Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2020 Jul 21;9:e55263. doi: 10.7554/eLife.55263

Early life adversity decreases pre-adolescent fear expression by accelerating amygdala PV cell development

Gabriela Manzano Nieves 1, Marilyn Bravo 1, Saba Baskoylu 1, Kevin G Bath 2,
Editors: Matthew N Hill3, Kate M Wassum4
PMCID: PMC7413666  PMID: 32692310

Abstract

Early life adversity (ELA) is associated with increased risk for stress-related disorders later in life. The link between ELA and risk for psychopathology is well established but the developmental mechanisms remain unclear. Using a mouse model of resource insecurity, limited bedding (LB), we tested the effects of LB on the development of fear learning and neuronal structures involved in emotional regulation, the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and basolateral amygdala (BLA). LB delayed the ability of peri-weanling (21 days old) mice to express, but not form, an auditory conditioned fear memory. LB accelerated the developmental emergence of parvalbumin (PV)-positive cells in the BLA and increased anatomical connections between PL and BLA. Fear expression in LB mice was rescued through optogenetic inactivation of PV-positive cells in the BLA. The current results provide a model of transiently blunted emotional reactivity in early development, with latent fear-associated memories emerging later in adolescence.

Research organism: Mouse

Introduction

Early life adversity (ELA) increases the lifetime risk for multiple forms of psychopathology, including anxiety disorders and major depressive disorder (Agid et al., 1999; Draijer and Langeland, 1999; Heim and Nemeroff, 2001; Koenen and Widom, 2009; Widom, 1999). ELA is associated with higher rates of negative outcomes than similar events experienced in adulthood (Felitti et al., 1998; Heim and Nemeroff, 2001; McCauley et al., 1997; Mullen et al., 1996; Salmon and Bryant, 2002). The increased lifetime risk for psychopathology is proposed to be the result of alterations in the developmental trajectories of brain centers regulating emotional learning and emotional expression. In agreement with this prediction, ELA has been shown to drive early engagement of the basolateral amygdala (BLA), a key node supporting emotional processing, threat assessment, and fear learning (Bath et al., 2016; Moriceau et al., 2009).

The neural circuit(s) supporting fear and aversive learning in adult rodents have been well characterized. Numerous studies have shown that the prelimbic (PL) subregion of the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) projects to the BLA (Do-Monte et al., 2015b; Vertes, 2004) and is necessary for fear retrieval (Burgos-Robles et al., 2009; Courtin et al., 2014; Sierra-Mercado et al., 2011), whereas the infralimbic (IL) subregion of mPFC supports extinction learning (Adhikari et al., 2015; Do-Monte et al., 2015a; Sierra-Mercado et al., 2011). Over early development, PL projections into amygdala begin to emerge around postnatal day (PND) 7, increasing through adolescence (Cunningham et al., 2002), and being pruned back during early adulthood (Bouwmeester et al., 2002; Cressman et al., 2010). The relatively late integration of the PL into the threat learning circuit may explain recent reports suggesting that the infant PL, unlike adult PL, is not involved in the expression of sustained fear responses (Chan et al., 2011). Investigating the impact of ELA on the development of the fear circuit will allow us to simultaneously identify potential mechanisms promoting developmental changes in learned aversive behaviors and possible underlying changes that may contribute to ELA-associated effects on anxiety, depressive, and threat learning behavior.

Recently, a rodent model of resource insecurity, limited bedding and nesting (LB), was developed to simulate aspects of resource insecurity and altered parental care in human populations (Bolton et al., 2019; Rice et al., 2008). LB has been shown to induce significant distress in the dam, alter patterns of maternal behavior (Bolton et al., 2019; Gallo et al., 2019; McLoyd, 1998; Rice et al., 2008), and delay developmental processes such as sexual maturation and physical growth (Manzano Nieves et al., 2019; Yam et al., 2017). In addition, LB impacts behavioral outcomes with effects that persist into adulthood, increasing depressive-like behaviors (Goodwill et al., 2019), altering behavioral response to stress (Cohen et al., 2013; Manzano-Nieves et al., 2018), and affecting hippocampal-dependent learning (Bath et al., 2016; Bath et al., 2017; Manzano-Nieves et al., 2018; Wang et al., 2011). However, the effects of LB on the development of neuronal populations and regions supporting fear learning and expression are less well understood. In this study, we tested the impact of LB on the development of subregions of mPFC, BLA, projections from mPFC to BLA, as well as their effect on the acquisition and expression of fear learning across early development.

Here, we demonstrate that LB delayed physical development in mice and altered developmental expression of fear learning. Specifically, LB resulted in decreased body weight and brain weight from infancy into pre-adolescence. Changes in physical development were accompanied by an increase in parvalbumin (PV)-positive cell density in the BLA of peri-weanling mice and sex-specific changes in mPFC to BLA anatomical connectivity during peri-adolescence. During the period of elevated PV-positive cell density, mice transiently failed to exhibit threat-associated freezing behavior, with expression of this learned fear re-emerging during the peri-adolescent and adolescent period. Furthermore, the diminished fear expression during the peri-weaning period could be rescued through optogenetic inactivation of PV-positive cells in the BLA. Based upon these results, an ELA-associated premature increase in PV-positive cells in BLA as associated with a transient decrease in the expression, but not acquisition of fear learning.

Results

Early life adversity decreases physical growth

In humans, ELA is associated with changes in expected weight (Hult et al., 2010; Maniam et al., 2014; Rondó et al., 2003; Wainstock et al., 2013), with low weight in infancy predicting poorer cognitive outcomes (Corbett and Drewett, 2004; Strathearn et al., 2001). Thus, weight may serve as a biomarker for altered neurodevelopment and later risk for pathology. To model ELA in the form of resource insecurity, dams and pups were placed in conditions of low bedding and nesting materials (LB) from PND 4–11 (Bolton et al., 2019; McLoyd, 1998; Rice et al., 2008). To assess the impact of LB on somatic and brain development, the body weight and brain weight of control (Ctrl) and LB reared mice was measured at select ages across early development (PND 16, 21, 28, and 35). We found that LB reared female mice weighed significantly less than control mice at PND 16, 21, and 28, with differences diminishing by PND 35 (Figure 1B top), while LB reared males weighed significantly less than control male mice at PND 21 and 28 (Figure 1B bottom). Examination of brain weight in females (Figure 1C top) revealed a significant decrease in brain weight at PND 21, 28 and 35. LB males had decreased brain weight when compared to control males at PND 16, 21, and 28, with differences resolved by PND 35 (Figure 1C bottom). We predicted that ELA may shift resources away from somatic development to spare brain development. To test this prediction, the brain-to-body weight ratio was calculated across developmental time points (Figure 1D). LB reared female mice had a higher brain-to-body weight ratio than female control mice at PND 16, 21 and 28, while LB males had an increased brain-to-body ratio at PND 21 and 28. Thus LB effects on brain and total body weight were not proportional, with a greater impact on the development of the body at select developmental time points.

Figure 1. Limited bedding (LB) resources early in life, altered somatic and brain development.

Figure 1.

(A) Timeline of resource restriction manipulation (PND 4–11) and description of developmental time points tested. Somatic effects of LB rearing on Males and Females. (B) Graphs depicting LB changes in total body weight for females (top) and males (bottom). Insets show a close up of mean and SEM for PND 16 data. When compared to ctrl females, LB females have decreased total weight at PND 16 (t11 = 2.84, p=0.015) 21 (t13 = 4.99, p<0.001) and 28 (t14 = 5.73, p<0.001), but not at PND 35 (t13 = 1.90, p=0.078). When compared to ctrl males, LB males have decreased total weight at PND 21 (t17 = 6.64, p<0.001) and 28 (t14 = 2.65, p=0.018), but not at 16 (t18 = 1.90, p=0.073) or 35 (t15 = 0.98, p=0.33). (Females: Ctrl n = 6, 8, 8, 7; LB n = 7, 7, 8, 8) (Males: Ctrl n = 10, 10, 8, 9; LB n = 10, 9, 8, 8). (C) Graphs depicting LB changes in total brain weight for females (top) and males (bottom). Insets show a close up of mean and SEM for PND 16 data. However, when we assessed brain weight, LB female mice did not differ from ctrl females at PND 16 (t9 = 2.11, p=0.063), with differences emerging at PND 21 (t13 = 5.62, p<0.001), 28 (t14 = 3.11, p=0.0075), and 35 (t12 = 3.55, p=0.0039). LB males had significantly different brain weight when compare to ctrl males at PND 16 (t13 = 2.29, p=0.038), 21 (t17 = 4.84, p<0.001) and 28 (t14 = 3.13, p=0.0073), but not at PND 35 (t14 = 0.40, p=0.69). (D) When we assessed brain to total weight ratio, female LB mice had a higher ratio at PND 16 (t9 = 3.59, p=0.0057), 21 (t13 = 3.69, p=0.0027), and 28 (t14 = 4.33, p<0.001), but not at PND 35 (t12 = 0.69, p=0.50) when compared to ctrl females. However, LB males did not significantly differ from ctrl males at PND 16 (t13 = 0.76, p=0.45), however, by PND 21 (t17 = 4.44, p<0.001) and 28 (t14 = 2.60, p=0.020) LB males had significantly higher brain/total weight ratio when compared to ctrl males, with effects dissipating by PND 35 (t14 = 0.51, p=0.61). For panels (C) and (D): (Females: Ctrl n = 6, 8, 8, 6; LB n = 5, 7, 8, 8) (Males: Ctrl n = 6, 10, 8, 8; LB n = 9, 9, 8, 8). Dots in panels represent individual data points. Bars represent group means + / - SEM. Unpaired two-tailed student t-tests were used for data analysis. *=p < 0.05, **=p < 0.01, ***=p < 0.001.

LB is associated with deficits in fear recall during the peri-weaning period

To determine if LB rearing affected learning and expression of a conditioned fear memory, mice underwent fear conditioning training and testing. The age (in days) at which each separate cohort of mice underwent each stage of auditory fear learning is shown in Figure 2A. To dissociate cue-memory from context-memory, mice were habituated to two contexts (context A and B) for 5 min per context for 2 consecutive days. Mice were then conditioned to six tones (75 dB, 30 s) that each co-terminated with a foot-shock (0.57 mA, 1 s) in context A and tested for memory recall 24 hr post-conditioning in context B (Figure 2B). In control mice, we observed a significant developmental increase in freezing behavior from PND 19 to PND 22. However, in LB reared mice, we found significantly lower levels of freezing compared to control mice during cue recall at PND 22, but not at later developmental time points (Figure 2C), indicating a possible delay in cue-associated fear learning in LB mice. Further, a significant decrease in freezing to the conditioning tones was observed in LB males at PND 21 and in LB females at PND 28. To determine if differences in recall at PND 22 were the result of diminished learning at PND 21, conditioning curves of control and LB mice were matched for levels of freezing during acquisition (Figure 2—figure supplement 1). Matching was conducted by mixing male and female data, and systematically removing individual animal data from the analysis until LB and Ctrl conditioning curves at PND 21 were overlapping and not significantly different. The results showed that deficits in fear expression could not be solely explained by differences in freezing observed during conditioning. To determine if the lower levels of freezing in LB mice at PND 22 were the result of deficits in acquisition, consolidation, or recall; separate cohorts of male and female mice were conditioned at PND 21 and tested at either 1 hr (pre-consolidation), 6 hr (post-consolidation), 24 hr (short-term recall), or 7 days (long-term recall) post conditioning (Figure 3A). At 1 hr, there was a small but significant decrease in freezing observed in LB reared mice compared to controls. At 6 hr, no differences in levels of freezing were detected between LB and control reared mice, indicating that LB mice could freeze to the cue and appeared to have consolidated the fear memory. By 24 hr, LB mice showed significantly lower freezing compared to control mice, suggestive of deficits in fear memory. Interestingly, at 7 days post conditioning, LB freezing levels did not differ from control levels. In aggregate, initial learning was intact but freezing to the conditioned tones was transiently impaired 24 hr post conditioning. Together the results suggest that LB reared mice form a cue-associated fear memory at PND 21 but are not able to behaviorally express this memory during early development. However, with time, behavioral expression of fear, in the form of freezing, re-emerges. Follow-up analysis on sex differences were performed (Figure 3B) showing that both LB males and LB females showed the decrease in freezing 24 hr post condition followed by an increase in freezing 7 days post conditioning. A significant decrease in freezing was observed 1 hr post conditioning in control females when compared to control males. A battery of tests were conducted in males and females to ensure that effects of fear expression were not due to differences in anxiety-like behavior (light/dark box test: Figure 3C and Figure 3—figure supplement 1; elevated plus maze: Figure 3—figure supplement 2), locomotion (open field test: Figure 3—figure supplement 3), or foot-shock sensitivity and reactivity (Figure 3—figure supplement 4). No general differences were detected between LB and control at PND 21 in anxiety-like behavior, locomotion, or somatosensation (see supplements for full stats).

Figure 2. LB affects the short-term expression of fear during early development.

(A) Table of the postnatal day at which each cohort underwent different phases of auditory cue conditioning. Different cohorts were used for each age group. (B) Schematic of auditory fear conditioning protocol. (C) Graphs of the percent time female (top) and male (bottom) mice spent freezing (immobile) during the conditioning and recall test. LB female (t21 = 2.79, p=0.010) and male (t20 = 3.39, p=0.0028) mice exhibited decreased fear expression at PND 22 when compared to age and sex matched controls, an effect not observed at other ages (Females: PND 17 (t20 = 1.43, p=0.16), 29 (t19 = 0.032, p=0.97), 51 (t21 = 0.73, p=0.47)) (Males: PND 17 (t24 = 0.68, p=0.50), 29 (t21 = 0.014, p=0.98), 51 (t19 = 1.42, p=0.16)). During auditory cue conditioning in females, a main effect of rearing condition was observed at PND 28 (F(1,19) = 7.70, p=0.012), but not at 16 (F(1,20) = 0.52, p=0.47), 21 (F(1,21) = 3.32, p=0.082), or 50 (F(1,21) = 1.33, p=0.26). Post-hoc analysis revealed that PND 28 LB females had significantly lower levels of freezing on the 5th (t114 = 3.50, p=0.0039) and 6th (t114 = 3.80, p=0.0014) tones. A main effect of tone during conditioning was observed in females at all ages (PND 16: F(5,100) = 2.70, p=0.024; PND 21: F(5,105) = 4.11, p=0.0019; PND 28: F(5,95) = 39.3, p<0.0001; PND 50: F(5,105) = 104.1, p<0.0001), indicating that female mice learned the tone/foot shock association at all ages tested. No interaction between tone trial and rearing condition where observed at PND 16 (F(5,100) = 1.51, p=0.19) or 21 (F(5,105) = 0.83, p=0.52); however, significant interactions where observed at PND 28 (F(5,95) = 3.79, p=0.0035) and 50 (F(5,105) = 2.45, p=0.037). During auditory cue conditioning in males, no main effect of rearing condition was observed at any age tested (PND 16: F(1,24) = 0.15, p=0.69; PND 21: F(1,20) = 3.32, p=0.083; PND 28: F(1,21) = 0.78, p=0.38; PND 50: F(1,19) = 0.0017, p=0.96). However, a post-hoc analysis revealed that PND 21 LB males had significantly lower levels of freezing on the 5th (t120 = 3.25, p=0.0088) and 6th (t120 = 2.78, p=0.036) tones when compared to ctrl males. During conditioning in males a main effect of tone trial was observed at all ages (PND 16: F(5,120) = 3.33, p=0.0074; PND 21: F(5,100) = 12.14, p<0.0001; PND 28: F(5,105) = 54.63, p<0.0001; PND 50: F(5,95) = 62.84, p<0.0001), indicating that male mice learned the tone/foot shock association at all ages tested. No interaction between tone trial and rearing condition where observed in male conditioning at PND 16 (F(5,120) = 0.42, p=0.83), 28 (F(5,105) = 0.59, p=0.70), or 50 (F(5,95) = 0.49, p=0.77); however, a significant interaction was observed at PND 21 (F(5,100) = 4.05, p=0.0022). (Females: Ctrl n = 11, 8, 12, 11; LB n = 11, 15, 9, 12) (Males: Ctrl n = 16, 9, 12, 12; LB n = 10, 13, 11, 12). For effects on memory recall, following matching for conditioning curves see Figure 3—figure supplement 4. Additional tests for somatosensation were conducted to ensure that differences in freezing were not due to differences in foot-shock sensitivity, see Figure 2—figure supplement 2. Bars represent group means + / - SEM. Two-way repeated measure ANOVA followed by a Sidak’s multiple comparison analysis was used to analyze the conditioning curves. Unpaired two-tailed student t-tests were used to analyze differences in recall tests. *=p < 0.05, **=p < 0.01, ***=p < 0.001.

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Effects on recall at PND 21 are not a consequence of conditioning deficits.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

Graph of subgroup of mixed sex mice matched for levels of freezing during conditioning. A repeated measure ANOVA revealed a main effect of tone presentation (F(5,125) = 10.59, p<0.0001), but no significant main effects of rearing condition (F(1,25) = 0.0038, p=0.95), or effect of interaction between tone presentation and rearing condition (F(5,125) = 0.39, p=0.85). Matching for conditioning did not alter effects on recall (t25 = 3.36, p=0.0025), with deficits in freezing expression in LB reared PND21 mice. For Ctrl n = 13, for LB n = 14. A repeated measure two-way ANOVA was used to test for differences in fear conditioning. A two-tailed paired student t-test was used to test for differences in the recall test. *=p < 0.05, **=p < 0.01, ***=p < 0.001.

Figure 3. LB affects the short-term expression of fear during early development.

(A) Schematic of experimental protocol (top). A mix of male and female mice were conditioned at PND 21 and tested at only one time point: 1 hr, 6 hr, 7 hr, or 7 days post-conditioning. Graph depicting changes in freezing levels of distinct cohorts of mice during recall tests at varying delays (bottom). LB mice had decreased freezing at 1 hr (t74 = 2.11, p=0.037), 24 hr (t48 = 3.41, p=0.0012) but not 6 hr (t55 = 1.66, p=0.10) or 7 days (t46 = 1.76, p=0.085) post-conditioning. (Ctrl n = 34, 30, 29, 27; LB n = 42, 27, 21, 21). (B) Re-analysis of data presented in panel A to reveal sex differences within the data. When compared to Ctrl males, Ctrl females had decreased freezing at 1 hr (t32 = 2.11, p=0.042), but not at 6 hr (t28 = 1.95, p=0.060), 24 hr (t27 = 1.89, p=0.069) or 7 days (t25 = 1.17, p=0.25) post-conditioning. LB males (t25 = 2.21, p=0.036) and LB females (t21 = 3.94, p=0.0007) had decreased freezing 24 hr post-conditioning when compared to sex matched controls. No other significant differences were observed between Ctrl males and LB males (1 hr: t42 = 1.32, p=0.19; 6 hr: t32 = 0.99, p=0.32; 7 days: t23 = 1.37, p=0.18), or between Ctrl females and LB females (1 hr: t30 = 0.85, p=0.39; 6 hr: t21 = 1.64, p=0.11; 7 days: t21 = 1.31, p=0.20). (Ctrl males n = 15, 19, 16, 14; Ctrl females n = 19, 11, 13, 13; LB males n = 29, 15, 11, 11; LB females n = 13, 12, 10, 10). (C) Depiction of the light/dark box used to assess anxiety-like behavior (top). A mix of male and female mice were placed in the dark side of the box, the latency to light (center) and the total time spent in the light side of the box (bottom) are shown. Total time spent in the light/dark box was 420 s. Age (F(2,135) = 19.2, p<0.0001), but not rearing condition (F(1,135) = 0.50, p=0.47) or age x rearing interaction (F(2,135) = 0.72, p=0.48), significantly affected the latency to exit the dark side of the box (center). PND 21 mice of both LB and control reared conditions took more time to enter the light side of the box when compared to mice from the same rearing condition at PND 28 (LB t135 = 3.63, p=0.0012; Ctrl t135 = 4.05, p=0.0003) and PND 35 (LB t135 = 3.09, p=0.0073; Ctrl t135 = 3.89, p=0.0005). However, a main effect of rearing condition on the total time mice spent in the light side of the light/dark box (F(2,135) = 3.91, p=0.022) was observed (bottom). A post-hoc analysis revealed that significant differences in rearing condition were only observed at PND 35 (t135 = 2.60, p=0.030). Furthermore, a significant main effect of age on the time spent in the light side (F(1,135) = 4.01, p=0.047) was observed. Specifically, control PND 35 mice spent significantly more time in the light side when compared to control reared mice aged PND 21 (t135 = 2.46, p=0.043) and PND 28 (t135 = 2.56, p=0.033). No interaction between age and rearing condition was observed (F(2,135) = 1.23, p=0.29). (Ctrl n = 14, 24, 23; LB n = 27, 22, 31). For effects of rearing condition by sex see Figure 3—figure supplement 1. Bars represent group means + / - SEM. Dots in panel (C) represent individual data points. Unpaired two-tailed student t-tests were used in (A) and (B). For (C) a two-way ANOVA followed by a Sidak’s multiple comparison analysis was used. *=p < 0.05, **=p < 0.01, ***=p < 0.001.

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. LB did not affect anxiety like behavior at PND 21.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

(A) Drawing of the light/dark box behavioral apparatus. (B) Graph showing the time in seconds that it took mice to exit the dark side of the box and enter the light side. A three-way ANOVA revealed a main effect of age (F(2,129) = 18.22, p<0.0001) but not of sex (F(1,129) = 0.21, p=0.64) or rearing condition (F(1,129) = 0.39, p=0.53). No significant interactions between age, sex and rearing condition were revealed. A follow-up Sidak multiple comparison analysis revealed that, in general, mice took longer to exit the dark side of the box and enter the light side of the box at PND 21 when compared to PND 28 (t129 = 5.57, p<0.0001) or PND 35 (t129 = 5.05, p<0.0001). (C) Graph showing the total amount of time mice spent in the light side of the box. A three-way ANOVA for sex, rearing condition, and age revealed significant main effect of age (F(2,129) = 3.23, p=0.042) but not of rearing condition (F(1,129) = 3.08, p=0.081) or sex (F(1,129) = 1.12, p=0.29). No significant interactions were observed. (D) When using a three-way ANOVA to assess global effects of mouse entries into the light side of the light dark box, we found a main effect of age (F(2,129) = 4.13, p=0.018), rearing condition (F(1,129) = 78.08, p<0.0001), but not sex (F(1,129) = 0.029, p=0.86). For males, a follow-up two-way ANOVA between Ctrl and LB revealed a significant effect of rearing condition (F(1,71) = 31.31, p<0.0001), but not of age (F(2,71) = 0.96, p=0.38) or age by rearing condition (F(2,71) = 0.72, p=0.48). Follow-up analysis in males revealed significant differences between ctrl and LB mice at PND 28 (t71 = 3.66, p=0.0014) and 35 (t71 = 4.23, p=0.0002), but not at 21 (t71 = 2.03, p=0.13). For females, a two-way ANOVA between Ctrl and LB revealed a significant effect of rearing condition (F(1,58) = 67.27, p<0.0001), age (F(2,58) = 6.45, p=0.0029), and age by rearing condition (F(2,58) = 4.06, p=0.022). Follow-up analysis in females revealed significant differences between ctrl and LB mice at PND 28 (t58 = 5.37, p<0.0001) and 35 (t58 = 6.90, p<0.0001), but not at 21 (t58 = 2.28, p=0.076). Ctrl males n = 8, 12, 15; Ctrl females n = 6, 12, 8; LB males n = 14, 14, 14; LB females n = 13, 8, 17. Dots represent individual data points. For all graphs the mean and SEM are presented. A three-way ANOVA was used to test for main effects of sex, age, and rearing condition. A two-way ANOVA followed by Sidak’s multiple comparison analysis was used to test for difference in rearing condition across age within a given sex. *=p < 0.05, **=p < 0.01, ***=p < 0.001.
Figure 3—figure supplement 2. LB mice spent more time performing head dips in the elevated plus maze (EPM) at PND 21.

Figure 3—figure supplement 2.

(A) Ctrl and LB mice spent a similar percentage of their time in the open arms of the EPM (F(1,45) = 0.30, p=0.58). No sex difference (F(1,45) = 0.31, p=0.57) or sex by rearing condition interactions (F(1,45) = 0.53, p=0.46) were observed for the percent time mice spent in the open arms. Further, no main effects were observed for the total distance traveled on the EPM (sex: F(1,45) = 3.52, p=0.067; rearing condition: F(1,45) = 1.64, p=0.20; sex x condition interaction: F(1,45) = 0.41, p=0.52). (B) When analyzing the time and number of times that the mouse looked over the edge of the open arms of the EPM (head dipping), we found that LB mice spent more time head dipping (F(1,45) = 4.30, p=0.043), but did not engage in more bouts (F(1,45) = 0.85, p=0.35). No main effect of sex or interaction was observed for the total amount of time mice spent head dipping (sex: F(1,45) = 0.63, p=0.42; sex x rearing condition: F(1,45) = 0.12, p=0.72), or for the total number of head dips (sex: F(1,45) = 2.28, p=0.13; sex by rearing condition: F(1,45) = 0.00047, p=0.98). Ctrl males n = 11; Ctrl females n = 10; LB males n = 18; LB females n = 10. Dots represent individual data points. For all graphs, the mean and SEM are presented. A two-way ANOVA was used to test differences between sexes and rearing conditions. *=p < 0.05, **=p < 0.01, ***=p < 0.001.
Figure 3—figure supplement 3. LB did not affect locomotion at PND 21.

Figure 3—figure supplement 3.

We used a three-way ANOVA to assess global effects of rearing condition on locomotion in an open-field assay. We found a main effect rearing condition (F(1,111) = 18.08, p<0.0001), but not of age (F(2,111) = 0.42, p=0.65) or sex (F(1,111) = 0.063, p=0.80). For males, a follow-up two-way ANOVA between Ctrl and LB revealed a significant effect of rearing condition (F(1,50) = 8.97, p=0.0042), but not age (F(2,50) = 0.92, p=0.40) or age by rearing condition (F(2,50) = 0.29, p=0.74). Follow-up analysis in males revealed no significant simple effects between ctrl and LB mice at any of the ages tested (PND 21: t50 = 1.17, p=0.57; PND 28: t50 = 1.79, p=0.22; PND 35: t50 = 2.28, p=0.078). For females, a two-way ANOVA between Ctrl and LB revealed a significant effect of rearing condition (F(1,61) = 9.48, p=0.0031), but not age (F(2,61) = 1.08, p=0.34) or age by rearing condition (F(2,61) = 2.41, p=0.097). Follow-up analysis in females revealed significant differences between ctrl and LB mice at PND 28 (t61 = 3.09, p=0.0090), but not at 21 (t61 = 0.37, p=0.97) or 35 (t61 = 1.56, p=0.32). Ctrl males n = 9, 6, 9; Ctrl females n = 14, 8, 8; LB males n = 10, 8, 14; LB females n = 12, 8, 17. Dots represent individual data points. For all graphs, the mean and SEM are presented. A three-way ANOVA was used to test for main effects of sex, age, and rearing condition. A two-way ANOVA followed by Sidak’s multiple comparison analysis was used to test for difference in rearing condition across age within a given sex. *=p < 0.05, **=p < 0.01, ***=p < 0.001.
Figure 3—figure supplement 4. LB did not affect somatosensation at PND 21.

Figure 3—figure supplement 4.

(A) To assess if LB affected the response of mice to the conditioning foot-shocks at PND 21, we plotted the mean velocity of mice during the one second foot-shock. A three-way repeated measures ANOVA did not reveal effects of rearing condition (F(1,142) = 0.70, p=0.40), sex (F(1,142) = 1.32, p=0.25), tone trial x sex (F(5,710) = 0.30, p=0.91), rearing condition x sex (F(1,142) = 1.41, p=0.23), tone x rearing condition x sex (F(5,710) = 0.29, p=0.91). However, we did find that a significant effect of tone trial (F(4.436,629.9) = 95.78, p<0.0001), showing that the velocity of the mice decreased as tone trials progressed, and a significant effect of tone by rearing condition (F(5,710) = 2.28, p=0.044). (Ctrl males n = 39, LB males n = 38, Ctrl females n = 39, LB females n = 30). (B) At PND 21, no main effects were observed for the minimum foot-shock intensity required for mice to flinch (sex: F(1,35) = 0.017, p=0.89; rearing condition: F(1,35) = 0.23, p=0.62; interaction: F(1,35) = 0.60, p=0.44) or audibly vocalize (sex: F(1,36) = 3.42, p=0.072; rearing condition: F(1,36) = 1.72, p=0.19; interaction: F(1,36) = 0.32, p=0.57). For flinch: Ctrl males = 12, females = 10; LB males = 8, females = 9. For audible vocalization: Ctrl males = 12, females = 10; LB males = 9, females = 9. For all graphs the mean and SEM are presented. A three-way repeated measure ANOVA was used to assess differences in shock reactivity during conditioning. A two-way ANOVA followed by Sidak’s multiple comparison analysis was used to assess differences in flinching and vocalization. *=p < 0.05, **=p < 0.01, ***=p < 0.001.

LB increased PV-positive cell density early in life in the BLA, but not mPFC

The altered trajectory of fear expression could have arisen from developmental changes in circuits supporting this behavior, including mPFC and BLA, which are known to be involved in emotional regulation and fear conditioning (Arruda-Carvalho and Clem, 2015; Etkin et al., 2011; Sierra-Mercado et al., 2011). Our prior work demonstrated that LB rearing accelerates markers of development, including PV interneuron maturation, in the hippocampus (Goodwill et al., 2018). Therefore, we predicted that deficits in freezing could be due to ELA effects on interneuron maturation in either the BLA or mPFC. To test this hypothesis, we tested for developmental changes in markers of subclasses of interneurons across these distinct brain structures.

Using immunohistochemistry, we labeled PV-positive interneurons, a late differentiating subclass of inhibitory interneurons (Bartolini et al., 2013; Mukhopadhyay et al., 2009; Rymar and Sadikot, 2007), in the brain of LB and control reared male mice at PND 16, 21, 28, 50, and 75. PV-positive interneurons are known to begin differentiating between PND 10–28, reaching a mature phenotype at approximately PND 30 (Berdel and Moryś, 2000; Dávila et al., 2008). In BLA, LB led to an increased PV-positive cell density at PND 21 when compared to control reared mice at this age (Figure 4A). No differences in PV-positive cell density was observed at PND 16, with both groups expressing low densities of PV-positive cells. Furthermore, the difference in PV-positive cell density between LB and control groups subsided by PND 28, with control PV-positive cell density rising to match those observed in LB mice. The increase in PV-positive cell density in the BLA of LB mice at PND 21 was not observed in the other regions of the fear circuit assessed here, including the prelimbic (PL; Figure 4A center) and infralimbic (IL; Figure 4A right) subregions of the mPFC. A non-significant decrease in PV-positive cell density in LB mice was observed at PND 50 in BLA, IL, and PL. This decrease in PV-positive cell density was not observed in other brain regions such as the primary motor cortex and somatosensory cortex (Figure 4—figure supplement 1). Thus, the decrease in PV-positive cell density may be indicative of changes in parvalbumin protein concentration within BLA, IL, and PL as mice transition into adulthood. Studies assessing protein concentrations in combination with additional labeling techniques will be needed to fully determine whether this represents a true decline. Together these findings suggest that LB increased PV positive cell density in the BLA but not the mPFC, suggesting region-specific effects of LB rearing.

Figure 4. LB alters select markers for neuronal populations in mPFC and BLA.

(A) Representative image of immunohistochemical labeling in the BLA of an LB PND 21 mouse (left). From left to right, graphs showing the density of PV-positive cells in the BLA, PL, and IL of male mice at different ages. Immunohistochemical analysis revealed that LB significantly increased Parvalbumin positive (PV+) neurons at PND 21 in BLA (t10 = 2.40, p=0.037) but not in PL (t10 = 0.83, p=0.42) or IL (t10 = 0.17, p=0.86). Except for PL at PND 75 (t9 = 2.85, p=0.018), no differences were observed at PND 16, 28, 50 or 75 respectively for BLA (t8 = 1.07, p=0.31; t8 = 0.85, p=0.41; t9 = 1.65, p=0.13; t9 = 0.48, p=0.63), PL (t8 = 1.26, p=0.24; t10 = 0.54, p=0.59; t11 = 1.40, p=0.18; t9 = 2.85, p=0.018), or IL (t8 = 1.03, p=0.33; t10 = 1.15, p=0.27; t11 = 1.12, p=0.28; t9 = 0.81, p=0.43). For BLA (Ctrl n = 5, 7, 5, 6, 6; LB n = 5, 5, 5, 5, 5). For PL and IL (Ctrl n = 5, 6,6,7,6; LB n = 5, 6, 6, 6, 5). (B) Western blot analysis of BLA tissue. Diagram and sample blots of PND 35 mice are shown. Graphs showing differing protein levels of mice from infancy into adolescence. No differences between LB and control mice were observed in BLA for calretinin (t11 = 1.90, p=0.083; t11 = 1.28, p=0.22; t11 = 0.37, p=0.71; t12 = 0.14, p=0.88), calbindin (t11 = 0.94, p=0.36; t11 = 0.12, p=0.90; t11 = 0.46, p=0.65; t12 = 0.53, p=0.60), VGLUT1 (t11 = 1.53, p=0.15; t12 = 0.26, p=0.79; t12 = 0.41, p=0.68; t12 = 0.18, p=0.85), or myelin basic protein (t11 = 1.08, p=0.30; t11 = 0.16, p=0.87; t12 = 0.86, p=0.40) levels. For calbindin, calretinin, and myelin basic protein (Ctrl n = 7, 7, 6, 7; LB n = 6, 6, 7, 7). For VGLUT1 (Ctrl n = 6, 7, 7, 7; LB n = 7, 7, 7, 7). (C) Western blot analysis of mPFC tissue. Diagram and sample blots of PND 35 mice are shown. Graphs showing differing protein levels of male mice from infancy into adolescence. No differences between LB and control mice were observed in mPFC for calretinin (t10 = 1.84, p=0.095, t12 = 1.71, p=0.11, t9 = 0.35, p=0.73, t11 = 0.88, p=0.39), calbindin (t10 = 2.01, p=0.071; t12 = 1.02, p=0.32; t9 = 0.19, p=0.85; t11 = 0.67, p=0.51), or VGLUT1 (t10 = 0.42, p=0.68; t12 = 0.15, p=0.88; t12 = 0.26, p=0.79; t12 = 0.85, p=0.41) levels. An increase in mPFC Myelin Basic Protein was observed exclusively at PND 28 (t9 = 2.54, p=0.031), but not at PND 21 (t12 = 0.80, p=0.43) or 35 (t11 = 0.97, p=0.35). For calbindin, calretinin, and myelin basic protein (Ctrl n = 6, 7, 6, 7; LB n = 6, 7, 5, 6). No effects of VGLUT1 were observed in the mPFC. For VGLUT1 (Ctrl n = 6, 7, 7, 7; LB n = 6, 7, 7, 7). Bars represent group means + / - SEM. Unpaired two-tailed t-tests between control and LB of a given age were used for A, B and C. *=p < 0.05, **=p < 0.01, ***=p < 0.001.

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Density of Immunohistochemically stained parvalbumin-positive cells (PV+) in the rostral primary motor cortex and the rostral primary somatosensory cortex of male mice.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

(A) LB mice had a significantly lower density of PV-positive cells compared to Ctrl at PND 16 (t9 = 3.21, p=0.010). By PND 21 LB cell density increased significantly above Ctrl levels (t9 = 2.76, p=0.022). No difference in PV+ cells were detected at PND 28 (t11 = 0.60, p=0.56) or 50 (t9 = 0.78, p=0.45). A significantly higher density of PV-positive cells was observed in LB relative to control mice at PND 75 (t9 = 2.49, p=0.033). (B) In somatosensory cortex, LB mice had a significantly lower density of PV-positive cells compared to Ctrl at PND 16 (t9 = 3.17, p=0.011), which was resolved by PND 21, with no significant differences being observed at PND 21 (t9 = 0.31, p=0.75), 28 (t11 = 0.60, p=0.55), 50 (t9 = 0.61, p=0.55), or 75 (t9 = 0.73, p=0.47). For both panels Ctrl n = 5, 5, 7, 6, 6; LB n = 6, 6, 6, 5, 5. For all graphs the mean and SEM are presented. A two-tailed paired student t-test was used to test between group differences. *=p < 0.05, **=p < 0.01, ***=p < 0.001.

Next, western blot analysis was used to determine if LB impacted protein levels for other classes of interneurons (calbindin/calretinin), glutamatergic neuronal markers (VGLUT1), or markers of myelination (myelin basic protein) across early developmental time points in male mice (Figure 4B and C). Western blot analysis allowed us to detect differences between LB and Ctrl mice that could not be detected through immunohistochemical labeling due to poor quality of staining in brains from young animals. In BLA, LB had no effect on calretinin, calbindin, VGLUT1, or myelin basic protein levels at any age tested (Figure 4B). Furthermore, in mPFC, LB did not affect levels of calretinin, calbindin, or VGLUT 1. However, LB was associated with increased myelin basic protein levels in mPFC at PND 28 (Figure 4C). Together, LB appeared to have driven a cell-type-specific developmental increase in PV-positive cells in BLA.

PV inhibition in BLA rescues the fear expression deficit

We then hypothesized that the precocious maturation of PV-positive cells in BLA may have impacted the expression of freezing behavior at PND 22. The focus on PV-positive cells was based on previous reports that have demonstrated a role for BLA PV-positive cells in modulating fear expression (Davis et al., 2017; Wolff et al., 2014). To test this hypothesis, we used transgenic mice that selectively express the optogenetic construct halorhodopsin in PV-positive cells. Mice that express halorhodopsin were also positive for EGFP, allowing us to verify ELA effects on cell density. This strategy allowed us to silence PV-positive cells in a time and region-specific manner.

We first replicated the ability of ELA to increase PV-positive cell density in the BLA by quantifying EGFP-positive cells in control and LB reared mice from the PV Halo mice (PV-CreHet/floxed NpHRHet), which co-express an EGFP reporter on PV-positive cells. Again, LB was associated with an increase in BLA PV-positive cell density at PND 21 mirroring prior immunohistochemical findings (Figure 5A). Specifically, at PND 21, LB PV Halo mice had a greater density of PV positive cells in the BLA compared to age matched control PV Halo mice (Figure 5A), with no effect of LB rearing on PV-positive cell density in PL or IL in this mouse line (Figure 5B). No effect of LB rearing at PND 21 was observed for other genetically labeled cell populations within the BLA, such as somatostatin and VGlut 2 (Figure 5—figure supplement 1).

Figure 5. Optogenetic inactivation of parvalbumin-positive (PV+) cells was sufficient to increase fear expression at PND 22 in LB mice.

(A) Representative image of PV-positive cells in BLA of Het PV-Cre/Het floxed halo (‘PV Halo’) mouse line (left). LB increased the density of PV-positive cells in the BLA (t8 = 3.49, p=0.0081; n = 5 per group). (B) Representative image of PV+ cells in the mPFC of a PV Halo (left). LB did not affect the density of PV-positive cells in PL (t6 = 1.72, p=0.13) or IL (t6 = 0.80, p=0.45) compared with control reared mice (n = 4 per group). (C) Graph showing conditioning and recall test. Optogenetic inhibition occurred during the conditioning tones. Ctrl and LB reared Light control (Het PV-Cre/null floxed Halo) and PV Halo (Het PV-Cre/Het floxed Halo) mice were bilaterally implanted with an optical fiber into BLA, optic fiber placements are shown (left). To assess effects of optogenetic inhibition of PV cells in LB reared mice we compared LB light controls versus LB PV Halo mice. When analyzing PV inhibition in LB mice, a main effect of tone presentation (F(5,45) = 6.97, p<0.0001) was observed, indicating mice had learned the tone/foot shock association. Optogenetic inactivation of PV positive cells in LB reared mice increased freezing during conditioning (F(1,9) = 6.21, p=0.034), and resulted in an increase in freezing on the recall test 24 hr later (t9 = 2.69, p=0.024). No interaction between tone presentation and experimental condition was observed (F(5,45) = 1.55, p=0.19) between LB light control and LB PV Halo mice. For LB: light control n = 5, for LB: PV Halo n = 6. When assessing the effects of PV-positive cell inhibition between Ctrl: Light control and Ctrl: PV Halo mice, a repeated measure ANOVA revealed a main effect of tone presentation (F(5,45) = 9.08, p<0.0001), and optogenetic inactivation (F(1,9) = 9.5, p=0.013). No main effect of tone by optogenetic inactivation (F(5,45) = 0.96, p=0.44) was observed. No differences in memory retrieval were observed between the Ctrl: light control and the Ctrl: PV Halo mice (t9 = 1.29, p=0.22). For additional locomotion controls and cFos analysis of inhibition see Figure 4—figure supplement 1. A repeat measure two-way ANOVA was used to assess differences in fear conditioning, for all other analysis two-tailed unpaired student t-tests were used. Bars represent group means + / - SEM. *=p < 0.05, **=p < 0.01, ***=p < 0.001.

Figure 5.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1. LB does not affect Somatostatin or VGlut2-positive cell densities in the BLA at PND 21.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1.

(A) Genetic labeling of somatostatin-positive cells (left), derived from Sst-Cre mice crossed with an Ai14 (Td-Tomato) reporter mouse. No significant differences were observed between Ctrl and LB male mice (t7 = 0.91, p=0.39). Ctrl n = 4; LB n = 5. (B) Genetic labeling of slc17a6 ‘VGlut2’-positive cells (left) generated by crossing a VGlut2-Cre mouse with an Ai14 (Td-Tomato) reporter mouse. No significant differences were observed between Ctrl and LB male mice (t6 = 1.06, p=0.32). Ctrl n = 4; LB n = 5. A two-tailed paired student t-test was used to test between group differences. For all graphs the mean and SEM are presented. *=p < 0.05, **=p < 0.01, ***=p < 0.001.
Figure 5—figure supplement 2. Optogenetic inhibition did not affect locomotion but did increase cFos in the BLA.

Figure 5—figure supplement 2.

(A) Graphs showing the effect of optogenetic inhibition of PV+ cells in the BLA on locomotion (left) and anxiety-like behavior (right). Optogenetic inhibition of PV+ cells did not affect distance traveled (t11 = 0.36, p=0.72) or the time spent in the center of the open field (t11 = 1.40, p=0.18) when compared to light controls. For light control n = 7, for PV Halo n = 6. (B) Graph showing the effects of optogenetic inactivation on cFos labeling within the BLA, following 15 min of unilateral inhibition in the home cage. Immunohistochemical labeling for cFos revealed that unilateral optogenetic inactivation of PV+ cells increased cFos-positive cells in the inhibited side of the brain (t4 = 2.89, p=0.044) when compared to the uninhibited side (n = 5). Two-tailed unpaired student t-tests were used to analyze data in panel A, while a two-tailed paired student t-test was used to assess differences in cFos labeling in panel B. For all graphs, the mean and SEM are presented and represent data from a mix of male and female mice. *=p < 0.05, **=p < 0.01, ***=p < 0.001.

To test if inhibiting PV-positive cells in the BLA of LB reared mice during fear learning could rescue the observed freezing deficits at PND 22, we optogenetically silenced PV positive cells in the BLA during fear conditioning. Consistent with our prediction, bilateral inactivation of PV-positive cells in the BLA during the conditioning tones resulted in increased freezing 24 hr later, during the recall test (Figure 5C). To ensure that the increased freezing was not due to effects on locomotion or anxiety-like behavior, mice were placed in an open field and PV-positive cells in the BLA were optogenetically inhibited and the behavior of mice was tracked. No effects of optogenetic inhibition of PV-positive cells were found for measures of anxiety-like behavior or general locomotor activity of mice (Figure 5—figure supplement 2A). To ensure that inhibition of PV-positive cells was modulating BLA activity, a subset of unilaterally implanted mice were administered light for 15 min while freely moving in their homecage. Since parvalbumin neurons work to inhibit neuronal activity, we hypothesized that inactivation of the inhibitory interneurons should lead to increased activity of excitatory neurons and therefore increased expression of the immediate early gene cFos. We found that mice exhibited increased cFos labeling in the optogenetically inhibited side when compared to the non-inhibited (control) side (Figure 5—figure supplement 2B). Further, electrophysiological control experiments carried out by our lab in the OFC of the same mouse line demonstrated robust inhibition of PV-positive cells in response to light (Goodwill et al., 2018). Together, these results suggest that the inhibition of PV-positive cells in the BLA of LB reared mice is sufficient to overcome LB induced freezing deficits at this age.

LB rearing increased PL to BLA anatomical connectivity in females

Previous research has demonstrated that ELA can alter mPFC to BLA connectivity in humans (Fan et al., 2014; Gee et al., 2013; Herringa et al., 2016) (reviewed in Herzberg and Gunnar, 2020; VanTieghem and Tottenham, 2018), and BLA to mPFC anatomical (Honeycutt et al., 2020), and functional (Bolton et al., 2018; Guadagno et al., 2018; Yan et al., 2017) connectivity in rodents. Since LB male and female mice exhibited diminished fear expression at PND 21, and PL projections into BLA are known to promote fear learning, while IL projections into BLA support fear extinction (Do-Monte et al., 2015a; Giustino and Maren, 2015; Lee and Choi, 2012; Sierra-Mercado et al., 2011), we sought to investigate whether LB altered anatomical connectivity between mPFC and BLA in our hands. To test if LB altered the timing and density of projections from mPFC to BLA, the retrograde tracer cholera toxin B (CTB) was injected unilaterally into the BLA and the number of labeled cells in PL and IL were quantified. Injections were performed 1 day prior to the time point of interest (e.g. PND 15 for PND 16) and perfusions performed 1 day later (e.g. PND 17 for PND 16). Four time points were tested (PND 16, 21, 28 and 35; Figure 6A–C). As labeling could be affected by efficiency of CTB uptake following injection and BLA placement, we used an ANCOVA analysis with BLA area at the site of injection and CTB injection area as covariates. Previous research has reported no effects of LB rearing on BLA volume at PND 20 (Guadagno et al., 2018). Therefore, we interpreted the corrected estimated mean densities, obtained through the ANCOVA analysis as accounting for between animal differences in CTB injection size and injection placement (potential limitations of this approach may be found in the Discussion and Materials and methods). An independent ANCOVA was conducted for each age group and each sex (for analysis of Ctrl males vs Ctrl females see Figure 6—figure supplement 1). In males, LB did not affect the density of PL to BLA or IL to BLA labeled cells at PND 16, 21, 28 or 35. However, LB female mice did show an increase in PL to BLA projecting cells at PND 21 and 28 when compared to Ctrl females (Figure 6D and E). LB females did not differ from Ctrl females in IL to BLA projection densities. Next, we sought to determine if LB altered the balance of PL and IL inputs into BLA across development. We determined the relative difference in PL and IL projections as a factor of the total number of labelled cells for each mouse ((PL - IL) / (PL + IL)). This value, or projection index, accounts for between-subject differences in labeling efficiency (Figure 6F). No differences in projection index were found between LB and Ctrl reared male mice. A significant increase in the projection index was observed in LB reared females when compared to Ctrl females at PND 16, but not at PND 21, 28 or 35, suggesting an upwards shift in PL over IL inputs into BLA at that age in females. These results suggest that females may be more sensitive to LB induced alterations in the development of mPFC anatomical connectivity to BLA.

Figure 6. LB altered mPFC to BLA anatomical connectivity in females but not males.

(A) Diagram of CTB 594 retrograde injection into the BLA. (B) Tracings of CTB injections into the BLA. (C) Example picture of injection in the BLA (left), and of cells labelled in mPFC (right). (D) Graphs showing the differences in PL to BLA corrected estimated mean densities with 95% confidence intervals from infancy (PND16) into adolescence (PND 35). In males, the density of PL to BLA projecting cells in LB mice did not differ at PND 16 (F(1,6) = 2.46, p=0.16), 21 (F(1,7) = 1.69, p=0.23), 28 (F(1,6) = 0.035, p=0.85) or 35 (F(1,6) = 3.08, p=0.13) when compared to control mice. In females, a significant increase in PL to BLA projecting cells density was observed in LB mice at PND 21 (F(1,4) = 21.74, p=0.010) and 28 (F(1,10) = 7.44, p=0.021) but not at PND 16 (F(1,6) = 1.47, p=0.27) or 35 (F(1,6) = 0.001, p=0.97). (E) Graphs showing the differences in IL to BLA corrected estimated mean densities with 95% confidence intervals from infancy into adolescence. LB did not affect the density of IL to BLA projections at any age for males (16: F(1,6) = 3.71, p=0.10; 21: F(1,7) = 0.22, p=0.65; 28: F(1,6) = 0.003, p=0.958; 35: F(1,6) = 2.17, p=0.19) or females (16: F(1,6) = 0.091, p=0.77; 21: F(1,4) = 5.35, p=0.082; 28: F(1,10) = 2.212, p=0.168; 35: F(1,6) = 5.10, p=0.065). For panels D and E (Ctrl male n = 5, 5, 5, 6; LB male n = 5, 6, 5, 4; Ctrl female n = 5, 4, 7, 5; LB female n = 5, 4, 7, 5). (F) Graphs showing the differences in projection index from infancy into adolescence. LB altered the balance of PL and IL projections to BLA at PND 16 in females (t7 = 3.09, p=0.0175). No other significant differences were observed in the projection index for males (16: t8 = 1.05, p=0.32; 21: t9 = 1.47, p=0.17; 28: t8 = 0.83, p=0.42; 35: t8 = 0.60, p=0.56) or females (21: t6 = 2.22, p=0.068; 28: t11 = 0.75, p=0.46; 35: t8 = 1.61, p=0.14). (Ctrl male n = 5, 5, 5, 6; LB male n = 5, 6, 5, 4; Ctrl female n = 4, 4, 6, 5; LB female n = 5, 4, 7, 5). For D and E dots represent estimated group means with 95% confidence intervals. Estimated means were derived from ANCOVA analysis using two covariates: 1) the area of the CTB injection and 2) the area of BLA where the injection occurred. An ANCOVA was run per age per sex. Statistical significance was determined through post ANCOVA univariate comparison. For panel F, dots represent individual values, bars represent group mean + / - SEM. Unpaired two-way student t-tests were used to assess statistical significance between control and LB mice per age per sex in panel F. *=p < 0.05, **=p < 0.01, ***=p < 0.001.

Figure 6.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1. Sex differences in mPFC to BLA projections in control reared mice.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1.

(A) Control females and males had significantly different corrected estimated mean densities of cells projecting from PL to BLA at PND 16 (F(1,6) = 6.18, p=0.047). Sex differences were no longer significant at PND 21(F(1,5) = 2.23, p=0.19), 28 (F(1,8) = 3.19, p=0.11), or 35 (F(1,7) = 0.90, p=0.37). No sex differences were observed between control reared females and males at any of the ages tested for IL to BLA projections (16: F(1,6) = 4.86, p=0.069; 21: F(1,5) = 0.31, p=0.59; 28: F(1,8) = 0.25, p=0.62; 35: F(1,7) = 0.97, p=0.35). (Ctrl males n = 5, 5, 5, 6; Ctrl females n = 5, 4, 7, 5). (B) Comparison of projection index between control reared male and female mice revealed a significant difference only at PND 21 (t7 = 2.40, p=0.047) but not at any of the other ages tested (16: t7 = 0.056, p=0.95; 28: t9 = 0.36, p=0.72; 35: t9 = 0.12, p=0.90). (Ctrl males n = 5, 5, 5, 6; Ctrl females n = 4, 4, 6, 5). For panel A, corrected estimated means and the 95% confidence intervals are shown. The corrected mean 95% confidence intervals were obtained through ANCOVA analysis run per brain region per age. Statistical significance was determined through post ANCOVA univariate comparison. For panel B, dots represent individual values, bars represent group mean + / - SEM. Unpaired two-way student t-tests were used to assess statistical significance between control and LB mice per age per sex in panel F. *=p < 0.05, **=p < 0.01, ***=p < 0.001.

Discussion

Here, we identify LB effects on the developmental expression of cued fear learning and provide a potential causal link between the neurodevelopmental and behavioral consequences of LB rearing on fear learning and developmental fear expression. We found that LB decreased physical growth during postnatal development, with a more robust effect on the body than the brain. Delayed brain growth was accompanied by significant effects on the timing of maturation (as indexed by developmental change in cell density or protein levels) for specific cell populations in select brain regions. LB led to a selective increase in PV-positive cell density within the BLA at PND 21 (peri-weaning). Furthermore, LB reared mice at PND 21 were able to learn, but not express, an auditory conditioned fear response, which was rescued by inhibiting PV-positive cell activity in the BLA of LB reared mice. In addition, LB rearing led to sex-specific changes in the anatomical connectivity of PL and BLA in female peri-adolescent mice. Together this data supports the idea that LB rearing alters the timing of maturation of subclasses of cells in key brain regions supporting threat learning and the expression of fear memories, with possible implications for later pathology development.

Previous research in humans has shown that childhood resource insecurity in the form of poverty, specifically during infancy, is associated with altered regional brain volume, with the prefrontal cortex and amygdala being affected (Hair et al., 2015; Hanson et al., 2013; Luby et al., 2013; Sheridan et al., 2012). However, the mechanisms by which poverty confers risk for altered brain development and how these factors impact the development of region and cell specific populations of neurons remains largely unknown. Here, rearing mice under conditions of resource restriction, altered the development of select populations of neurons in the mPFC and BLA, key regions involved in fear learning and threat assessment. In the BLA, LB led to an earlier rise in the density of PV positive cells, and a sex and age-specific increase in PL inputs to BLA. Further, LB altered levels of myelin basic protein at select developmental time points in the mPFC, effects that may contribute to altered development and functional readouts for behaviors that were not assessed here. Consistent with our results, studies in humans have found that childhood poverty decreases prefrontal activity in adulthood and reduces the ability of prefrontal cortex to suppress amygdala activity (Kim et al., 2013).

Given the developmental changes observed in LB reared mice, we sought to test if these effects may be related to changes in threat assessment and responding to fear associated stimuli. As the mPFC and BLA are critical for fear learning, mice at different developmental ages underwent Pavlovian tone-shock association learning (Anglada-Figueroa and Quirk, 2005; Arruda-Carvalho and Clem, 2015; Etkin et al., 2011; Giustino and Maren, 2015; Sierra-Mercado et al., 2011). Control reared mice showed the expected developmental trend of being able to condition to a tone-shock association at PND 21, but not at PND 16. Interestingly, LB reared animals were unable to express the conditioned memory 24 hr after conditioning at either PND 16 or PND 21, indicating a later developmental emergence of this behavior. At PND 21, LB mice were able to express the conditioned memory at a short delay (6 hr), indicating that deficits in freezing were not due to an inability to engage the freezing response. Furthermore, the freezing response re-emerged 7 days following conditioning, indicating that learning had occurred. Thus, LB may disrupt the developmental ability of animals to express, but not form, specific types of conditioned fear memories.

Previous research from our group and others have found effects of LB on depressive-like behavior in females (Goodwill et al., 2019), but failed to find significant differences in anxiety-like behavior in adult mice (Goodwill et al., 2019; Manzano Nieves et al., 2019; Manzano-Nieves et al., 2018; Naninck et al., 2015). Consistent with previous work, we did not find an effect of LB on general measures of anxiety. However, LB mice, when compared to Ctrls, had increased total time performing head dips in the elevated plus maze (EPM) at PND 21 and decreased number of entries in the light/dark box at PND 28 and 35, suggesting that LB rearing may be altering aspects of risk assessment, which were not directly tested in this study. Furthermore, we report no effects of LB rearing on auditory fear conditioning in young adults. This finding is in conflict with previous reports demonstrating a significant effect of LB rearing on auditory fear conditioning and expression (Guadagno et al., 2018). Differences between our fear conditioning results and Guadagno et al., 2018 may be due to species differences (activity levels, learning differences, stress tolerance) or LB protocol (administration from PND 1–9 vs PND 4–11, cage changes versus no cage changes). It is possible that differences in LB paradigm may alter the frequency of abusive behaviors leading to differences in outcomes (Gallo et al., 2019). Detailed, meticulous studies comparing the timing of LB protocol administration and its effects on maternal behavior will be needed to understand how timing and duration of LB rearing impact auditory fear conditioning.

Previous research from our lab has demonstrated that LB can induce accelerated PV-positive cell differentiation in the hippocampus (Bath et al., 2016), but may also selectively decrease PV-positive cell counts in brain regions such as the orbitofrontal cortex in adult females (Goodwill et al., 2018). Consistent with reported effects in the hippocampus (Bath et al., 2016), we found that LB led to an earlier rise in PV-positive cell density in BLA, with PV-positive cell density being higher in LB reared mice compared to controls at PND 21. However, PV-positive cells are not increased across all brain regions or across all timepoints. Analysis of PV-positive cell density in rostral primary somatostatin cortex (S1) and rostral primary motor cortex (M1) revealed decreased cell counts in LB mice at PND 16 when compared to controls (Figure 4—figure supplement 1). The decreased PV-positive cell density in M1 and S1 were not persistent, with no effects being observed in S1 at any other age tested, and with increased PV-positive cell density being observed in M1 at PND 21 and 75. Together our data on PV-positive cell density across development and across brain regions revealed a mosaic effect of LB on PV-positive cell density. The mosaic effect may be the consequence of differences in the developmental timing of maturation of these brain regions, with seeding of PV-positive precursors occurring prior to the ELA manipulation in some areas. Alternatively, this may represent differing sensitivity of these regions to the stress signals (e.g. through differing regional expression of glucocorticoid (GR) and mineralocorticoid (MR) receptors, whose activation may have transcriptional effects impacting cellular maturation and plasticity). Future research testing potential mechanisms underlying region-specific sensitivity will be needed.

PV-positive neuronal activity in the BLA has been shown to be capable of modulating fear expression (Davis et al., 2017; Wolff et al., 2014). In addition, PV-positive cell density has previously been shown to be negatively correlated with innate threat responses in post-weanling rats (Santiago et al., 2018). Consistent with the previously described role of PV-positive cells in BLA, we found that mice reared under LB conditions had a decreased fear response, as indexed by lower freezing to a tone previously paired with a shock at PND 21. We posit that the precocious rise in PV positive cells in BLA may have blunted the ability to express, but not learn a fear association. In support of this interpretation of the data, optogenetic inhibition of PV-positive cells in BLA was sufficient to rescue the low freezing phenotype of PND 21 LB reared mice. However, other inhibitory neuronal populations within the BLA are also capable of modulating fear expression (Krabbe et al., 2018; Lucas and Clem, 2018; Rovira-Esteban et al., 2019; Wolff et al., 2014), thus, questions regarding the potential of other populations of neurons to be able to mask the effect of LB rearing remain to be explored. Although we did not observe differences between LB and control mice in somatostatin or VGLUT 2 cell densities at PND 21, this does not rule out the possibility of changes in physiological properties of these classes of cells. LB in rats has been shown to increase spine density and dendritic length of neurons and increase the excitability of BLA (Guadagno et al., 2018). Thus, additional studies will be needed to fully examine the role of the effects of LB rearing on diverse neuronal populations and on behaviors across development. The work included here significantly extends our knowledge of the role of PV positive cells in the development of fear expression, and possible mechanisms through which ELA can drive developmental changes in threat assessment and fear expression.

We show that LB increased PV-positive cell counts, and that this effect may have driven the decrease in fear expression at PND 21. However, it is also possible that LB-induced changes in mPFC to BLA anatomical connectivity may have contributed to the decreased freezing observed at PND 22. Previous studies in rats have shown that the mPFC can modulate auditory fear conditioning as early as PND 24, but not at PND 17 (Kim et al., 2009). Furthermore, it has been shown that stimulation of the IL subregion of the mPFC can decrease fear expression (Do-Monte et al., 2015a). Through the analysis of PL to BLA and IL to BLA projection densities, we found that LB females had increased PL to BLA estimated mean densities at PND 21 and 28. LB males did not significantly differ from Ctrl males at any age. Given that the deficits in fear expression at PND 22 are observed in both males and females, it is unlikely that differences in mPFC connectivity are driving the observed effects on cue associated fear learning. Additional experiments will be required to determine if increased PL to BLA anatomical connectivity in LB females is leading to increased functional connectivity, or the sex selective elevation in depressive-like behaviors we previously observed in females following LB rearing (Goodwill et al., 2019). Furthermore, future studies will be needed to examine the mechanisms and the consequences of the increased anatomical connectivity between these regions.

In sum, this work reveals complex effects of limited resource rearing, on the timing of neuronal and circuit maturation, and the developmental expression of fear learning. We found that resource restriction in the form of LB during early development can drive regional and cell selective effects on maturation, with profound implications for behavioral development. Specifically, we have found that LB increased PV-positive cell density in the BLA at PND 21, and transiently altered the ability of mice to express a threat-associated memory. The transient suppression of threat-associated fear behavior may increase risk taking (Haushofer and Fehr, 2014), increase the chances of incurring a secondary stressor, and mask emotional symptoms of early life trauma during the peri-weaning period, only to emerge as pathology in adolescence. Such observations may explain the latent period of neuropsychiatric symptom expression in children exposed to early life trauma (Teicher et al., 2009). Future studies assessing the contribution of altered development of these fear circuits to later symptom development will be needed.

Materials and methods

Key resources table.

Reagent type
(species) or resource
Designation Source or reference Identifiers Additional
information
Strain, strain background pvalbtm1(cre)ArbrPV-Cre’ The Jackson Laboratory RRID:IMSR_JAX:008069
Strain, strain background Gt(ROSA)26Sortm39(CAG-hop/EYFP)/HZE
‘floxed Halo’
The Jackson Laboratory RRID:IMSR_JAX:014539
Strain, strain background Ssttm2.1(cre)Zjhsomatostatin-Cre’ The Jackson Laboratory RRID:IMSR_JAX:013044
Strain, strain background Slc17a6tm2(cre)Lowl‘vGlut2-Cre’ The Jackson Laboratory RRID:IMSR_JAX:016963
Strain, strain background Gt(ROSA)26Sortm14(CAG-tdTomato)Hze mice
‘Ai14’
The Jackson
Laboratory
RRID:IMSR_JAX:007908
Antibody Anti-Parvalbumin
(rabbit polyclonal)
Millipore Sigma RRID:AB_838238 (1:1,000)
Antibody Anti-c-Fos
(rabbit polyclonal)
Millipore Sigma RRID:AB_2631318 (1:20,000)
Antibody Anti- β-Tubulin (mouse monoclonal) Cell Signaling RRID:AB_2715541 (1:2,000)
Antibody Anti-GAPDH
(rabbit monoclonal)
Cell Signaling RRID:AB_561053 (1:2,000)
Antibody Anti-Calretinin
(mouse monoclonal)
Swant RRID:AB_10000320 (1:500)
Antibody Anti-Calbindin
(mouse monoclonal)
Swant RRID:AB_10000347 (1:500)
Antibody Anti-Myelin Basic Protein
(rabbit polyclonal)
Abcam RRID:AB_1141521 (1:1000)
Antibody Anti-VGLUT1
(rabbit polyclonal)
Millipore Sigma RRID:AB_2814811 (1:1000)
Antibody Anti-Mouse IgG (H+L) (donkey polyclonal) Jackson ImmunoResearch RRID:AB_2340770 (1:2000)
Antibody Anti-Rabbit IgG (H+L)
(donkey polyclonal)
Jackson ImmunoResearch RRID:AB_10015282 (1:2000)
Chemical compound, drug Cholera Toxin Subunit B (Recombinant), Alexa Fluor 594 Conjugate Fisher Scientific C22842 1.0 mg/ml
Software, algorithm Noldus Ethovision XT Noldus RRID:SCR_000441 Version 11
Software, algorithm SPSS SPSS RRID:SCR_002865 Version 26
Software, algorithm GraphPad Prism GraphPad RRID:SCR_002798 Version 8

Subjects

Approximately 1091 C57BL/6N wildtype and 63 transgenic, male and female, mice were used in this study. Original breeding stock was ordered from Charles River Labs. All wild-type C57BL/6N mice were bred in house. For optogenetic experiments, pvalbtm1(cre)ArbrPV-Cre’ (JAX#008069) and Gt(ROSA)26Sortm39(CAG-hop/EYFP)/HZE ‘floxed Halo’ (JAX#014539) mouse lines were derived from a breeding stock acquired from Jackson laboratories. For optogenetic experiments, homozygous PV-Cre mice were bred with heterozygous floxed Halo mice resulting in two groups of offspring, Het PV-Cre/null floxed Halo (Light Controls) and Het PV-Cre/Het floxed Halo (PV Halo). For genetic labeling of select neuronal populations, homozygous Ssttm2.1(cre)Zjhsomatostatin-Cre’ (JAX# 013044) or homozygous Slc17a6tm2(cre)Lowl‘vGlut2-Cre’ (JAX#016963) mice were crossed with homozygous Gt(ROSA)26Sortm14(CAG-tdTomato)Hze mice ‘Ai14’ (JAX#007908) mice to allow expression of the Ai14 reporter in a Cre-dependent manner. All animals were housed according to NIH guidelines and maintained on a 12 hr light:dark cycle. Lights were on from 7:30 am to 7:30 pm, with all experiments being conducted during the light period. Mice had free access to food and water throughout the study. All animal procedures were approved by the Brown University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee and consistent with the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals.

Fragmented maternal care

LB was modeled through a resource restriction paradigm, in which dam and pups were placed in low bedding conditions with limited access to nesting material for 7 consecutive days (PND four through PND 11). This manipulation results in a fragmentation in maternal care (Bath et al., 2016; Rice et al., 2008). Four days after the birth of a litter (PND 4), the dam and pups were transferred from their standard home cage with cob bedding and a 4 × 4 cm cotton nestlet to an LB cage containing a wire mesh floor and a 2 × 4 cm cotton nestlet. The mice continued to have ad libitum access to food and water. Following 1 week (PND 11), pups and dams were returned to their standard housing with full bedding and nesting material. Standard reared mice (designated as Controls—Ctrl) were left undisturbed in a standard home cage until weaning. All pups were weaned and sex segregated at PND 21, with the exception of mice tested at PND 21, which were weaned following the completion of fear conditioning experiments at PND 22.

Mouse body and brain weight

To analyze mouse body and brain weights mice were deeply anesthetized with pentobarbital (Beuthanasia 150 mg/kg IP). Mice were first weighed to obtain the full body weight, then the brains were quickly removed. To ensure that brain collection was complete and carried out in an identical manner between groups, the brain stem was cut at the level of the occipital bone and the premaxila and nasal bones were crushed at the rostral most level of the eyes. This allowed us to remove the whole brain with the cerebellum and the intact olfactory bulbs. Brains were then weighed. One mouse was sacrificed at a time to ensure minimum protein degradation as brains were subsequently flash frozen prior to protein extraction for western blot analysis.

Fear conditioning

Fear conditioning was carried out in Med Associates (St. Albans City, VT) operant chambers. On days 1 and 2, mice were habituated to two distinct (differing in color, texture, and smell) chambers. Habituation trials lasted 5 min per chamber and were counterbalanced. On day 3, mice received tone-shock associative learning in the fear conditioning chamber. During fear conditioning, mice were presented with six tones (30 s, 4 KHz, 75 dB) with each tone co-terminating with a 1 s foot-shock (0.57 mA). Tone shock pairings were separated by an inter-trial interval of 1.5 min. Testing for fear expression occurred on day 4 of the testing protocol, unless otherwise stated. Fear expression testing consisted of exposing mice to two tones in the control chamber (habituated chamber where no fear conditioning occurred). Different cohorts of animals, across multiple litters were used to test mice at the different developmental time points. Freezing behavior was scored automatically by the activity tracker module in Noldus Ethovision XT 11.0 and verified from video by observers blind to treatment and condition but not to age (as age could be inferred based upon differences in mouse size).

Light/dark box

Mice were tested in a Light/dark box that was built in house. To begin a trial, mice were placed in the dark side of the box which was connected to a light side by a small opening (~6×6 cm). To increase the brightness of the light side (~139 Lux), a lamp, pointing toward the light side, was mounted on the lid of the dark side. The dimensions of the dark and light side of the chamber were the same and measured (height = 23 cm, width = 22 cm, length = 26 cm). A trial lasted a total of 10 min. Activity of the mouse during a trial was recorded and analyzed using Ethovision XT 11.0 software, with latency to first exit being hand scored by an observer blind to sex, age, and condition.

Immunohistochemistry

To assess the relative density of PV-positive cells and c-Fos expressing cells, immunohistochemistry was performed on control and LB mice on the days stated in each experiment. Briefly, mice were deeply anesthetized with pentobarbital, transcardially perfused with buffered saline followed by 4% paraformaldehyde, and processed for immunohistochemistry as previously described (Bath et al., 2016). For PV-positive cell labeling, a rabbit anti-parvalbumin antibody (1:1,000; Millipore) was used. For c-Fos labeling a rabbit anti c-Fos (1:20,000; Millipore) was used. Brain sections (40 µm) were mounted on charged glass slides, counter stained using a Hema three staining set (Fisher Scientific Company), dehydrated, and coverslipped for imaging. For prelimbic, infralimbic, rostral primary motor cortex, and rostral primary somatosensory cortex, brain sections from AP 1.94 to 1.54 were analyzed. For the amygdala, brain sections from AP −1.22 to −2.06 were analyzed.

Western blot

Male mice were sacrificed, brains were quickly dissected, weighed, and flash frozen on dry ice. The medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), and the basolateral amygdala (BLA) were dissected and stored at −80°C until processing.

Tissues were homogenized in RIPA buffer (with 1% protease and phosphatase inhibitor cocktail, Fisher Scientific) and supernatant was collected following centrifuging at 14,000 rpm at 4°C for 10 min. Protein concentration was determined with a bicinchoninic acid (BCA) kit (Thermo Scientific, Waltham, MA). Protein lysates were each diluted to 1.0 mg/mL with RIPA buffer, heated at 90°C for 10 min, and proteins separated by gel electrophoresis on a 12% sodium dodecyl sulfate–polyacrylamide gel electrophoresis (SDS-PAGE) gel, and transferred to polyvinylidene difluoride (PVDF) membrane. For the remaining portion of the western blot protocol, transfer membranes were kept at 4°C.

Membranes were blocked for 1 hr in 5% non-fat milk in Tris-buffered saline Tween-20 (TBST, containing 10 mM Tris, 150 mM NaCl, and 0.1% Tween-20, pH 7.6), followed by incubation with primary antibodies diluted in 5% non-fat milk/0.5% bovine serum albumin in TBST at 4°C overnight. Membranes were washed with TBST three times (15 min per wash) and incubated with secondary antibody in 5% non-fat milk/0.5% bovine serum albumin in TBST for 1 hr. Membranes were then washed with TBST three times (15 min per wash), then visualized with Amersham ECL Western Blotting Detection Reagent (RPN2106, GE Life Sciences) using a C600 Azure Biosystems imaging system (Dublin, CA). Densitometry analysis was conducted with a gel imaging module of NIH ImageJ software.

Primary antibodies used for western blotting

Mouse anti-β-Tubulin (1:2000, Cell Signaling Technology), rabbit anti-GAPDH (1:2000, Cell Signaling Technology), mouse anti-Calretinin (1:500, Swant), mouse anti-Calbindin (1:500, Swant) rabbit anti-Myelin Basic Protein (1:1000, Abcam), rabbit anti-VGLUT1 (1:1000, Millipore). Secondary antibodies used for this study were: HRP conjugated donkey anti-mouse (1:2000, Jackson ImmunoResearch) and donkey anti-rabbit (1:2000, Jackson ImmunoResearch).

Cholera toxin B injections

Alexa 594 conjugated cholera toxin B (CTB) (Fisher Scientific) was used to retrogradely label the projections from PL and IL to BLA, in male and female mice, at postnatal ages PND 16, 21, 28, and 35. CTB (1.0 mg/mL) was injected (0.15 ul) into the left BLA 1 day prior to the time-point of interest (e.g. PND 15 for PND 16). In order to inject the CTB into BLA across development separate coordinates were used for each age. Developmentally appropriate coordinates were empirically derived from pilot surgeries. Coordinates used for the CTB injections were as follows: (PND 15: DV = −5.075, ML = −3.05, AP = −1.1; PND 20: DV = −5.1, ML = −3.1, AP = −1.15; PND 34: DV = −5.2, ML = −3.15, AP = −1.2). Mice were perfused 48 hr post-injection. The brain was dissected, sectioned (40 µm), mounted, counter-stained with DAPI (Fisher Scientific) and visualized using a fluorescent microscope. The density of CTB positive cells in the PL and IL was measured.

Optogenetic surgery and inhibition of PV+ cells

Female mice homozygous for Cre under the control of a parvalbumin driver (JAX#008069- Pvalbtm1(cre)Arbr) were crossed with a male heterozygous floxed Halo (JAX#014539- Gt(ROSA)26Sortm39(CAG-hop/EYFP)Hze). The cross resulted in the selective expression of halorhodopsin in PV+ cells (Het PV-Cre, Het Halo ‘PV Halo mice’) and mice from the same litter that were Cre positive, but lacked the optogenetic channel (Het PV-Cre, null floxed Halo ‘Light control’). Mice were bilaterally implanted with an in-house made ceramic optic fiber (Ø200 µm Core, 0.50 NA; Thorlabs, Newtown, NJ) at PND 15 above BLA (Placements: DV = −5.1, ML = = + / - 3.1, AP = −1.15). For surgeries, mice were anesthetized with isoflurane gas anesthesia (2.0%–2.5% in 1 l/min oxygen) and secured to the stereotaxic apparatus. The scalp was shaved and cleaned, Buprenex (0.1 mg/kg, as an analgesic) was administered intraperitoneally and lidocaine was applied. The skin above the skull was removed to expose the skull. A ∼1 mm–diameter craniotomy was drilled above BLA. The right side implant was lowered and temporarily secured with Metabond (Parkell Inc, Brentwood, NY) while the left side fiber optic was placed. Following the implantation of the second optic fiber, a thin layer of metabond was placed above the skull to adhere the dental acrylic to the skull. After the dental adhesive solidified, isoflurane administration was stopped, and mice were allowed to wake.

Mice began fear conditioning protocol at PND 19 (as described above), with optogenetic inhibition occurring at PND 21 (Figure 5). During conditioning, PV+ cells in the BLA were photo-inhibited with constant light (620 nm Plexbright LED, Plexon, Dallas, TX), using an LED driver (Plexon, Dallas, TX) during the 30 s of the tone (including the 1 s foot-shock). The light power delivered, as measured through the optic fiber pre-implant, ranged from 1.5 to 2 mW per side. Following the fear conditioning protocol, a random subset of the mice were tested for locomotion in an open field under conditions of light stimulation.

Microscopy

Neurolucida software was used to analyze immunohistochemical data. Either a light (for DAB staining) or epi-fluorescent microscope (for fluorescence) was used when appropriate. For quantification of neuronal cell density, brain regions were traced at 4x magnification and borders were defined as shown in Paxinos and Franklin mouse brain atlas. Immunoreactive or fluorescent positive neurons within each region were identified by an observer blind to condition and treatment (10x). All region contours with identified cells were saved and the number of cells and area within each contour was assessed using StereoInvestigator. For each brain region, 3–4 sections per brain were averaged to obtain a mean density.

Statistical analyses

A two-tailed student’s t-test was used to compare between two groups. When more than two groups were assessed the appropriate ANOVA was performed as stated in the figure legends. All ANOVA tests were followed by Sidak’s multiple comparison test, assessing the effects of treatment at each given age and/or assessing developmental differences within each treatment.

Two separate analyses of CTB retrograde injection data are presented in Figure 6. For Figure 6D–E, the density of CTB-positive cells in PL and IL (obtained as described in the microscopy section above), was analyzed through the use of an ANCOVA analysis. Two covariates were used in the ANCOVA analysis: 1) the area of the BLA at the site of the injection and 2) the area of the injected CTB at the site of the injection. The site of the injection was defined as the brain section where the needle track was at the ventral most point. The area of the injected CTB was used as a correction for injected volume, while the area of BLA at the injected site was used as a correction for rostral - caudal placement, as the BLA increases in area as you move caudally. Using the two covariates, the ANCOVA analysis performed per age per sex per region, returned an estimated mean value, the 95% confidence intervals, and the pairwise comparison statistics presented in Figure 6D–E. A secondary analysis, projection index, correcting for efficiency of labeling is presented in Figure 6F. For this analysis, the density of CTB labeling (obtained as described in the microscopy section above) in IL was subtracted from PL (PL-IL) and divided by the total number of cells labeled (PL+IL). The projection index [(PL-IL)/(PL+IL)] thus accounts for differences in labeling efficiencies between mice. This data is presented as mean + / - SEM, with statistical significance being determined by a two-tailed student t-test conducted per age, per sex, as detailed in the figure legend.

With the exception of data in Figure 6D–E, Statistical analysis was performed using Prism Graphpad statistical and graphing software. Figure 6D–E was analyzed with SPSS statistical software. All data were graphed using Prism Graphpad statistical and graphing software. For all statistical analyses, statistical significance was defined as p<0.05.

Methods relevant to supplementary figures

Open field test

To test for differences in locomotor activity and anxiety-like behavior, mice were placed in an open field arena as previously described (Goodwill et al., 2019). Distance moved and the time spent in the center of the arena were recorded during a 7 (Figure 3—figure supplement 3) or 5 (Figure 5—figure supplement 2) minute test using the Ethovision video-tracking system. The arena was digitally divided into two zones (center and periphery), as previously described (Goodwill et al., 2019). Decreased time in the center was used as an indicator of anxiety-like behavior.

Elevated plus maze

To assess anxiety-like behavior at PND 21 mice were placed in an elevated plus maze as previously described (Manzano Nieves et al., 2019; Manzano-Nieves et al., 2018). The EPM consisted of two open (unprotected) and two closed (protected) arms. Greater time in the closed (protected) arms is defined as higher anxiety-like behavior. Mice were allowed 7 min to explore the maze. The time spent in the protected vs. unprotected arms was assessed. Videos were recorded, and behavior was tracked using Noldus Ethovision XT 10.0 software. All trials were conducted under low-light conditions (~109 Lux). Time spent and distance walked in the open and closed arms of the EPM were assessed using the mouse tracking module. The amount of time, and number of instances, that the animals spent with their head below the surface of the open arms (Head Dips) was manually scored by an independent observer blind to the sex and condition of the mice.

Shock sensitivity assay

To assess the minimum foot-shock intensity required to elicit a behavioral response (visible flinch or audible vocalization), PND 21 mice were placed in an operant conditioning chamber (Med associates, Fairfax, VT). Mice were exposed to a series of foot shocks, beginning at 0.06 mA and increasing at 0.02 mA intervals (Manzano-Nieves et al., 2018). Each shock intensity was presented three times. The amplitude of the foot-shock at which a given mouse first flinched, and/or audibly vocalized to 2 out of 3 foot shocks at a given intensity was recorded by two independent observers blind to condition to insure agreement on these measures. Flinching was defined as the mouse moving its body reflexively downward, making its body smaller, directly following a foot-shock. Vocalization was defined as the emittance of an audible sound.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by funding by the National Institutes of Health, from the NIMH (MH115914- KGB; MH115049- KGB) and NINDS (NS105219- GMN). The authors thank Angelica Johnson for providing the hand scored data for the EPM, as well as Hyeyoung Shin and Roberto Andres Aponte Rivera for providing valuable feedback on the project. We thank the Barry Connors lab and the Christopher Moore lab at Brown University for facilitating access to the transgenic mice, and the Rebecca Burwell lab for permitting us the use of their lab’s microscope.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Kevin G Bath, Email: Kevin_Bath@Brown.edu.

Matthew N Hill, University of Calgary, Canada.

Kate M Wassum, University of California, Los Angeles, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • National Institutes of Health MH115914 to Kevin G Bath.

  • National Institutes of Health MH115049 to Kevin G Bath.

  • National Institutes of Health NS105219 to Gabriela Manzano Nieves.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing - original draft.

Supervision, Investigation, Methodology, Writing - review and editing.

Investigation, Methodology, Writing - review and editing.

Conceptualization, Resources, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing - review and editing.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: This study was performed in strict accordance with the recommendations in the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals of the National Institutes of Health. All of the animals were handled according to approved institutional animal care and use committee (IACUC) protocols (#19-10-0003) of Brown University.

Additional files

Transparent reporting form

Data availability

Data has been deposited in the Brown Digital Repository with the following https://doi.org/10.26300/9krc-h052.

The following dataset was generated:

Bath KG, Nieves GM, Bravo M, Baskoylu S. 2020. Data from Early life adversity decreases pre-adolescent fear expression by accelerating amygdala PV cell development. Brown Digital Repository.

References

  1. Adhikari A, Lerner TN, Finkelstein J, Pak S, Jennings JH, Davidson TJ, Ferenczi E, Gunaydin LA, Mirzabekov JJ, Ye L, Kim SY, Lei A, Deisseroth K. Basomedial amygdala mediates top-down control of anxiety and fear. Nature. 2015;527:179–185. doi: 10.1038/nature15698. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Agid O, Shapira B, Zislin J, Ritsner M, Hanin B, Murad H, Troudart T, Bloch M, Heresco-Levy U, Lerer B. Environment and vulnerability to major psychiatric illness: a case control study of early parental loss in major depression, bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Molecular Psychiatry. 1999;4:163–172. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000473. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Anglada-Figueroa D, Quirk GJ. Lesions of the basal amygdala block expression of conditioned fear but not extinction. Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;25:9680–9685. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2600-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Arruda-Carvalho M, Clem RL. Prefrontal-amygdala fear networks come into focus. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience. 2015;9:145. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2015.00145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bartolini G, Ciceri G, Marín O. Integration of GABAergic interneurons into cortical cell assemblies: lessons from embryos and adults. Neuron. 2013;79:849–864. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.08.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bath KG, Manzano-Nieves G, Goodwill H. Early life stress accelerates behavioral and neural maturation of the hippocampus in male mice. Hormones and Behavior. 2016;82:64–71. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2016.04.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bath KG, Nitenson AS, Lichtman E, Lopez C, Chen W, Gallo M, Goodwill H, Manzano-Nieves G. Early life stress leads to developmental and sex selective effects on performance in a novel object placement task. Neurobiology of Stress. 2017;7:57–67. doi: 10.1016/j.ynstr.2017.04.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Berdel B, Moryś J. Expression of calbindin-D28k and parvalbumin during development of rat's basolateral amygdaloid complex. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience. 2000;18:501–513. doi: 10.1016/S0736-5748(00)00024-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bolton JL, Molet J, Regev L, Chen Y, Rismanchi N, Haddad E, Yang DZ, Obenaus A, Baram TZ. Anhedonia following Early-Life adversity involves aberrant interaction of reward and anxiety circuits and is reversed by partial silencing of amygdala Corticotropin-Releasing hormone gene. Biological Psychiatry. 2018;83:137–147. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.08.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bolton JL, Short AK, Simeone KA, Daglian J, Baram TZ. Programming of Stress-Sensitive neurons and circuits by Early-Life experiences. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2019;13:30. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bouwmeester H, Smits K, Van Ree JM. Neonatal development of projections to the basolateral amygdala from prefrontal and thalamic structures in rat. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2002;450:241–255. doi: 10.1002/cne.10321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Burgos-Robles A, Vidal-Gonzalez I, Quirk GJ. Sustained conditioned responses in prelimbic prefrontal neurons are correlated with fear expression and extinction failure. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:8474–8482. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0378-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Chan T, Kyere K, Davis BR, Shemyakin A, Kabitzke PA, Shair HN, Barr GA, Wiedenmayer CP. The role of the medial prefrontal cortex in innate fear regulation in infants, juveniles, and adolescents. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:4991–4999. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5216-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Cohen MM, Malter Cohen M, Jing D, Yang RR, Tottenham N, Lee FS, Casey BJ. Early-life stress has persistent effects on amygdala function and development in mice and humans. PNAS. 2013;110:3110. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1310163110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Corbett SS, Drewett RF. To what extent is failure to thrive in infancy associated with poorer cognitive development? A review and meta-analysis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2004;45:641–654. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00253.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Courtin J, Chaudun F, Rozeske RR, Karalis N, Gonzalez-Campo C, Wurtz H, Abdi A, Baufreton J, Bienvenu TCM, Herry C. Prefrontal parvalbumin interneurons shape neuronal activity to drive fear expression. Nature. 2014;505:92–96. doi: 10.1038/nature12755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Cressman VL, Balaban J, Steinfeld S, Shemyakin A, Graham P, Parisot N, Moore H. Prefrontal cortical inputs to the basal amygdala undergo pruning during late adolescence in the rat. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2010;518:2693–2709. doi: 10.1002/cne.22359. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Cunningham MG, Bhattacharyya S, Benes FM. Amygdalo-cortical sprouting continues into early adulthood: Implications for the development of normal and abnormal function during adolescence. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2002;453:116–130. doi: 10.1002/cne.10376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Dávila JC, Olmos L, Legaz I, Medina L, Guirado S, Real MA. Dynamic patterns of colocalization of Calbindin, parvalbumin and GABA in subpopulations of mouse basolateral amygdalar cells during development. Journal of Chemical Neuroanatomy. 2008;35:67–76. doi: 10.1016/j.jchemneu.2007.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Davis P, Zaki Y, Maguire J, Reijmers LG. Cellular and oscillatory substrates of fear extinction learning. Nature Neuroscience. 2017;20:1624–1633. doi: 10.1038/nn.4651. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Do-Monte FH, Manzano-Nieves G, Quiñones-Laracuente K, Ramos-Medina L, Quirk GJ. Revisiting the role of infralimbic cortex in fear extinction with optogenetics. Journal of Neuroscience. 2015a;35:3607–3615. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3137-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Do-Monte FH, Quiñones-Laracuente K, Quirk GJ. A temporal shift in the circuits mediating retrieval of fear memory. Nature. 2015b;519:460–463. doi: 10.1038/nature14030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Draijer N, Langeland W. Childhood trauma and perceived parental dysfunction in the etiology of dissociative symptoms in psychiatric inpatients. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 1999;156:379–385. doi: 10.1176/ajp.156.3.379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Etkin A, Egner T, Kalisch R. Emotional processing in anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2011;15:85–93. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.11.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Fan Y, Herrera-Melendez AL, Pestke K, Feeser M, Aust S, Otte C, Pruessner JC, Böker H, Bajbouj M, Grimm S. Early life stress modulates amygdala-prefrontal functional connectivity: Implications for oxytocin effects. Human Brain Mapping. 2014;35:5328–5339. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22553. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Felitti VJ, Anda RF, Nordenberg D, Williamson DF, Spitz AM, Edwards V, Koss MP, Marks JS. Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults. the adverse childhood experiences (ACE) Study. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 1998;14:245–258. doi: 10.1016/s0749-3797(98)00017-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Gallo M, Shleifer DG, Godoy LD, Ofray D, Olaniyan A, Campbell T, Bath KG. Limited bedding and nesting induces maternal behavior resembling both hypervigilance and abuse. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2019;13:167. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00167. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Gee DG, Gabard-Durnam LJ, Flannery J, Goff B, Humphreys KL, Telzer EH, Hare TA, Bookheimer SY, Tottenham N. Early developmental emergence of human amygdala-prefrontal connectivity after maternal deprivation. PNAS. 2013;110:15638–15643. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1307893110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Giustino TF, Maren S. The role of the medial prefrontal cortex in the conditioning and extinction of fear. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2015;9:298. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Goodwill HL, Manzano-Nieves G, LaChance P, Teramoto S, Lin S, Lopez C, Stevenson RJ, Theyel BB, Moore CI, Connors BW, Bath KG. Early life stress drives Sex-Selective impairment in reversal learning by affecting parvalbumin interneurons in orbitofrontal cortex of mice. Cell Reports. 2018;25:2299–2307. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2018.11.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Goodwill HL, Manzano-Nieves G, Gallo M, Lee H-I, Oyerinde E, Serre T, Bath KG. Early life stress leads to sex differences in development of depressive-like outcomes in a mouse model. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2019;44:711–720. doi: 10.1038/s41386-018-0195-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Guadagno A, Wong TP, Walker CD. Morphological and functional changes in the preweaning basolateral amygdala induced by early chronic stress associate with anxiety and fear behavior in adult male, but not female rats. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry. 2018;81:25–37. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2017.09.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Hair NL, Hanson JL, Wolfe BL, Pollak SD. Association of child poverty, brain development, and academic achievement. JAMA Pediatrics. 2015;169:822–829. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2015.1475. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Hanson JL, Hair N, Shen DG, Shi F, Gilmore JH, Wolfe BL, Pollak SD. Family poverty affects the rate of human infant brain growth. PLOS ONE. 2013;8:e80954. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0080954. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Haushofer J, Fehr E. On the psychology of poverty. Science. 2014;344:862–867. doi: 10.1126/science.1232491. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Heim C, Nemeroff CB. The role of childhood trauma in the neurobiology of mood and anxiety disorders: preclinical and clinical studies. Biological Psychiatry. 2001;49:1023–1039. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01157-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Herringa RJ, Burghy CA, Stodola DE, Fox ME, Davidson RJ, Essex MJ. Enhanced Prefrontal-Amygdala connectivity following childhood adversity as a protective mechanism against internalizing in adolescence. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging. 2016;1:326–334. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2016.03.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Herzberg MP, Gunnar MR. Early life stress and brain function: Activity and connectivity associated with processing emotion and reward. NeuroImage. 2020;209:116493. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116493. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Honeycutt JA, Demaestri C, Peterzell S, Silveri MM, Cai X, Kulkarni P, Cunningham MG, Ferris CF, Brenhouse HC. Altered corticolimbic connectivity reveals sex-specific adolescent outcomes in a rat model of early life adversity. eLife. 2020;9:e52651. doi: 10.7554/eLife.52651. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Hult M, Tornhammar P, Ueda P, Chima C, Bonamy AK, Ozumba B, Norman M. Hypertension, diabetes and overweight: looming legacies of the biafran famine. PLOS ONE. 2010;5:e13582. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0013582. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Kim JH, Hamlin AS, Richardson R. Fear extinction across development: the involvement of the medial prefrontal cortex as assessed by temporary inactivation and immunohistochemistry. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:10802–10808. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0596-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Kim P, Evans GW, Angstadt M, Ho SS, Sripada CS, Swain JE, Liberzon I, Phan KL. Effects of childhood poverty and chronic stress on emotion regulatory brain function in adulthood. PNAS. 2013;110:18442–18447. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1308240110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Koenen KC, Widom CS. A prospective study of sex differences in the lifetime risk of posttraumatic stress disorder among abused and neglected children grown up. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 2009;22:566–574. doi: 10.1002/jts.20478. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Krabbe S, Gründemann J, Lüthi A. Amygdala inhibitory circuits regulate associative fear conditioning. Biological Psychiatry. 2018;83:800–809. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.10.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Lee YK, Choi JS. Inactivation of the medial prefrontal cortex interferes with the expression but not the acquisition of differential fear conditioning in rats. Experimental Neurobiology. 2012;21:23–29. doi: 10.5607/en.2012.21.1.23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Luby J, Belden A, Botteron K, Marrus N, Harms MP, Babb C, Nishino T, Barch D. The effects of poverty on childhood brain development: the mediating effect of caregiving and stressful life events. JAMA Pediatrics. 2013;167:1135–1142. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2013.3139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Lucas EK, Clem RL. GABAergic interneurons: The orchestra or the conductor in fear learning and memory? Brain Research Bulletin. 2018;141:13–19. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2017.11.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Maniam J, Antoniadis C, Morris MJ. Early-Life stress, HPA Axis adaptation, and mechanisms contributing to later health outcomes. Frontiers in Endocrinology. 2014;5:73. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2014.00073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Manzano Nieves G, Schilit Nitenson A, Lee HI, Gallo M, Aguilar Z, Johnsen A, Bravo M, Bath KG. Early life stress delays sexual maturation in female mice. Frontiers in Molecular Neuroscience. 2019;12:27. doi: 10.3389/fnmol.2019.00027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Manzano-Nieves G, Gaillard M, Gallo M, Bath KG. Early life stress impairs contextual threat expression in female, but not male, mice. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2018;132:247–257. doi: 10.1037/bne0000248. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. McCauley J, Kern DE, Kolodner K, Dill L, Schroeder AF, DeChant HK, Ryden J, Derogatis LR, Bass EB. Clinical characteristics of women with a history of childhood abuse: unhealed wounds. Jama. 1997;277:1362–1368. doi: 10.1001/jama.1997.03540410040028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. McLoyd VC. Socioeconomic disadvantage and child development. American Psychologist. 1998;53:185–204. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.53.2.185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Moriceau S, Shionoya K, Jakubs K, Sullivan RM. Early-life stress disrupts attachment learning: the role of amygdala corticosterone, locus ceruleus corticotropin releasing hormone, and olfactory bulb norepinephrine. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:15745–15755. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4106-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Mukhopadhyay A, McGuire T, Peng C-Y, Kessler JA. Differential effects of BMP signaling on parvalbumin and somatostatin interneuron differentiation. Development. 2009;136:2633–2642. doi: 10.1242/dev.034439. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Mullen PE, Martin JL, Anderson JC, Romans SE, Herbison GP. The long-term impact of the physical, emotional, and sexual abuse of children: A community study. Child Abuse & Neglect. 1996;20:7–21. doi: 10.1016/0145-2134(95)00112-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Naninck EFG, Hoeijmakers L, Kakava-Georgiadou N, Meesters A, Lazic SE, Lucassen PJ, Korosi A. Chronic early life stress alters developmental and adult neurogenesis and impairs cognitive function in mice. Hippocampus. 2015;25:309–328. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22374. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Rice CJ, Sandman CA, Lenjavi MR, Baram TZ. A novel mouse model for acute and long-lasting consequences of early life stress. Endocrinology. 2008;149:4892–4900. doi: 10.1210/en.2008-0633. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Rondó PH, Ferreira RF, Nogueira F, Ribeiro MC, Lobert H, Artes R. Maternal psychological stress and distress as predictors of low birth weight, prematurity and intrauterine growth retardation. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2003;57:266–272. doi: 10.1038/sj.ejcn.1601526. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Rovira-Esteban L, Gunduz-Cinar O, Bukalo O, Limoges A, Brockway E, Müller K, Fenno L, Kim YS, Ramakrishnan C, Andrási T, Deisseroth K, Holmes A, Hájos N. Excitation of diverse classes of cholecystokinin interneurons in the basal amygdala facilitates fear extinction. Eneuro. 2019;6:ENEURO.0220-19.2019. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0220-19.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Rymar VV, Sadikot AF. Laminar fate of cortical GABAergic interneurons is dependent on both birthdate and phenotype. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2007;501:369–380. doi: 10.1002/cne.21250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Salmon K, Bryant RA. Posttraumatic stress disorder in children: The influence of developmental factors. Clinical Psychology Review. 2002;22:163–188. doi: 10.1016/S0272-7358(01)00086-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Santiago AN, Lim KY, Opendak M, Sullivan RM, Aoki C. Early life trauma increases threat response of peri-weaning rats, reduction of axo-somatic synapses formed by parvalbumin cells and perineuronal net in the basolateral nucleus of amygdala. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2018;526:2647–2664. doi: 10.1002/cne.24522. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Sheridan MA, Sarsour K, Jutte D, D'Esposito M, Boyce WT. The impact of social disparity on prefrontal function in childhood. PLOS ONE. 2012;7:e35744. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0035744. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Sierra-Mercado D, Padilla-Coreano N, Quirk GJ. Dissociable roles of prelimbic and infralimbic cortices, ventral Hippocampus, and basolateral amygdala in the expression and extinction of conditioned fear. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36:529–538. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.184. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Strathearn L, Gray PH, O'Callaghan MJ, Wood DO. Childhood neglect and cognitive development in extremely low birth weight infants: a prospective study. Pediatrics. 2001;108:142–151. doi: 10.1542/peds.108.1.142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Teicher MH, Samson JA, Polcari A, Andersen SL. Length of time between onset of childhood sexual abuse and emergence of depression in a young adult sample: a retrospective clinical report. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2009;70:684–691. doi: 10.4088/JCP.08m04235. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. VanTieghem MR, Tottenham N. Neurobiological programming of early life stress: functional development of Amygdala-Prefrontal circuitry and vulnerability for Stress-Related psychopathology. Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences. 2018;38:117–136. doi: 10.1007/7854_2016_42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Vertes RP. Differential projections of the infralimbic and prelimbic cortex in the rat. Synapse. 2004;51:32–58. doi: 10.1002/syn.10279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Wainstock T, Anteby E, Glasser S, Shoham-Vardi I, Lerner-Geva L. The association between prenatal maternal objective stress, perceived stress, preterm birth and low birthweight. The Journal of Maternal-Fetal & Neonatal Medicine. 2013;26:973–977. doi: 10.3109/14767058.2013.766696. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Wang X-D, Chen Y, Wolf M, Wagner KV, Liebl C, Scharf SH, Harbich D, Mayer B, Wurst W, Holsboer F, Deussing JM, Baram TZ, Müller MB, Schmidt MV. Forebrain CRHR1 deficiency attenuates chronic stress-induced cognitive deficits and dendritic remodeling. Neurobiology of Disease. 2011;42:300–310. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2011.01.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Widom CS. Posttraumatic stress disorder in abused and neglected children grown up. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 1999;156:1223–1229. doi: 10.1176/ajp.156.8.1223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Wolff SBE, Gründemann J, Tovote P, Krabbe S, Jacobson GA, Müller C, Herry C, Ehrlich I, Friedrich RW, Letzkus JJ, Lüthi A. Amygdala interneuron subtypes control fear learning through disinhibition. Nature. 2014;509:453–458. doi: 10.1038/nature13258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Yam KY, Naninck EFG, Abbink MR, la Fleur SE, Schipper L, van den Beukel JC, Grefhorst A, Oosting A, van der Beek EM, Lucassen PJ, Korosi A. Exposure to chronic early-life stress lastingly alters the adipose tissue, the leptin system and changes the vulnerability to western-style diet later in life in mice. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2017;77:186–195. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.12.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Yan C-G, Rincón-Cortés M, Raineki C, Sarro E, Colcombe S, Guilfoyle DN, Yang Z, Gerum S, Biswal BB, Milham MP, Sullivan RM, Castellanos FX. Aberrant development of intrinsic brain activity in a rat model of caregiver maltreatment of offspring. Translational Psychiatry. 2017;7:e1005. doi: 10.1038/tp.2016.276. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Matthew N Hill1
Reviewed by: Claire-Dominique Walker2

In the interests of transparency, eLife publishes the most substantive revision requests and the accompanying author responses.

Thank you for submitting your article "Early life adversity decreases fear expression in pre-adolescence by accelerating amygdalar parvalbumin cell development" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, one of whom is a member of our Board of Reviewing Editors, and the evaluation has been overseen by Kate Wassum as the Senior Editor. The following individual involved in review of your submission has agreed to reveal their identity: Claire-Dominique Walker (Reviewer #3).

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

This manuscript examined the impact of early adversity, specifically that caused by resource insecurity, here modeled by limited bedding, on physical and neural development and the trajectory of emotional learning. The experiments are conducted over an age period adequately spanning the late neonatal, early juvenile, adolescent and adult age, in line with the period of postnatal maturation of anatomical connections between basolateral amygdala and prefrontal cortex. This kind of time course work is essential because snapshots of behavior or brain function at single time points either completely miss effects or lead to erroneous conclusions as to the direction of effect. Limited bedding impaired somatic and neural development at a gross level, but resulted in a transient suppression of fear memory recall, which occurs coincident with an elevation in parvalbumin cell density in the basolateral amygdala. This impairment in fear recall was rescued by optogenetic inactivation of basolateral amygdala parvalbumin neurons during fear learning. Thus limited bedding-induced accelerated development of basolateral amygdala parvalbumin neurons seems not influence fear memory consolidation, but does impair recall of the memory the following day. Overall, these studies are carefully run, with internal replication built in and thoughtful data analysis.

Overall, all of the reviewers were positive about the research in the manuscript, and there was only one essential revision issue that was brought up by all reviewers which requires attention, and this is the lack of an appropriate no-LB control optogenetic study on memory recall to determine if the effects of inhibition of PV+ neurons in the BLA have an influence in control mice. Given the nature of this study, it was felt that the addition of this control group would be achievable within the designated timeframe, and so all reviewers agreed this was an essential revision.

In addition to this revision there are several other more minor issues which the reviewers felt required attention and they are listed below. Please address all of these issues in your response letter.

Reviewer comments to address:

1) It is not clear if there has been work done on the fidelity of the mouse Cre line being used demonstrating that halorhodopsin expressing cells are indeed PV+ GABA interneurons. If so, could the authors mention a citation for this, and if not, it would be probably good for the authors to at least show colocalization for Cre expression with vGAT and PV.

2) Why was constant light used to drive halorhodopsin inhibition rather than light pulses? There is evidence that constant light overheats tissue and leads to inadvertent transcriptional activity if tissue is not damaged. For prolonged inhibition of activity, a chemogenetic approach may be more desirable. New work is not necessary (suggested for future) but justification of constant light in methods is needed.

3) It makes sense that releasing PV inhibition in the BLA could enhance freezing behavior. While the authors do not find an effect of ELA on other inhibitory neurons, a valuable control would still be testing whether silencing of any other inhibitory cell type in the BLA produces a similar enhancement of freezing. Given the significant extra work to provide evidence of specificity, authors may instead choose to note possibility/limitations in the text.

4) Why was interneuron maturation assessed by IHC for PV-expressing neurons but by Western for other classes of cells?

5) Attempts to control for CTB injection differences by covarying for injection area are to be commended. It would also be useful to calculate a simple ratio of projection density / injection area to see if significant differences remain.

6) Please remove assertions that the limited bedding mouse paradigm models poverty and/or refugee experience, which is significantly more complex in humans and involves layers of society and not just “disorganized” care. Bedding is only one resource that is restricted for the mice, whereas human poverty impacts nutritional quality, social stability, educational opportunities, etc.

7) Additional discussion of why/how the LB paradigm could accelerate PV+ maturation in some brain regions and delay it in others (encompassing several papers from this lab) is warranted.

8) Why would LB from 4-11 only affect density measures at P35, and only alter ratio at PN21 (Figure 5)? It is not clear how the authors interpret these delayed effects and differences in timing.

9) Reduced body weight gain has been demonstrated in virtually every study using the LB paradigm in both mice and rats. Ratio of brain weight over body weight nicely shows that there is possibly an element of brain sparing to this effect. In Figure 1, data for PND16 should be drawn on a different scale as it is difficult to evaluate the group difference at that age. Obvious and significant differences are reported for older ages. Figure 1—figure supplement 1, the color code for males is confusing and not well contrasted. Brain/body weight is certainly one index of somatic development, but it would have been nice to complement with anogenital distance in both sexes. Do the authors have data pertaining to this variable between controls and LB mice?

10) How exact is the assumption that PV+ cell number is associated with changes in cell differentiation? The authors indicate that LB accelerates the differentiation of PV in the BLA, however, differences could also be due to the level of immunostained signal in the BLA at young ages. Replace the term "differentiation" by increased PV-positive cell number.

11) In Figure 3, the standard errors for PV density are very large for PND21 in the BLA. There is a clear overlap of the standard errors suggesting that the Ctl and LB groups are not significantly different. In this case, the authors should correct their statistical analyses to perform a 2 way ANOVA (age x treatment) followed by post-hoc tests instead of a series of t-tests. This is a critical point as it is one of the main results of the manuscript.

12) The experiments with optogenetic inhibition of PV+ cells in the BLA are very elegant and carefully designed. In Figure 4C, placement of the optical fiber suggests that the LA rather than the BLA was inhibited. Disinhibition of the LA will likely lead to BLA activation and a similar increase in %freezing, but this should be discussed. Please provide the bregma level for the fiber optic placement. Provide a better photomicrograph of mPFC GFP staining. Even with higher magnification, there are no cells that can be detected.

13) ".…the impact of LB on mPFC and BLA connectivity remained unknown." This is inaccurate as other studies have examined connectivity between these two structures after ELA. Please mention and reference these important imaging (DTI and rsfMRI) studies: Bolton et al., 2018, Yan et al., 2010 and Guadagno et al., 2018. The authors could precise: "anatomical connectivity". Throughout the manuscript "anatomical connectivity" should be used. It is unclear how the CTB injection size was determined? In fact, it should be a volume of injection since several sections likely contained CTB. How projection density was normalized should be explained in greater details in methods.

14) Please provide high resolution photomicrographs of the injection site in the BLA as well as the projection areas in mPFC. What is the background for this type of labeling? Please indicate whether you found layer differences in the density of labeling in the mPFC IL and PL.

15) The authors provide figures in the manuscript where data from both sexes are pooled, yet in the supplementary figures, there are significant effects of sex and age on the %freezing during conditioning. I wonder whether these data should not be presented in the manuscript as well as complement to Figure 2. Interestingly also, Figure 2—figure supplement 1 fails to find significant differences in %freezing behavior between Ctl and LB adult mice (both male and female). This is surprising given the assumption that ELA leads to phenotypes of increased anxiety and fear responsiveness as demonstrated by several other groups in similar conditions. This should be discussed.

eLife. 2020 Jul 21;9:e55263. doi: 10.7554/eLife.55263.sa2

Author response


Reviewer comments to address:

1) It is not clear if there has been work done on the fidelity of the mouse Cre line being used demonstrating that halorhodopsin expressing cells are indeed PV+ GABA interneurons. If so, could the authors mention a citation for this, and if not, it would be probably good for the authors to at least show colocalization for Cre expression with vGAT and PV.

We thank the reviewer for raising this point. The group that generated this PV-Cre line demonstrated that more than 90% of PV+ cells are labeled in this mouse line (Hippenmeyer et al., 2005 PLoS Biol). Further, as part of a collaboration for a separate project with Dr. Barry Connors (Goodwill et al., 2019), we have characterized the intrinsic properties of PV Cre x NpHR positive cells in the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) of slices obtained from this line of mice (unpublished data). Based on characterizing the intrinsic property of PV-Cre cells in the OFC, we are confident that the clear majority of the cells (all cells patched to date) exhibit all of the properties of GABAergic PV+ cells. Similar work has been carried out using this same PV-Cre mouse line in combination with AVV-NpHR-EGFP cell infection, to show selective silencing of PV+ cell populations (Royer et al., 2012 Nat Neuro).

2) Why was constant light used to drive halorhodopsin inhibition rather than light pulses? There is evidence that constant light overheats tissue and leads to inadvertent transcriptional activity if tissue is not damaged. For prolonged inhibition of activity, a chemogenetic approach may be more desirable. New work is not necessary (suggested for future) but justification of constant light in methods is needed.

We agree with the reviewer and had a similar concern prior to undertaking these studies, which motivated our specific approach. We previously carried out in vitro optogenetic inhibition of parvalbumin positive cells using NpHR in slices derived from this same mouse line (aforementioned collaboration with Dr. Connors, also see publication- Goodwill et al., 2019). Recordings were carried out at three different current intensities in the same cell from a PV Cre x NpHR mouse. Patched cells had all of the properties of fast spiking PV+ cells under both patch and current stimulation conditions. We found that spiking of these cells could be immediately silenced upon light stimulation, that this silencing was maintained through a 20 minute protocol, and that normative spiking resumed upon light removal, with no rebound inhibition, no enhanced spiking following release, and no obvious effects on the intrinsic properties of these cells. We would like to also note that in our protocol, we employed the use of light generated from LED drivers, which provide lower power, but effective modulation of NpHR. Use of lower power light sources may eliminate potential heating or transcriptional effects that may be observed with continuous higher power light stimulation when using lasers as the light source. Despite this, we have now included a caveat about the use of constant light and possible benefits of using other approaches.

3) It makes sense that releasing PV inhibition in the BLA could enhance freezing behavior. While the authors do not find an effect of ELA on other inhibitory neurons, a valuable control would still be testing whether silencing of any other inhibitory cell type in the BLA produces a similar enhancement of freezing. Given the significant extra work to provide evidence of specificity, authors may instead choose to note possibility/limitations in the text.

We thank the reviewers for this comment and agree that there may be potential effects of silencing of other populations of cells on freezing behavior. For example, in adult animals, manipulation of CCK cells alters engagement of freezing behavior (Rovira Esteban et al., 2019). We agree that the manipulation of activity of a number of other cellular populations may in fact drive changes in freezing behavior (augmentation or inhibition). Our studies, and the choice of inhibition of PV positive cells in BLA, were focused on the observation of altered PV positive cell density at early developmental timepoints, and the attempt to provide proof of concept that they could be influencing freezing levels by releasing the believed net effect of an increased PV influence on regional activity at this age. Our observations support the argument that decreasing the influence of PV at this age can restore freezing behavior, and in the anticipated direction. Experiments manipulating other classes of cells could have interesting and important effects and add to our understanding of the effects of activity of these cells in regional activity and will be important studies to carry out in the future. However, we believed that such studies were outside of the scope of the current manuscript, as we had observed limited effect of LB rearing on gross metrics of cellular maturation for other classes of cells (e.g. SST and vGLut). Experiments silencing SST+ cells may also support changes in freezing behavior, which would be interesting, but may merely represent another mechanism of altering behavioral output, but one that was not likely impacted by LBN rearing. We have added text indicating this caveat and the need to look at the influence of manipulating additional classes of cells on this behavior over development.

4) Why was interneuron maturation assessed by IHC for PV-expressing neurons but by Western for other classes of cells?

We thank the reviewers for bringing up this important point. We had planned to use multiple methodologies for quantification of cell density over development in the varied classes of cells. However, for IHC approaches, the PV antibody was the most robust marker used across development. Work with other antibodies provided exceedingly poor staining in tissue at the early developmental ages that were being assessed (possibly due to developmental changes in antigen availability). Variability in the quality of IHC hampered our ability to conduct such developmental assays for a broader range of markers. Given these limitations, we moved to using alternate methods to verify results for glutamatergic neurons and somatostatin positive neurons. We used additional mice that genetically express Cre recombinase under either the somatostatin (SST) or glutamatergic (Vglut2) promoter. We crossed each of these lines of mice with an Ai14 (Td-tomato reporter mouse). Using this method, we verified a subset of the findings showing no effect of LB rearing on SST+ or vGlut2+ cell density in BLA at P21. This is now included in the supplemental data.

5) Attempts to control for CTB injection differences by covarying for injection area are to be commended. It would also be useful to calculate a simple ratio of projection density / injection area to see if significant differences remain.

Thank you for this suggestion. After running the requested analysis we found that increased PL to BLA projections at PND 21 (p = 0.000757) remained significant while differences at PND 28 were no longer significant (p = 0.099). This analysis strategy did not impact statistical significance for other comparisons.

6) Please remove assertions that the limited bedding mouse paradigm models poverty and/or refugee experience, which is significantly more complex in humans and involves layers of society and not just “disorganized” care. Bedding is only one resource that is restricted for the mice, whereas human poverty impacts nutritional quality, social stability, educational opportunities, etc.

We thank the reviewer for this point and agree that the human condition is much more complex than what is modeled with the limited bedding intervention in mice. We have deleted the requested assertion and have revised the manuscript to reflect that we are observing effects of loss of maternal resources on pup care and development, and do not overreach with our description of this mimicking poverty/refugee experiences.

7) Additional discussion of why/how the LB paradigm could accelerate PV+ maturation in some brain regions and delay it in others (encompassing several papers from this lab) is warranted.

We thank the reviewer for this point and have added brief discussion of this in a recent review paper (Bath 2020, TINS). We now include additional supplemental data showing that LB leads to significant delays in the developmental increase in PV positive cell density in the primary motor and primary somatosensory cortex, supporting those assertions. We have also added a brief discussion of why regionally specific effects may be occurring.

8) Why would LB from 4-11 only affect density measures at P35, and only alter ratio at PN21 (Figure 5)? It is not clear how the authors interpret these delayed effects and differences in timing.

This is an excellent question, which we are currently working to understand. Based upon our prior work in hippocampus and other brain regions, we know that LB from P4-11 may be impacting the migration (less likely) or timing of maturation and differentiation in at least some classes of cells (we focused on PV positive cells). In that prior work, we have found in control animals that PV expression begins to elevate in these regions well after the LB manipulation (P16- Bath et al., 2016) and not reaching peak levels until ~P28. Further, this inhibition likely plays a role in the maturation of the local circuit, functioning in the stabilization and pruning of later developing projections from other brain regions. Thus, LB rearing may have cascading downstream effects on additional maturational processes, which may not manifest until later timepoints. Further, some of these effects may persist, or resolve due to ongoing plasticity in the system, or utilizing alternate strategies and computations to drive shifts in behavior.

9) Reduced body weight gain has been demonstrated in virtually every study using the LB paradigm in both mice and rats. Ratio of brain weight over body weight nicely shows that there is possibly an element of brain sparing to this effect. In Figure 1, data for PND16 should be drawn on a different scale as it is difficult to evaluate the group difference at that age. Obvious and significant differences are reported for older ages. Figure 1—figure supplement 1, the color code for males is confusing and not well contrasted. Brain/body weight is certainly one index of somatic development, but it would have been nice to complement with anogenital distance in both sexes. Do the authors have data pertaining to this variable between controls and LB mice?

We have now included an inset in panels A and B of Figure 1 that provides a zoomed in version of PND 16 data. We have changed the colors in the Figure 1—figure supplement 1 to enhance contrast. We agree that anogenital distance would be a useful complementary measure of somatic development. However, the anogenital distance for Ctrl and LB mice was not recorded as part of this study and we are unable to obtain this data at this time. However, we will strive to include it as part of our developmental measures for future studies.

10) How exact is the assumption that PV+ cell number is associated with changes in cell differentiation? The authors indicate that LB accelerates the differentiation of PV in the BLA, however, differences could also be due to the level of immunostained signal in the BLA at young ages. Replace the term "differentiation" by increased PV-positive cell number.

We have amended the manuscript to reflect the reviewer’s comment. The increase in PV positive cells was confirmed with a reporter mouse line that would be less sensitive to fluctuating PV levels from immunostaining. The term “differentiation” was somewhat loosely used to mean the developmental change in the expression of the PV protein, which we interpreted as the differentiation of this class of cells to express a PV phenotype, not necessarily differentiation from precursor to mature phenotype. To address the reviewer concern, we have now replaced the term differentiation with “increased PV-positive cell density”.

11) In Figure 3, the standard errors for PV density are very large for PND21 in the BLA. There is a clear overlap of the standard errors suggesting that the Ctl and LB groups are not significantly different. In this case, the authors should correct their statistical analyses to perform a 2 way ANOVA (age x treatment) followed by post-hoc tests instead of a series of t-tests. This is a critical point as it is one of the main results of the manuscript.

We thank the reviewers for bringing this to our attention. There was an issue in the rendering of the figure due to last minute changes in the manuscript between the editorial review and the scientific review process, where the graph was accidentally changed to depict standard deviation instead of standard error of the mean. We have now corrected this issue to show the mean and SEM for the data presented. We agree with the reviewer that a 2-way ANOVA would have been a better statistical approach. However, due to logistical issues associated with the number of animals and sections prepared, immunostaining was performed separately for each age. This meant that we could only ensure that the various factors associated with differences in quality of labeling (e.g. incubation durations, separate preparations of primary antibody incubation solution, washes, separate preparation of secondary incubation reagents, and development reagents and incubations) were matched within an age group but not across ages. Because of the way the data was generated we did not feel that it was appropriate to include analysis across ages. The same is true for the western blots.

12) The experiments with optogenetic inhibition of PV+ cells in the BLA are very elegant and carefully designed. In Figure 4C, placement of the optical fiber suggests that the LA rather than the BLA was inhibited. Disinhibition of the LA will likely lead to BLA activation and a similar increase in %freezing, but this should be discussed. Please provide the bregma level for the fiber optic placement. Provide a better photomicrograph of mPFC GFP staining. Even with higher magnification, there are no cells that can be detected.

In accordance with previous work done by members of this research team (Goodwill et al., 2018, Do Monte, 2015) we aimed to place optical fibers on the surface of the brain region to be optically inhibited. However, we do recognize that in this study, the placement of the fibers has the potential to also influence cellular activity in LA. We have now added this caveat to the discussion of our results.

The targeted placement of the optical fibers were as follows: DV = -5.1, ML = = +/- 3.1, AP = -1.15 and is included in the Materials and methods section.

We agree that it was hard to identify the PV+ cells within mPFC and now provide a zoomed in image as part of Figure 5.

13) ".…the impact of LB on mPFC and BLA connectivity remained unknown." This is inaccurate as other studies have examined connectivity between these two structures after ELA. Please mention and reference these important imaging (DTI and rsfMRI) studies: Bolton et al., 2018, Yan et al., 2010 and Guadagno et al., 2018. The authors could precise: "anatomical connectivity". Throughout the manuscript "anatomical connectivity" should be used. It is unclear how the CTB injection size was determined? In fact, it should be a volume of injection since several sections likely contained CTB. How projection density was normalized should be explained in greater details in methods.

We have now provided a more precise description of anatomical connectivity throughout the manuscript. We apologize for the lack of discussion of these additional studies and have now included the recommended citation including additional citation of the human literature. We have also included how the data was normalized in greater detail with the Materials and methods section.

14) Please provide high resolution photomicrographs of the injection site in the BLA as well as the projection areas in mPFC. What is the background for this type of labeling? Please indicate whether you found layer differences in the density of labeling in the mPFC IL and PL.

An example image of the CTB injections into BLA and the cell labeling in mPFC has now been included in Figure 6. Unfortunately, due to limitations at the time of data collection, no layer information was recorded for this dataset.

15) The authors provide figures in the manuscript where data from both sexes are pooled, yet in the supplementary figures, there are significant effects of sex and age on the %freezing during conditioning. I wonder whether these data should not be presented in the manuscript as well as complement to Figure 2. Interestingly also, Figure 2—figure supplement 1 fails to find significant differences in %freezing behavior between Ctl and LB adult mice (both male and female). This is surprising given the assumption that ELA leads to phenotypes of increased anxiety and fear responsiveness as demonstrated by several other groups in similar conditions. This should be discussed.

We thank the reviewer for this request, as we had collected data in both male and female mice for the bulk of our measures, because we are very interested in potential sex differences (Bath 2020- TINS). In prior manuscripts, reviewers have had difficulty with the either the discussion of the complexity associated with sex differences or the additional length of results and discussion when separating out sex specific data, and thus we tried to keep the focus on the main effect (suppressed freezing at P21). We now have happily amended Figure 2 to show the sex specific effects and separated out male and female data in other figures. We have also included a discussion addressing the lack of significant increases in fear expression and anxiety-like behaviors within our LB reared mice. Previous research from our group (Goodwill et al., 2019, Manzano Nieves et al., 2018, Manzano Nieves et al., 2019) and others (Naninck et al., 2015; Others) have found effects of LB on depressive-like behavior in females but failed to find significant differences in anxiety-like behavior in adult mice. Ongoing work in our lab has replicated the lack of effect in auditory fear conditioning in adult mice. However, we are aware that other groups (Guadagno et al., 2018) have found significant effects of LB rearing on fear conditioning and expression. Differences between our fear conditioning results and Guadagno et al., 2018 may be due to species (differences in activity levels, learning differences, stress tolerance) or LB protocol (administration from PND 1-9 vs PND 4-11).

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Data Citations

    1. Bath KG, Nieves GM, Bravo M, Baskoylu S. 2020. Data from Early life adversity decreases pre-adolescent fear expression by accelerating amygdala PV cell development. Brown Digital Repository. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]

    Supplementary Materials

    Transparent reporting form

    Data Availability Statement

    Data has been deposited in the Brown Digital Repository with the following https://doi.org/10.26300/9krc-h052.

    The following dataset was generated:

    Bath KG, Nieves GM, Bravo M, Baskoylu S. 2020. Data from Early life adversity decreases pre-adolescent fear expression by accelerating amygdala PV cell development. Brown Digital Repository.


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES