Abstract
Intimate partner violence (IPV) among male couples is increasingly recognized as a public health concern. Research on IPV in opposite sex couples indicates frequent underreporting of IPV and high levels of discordance in reporting among dyads. Concordance studies inform refinement methods to measure the experience of IPV among dyads; however the lack of dyadic studies of male couples impedes our understanding of the extent to which IPV is differentially reported in male-male dyads. This study utilized baseline data from a randomized controlled trial of a behavioral intervention to optimize antiretroviral therapy (ART) adherence among 160 sero-discordant male couples in three US cities and provides the first analysis of concordance in reporting IPV among male couples. Low degrees of concordance in the reporting of IPV were identified among male dyads, with a greater proportion of men reporting violence perpetration than experiencing violence. The greater reporting of IPV perpetration may be linked to adherence to concepts of masculinity. The results underscore the unique experiences of IPV among male couples and the need to reexamine current IPV measurement and intervention strategies.
Keywords: Intimate partner violence, men who have sex with men, concordance, dyadic
Introduction
Over the past ten years there has been a growth in research illustrating that men who have sex with men (MSM) experience intimate partner violence (IPV) at rates that are substantially higher than those experienced by men who do not have sex with men, and rates that are comparable or higher to those among heterosexual women (Finneran & Stephenson, 2013). Estimated prevalence for the recent (past 12 month) receipt of IPV among MSM range from 12% (Stephenson, Khosropour, & Sullivan, 2010) to 45% (Craft & Serovich, 2005) for physical IPV, 1.8% (Bartholomew, Regan, White, & Oram, 2008) to 33% (Craft & Serovich, 2005) for sexual IPV, and 28% (Pruitt, White, Mitchell, & Stephenson, 2015) to 64% (Bartholomew et al., 2008) for emotional/psychological. Prevalence rates for recent (12 month) experience of any form of IPV range from 32% (Houston & McKirnan, 2007) to 54% (Pantalone, Schneider, Valentine, & Simoni, 2012). Perpetration rates of violence have been comparatively less studied among MSM, and estimates for recent (past 12 months) range from to 8.3% (Carvalho, Lewis, Derlega, Winstead, & Viggiano, 2011) to 36% (Welles, Corbin, Rich, Reed, & Raj, 2011) – both substantially higher than those reported by men who have sex with women, yet similar to the prevalence reported by women in opposite sex relationships. The large variation in estimates of experience and perpetration rates for IPV among MSM reflects variations in sample sizes and populations, with higher rates identified among clinic populations versus population-based estimates. Of particular importance to MSM is emergent evidence demonstrating a link between IPV and risk for HIV infection (Feldman, Ream, Díaz, & El-Bassel, 2008; Greenwood et al., 2002; Koblin et al., 2006; Relf, 2001; Stephenson, de Voux, & Sullivan, 2011).
As an illegal and socially taboo behavior, IPV is often differentially reported by partners with inter-partner agreement on whether IPV exists in a relationship frequently low (Halim, Steven, Reich, Badi, & Messersmith, 2018). Several studies consistently report low agreement between opposite-sex partners on the presence of IPV in their relationship, with differential reporting of perpetration violence being more common than the differential reporting of victimization (Panuzio et al., 2006; Shafer, Caetano and & Clark, 2002: Freeman, Schumacher, & Coffey, 2015; Halim et al., 2018; Marshall, Panuzio, Makin-Byrd, Taft, & Holtzworth- Munroe, 2011; Strandmoen, Askeland, Tjersland, Wentzel-Larsen, & Heir, 2016). However, there is some evidence that women report both higher levels of experience of IPV than the levels of perpetration reported by men (Desmarais et al., 2012a: Desmarais et al., 2012b). Reports of both experience and perpetration of IPV may be impacted by social desirability bias (Freeman et al., 2015), as well as misinterpretation of questions or forgetting instances of IPV, a common limitation of retrospective self-reports (Halim et al., 2018). Low levels of agreement between partners in the reporting of IPV is a serious limitation to the accurate reporting of IPV prevalence. Accurate measures of IPV prevalence are needed to inform the development of interventions for primary and secondary prevention of IPV, especially in surveys where only one partner responds. Understanding the extent of disagreement in the reporting of IPV among partners, and the degree of under-reporting of both victimization and perpetration, are important steps in refining and developing more accurate methods to measure the experience of IPV among dyads (Halim et al., 2018) that can inform the development and content of IPV reduction interventions for male couples.
Research examining the under-reporting of IPV among dyads has to date focused on male-female couples, and we are not aware of any studies have examined the under-reporting of IPV among two men. Studies of the experience of IPV among MSM have largely collected data from individuals, not male dyads. A male dyad refers to a male-male relationship and does not connote a specific identity: men in a male dyad may or may not have a gay/ bisexual identity, or may be behaviorally MSM. For brevity, we refer to male couples in this paper as two men in a self-reported relationship with no indication of what this means for their sexual identity. While some recent studies have sought to understand dyadic factors shaping the experience of IPV among male dyads, these have done so by collecting data from only one member of the couple, and asking them to provide data on their partner (Stephenson, Rentsch, Salazar, & Sullivan, 2011; Stephenson, Sato, & Finneran, 2013). The lack of dyadic studies that have collected IPV data from both members of the male couple has limited the ability to assess the agreement between partners on IPV experience and perpetration in male couples. The current study uses baseline data from a randomized controlled trial (RCT) of male couples enrolled from three US cities (Atlanta, Boston and Chicago) collected from both members of the dyad about recent victimization and experience of IPV, to quantify the degree of partner agreement around presence of IPV in the relationship. This study is the first to analyze concordance in reporting IPV among MSM couples, and has to the potential to inform the development and refinement of measures for accurately assessing IPV in male couples.
Methods
This analysis uses data from the baseline survey of Stronger Together, an ongoing RCT of male couples in Atlanta, Boston and Chicago (clinicaltrials.org reference #NCT01772992) (Stephenson et al., 2017). Male couples were recruited through online and in-person outreach efforts, including Facebook, Twitter, Scruff, Grindr and social marketing campaigns of the sites conducting the intervention. Additionally, flyers and posters were displayed in the sites and at local gay-targeted venues. Information on the study was also displayed at the HIV testing check-in at each site.
The RCT recruited male couples, defined as two cisgender males (assigned male sex at birth and currently identify as male), aged at least 18 years old, and who reported being in a partnership for at least one month. Partnership was assessed as “Do you have a primary male partner, that is, someone you feel emotionally, romantically committed to above others?” Participants had to be residents of metro Atlanta, Boston or Chicago for at least 3 months. Those who presented for participation in the trial completed informed consent documentation before taking the baseline survey. The informed consent and survey processes were conducted with each member of the dyad in a separate room. Of the 410 individuals who presented for participation, 398 consented and were eligible to take the baseline survey (97%). Of the 398 participants who completed the baseline survey, 78 individuals (20%) were excluded from analysis due to missing responses. The most common areas of missing data were in employment, age and HIV status. Sensitivity analysis determined that the missing data were missing completely at random (MCAR) and therefore multiple imputation techniques were not applied. Importantly, respondents with missing data were not significantly different in their reporting of IPV than those with complete data. Data from 160 male couples were analyzed as dyads, with the responses of each partner used to create measures of dyadic differences. A description of the protocol for the full study can be found elsewhere (Stephenson et al., 2017). Approval for this study was obtained from the Michigan Institutional Review Board.
Participants self-reported demographic and socio-economic characteristics including age, race/ethnicity, education level, employment status, annual household income, arrest history, alcohol and drug use, sexual orientation, and HIV status. The survey measured both recent (previous 12 months) experience (victimization) and perpetration of IPV using the Gay and Bisexual Men (IPV-GBM) scale (Experience: Cronbach’s alpha > 0.78: Perpetration Cronbach’s alpha >0.76) adapted from the Conflict Tactics scale to more accurately measure IPV among gay and bisexual men (Stephenson & Finneran, 2013). The IPV-GBM scale was developed to measure the experience of IPV in male couples. Full details of the scale development and validation can be found at Stephenson & Finneran (2013). Briefly, ten focus group discussions were held with men in Atlanta to explore perceptions of IPV: men listed actions they felt would constitute IPV. More than 30 items were generated from the focus groups, and after expert review to remove repeat and similar items, 23 IPV actions were identified. These items were included in a survey of 1,075 MSM in Atlanta, who were asked if they endorsed each item as IPV, and whether they had experienced or perpetrated each item in the last 12 months. Factor analysis conducted on the survey data: the IPV-GMB scale produced from this analysis consists of 23 items over four domains of IPV. The IPV-GBM has been used in samples of MSM and gay/ bisexual men to measure the recent prevalence of IPV and to explore associations between IPV and HIV risk behaviors (Stephenson et al., 2013: Stephenson et al., 2017: Stephenson and Finneran 2017a: Stephenson and Finneran 2017b: Davis et al., 2016). The scale encompasses 23 measures over four domains of IPV: physical and sexual (e.g. “[partner] punched, hit, or slapped you?”, “ [partner] raped you?”), emotional (“e.g. “ [partner] told you to ‘act straight’ around certain people?”, “ [partner] called you fat or ugly?”), controlling (e.g. “[partner] prevented you from seeing your family?”, “[partner] prevented you from seeing your friends?”), and monitoring (e.g. “[partner] demanded access to you email?”, “[partner] read your text messages without your knowledge?”). Respondents provided a nickname for their partner, which was then piped into the question to replace [partner]. Questions were asked about the experience of IPV in the past 12 months and the perpetration of IPV in the past 12 months, as separate sets of questions (“Has your partner done any of the following to you in the past 12 months?”, “Have you done any of the following to your partner in the past 12 months?”. Responses to these questions were compared to determine agreement in reporting of IPV experience and perpetration among dyads.
Statistical analyses were conducted using SAS 9.4. Descriptive statistics were used to summarize the demographic characteristics of the sample, as well as participants’ experience and perpetration of IPV. Three measures were calculated to assess concordance between partner responses to questions around IPV. Observed agreement (P0) was obtained by taking the ratio of the number of responses for which both partners agreed to the total number of responses. One limitation of this measure is that it does not account for the possibility that sometimes partners might agree on a specific characteristic solely due to chance. Cohen’s kappa statistic (K) was calculated as the ratio of P0 minus the chance-expected agreement and 1 minus the chance-expected agreement, and corrects for agreement expected to occur by chance. Cohen’s kappa statistic (K), introduced in 1960 (Cohen, 1960), corrects for the amount of agreement that can be expected to occur by chance, and has been frequently employed to measure inter-rater reliability in clinical studies (Brennan & Hays, 1992; Sim & Wright, 2005). A common criticism of K is that it is highly dependent on prevalence, defined as the probability with which a specific characteristic is classified into a particular response category (Cicchetti & Feinstein, 1990; Feinstein & Cicchetti, 1990). Variations in prevalence might result in low values of K for some characteristics, but this does not necessarily reflect a low level of agreement between partners. The prevalence-adjusted bias-adjusted kappa statistic (PABAK), proposed by Byrt in 1993 (Byrt, Bishop, & Carlin, 1993), accounts for imbalances caused by differences in prevalence while assuming the absence of any systematic errors in classification. Prevalence-adjusted bias-adjusted kappa statistic (PABAK), was obtained by subtracting 1 from two times the value of P0, and accounts for imbalances caused by differences in prevalence while assuming the absence of any systematic errors in classification. PABAK reporting is being increasingly recommended as a supplement to K in healthcare research (Girianelli & Santos Thuler, 2007; Mak, Yau, & Chan, 2004). PABAK measures the degree of concordance in the reporting of IPV, while accounting for the underlying differences in prevalence. McHugh’s (2012) recommendations were used to interpret PBAK values as follows: ≤0.59 indicates “weak” concordance, 0.60–0.79 indicates “moderate” concordance, and ≥0.80 indicates “strong” concordance.
All three concordance measures were calculated for the overall sample. Variations in the reporting of IPV may be shaped by different life experiences within couples, i.e. one member of the couple may be older, and have more experience of relationships. Variations in the reporting of IPV may also vary by factors that shape the risk of IPV, i.e. couples with open sexual agreements may experience more conflict than monogamous couples. Analysis was also stratified by age group match (both partners are in the same age group or partners are in different age groups), race and ethnicity match (same race or different races), dyadic HIV sero-status (sero-concordant negative or sero-discordant), cohabitation status (cohabitate, do not cohabitate, disagree about cohabitation status), and mutual agreement about sex with outside partners (have an agreement, do not have an agreement, disagree about mutual agreement). Agreement was categorized as “poor” (K<0.00), “slight” (0.00≤K≤.20), “fair” (0.21≤K≤0.40), “moderate” (0.41≤K≤0.60), “substantial” (0.61≤K≤0.80) or “almost perfect” (0.81≤K≤1.00) (Landis & Koch, 1977).
Results
Demographic characteristics of the 320 participants are summarized in Table 1. This sample is diverse in terms of age, with 20% aged 18–24 years (65/320), 38% aged 25–34 years (121/320), 18% aged 35–44 years (59/320), and 23% aged 45 years or older (75/320). The sample, however, is mostly white non-Hispanic (72%: 231/320), with 12% identifying as black non-Hispanic (37/320), 9% as white-Hispanic (29/320), and 7% as another race or reporting being multiracial (23/320). The majority of participants were highly educated, with 69% reporting a bachelor’s degree or higher (219/320). Ten percent reported being unemployed, retired, or receiving disability assistance (32/320), and over one-quarter reported having been arrested (27%: 87/320). Average relationship length was approximately 3 years (range 6 months to 11 years). Less than one-quarter reported binge drinking behavior in the past year (22%: 71/320), and less than one-fifth reported polydrug use in the past year (18%: 58/320).
TABLE 1:
Characteristic | n | (%) |
---|---|---|
Age group (years)a | ||
18–24 | 65 | (20) |
25–34 | 121 | (38) |
35–44 | 59 | (18) |
≥ 45 | 75 | (23) |
Race and ethnicity | ||
White, non-Hispanic | 231 | (72) |
Black/African American, non-Hispanic | 37 | (12) |
Hispanic | 29 | (9) |
Otherb | 23 | (7) |
Education | ||
Associate’s/Technical degree or lowerc | 101 | (32) |
Bachelor’s degree | 136 | (43) |
Master’s degree or higher Employment |
83 | (26) |
Employed (full-time or part-time) | 209 | (65) |
Student (full-time or part-time) | 79 | (25) |
Otherd | 32 | (10) |
Annual household income | ||
< $15,000 | 32 | (10) |
$15,001-$50,000 | 110 | (34) |
$50,001-$100,000 | 94 | (29) |
≥ $100,001 | 84 | (26) |
Arrest history | ||
Ever been arrested | 87 | (27) |
Never been arrested | 233 | (73) |
Binge drinking in the past yeare | ||
Yes | 71 | (22) |
No | 249 | (78) |
Polydrug use in the past yearf | ||
Yes | 58 | (18) |
No | 262 | (82) |
Sexual orientation | ||
Homosexual/gay | 287 | (90) |
Otherg | 33 | (10) |
Self-reported HIV status | ||
Negative | 270 | (84) |
Positive | 50 | (16) |
Age: Mean=36, Median=34, Range=19–69.
Includes 15 multiracial, 6 Asian, 1 Native American/Alaskan Native, and 1 other.
Includes 77 with an Associate’s/Technical degree or some college education, 23 with a high school diploma or General Educational Development (GED), and 1 with some high school education.
Includes 17 receiving disability benefits, 12 retired, 2 unemployed, and 1 unknown.
Defined as having 6 or more drinks on a single occasion.
Defined as using 2 or more psychoactive substances in combination on a single occasion.
Includes 18 bisexual, 11 queer, 2 questioning/unsure, and 2 other.
Concordance in reporting IPV
This sample reported a high prevalence of any form of IPV (Table 2), with 41% indicating experiencing any type of IPV with their current partner in the past year (132/320). Emotional IPV was the most commonly reported experience of IPV at 38% (123/320), followed by monitoring IPV at 30% (96/320), physical IPV at 18% (56/320), and controlling IPV at 12% (37/320). In contrast, this sample reported higher prevalence of IPV perpetration, at 50% (160/320). Emotional IPV was the most commonly reported form of IPV perpetrated at 39% (125/320), followed by monitoring IPV at 37% (117/320), physical IPV at 17% (53/320), and controlling IPV at 8% (24/320).
TABLE 2:
Experience IPV | Perpetrate IPV | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Characteristic | n | (%) | n | (5) |
Any type of IPV | 132 | (41) | 160 | (50) |
Physical IPV | 56 | (18) | 53 | (17) |
Have been punched, hit or slapped | 20 | (6) | 21 | (7) |
Have been kicked | 3 | (1) | 6 | (2) |
Have been pushed or shoved | 34 | (11) | 29 | (9) |
Have been forced to do something sexual against my will | 1 | (0) | 0 | (0) |
Have been raped | 0 | (0) | 0 | (0) |
Have had my personal property damaged or destroyed | 38 | (12) | 21 | (7) |
Emotional IPV | 123 | (38) | 125 | (39) |
Have been called fat or ugly | 42 | (13) | 32 | (10) |
Have been asked to “act straight” around certain people | 22 | (7) | 21 | (7) |
Have been criticized about my clothes | 110 | (34) | 117 | (37) |
Controlling IPV | 37 | (12) | 24 | (8) |
Have been prevented from seeing my family | 5 | (2) | 1 | (0) |
Have been prevented from seeing my friends | 24 | (8) | 15 | (5) |
Have been prevented from seeing my partner’s family | 11 | (3) | 8 | (3) |
Have been prevented from seeing my partner’s friends | 13 | (4) | 9 | (3) |
Monitoring IPV | 96 | (30) | 117 | (37) |
Have been demanded to provide access to my cell phone | 46 | (14) | 35 | (11) |
Have been demanded to provide access to my email | 20 | (6) | 18 | (6) |
Have had my text messages read without my knowledge | 71 | (22) | 96 | (30) |
Have had my email read without my knowledge | 42 | (13) | 60 | (19) |
Have had my social networking pages been posted on repeatedly | 31 | (10) | 20 | (6) |
There were generally low levels of agreement between partners’ reports of experience and perpetration of IPV in the relationship (Table 3). Unadjusted kappa values demonstrate only “slight” agreement for reports of controlling IPV (K=.16) and “fair” agreement for any type of IPV (K=.26), physical IPV (K=.27), emotional IPV (K=.25), and monitoring IPV (K=.26). The PABAK, which adjusts for prevalence and bias in the kappa statistic, indicates different levels of agreement with “poor” agreement for any type of IPV (PABAK= −.06), “slight” agreement for emotional IPV (PABAK=.01) and monitoring IPV (PABAK=.05), “fair” agreement for physical IPV (PABAK=.39), and “moderate” agreement for controlling IPV (PABAK=.54).
TABLE 3:
Characteristic | Fa | P0b | Kc | PABAKd |
---|---|---|---|---|
Any type of IPVe | 75/160 | 47 | 0.26 | −0.06 |
Physical IPVe | 111/160 | 69 | 0.27 | 0.39 |
Emotional IPVe | 81/160 | 51 | 0.25 | 0.01 |
Controlling IPVe | 123/160 | 77 | 0.16 | 0.54 |
Monitoring IPVe | 84/160 | 53 | 0.26 | 0.05 |
Frequency of agreement.
Observed percent agreement (%).
Cohen’s kappa statistic.
Prevalence-adjusted bias-adjusted kappa statistic.
Concordance in reporting by age match
Among couples for whom both partners were in the same age group (n=102), unadjusted kappa values generally show “fair” agreement for reporting any IPV, and “slight” agreement for controlling IPV (K=.11) (Table 4). However, adjusted values show “poor” agreement for any type of IPV (PABAK= −.02), “slight” agreement for emotional IPV (PABAK=.02) and monitoring IPV (PABAK=.12), “fair” agreement for physical IPV (PABAK=.27), and “moderate” agreement for controlling IPV (PABAK=.51). Couples with members in different age groups (n=58) showed roughly similar levels of prevalence-adjusted bias-adjusted agreement, except there was higher concordance on physical IPV (PABAK=.59) and lower concordance on monitoring IPV (PABAK= −.07).
TABLE 4:
Characteristic | Fa | P0b | Kc | PABAKd | Fa | P0b | Kc | PABAKd |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age group match |
||||||||
Same (102 couples) | Different (58 couples) | |||||||
Any type of IPVe | 50/102 | 49 | 0.27 | −0.02 | 25/58 | 43 | 0.22 | −0.14 |
Physical IPVe | 65/102 | 64 | 0.27 | 0.27 | 46/58 | 79 | 0.17 | 0.59 |
Emotional IPVe | 52/102 | 51 | 0.27 | 0.02 | 29/58 | 50 | 0.19 | 0.00 |
Controlling IPVe | 77/102 | 75 | 0.11 | 0.51 | 46/58 | 79 | 0.25 | 0.59 |
Monitoring IPVe | 57/102 | 56 | 0.31 | 0.12 | 27/58 | 47 | 0.17 | −0.07 |
Race and ethnicity match |
||||||||
Same (97 couples) | Different (63 couples) | |||||||
Any type of IPVe | 47/97 | 48 | 0.28 | −0.03 | 28/63 | 44 | 0.23 | −0.11 |
Physical IPVe | 63/97 | 65 | 0.25 | 0.30 | 48/63 | 76 | 0.29 | 0.52 |
Emotional IPVe | 46/97 | 47 | 0.21 | −0.05 | 35/63 | 56 | 0.31 | 0.11 |
Controlling IPVe | 78/97 | 80 | 0.17 | 0.61 | 45/63 | 71 | 0.14 | 0.43 |
Monitoring IPVe | 53/97 | 55 | 0.25 | 0.09 | 31/63 | 49 | 0.26 | −0.02 |
Dyadic HIV serostatus |
||||||||
Seroconcordant negative (110 couples) | Serodiscordant (50 couples) | |||||||
Any type of IPVe | 44/110 | 40 | 0.15 | −0.20 | 31/50 | 62 | 0.27 | 0.24 |
Physical IPVe | 80/110 | 73 | 0.30 | 0.45 | 31/50 | 62 | 0.20 | 0.24 |
Emotional IPVe | 51/110 | 46 | 0.20 | −0.07 | 30/50 | 60 | 0.36 | 0.20 |
Controlling IPVe | 84/110 | 76 | 0.19 | 0.53 | 39/50 | 78 | 0.09 | 0.56 |
Monitoring IPVe | 56/110 | 51 | 0.25 | 0.02 | 28/50 | 56 | 0.31 | 0.12 |
Frequency of agreement.
Observed percent agreement (%).
Cohen’s kappa statistic.
Prevalence-adjusted bias-adjusted kappa statistic.
Concordance in reporting by race/ethnicity
Couples of the same race/ethnicity (n=97) exhibited similar “fair” levels of agreement for all measures of IPV, except for controlling IPV with “slight” agreement (K=.17). Adjusted values reveal lower levels of concordance for any type of IPV (PABAK= −.03), emotional IPV (PABAK= −.05), and monitoring IPV (PABAK=.09), while controlling IPV had higher concordance (PABAK=.61). Interracial couples (n=63) only differed from same-race couples in adjusted values of agreement, showing higher agreement for physical IPV (PABAK=.52) and emotional IPV (PABAK=.11), and lower agreement for controlling IPV (PABAK=.43) and monitoring IPV (PABAK= −.02).
Concordance in reporting by couple HIV sero-status
Couples who were sero-concordant negative (n=110) exhibited similarly low levels of concordance as with previous groups (any type K=.15, physical K=.3, emotional K=.2, controlling K=.19, monitoring K=.25). Adjusted values, however, indicate “poor” agreement for any type of IPV (PABAK= −.2) and emotional IPV (PABAK= −.07), “slight” agreement for monitoring IPV (PABAK=.02), and “moderate” agreement for physical IPV (PABAK=.45) and controlling IPV (PABAK=.53). Sero-discordant couples (n=50) showed differences with higher concordance on any type of IPV, both unadjusted (K=.27) and adjusted (PABAK=.24), higher adjusted concordance on emotional IPV (PABAK=.2), and lower adjusted concordance on physical IPV (PABAK=.24).
Concordance in reporting by relationship characteristics
Analyzing differences in relationship characteristics (Table 5) yielded similar results. Looking at couples who cohabitate (n=117), unadjusted concordance was “slight” for physical IPV (K= .19) and controlling IPV (K= .18) and “fair” for any type of IPV (K=.25), emotional IPV (K=.23), and monitoring IPV (K=.23). Adjusted values showed higher agreement on physical IPV (PABAK=.23) and controlling IPV (PABAK=.5), and lower concordance on any type of IPV (PABAK= −.09), emotional IPV (PABAK= −.06), and monitoring IPV (PABAK=−.04). Couples who do not cohabitate (n=34) showed lower unadjusted concordance in any type of IPV (K=.18) and controlling IPV (K= −.06), and unadjusted values were for higher physical IPV (K=.47). Adjusted agreement in this group was higher than in couples who cohabitate for physical IPV (PABAK=.82) and controlling IPV (PABAK=.65), but lower for any type of IPV (PABAK= −.06) and emotional IPV (PABAK= .12). Couples who disagreed on their cohabitation status (n=9) generally had higher levels of agreement about IPV compared to the other two groups. All the unadjusted agreement levels were higher than the other two groups (any type K=.47, physical K=.75, controlling K=.28, monitoring K=.43), except for emotional IPV which showed “fair” agreement as the other two groups (K=.36). The adjusted statistic remained higher than both groups (any type PABAK=.33, emotional PABAK=.56) or about as high as the other groups (physical PABAK=.78, controlling PABAK=.56, monitoring PABAK=.33).
TABLE 5:
Characteristic | Fa | P0b | Kc | PABAKd | Fa | P0b | Kc | PABAKd | Fa | P0b | Kc | PABAKd |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Cohabitation status |
||||||||||||
Cohabitate (117 couples) | Do not cohabitate (34 couples) | Disagree about cohabitation status (9 couples) | ||||||||||
Any type of IPVe | 53/117 | 45 | 0.25 | −0.09 | 16/34 | 47 | 0.18 | −0.06 | 6/9 | 67 | 0.47 | 0.33 |
Physical IPVe | 72/117 | 62 | 0.19 | 0.23 | 31/34 | 91 | 0.47 | 0.82 | 8/9 | 89 | 0.75 | 0.78 |
Emotional IPVe | 55/117 | 47 | 0.23 | −0.06 | 19/34 | 56 | 0.21 | 0.12 | 7/9 | 78 | 0.36 | 0.56 |
Controlling IPVe | 88/117 | 75 | 0.18 | 0.50 | 28/34 | 82 | −0.06 | 0.65 | 7/9 | 78 | 0.28 | 0.56 |
Monitoring IPVe | 56/117 | 48 | 0.23 | −0.04 | 22/34 | 65 | 0.24 | 0.29 | 6/9 | 67 | 0.43 | 0.33 |
Mutual agreement about sex with outside partners |
||||||||||||
Have an agreement (110 couples) | Do not have an agreement (10 couples) | Disagree about mutual agreement (40 couples) | ||||||||||
Any type of IPVe | 45/110 | 41 | 0.20 | −0.18 | 7/10 | 70 | 0.52 | 0.40 | 23/40 | 58 | 0.36 | 0.15 |
Physical IPVe | 76/110 | 69 | 0.30 | 0.38 | 8/10 | 80 | 0.51 | 0.60 | 27/40 | 68 | 0.08 | 0.35 |
Emotional IPVe | 54/110 | 49 | 0.23 | −0.02 | 6/10 | 60 | 0.19 | 0.20 | 22/40 | 55 | 0.31 | 0.10 |
Controlling IPVe | 87/110 | 79 | 0.13 | 0.58 | 8/10 | 80 | −0.11 | 0.60 | 28/40 | 70 | 0.22 | 0.40 |
Monitoring IPVe | 53/110 | 48 | 0.24 | −0.04 | 7/10 | 70 | 0.39 | 0.40 | 24/40 | 60 | 0.29 | 0.20 |
Frequency of agreement.
Observed percent agreement (%).
Cohen’s kappa statistic.
Prevalence-adjusted bias-adjusted kappa statistic.
Couples with a sexual agreement about having sex with outside partners only showed “fair” agreement (unadjusted) across all categories, except controlling IPV which was only “slight” agreement (K=.13). Adjusted values generally yielded “poor” agreement, while showing “fair” agreement for physical IPV (PABAK=.38) and “moderate” agreement for controlling IPV (PABAK=.58). Unadjusted concordance levels were lower among couples who did not have a sexual agreement (n=10) for controlling IPV (K= −.11) and emotional IPV (K=.19) but were higher for physical IPV (K=.51) and for any type of IPV (K=.52). Adjusted statistics showed higher degrees of concordance on IPV across all measures of IPV in this group. Couples who disagreed on their sexual agreement (n=40) often showed lower degrees of concordance, having the lowest unadjusted concordance among the three groups in physical IPV (K=.08), but the highest unadjusted concordance of the groups for controlling IPV (K=.22). Adjusted concordance levels were typically higher than couples with a sexual agreement but lower than those without (any type PABAK=.15, physical PABAK=.35, emotional PABAK=.1, controlling PABAK=.4, monitoring PABAK=.2).
Discussion
In general, concordance in reporting of recent IPV among this sample of male couples was only low to moderate. The reporting of perpetration of recent IPV was higher than the reporting of recent victimization. This contrasts with studies of opposite sex couples in which there is evidence that women report higher levels of victimization in contrast to the levels of perpetration reported by their male partners (Halim et al., 2018: Panuzio et al., 2006; Shafer, Caetano and & Clark, 2002: Freeman, Schumacher, & Coffey, 2015). Across comparison groups of differences in age, race, HIV serostatus, cohabitation status and sexual agreement, kappa and PABAK values were consistently in the “moderate” or below range, indicating a lack of agreement of the presence of IPV in relationships across a range of individual demographic and relationship contexts.
There are several possible explanations for the generally low levels of agreement in the reporting of the presence of recent IPV in this sample of male couples. It is possible that men do not perceive all the acts they experience as IPV. The questions used to assess IPV ask men to respond to a list of acts (including physical, emotional, controlling, and monitoring) and note whether their partner has done this to them, or they have done this to their partner (as separate questions) in the previous 12 months. The questions do not directly ask the respondents if they have experienced or perpetrated IPV: they report the experience or perpetration of acts, which are then used to classify the presence or absence of IPV in the relationship. While some acts listed fall into more widely accepted definitions of violence, i.e. forced sex, other acts may not be commonly perceived as IPV. If we accept that IPV is a socially undesirable behavior that is likely to be under-reported, men may under-report the prevalence of acts that are seen as IPV, yet may not necessarily under-report acts that they do not perceive as IPV. The only form of IPV for which there was moderate agreement was controlling behaviors; this includes behaviors such as stopping a partner from seeing their friends of family. This may not be perceived as an act of IPV, and therefore may be more accurately reported and have more agreement of reporting among dyads.
However, this does not explain the rather surprising result that overall, the perpetration of IPV was more commonly reported than the experience of IPV. The answer to this may lie in associations between perpetrations of violence and traditional constructs of masculinity. Literature examining the experience of IPV among opposite sex couples has noted an association between the perpetration of IPV by men against women and men’s beliefs and adherence to traditional norms that view masculinity as dominant, violent, or controlling (Moore & Stuart, 2005). There is also evidence that men may use violence as a resource for overcoming challenges to masculinity (i.e. unemployment) (Messerschmidt, 2000). Hunt et al., (2016) hypothesize that the persisting stereotype that gay men are not masculine may lead gay men to be vulnerable to threats to their masculinity, and react to this challenge by distancing themselves from feminine-stereotyped gay men or by attempting to present themselves as more masculine. Acting violently towards a partner may be a tactic to reinforce masculinity. Just as reporting IPV may validate an individual’s masculinity, not reporting IPV may equally be an effort to validate masculinity. Admitting to experiencing IPV may be seen as a challenge to masculinity (Hunt, Fasoli, Carnaghi, & Cadinu, 2016).
Alternatively, some the difference in the reporting of experience and perpetration may lie in the nature of the items included in the IPV-GBM scale. The monitoring domain includes items such as “having read emails without knowledge”: this would naturally be easier to report more perpetration that experience, unless the victim was aware it had happened. Hence, to some extent, the over-reporting of IPV perpetration may be linked to specific acts of IPV. Further research is warranted, perhaps with qualitative methods, to disentangle the extent to which over-reporting of perpetration relative to experience of IPV represents measurement error or real experiences.
Although this is the first study to examine concordance in the reporting of IPV among male couples, there are several limitations to consider. The sample was not racially diverse, and the results may not be representative of non-white male couples. Participants who identified as multi-racial, Asian, or Native American were too few for statistical analysis and were categorized as one group, limiting our ability to examine the concordance of IPV among male couples from a range of racial and ethnic groups. Only fifty participants reported positive HIV sero-status, and all were in a sero-discordant partnership; hence that data from sero-concordant positive couples is still lacking, although there is no evidence to suggest that male sero-concordant positive couples experience higher levels of IPV or would report IPV differently. The survey did not collect data on degree of relationship commitment, or measures of the social desirability of IPV, which may also shape the differential reporting of IPV. Couples included in the analysis are also selective in that they are couples who have agreed to be in a RCT together. These couples may be expected to report lower levels of IPV, either because the relationship communication and functioning required to enter a RCT together may be associated with lowered experience of IPV or, alternatively, couples may fear that reporting IPV may exclude them from enrollment in the RCT and hence under-report the experience of IPV. In the current RCT, reporting recent experience or perpetration of IPV was an exclusion criterion for enrolling in the RCT. The measure of IPV used, the IPB-GBM scale, is unique to the experience of IPV among MSM: however, the inclusion of damage to property under physical IPV may give a higher prevalence of IPV than is found with more traditional measures of IPV.
The results point to high levels of discordance in the reporting of IPV among this sample of male couples from three U.S cities. While further work is warranted with a larger, more racially and ethnically diverse sample, the results present preliminary evidence that male couples discordantly report IPV. The differential reporting of IPV may be shaped by desires to conform to constructs of masculinity, leading to biases in the reporting of both the experience and perpetration of IPV, or may also be shaped by measurement error. Further work is required, adopting qualitative approaches, to understand the factors that shape gay men’s desire to report their experiences or perpetration of IPV, and to examine how current measures of IPV may be adapted to reflect these challenges.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This Author Accepted Manuscript is a PDF file of a an unedited peer-reviewed manuscript that has been accepted for publication but has not been copyedited or corrected. The official version of record that is published in the journal is kept up to date and so may therefore differ from this version.
References
- Bartholomew K, Regan KV, White MA, & Oram D (2008). Patterns of Abuse in Male Same-Sex Relationships. Violence and Victims, 23(5), 617–636. doi: 10.1891/0886-6708.23.5.617 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brennan PF, & Hays BJ (1992). Focus on psychometrics the kappa statistic for establishing interrater reliability in the secondary analysis of qualitative clinical data. Research in nursing & health, 15(2), 153–158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Byrt T, Bishop J, & Carlin JB (1993). Bias, prevalence and kappa. Journal of clinical epidemiology, 46(5), 423–429. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carvalho AF, Lewis RJ, Derlega VJ, Winstead BA, & Viggiano C (2011). Internalized Sexual Minority Stressors and Same-Sex Intimate Partner Violence. Journal of Family Violence, 26(7), 501–509. doi: 10.1007/s10896-011-9384-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Cicchetti DV, & Feinstein AR (1990). High agreement but low kappa: II. Resolving the paradoxes. Journal of clinical epidemiology, 43(6), 551–558. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen J (1960). A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educational and psychological measurement, 20(1), 37–46. [Google Scholar]
- Craft SM, & Serovich JM (2005). Family-of-origin factors and partner violence in the intimate relationships of gay men who are HIV positive. J Interpers Violence, 20(7), 777–791. doi: 10.1177/0886260505277101 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis A, Kaighobadi F, Stephenson R, Rael C, and Sandfort T (2016): Associations between Alcohol Use and Intimate Partner Violence among Men Who Have Sex with Men. LGBT Health 3(6), 400–406. doi: 10.1089/lgbt.2016.0057 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Desmarais SL, Reeves KA, Nicholls TL, Telford R, & Fiebert MS (2012a). Prevalence of physical violence in intimate relationships: Part 1. Rates of male and female victimization. Partner Abuse, 3(2), 140–169. [Google Scholar]
- Desmarais SL Reeves KA, Nicholls TL, Telford R, & Fiebert MS(2012b). Prevalence of physical violence in intimate relationships: Part 2. Rates of male and female perpetration. Partner Abuse, 3(2), 170–198. [Google Scholar]
- Feldman MB, Ream GL, Diaz RM, & El-Bassel N (2008). Intimate Partner Violence and HIV Sexual Risk Behavior Among Latino Gay and Bisexual Men: The Role of Situational Factors. Journal of LGBT Health Research, 3(4), 75–87. doi: 10.1080/15574090802226618 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feinstein AR, & Cicchetti DV (1990). High agreement but low kappa: I. The problems of two paradoxes. Journal of clinical epidemiology, 43(6), 543–549. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finneran C, & Stephenson R (2013). Intimate partner violence among men who have sex with men: a systematic review. Trauma Violence Abuse, 14(2), 168–185. doi: 10.1177/1524838012470034 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freeman AJ, Schumacher JA, & Coffey SF (2015). Social desirability and partner agreement of men’s reporting of intimate partner violence in substance abuse treatment settings. J Interpers Violence, 30(4), 565–579. doi: 10.1177/0886260514535263 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Girianelli VR, & Santos Thuler LC (2007). Evaluation of agreement between conventional and liquid-based cytology in cervical cancer early detection based on analysis of 2,091 smears: experience at the Brazilian National Cancer Institute. Diagn Cytopathol, 35(9), 545–549. doi: 10.1002/dc.20699 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greenwood GL, Relf MV, Huang B, Pollack LM, Canchola JA, & Catania JA (2002). Battering Victimization Among a Probability-Based Sample of Men Who Have Sex With Men. American Journal of Public Health, 92(12), 1964–1969. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Halim N, Steven E, Reich N, Badi L, & Messersmith L (2018). Variability and validity of intimate partner violence reporting by couples in Tanzania. PLoS One, 13(3), e0193253. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0193253 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Houston E, & McKirnan DJ (2007). Intimate partner abuse among gay and bisexual men: risk correlates and health outcomes. J Urban Health, 84(5), 681–690. doi: 10.1007/s11524-007-9188-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hunt CJ, Fasoli F, Carnaghi A, & Cadinu M (2016). Masculine self-presentation and distancing from femininity in gay men: An experimental examination of the role of masculinity threat. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 17(1), 108–112. doi: 10.1037/a0039545 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Koblin BA, Torian L, Xu G, Guilin V, Makki H, Mackellar D, & Valleroy L (2006). Violence and HIV-related risk among young men who have sex with men. AIDS Care, 18(8), 961–967. doi: 10.1080/09540120500467182 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Landis JR, & Koch GG (1977). The Measurement of Observer Agreement for Categorical Data. Biometrics, 33(1), 159. doi: 10.2307/2529310 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mak HK, Yau KK, & Chan BP (2004). Prevalence-adjusted bias-adjusted kappa values as additional indicators to measure observer agreement. Radiology, 232(1), 302–303. doi: 10.1148/radiol.2321031974 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marshall AD, Panuzio J, Makin-Byrd KN, Taft CT, & Holtzworth-Munroe A (2011). A multilevel examination of interpartner intimate partner violence and psychological aggression reporting concordance. Behav Ther, 42(3), 364–377. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2010.09.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McHugh ML (2012). Interrater reliability: The kappa statistic. Biochemia Medica, 22(3), 276–282. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Messerschmidt JW (2000). Becoming “Real Men”: Adolescent Masculinity Challenges and Sexual Violence. Men and Masculinities, 2(3), 286–307. doi: [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Moore TM, & Stuart GL (2005). A Review of the Literature on Masculinity and Partner Violence. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 6(1), 46–61. doi: 10.1037/1524-9220.6.1.46 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Pantalone DW, Schneider KL, Valentine SE, & Simoni JM (2012). Investigating partner abuse among HIV-positive men who have sex with men. AIDS Behav, 16(4), 1031–1043. doi: 10.1007/s10461-011-0011-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Panuzio J, O’Farrell TJ, Marshall AD, Murphy CM, Murphy M, & Taft CT (2006). Intimate partner aggression reporting concordance and correlates of agreement among men with alcohol use disorders and their female partners. Assessment, 13(3), 266–279 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pruitt KL, White D, Mitchell JW, & Stephenson R (2015). Sexual agreements and intimate-partner violence among male couples. International Journal of Sexual Health, 27(4), 429–441. doi: 10.1080/19317611.2015.1037037 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Relf MV (2001). Battering and HIV in Men Who Have Sex With Men: A Critique and Synthesis of the Literature. Journal of the Association of Nurses in AIDS Care, 12(3), 41–48. doi: 10.1016/s1055-3290(06)60143-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schafer J, Caetano E and Clark CL (2002): Agreement about violence in US couples. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 17(4):457–470 [Google Scholar]
- Sim J, & Wright CC (2005). The kappa statistic in reliability studies: Use, interpretation, and sample size requirements. Physical therapy, 85(3), 257–268. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stephenson R, de Voux A, & Sullivan PS (2011). Intimate Partner Violence and Sexual Risk-taking among Men Who Have Sex with Men in South Africa. Western Journal of Emergency Medicine, 12(3), 343–347. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stephenson R, & Finneran C (2013). The IPV-GBM scale: a new scale to measure intimate partner violence among gay and bisexual men. PLoS One, 8(6), e62592. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0062592 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stephenson R and Finneran C: Receipt and Perpetration of Intimate Partner Violence and Condomless Anal Intercourse among gay and bisexual men in Atlanta (2017a): AIDS and Behavior 21(8): 2253–2260. Doi: 10.1007/s10461-017-1709-6/ [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stephenson R and Finneran C (2017b): Minority stress and intimate partner violence among gay and bisexual men in Atlanta. American Journal of Men’s Health 11(4), 952–961. doi: 10.1177/1557988316677506 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stephenson R, Khosropour C, & Sullivan P (2010). Reporting of Intimate Partner Violence among Men Who Have Sex with Men in an Online Survey. Western Journal of Emergency Medicine, 11(3), 5. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stephenson R, Rentsch C, Salazar LF, & Sullivan PS (2011). Dyadic Characteristics and Intimate Partner Violence among Men Who Have Sex with Men. Western Journal of Emergency Medicine, 12(3), 324–332. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stephenson R, Sato KN, & Finneran C (2013). Dyadic, Partner, and Social Network Influences on Intimate Partner Violence among Male-Male Couples. West J Emerg Med, 14(4), 316–323. doi: 10.5811/westjem.2013.2.15623 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stephenson R, Suarez NA, Garofalo R, Hidalgo MA, Hoehnle S, Thai J, . . . Sullivan P (2017). Project Stronger Together: Protocol to Test a Dyadic Intervention to Improve Engagement in HIV Care Among Sero-Discordant Male Couples in Three US Cities. JMIR Research Protocols, 6(8), e170. doi: 10.2196/resprot.7884 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strandmoen JF, Askeland IR, Tjersland OA, Wentzel-Larsen T, & Heir T (2016). Intimate Partner Violence in Men Voluntarily Attending Treatment: A Study of Couple Agreement. Violence Vict, 31(1), 124–134. doi: 10.1891/0886-6708.VV-D-13-00193 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Welles SL, Corbin TJ, Rich JA, Reed E, & Raj A (2011). Intimate partner violence among men having sex with men, women, or both: early-life sexual and physical abuse as antecedents. J Community Health, 36(3), 477–485. doi: 10.1007/s10900-010-9331-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]