Abstract
Objective
To evaluate the cognitive function of Chinese patients with Parkinson's disease PD postsubthalamic nucleus deep brain stimulation (STN‐DBS).
Methods
Cognitive function was assessed by neuropsychological methods in PD patients. Twenty matched healthy persons served as normal controls. t test, analysis of variance, and chi‐square analysis were used to compare the difference among the groups. Reliable change index was utilized to analyze the changes in cognition from the individual level.
Results
(a) Improvement in motor function was significantly better after STN‐DBS (P < .01). (b) Notably, the increase error rates of implicit SRTT (serial reaction time task) was significantly higher after STN‐DBS as compared with the conservative therapy group (P = .03). (c) The decline of verbal fluency (explicit) was also significantly higher after STN‐DBS than that in the medication therapy group (P = .03). (d) In the explicit clock‐drawing test, scores had significantly improved after STN‐DBS (P = .04).
Conclusions
STN‐DBS as a neuromodulatory tool in the Chinese PD population not only improves motor symptoms but also cognitive function to a certain extent, such as the decline of executive function and verbal fluency. The explicit cognitive decline was significantly quicker than that in patients on medication therapy. The improvement of visiospatial function was also noted. Implicit memory impairment during the 1‐year follow‐up period was not observed.
Keywords: cognitive function, deep brain stimulation, explicit memory, implicit memory, neuromodulatory, Parkinson's disease, subthalamic nucleus
1. INTRODUCTION
Parkinson's disease (PD) refers to the disruption of the motor loop between the basal ganglia and motor cortex induced by the loss of dopaminergic neurons in the substantia nigra, resulting in resting tremor, muscle tension increase, slow motion, and postural disturbance. 1 PD has become a common neurodegenerative disease in middle‐aged and elderly people. Besides motor symptoms, PD patients also show nonmotor symptoms, such as cognitive impairment, autonomic dysregulation, and sleep disorders, which seriously affect the quality of their life. 2 Currently, deep brain stimulation (DBS) is gradually being used on a large scale, owing to the significant success it has shown in improving motor symptoms, quality of life, and reducing daily drug dependency. 3 , 4
Effects of STN‐DBS on PD patient's cognitive function are still not understood. It is believed that STN‐DBS significantly alleviates motor symptoms through cortex‐basal ganglia‐thalamus‐cortex loop modulation. 5 , 6 However, dose STN‐DBS have similar effect on the cognitive nerve loop? As an increasing number of PD patients choose DBS, there has noticeably an arising postoperative complications particularly relating to cognitive function. A randomized, double‐blind clinical trial confirmed improvement of STN/globus pallidus internus (GPi) DBS on motor symptoms, but found decrease in memory, verbal fluency, and visuospatial function in PD patients after DBS. 7 However, Halpern et al 8 believed that working memory and psychomotor speed were improved after surgery, and that only verbal fluency and executive function decreased. Williams et al 9 found that the incidence of dementia was significantly higher in the STN‐DBS group (32%) than that in the optimal medication therapy group (16%) after 2 years of follow‐up. Aybek et al 10 reported that postoperative dementia is a natural progression of PD.
Memory is one of the key components of cognition. Previous studies have shown that STN‐DBS could interfere working memory. 11 , 12 It has been reported, instances of some patients that could not recognize facial stimuli 13 or discern vocal emotion. 14 We supposed STN‐DBS might impact patients' implicit memory and cause some slight cognitive change. This study attempts to explore the effects of STN‐DBS on cognitive function and its potential mechanism by means of a specific neuropsychological tool.
2. SUBJECTS AND METHODS
The ethics committee of Shanghai Jiao Tong University School of Medicine approved the study (for project number 81071065#). The authors received written informed consent from every participant.
2.1. Patients and groups
Forty‐three PD patients were selected from the Shanghai JiaoTong University School of Medicine in China from August 2013 to August 2015. All subjects were in accordance with the diagnostic criteria for idiopathic Parkinson's disease of United Kingdom Parkinson's Disease Society Brain Bank. 15 None of the patients met the following conditions: (a) invalid dopaminergic therapy; (b) severe psychiatric symptoms, such as schizophrenia symptoms, apathy, suicidal tendencies, and severe depression; (c) history of stroke, brain injury, and intracranial infection; (d) severe heart, liver, lung, kidney, and blood system, endocrine system diseases; (e) history of drug abuse. Three cases were dropped out due to good motor symptoms control and patients delayed follow‐up time point more than 3 months. The drop‐out rate was less than 10%. Twenty PD patients received bilateral STN‐DBS in the Department of Neurosurgery as the STN‐DBS group (DBS group). And twenty PD patients without surgery were matched with STN‐DBS group, received the optimal medication therapy as the medication therapy group (MED group).Twenty (10 females) matched healthy old people, composed the control group, with average age 61.1 ± 5.12 years, education time 8.30 ± 4.95 year). No difference about gender, age, and education time between groups.
All the patients were evaluated for motor symptoms and cognition impairment at the beginning of the study and after 1‐year follow‐up. As a control, 20 healthy candidates were also evaluated for cognition impairment. This control group was then compared with DBS and MED group at baseline. The declination of cognition between DBS group and MED group was compared, read the flowcharts in Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Flowcharts of the study designed
2.2. Clinical evaluation
2.2.1. Assessment of motor symptoms
Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale 16 and Modified Hoehn&Yahr stage (H‐Y stage) 17 were assessed by two certificated neurologists (Dr Wu Xi and Dr Qiu Yiqing) to evaluated patients' motor symptoms at “On time” and “Off time” (patient was not taken medication for 12 hours).
2.2.2. Assessment of cognition
Tests were administered by the neuropsychologist (Dr Wang Xiaoping), included Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE), 18 Wechsler Memory Scale, 19 Verbal fluency, Clock‐drawing, Degraded picture naming, and serial reaction time task(SRTT). 20 The assessment of cognition was launched at “On time” on the day after motor symptoms assessment. The SRTT software version can count response time (Tavg), error rates, and minimum reaction time (Tmin). Attention is quantified by minimum reaction time (Tmin).
2.3. STN‐DBS operation and confirm the leads localization
2.3.1. Operative procedure
All surgeries performed with preoperative cranial 3.0T MRI and standardized stereotactic procedures. The stereotactic apparatus was Leksell G frame (Elekta). The coordinates calculated and trajectory designed with Medtronic S7 Neuro Navigation System (Medtronic Navigation) to target the STN. Bilaterally Medtronic DBS electrodes (Model 3389 electrode; Medtronic) implanted under local anesthesia and IPG (Activa PC; Medtronic) implanted under general anesthesia. All the operations achieved in 1 day. Postsurgery stimulation parameters were set 4 weeks postoperation and gradually achieved the optimal stimulation parameters in 3 months.
2.3.2. Leads localization
The Medtronic S7 Neuro Navigation System (Medtronic Navigation) was used to merge preoperative 3.0T‐MRI images with postoperative CT images. The CT scan images were calibrated at 1 mm thickness and without pneumocranium. The Lead‐DBS software 21 was used to show the electrodes and active contacts the same as our previous work done. 22
2.4. Statistical analysis
Comparison of the overall level—Data were analyzed with SPSS 19.0 software. Measurement data were expressed as mean ± SD or min‐max. A value of α = .05 was considered statistically significant difference. Self‐control group was evaluated with paired t tests. Group comparison evaluated with student t test. Data that not obey normal distribution were analyzed with Mann‐Whitney U rank sum test. Count data were compared by using chi‐square test.
Comparison of the individual level—Some individual cognitive differences may be masked after averaging. Several reliable change indices (RCIs) exist to evaluate statistically significant individual change with repeated neuropsychological assessment. We present reliable change indices (RCIs) to compare statistically reliable cognitive changes between the groups for each neuropsychological measure over time. 23 According to the reliable change indices (RCIs), RCI = (X2−X1)/SEdiff, where X1 is the patient's baseline score, X2 is the patient's 1‐year score, and Sdiff is the standard error of the difference between the test scores. RCI > 1.96 represents remarkable changes before and after test.
3. RESULTS
3.1. Two patients' groups
STN‐DBS group and MED group were both including 20 patients (10 female and 10 male). Two groups had no significant difference at gender, age, disease duration, H –Y grade, UPDRS sore, and LED at baseline (see Table 1).
Table 1.
Clinical data for patients
Baseline data | STN‐DBS Group | MED Group | P value |
---|---|---|---|
Number of patients | 20 | 20 | |
Gender | M10/F10 | M10/F10 | 1.00 |
Age | 59 ± 4.23 | 58.35 ± 6.77 | .718 |
Disease duration | 9.55 ± 2.35 | 8.65 ± 2.3 | .229 |
H‐Y grade(med off) | 2.5(2)3(13)4(3)5(2) | 2.5(3)3(14)4(2)5(1) | .401 |
UPDRS‐Ⅰ | 4.15 ± 1.31 | 4.85 ± 2.28 | .241 |
UPDRS‐Ⅱ(med off) | 19.2 ± 4.65 | 18.55 ± 5.73 | .696 |
UPDRS‐Ⅲ(medon) | 22.3 ± 10.7 | 20.6 ± 7.32 | .670 |
UPDRS‐Ⅲ (medoff) | 48.5 ± 14.4 | 46.70 ± 11.1 | .560 |
UPDRS‐Ⅳ | 5.90 ± 2.36 | 5.85 ± 2.13 | .944 |
LED | 785 ± 236 | 773 ± 352 | .906 |
1‐y follow‐up | |||
UPDRS‐Ⅲ (medon) | 20.9 ± 7.13 | ||
UPDRS‐Ⅲ (medoff IPG on) | 24.4 ± 8.71 | ||
UPDRS‐Ⅲ (medoff IPG off) | 45.8 ± 10.4 | 47.3 ± 11.1 | |
LED | 407 ± 197 | 832 ± 260 |
Gender compare used Fisher's exact probability. Medoff: Off time that stopped anti‐parkinsonism medication at least 12 h; Medon: On time with anti‐parkinsonism medication.
Abbreviation: LED, Levodopa equivalent dose; MED, medication therapy; STN‐DBS, subthalamic nucleus deep brain stimulation.
3.2. Deep brain stimulation
All 20 STN‐DBS patients had no surgical and hardware‐related complications after 1‐year follow‐up. Two of the patients gradually abandon anti‐parkinsonism medication after the optimal stimulation. The location of electrodes and active contacts were shown in Figure 2. Nineteen patients and 39 electrode used monopolar stimulation, but one patients' left electrode used interleaving mode for speech stuttering. The average stimulation parameters were amplitude of 2.35 ± 0.20 volts, frequency of 130 ± 28.5 hertz, and pulse width of 74.6 ± 17.5 microseconds.
Figure 2.
Location of leads and active contacts. The figure showed red nucleus (dark red), subthalamic nucleus (yellow), internal globuspallidus (green), and external globuspallidus (glue). The active contacts were red, and their centers were marked as red spots
3.3. Assessment of motor symptoms
No significant difference in UPDRS scores was detected between the DBS group and MED group at baseline. As displayed in Table 1, motor function were significantly improved 49.7% in the DBS group postoperation (MED off IPG on) compared with that preoperation (MED off) (P < .001), postoperative required drug dose significantly decreased 48.2% (P < .001). Two patients totally stop taking medication at the optimal DBS stimulation.
3.4. Assessment of cognitive function
3.4.1. Memory
Memory quotient measured by Wechsler Memory Scale reflects explicit memory. Student‐Newman‐Keuls pairwise comparison test results showed that memory quotient was significantly lower in the DBS and MED groups than in the control group (F = 8.62, P < .01; Figure 3A). There was no significant difference in long‐term memory and instantaneous memory obtained by the superposition of the scale of the same type of subtest among DBS, MED and control groups (F = 2.13, P > .05;F = 1.54; P > .05). Short‐term memory was significantly decreased in the DBS and MED groups compared with the control group (F = 4.56, P < .05; Figure 3B).There was no significant difference in memory quotient and each subtest before and after surgery in the DBS group (P > .05). There was no significant difference in memory quotient and each subtest before and after 1‐year follow‐up in the MED group (P > .05). And there was no significant change in the range of memory quotient of the two groups. Changes in memory quotient were considered as an observed value in the DBS and MED groups before and after 1‐year follow‐up. No significant difference in the change was detected between the two groups (F = 0.07, P = .800; Table 2).
Figure 3.
Comparison of memory and cognitive index in each group at baseline and line graph of response time in each group. Note: *P < .05, vs control group
Table 2.
Comparison of cognitive spectrum in each group
Item | Control group | DBS group (Preoperatively) | DBS group (Postoperatively) | MED group (1‐y follow‐up) | MED group (1‐y follow‐up) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Memory quotient (min‐max) |
101 ± 6.45 |
89.4 ± 17.41* (55‐126) |
89.2 ± 17.2* (60‐128) |
82.9 ± 14.0* (51‐110) |
81.6 ± 17.1* (52‐108) |
Change | −0.10 ± 9.21 | −1.24 ± 10.7 | |||
Tavg (second) | 1.09 ± 0.32 |
1.54 ± 0.82* (0.73‐4.42) |
1.62 ± 0.80 (0.73‐3.39) |
1.44 ± 0.54* (0.71‐4.02) |
1.50 ± 0.65 (0.72‐3.04) |
Change | 0.025 ± 0.53 | 0.05 ± 0.56 | |||
DPN | 8.50 ± 3.09 |
7.40 ± 3.56 (4‐22) |
7.60 ± 3.09 (6‐25) |
7.30 ± 3.00 (3‐19) |
7.05 ± 3.20 (2‐20) |
Change | 0.2 ± 2.00 | 0.2 ± 2.00 | |||
Error rate (%) | 4.84 ± 12.7 |
9.71 ± 14.3* (1.7‐66.1) |
13.4 ± 10.7* (1.2‐40.9) |
12.6 ± 16.9* (1.1‐69.1) |
9.50 ± 12.0 (0.8‐58.8) |
Change | 3.72 ± 12.6 | −3.05 ± 6.56 ▲ | |||
Verbal fluency | 16.8 ± 2.54 |
13.1 ± 4.31* (6‐23) |
(10‐24) |
14.4 ± 4.69* (6.5‐25) |
14.3 ± 6.21* (5‐24) |
Change | −1.35 ± 0.85 | −0.85 ± 3.58 ▲ | |||
Clock‐drawing test | 2.85 ± 0.36 |
2.25 ± 0.79* (0‐3) |
2.70 ± 0.57 # (0‐3) |
2.30 ± 0.92* (0‐3) |
2.05 ± 0.83* (0‐3) |
Change | 0.45 ± 0.89 | −0.25 ± 0.85 ▲ | |||
Tmin (second) | 0.24 ± 0.16 |
0.40 ± 0.24* (0.02‐0.78) |
0.28 ± 0.21 # (0.02‐0.75) |
0.34 ± 0.19* (0.04‐0.72) |
0.28 ± 0.20 (0.02‐0.70) |
Change | −0.12 ± 0.22 | −0.08 ± 021 | |||
Mini Mental State Examination | 27.6 ± 1.79 | 26.9 ± 2.07 | 26.8 ± 2.57 | 25.8 ± 4.19 | 26.1 ± 3.12 |
Change = postoperation—preoperation or after 1‐y follow‐up—before 1‐y follow‐up. In the clock‐drawing test, Mann‐Whitney U rank sum test was used to compare the difference among DBS, MED, and control groups. Paired rank sum test was utilized to compare the difference before and after surgery or 1‐y follow‐up.
P < .01, vs control group;
P < .05, vs preoperation;
P < .05, vs DBS group using MANOVA.
The SRTT was a task of procedural learning. Analysis of variance showed significant differences in Tavg among DBS group, MED group and control group (F = 4.90, P < .05). Student‐Newman‐Keuls pairwise comparison test results showed that Tavg was significantly higher in the DBS and MED groups than the control group. No significant difference in Tavg was detectable between the MED and DBS groups at baseline (Figure 3C). Analysis of variance results did not show significant differences in the number of degraded picture naming among the DBS and MED groups at baseline and control group (F = −1.19, P = .23; Figure 3D). There was no significant difference in Tavg among different blocks in the DBS and MED groups (preoperatively F = 0.28, P = .98; postoperatively F = 0.37, P = .95; 1 year before medication therapy F = 1.18, P = .32; 1 year after medication therapy F = 1.86, P = .06). In the control group, Tavg displayed reduced across the blocks of repeated sequence trials, until to block 5 the rule disappeared, subsequent Tavg increased gradually. After the rule restored again, Tavg reduced again. It conformed to the learning rule of implicit memory inhuman. Except patients after DBS, the remaining PD patients presented significantly prolonged Tavg from the block 8. This indicates that after repeated operation, PD patients entered the fatigue period. There is no such phenomenon in the test after DBS, and a curved shape is close to the control. Our results demonstrated that Tavg had an increasing trend after DBS, but the difference was not statistically significant (t=−0.81, P = .43). Tavg showed an increased trend in the MED group compared with that after 1‐year follow‐up, also no significantly difference (t=−0.31, P = .76). There were no significant differences in the number of degraded picture naming between the two groups (t=−0.450, P = .66; t = 0.324, P = .71). Tavg increments were (0.025 ± 0.53) seconds and (0.05 ± 0.56) seconds after 1‐year following up in the DBS and MED groups, respectively. Considering levodopa equivalent dose altered before and after 1‐year follow‐up, multivariate analysis of variance indicated that there was no significant difference in the increased trend of Tavg in both groups (F = 0.001, P = .98; Table 2).
3.5. Executive function
Error rates in serial reaction time task in the control group, before and after DBS in the DBS group, before and after 1‐year follow‐up in the MED group were (4.84 ± 12.7)%,(9.71 ± 14.3)%, (13.4 ± 10.7)%, (12.6 ± 16.9)%, and (9.50 ± 12.0)%, respectively, there was not any significant difference in error rate detected in DBS and MED groups, compared with it at baseline(t=−1.31, P = .21 in the DBS group; t = 1.97, P = .07 in the MED group) after a 1‐year follow‐up period. The increased trend of error rate was significantly higher in the DBS group after surgery compared with the MED group (F = 6.16, P = .03; Table 2).
3.6. Speech function
There was a significant difference in verbal fluency among the control group, DBS group (preoperative) and MED group (before 1‐year follow‐up) (P < .01). This could have been as a declined language function in PD patients (Figure 3F). Verbal fluency decreased after DBS (t = 2.48, P < .05). No significant difference in verbal fluency was determined between 1‐year follow‐up and baseline data in the MED group (t=−1.06, P = .302). The decreased trend was significantly higher in the DBS group than in the MED group (F = 6.27, P = .03; Table 2).
3.7. Visuospatial ability
Clock‐drawing test showed a significantly lower test score in the DBS and MED groups at baseline compared to the control group (DBS group: z = −2.76, P < .01; MED group: z = −2.55, P < .01; Figure 3G). The score was significantly increased after surgery in the DBS group (z = −2.07, P < .05). Moreover, no significant difference was detected between DBS group postoperatively and control group, suggesting that postoperative clock‐drawing test results were close to normal levels. Clock‐drawing test score was decreased after 1‐year follow‐up in the MED group, but the difference was not statistically significant (z = −1.31, P = .19). Variations were compared between the two groups, multivariate analysis of variance of levodopa equivalent dose variation demonstrated significant difference (F = 5.71, P = .04; Table 2).
3.8. Attention
Tmin was significantly increased in the DBS and MED groups at baseline compared with the control group (t = 2.41, P < .05; Figure 3H). Tmin had a significant decrease after BDS (t = 2.83, P < .05). Tmin also exhibited a decreased trend in the MED group after 1‐year follow‐up, but no significant difference (t = 1.18, P = .26). No significant difference in variations was found between the two groups (F = 2.83, P = .12) (Table 2).
3.9. Comparison of reliable change index from the individual level
Each test values were calculated in the DBS and MED groups based on reliable change index. The declined, remained stable, and improved percentages of each test are listed in Table 3. Significant differences on verbal fluency and error rate were seen between the DBS and MED groups (x2 = 7.03, P = .03; x2 = 6.72, P = .04).
Table 3.
Declined, remained stable, and improved percentages in each test in the DBS and MED groups
DBS group | MED group | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Declined (%) | Remained stable (%) | Improved (%) | Declined (%) | Remained stable (%) | Improved (%) | |
Clock‐drawing test | 0 | 85 | 15 | 10 | 90 | 0 |
Verbal fluency* | 20 | 80 | 0 | 0 | 85 | 15 |
Degraded picture naming | 0 | 95 | 5 | 5 | 85 | 10 |
Error rate* | 5 | 75 | 20 | 30 | 70 | 0 |
Tavg (second) | 10 | 85 | 5 | 0 | 100 | 0 |
Tmin (second) | 20 | 80 | 0 | 12 | 88 | 0 |
Memory quotient | 10 | 85 | 5 | 12 | 76 | 12 |
Abbreviations: DBS, deep brain stimulation; MED, medication therapy.
P < .05 in chi‐square test.
4. DISCUSSION
Several and nonexclusive mechanisms have been proposed to underlie the beneficial effects of DBS, such as electrical and neurochemical effects of stimulation, modulation of oscillatory activity, synaptic plasticity, and, potentially, neuroprotection and neurogenesis. 24 It is generally believed that cognitive function is closely associated with the prefrontal lobe‐basal ganglia‐thalamic circuit. 25 Any affected link on the loop will cause the change of cognitive function, which may be the basis of STN‐DBS affecting the cognitive function of PD patients. The patients with PD are facing the damage of cognition, mainly involving memory, executive function, attention, and verbal fluency. To investigate the cognitive changes after STN‐DBS is the natural cognitive outcome of PD or the impact of DBS and the potential pathogenesis, this study explored the change of cognitive spectrum of PD patients after operation by using a variety of neuropsychological assessment scales.
Our results demonstrated that explicit memory was really impaired in PD patients, mainly in short‐term memory impairment. However, DBS did not noticeably affect explicit memory compared with patients that received the simple medication therapy. And Rothlind et al 26 agreed with us on the change of explicit memory. With the in‐depth study of memory, a theory of implicit memory is presented which describes it as a form of general plasticity within processing networks that adaptively improve function via experience. 27 The line graph of normal controls in serial reaction time task test showed the priming effect of implicit memory on later learning. Compared with the health, implicit memory was impaired in PD patients, but STN‐DBS did not evidently deteriorate implicit memory. Different types of memory have different neural structures and loop. The medial temporal lobe and the hippocampus are involved in explicit memory, while basal ganglia is involved in implicit memory. 28 Explicit and implicit memory systems act the memory function through different cognitive mechanisms, but they influence each other. 29 The basal ganglia are likely to be the linked between the two memory systems. 30 However, the effects of basal ganglia on the memory system and its relationship with explicit and implicit memories deserve further investigations. It is emphasized that the healthy control group also was scored by the implicit and explicit cognition tests, so to observe and adjust the age‐dependent tendency of natural cognitive outcomes for a year.
Verbal fluency decline is an important cognitive impairment in PD patients. Regarding the changes in verbal fluency after DBS, the views of scholars are mainly divided into two factions: (a) The decrease in verbal fluency after DBS is associated with cognitive decline in PD. 9 , 31 (b) DBS induced a decrease in verbal fluency. 32 , 33 Our results confirmed that STN‐DBS definitely has a bad effect on verbal fluency. Its internal mechanism may include micro‐damage after surgery or current stimulation. It was demonstrated that PD patients do experience executive dysfunction and STN‐DBS worsened the decline of executive function, which is not the natural cognitive outcome in PD. In addition, we found that executive function obviously decreased in 20% patients after surgery. Smeding et al 30 and Williams et al 9 assessed executive function with other neuropsychological measures and got the same conclusions. Nevertheless, effect of STN‐DBS on executive function of PD patients still needs more data to support. Visuospatial dysfunction is also considered common cognitive impairment in PD patients (especially the middle stage and the advanced). Results exhibited that STN‐DBS improved visuospatial function to a certain degree and 15% patients had improved after DBS. It is believed that visuospatial function is related to the visual cortex currently 31 , 32 , 33 . Whether the improvement of STN‐DBS on visuospatial function is induced by current diffusion to stimulate visual cortical pathway or change of dopamine concentration after surgery, or other mechanisms, deserves further investigations. Another parameter of cognitive impairment in PD patients was attention. STN‐DBS may improve attention, but cannot completely eliminate the possibility of medication therapy.
In conclusion, STN‐DBS obviously improved motor symptoms, and simultaneously altered cognitive function, such as the decreased executive function and verbal fluency. The decline is more obvious than the natural cognitive decline in patients with PD. Visuospatial function was improved, and attention was possibly improved.
Explicit memory and overall cognitive function did not noticeably alter. Implicit memory was retained, and STN‐DBS did not affect implicit memory in a short period. Long‐term effects need to be made clear by longer follow‐up and more in‐depth study.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
ETHICAL APPROVAL
This study was performed in accordance with the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki (1964) and was approved by the medical ethics committee JiaoTong University School of Medicine. All subjects provided written informed consent prior to their inclusion in the study.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The authors express many thanks to Drs Lassonde M, Hu XW, and Wu B. WXP is funded by National Natural Science Foundation of China (81071065# and 81671103#), and Key special topics of National Ministry of Science and Technology (2016YFC0105900).
You Z, Wu Y, Wu R, et al. Efforts of subthalamic nucleus deep brain stimulation on cognitive spectrum: From explicit to implicit changes in the patients with Parkinson's disease for 1 year. CNS Neurosci Ther. 2020;26:972–980. 10.1111/cns.13392
ZhiFei You and Yi‐ Ying Wu contributed equally to this work
Funding information
This paper is supported by National Natural Foundation of China (81071065# and 81671103#).
Contributor Information
Xi Wu, Email: wuxi_smmu@sina.com.
Xiao‐Ping Wang, Email: x_p_wang@sjtu.edu.cn.
REFERENCES
- 1. Udupa K, Chen R. Role of dopamine in motor cortex plasticity in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. 2016;31:43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Meissner WG, Frasier M, Gasser T, et al. Priorities in Parkinson's disease research. Nat Rev Drug Discovery. 2011;10:377‐393. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Hickey P, Stacy M. Deep brain stimulation: a paradigm shifting approach to treat Parkinson's disease. Frontiers Neurosci. 2016;10:173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Boel JA, Odekerken VJ, Schmand BA, et al. Cognitive and psychiatric outcome 3 years after globus pallidus pars interna or subthalamic nucleus deep brain stimulation for Parkinson's disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2016;33:90‐95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Naskar S, Sood SK, Goyal V, Dhara M. Mechanism(s) of deep brain stimulation and insights into cognitive outcomes in Parkinson's disease. Brain Res Rev. 2010;65:1‐13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Santaniello S, McCarthy MM, Montgomery EB Jr, Gale JT, Kopell N, Sarma SV. Therapeutic mechanisms of high‐frequency stimulation in Parkinson's disease and neural restoration via loop‐based reinforcement. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2015;112(6):E586‐E595. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Weaver FM, Follett K, Stern M, et al. Bilateral deep brain stimulation vs best medical therapy for patients with advanced Parkinson disease: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA. 2009;301:63‐73. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Halpern CH, Rick JH, Danish SF, Grossman M, Baltuch GH. Cognition following bilateral deep brain stimulation surgery of the subthalamic nucleus for Parkinson's disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2009;24:443‐451. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Williams AE, Arzola GM, Strutt AM, Simpson R, Jankovic J, York MK. Cognitive outcome and reliable change indices two years following bilateral subthalamic nucleus deep brain stimulation. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2011;17:321‐327. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Aybek S, Gronchi‐Perrin A, Berney A, et al. Long‐term cognitive profile and incidence of dementia after STN‐DBS in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. 2007;22:974‐981. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Hughes AJ, Daniel SE, Kilford L, Lees AJ. Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinson's disease: a clinico‐pathological study of 100 cases. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1992;55:181‐184. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Stebbins GT, Goetz CG, Burn DJ, Jankovic J, Khoo TK, Tilley BC. How to identify tremor dominant and postural instability/gait difficulty groups with the movement disorder society unified Parkinson's disease rating scale: comparison with the unified Parkinson's disease rating scale. Mov Disord. 2013;28:668‐670. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Goetz CG, Poewe W, Rascol O, et al. Movement Disorder Society Task Force Report on the Hoehn and Yahr staging scale: status and recommendations. Mov Disord. 2004;19:1020‐1028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. "Mini‐mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12:189‐198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Soble JR, Eichstaedt KE, Waseem H, et al. Clinical utility of the Wechsler Memory Scale‐Fourth Edition (WMS‐IV) in predicting laterality of temporal lobe epilepsy among surgical candidates. Epilepsy Behav. 2014;41:232‐237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Wang XP, Sun BM, Ding HL. Changes of procedural learning in Chinese patients with non‐demented Parkinson disease. Neurosci Lett. 2009;449:161‐163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Horn A, Kuhn AA. Lead‐DBS: a toolbox for deep brain stimulation electrode localizations and visualizations. NeuroImage. 2015;107:127‐135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Yang C, Qiu Y, Wu X, Wang J, Wu Y, Hu X. Analysis of contact position for subthalamic nucleus deep brain stimulation‐induced hyperhidrosis. Parkinson's disease. 2019;2019:8180123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Hinton‐Bayre AD. Clarifying discrepancies in responsiveness between reliable change indices. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2016;31(7):754‐768. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Herrington TM, Cheng JJ, Eskandar EN. Mechanisms of deep brain stimulation. J Neurophysiol. 2016;115:19‐38. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Wu B, Han L, Sun BM, Hu XW, Wang XP. Influence of deep brain stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus on cognitive function in patients with Parkinson's disease. Neuroscience bulletin. 2014;30:153‐161. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Rothlind JC, York MK, Carlson K, et al. Neuropsychological changes following deep brain stimulation surgery for Parkinson's disease: comparisons of treatment at pallidal and subthalamic targets versus best medical therapy. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2015;86:622‐629. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Reber PJ. The neural basis of implicit learning and memory: a review of neuropsychological and neuroimaging research. Neuropsychologia. 2013;51:2026‐2042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Janssen S, Bolte B, Nonnekes J, et al. Usability of three‐dimensional augmented visual cues delivered by smart glasses on (Freezing of) Gait in Parkinson's disease. Front Neurol. 2017;8:279. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Dew IT, Cabeza R. The porous boundaries between explicit and implicit memory: behavioral and neural evidence. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2011;1224:174‐190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Wilkinson L, Khan Z, Jahanshahi M. The role of the basal ganglia and its cortical connections in sequence learning: evidence from implicit and explicit sequence learning in Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:2564‐2573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Kishore A, Rao R, Krishnan S, et al. Long‐term stability of effects of subthalamic stimulation in Parkinson's disease: Indian Experience. Mov Disord. 2010;25:2438‐2444. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Fasano A, Romito LM, Daniele A, et al. Motor and cognitive outcome in patients with Parkinson's disease 8 years after subthalamic implants. Brain. 2010;133:2664‐2676. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Witt K, Daniels C, Reiff J, et al. Neuropsychological and psychiatric changes after deep brain stimulation for Parkinson's disease: a randomised, multicentre study. Lancet Neurol. 2008;7:605‐614. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Smeding HM, Speelman JD, Koning‐Haanstra M, et al. Neuropsychological effects of bilateral STN stimulation in Parkinson disease: a controlled study. Neurology. 2006;66:1830‐1836. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Pereira JB, Junque C, Marti MJ, Ramirez‐Ruiz B, Bargallo N, Tolosa E. Neuroanatomical substrate of visuospatial and visuoperceptual impairment in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. 2009;24:1193‐1199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Shen B, Wang ZR, Wang XP. The fast spiking subpopulation of striatal neurons coding for temporal cognition of movements. Frontiers Cell Neurosci. 2017;11:406. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Hu HL, Ni XS, Duff‐Canning S, Wang XP. Oxidative damage of copper chloride overload to the cultured rat astrocytes. Am J Transl Res. 2016;8:1273‐1280. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]