resulting from the presence of chronic and unpleasant symptoms. At first glance, in the case of alpha rhythm, our results do not appear consistent with either previous evidence on troublesome tinnitus [56,59,65] or the literature on chronic pain (see: [117] for a review) which both suggest elevated rather than reduced activity within this band at rest. Chronic high tinnitus-related distress is supposed to be apparent during the resting state due to the absence of any stimulus presentation and the fact that it is hard to keep attention away from persistent and annoying sound. Therefore, the high distress tinnitus patients when concentrating on tinnitus are supposed to produce the EEG activity comparable to that observed at “pure” rest.

The resting state might be, however, considered just as a state of relaxed, calm mind. In this case it could be more similar to the condition, explored in the present study, when patients are requested to focus on the own body (a legitimate therapeutic technique to achieve a state of relaxed, calm mind). When we take into account the resembles between both these states, our results in the alpha frequency band (increased activity under the BFC relative to the TFC) are congruent with previous findings [56,59,65].

In our study decreased current density within low alpha band in the HD patients was observed under the TFC, compared to the BFC, in the right frontal and anterior cingulate cortices (Fig 5A, Table 3). Since these regions are thought to form the pain network [118], the above-described differences might not be unique for tinnitus but rather associated with chronic, troublesome ailment. Additionally, our results are consistent with the valance hypothesis suggesting the right hemisphere dominance for negative emotions and left hemisphere dominance for positive emotions [119]. The above differences may, then, reflect affective aspects of tinnitus.

We found increased gamma (30–40 Hz) power over the right anterior area in the TFC relative to the BFC in all patients but especially in bothersome tinnitus sufferers (Fig 3, S3 and S4 Tables). Gamma activity associated with tinnitus-related distress has been relatively well documented [46,66,67,120], however, comparable number of evidence failed to demonstrate any relationship [56,59,65,121]. Since in our study in the HD group the between-condition difference in gamma power was found only in one cluster (and also the high and low distress tinnitus groups did not substantially different in the activity within this frequency band) it is hard to explain increased gamma, observed in the TFC, in terms of tinnitus-related distress. We rather claim that higher gamma, thought to be associated with a state of active information processing [122] may be indicative for greater cognitive/emotional involvement in the analysis of sensations associated with bothersome tinnitus. Notably, the gamma effect was also observed not only for the HD patients but in the whole study group which might indicate that more active processing of tinnitus rather than related distress, is reflected in this high-frequency range.

Similarly to the outcomes of the present study, in the literature on chronic pain enhanced gamma activity over the prefrontal cortex has been found at rest [123125] or when noxious-attended condition was contrasted with the resting state [126]. Therefore, increased gamma power in the HD patients in the TFC, compared to the BFC, might reflect unpleasantness and distress not specifically related to tinnitus. However, since the alterations in chronic pain have been mostly described for much higher gamma band (> 60 Hz) than the frequency range analyzed in our study, we cannot discuss our outcomes in terms of the analogy between tinnitus and chronic pain conditions.

Although we did not observe any significant differences between the TFC and BFC in beta power, in this contrast we found higher current source density in the middle beta frequency range (15–18 Hz) in the precuneus/posterior cingulate but only in the HD group (Fig 5B, Table 3). Since this rhythm, just as the precuneus activity, is thought to be involved in attentional processes [127,128], the above effect most probably reflect more cognitive resources allocated to tinnitus as compared to sensations derived from the own body.

As expected, in the present study there were not much significant differences between bioelectrical activity in the BFC and TFC in patients with low tinnitus-related distress. Thus, non-bothersome tinnitus appears not to be an affective, meaningful sound deserving of special attention. In this case it will not be processed differently to any other relatively neutral stimulus such as sensations from one’s own body. However, we observed slightly enhanced delta power in the TFC compared to the BFC over left anterior area only in the LD patients (Fig 3, S3 and S4 Tables). Since frontal delta rhythm is thought to increase in various tasks (e.g. semantic processing, mental calculation, see: [129] for a review), the above effect might reflect more cognitive efforts devoted to active listening of tinnitus (definitely the forced concentrating on a tinnitus sound is not a natural situation for the patients who no longer pay attention to it) than focusing on one’s body part.

Beta rhythm (12–25 Hz) is increased in high vs low tinnitus-related distress

The high-distress compared to low-distress tinnitus patients showed increased low-frequency beta power (12–15 Hz) over the left-middle area in both experimental conditions whereas in the BFC this effect was observed only over the left posterior region (Fig 4; S5 and S6 Tables). Low-frequency beta power over the sensorimotor cortex is called the sensorimotor rhythm (SMR), which is thought to reflect an alert state of mind during active inhibition of motor behavior [130]. It becomes desynchronized while imagining body movement [131]. Neurofeedback trainings aimed at increasing SMR has been used to lower seizure activity [132], enhance semantic memory [133], and sustain attention [134,135]. In the present study, the HD compared to the LD group showed, under both conditions, increased low beta power at the C3 and T3 electrodes (i.e. over the sensory and motor cortices). It is therefore possible that the between-group differences observed in our study in the range of 12–15 Hz were from the SMR. In this case, the HD group may have put more effort into inhibiting body movement compared to the LD group (although prior to recording all subjects were asked to remain still to avoid EEG artifacts).

The 12–15 Hz rhythm outside the motor cortex is thought to be related to relaxed attention such as those involved in pleasant activities [136]. In the present study, there was greater low beta power over the left temporo-parieto-occipital cortex in the high-distress tinnitus patients compared to the low-distress patients, but only when they were concentrating on sensations from their own body. Therefore, these between-group differences might just indicate that not focusing on tinnitus is a much more pleasant state for the HD group than the LD group. This conclusion follows naturally from considering that patients with more bothersome tinnitus could feel relief when they don’t have to monitor and analyze a distressing sound in their head. Finally, since the posterior brain area is thought to be involved in various attentional processes [137], enhanced low beta power in this region in the HD, compared to the LD patients, might also indicate greater attentional resources allocated to the BFC’s task in the HD group.

In the present study the middle (15–18 Hz) beta power in both BFC and TFC was increased over the left posterior area in the HD compared to the LD group (Fig 4, S5 and S6 Tables). This frequency band is thought to reflect activity of the noradrenergic system, which is involved in sustained attention and working memory [128]. Previous studies have shown that an enhancement of the 15–18 Hz rhythm after neurofeedback training correlated with better continuous performance [135] or sustained attention [134,138,139]. Augmented beta power was also accompanied by higher P300 amplitude in an oddball paradigm [134,135], which might also be considered an index of higher cortical excitation [140] or better working memory [141]. In this context, increased middle beta power in the HD, relative to the LD patients, in the BFC and TFC, might reflect more attentional resources engaged in these conditions by the first of these groups.

In the present study augmented low and middle beta power in bothersome tinnitus (compared to non-bothersome tinnitus) was observed in the left hemisphere, especially in the posterior area. These outcomes might be explained in terms of left hemispheric dominance in selective attention to fine-grained auditory information [142]. Accordingly, patients with high-distress tinnitus, especially when they are asked to focus on it, might be able to notice even very subtle characteristic in its sound, which mainly involves the left auditory cortex. The advantage of the left posterior region observed in the BFC might, on the other hand, result from more careful and detailed analysis of sensations derived from own body by the HD group.

Relative to LD patients, our HD group demonstrated enhanced high beta (18–25 Hz) over the left posterior area in both BFC and TFC, but only over the central region under the BFC. High-frequency oscillations are thought to reflect stress, anxiety, and increased cortical hyperactivity [83,143]. The locally augmented amplitude of the high beta band is also related to extensive muscle tension, and is most apparent in temporal brain regions [144]. Therefore, the increased high beta rhythm in our patients with bothersome tinnitus might be evidence of anxiety or increased cortical arousal rather than of tinnitus perception itself.

Our finding that the HD group had, under both experimental conditions, enhanced high beta activity over the left, but not the right, posterior area is consistent with evidence for involvement of the left temporal region in auditory hallucinations [145]. Tinnitus can be considered an hallucination, since it is present in the absence of any actual sound. Musical phenomena in tinnitus patients are thought to be due not to psychosis but to their emotional experience [146]. Hence, increased higher beta power over the left posterior area in our HD group, compared to the LD group, might indicate that high-distress tinnitus, relative to a non-bothersome phantom sound, is a clear and strong auditory phenomenon.

Limitations of the study

Including to the study only the patients with tinnitus (without healthy controls) deprived us of the ability to draw reliable inferences about “increased” or “decreased” power within particular frequency bands in tinnitus. Specifically, without controls we could only conclude that bioelectrical activity was lower or higher in one clinical group relative to the other, but of course it is possible that in both these groups the compared parameter (e.g. band power) is abnormal. Thus, in future studies it is recommended that healthy controls be included whose only task is to concentrate on the body part.

When viewing the results one should be also acknowledged that any EEG based approach does not allow to analyze subcortical activity which constrains the effects to cortical sources.

Conclusions

The most pronounced differences between concentrating on bothersome tinnitus and focusing on a one’s body part were found in the high-distress tinnitus patients, in the alpha frequency band (i.e. decreased frontally distributed low alpha power and reduced high alpha rhythm over the central and posterior areas). These effects might reflect higher cortical excitability or more active cognitive and emotional processing of tinnitus. Altered low alpha current density in the frontal and anterior cingulate cortices in the high tinnitus distress patients suggests sharing the attentional/emotional network with pain experience. Increased gamma power might reflect more active processing of bothersome tinnitus sensation whereas higher middle beta current density in the precuneus, observed in the tinnitus-focused condition compared to concentrating on the body, could be indicative for more attention resources allocated to troublesome tinnitus. Increased low beta (12–15 Hz) over the sensorimotor cortex and posterior middle and high beta (15–25 Hz) activity in the patients with high tinnitus-related distress, irrespectively of the condition, may reflect more thorough cognitive/emotional processing of sensations derived from the own body (not specifically associated with tinnitus).

Supporting information

S1 Table. The mean absolute power and standard error (in brackets) values for different frequency bands in the contrast: Body Focus Condition (BFC) versus tinnitus focus condition (TFC), calculated in each cluster for the whole study sample (n = 67).

(PDF)

S2 Table. Post-hoc pairwise t-tests results for the contrast: Body Focus Condition (BFC) versus Tinnitus Focus Condition (TFC) calculated in each cluster for the whole study sample (n = 67).

Items in bold are significant based on p-values.

(PDF)

S3 Table. The mean absolute power and standard error (in brackets) values for different frequency bands in the contrast: Body Focus Condition (BFC) versus Tinnitus Focus Condition (TFC), calculated in each cluster separately for the low tinnitus-related distress (LD) and high tinnitus-related distress (HD) group.

(PDF)

S4 Table. Post-hoc pairwise t-tests results for the contrast: Body Focus Condition (BFC) versus Tinnitus Focus Condition (TFC), calculated in each cluster separately for the high tinnitus-related distress (HD) and low tinnitus-related distress (LD) group.

Items in bold are significant based on p-values.

(PDF)

S5 Table. The mean absolute power and standard error (in brackets) values for different frequency bands in the contrast: High tinnitus-related distress (HD) versus low tinnitus-related distress (LD) group, calculated in each cluster separately for the Body Focus Condition (BFC) and Tinnitus Focus Condition (TFC).

(PDF)

S6 Table. Post-hoc pairwise t-tests results for the contrast: High tinnitus-related distress (HD) versus low tinnitus-related distress (LD) group, calculated in each cluster separately for the Body Focus Condition (BFC) and Tinnitus Focus Condition (TFC).

Items in bold are significant based on p-values.

(PDF)

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to Andrew Bell for English language editing and valuable comments on the paper.

Data Availability

All data used in the analysis is available on figShare database with the accompanying DOI: https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.12643403.v1.

Funding Statement

This study was supported by a grant from the Polish 1015 National Science Centre (UMO nr 2011/03/D/NZ4/02431). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Eggermont JJ. The auditory cortex and tinnitus—a review of animal and human studies. Eur J Neurosci. 2015;41: 665–676. 10.1111/ejn.12759 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Henry JA, Dennis KC, Schechter MA. General Review of Tinnitus Prevalence, Mechanisms, Effects, and Management. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2005;48: 1204–1235. 10.1044/1092-4388(2005/084) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Zeman F, Koller M, Langguth B, Landgrebe M, others. Which tinnitus-related aspects are relevant for quality of life and depression: results from a large international multicentre sample. Health Qual Life Outcomes. 2014;12 Available from: http://www.biomedcentral.com/content/pdf/1477-7525-12-7.pdf [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Stevens C, Walker G, Boyer M, Gallagher M. Severe tinnitus and its effect on selective and divided attention. Int J Audiol. 2007;46: 208–216. 10.1080/14992020601102329 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Rossiter S, Stevens C, Walker G. Tinnitus and its effect on working memory and attention. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2006;49: 150–160. 10.1044/1092-4388(2006/012) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Roberts LE, Husain FT, Eggermont JJ. Role of attention in the generation and modulation of tinnitus. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2013;37: 1754–1773. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.07.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Langguth B, Landgrebe M, Kleinjung T, Sand GP, Hajak G. Tinnitus and depression. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2011;12: 489–500. 10.3109/15622975.2011.575178 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Cederroth CR, Gallus S, Hall DA, Kleinjung T, Langguth B, Maruotti A, et al. Editorial: Towards an Understanding of Tinnitus Heterogeneity. Front Aging Neurosci. 2019;11: 53 10.3389/fnagi.2019.00053 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Okamoto H, Stracke H, Stoll W, Pantev C. Listening to tailor-made notched music reduces tinnitus loudness and tinnitus-related auditory cortex activity. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2010;107: 1207–1210. 10.1073/pnas.0911268107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Sheppard A, Stocking C, Ralli M, Salvi R. A review of auditory gain, low-level noise and sound therapy for tinnitus and hyperacusis. Int J Audiol. 2019; 1–11. 10.1080/14992027.2019.1660812 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Argstatter H, Grapp M, Plinkert PK, Bolay HV. Heidelberg Neuro-Music Therapy for chronic-tonal tinnitus—treatment outline and psychometric evaluation. 2012;17(1): 31–41. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Mielczarek M, Norena A, Schlee W, Olszewski J. Excitation of the Auditory System as a Result of Non-invasive Extra-Cochlear Stimulation in Normal Subjects and Tinnitus Patients. Front Neurosci. 2018;12: 146 10.3389/fnins.2018.00146 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Andersson G. Psychological aspects of tinnitus and the application of cognitive–behavioral therapy. Clin Psychol Rev. 2002;22: 977–990. 10.1016/s0272-7358(01)00124-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Nemade SV, Shinde KJ. Clinical Efficacy of Tinnitus Retraining Therapy Based on Tinnitus Questionnaire Score and Visual Analogue Scale Score in Patients with Subjective Tinnitus. Turk Arch Otorhinolaryngol. 2019;57: 34–38. 10.5152/tao.2019.3091 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Jastreboff PJ, Jastreboff MM. Tinnitus Retraining Therapy (TRT) as a Method for Treatment of Tinnitus and Hyperacusis Patients. J Am Acad Audiol. 2000;11: 162–177. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.McKenna L, Marks EM, Vogt F. Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy for Chronic Tinnitus: Evaluation of Benefits in a Large Sample of Patients Attending a Tinnitus Clinic. Ear Hear. 2018;39:359–366. 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000491 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Niedziałek I, Raj-Koziak D, Milner R, Wolak T, Ganc M, Wójcik J, et al. Effect of yoga training on the tinnitus induced distress. Complement Ther Clin Pract. 2019;36: 7–11. 10.1016/j.ctcp.2019.04.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Roland LT, Lenze EJ, Hardin FM, Kallogjeri D, Nicklaus J, Wineland AM, et al. Effects of Mindfulness Based Stress Reduction Therapy on Subjective Bother and Neural Connectivity in Chronic Tinnitus. Otolaryngol Neck Surg. 2015;152: 919–926. 10.1177/0194599815571556 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Thompson DM, Hall DA, Walker D-M, Hoare DJ. Psychological Therapy for People with Tinnitus: A Scoping Review of Treatment Components. Ear Hear. 2017;38:149–158. 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000363 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.De Ridder D, Song J-J, Vanneste S. Frontal Cortex TMS for Tinnitus. Brain Stimulat. 2013;6: 355–362. 10.1016/j.brs.2012.07.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Theodoroff SM, Griest SE, Folmer RL. Transcranial magnetic stimulation for tinnitus: using the Tinnitus Functional Index to predict benefit in a randomized controlled trial. Trials. 2017;18: 64 10.1186/s13063-017-1807-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Song J-J, Vanneste S, Van de Heyning P, De Ridder D. Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation in Tinnitus Patients: A Systemic Review and Meta-Analysis. Sci World J. 2012; 2012:427941 10.1100/2012/427941 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Güntensperger D, Thüring C, Meyer M, Neff P, Kleinjung T. Neurofeedback for Tinnitus Treatment–Review and Current Concepts. Front Aging Neurosci. 2017;9: 386 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00386 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Milner R, Lewandowska M, Ganc M, Cieśla K, Niedziałek I, Skarżyński H. Slow Cortical Potential Neurofeedback in Chronic Tinnitus Therapy: A Case Report. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback. 2016;41: 225–249. 10.1007/s10484-015-9318-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Henry JA, Roberts LE, Caspary DM, Theodoroff SM, Salvi RJ. Underlying Mechanisms of Tinnitus: Review and Clinical Implications. J Am Acad Audiol. 2014;25: 5–126. 10.3766/jaaa.25.1.2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Eggermont JJ, Tass PA. Maladaptive Neural Synchrony in Tinnitus: Origin and Restoration. Front Neurol. 2015;6:29 10.3389/fneur.2015.00029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Jastreboff PJ. Phantom auditory perception (tinnitus): mechanisms of generation and perception. Neurosci Res. 1990;8: 221–254. 10.1016/0168-0102(90)90031-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Jastreboff PJ, Hazell JWP. A neurophysiological approach to tinnitus: Clinical implications. Br J Audiol. 1993;27: 7–17. 10.3109/03005369309077884 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.De Ridder D, Vanneste S, Langguth B, Llinas R. Thalamocortical Dysrhythmia: A Theoretical Update in Tinnitus. Front Neurol. 2015;6:124 10.3389/fneur.2015.00124 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.De Ridder D, Elgoyhen AB, Romo R, Langguth B. Phantom percepts: Tinnitus and pain as persisting aversive memory networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2011;108: 8075–8080. 10.1073/pnas.1018466108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Tonndorf J. The analogy between tin&us and pain: A suggestion for a physiological basis of chronic tinnitus. Hear Res. 1987;28:271–275. 10.1016/0378-5955(87)90054-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Moller AR. Similarities between severe tinnitus and chronic pain. J Am Acad Audiol. 2000;11:115–124. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Noreña AJ. An integrative model of tinnitus based on a central gain controlling neural sensitivity. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35: 1089–1109. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.11.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Meyer M, Langguth B, Kleinjung T, Møller AR. Plasticity of Neural Systems in Tinnitus. Neural Plast. 2014;2014: 1–2. 10.1155/2014/968029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Roberts LE. Neural plasticity and its initiating conditions in tinnitus. HNO. 2018;66: 172–178. 10.1007/s00106-017-0449-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Eggermont JJ, Roberts LE. The neuroscience of tinnitus. Trends Neurosci. 2004;27: 676–682. 10.1016/j.tins.2004.08.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Zenner HP, Pfister M, Birbaumer N. Tinnitus Sensitization: Sensory and Psychophysiological Aspects of a New Pathway of Acquired Centralization of Chronic Tinnitus. Otol Neurotol. 2006;27: 1054–1063. 10.1097/01.mao.0000231604.64079.77 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.De Ridder D, Vanneste S, Freeman W. The Bayesian brain: Phantom percepts resolve sensory uncertainty. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014;44: 4–15. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.04.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Leaver AM, Renier L, Chevillet MA, Morgan S, Kim HJ, Rauschecker JP. Dysregulation of Limbic and Auditory Networks in Tinnitus. Neuron. 2011;69: 33–43. 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.12.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Rauschecker JP, Leaver AM, Mühlau M. Tuning Out the Noise: Limbic-Auditory Interactions in Tinnitus. Neuron. 2010;66: 819–826. 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.04.032 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Rauschecker JP, May ES, Maudoux A, Ploner M. Frontostriatal Gating of Tinnitus and Chronic Pain. Trends Cogn Sci. 2015;19: 567–578. 10.1016/j.tics.2015.08.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.De Ridder D, Vanneste S, Weisz N, Londero A, Schlee W, Elgoyhen AB, et al. An integrative model of auditory phantom perception: Tinnitus as a unified percept of interacting separable subnetworks. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014;44: 16–32. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.03.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Sedley W, Friston KJ, Gander PE, Kumar S, Griffiths TD. An Integrative Tinnitus Model Based on Sensory Precision. Trends Neurosci. 2016;39: 799–812. 10.1016/j.tins.2016.10.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Ashton H, Reid K, Marsh R, Johnson I, Alter K, Griffiths T. High frequency localised “hot spots” in temporal lobes of patients with intractable tinnitus: A quantitative electroencephalographic (QEEG) study. Neurosci Lett. 2007;426:23–28. 10.1016/j.neulet.2007.08.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Llinás R, Urbano FJ, Leznik E, Ramírez RR, van Marle HJF. Rhythmic and dysrhythmic thalamocortical dynamics: GABA systems and the edge effect. Trends Neurosci. 2005;28: 325–333. 10.1016/j.tins.2005.04.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Van der Loo E, Gais S, Congedo M, Vanneste S, Plazier M, Menovsky T, et al. Tinnitus Intensity Dependent Gamma Oscillations of the Contralateral Auditory Cortex. PLoS One. 2009;4(10):e7396 10.1371/journal.pone.0007396 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Moazami-Goudarzi M, Michels L, Weisz N, Jeanmonod D. Temporo-insular enhancement of EEG low and high frequencies in patients with chronic tinnitus. QEEG study of chronic tinnitus patients. BMC Neurosci. 2010;11:40 10.1186/1471-2202-11-40 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Schlee W, Schecklmann M, Lehner A, Kreuzer PM, Vielsmeier V, Poeppl TB, et al. Reduced Variability of Auditory Alpha Activity in Chronic Tinnitus. Neural Plast. 2014;2014: 436146 10.1155/2014/436146 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Weisz N, Moratti S, Meinzer M, Dohrmann K, Elbert T. Tinnitus Perception and Distress Is Related to Abnormal Spontaneous Brain Activity as Measured by Magnetoencephalography. PLoS Med. 2005;2: e153 10.1371/journal.pmed.0020153 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Adjamian P, Sereda M, Zobay O, Hall DA, Palmer AR. Neuromagnetic Indicators of Tinnitus and Tinnitus Masking in Patients with and without Hearing Loss. J Assoc Res Otolaryngol. 2012;13: 715–731. 10.1007/s10162-012-0340-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Burton H, Wineland A, Bhattacharya M, Nicklaus J, Garcia KS, Piccirillo JF. Altered networks in bothersome tinnitus: a functional connectivity study. BMC Neurosci. 2012;13: 3 10.1186/1471-2202-13-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Chen Y-C, Chen H, Bo F, Xu J, Deng Y, Lv H, et al. Tinnitus distress is associated with enhanced resting-state functional connectivity within the default mode network. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2018;14: 1919–1927. 10.2147/NDT.S164619 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Chen Y-C, Xia W, Chen H, Feng Y, Xu J-J, Gu J-P, et al. Tinnitus distress is linked to enhanced resting-state functional connectivity from the limbic system to the auditory cortex: Altered Effective Connectivity in Tinnitus. Hum Brain Mapp. 2017;38: 2384–2397. 10.1002/hbm.23525 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.De Ridder D, Vanneste S, Congedo M. The Distressed Brain: A Group Blind Source Separation Analysis on Tinnitus. PLoS ONE. 2011;6: e24273 10.1371/journal.pone.0024273 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Feng Y, Chen Y-C, Lv H, Xia W, Mao C-N, Bo F, et al. Increased Resting-State Cerebellar-Cerebral Functional Connectivity Underlying Chronic Tinnitus. Front Aging Neurosci. 2018;10:59 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00059 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Joos K, Vanneste S, De Ridder D. Disentangling Depression and Distress Networks in the Tinnitus Brain. Langguth B, editor. PLoS ONE. 2012;7: e40544 10.1371/journal.pone.0040544 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Kandeepan S, Maudoux A, Ribeiro de Paula D, Zheng JY, Cabay JE, Gómez F, et al. Tinnitus distress: a paradoxical attention to the sound? J Neurol. 2019;266(9):2197–2207. 10.1007/s00415-019-09390-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Meyer M, Luethi MS, Neff P, Langer N, Büchi S. Disentangling Tinnitus Distress and Tinnitus Presence by Means of EEG Power Analysis. Neural Plast. 2014;2014: 1–13. 10.1155/2014/468546 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Vanneste S, Plazier M, der Loo E van, de Heyning PV, Congedo M, De Ridder D. The neural correlates of tinnitus-related distress. NeuroImage. 2010;52: 470–480. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Wineland AM, Burton H, Piccirillo J. Functional connectivity networks in nonbothersome tinnitus. Otolaryngol—Head Neck Surg. 2012;147: 900–906. 10.1177/0194599812451414 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Biswal BB, Kylen JV, Hyde JS. Simultaneous assessment of flow and BOLD signals in resting-state functional connectivity maps. NMR Biomed. 1997;10: 165–170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA, Shulman GL. A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2001;98: 676–682. 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Møller AR,Langguth B, De Ridder D, Kleinjung T.Textbook of Tinnitus. New York: Springer-Verlag; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Zobay O, Palmer AR, Hall DA, Sereda M, Adjamian P. Source Space Estimation of Oscillatory Power and Brain Connectivity in Tinnitus. PloS One. 2015;10: e0120123 10.1371/journal.pone.0120123 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Mohsen S, Mahmoudian S, Talbian S, Pourbakht A. Correlation Analysis of the Tinnitus Handicap Inventory and Distress Network in Chronic Tinnitus: An EEG Study. Basic Clin Neurosci J. 2019;10:499–514. 10.32598/bcn.9.10.215 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Schlee W, Mueller N, Hartmann T, Keil J, Lorenz I, Weisz N. Mapping cortical hubs in tinnitus. BMC Biol. 2009;7: 80 10.1186/1741-7007-7-80 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Van der Loo E, Congedo M, Vanneste S, De Heyning PV, De Ridder D. Insular lateralization in tinnitus distress. Auton Neurosci. 2011;165: 191–194. 10.1016/j.autneu.2011.06.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Melzack R. Pain and the Neuromatrix in the Brain. J Dent Educ. 2001;65: 1378–1382. 10.1002/j.0022-0337.2001.65.12.tb03497 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Biswal BB, Kylen JV, Hyde JS. Simultaneous assessment of flow and BOLD signals in resting-state functional connectivity maps. NMR Biomed. 1997;10: 165–170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Dosenbach NU, Fair DA, Miezin FM, Cohen AL, Wenger KK, Dosenbach RA, et al. Distinct brain networks for adaptive and stable task control in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2007;104: 11073–11078. 10.1073/pnas.0704320104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Fassbender C, Simoes-Franklin C, Murphy K, Hester R, Meaney J, Robertson IH, et al. The Role of a Right Fronto-Parietal Network in Cognitive Control. J Psychophysiol. 2006;20: 286–296. 10.1027/0269-8803.20.4.286 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Golm D, Schmidt-Samoa C, Dechent P, Kröner-Herwig B. Neural correlates of tinnitus related distress: An fMRI-study. Hear Res. 2013;295: 87–99. 10.1016/j.heares.2012.03.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Golm D, Schmidt-Samoa C, Dechent P, Kröner-Herwig B. Tinnitus- related distress: evidence from fMRI of an emotional stroop task. BMC Ear Nose Throat Disord. 2016;16: 10 10.1186/s12901-016-0029-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Carpenter-Thompson JR, Schmidt S, McAuley E, Husain FT. Increased Frontal Response May Underlie Decreased Tinnitus Severity. PLOS ONE. 2015;10: e0144419 10.1371/journal.pone.0144419 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Neff P, Hemsley C, Kraxner F, Weidt S, Kleinjung T, Meyer M. Active listening to tinnitus and its relation to resting state EEG activity. Neurosci Lett. 2019;694: 176–183. 10.1016/j.neulet.2018.11.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Kropotov JD. Quantitative EEG, Event-Related Potentials and Neurotherapy.San Diego: Elsevier Science; 2010. [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Langner R, Eickhoff SB. Sustaining attention to simple tasks: A meta-analytic review of the neural mechanisms of vigilant attention. Psychol Bull. 2013;139: 870–900. 10.1037/a0030694 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Liu A, Harris AM, Atkinson AP, Reed CL. Dissociable Processing of Emotional and Neutral Body Movements Revealed by μ-Alpha and Beta Rhythms. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2018;13:1269–1279. 10.1093/scan/nsy094 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Carretié L, Albert J, López-Martín S, Tapia M. Negative brain: An integrative review on the neural processes activated by unpleasant stimuli. Int J Psychophysiol. 2009;71: 57–63. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.07.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Tracey I, Mantyh PW. The Cerebral Signature for Pain Perception and Its Modulation. Neuron. 2007;55: 377–391. 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.07.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Güntekin B, Başar E. A review of brain oscillations in perception of faces and emotional pictures. Neuropsychologia. 2014;58: 33–51. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.03.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Novembre G, Pawar VM, Kilintari M, Bufacchi RJ, Guo Y, Rothwell JC, et al. The effect of salient stimuli on neural oscillations, isometric force, and their coupling. NeuroImage. 2019;198: 221–230. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.05.032 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Kropotov J. Beta and Gamma Rhythms. In: Kropotov J, editor. Functional Neuromarkers in Psychiatry. Applications for diagnosis and treatment London: Academic Press; 2016. [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Engel AK, Fries P. Beta-band oscillations—signalling the status quo? Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2010;20: 156–165. 10.1016/j.conb.2010.02.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Formaggio E, Storti SF, Pastena L, Melucci M, Ricciardi L, Faralli F, et al. How Expertise Changes Cortical Sources of EEG Rhythms and Functional Connectivity in Divers Under Simulated Deep-Sea Conditions. IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng. 2019;27: 450–456. 10.1109/TNSRE.2019.2894848 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Skarżynski PH, Raj-Koziak D, J. Rajchel J, Pilka A, Wlodarczyk AW, Skarzynski H. Adaptation of the Tinnitus Handicap Inventory into Polish and its testing on a clinical population of tinnitus sufferers. Int J Audiol. 2017;56: 711–715. 10.1080/14992027.2017.1319080 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.British Society of Audiology. Recommended Procedure. Pure-tone air-conduction and bone conduction threshold audiometry with and without masking. [Internet]. 2011. Available from: http://www.thebsa.org.uk/wp787content/uploads/2014/04/BSA_RP_PTA_FINAL_24Sept11_MinorAmend06Feb12.pdf
  • 88.Skarżynski PH, Raj-Koziak D, J. Rajchel J, Pilka A, Wlodarczyk AW, Skarżynski H. Adaptation of the Tinnitus Handicap Inventory into Polish and its testing on a clinical population of tinnitus sufferers. Int J Audiol. 2017;56: 711–715. 10.1080/14992027.2017.1319080 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Beck AT, Steer RA, Carbin MG. Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: Twenty-five years of evaluation. Clin Psychol Rev. 1988;8: 77–100. 10.1016/0272-7358(88)90050-5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene RE. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto: CA: Consulting Psychologists Press; 1970. [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Jasper HH. The ten twenty electrode system of the international federation. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1958;10: 371–375. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Thatcher RW. Handbook of quantitative electroencephalography and EEG biofeedback. St Petersburg: FL: Anipublishing; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Salinsky MC, Oken BS, Morehead L. Test-retest reliability in EEG frequency analysis. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1991;79: 382–392. 10.1016/0013-4694(91)90203-g [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Gasser T, Bächer P, Steinberg H. Test-retest reliability of spectral parameters of the EEG. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1985;60: 312–319. 10.1016/0013-4694(85)90005-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Kaiser DA, Sterman MB. Automatic artifact detection, overlapping windows, and state transitions. J Neurother. 2000;4: 85–92. [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Frossard J, Renaud O. permuco: Permutation tests for regression (repeated measures). R package version 1.1.0. 2019. Available from: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/permucopermuco.pdf.
  • 97.Lenth, R. Emmeans: Estimated Marginal Means, aka Least‐Squares Means. Rpackage version 1.4.7. 2020. Available from: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/emmeans emmeans.pdf.
  • 98.Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the False Discovery Rate: A Practical and Powerful Approach to Multiple Testing. J R Stat Soc Ser B Methodol. 1995;57: 289–300. 10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Kaiser DA, Sterman MB. Automatic Artifact Detection, Overlapping Windows, and State Transitions. J Neurother. 2000;4: 85–92. 10.1300/J184v04n03_06 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Pascual-Marqui RD. Standardized low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (sLORETA): technical details. Methods Find Exp Clin Pharmacol. 2002;24 Suppl D:5–12. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Fuchs M, Kastner J, Wagner M, Hawes S, Ebersole JS. A standardized boundary element method volume conductor model. Clin Neurophysiol. 2002;113: 702–712. 10.1016/s1388-2457(02)00030-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Evans AC, Collins DL, Mills SR, Brown ED, Kelly RL, Peters TM. 3D statistical neuroanatomical models from 305 MRI volumes. 1993 IEEE Conference Record Nuclear Science Symposium and Medical Imaging Conference. San Francisco, CA, USA: IEEE; 1993: 1813–1817. 10.1109/NSSMIC.1993.373602 [DOI]
  • 103.Sekihara K, Sahani M, Nagarajan SS. Localization bias and spatial resolution of adaptive and non-adaptive spatial filters for MEG source reconstruction. NeuroImage. 2005;25: 1056–1067. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.11.051 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Wagner M, Fuchs M, Kastner J. Evaluation of sLORETA in the Presence of Noise and Multiple Sources. Brain Topogr. 2003;16: 277–280. 10.1023/B:BRAT.0000032865.58382.62 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Nichols TE, Holmes AP. Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: A primer with examples. Hum Brain Mapp. 2002;15: 1–25. 10.1002/hbm.1058 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Keng S-L, Smoski MJ, Robins CJ. Effects of mindfulness on psychological health: A review of empirical studies. Clin Psychol Rev. 2011;31: 1041–1056. 10.1016/j.cpr.2011.04.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Barcelona J, Fahlman M, Churakova Y, Canjels R, Mallare J, van den Heuvel MI. Frontal alpha asymmetry during prayerful and resting states: An EEG study in Catholic sisters. Int J Psychophysiol. 2020;155: 9–15. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2020.04.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Bing-Canar H, Pizzuto J, Compton RJ. Mindfulness-of-breathing exercise modulates EEG alpha activity during cognitive performance: Mindfulness and error-related EEG alpha. Psychophysiology. 2016;53: 1366–1376. 10.1111/psyp.12678 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Chiesa A. Zen Meditation: An Integration of Current Evidence. J Altern Complement Med. 2009;15: 585–592. 10.1089/acm.2008.0416 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Faber PL, Lehmann D, Gianotti LRR, Milz P, Pascual-Marqui RD, Held M, et al. Zazen meditation and no-task resting EEG compared with LORETA intracortical source localization. Cogn Process. 2015;16: 87–96. 10.1007/s10339-014-0637-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Ivanovski B, Malhi GS. The psychological and neurophysiological concomitants of mindfulness forms of meditation. Acta Neuropsychiatr. 2007;19: 76–91. 10.1111/j.1601-5215.2007.00175.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Lagopoulos J, Xu J, Rasmussen I, Vik A, Malhi GS, Eliassen CF, et al. Increased Theta and Alpha EEG Activity During Nondirective Meditation. J Altern Complement Med. 2009;15: 1187–1192. 10.1089/acm.2009.0113 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Klimesch W, Sauseng P, Hanslmayr S. EEG alpha oscillations: The inhibition–timing hypothesis. Brain Res Rev. 2007;53: 63–88. 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2006.06.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Hartmann T, Schlee W, Weisz N. It’s only in your head: Expectancy of aversive auditory stimulation modulates stimulus-induced auditory cortical alpha desynchronization. NeuroImage. 2012;60: 170–178. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.De Cesarei A, Codispoti M. Affective modulation of the LPP and α-ERD during picture viewing: Affective modulation of ERPs and EEG oscillations. Psychophysiology. 2011;48: 1397–1404. 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01204.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Hauck M, Domnick C, Lorenz J, Gerloff C, Engel AK. Top-down and bottom-up modulation of pain-induced oscillations. Front Hum Neurosci. 2015;9:375 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00375 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Pinheiro ES dos S, Queirós FC de, Montoya P, Santos CL, Nascimento MA do, Ito CH, et al. Electroencephalographic Patterns in Chronic Pain: A Systematic Review of the Literature. Schalk G, editor. PLOS ONE. 2016;11: e0149085 10.1371/journal.pone.0149085 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Morton D, Jones A, Sandhu J. Brain imaging of pain: state of the art. J Pain Res. 2016;9:613–624. 10.2147/JPR.S60433 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Ahern GL, Schwartz GE. Differential lateralization for positive versus negative emotion. Neuropsychologia. 1979;17:693–698. 10.1016/0028-3932(79)90045-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Riha C, Güntensperger D, Kleinjung T, Meyer M. Accounting for Heterogeneity: Mixed-Effects Models in Resting-State EEG Data in a Sample of Tinnitus Sufferers. Brain Topogr. 2020;33:413–424. 10.1007/s10548-020-00772-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Balkenhol T, Wallhäusser-Franke E, Delb W. Psychoacoustic Tinnitus Loudness and Tinnitus-Related Distress Show Different Associations with Oscillatory Brain Activity. Langguth B, editor. PLoS ONE. 2013;8: e53180 10.1371/journal.pone.0053180 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Sörnmo L, Laguna P.The electroencephalogram—A brief background In: Sörnmo L, Laguna P, editors. Bioelectrical signal processing in cardiac and neurological applications. Burlington: Academic Press; 2005. pp. 25–51. [Google Scholar]
  • 123.May ES, Nickel MM, Ta Dinh S, Tiemann L, Heitmann H, Voth I, et al. Prefrontal gamma oscillations reflect ongoing pain intensity in chronic back pain patients. Hum Brain Mapp. 2019;40: 293–305. 10.1002/hbm.24373 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Schulz E, May ES, Postorino M, Tiemann L, Nickel MM, Witkovsky V, et al. Prefrontal Gamma Oscillations Encode Tonic Pain in Humans. Cereb Cortex. 2015;25: 4407–4414. 10.1093/cercor/bhv043 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Zhou R, Wang J, Qi W, Liu F-Y, Yi M, Guo H, et al. Elevated Resting State Gamma Oscillatory Activities in Electroencephalogram of Patients With Post-herpetic Neuralgia. Front Neurosci. 2018;12: 750 10.3389/fnins.2018.00750 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Peng W, Hu L, Zhang Z, Hu Y. Changes of Spontaneous Oscillatory Activity to Tonic Heat Pain. Zuo X-N, editor. PLoS ONE. 2014;9: e91052 10.1371/journal.pone.0091052 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.Cavanna AE, Trimble MR. The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain. 2006;129: 564–583. 10.1093/brain/awl004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Posner MI, Petersen SE. The Attention System of the Human Brain. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1990;13:25–42. 10.1146/annurev.ne.13.030190.000325 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Harmony T. The functional significance of delta oscillations in cognitive processing. Front Integr Neurosci. 2013;7:83 10.3389/fnint.2013.00083 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 130.Howe RC, Sterman MB. Cortical-subcortical EEG correlates of suppressed motor behavior during sleep and waking in the cat. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1972;32: 681–695. 10.1016/0013-4694(72)90104-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 131.Pfurtscheller G, Brunner C, Schlögl A, Lopes da Silva FH. Mu rhythm (de)synchronization and EEG single-trial classification of different motor imagery tasks. NeuroImage. 2006;31: 153–159. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 132.Sterman MB, Macdonald LR, Stone RK. Biofeedback training of the sensorimotor electroencephalogram rhythm in man: effects on epilepsy. Epilepsia. 1974;15:395–416. 10.1111/j.1528-1157.1974.tb04016.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 133.Vernon D, Egner T, Cooper N, Compton T, Neilands C, Sheri A, et al. The effect of training distinct neurofeedback protocols on aspects of cognitive performance. Int J Psychophysiol. 2003;47: 75–85. 10.1016/s0167-8760(02)00091-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 134.Egner T, Gruzelier JH. EEG Biofeedback of low beta band components: frequency-specific effects on variables of attention and event-related brain potentials. Clin Neurophysiol. 2004;115(1):131–139. 10.1016/s1388-2457(03)00353-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 135.Egner T, Gruzelier JH. Learned self-regulation of EEG frequency components affects attention and event- related brain potentials in humans. Neuroreport. 2001;12:4155–4159. 10.1097/00001756-200112210-00058 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 136.Collura TF. Siever D. Auditory-visual entrainment in relation to mental health and EEG. In: Budzynski T, Kogan Budzynski H, Evans JR, Abarbanel, editors. Introduction to quantitative EEG and neurofeedback. Advanced theory and applications. 2nd ed Burlington, London, San Diego, New York: Academic Press; 2009. pp. 195–224. [Google Scholar]
  • 137.Behrmann M, Geng JJ, Shomstein S. Parietal cortex and attention. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2004; 14:212–217. 10.1016/j.conb.2004.03.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 138.Lubar JO, Lubar JF. Electroencephalographic biofeedback of SMR and beta for treatment of attention deficit disorders in a clinical setting. Biofeedback Self Regul. 1984;9:1–23. 10.1007/BF00998842 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 139.Linden M, Habib T, Radojevic V. A controlled study of the effects of EEG biofeedback on cognition and behavior of children with attention deficit disorder and learning disabilities.Biofeedback Self Regul. 1996;21:35–49. 10.1007/BF02214148 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 140.Polich J, Kok A. Cognitive and biological determinants of P300: an integrative review. Biol. Psychol. 1995;41:103–146. 10.1016/0301-0511(95)05130-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 141.Donchin E, Coles MGH. Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? Behav Brain Sci. 1988; 11:357–427. [Google Scholar]
  • 142.Bidet-Caulet A, Fischer C, Besle J, Aguera P-E, Giard M-H, Bertrand O. Effects of Selective Attention on the Electrophysiological Representation of Concurrent Sounds in the Human Auditory Cortex. J Neurosci. 2007;27: 9252–9261. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1402-07.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 143.Wang S-Y, Lin I-M, Fan S-Y, Tsai Y-C, Yen C-F, Yeh Y-C, et al. The effects of alpha asymmetry and high-beta down-training neurofeedback for patients with the major depressive disorder and anxiety symptoms. J Affect Disord. 2019;257: 287–296. 10.1016/j.jad.2019.07.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 144.Van de Velde M, van Erp G, Cluitmans PJM. Detection of muscle artefact in the normal human awake EEG. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1998;107: 149–158. 10.1016/S0013-4694(98)00052-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 145.Lutterveld R van, Sommer IEC, Ford JM. The Neurophysiology of Auditory Hallucinations–A Historical and Contemporary Review. Front Psychiatry. 2011;2:28 10.3389/fpsyt.2011.00028 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 146.Santos R, Sanchez T, Bento R, Lucia M. Auditory hallucinations in tinnitus patients: Emotional relationships and depression. Int Arch Otorhinolaryngol. 2013;16: 322–327. 10.7162/S1809-97772012000300004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Manuel S Malmierca

1 May 2020

PONE-D-20-06961

Electrophysiological correlates of focused attention on low- and high-distressed tinnitus.

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Milner,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

It has been unusually difficult to find a second reviewer, but I decided to carry on because reviewer#1 has provided excellent comments that will help you to improve the final version of your interesting manuscript. I am sure that addressing his/her comments will make your manuscript clearer and stronger.

We would appreciate receiving your revised manuscript by Jun 15 2020 11:59PM. When you are ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter.

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). This letter should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Manuscript'.

Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Manuel S. Malmierca, M.D.,Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: General:

This study addresses resting-state EEG in humans subjects with tinnitus, which has been an area of substantial interest and influence in the field of tinnitus neuroscience for at least 2 decades. Unfortunately this field has almost entirely neglected to control for some factors that might well impact greatly on ongoing brain activity, namely hearing loss and, particularly, attention. This study is one of only two to explicitly control and measure the correlates of attention in this context, and is the only one to include the all-important condition of diverting attention away from tinnitus.

Some general notes are below, followed by a moderate number of suggestions that might improve the quality of results and impact of the findings. However, nothing is ‘wrong’ as such with the manuscript in its present form, so I have generally kept these as optional.

Good sample size

Clear novelty over the Neff study (62), and is the first study to contrast attention toward vs away from tinnitus. This is a very important area of resting-state brain activity research in tinnitus, which until the present study and (62) has been totally neglected in the past 2 decades of this line of research.

Quality of writing is satisfactory

Introduction is generally factual, and avoids unwarranted speculation

Subjects well-matched for age, sex, tinnitus variables and hearing

Appropriate and well-performed methods. Appropriate use of statistical tests, and control for multiple comparisons.

The discussion is categorised by finding, but within those categories is rather sprawling and vague, discussing many possibilities for each finding, and arguing in favour of none specifically. However, I do not think there is necessarily anything wrong with this, and it is definitely preferable to confidently stating unjustified conclusions. The discussion in its present form does speculate a lot in many places, including into some rather tenuous explanations, but in every case it is clearly presented as speculation, and therefore I think that is, once again, fine.

Major:

There is little mention made of how the brain changes associated with tinnitus distress compare to those associated with other chronic aversive sensory conditions, such as chronic pain, on which there is a substantial literature. When I last looked at this, albeit some years ago, there were striking parallels. I do not feel strongly that a discussion of this should necessarily be added to the manuscript, as it would require significant work, but if included I think it would make the manuscript considerably more interesting and useful, as it would help determine the extent to which the observed results are generic features of distress or chronic symptoms (more likely) or specific to tinnitus (less likely).

Was any analysis done on gamma band activity? There has been a lot of interest in resting-state EEG gamma in tinnitus, and it would be useful to know whether the present findings can contribute to this ongoing area of interest.

It is a shame that all analysis has been performed in sensor space as opposed to source space. As there is such a loose correspondence between specific electrodes and brain areas, this means that it is hard to compare the present findings with most of the previous literature, which was presented in source space, mainly using sLORETA. Again this is optional (PLoS One only requires the experiments to be properly conducted and reported), but the findings would be so very much more useful, interesting and impactful if an sLORETA analysis were performed. The sLORETA software is very easy and quick to use, free, and accepts data in a .txt file format following simple conventions such that it can easily accept preprocessed data from other software packages. Reanalysis with sLORETA should take less than a week, and would add so much. But, this is the authors’ decision.

I rarely say this in peer reviews, but I wonder if the authors’ analysis has been overly conservative, in that I do not think depression and anxiety scores necessarily need to be factored out when conducting the statistical analyses. Yes, they are correlated to tinnitus distress, but unless the subjects had pre-existing mental health conditions prior to tinnitus then these scores are probably consequences of tinnitus distress (or at least an inherent part of the mechanism of tinnitus distress), and hence factoring them out will probably remove a lot of variance from the data that genuinely reflects the variable of interest (tinnitus distress). Other variables (age, sex, hearing, duration, ear) did not vary significantly between groups, and therefore I would be satisfied to just leave it at that and not address them in the analysis. With this in mind, I am not sure the PCA-based analysis is necessary, but I will leave this to the authors’ discretion.

I also wonder (though am not sure) if the use of ANOVA here has applied too harsh a statistical penalty. We must bear in mind that the different ROIs, and different frequency bands, are far from independent. This is because many effects are common across multiple frequency bands, and most brain sources project much more widely to the scalp than just one of the authors’ ROIs as used here. I believe that the ANOVA will assume independence of frequencies and ROIs, and therefore its results will be much too conservative. In situations such as these, I tend to base all my stats on non-parametric permutation testing (i.e. run the analysis, say, 1000 times over but randomising the subject group labels each time, and taking the maximum group difference observed in each permutation to construct a null distribution, and finally use the 0.05 position in the sorted null distribution as the significance threshold for the real data). This technique works across all types of data, makes no assumptions about distributions, corrects appropriately for multiple comparisons, and is well-suited to large multivariate datasets. Once again, I do not feel strongly that the authors do this in this case, but if they choose not to then it should be made clear how conservative the results are likely to have been as a result of the methods chosen, and how this impacts on the interpretation of any negative results in particular frequency bands and/or brain locations. Note that sLORETA has this method, or an equivalent, built into its functionality, should the authors decide to follow my recommendation to perform a source space analysis.

Related to my comment in the ‘general’ section, regarding the discussion. Given the lack of a clear unifying interpretation of the findings, the authors might consider adopting a ‘less is more’ approach, and trying to keep the discussion brief and tidy, as the huge number of potential, and potentially conflicting, speculative interpretations in the present version is hard to derive any kind of take-home message from, and therefore does not really give any additional value to the reader. However, it is the authors’ right to speculate as much as they wish, and I will not insist on this section being changed.

Minor:

Two successive sentences in the abstract describe results with and without different degrees of correction, and appear to contradict each other. I would advise sticking to just one analysis, hence one set of findings, in the abstract. As I argue in ‘major’, I would for once advocate focusing on the less conservative analysis.

In the introduction, discussion of the resting-state EEG literature does not mention that all these prior studies failed to control for hearing loss (and sometimes even for age), and omits Adjamian et al. (2012 – JARO) which is rare in actually controlling for hearing loss (and did not replicate most of the previously reported difference).

The introduction presents the thalamocortical dysrhythmia theory as if it is accepted fact. I would suggest stating it as a possible theory, and also perhaps acknowledging that the studies supporting it did not control for hearing loss (or attention!), and likewise that they only recorded from cortex, to the link to thalamus is speculative.

The statement “probably due to impaired habituation” should be rephrased, as by definition what the authors are describing is impaired habituation.

The statement “whereas less persistent tinnitus is associated”: the authors presumably mean ‘less bothersome’, or noticeable a smaller proportion of the time.

Line 102 should read “… is related to…”

129: “highly” not “high”

135: why “paradoxical”? It seems intuitive that this should be the case

142: “suggest” not “confirm”

145: “correlates of their EEG” does not make sense. Do the authors mean “EEG correlates of tinnitus-directed attention” or similar?

Lines 148-151: The rationale is not made clear for why the present study should provide insights not already provided by the Neff study (62). In my mind, the novelties of the present study are the stratification by distress level, and the use of an ‘attend-away’ condition (the body-focused condition). In principle, there might not be much or any difference between attending to tinnitus vs. resting state if subjects are already attending to their tinnitus in resting state, whereas a condition requiring attention away from tinnitus might be more likely to reveal differences.

Was any verification made, after the experiment, of how much subjects adhered to the instructed focus of attention for each block (and whether they had their eyes open or closed)?

Where statistical results are reported, are the authors able to provide p values? Readers will find these the easiest measures to gain a quantitative impression of. I note there are p values in the figures, but it would be nice to have them in the text also.

Line 504: I wouldn’t make too much of ref (39), because in hindsight the results almost certainly just indicated an excess of jaw muscle artefact in the tinnitus group

My PDF file did not seem to include any captions for the main text figures. I assume that these are present somewhere.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: William Sedley

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files to be viewed.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email us at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2020 Aug 5;15(8):e0236521. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0236521.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


2 Jul 2020

We would like to thank the Reviewer for thorough reading of our manuscript and for kind comments and constructive suggestions. We really appreciate all of them and consider as very helpful for improving the manuscript. We agree with all comments and we have revised the manuscript accordingly. We responded to the comments and suggestions chronologically, beginning from the major remarks. Our responses follow the Reviewer’s comments (in italics).

Major remarks

“There is little mention made of how the brain changes associated with tinnitus distress compare to those associated with other chronic aversive sensory conditions, such as chronic pain, on which there is a substantial literature. When I last looked at this, albeit some years ago, there were striking parallels. I do not feel strongly that a discussion of this should necessarily be added to the manuscript, as it would require significant work, but if included I think it would make the manuscript considerably more interesting and useful, as it would help determine the extent to which the observed results are generic features of distress or chronic symptoms (more likely) or specific to tinnitus (less likely).”

Replay:

Thank you for this suggestion. Since we agree that tinnitus and other aversive sensory conditions (especially chronic pain) have a lot in common, we’ve added some paragraphs addressing this issue to the Discussion section (page 20; line 459-469 – marked-up copy of manuscript).

“Was any analysis done on gamma band activity? There has been a lot of interest in resting-state EEG gamma in tinnitus, and it would be useful to know whether the present findings can contribute to this ongoing area of interest.”

Replay:

Yes, the current version of the manuscript contains the outcomes of both EEG power and current density (sLORETA) analysis for 8 frequency bands (including gamma).

„It is a shame that all analysis has been performed in sensor space as opposed to source space. As there is such a loose correspondence between specific electrodes and brain areas, this means that it is hard to compare the present findings with most of the previous literature, which was presented in source space, mainly using sLORETA. Again this is optional (PLoS One only requires the experiments to be properly conducted and reported), but the findings would be so very much more useful, interesting and impactful if an sLORETA analysis were performed. The sLORETA software is very easy and quick to use, free, and accepts data in a .txt file format following simple conventions such that it can easily accept preprocessed data from other software packages. Reanalysis with sLORETA should take less than a week, and would add so much. But, this is the authors’ decision.”

Replay:

In the “Limitations of the study” section in previous version of the manuscript we’ve mentioned about calculating LORETA and presenting the results in our next work. However, following the suggestion, we have added the outcomes of current density analysis to this manuscript. At the same time, because after re-computing EEG power data, some new interesting effects came up, we’ve decided to include the results of both analyses, in sensor and source space.

“I rarely say this in peer reviews, but I wonder if the authors’ analysis has been overly conservative, in that I do not think depression and anxiety scores necessarily need to be factored out when conducting the statistical analyses. Yes, they are correlated to tinnitus distress, but unless the subjects had pre-existing mental health conditions prior to tinnitus then these scores are probably consequences of tinnitus distress (or at least an inherent part of the mechanism of tinnitus distress), and hence factoring them out will probably remove a lot of variance from the data that genuinely reflects the variable of interest (tinnitus distress). Other variables (age, sex, hearing, duration, ear) did not vary significantly between groups, and therefore I would be satisfied to just leave it at that and not address them in the analysis. With this in mind, I am not sure the PCA-based analysis is necessary, but I will leave this to the authors’ discretion.”

Replay:

Actually, those of us who are clinicians working with tinnitus patients, are convinced that when examining their brain activity, it is always worth to consider the tinnitus characteristics (etiology, duration, persistence, etc.) and accompanying symptoms (in addition to the results of audiological tests, the measures of depression and anxiety levels, an ability to cope with stress, etc.). It happens that these psychological measures are more useful for proper diagnosis and treatment than just the subjectively-reported tinnitus intrusiveness (by the way a psychological interview would be more informative than any questionnaires). However, we agree that in this study, where the compared groups are well matched in terms of demographic variables (sex and age), the audiogram profile (up to 8 kHz) and tinnitus duration, as well as the anxiety and depression levels in the whole sample are relatively low (suggesting that they are simply a consequence of tinnitus distress) it is not necessary to include all these variables into the statistical model. Therefore, we decided to remove from the manuscript the paragraphs concerning the data computations when all these factors were controlled. We also gave up the results of PCA-based analysis.

“I also wonder (though am not sure) if the use of ANOVA here has applied too harsh a statistical penalty. We must bear in mind that the different ROIs, and different frequency bands, are far from independent. This is because many effects are common across multiple frequency bands, and most brain sources project much more widely to the scalp than just one of the authors’ ROIs as used here. I believe that the ANOVA will assume independence of frequencies and ROIs, and therefore its results will be much too conservative. In situations such as these, I tend to base all my stats on non-parametric permutation testing (i.e. run the analysis, say, 1000 times over but randomising the subject group labels each time, and taking the maximum group difference observed in each permutation to construct a null distribution, and finally use the 0.05 position in the sorted null distribution as the significance threshold for the real data). This technique works across all types of data, makes no assumptions about distributions, corrects appropriately for multiple comparisons, and is well-suited to large multivariate datasets. Once again, I do not feel strongly that the authors do this in this case, but if they choose not to then it should be made clear how conservative the results are likely to have been as a result of the methods chosen, and how this impacts on the interpretation of any negative results in particular frequency bands and/or brain locations. Note that sLORETA has this method, or an equivalent, built into its functionality, should the authors decide to follow my recommendation to perform a source space analysis.”

Replay:

Thank you for this suggestion. In fact while analyzing the EEG data we had a feeling that we could get more effects from our dataset than we reported in the previous version of the manuscript. Therefore, intending to be not very far from the analysis that we did last time, we decided to calculate repeated measures ANOVA on QEEG data and also provide p-values based on the permutation methods that handle nuisance variables. This approach allows to reveal some new significant effects that also need to be discussed (please find the parts of the Results and Discussion Sections written in bold). Following the recommendation we also added sLORETA outcomes to the manuscript.

“Related to my comment in the ‘general’ section, regarding the discussion. Given the lack of a clear unifying interpretation of the findings, the authors might consider adopting a ‘less is more’ approach, and trying to keep the discussion brief and tidy, as the huge number of potential, and potentially conflicting, speculative interpretations in the present version is hard to derive any kind of take-home message from, and therefore does not really give any additional value to the reader. However, it is the authors’ right to speculate as much as they wish, and I will not insist on this section being changed.”

Replay:

In the current version of the manuscript we’ve done our best to make the Discussion section more clear and concise. We've also put much effort to, wherever it was possible, avoid speculative interpretations. The “Conclusions” was added to provide a take-home message.

Minor:

„Two successive sentences in the abstract describe results with and without different degrees of correction, and appear to contradict each other. I would advise sticking to just one analysis, hence one set of findings, in the abstract. As I argue in ‘major’, I would for once advocate focusing on the less conservative analysis.”

Since we decided to re-compute the data, the abstract was also re-written accordingly.

“In the introduction, discussion of the resting-state EEG literature does not mention that all these prior studies failed to control for hearing loss (and sometimes even for age), and omits Adjamian et al. (2012 – JARO) which is rare in actually controlling for hearing loss (and did not replicate most of the previously reported difference).”

Thank you for pointed out this issue. We added a sentence about this to Introduction (page 4; lines 89-90 – marked-up copy of manuscript).

“The introduction presents the thalamocortical dysrhythmia theory as if it is accepted fact. I would suggest stating it as a possible theory, and also perhaps acknowledging that the studies supporting it did not control for hearing loss (or attention!), and likewise that they only recorded from cortex, to the link to thalamus is speculative.”

We are sorry if we’ve made an impression that we consider the thalamocortical dysrhythmia theory as widely accepted. We are aware of the fact that this model is only one possible among others (we’ve already mentioned about them in Introduction, lines 75-78). Therefore, we corrected a little bit a paragraph that covers the theories explaining tinnitus (lines 66-78) and also a sentence elsewhere in the text (lines 83-84).

“The statement “probably due to impaired habituation” should be rephrased, as by definition what the authors are describing is impaired habituation”.

Thank you. We’ve corrected this sentence (lines 91-92).

“The statement “whereas less persistent tinnitus is associated”: the authors presumably mean ‘less bothersome’, or noticeable a smaller proportion of the time.”

Yes, of course we meant “less bothersome” not “less persistent” tinnitus. Thank you for catching this mistake.

“Line 102 should read “… is related to…

The suggested correction has been made.

“129: “highly” not “high””.

This has been corrected (line 137).

“135: why “paradoxical”? It seems intuitive that this should be the case”

The “paradoxical” has been removed from this sentence. In this context this word truly makes no sense.

“142: “suggest” not “confirm””

This correction has been made (line 150).

“145: “correlates of their EEG” does not make sense. Do the authors mean “EEG correlates of tinnitus-directed attention” or similar?”

Thank you, we fully agree with that. This sentence has been re-written (line 152).

“Lines 148-151: The rationale is not made clear for why the present study should provide insights not already provided by the Neff study (62). In my mind, the novelties of the present study are the stratification by distress level, and the use of an ‘attend-away’ condition (the body-focused condition). In principle, there might not be much or any difference between attending to tinnitus vs. resting state if subjects are already attending to their tinnitus in resting state, whereas a condition requiring attention away from tinnitus might be more likely to reveal differences.”

Thank you for bringing up this important issue. Resting-state brain activity is defined as activity in the brain when a subject is awake but not performing a specific cognitive task or responding to sensory stimuli (e.g. Fox & Greicius, 2010). Indeed, in this context in the present study EEG data were recorded during the resting-state (understood as the absence of overt task performance or stimulation). It is also true that we expected to observe the most pronounced differences in the brain activity of high distress patients between the conditions when they were attending to, or not, to their tinnitus. We are not, however, convinced that a state of concentrating on tinnitus is the same as the “resting state” (even in bothersome tinnitus), especially when, as it was in the current study, patients were asked to keep their eyes closed (this condition favors focusing on the inner world). We believe that in our study a way of formulating an instruction given to a patient, actually mattered. Typically when we measure spontaneous brain activity (with the use of EEG or fMRI) we ask a subject to sit (or lie) with eyes open, eyes fixated on a target or eyes closed, and relax, not thinking about anything special. We have had some experience in conducting resting-state studies either with the use of EEG or fMRI technique, and it is almost impossible to request subjects only to keep their eyes open or closed because they often ask what exactly they are supposed to do during the experiment. While preparing the current study we were concerned that such instruction might be misunderstood, especially by patients with high tinnitus-related distress. Specifically they might have found this instruction encouraging not to focus on tinnitus (which could have been related to much cognitive effort reflected in EEG activity). Therefore, we decided to compare brain activity between more “controlled” resting-state conditions.

“Was any verification made, after the experiment, of how much subjects adhered to the instructed focus of attention for each block (and whether they had their eyes open or closed)?”

Our participants kept their eyes closed during both blocks of recording EEG signal (line 231). We did not mention about it in the previous version of the manuscript but immediately after completing each block all patients were requested to assess how well they had been able to follow the instruction on the 5-point scale from “1” (completely failed) to “5” (completely done). Only those persons who reported “5” or “4” (almost completely done) in both conditions were included to the study. However, since only few persons admitted that they failed to follow the instruction, we were not sure whether other declarations were true and, therefore, we finally decided to not insert this information into the manuscript (and we would like to keep it that way). In future studies we should definitely consider a better verification procedure of the instruction adherence.

“Where statistical results are reported, are the authors able to provide p values? Readers will find these the easiest measures to gain a quantitative impression of. I note there are p values in the figures, but it would be nice to have them in the text also.”

In the current version of the manuscript p values were included in both figures and in the text (please note this in the Results section).

“Line 504: I wouldn’t make too much of ref (39), because in hindsight the results almost certainly just indicated an excess of jaw muscle artefact in the tinnitus group”

We take this hint into consideration and we did not discuss our results with the reference to this paper in the he current version of the manuscript.

“My PDF file did not seem to include any captions for the main text figures. I assume that these are present somewhere.”.

We are very sorry for that. We hope that in the corrected manuscript the captions for all figures are in the right place. In accordance with the Plos One guidelines, we included them in the text of the manuscript.

References

Fox MD, Greicius M. Clinical applications of resting state functional connectivity. Front Syst Neurosci. 2010;4:19. Published 2010 Jun 17. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2010.00019

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to reviewers.docx

Decision Letter 1

Manuel S Malmierca

9 Jul 2020

Electrophysiological correlates of focused attention on low- and high-distressed tinnitus.

PONE-D-20-06961R1

Dear Dr. Milner,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Manuel S. Malmierca, M.D.,Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: I really appreciate the authors' careful consideration of all my points, and the substantial work and effort they have undergone in order to fully embrace and address these.

The new version of the manuscript is much improved by the additional source space analysis, and more relaxed statistical analyses, and the results that these have produced. I also note the numerous instances of rewording in the manuscript which has helped it read in a more even-handed way (though this was never much of an issue to begin with).

The synthesis of results, and discussion, seem to deliver more of a clear take-home message too.

Overall, I continue to like this manuscript, and think it has been greatly further improved by the additional work and effort. I am enthusiastic to recommend publication in its present form.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: William Sedley

Acceptance letter

Manuel S Malmierca

17 Jul 2020

PONE-D-20-06961R1

Electrophysiological correlates of focused attention on low- and high-distressed tinnitus.

Dear Dr. Milner:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Prof. Dr. Manuel S. Malmierca

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Table. The mean absolute power and standard error (in brackets) values for different frequency bands in the contrast: Body Focus Condition (BFC) versus tinnitus focus condition (TFC), calculated in each cluster for the whole study sample (n = 67).

    (PDF)

    S2 Table. Post-hoc pairwise t-tests results for the contrast: Body Focus Condition (BFC) versus Tinnitus Focus Condition (TFC) calculated in each cluster for the whole study sample (n = 67).

    Items in bold are significant based on p-values.

    (PDF)

    S3 Table. The mean absolute power and standard error (in brackets) values for different frequency bands in the contrast: Body Focus Condition (BFC) versus Tinnitus Focus Condition (TFC), calculated in each cluster separately for the low tinnitus-related distress (LD) and high tinnitus-related distress (HD) group.

    (PDF)

    S4 Table. Post-hoc pairwise t-tests results for the contrast: Body Focus Condition (BFC) versus Tinnitus Focus Condition (TFC), calculated in each cluster separately for the high tinnitus-related distress (HD) and low tinnitus-related distress (LD) group.

    Items in bold are significant based on p-values.

    (PDF)

    S5 Table. The mean absolute power and standard error (in brackets) values for different frequency bands in the contrast: High tinnitus-related distress (HD) versus low tinnitus-related distress (LD) group, calculated in each cluster separately for the Body Focus Condition (BFC) and Tinnitus Focus Condition (TFC).

    (PDF)

    S6 Table. Post-hoc pairwise t-tests results for the contrast: High tinnitus-related distress (HD) versus low tinnitus-related distress (LD) group, calculated in each cluster separately for the Body Focus Condition (BFC) and Tinnitus Focus Condition (TFC).

    Items in bold are significant based on p-values.

    (PDF)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to reviewers.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    All data used in the analysis is available on figShare database with the accompanying DOI: https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.12643403.v1.


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES