Skip to main content
Elsevier - PMC COVID-19 Collection logoLink to Elsevier - PMC COVID-19 Collection
. 2020 Aug 9;118:411–425. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.07.032

Developmental trajectory of interpersonal motor alignment: Positive social effects and link to social cognition

Birgit Rauchbauer a,b,c,*, Marie-Hélène Grosbras a
PMCID: PMC7415214  PMID: 32783968

Highlights

  • Interpersonal motor alignment (IMA) has positive effects on healthy social life.

  • IMA - mimicry, synchrony, automatic imitation - is studied throughout development.

  • It relies on motor resonance brain mechanisms identified throughout development.

  • It is modulated by contextual and personal factors.

  • IMA is underinvestigated in adolescence, yet it may aid to enhance resilience.

Keywords: Interpersonal motor alignment, Mimicry, Synchrony, Automatic imitation, Action observation, Social cognition, Development, Adolescence

Abstract

Interpersonal motor alignment is a ubiquitous behavior in daily social life. It is a building block for higher social cognition, including empathy and mentalizing and promotes positive social effects. It can be observed as mimicry, synchrony and automatic imitation, to name a few. These phenomena rely on motor resonance processes, i.e., a direct link between the perception of an action and its execution. While a considerable literature debates its underlying mechanisms and measurement methods, the question of how motor alignment comes about and changes in ontogeny all the way until adulthood, is rarely discussed specifically. In this review we will focus on the link between interpersonal motor alignment, positive social effects and social cognition in infants, children, and adolescents, demonstrating that this link is present early on in development. Yet, in reviewing the existing literature pertaining to social psychology and developmental social cognitive neuroscience, we identify a knowledge gap regarding the healthy developmental changes in interpersonal motor alignment especially in adolescence.

1. Scope, definitions and why study interpersonal motor alignment

In our everyday social lives, we unconsciously and automatically align our behavior to the people around us. We define this interpersonal alignment as the reciprocal matching of behavior, posture, facial or vocal expression to the interaction partner. We propose to review the development of interpersonal motor alignment appearing as synchrony, mimicry or automatic imitation, the former two being ubiquitous in daily social life. We focus on interpersonal motor alignment that is automatic, spontaneous and mostly unconscious, as opposed to interpersonal coordination that is intentional and conscious, such as in cooperative tasks.

Synchrony refers to the temporally matched behaviors of interaction partners. Through temporal motor matching, it allows precise prediction of interactive behavior (Hove and Risen, 2009; Launay et al., 2016; Wiltermuth and Heath, 2009). Synchrony may include verbal and non-verbal communicative, as well as emotional behaviors (Leclère et al., 2014). In this review we focus on studies investigating the temporal and rhythmic matching of topographically isomorph and dynamic behaviors, such as during dance, but will also touch upon rhythmic vocal and musical synchrony, such as during singing and drumming. While synchrony refers to simultaneous motor behavior, mimicry refers to the matching of behavior occurring with a slight temporal delay, of the order of 3–5 seconds (Chartrand and Bargh, 1999). This phenomenon has also been coined the Chameleon-effect (Chartrand and Bargh, 1999), alluding to the way chameleons change their colour to fit their environment. Automatic imitation on the other hand, is, with the exception of the kids’ play “Simon says” that is prominent in different cultures and languages, mainly used as an experimental paradigm in cognitive psychology/neuroscience to assess underlying mechanisms of motor alignment (Brass et al., 2000). Automatic imitation is most often instantiated in a stimulus-response compatibility (SRC) paradigm (e.g., Brass et al., 2000; Kilner et al., 2003), in which observing an irrelevant movement interferes with target movement execution. If the irrelevant action matches the target movement, response execution is facilitated, while a mismatch requires inhibition of the perceived action representation to execute the target response and thus impairs execution. This paradigm has the advantage of high experimental controllability, implementation as a within-subject, repeated measures design and suitability for neuroscientific investigations, with, for example functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) (e.g., Brass et al., 2005; Brass et al., 2009), electroencephalography (EEG) (e.g., Deschrijver et al., 2017; Rauchbauer et al., 2018), transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) (e.g., Catmur et al., 2011; Catmur et al., 2007; Mengotti et al., 2013; Sowden and Catmur, 2015) or transcranial direct current stimulation (e.g., Santiesteban et al., 2012). In the developmental literature the majority of studies refer to the effect of motor interference between target movement and task-irrelevant simultaneous movement as “motor interference task”. In this article we use motor interference interchangeably with automatic imitation. Also, experimentally, automatic imitation is evidenced by recordings of brain activity in motor neuronal circuits during passive observation of others’ actions, without the task requirements of response execution, which we will review as action-observation studies, a special case of instantiation of interpersonal motor alignment.

These phenomena, synchrony, mimicry, automatic imitation and action observation, are thought to rely on motor resonance processes, i.e. a direct link between the execution and perception of an action (for review see Rizzolatti and Craighero, 2004). The perception-action link is modulated by the action repertoire and proficiency of the observer (Calvo-Merino et al., 2004) and is also presumably involved in action understanding and learning.

The investigation of the perception-action link is tightly related to discussions on the mirror neurons system. These neurons originally discovered in monkey’s ventral premotor cortex (F5) fire on observation and execution of the same action (for review see Rizzolatti and Craighero, 2004). A human mirror network has also been proposed (e.g., Brass and Heyes, 2005; Catmur et al., 2007; Keysers and Gazzola, 2010; Mukamel et al., 2010), which may provide a direct matching between our interacting partner’s actions and our own motor programs and thereby support motor resonance. While a mirror mechanism as a basis for action understanding has been criticized (Hickok, 2009), there is evidence for its participation in understanding actions in one’s own motor repertoire. This can occur even for recognition of actions performed by a different species, while behaviors outside one’s repertoire (e.g., barking) are instead mapped onto the visual and not the motor system (Buccino et al., 2004).

Yet motor resonance might not be the sole support for interpersonal motor alignment, which most often implies a tight temporal coupling between the interaction partner’s and one’s own movement. It may also involve brain mechanisms related to temporal processing (see e.g., Coull et al., 2011; Merchant et al., 2013), for detecting timing of events and rhythmic properties. There is a tight relationship between these brain circuits and neural networks implicated in motor processes, as evidenced also by the fact that action facilitates time estimation in adults and children (Monier et al., 2019). In particular, the striatum and the supplementary motor area have been shown to be involved in perceiving time as well as in producing timed motor actions, rhythmic sequences and coordination of motor actions (Coull et al., 2011; Merchant et al., 2013), and are thus likely to play a key role in interpersonal motor alignment. Yet, the interactions between motor resonance mechanisms and temporal processing may differ between synchrony, mimicry and automatic imitation given their specific reliance on precise timing.

From a developmental perspective, interpersonal motor alignment is tightly linked to the ongoing discussion of the origins of the human mirror mechanisms. Debated accounts range from completely innate mechanisms evolved through selection pressure (Meltzoff and Moore, 1997), to a sole product of associative sensorimotor learning (Keysers and Perrett, 2004; Oostenbroek et al., 2016), and experience (Catmur et al., 2007; Catmur et al., 2009; Heyes, 2011; Heyes et al., 2005; Ray and Heyes, 2011) with mixed-accounts in between (Farmer et al., 2018; Quadrelli and Turati, 2015). While it is not our goal to contribute directly to this discussion, we argue that a critical review of the literature on the development of interpersonal motor alignment and its link to the construction of social abilities across the lifespan is currently lacking in this debate. Yet, including developmental aspects not just at the beginning of life, but at all ages until adulthood, may advance apprehension of the underlying mechanisms of interpersonal motor alignment. Considering the positive effects of interpersonal (motor) alignment, a deeper insight into its underlying processes at different stages of development across the lifespan, may offer a more nuanced, targeted insight.

1.1. Interpersonal motor alignment in adult studies

Synchrony and mimicry both have been suggested to promote positive social behaviors. Joint singing, for instance, has positive, and fast, “ice-breaking” (Pearce et al., 2015) effects on social bonding (Pearce et al., 2015; Weinstein et al., 2016). The accelerated positive effects of joint singing may be product of multimodal motor synchronization of laryngeal muscles and respiration patterns and the production and perception of the same sounds (Good and Russo, 2016). Relatedly, synchronized activation during joint laughter (Dunbar et al., 2011), physical activity, such as dance (Tarr et al., 2016) and rowing (Cohen et al., 2010), leads to similar cohesive group effects. Group synchrony may reinforce a group’s cooperative tendency (Reddish et al., 2013). A group moving together may become a collective social unit (Good and Russo, 2016) through boundary loss and getting into a “we”-mode (McNeill, 1997). But apart from (inter)active motor alignment, already the mere observation of interpersonal coordination enhances the perception of commitment to joint action (Michael et al., 2016), rapport (Miles et al., 2009), a feeling of unity (Lakens, 2010; Lakens and Stel, 2011), and of a shared goal and cohesiveness (Ip et al., 2006).

In line with this, mimicry has been suggested to enhance liking, social cohesion and prosocial behavior towards the interaction partner (Chartrand and Lakin, 2013; Chartrand et al., 2005; Duffy and Chartrand, 2015; Van Baaren et al., 2009). Prosociality after being mimicked may be enhanced towards the mimicking confederate, but moreover extends to an unknown experimenter and to charities (Duffy and Chartrand, 2015; Van Baaren et al., 2004; Van Baaren et al., 2009), as well as strangers on a street (Fischer-Lokou et al., 2011). Mimicry has thus been suggested to act as a “social glue” that may have an evolutionary function for establishing and maintaining social relations (Lakin et al., 2003) or to regain inclusion into a group (Lakin et al., 2008).

While some reports suggest that interpersonal motor alignment is enhanced towards in-, as compared to out-group members (e.g. (Bourgeois and Hess, 2008; Yabar et al., 2006), studies show that the positive effects of interpersonal motor alignment extend to intergroup relations. Enhanced synchrony with an out-group member may reduce intergroup boundaries and support closeness (Miles et al., 2011), which may be mediated by an increased perception of interpersonal similarity through the display of interpersonal synchrony (Rabinowitch and Knafo-Noam, 2015; Valdesolo and DeSteno, 2011; Valdesolo et al., 2010). Likewise, mimicry increases sympathy, closeness and the perception of harmonious interaction with an virtual avatar posing as an out-group member (Hasler et al., 2014). Synchronous and mimicry behavior may enhance perceived self-other overlap between self-generated and other-produced movements. A constant matching between interactive movements may enforce an overlap in the shared representations of perception and execution of an action. This self-other overlap may lead to social cohesion (Lang et al., 2017; Tarr et al., 2014), extending also to out-group members and contribute to overcoming group boundaries. The positive intergroup effects of interpersonal motor alignment have also been demonstrated using the tightly controlled automatic imitation tasks (Gleibs et al., 2016; Marsh et al., 2016; Rauchbauer et al., 2015; Rauchbauer et al., 2016).

It has been suggested that automatic imitation measures covert (Cracco et al., 2018; Cracco and Brass, 2019; Heyes, 2011), and to some extent overt, imitation (Cracco and Brass, 2019), but this has recently been a matter of debate (cf. (Cracco and Brass, 2019; Ramsey, 2018). While mimicry and automatic imitation may not be correlated (Genschow et al., 2017), being mimicked may reduce inhibitory mechanisms of automatic imitation (Rauchbauer et al., 2020), potentially indicating an interrelationship between the two phenomena. In any case, automatic imitation tasks allow investigators to test a variety of modulatory factors in controlled settings using both behavioral and neuroscientific methods. Thereby they have provided evidence that interpersonal motor alignment is modulated by social context, like group membership (Gleibs et al., 2016; Marsh et al., 2016; Rauchbauer et al., 2015; Rauchbauer et al., 2016), social contagion and group size (Cracco and Brass, 2018a, 2018b), eye-contact (Marsh et al., 2016; Wang et al., 2011), and emotional facial stimuli (Butler et al., 2016) (see also for meta analyses: Cracco et al., 2018). They may also have the potential to contribute to identifying the factors that link interpersonal motor alignment and positive social behaviors.

Divergent accounts suggest that increased social closeness and helping behavior may either rely on topographically isomorphic body movements (Majdandžić et al., 2016), whereas other reports suggest a role of perceived contingency (i.e., predictive relationship) of one’s own and the interaction partner’s movements, rather than on their similarity (i.e., topographic isomorphism) (Catmur and Heyes, 2013). Yet again, other accounts suggest that social affiliation may not only be linked to high contingency, but also high contiguity (i.e., temporal proximity) of movements (Dignath et al., 2018) and effector matching (Sparenberg et al., 2012).

Moreover, both synchronous movements and exertion have been shown to induce beneficial social effects (Tarr et al., 2015). Similarly, the choice of control condition in a mimicry or synchrony induction could potentially influence social-cognitive variables. While prominent studies on mimicry have used control conditions without any movement (e.g., Chartrand and Bargh, 1999; Van Baaren et al., 2004), other studies have used Anti-Mimicry conditions (Dalton et al., 2010; Finkel et al., 2006; Kühn et al., 2010; Rauchbauer et al., 2020). In an Anti-Mimicry control condition, the confederate’s movements will be topographically misaligned to the participant’s, as compared to the Mimicry condition in which they are aligned. A control condition without movement may itself evoke negative social effects due to perceiving the interaction partner uninterested and passive. Conversely, an Anti-Mimicry condition may also induce positive effects itself in case of perceived temporal contingency (Catmur and Heyes, 2013) or high exertion (Tarr et al., 2015) of movements. Thus, the mechanisms by which mimicry and synchrony may induce positive social-cognitive effects may not be entirely clear yet and lead to divergent results. As such, it has recently been suggested that being mimicked by topographically isomorph postures as compared to an Anti-Mimicry condition, does not influence experienced social cohesion with the confederate, measured with a rating scale (Rauchbauer et al., 2020). Thus, there are certain contradictions in the literature of interpersonal motor alignment, which may require in-depth analysis. To date it remains unclear as to which conditions and mechanisms may give rise to which positive social-cognitive effects via synchrony or mimicry. For example, while mimicry seems to evoke a kind of generalized prosociality, synchronous behavior may rather evoke directed prosociality (Cirelli, 2018). However, some studies have demonstrated that also synchrony induces generalized prosocial behavior (Reddish et al., 2014; Reddish et al., 2016).

The aforementioned studies rely only on adult populations. Yet, interpersonal motor alignment is pertinent throughout ontogeny and may play an important part of a healthy social life, as a building block for higher social cognition, including empathy. Indeed inter-individual differences in experience of and ability for interpersonal motor alignment early in infancy is associated with empathic abilities in adolescence as well as with the ability to engage in intimate relationships across the lifespan (Feldman, 2007b, 2007c), suggesting a protective buffering role against psychosocial maladjustment. The “motor theory of empathy” suggests that human mirror mechanisms may mediate the understanding of others’ intentions and feelings (Rizzolatti and Fabbri-Destro, 2008) and that empathy may originate from the perception-action link (Iacoboni, 2009; Leslie et al., 2004). As such, action understanding may play a role in feeling emotions and empathizing with others (Carr et al., 2003). Yet, this theory has also been criticized, arguing that contrary to previously mentioned findings, empathy does not necessarily involve the activation of the mirror neuron system (MNS) (De Vignemont and Singer, 2006), or that its involvement may depend on the specific form of empathy (Baird et al., 2011).

Regardless of this debate, studies show that right after birth, social contingencies in the form of multimodal responses by the mother to the infant’s signals, and contingency detection on the side of the infant, aid to form reliable reciprocal interactions. Synchronous interactions, starting with caregiver-infant interactions, may provide a buffer against psychosocial maladjustment, shape empathic capacities in adolescents and lay the basis for engagement in intimate relationships across the lifespan (Feldman, 2007b, 2007c). Therefore, it is important to integrate the ontogenetic dimension while building accounts of these functions during typical and atypical developments.

In the next sections we review existing behavioral and neuroimaging research on synchrony, mimicry and automatic imitation in infants, children and adolescents. For this we conducted a systematic search on Pubmed (https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/; as of 15 May 2020), presented in Table 1, Table 2, Table 3 , and analyzed the main findings. Keywords included (Infant OR Development) AND (Mimicry OR Synchrony OR Automatic Imitation OR Action Observation) AND (experiment OR EEG OR fMRI OR fNIRS OR EEG). Then results were filtered to select only the studies that focused on interpersonal motor alignment as we have circumscribed above, in particular excluding experiments on instructed or delayed imitation. We also excluded studies that concerned only atypical populations or provided only qualitative data. This list, while probably not exhaustive due to limitations of the search algorithm, provides a panorama of the state of the research in this domain so far. We analyzed the main findings with regards to the nature of interpersonal motor alignment and its relationships with other social behaviors, on the one hand, and its neural mechanisms, on the other hand, at different ages.

Table 1.

Experimental studies on interpersonal motor alignment on infants. Systematic search of studies on interpersonal motor alignment in infants (age <30 months) The table shows references with authors and year of publication, the type of interpersonal motor alignment (IMA), main findings and method used; * denotes studies mentioned in the text, EEG = electroencephalography, (f)NIRS = (functional) near-infrared spectroscopy, EMG = electromyography, n = number of participants (in the final sample of analyzed data).

Authors IMA Main finding Method Age in months, sample size
*Cannon et al., 2015 Action observation Mu suppression (posterior sites) correlated with motor skills. EEG 8−10 months, n = 21
*Carpenter et al., 2013 Mimicry Being mimicked increases help to experimenter and stranger Behavior 18 months, n = 48
* Cirelli et al., 2014 Synchrony More helping towards an adult moving in synchrony than asynchronously or a stranger Behavior 14 months, n = 48
*Cirelli et al. (2016) Synchrony Prosocial effect of synchrony transfers to affiliate Behavior 14 months, n = 48
*Debnath et al., 2019 Action observation Mu suppression at central location and connectivity central-occipital EEG 9 months, n = 46
de Klerk et al., 2015 Action observation Mu suppression independent of experience (walking) EEG 7−9 months, n = 31
*de Klerk et al., 2019a Mimicry (face and hand) More facial mimicry in infants whose mother imitate more; no correlation for hand mimicry Behavior, EMG 4 months, n = 27
de Klerk et al., 2019b Mimicry (face) More mimicry and temporal cortex activity for videos of same-language (compared to foreign) speaking actor fNIRS, EMG 11 months, n = 55
de Klerk et al., 2018 Mimicry Facial mimicry increases with direct gaze; associated with superior temporal activity. fNIRS, EMG 4 months, n = 60
Fawcett and Liszkowski, 2012 Mimicry Being mimicked or mimicking spontaneously increases play initiation with adults Behavior 18 months, n = 32
Filippi et al., 2016 Action observation Mu suppression predicts subsequent goal imitation EEG 6−8 months, n = 36
*Grossmann et al., 2013 Action observation Premotor and temporal cortex activation when watching human and robot-like movements fNIRS 4 months, n = 15
Isomura and Nakano, 2016 Mimicry Mimicry only for audio-visual (but not unimodal) stimuli (emotional face) Behavior, EMG 4−5 months, n = 15
*Jones, 2007 Mimicry – Automatic imitation Encouraged imitation of parents actions appears slowly with age, dependent on behavior Behavior 6−20 months, n = 162
Kaiser et al., 2017 Mimicry Face mimicry dependent on emotion in 7 m. old; likely to include evaluative processes Behavior, EMG 4 months, n = 27; 7 months, n = 24)
*Langeloh et al., 2018 Action observation Mu suppression stronger for unusual than usual actions EEG 12 months, n = 42
*Lloyd-Fox et al., 2015 Action observation Temporal cortex activation correlates with fine motor skills (grasp and lift) fNIRS 4−6 months, n = 24
*Marshall and Meltzoff, 2011 Action observation Mu suppression(fronto central) when watching live actions EEG 14 months, n = 38
*Montirosso et al., 2019 Action observation Mu suppression (frontal parietal) when watching live actions EEG 14 months, n = 33
*Oostenbroek et al., 2016 Imitation No spontaneous imitation face and hand movements Behavior Newborn – 2months, n = 106
Pratt et al., 2015 Synchrony Mother child synchrony in face to face interaction associated with stress reduction depending on temperament. Behavior 4−6 months, n = 132
*Rayson et al., 2016 Action observation Mu suppression (central; bilateral) when watching videos of facial movements EEG 30 months, n = 17
*Reid et al., 2011 Action observation Mu suppression only when observing actions that engage the child. EEG 14 months, n = 10
Ruysschaert et al., 2013 Action observation Mu suppression for live but not video stimuli. EEG 18−36 months, n = 34
*Saby et al., 2012 Action observation Mu suppression greater for actions that match action just executed by the infant EEG 14 months, n = 16
Saby et al., 2013 Action observation Mu suppression during observation with somatotopic pattern (hand, foot) EEG 14 months, n = 32
*Shimada and Hiraki, 2006 Action observation Motor activity during execution and observation, more in live setting than video fNIRS 6−7 months, n = 13
*Somogyi and Esseily, 2014 Mimicry Being mimicked increases tool-use learning by observation. Behavior 16 months, n = 48
*Southgate et al., 2009 Action observation Mu suppression starting before onset of observed movement. EEG 9 months, n = 15
*Stapel et al., 2010 Action observation Mu suppression stronger for extraordinary actions EEG 12 months, n = 12
*Tunçgenç et al., 2015 Synchrony 12 months-old but not 9 months old prefer social character (but not object(that have moved synchronously with them Behavior 9 months, n = 41; 12 months, n = 40
Upshaw et al., 2016 Action observation Mu suppression during observation related to grip strength EEG 12 months, n = 12
Vacaru et al., 2019 Mimicry (face) Spontaneous mimicry of face pictures. Effect of attachment style, not inhibitory control Behavior, EMG 3 months, n = 42
*Virji‐Babul et al., 2012 Action observation Mu suppression when watching videos of reaching, walking or object motion EEG 4−11 months, n = 14
*Warreyn et al., 2013 Action observation Mu suppression for object-directed and mimicked actions EEG 18−30, n = 17
*Yoo et al., 2015 Action observation Mu suppression during observation of grasp-with-tool actions. EEG 9 months, n = 26; 12 months, n = 34
*Zmyj et al., 2012 Imitation Spontaneous imitation more likely when watching peers than older children or adults Behavior 14 months, n = 36

Table 2.

Experimental studies on interpersonal motor alignment on children and pre-adolescents. Result from a systematic search of studies indexed on Pubmed on interpersonal motor alignment in children (age 2.5- 12 years). The table shows references with authors and year of publication, the type of interpersonal motor alignment (IMA), main findings and method used; * denotes studies mentioned in the text, AON = action observation network, EEG = electroencephalography, ERP = event-related brain potential, (f)NIRS = (functional) near-infrared spectroscopy, EMG = electromyography, n = number of participants (in the final sample of analyzed data).

Authors IMA Main finding Method Age in years, sample size
Berchio et al., 2014 Action observation Ventral and dorsal fronto-parietal circuits recruited at different time EEG (ERP, high density) 10, n = 12
*Bernier et al., 2013 Action Observation Mu suppression in most children (hand actions); correlated to face imitation abilities EEG 5−7, n = 19
*Biagi et al., 2016 Action observation Same network as in adults but more variability fMRI 7−15, n = 12
*Brunsdon et al., 2020 Action observation Increases mu and beta suppression with age throughout adolescence EEG 10−86, n = 301
Bryant and Cuevas, 2019 Action observation Mu, but not beta, suppression for observation of tool use; independent of experience EEG 3−6, n = 21
Bureau et al., 2014 Synchrony Greater dyadic synchrony with mother during free play. Linked to attachment quality Behavior 3.5−4 n = 107
*Cochin et al., 2001 Action observation Mu suppression left central theta band. No age effect EEG 2−8, n = 30
Deschamps et al., 2015 Action observation Mimicry of all expressions (face) EMG 6−7, n = 27
*Eismont et al., 2017 Action observation No age effect for mu suppression when watching or doing hand drawing EEG 4−14, n = 53
Endedijk et al., 2017 Action observation Mu and beta power increase watching action videos; correlated with peer cooperation EEG 4.5, n = 29
*Fecteau et al., 2004 Action observation Mu desynchronization over sensorimotor cortex during observation (live) and execution Subdural electrodes 3, n = 1
*Good and Russo, 2016 Synchrony Group singing enhances prosociality Behavior 7, n = 50
*Grosbras et al., 2007 Action observation Functional connectivity within AON related to resistance to peer influence fMRI 10, n = 46
Helt et al., 2010 Mimicry Contagious yawning frequency increases until 4 Behavior 1−15, n = 186
*Kajiume et al., 2013 Action observation Ventral premotor activity during observation and imitation of hand actions fNIRS 9−13, n = 6
*Kirschner and Tomasello, 2009 Synchrony Higher synchrony with adult drummer than with drumming machine Behavior 2.5−4.5, n = 36
*Kirschner and Tomasello, 2010 Synchrony Joint singing or dancing enhances helping behavior Behavior 4−5, n = 96
Kleinspehn-Ammerlahn et al., 2011 Synchrony Better (instructed) synchrony with age and with an older partner. Behavior 5, n = 18; 12, n = 18
*Lepage and Théoret, 2006 Action observation Mu suppression for observation (live) and execution object-directed actions. No age effect EEG 4.5−11, n = 18
Liao et al., 2015 Action observation Mu (6−9 hz) and beta (15−18hz) suppression when watching mother’s action EEG 3.5, n = 11
Lust et al., 2019 Action observation Mu suppression stronger during observation to imitate vs in order to detect oddball EEG 9−13, n = 15
*Marshall et al., 2010 Automatic Imitation Interference (motor contagion) stronger when observing a peer than an adult. Behavior 4, n = 25
Martineau et al., 2008 Action observation Mu suppression in central, frontal and temporal locations EEG 5−7.5, n = 14
*Martineau and Cochin, 2003 Action observation Decrease theta power in theta frequency band when watching videos human movement EEG 2.5−7.5, n = 34
Meyer et al., 2011 Action observation Mu and beta suppression during observation while engaged in a joint action game EEG 3, n = 7
*Morales et al., 2019 Action observation Similar network as in adults. Less activation anterior parietal fMRI 7−10, n = 21
Nishimura et al., 2018 Action observation Mu suppression during observation weaker than in adults and less in allocentric compared to egocentric configuration EEG 8−12, n = 28
Nobusako et al., 2018 Automatic imitation Effect of congruency on trajectory when drawing on tablet in front of experimenter; No age effect Behavior 1.5−7, n = 42
Oberman et al., 2008 Action observation Mu (8−13 Hz) for familiar and stranger hand actions EEG 8−12, n = 13
*Oberman et al., 2013 Action observation Mu suppression during observation (not execution) gets stronger with age EEG 6−17, n = 51
*Ohnishi et al., 2004 Action observation Same network as observed in adult studies fMRI 7−10, n = 11
O’Sullivan et al., 2018 Automatic imitation Correct movement and reaction time effects (live). No age effect Behavior 3−7, n = 72
*Rabinowitch and Knafo-Noam, 2015 Synchrony Child-child synchronous rhythmic interaction increases feeling of closeness Behavior 8−9, n = 148
*Rabinowitch and Meltzoff, 2017a Synchrony Synchronous and asynchronous sway increase sharing Behavior 4, n = 162
*Rabinowitch and Meltzoff, 2017b Synchrony Synchronization during swing sway increases cooperation and give behavior Behavior 4, n = 162
*Reindl et al., 2018 Brain synchrony Parent-child prefrontal synchrony during cooperative game; predictive of performance fNIRS hyperscanning 5−9, n = 33
Ruysschaert et al., 2014 Action observation Mu (9 Hz) suppression during observation of goal directed and pantomime movement EEG 2−5.5, n = 19
*Saby et al., 2011 Automatic Imitation Observed movement interference on line tracing depends on expectancy on biological motion Behavior 4−5, n = 61
*Shaw et al., 2011b Action observation Same network as in adults studies. Small gender-dependent age effects in intensity and extent fMRI 10−13 (longitudinal), n = 65
*Shaw et al., 2011a Action observation Gender dependent effect of emotion depicted in the observed action on AON activity fMRI 10−13 (longitudinal), n = 65
Simpson and Carroll, 2014 Automatic imitation Great difficulty (60 % correct) in performing action different from experimenter Behavior 3−4, n = 24
Su et al., 2020 Synchrony – Action Observation Less synchrony with experimenter in children. No age difference for brain activity: STS for observation. Right STS, IPL and IFG for synchronized behavior. fNIRS, Behavior 10, n = 17; 22, n = 15
*Tunçgenç and Cohen, 2018 Synchrony More helping behavior after synchronous play Behavior 4−6 n = 4 × 19
*van Schaik et al., 2016v Automatic Imitation More interference for out-group adult Behavior 4−6, n = 65
van Schaik and Hunnius, 2016v Mimicry More mimicry (head and hand movement) of videos of in-group adults Behavior 3, n = 25; 4−6, n = 40
Van Schaik and Hunnius, 2018 Mimicry No mimicry when watching storytelling videos of experimenters with positive or negative interaction Behavior 5, n = 20
Vink et al., 2017 Synchrony More postural sway synchrony in pairs of children with higher popularity Behavior 9−13, n = 392
Xavier et al., 2018 Synchrony Improvement with age in instructed synchronization of movement and posture with virtual character Behavior 6−19, n = 38
*Yu and Kushnir, 2014 Mimicry & Imitation Only 2 years old are influenced by mimicry for subsequent imitation tasks Behavior Exp 1: 2 & 4, n = 36 Exp 2: 2, n = 12

Table 3.

Experimental studies on interpersonal motor alignment on adolescents (13-17 years). Result from a systematic search of studies indexed on Pubmed on interpersonal motor alignment in adolescents. The table shows references with authors and year of publication, the type of interpersonal motor alignment (IMA), main findings and method used; * denotes studies mentioned in the text, EEG = electroencephalography, EMG = electromyography, fMRI = functional magnetic resonance imaging, IFG = inferior frontal gyrus, MTG = middle temporal gyrus, SMG = supramarginal gyrus, n = number of participants (in the final sample of analyzed data).

Authors IMA Main finding Method Age in years, sample size
Ardizzi et al., 2014 Mimicry (face) More spontaneous mimicry of videos of facial expressions of peers Behavior, EMG 15−19, n = 20
*Bevilacqua et al., 2019 Brain synchrony No correlation between student-student and student-teacher synchrony and memory retention EEG hyperscanning 16−18, n = 12
*Brunsdon et al., 2020 Action observation Increases mu and beta suppression with age throughout adolescence EEG 10−86, n = 301
*Cook and Bird, 2011 Automatic imitation Congruency effect similar to adults; modulated by social priming in adults only. Behavior 13, n = 34 Adults n = 56
*Dikker et al., 2017 Brain synchrony Intersubject brain-to-brain synchrony in classroom modulated by closeness and shared attention. EEG hyperscanning 17−18, n = 12
Fitzpatrick et al., 2016 Synchrony Spontaneous phase entrainment with parent during pendulum swing Behavior 12−16, n = 9
Fourie et al., 2020 Action observation AON activity stronger with age and higher for communicative gestures in IFG and MTG fMRI 9.5–17, n = 16
Gordon et al., 2020 Automatic imitation Strong effect, modulated by block congruency rate Behavior 14−24 n = 50
*Oberman et al., 2013 Action observation Mu suppression during observation but not executions increases with age EEG 6−17, n = 51
Pokorny et al., 2015 Action observation Same network as in adults studies. Effect of object presence in IFG and SMF fMRI 9−17, n = 18
Xavier et al., 2018 Synchrony Improvement with age in instructed synchronization of movement and posture with virtual character Behavior 6−19, n = 38
* Wadsworth et al., 2017 Imitation Online Instructed imitation of hand postures; AON activity except in STS fMRI 8−17, n = 15

2. Investigation of interpersonal motor alignment, its positive social effects and link to social cognition during infancy, childhood and adolescence

2.1. Synchrony

Humans may be biologically prepared for coordinated interaction due to their responsiveness to rhythmical information and build-in tendency to detect contingencies (Feldman, 2007b). A recent review suggests that the establishment of interpersonal synchrony in reciprocal interactions is supported by adults’ rhythmical information to their infants (Markova et al., 2019). These rhythms are often spontaneously displayed by caregivers in the form of affective touch or singing to aid the infant’s affect regulation (Provasi et al., 2014). The caregiver’s adaptive signals to the newborn’s behavior form social contingencies, which the infant readily detects. These interpersonal mechanisms appearing in the first months of life may be multimodal precursors for synchronous interactions, including, apart from movements, vocalizations, gaze, touch, affect, position or proximity to each other. As the infant grows, the time lag between behavior and response diminishes and social contingencies develop into synchronous behaviors (Feldman, 2007b, 2007c). A longitudinal study points towards crucial positive long-term effects of socially contingent and synchronous interactions. Affective synchrony in mother-infant interaction from three and nine months of age predicted self-regulatory capabilities in two, four- and six-year olds. The same study also found that synchronous mother-infant interactions at three months of age predicted empathic abilities in thirteen-year old adolescents (Feldman, 2007a).

Apart from interactions with their primary caregiver, 12- and 14-month-old infants already use interpersonal movement information to guide social expectations. As such they attend to and interpret interpersonal synchronous and asynchronous behavior in a socially meaningful way (Cirelli, 2018; Fawcett and Tunçgenç, 2017). They seem to prefer synchronously moving partners and show spontaneous helping behavior towards them and their affiliates (Cirelli et al., 2014; Cirelli et al., 2016; Tunçgenç et al., 2015). Even more so, when observing asynchronous dyadic interactions, infants believe the interaction partners to be nonaffiliates (Cirelli et al., 2018). This suggests that they can infer third-party affiliation on the basis of synchrony (Cirelli, 2018; Fawcett and Tunçgenç, 2017).

Synchrony, as a social signal per se, may be a cue for self-similarity from the beginning of life on (Leclère et al., 2014; Xavier et al., 2016), increasing prosocial behavior and encouraging empathy and affiliation (for review see Cirelli, 2018). In four-year-old children, synchronous interactions of only three minutes may already lead to enhanced peer cooperation. In children of eight to nine years of age they enhance closeness and feelings of similarity (Rabinowitch and Knafo-Noam, 2015; Rabinowitch and Meltzoff, 2017a). These positive effects extend from synchrony in direct interaction to joint music making and rhythmic synchrony. Children synchronize drumming with higher accuracy in a social condition, which elicits cooperative behavior and fairness (Kirschner and Tomasello, 2009, 2010; Rabinowitch and Meltzoff, 2017a). Also cooperative singing with peers, as reported for adults (Pearce et al., 2015), increases group cooperation in children of seven to eight years more than art or competitive games (Good and Russo, 2016). Furthermore, the positive effects of synchronous singing have been reported to contribute to feelings of social inclusion in refugee children (Marsh, 2017; Marsh and Dieckmann, 2017). This suggests, that singing in synchrony, just as moving in synchrony (Tunçgenç and Cohen, 2016) or joint music making (Marsh, 2012, 2017), may support the establishment of a collective group membership by forging intergroup bonds.

In summary, synchronous behaviors can be observed, in different forms, throughout infancy and childhood (for a list of experimental studies on interpersonal motor alignment in developmental population see Table 1, Table 2, Table 3). First mainly restricted to caregivers, they extend to peer relations and can also be recognized in other dyads during observation. In all cases the ability to engage in synchronous behavior has positive prosocial outcomes especially with regards to interpersonal affiliation, similarly to what we have discussed in adults.

2.2. Mimicry, conscious and automatic imitation

Interpersonal motor alignment in neonates has been evidenced by seminal studies showing imitative hand opening and mouth protrusion movements (Meltzoff and Moore, 1997). This finding has led to an ongoing debate on whether spontaneous imitation is innate or not (e.g., Anisfeld, 1996; Jones, 2017; Kennedy-Costantini et al., 2017; Keven and Akins, 2017; Libertus et al., 2017; Meltzoff, 2017; Meltzoff et al., 2018; Oostenbroek et al., 2016). Alternatively, it has been suggested that imitation is learned through an associative learning sequence (Catmur et al., 2009), Hebbian learning (Keysers and Perrett, 2004) and correlated sensorimotor experiences, forming perception-action couplings through interaction with the primary caregiver (de Klerk et al., 2019a). As mentioned above, it is not our aim to contribute to this discussion in the present article, but rather to summarize facts about interpersonal motor alignment and their social correlates at different ages.

A large corpus of work shows that, during the first two years of life, infants faithfully imitate adults actions or just their goal (social or instrumental) in a variety of contexts (Yu and Kushnir, 2014). Children of pre-school age imitate not only causally relevant, but also irrelevant actions (i.e. sequences of movements that are not necessary to achieve the desired goal). This “over-imitation” (e.g., Nielsen and Blank, 2011; Over and Carpenter, 2012, 2013), may be an important learning mechanism to acquire cultural expertise. It seems to also have social reasons, such as the identification with a model and the social group in general. It may occur through social pressure (Over and Carpenter, 2012), but can also be used to overcome in-group ostracism (Over and Carpenter, 2009; Watson-Jones et al., 2016). The dual role of imitation, for social learning and for establishing group cohesion is supported by studies showing that 14-month-olds are more likely to imitate communicative gestures and familiar actions performed by same-age infants, than when they are performed by older children and adults (Zmyj et al., 2012). Nevertheless, in a context when they are presented with a novel object, they are more likely to reproduce the action that they have seen an adults perform on this object (Zmyj et al., 2011). This suggests that infants and toddlers may more likely align their behavior to that of an adult role-model in a learning context. In an affiliative context though it seems that children would preferentially imitate their peers, potentially to enhance group cohesion. Similarly, having been mimicked by an adult, 14 –18-month-olds learned better by observation (Somogyi and Esseily, 2014), and were more prone to help this adult (Over and Carpenter, 2013). This prosocial behavior can also extend to a stranger (Carpenter et al., 2013). This suggests that being mimicked may evoke a generalized prosociality (Cirelli, 2018).

The positive effects of interpersonal motor alignment throughout development are not only shown during imitation, mimicry and synchrony, but also in the more experimental environment of automatic imitation. In motor interference tasks, children are asked to perform straight lines in a vertical or horizontal movement on a tablet computer screen using a stylus, while an interaction partner performs a congruent or incongruent drawing movement (Marshall et al., 2010; Saby et al., 2011; van Schaik et al., 2016v). Using this task, automatic imitation was increased in four- to six-year-olds when interacting with an out-group member (van Schaik et al., 2016). This suggests, in line with adult studies (Rauchbauer et al., 2016), that even during automatic imitation the regulation of interpersonal motor alignment may be used to overcome intergroup differences. Also, 4-year-old children show a greater motor interference effect in interaction with peers, rather than adults (Marshall et al., 2010), in line with the studies mentioned above on imitation (Zmyj et al., 2012, 2011). Thus, already during childhood, group membership seems to influence the perception-action link. This is also demonstrated in a study with children of four and five years of age in which automatic imitation is modulated according to animacy beliefs of an interaction puppet (Saby et al., 2011). Yet, a recent meta-analyses suggested that automatic imitation may not be sensitive to animacy beliefs (Cracco et al., 2018).

Group membership may influence interpersonal motor alignment via top-down modulation. For instance, it has been suggested that during conscious imitation neural activity in a wide range of brain areas, and not only early visual areas, is modulated by the race of the model (Losin et al., 2012). This may be driven by socially learned associations concerning race, rather than self-similarity (Losin et al., 2014). This seems in line with motivational theories of automatic imitation, such as the social top-down response modulation account (STORM; Wang and Hamilton, 2012). These accounts suggest that the motivation to affiliate (the wish to be liked by the interaction partner) may increase imitation. This has also been suggested to influence mimicry, via a strengthened the perception-action link (Chartrand et al., 2005; Lakin and Chartrand, 2003; Lakin et al., 2008). Group membership may be one salient social factor activating affiliative motivation. High motivation to affiliate with the in-group has been shown to moderate the influence of group membership during imitation (Genschow and Schindler, 2016). Other studies have found higher imitation for out-group members, which could suggest affiliative motivation for appeasement (Rauchbauer et al., 2015, Rauchbauer et al., 2016). Contrary to these studies conducted in adult samples, social modulation of automatic imitation may be absent in adolescents. While automatic imitation has been observed to the same level as in adults in adolescents, no modulation by pro-social priming has been observed (Cook and Bird, 2011). This would indicate that the adaptability and social function of interpersonal motor alignment is still fine tuning in adolescence.

Apart from Cook and Bird (2011) and as pointed in the previous sections, to this date, investigation of interpersonal motor alignment in adolescence is scarce (see Table 3). This may be due to the assumption of social cognitive maturity by mid-childhood (Blakemore and Mills, 2014), leading to a wealth of studies of interpersonal motor alignment during child- and adulthood, but skipping the period of adolescence.

Yet, adolescence is an important social transition period, with ongoing brain development. This absence of studies warrants conclusions of social effects of adolescent interpersonal motor alignment and encourages further investigation of its potential positive effects. This is especially the case since adolescence is a period of enhanced importance of the peer group, sensitivity for social acceptance and rejection, as well as continuing structural and functional brain development (for review see Blakemore and Mills, 2014; Grosbras et al., 2007; Mills et al., 2016). A more in-depth investigation of interpersonal motor alignment during adolescent interaction could inform on healthy adolescent social cognitive development. This could have implications with respect to psychopathology with a prevalent onset in adolescence like conduct disorder, social anxiety or schizophrenia. As these disorders have been associated with altered brain development, it is also paramount to link those observations to knowledge about subserving brain organisation and its development. In the next section we review studies using neuroscientific methods to investigate brain correlates of interpersonal motor alignment across development.

3. Brain correlates of interpersonal motor alignment and the action-observation network

3.1. Brain correlates of interpersonal motor alignment in adult populations

The neural correlates of interpersonal motor alignment have been investigated in adults in three different categories of studies. First, the regulation of automatic imitative tendencies, second, motor resonance phenomena investigated via action observation studies, and third, synchronized brain activity. Here we give only a brief overview of some adult studies that have used various neuroscientific methods, as our aim is to focus on the developmental aspect. This section illustrates that, although neural correlates of interpersonal motor alignment have been investigated broadly, both with respect to experimental paradigms and research methods, as a whole the corpus evidence converges to show that specific brain circuits and mechanisms are involved in the aligment of behaviors during interaction.

Automatic imitation tasks have been used to assess brain regions specifically involved in the control of imitative tendency. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies indicate the engagement of prefrontal (inferior frontal and medial prefrontal) and parieto-temporal regions when subjects imitate a movement (Carr et al., 2003) or perform a motor interference task (e.g., Brass et al., 2005; Mengotti et al., 2013; Rauchbauer et al., 2015). Patients with frontal lobe lesions tend to over-imitate, which suggests the existence of specific mechanisms to control this function (Brass et al., 2003). Non-invasive brain stimulation studies also show that disruption in inferior frontal regions interferes with imitation of simple finger movements (Heiser et al., 2003). Moreover, stimulating posterior regions in the parietal opercular region (Mengotti et al., 2013) or temporo-parietal junction (Sowden and Catmur, 2013) impedes the ability to repress the automatic tendency to imitate.

Motor resonance phenomena, tightly linked to automatic imitation, can also be revealed in passive action observation tasks that do not require explicit control over imitation. Already observing other people’s actions consistently engages a set of brain regions to a greater extent than watching other categories of visual movement. This “Action Observation Network” (AON) encompasses the human mirror neuron system, which, alongside fronto-parietal regions and temporal cortices, is also engaged during action execution (Caspers et al., 2010; Grosbras et al., 2012). Thus, it contains an implicit “motor resonance” system, coupled with a mechanism to inhibit actual movement. This is also evidenced in indirect electrophysiological measures of brain activity using electro- or magneto-encephalography (EEG and MEG), which show the same signature of brain activity, namely a decrease in power in mu (8−13 Hz), and often also beta (15−20 Hz), frequency bands over the motor cortex (central electrodes) for both action execution and observation (for review see Marshall and Meltzoff, 2011). Experiments in non-human primates indicate that the mu rhythm would reflect activity of motor and mirror neurons (Bimbi et al., 2018).

In addition, interpersonal synchrony has been associated to inter-brain synchrony, which has been measured directly during hyperscanning that is data recorded using neuroscientific methods, such as EEG or fMRI, simultaneously on multiple partners engaged in interpersonal motor alignment. Simultaneous EEG recordings in a dyad showed enhanced correlation in the theta and beta frequencies across brains during the execution of simple coordinated finger or hand movements (Dumas et al., 2010; Yun et al., 2012). In cooperative settings, inter-brain activity coherence was also demonstarted using near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) (Cui et al., 2012; Funane et al., 2011). Cooperative or affiliative behavior in economic games (Astolfi et al., 2014; De Vico Fallani et al., 2010), as well as in more ecological situations like flight behavior cooperation in professional pilots (Toppi et al., 2016) and interaction in romantic partners (Kinreich et al., 2017) was furthermore predicted by different patterns of brain-to-brain synchrony (most often in theta range and frontal cortex). In the same vein, it has been suggested that the greater the coupling between speakers’ and listeners’s brains, the greater the understanding between partners in communication (Stephens et al., 2010). A recent study showed a causal effect of neural synchrony on behavioral synchrony by entraining the motor activity of two individuals at the same time with transcranial alternating current stimulation: in-phase 20 Hz stimulation facilitated the establishment interpersonal movement synchrony in a joint finger tapping task (Novembre et al., 2017). Describing inter-brain coupling throughout development is a key part of gaining full understanding of these meachnisms. Yet, developmental brain imaging studies have been mainly concerned with single subjects set ups, only a few implementing dual scanning paradigms.

Next, we review the main findings in infants and toddlers, primarily using electro-encephalography (EEG) and functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS), and then turn to studies involving children and adolescents, which are mainly using fMRI. Table 1, Table 2, Table 3 present a more complete list of developmental studies involving the different sorts of interpersonal motor alignment in these populations.

3.2. Brain correlates of interpersonal motor alignment and action observation in infants and toddlers

Experiments using EEG or fNIRS have described components of the action observation network present very early on in infancy (see Table 1). Desynchronisation of the equivalent of the mu rhythm in infants (6−9 Hz over central cortex) (Marshall and Meltzoff, 2011), has been reported in 4−11-month-olds when seeing adults walking (Virji-Babul et al., 2012). Similarly, 9-month-old infants show mu rhythm desynchronization while they observe actors performing reach and grasp movements (Debnath et al., 2019; Southgate et al., 2009). This was also reported in 9- and 12-month-olds observing tool use (Yoo et al., 2015), and in 18–36 month-olds watching actors pantomime actions without an actual object present (Warreyn et al., 2013). Moreover, mu rhythm desynchronization has been shown in 12-month olds observing unusual movements depending on the use of objects (e.g., bringing a phone to the mouth compared to a cup) (Stapel et al., 2010) and in 12–14 month olds depending on the unexpected use of effectors for the action (e.g., while having the hands free, using the head to turn on a lamp) (Langeloh et al., 2018). Mu desynchronization in 30-month-old infants during observation of facial movements furthermore suggests an already functioning mirror mechanism of facial expressions during early stages of development (Rayson et al., 2016). Desynchronization in the motor cortex in the frequency range of 8−12 Hz for both execution and observation of drawing actions has been confirmed in a 36-month-old child with pre-surgical cortical electrodes implantation using intracranial recording (Fecteau et al., 2004). Using fNIRS, Shimada and Hiraki (2006) showed that 6−7 months infants who passively observed objected directed hand-arm actions engaged, although to a lesser extent, the same brain regions that when they manipulated the object themselves (Shimada and Hiraki, 2006). Thus, these studies converge to indicate an overlap between action execution and observation very early on in development. The relevance of automatic motor system engagement during passive action observation for studying interpersonal motor alignment is further supported by studies showing that motor resonance in 14-month-old infants is enhanced in an interaction context compared to the direct copying of adults’ gestures. This points towards a stronger mirror resonance mechanism during interaction already in infants (Reid et al., 2011; Saby et al., 2012).

Yet, the automatic engagement of neural mirror mechanism may depend on experience (for review see Marshall & Meltzoff, 2014). This is reflected in studies showing that 4-month-olds show the same response to movements performed by an artificial agent or by a human (Grossmann et al., 2013). Furthermore, studies have reported that the degree of mu desynchronization during action observation is cumulatively correlated with motor skills according to experience ((e.g. grasping (Cannon et al., 2015) or crawling (van Elk et al., 2008)). As such, stronger mu- and beta-desynchronization have been observed in 14- to 16-month-olds watching videos of other infants crawling, as compared to seeing them walking, for which they hadn’t developed rich experience yet (van Elk et al., 2008). Interestingly, similar findings have also been reported in very young macaques who exhibit signs of EEG desynchronization in sensorimotor cortices during grasping observation in the first two weeks of life, increasing as a function of rudimentary grasp development (Festante et al., 2018). These findings are compatible with a narrowing processing dependent on motor experience (Lloyd-Fox et al., 2013). However, these results are contradicted by a study, showing that sensorimotor activation in response to videos of someone walking is present even for infants (4−11 months old) who don’t yet walk. This could suggest that visual familiarity with an action is sufficient to drive motor resonance, without direct implication for behavior (Virji-Babul et al., 2012). In the same vein, Shimada and Hiraki (2006) showed that motor engagement during action observation in 6-month-olds was larger for live than video-taped stimuli, the latter being supposedly less familiar at this age. Alternatively, some authors have suggested that the relationship between motor competencies and motor resonance might be more apparent in older than younger infants (12-month-old compared to 9 month olds) (Yoo et al., 2015). In addition, experience beyond specific action execution skills may also be important. As such, general early life history may have an impact on the degree of interpersonal brain resonance. Fourteen-month-old preterm infants show mu suppression during action observation only in the right parietal regions, whereas full term infants showed the effect in a bilateral fronto-parietal network. Yet, no difference between groups was observed for action execution (Montirosso et al., 2019).

Another line of research has looked at interpersonal neural synchrony during infant social interactions. For instance, Leong et al. (2017) showed that phase-locking of brain activity between infants and parents was related to communication features, like direct gaze or duration of vocalizations. This finding could be interpreted as a mechanism for aligning the periods of higher sensory receptivity between interpersonal partners (Giraud and Poeppel, 2012) and facilitate learning. Recently, some studies have used dual fNIRS scanning to look at correlation in brain activity between infant and parent. The strength of association between parent and child activity in prefrontal regions was increased when they were engaged in cooperative as compared to independent behavior (Miller et al., 2019). Thus, some mirror like mechanisms seem to be present in the brain early in life and well established in the first two years potentially supporting learning, as well as sensorimotor and socio-emotional development. More data is needed to draw stronger conclusions, however, in particular with respect to their link to the positive social effects described in the first section.

3.3. Brain correlates of action observation in children and adolescents

Most often data from infant populations are directly compared to adult studies, leading to discontinuity in the literature with respect to changes later in development. The reason for this may be that the methods and questions are often different when studying older children and adolescents. For these age groups most investigation has focused on action observation paradigms, which can give first insights into the perception-action link underlying interpersonal motor alignment during these years of development (see Table 2, Table 3).

A handful of studies have investigated motor activity during observation of others in older children and young adolescents using the mu suppression index, confirming findings from infants and adult studies. Eismont and colleagues (2017) reported desynchronization of the mu rhythm (taking into account the differences in dominant mu frequencies at different age ranging from 6−13 Hz) over central electrodes in children aged 4−14 years, during execution, observation and imitation of arm movements, with a stable effect across ages. Similar effects were also reported from other research groups, in 2-,4-, 5-, and 11-year-olds respectively (Cochin et al., 2001; Lepage and Théoret, 2006; Martineau and Cochin, 2003). Extending these results, Bernier and colleagues, while showing similar effects in 5−7 year-olds, demonstrated that the strength of the interpersonal motor resonance effect was correlated with a measure of facial imitation abilities (Bernier et al., 2013). Altogether these EEG studies demonstrate a signature of the engagement of children’s own motor system when they observe actions from others that may be stable during childhood.

Nonetheless pulling data from five different studies Oberman and colleagues (2013) concluded that mu suppression might indeed decrease with age (between 6 and 17). This is consistent with the report of Cheng and colleagues who observed higher mu suppression in 3- to 9-year-old children compared to adults (Cheng et al., 2014). Yet, this is inconsistent with other reports of weaker suppression in 8−12 year old children compared to adults, especially when watching movements from an allocentric perspective (Nishimura et al., 2018), or of an increase from 10 to adulthood (Brunsdon et al., 2020). More data from adolescents would be needed to ascertain this developmental trajectory. Other modulating factors such as attention and engagement with the stimulus, for example due to affiliation should also be investigated. Interestingly, Cheng and colleagues reported that contrary to adults, mu suppression in children was not modulated by the emotional content (painful or not) of the observed videos. This is in line with a study that used transcranial magnetic stimulation of motor cortex in combination with electromyography to measure motor resonance, and showed that contrary to adults, motor resonance was not modulated by emotion in 17 year olds (Salvia et al., 2016).

The presence of motor activity during action observation is confirmed using other methodologies. In a small sample of pre-adolescents (age 9−13), Kajume and colleagues used fNIRS and reported increased activity, compared to a baseline control, in the inferior-frontal / premotor regions when participants observed or imitated object-directed actions (Kajiume et al., 2013). A handful of fMRI studies also confirm that children and adolescents, similarly to adults, recruit the AON when observing object-directed hand actions (Biagi et al., 2016; Ohnishi et al., 2004; Shaw et al., 2011a, 2011b). When comparing the activity in the AON directly between children (7−15) and adults, Biagi and colleagues (2016) observed less lateralization to the left in children. This is partly consistent with a longitudinal study showing decreasing activity with age in the right parietal cortex during action observation (although mainly in males) (Shaw et al., 2011b).

Assessing both action execution and observation suggests that the extent of shared activation, reflecting mirror activity, would increase from child- (age 7−10) to adulthood (Morales et al., 2019). Structurally also, areas in the AON, as well as other regions of the social brain, undergo developmental changes throughout adolescence (Mills et al., 2016). Moreover, the activation of the mirror system or the AON is modulated by personal and social factors in development. For instance, children and adolescents (age 9–15) with autism showed higher precentral and middle temporal activity in an action simulation task (i.e., when simulation is necessary to solve a problem), than age-matched control participants (Wadsworth et al., 2017). For passive observation of emotional actions, adolescents with a higher resistance to peer pressure show more coordinated brain activity in the right dorsal premotor and the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, than adolescents with low resistance to peer pressure. Thus, the propensity to withhold social peer pressure seems related to neural interaction when observing emotional actions (Grosbras et al., 2007). Indeed, the AON seems to be modulated by the emotional connotation of the observed action, with higher activity in fronto-parietal regions and additional supramarginal medial prefrontal and amygdala activity for angry as compared to emotionally neutral hand movements (Grosbras et al., 2007; Shaw et al., 2011b). This emotional modulation is however expressed differently in boys and girls across development. At the age of 10 and 11.5 years, both girls and boys show a common level of activity in the AON during observation of angry hand movements, specifically in the posterior parietal cortex, extending into the parieto-occipital junction, the fusiform gyrus, cerebellum, right inferior parietal lobule (IPL), dorsal pre-motor cortex (PMC), intraparietal sulcus (IPS) and orbito-frontal cortex (OFC). Yet, by the age of 13, the same male participants showed higher engagement of a “socioemotional” network than the AON, as compared to their female counterparts. This network comprised brain regions of the so-called social brain, specific to the processing of emotional actions including the temporo-parietal junction, the orbitofrontal cortex and the insula. This suggests that male adolescents around the age of thirteen years do not recruit action observation, but rather socioemotional processes when observing angry hand actions (Shaw et al., 2011a). These findings are paralleled by observations of structural brain development: adolescents with a higher degree of resistance to peer influence show higher interregional correlation of cortical thickness between nodes of the AON (Paus, Toro, et al., 2008). This underlines that the importance of the peer group on behavior could be related to brain development of areas related to social cognition.

Another line of investigation looked directly at interindividual brain synchrony in social settings, using dual-functional fNIRS in naturalistic interactions between caregivers and their children of preschool (mean age of five years) (Nguyen et al., 2020) and school age (five to nine years of age) (Reindl et al., 2018). High neural synchrony between children and caregivers correlated positively with behavioral reciprocity, predicted problem-solving success (Nguyen et al., 2020) and cooperative performance (Reindl et al., 2018). It has thus been suggested that neural synchrony may be a biomarker for interaction quality between a child and the caregiver, representing a neural mechanism for emotional connection linked to the development of adaptive emotion regulation. In a study on adolescents (17 and 18 years), Dikker et al. (2017) used portable EEG devices in a highschool classroom and observed that general measures of synchrony in the group (i.e., coherence between responses in multiple brain areas) were highly correlated with the level of engagement and enjoyment reported by students. Furthermore, they showed that, as in adults, social priming through engagement in eye contact increased interbrain synchrony within student pairs. Nonetheless, in another study with the same protocol, Bevilacqua and colleagues (2019) reported that the level of “interbrain synchrony” was not related to memory retention. Research in this direction should be pursued to explore to what extent interpersonal neural synchrony relates to interpersonal alignment and to social facilitation at different stages of development.

In particular, the continuing development brain areas important for social processing during adolescence suggests interpersonal motor alignment as a potential connective element between the two networks for healthy adolescent development. This could furthermore inform maladaptive development, for example in autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and psychopathologies, such as schizophrenia, social anxiety and depression, whose onsets often fall into adolescence (Paus, Keshavan, et al., 2008). Adolescence, as a formative transition period from child-, to adulthood, may be the optimal period to intervene and prevent psychopathologies (Eldreth et al., 2013)

4. Implications: the importance of studying interpersonal motor alignment in healthy development, including adolescence

As we hope to have demonstrated, interpersonal motor alignment is an important social signal for the establishment and maintenance of relationships and group cohesion throughout development. Even more so, the lack of coordinated interactions starting already at birth may lead to persistent difficulties throughout life in domains of social and emotional development, as well as self-regulation and the capacity for intimate relationships (Feldman, 2007b, 2007c). We contend that this importance of interpersonal motor alignment continues throughout development, extending from early relations with primary caregivers to family and peer relationships. Especially during adolescence, given the reorientation of the social focus to peers and away from family members, social contingencies and interpersonal behavioral alignment with peers may come into focus.

Reliable reciprocated interpersonal motor alignment throughout childhood and adolescence could contribute to strengthen the link between functional and structural brain development in areas related to the social brain and the AON and thereby enhance social and emotional resilience during adolescence. This is underlined by findings showing that socially contingent and synchronous interactions between caregivers and infants predict adolescent empathic abilities (Feldman, 2007a). But also, adolescents with higher resistance to peer influence show higher cortical thickness between nodes of the AON (Paus, Toro, et al., 2008) and highly coordinated brain activity in areas related to action perception and decision making (Grosbras et al., 2007). Conversely, unreliable alignment with peers or the family could augment feelings of social rejection and stress, in a period already marked by heightened sensitivity to peer rejection (Sebastian et al., 2010).

Social stressors such as (cyber-) bullying (Crick et al., 2001), social exclusion (Crone and Konijn, 2018) and enhanced risk-taking behavior (Chein et al., 2011; van Oosten and Vandenbosch, 2017v) peak during adolescence. Moreover, adolescence, as a time of substantial neurobiological and behavioral changes, confers a vulnerability for certain types of psychopathologies (Paus, Keshavan, et al., 2008). Continuing interpersonal motor alignment throughout adolescence may strongly support the establishment and maintenance of resilience and coping mechanisms. Yet research in this domain is critically lacking.

Indeed, as demonstrated throughout this article, reliable reciprocity through interpersonal motor alignment has positive social and emotional effects, which are, with the exception for action observation, underinvestigated during adolescence. Yet, especially its potentially strengthening effects of interpersonal motor alignment through mimicry and synchronous behavior on the connection between the AON and other brain regions implicated in social cognition, may have beneficial effects on adolescent resilience. This remains to be investigated, but interpersonal motor alignment programs to strengthen social resilience during adolescence and intervention programs targeting adolescents’ psychological or psychiatric problems could be envisioned. This could be implemented, in (online) video games and social media to strengthen healthy adolescent development through interpersonal motoric reciprocity. Implementation on social media, via videoconferencing tools or on- or offline video games, may help reaching out to adolescents and making interventions more accessible to them. Yet, while this idea is attractive, technical limitations, such as delays of timing due to different internet connections, may hinder smooth interaction and impede mechanisms mediating positive effects. Aiming to overcome this problem, several software programs have been suggested for musicians and to sing together1 . Social media and videoconferencing tools allow adolescents to stay connected, even in the face of isolation. This could be observed recently during social isolation in the wake of the COVID-19 pandemic, when two third of American teenagers reported using video chat to stay connected with their peers2 . This makes this avenue worth pursuing.

5. Conclusion

This is the first integrative review of interpersonal motor alignment and its positive social functions from a developmental perspective. It highlights the importance of reliable interpersonal motor alignment for healthy social development and especially points towards the need to extend this research to adolescence, a period of enhanced social sensitivity. Indeed, although interpersonal motor alignment may link social cognition to cognitive control and may have an important role in healthy adolescent interactions, it is still underinvestigated during adolescence. Moreover, since adolescence is a time of substantial neurobiological and behavioral changes, it may confer a psychopathological vulnerability. Studying interpersonal motor alignment as one of the links between social cognition and cognitive control may inform motor therapies for prevention and recovery and aid healthy adolescent development.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the Fondation pour la Recherche Médicale (FRM, SPF20171039127) to BR, the grants from the Agence Nationale de la Recherche (ANR, France) ANR-14-ACHN-0023 and ANR-11-IDEX-0001-02) to MHG and benefited from the grants ANR-16-CONV-0002 (ILCB), ANR-11-LABX-0036 (BLRI) and the Excellence Initiative of Aix-Marseille University (A*MIDEX).

Footnotes

References

  1. Anisfeld M. Only tongue protrusion modeling is matched by neonates. Dev. Rev. 1996;16(2):149–161. [Google Scholar]
  2. Ardizzi M., Sestito M., Martini F., Umilta M.A., Ravera R., Gallese V. When age matters: differences in facial mimicry and autonomic responses to peers’ emotions in teenagers and adults. PLoS One. 2014;9(10) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0110763. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Astolfi L., Toppi J., Vogel P., Mattia D., Babiloni F., Ciaramidaro A., Siniatchkin M. Investigating the neural basis of cooperative joint action. An EEG hyperscanning study. 2014 36th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society; IEEE; 2014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Baird A.D., Scheffer I.E., Wilson S.J. Mirror neuron system involvement in empathy: a critical look at the evidence. Soc. Neurosci. 2011;6(4):327–335. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2010.547085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Berchio C., Rihs T.A., Michel C.M., Brunet D., Apicella F., Muratori F. Parieto-frontal circuits during observation of hidden and visible motor acts in children. A high-density EEG source imaging study. Brain Topogr. 2014;27(2):258–270. doi: 10.1007/s10548-013-0314-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bernier R., Aaronson B., McPartland J. The role of imitation in the observed heterogeneity in EEG mu rhythm in autism and typical development. Brain Cogn. 2013;82(1):69–75. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2013.02.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bevilacqua D., Davidesco I., Wan L., Chaloner K., Rowland J., Ding M. Brain-to-brain synchrony and learning outcomes vary by student–teacher dynamics: evidence from a real-world classroom electroencephalography study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2019;31(3):401–411. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Biagi L., Cioni G., Fogassi L., Guzzetta A., Sgandurra G., Tosetti M. Action observation network in childhood: a comparative fMRI study with adults. Dev. Sci. 2016;19(6):1075–1086. doi: 10.1111/desc.12353. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bimbi M., Festante F., Coudé G., Vanderwert R.E., Fox N.A., Ferrari P.F. Simultaneous scalp recorded EEG and local field potentials from monkey ventral premotor cortex during action observation and execution reveals the contribution of mirror and motor neurons to the mu-rhythm. NeuroImage. 2018;175:22–31. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.03.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Blakemore S.-J., Mills K.L. Is adolescence a sensitive period for sociocultural processing? Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014;65:187–207. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bourgeois P., Hess U. The impact of social context on mimicry. Biol. Psychol. 2008;77(3):343–352. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.11.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Brass M., Heyes C. Imitation: is cognitive neuroscience solving the correspondence problem? Trends Cogn. Sci. 2005;9(10):489–495. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Brass M., Bekkering H., Wohlschläger A., Prinz W. Compatibility between observed and executed finger movements: comparing symbolic, spatial, and imitative cues. Brain Cogn. 2000;44(2):124–143. doi: 10.1006/brcg.2000.1225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Brass M., Derrfuss J., Matthes-von Cramon G., von Cramon D.Y. Imitative response tendencies in patients with frontal brain lesions. Neuropsychology. 2003;17(2):265. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.17.2.265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Brass M., Derrfuss J., von Cramon D.Y. The inhibition of imitative and overlearned responses: a functional double dissociation. Neuropsychologia. 2005;43(1):89–98. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.06.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Brass M., Ruby P., Spengler S. Inhibition of imitative behaviour and social cognition. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2009;364(1528):2359–2367. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Brunsdon V.E.A., Bradford E.E.F., Smith L., Ferguson H.J. Short-term physical training enhances mirror system activation to action observation. Soc. Neurosci. 2020;15(1):98–107. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2019.1660708. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Bryant L.J., Cuevas K. Effects of active and observational experience on EEG activity during early childhood. Psychophysiology. 2019;56(7) doi: 10.1111/psyp.13360. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Buccino G., Lui F., Canessa N., Patteri I., Lagravinese G., Benuzzi F. Neural circuits involved in the recognition of actions performed by nonconspecifics: an fMRI study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2004;16(1):114–126. doi: 10.1162/089892904322755601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Bureau J., Yurkowski K., Schmiedel S., Martin J., Moss E., Pallanca D. Making children laugh: parent–child dyadic synchrony and preschool attachment. Infant Ment. Health J. 2014;35(5):482–494. doi: 10.1002/imhj.21474. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Butler E.E., Ward R., Ramsey R. The influence of facial signals on the automatic imitation of hand actions. Front. Psychol. 2016:7. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01653. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Calvo-Merino B., Glaser D.E., Grèzes J., Passingham R.E., Haggard P. Action observation and acquired motor skills: an fMRI study with expert dancers. Cereb. Cortex. 2004;15(8):1243–1249. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Cannon E.N., Simpson E.A., Fox N.A., Vanderwert R.E., Woodward A.L., Ferrari P.F. Relations between infants’ emerging reach-grasp competence and event-related desynchronization in EEG. Dev. Sci. 2015;19(1):50–62. doi: 10.1111/desc.12295. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Carpenter M., Uebel J., Tomasello M. Being mimicked increases prosocial behavior in 18-Month-Old infants. Child Dev. 2013;84(5):1511–1518. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12083. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Carr L., Iacoboni M., Dubeau M.-C., Mazziotta J.C., Lenzi G.L. Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: a relay from neural systems for imitation to limbic areas. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2003;100(9):5497–5502. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0935845100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Caspers S., Zilles K., Laird A.R., Eickhoff S.B. ALE meta-analysis of action observation and imitation in the human brain. NeuroImage. 2010;50(3):1148–1167. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Catmur C., Heyes C. Is it what you do, or when you do it? The roles of contingency and similarity in pro‐social effects of imitation. Cogn. Sci. 2013;37(8):1541–1552. doi: 10.1111/cogs.12071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Catmur C., Walsh V., Heyes C. Sensorimotor learning configures the human mirror system. Curr. Biol. 2007;17(17):1527–1531. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Catmur C., Walsh V., Heyes C. Associative sequence learning: the role of experience in the development of imitation and the mirror system. Philos. Trans. Biol. Sci. 2009;364(1528) doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0048. http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/364/1528/2369.abstract 2369 LP – 2380. Retrieved from. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Catmur C., Mars R.B., Rushworth M.F., Heyes C. Making mirrors: premotor cortex stimulation enhances mirror and counter-mirror motor facilitation. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2011;23(9):2352–2362. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21590. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Chartrand T.L., Bargh J.A. The chameleon effect: the perception–behavior link and social interaction. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1999;76(6):893. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.76.6.893. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Chartrand T.L., Lakin J.L. The antecedents and consequences of human behavioral mimicry. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2013;64:285–308. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143754. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Chartrand T.L., Maddux W.W., Lakin J.L. Beyond the perception-behavior link: the ubiquitous utility and motivational moderators of nonconscious mimicry. New Unconscious. 2005:334–361. [Google Scholar]
  34. Chein J., Albert D., O’Brien L., Uckert K., Steinberg L. Peers increase adolescent risk taking by enhancing activity in the brain’s reward circuitry. Dev. Sci. 2011;14(2):F1–F10. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01035.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Cheng Y., Chen C., Decety J. An EEG/ERP investigation of the development of empathy in early and middle childhood. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2014;10:160–169. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2014.08.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Cirelli L.K. How interpersonal synchrony facilitates early prosocial behavior. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2018;20:35–39. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.08.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Cirelli L.K., Einarson K.M., Trainor L.J. Interpersonal synchrony increases prosocial behavior in infants. Dev. Sci. 2014;17(6):1003–1011. doi: 10.1111/desc.12193. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Cirelli L.K., Wan S.J., Trainor L.J. Social effects of movement synchrony: increased infant helpfulness only transfers to affiliates of synchronously moving partners. Infancy. 2016;21(6):807–821. [Google Scholar]
  39. Cirelli L.K., Wan S.J., Johanis T.C., Trainor L.J. Infants’ use of interpersonal asynchrony as a signal for third-party affiliation. Music. Sci. 2018 [Google Scholar]
  40. Cochin S., Barthelemy C., Roux S., Martineau J. Electroencephalographic activity during perception of motion in childhood. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2001;13(9):1791–1796. doi: 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01544.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Cohen E.E.A., Ejsmond-Frey R., Knight N., Dunbar R.I.M. Rowers’ high: behavioural synchrony is correlated with elevated pain thresholds. Biol. Lett. 2010;6(1):106–108. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2009.0670. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Cook J., Bird G. Social attitudes differentially modulate imitation in adolescents and adults. Exp. Brain Res. 2011;211(3–4):601–612. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2584-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Coull J.T., Cheng R.-K., Meck W.H. Neuroanatomical and neurochemical substrates of timing. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36(1):3–25. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Cracco E., Brass M. Automatic imitation of multiple agents: Simultaneous or random representation? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 2018;44(5):729. doi: 10.1037/xhp0000489. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Cracco E., Brass M. The role of sensorimotor processes in social group contagion. Cogn. Psychol. 2018;103:23–41. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2018.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Cracco E., Brass M. Reaction time indices of automatic imitation measure imitative response tendencies. Conscious. Cogn. 2019;68:115–118. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2019.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Cracco E., Bardi L., Desmet C., Genschow O., Rigoni D., De Coster L. Automatic imitation: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 2018;144(5):453. doi: 10.1037/bul0000143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Crick N.R., Nelson D.A., Morales J.R., Cullerton-Sen C., Casas J.F., Hickman S.E. Peer Harassment in School: The Plight of the Vulnerable and Victimized. 2001. Relational victimization in childhood and adolescence; pp. 196–214. [Google Scholar]
  49. Crone E.A., Konijn E.A. Media use and brain development during adolescence. Nat. Commun. 2018;9(1):588. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-03126-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Cui X., Bryant D.M., Reiss A.L. NIRS-based hyperscanning reveals increased interpersonal coherence in superior frontal cortex during cooperation. NeuroImage. 2012;59(3):2430–2437. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Dalton A.N., Chartrand T.L., Finkel E.J. The schema-driven chameleon: how mimicry affects executive and self-regulatory resources. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2010;98(4):605. doi: 10.1037/a0017629. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. de Klerk C.C.J.M., Johnson M.H., Heyes C.M., Southgate V. Baby steps: investigating the development of perceptual–motor couplings in infancy. Dev. Sci. 2015;18(2):270–280. doi: 10.1111/desc.12226. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. de Klerk C.C.J.M., Hamilton A.F., de C., Southgate V. Eye contact modulates facial mimicry in 4-month-old infants: an EMG and fNIRS study. Cortex. 2018;106:93–103. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.05.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. de Klerk C.C.J.M., Bulgarelli C., Hamilton A., Southgate V. Selective facial mimicry of native over foreign speakers in preverbal infants. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2019;183:33–47. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2019.01.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. de Klerk C.C.J.M., Lamy‐Yang I., Southgate V. The role of sensorimotor experience in the development of mimicry in infancy. Dev. Sci. 2019;22(3):e12771. doi: 10.1111/desc.12771. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. De Vico Fallani F., Nicosia V., Sinatra R., Astolfi L., Cincotti F., Mattia D. Defecting or not defecting: how to “Read” human behavior during cooperative games by EEG measurements. PLoS One. 2010;5(12):e14187. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0014187. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. De Vignemont F., Singer T. The empathic brain: how, when and why? Trends Cogn. Sci. 2006;10(10):435–441. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.08.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Debnath R., Salo V.C., Buzzell G.A., Yoo K.H., Fox N.A. Mu rhythm desynchronization is specific to action execution and observation: evidence from time-frequency and connectivity analysis. NeuroImage. 2019;184:496–507. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.09.053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Deschamps P.K.H., Coppes L., Kenemans J.L., Schutter D., Matthys W. Electromyographic responses to emotional facial expressions in 6–7 year olds with autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2015;45(2):354–362. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1890-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Deschrijver E., Wiersema J.R., Brass M. The influence of action observation on action execution: dissociating the contribution of action on perception, perception on action, and resolving conflict. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2017;17(2):381–393. doi: 10.3758/s13415-016-0485-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Dignath D., Lotze-Hermes P., Farmer H., Pfister R. Contingency and contiguity of imitative behaviour affect social affiliation. Psychol. Res. 2018;82(4):819–831. doi: 10.1007/s00426-017-0854-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Dikker S., Wan L., Davidesco I., Kaggen L., Oostrik M., McClintock J. Brain-to-Brain synchrony tracks real-world dynamic group interactions in the classroom. Curr. Biol. 2017;27(9):1375–1380. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.04.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Duffy K.A., Chartrand T.L. Mimicry: causes and consequences. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2015;3:112–116. [Google Scholar]
  64. Dumas G., Nadel J., Soussignan R., Martinerie J., Garnero L. Inter-brain synchronization during social interaction. PLoS One. 2010;5(8):e12166. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0012166. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Dunbar R.I.M., Baron R., Frangou A., Pearce E., van Leeuwin E.J.C., Stow J. Social laughter is correlated with an elevated pain threshold. Proc. R. Soc. Lond., B, Biol. Sci. 2011 doi: 10.1098/rspb.2011.1373. rspb20111373. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Eismont E.V., Makhin S.A., Bakunova A.V., Kaida A.I., Pavlenko V.B. Properties of the EEG μ rhythm and its reactivity during the performance, observation, imitation, and auditory recognition of movements in children aged 4–14 years. Hum. Physiol. 2017;43(3):274–279. doi: 10.1134/s0362119717030057. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  67. Eldreth D., Hardin M.G., Pavletic N., Ernst M. Adolescent transformations of behavioral and neural processes as potential targets for prevention. Prev. Sci. 2013;14(3):257–266. doi: 10.1007/s11121-012-0322-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Endedijk H.M., Meyer M., Bekkering H., Cillessen A.H.N., Hunnius S. Neural mirroring and social interaction: motor system involvement during action observation relates to early peer cooperation. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2017;24:33–41. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2017.01.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Farmer H., Ciaunica A., Hamilton A.Fde C. The functions of imitative behaviour in humans. Mind Lang. 2018;33(4):378–396. doi: 10.1111/mila.12189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Fawcett C., Liszkowski U. Mimicry and play initiation in 18-month-old infants. Infant Behav. Dev. 2012;35(4):689–696. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2012.07.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Fawcett C., Tunçgenç B. Infants’ use of movement synchrony to infer social affiliation in others. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2017;160:127–136. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.03.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Fecteau S., Carmant L., Tremblay C., Robert M., Bouthillier A., Théoret H. A motor resonance mechanism in children? Evidence from subdural electrodes in a 36-month-old child. NeuroReport. 2004;15(17):2625–2627. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200412030-00013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Feldman R. Mother‐infant synchrony and the development of moral orientation in childhood and adolescence: direct and indirect mechanisms of developmental continuity. Am. J. Orthopsychiatry. 2007;77(4):582–597. doi: 10.1037/0002-9432.77.4.582. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Feldman R. Parent–infant synchrony: biological foundations and developmental outcomes. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2007;16(6):340–345. [Google Scholar]
  75. Feldman R. Parent–infant synchrony and the construction of shared timing; physiological precursors, developmental outcomes, and risk conditions. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2007;48(3–4):329–354. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01701.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Festante F., Vanderwert R.E., Sclafani V., Paukner A., Simpson E.A., Suomi S.J. EEG beta desynchronization during hand goal-directed action observation in newborn monkeys and its relation to the emergence of hand motor skills. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2018;30:142–149. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.02.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Filippi C.A., Cannon E.N., Fox N.A., Thorpe S.G., Ferrari P.F., Woodward A.L. Motor system activation predicts goal imitation in 7-month-old infants. Psychol. Sci. 2016;27(5):675–684. doi: 10.1177/0956797616632231. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Finkel E.J., Campbell W.K., Brunell A.B., Dalton A.N., Scarbeck S.J., Chartrand T.L. High-maintenance interaction: inefficient social coordination impairs self-regulation. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2006;91(3):456. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.3.456. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Fischer-Lokou J., Martin A., Guéguen N., Lamy L. Mimicry and propagation of prosocial behavior in a natural setting. Psychol. Rep. 2011;108(2):599–605. doi: 10.2466/07.17.21.PR0.108.2.599-605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Fitzpatrick P., Frazier J.A., Cochran D.M., Mitchell T., Coleman C., Schmidt R.C. Impairments of social motor synchrony evident in autism spectrum disorder. Front. Psychol. 2016;7:1323. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01323. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Fourie E., Palser E.R., Pokorny J.J., Neff M., Rivera S.M. Neural processing and production of gesture in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Front. Psychol. 2020;10:3045. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.03045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Funane T., Kiguchi M., Atsumori H., Sato H., Kubota K., Koizumi H. Synchronous activity of two people’s prefrontal cortices during a cooperative task measured by simultaneous near-infrared spectroscopy. J. Biomed. Opt. 2011;16(7):77011. doi: 10.1117/1.3602853. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Genschow O., Schindler S. The influence of group membership on cross-contextual imitation. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2016;23(4):1257–1265. doi: 10.3758/s13423-015-0983-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Genschow O., van Den Bossche S., Cracco E., Bardi L., Rigoni D., Brass M. Mimicry and automatic imitation are not correlated. PLoS One. 2017;12(9):e0183784. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0183784. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Giraud A.-L., Poeppel D. Cortical oscillations and speech processing: emerging computational principles and operations. Nat. Neurosci. 2012;15(4):511–517. doi: 10.1038/nn.3063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Gleibs I.H., Wilson N., Reddy G., Catmur C. Group dynamics in automatic imitation. PLoS One. 2016;11(9):e0162880. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0162880. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Good A., Russo F.A. Singing promotes cooperation in a diverse group of children. Soc. Psychol. 2016;47(6):340–344. [Google Scholar]
  88. Gordon A., Geddert R., Hogeveen J., Krug M.K., Obhi S., Solomon M. Not so automatic imitation: expectation of incongruence reduces interference in both autism Spectrum disorder and typical development. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2020:1–14. doi: 10.1007/s10803-019-04355-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Grosbras M.-H., Jansen M., Leonard G., McIntosh A., Osswald K., Poulsen C. Neural mechanisms of resistance to peer influence in early adolescence. J. Neurosci. 2007;27(30):8040–8045. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1360-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Grosbras M., Beaton S., Eickhoff S.B. Brain regions involved in human movement perception: a quantitative voxel‐based meta‐analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2012;33(2):431–454. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21222. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Grossmann T., Cross E.S., Ticini L.F., Daum M.M. Action observation in the infant brain: the role of body form and motion. Soc. Neurosci. 2013;8(1):22–30. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2012.696077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Hasler B.S., Hirschberger G., Shani-Sherman T., Friedman D.A. Virtual peacemakers: mimicry increases empathy in simulated contact with virtual outgroup members. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 2014;17(12):766–771. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2014.0213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Heiser M., Iacoboni M., Maeda F., Marcus J., Mazziotta J.C. The essential role of Broca’s area in imitation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2003;17(5):1123–1128. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02530.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Helt M.S., Eigsti I., Snyder P.J., Fein D.A. Contagious yawning in autistic and typical development. Child Dev. 2010;81(5):1620–1631. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01495.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Heyes C. Automatic imitation. Psychol. Bull. 2011;137(3):463–483. doi: 10.1037/a0022288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Heyes Cecilia, Bird G., Johnson H., Haggard P. Experience modulates automatic imitation. Cogn. Brain Res. 2005;22(2):233–240. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.09.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Hickok G. Eight problems for the mirror neuron theory of action understanding in monkeys and humans. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2009;21(7):1229–1243. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Hove M.J., Risen J.L. It’s all in the timing: interpersonal synchrony increases affiliation. Soc. Cogn. 2009;27(6):949–960. [Google Scholar]
  99. Iacoboni M. Imitation, empathy, and mirror neurons. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2009;60:653–670. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Ip W., Chiu C., Wan C. Birds of a feather and birds flocking together: physical versus behavioral cues may lead to trait-versus goal-based group perception. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2006;90(3):368. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.90.3.368. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Isomura T., Nakano T. Automatic facial mimicry in response to dynamic emotional stimuli in five-month-old infants. Proc. Royal Soci. B: Biol. Sci. 2016;283(1844):20161948. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2016.1948. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Jones S.S. Imitation in infancy: the development of mimicry. Psychol. Sci. 2007;18(7):593–599. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01945.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Jones S. Can newborn infants imitate? Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 2017;8(1–2):e1410. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Kaiser J., Crespo-Llado M.M., Turati C., Geangu E. The development of spontaneous facial responses to others’ emotions in infancy: an EMG study. Sci. Rep. 2017;7(1):1–10. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-17556-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Kajiume A., Aoyama-Setoyama S., Saito-Hori Y., Ishikawa N., Kobayashi M. Reduced brain activation during imitation and observation of others in children with pervasive developmental disorder: a pilot study. Behav. Brain Funct. 2013;9(1):21. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-9-21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Kennedy-Costantini S., Oostenbroek J., Suddendorf T., Nielsen M., Redshaw J., Davis J. There is no compelling evidence that human neonates imitate. Behav. Brain Sci. 2017;40 doi: 10.1017/S0140525X16001898. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Keven N., Akins K.A. Neonatal imitation in context: sensorimotor development in the perinatal period. Behav. Brain Sci. 2017;40 doi: 10.1017/S0140525X16000911. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Keysers C., Gazzola V. Social neuroscience: mirror neurons recorded in humans. Curr. Biol. 2010;20(8):R353–R354. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.03.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Keysers C., Perrett D.I. Demystifying social cognition: a Hebbian perspective. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2004;8(11):501–507. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.09.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Kilner J.M., Paulignan Y., Blakemore S.-J. An interference effect of observed biological movement on action. Curr. Biol. 2003;13(6):522–525. doi: 10.1016/s0960-9822(03)00165-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Kinreich S., Djalovski A., Kraus L., Louzoun Y., Feldman R. Brain-to-Brain synchrony during naturalistic social interactions. Sci. Rep. 2017;7(1) doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-17339-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Kirschner S., Tomasello M. Joint drumming: social context facilitates synchronization in preschool children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2009;102(3):299–314. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2008.07.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Kirschner S., Tomasello M. Joint music making promotes prosocial behavior in 4-year-old children. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2010;31(5):354–364. [Google Scholar]
  114. Kleinspehn-Ammerlahn A., Riediger M., Schmiedek F., von Oertzen T., Li S.-C., Lindenberger U. Dyadic drumming across the lifespan reveals a zone of proximal development in children. Dev. Psychol. 2011;47(3):632. doi: 10.1037/a0021818. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Kühn S., Müller B.C.N., van Baaren R.B., Wietzker A., Dijksterhuis A., Brass M. Why do I like you when you behave like me? Neural mechanisms mediating positive consequences of observing someone being imitated. Soc. Neurosci. 2010;5(4):384–392. doi: 10.1080/17470911003633750. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Lakens D. Movement synchrony and perceived entitativity. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2010;46(5):701–708. [Google Scholar]
  117. Lakens D., Stel M. If they move in sync, they must feel in sync: movement synchrony leads to attributions of rapport and entitativity. Soc. Cogn. 2011;29(1):1–14. [Google Scholar]
  118. Lakin Jessica L., Chartrand T.L. Using nonconscious behavioral mimicry to create affiliation and rapport. Psychol. Sci. 2003;14(4):334–339. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.14481. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Lakin J.L., Jefferis V.E., Cheng C.M., Chartrand T.L. The chameleon effect as social glue: evidence for the evolutionary significance of nonconscious mimicry. J. Nonverbal Behav. 2003;27(3):145–162. doi: 10.1023/a:1025389814290. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  120. Lakin Jessica L., Chartrand T.L., Arkin R.M. I am too just like you nonconscious mimicry as an automatic behavioral response to social exclusion. Psychol. Sci. 2008;19(8):816–822. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02162.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Lang M., Bahna V., Shaver J.H., Reddish P., Xygalatas D. Sync to link: endorphin-mediated synchrony effects on cooperation. Biol. Psychol. 2017;127:191–197. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Langeloh M., Buttelmann D., Matthes D., Grassmann S., Pauen S., Hoehl S. Reduced mu power in response to unusual actions is context-dependent in 1-year-olds. Front. Psychol. 2018;9:36. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Launay J., Tarr B., Dunbar R.I.M. Synchrony as an adaptive mechanism for large‐scale human social bonding. Ethology. 2016;122(10):779–789. [Google Scholar]
  124. Leclère C., Viaux S., Avril M., Achard C., Chetouani M., Missonnier S., Cohen D. Why synchrony matters during mother-child interactions: a systematic review. PLoS One. 2014;9(12):e113571. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0113571. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Leong V., Byrne E., Clackson K., Georgieva S., Lam S., Wass S. Speaker gaze increases information coupling between infant and adult brains. Cold Spring Harbor Lab. 2017 doi: 10.1101/108878. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Lepage J., Théoret H. EEG evidence for the presence of an action observation–execution matching system in children. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2006;23(9):2505–2510. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04769.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Leslie K.R., Johnson-Frey S.H., Grafton S.T. Functional imaging of face and hand imitation: towards a motor theory of empathy. Neuroimage. 2004;21(2):601–607. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.09.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Liao Y., Acar Z.A., Makeig S., Deak G. EEG imaging of toddlers during dyadic turn-taking: mu-rhythm modulation while producing or observing social actions. NeuroImage. 2015;112:52–60. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Libertus K., Libertus M.E., Einspieler C., Marschik P.B. “What” matters more than" Why"-Neonatal behaviors initiate social responses. Behav. Brain Sci. 2017:40. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X16001916. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Lloyd-Fox S., Wu R., Richards J.E., Elwell C.E., Johnson M.H. Cortical activation to action perception is associated with action production abilities in young infants. Cereb. Cortex. 2013;25(2):289–297. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht207. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Lloyd-Fox S., Wu R., Richards J.E., Elwell C.E., Johnson M.H. Cortical activation to action perception is associated with action production abilities in young infants. Cereb. Cortex. 2015;25(2):289–297. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht207. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Losin E.A.R., Iacoboni M., Martin A., Cross K.A., Dapretto M. Race modulates neural activity during imitation. Neuroimage. 2012;59(4):3594–3603. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.074. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Losin E.A.R., Cross K.A., Iacoboni M., Dapretto M. Neural processing of race during imitation: self‐similarity versus social status. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2014;35(4):1723–1739. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22287. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Lust J.M., Van Schie H.T., Wilson P.H., Van der Helden J., Pelzer B., Steenbergen B. Activation of Mirror Neuron Regions is Altered in Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD)–Neurophysiological Evidence using an Action Observation Paradigm. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2019;13:232. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00232. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Majdandžić J., Rauchbauer B., Gerger G., Maksimova M., Huber-Huber C., Lamm C. How imitation affects empathy and prosocial behavior: the role of movement congruency and temporal contingency. Poster Presented at the 3rd International Conference of the European Society for Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience (ESCAN); Porto, Portugal. June; 2016. [Google Scholar]
  136. Markova G., Nguyen T., Hoehl S. Neurobehavioral interpersonal synchrony in early development: the role of interactional rhythms. Front. Psychol. 2019 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02078. https://www.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02078 Retrieved from. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Marsh K. The beat will make you be courage”: the role of a secondary school music program in supporting young refugees and newly arrived immigrants in Australia. Res. Stud. Music. Educ. 2012;34(2):93–111. [Google Scholar]
  138. Marsh K. Creating bridges: music, play and well-being in the lives of refugee and immigrant children and young people. Music. Educ. Res. 2017;19(1):60–73. [Google Scholar]
  139. Marsh K., Dieckmann S. Contributions of playground singing games to the social inclusion of refugee and newly arrived immigrant children in Australia. Education 3-13. 2017;45(6):710–719. [Google Scholar]
  140. Marsh L.E., Bird G., Catmur C. The imitation game: effects of social cues on ‘imitation’are domain-general in nature. NeuroImage. 2016;139:368–375. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.06.050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Marshall P.J., Meltzoff A.N. Neural mirroring systems: exploring the EEG mu rhythm in human infancy. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2011;1(2):110–123. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2010.09.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Marshall P.J., Meltzoff A.N. Neural mirroring mechanisms and imitation in human infants. Philos. Trans. Biol. Sci. 2014;369(1644):20130620. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0620. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Marshall P.J., Bouquet C.A., Thomas A.L., Shipley T.F. Motor contagion in young children: exploring social influences on perception–action coupling. Neural Netw. 2010;23(8–9):1017–1025. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2010.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Martineau J., Cochin S. Visual perception in children: human, animal and virtual movement activates different cortical areas. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2003;51(1):37–44. doi: 10.1016/s0167-8760(03)00151-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Martineau Joëlle, Cochin S., Magne R., Barthelemy C. Impaired cortical activation in autistic children: is the mirror neuron system involved? Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2008;68(1):35–40. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. McNeill W.H. Harvard University Press; 1997. Keeping Together in Time. [Google Scholar]
  147. Meltzoff A.N. Elements of a comprehensive theory of infant imitation. Behav. Brain Sci. 2017;40 doi: 10.1017/S0140525X1600193X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  148. Meltzoff A.N., Moore M.K. Explaining facial imitation: a theoretical model. Early Dev. Parent. 1997;6(3–4):179. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-0917(199709/12)6:3/4<179::AID-EDP157>3.0.CO;2-R. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  149. Meltzoff A.N., Murray L., Simpson E., Heimann M., Nagy E., Nadel J. Re‐examination of Oostenbroek et al.(2016): Evidence for neonatal imitation of tongue protrusion. Dev. Sci. 2018;21(4):e12609. doi: 10.1111/desc.12609. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  150. Mengotti P., Ticini L.F., Waszak F., Schütz‐Bosbach S., Rumiati R.I. Imitating others’ actions: transcranial magnetic stimulation of the parietal opercula reveals the processes underlying automatic imitation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2013;37(2):316–322. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. Merchant H., Harrington D.L., Meck W.H. Neural basis of the perception and estimation of time. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2013;36:313–336. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062012-170349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  152. Meyer M., Hunnius S., van Elk M., van Ede F., Bekkering H. Joint action modulates motor system involvement during action observation in 3-year-olds. Exp. Brain Res. 2011;211(3–4):581–592. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2658-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  153. Michael J., Sebanz N., Knoblich G. Observing joint action: coordination creates commitment. Cognition. 2016;157:106–113. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.08.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  154. Miles L.K., Nind L.K., Macrae C.N. The rhythm of rapport: interpersonal synchrony and social perception. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2009;45(3):585–589. [Google Scholar]
  155. Miles L.K., Lumsden J., Richardson M.J., Macrae C.N. Do birds of a feather move together? Group membership and behavioral synchrony. Exp. Brain Res. 2011;211(3–4):495–503. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2641-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  156. Miller J.G., Vrtička P., Cui X., Shrestha S., Hosseini S.M.H., Baker J.M., Reiss A.L. Inter-brain synchrony in mother-child dyads during cooperation: an fNIRS hyperscanning study. Neuropsychologia. 2019;124:117–124. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.12.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  157. Mills K.L., Goddings A.-L., Herting M.M., Meuwese R., Blakemore S.-J., Crone E.A. Structural brain development between childhood and adulthood: convergence across four longitudinal samples. Neuroimage. 2016;141:273–281. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.07.044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  158. Monier F., Droit‐Volet S., Coull J.T. The beneficial effect of synchronized action on motor and perceptual timing in children. Dev. Sci. 2019;22(6):e12821. doi: 10.1111/desc.12821. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  159. Montirosso R., Piazza C., Giusti L., Provenzi L., Ferrari P.F., Reni G., Borgatti R. Exploring the EEG mu rhythm associated with observation and execution of a goal-directed action in 14-month-old preterm infants. Sci. Rep. 2019;9(1) doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-45495-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  160. Morales S., Bowman L.C., Velnoskey K.R., Fox N.A., Redcay E. An fMRI study of action observation and action execution in childhood. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2019;37 doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2019.100655. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  161. Mukamel R., Ekstrom A.D., Kaplan J., Iacoboni M., Fried I. Single-neuron responses in humans during execution and observation of actions. Curr. Biol. 2010;20(8):750–756. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.02.045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  162. Nguyen T., Schleihauf H., Kayhan E., Matthes D., Vrtička P., Hoehl S. The effects of interaction quality on neural synchrony during mother-child problem solving. Cortex. 2020;124:235–249. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.11.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  163. Nielsen M., Blank C. Imitation in young children: when who gets copied is more important than what gets copied. Dev. Psychol. 2011;47(4):1050. doi: 10.1037/a0023866. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  164. Nishimura Y., Ikeda Y., Suematsu A., Higuchi S. Effect of visual orientation on mu suppression in children: a comparative EEG study with adults. J. Physiol. Anthropol. 2018;37(1):16. doi: 10.1186/s40101-018-0175-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  165. Nobusako S., Sakai A., Tsujimoto T., Shuto T., Nishi Y., Asano D. Deficits in visuo-motor temporal integration impacts manual dexterity in probable developmental coordination disorder. Front. Neurol. 2018;9:114. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.00114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  166. Novembre G., Knoblich G., Dunne L., Keller P.E. Interpersonal synchrony enhanced through 20 Hz phase-coupled dual brain stimulation. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2017;12(4):662–670. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw172. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  167. O’Sullivan E.P., Bijvoet-van den Berg S., Caldwell C.A. Automatic imitation effects are influenced by experience of synchronous action in children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2018;171:113–130. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2018.01.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  168. Oberman L.M., Ramachandran V.S., Pineda J.A. Modulation of mu suppression in children with autism spectrum disorders in response to familiar or unfamiliar stimuli: the mirror neuron hypothesis. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46(5):1558–1565. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.01.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  169. Oberman L.M., McCleery J.P., Hubbard E.M., Bernier R., Wiersema J.R., Raymaekers R., Pineda J.A. Developmental changes in mu suppression to observed and executed actions in autism spectrum disorders. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2013;8(3):300–304. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsr097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  170. Ohnishi T., Moriguchi Y., Matsuda H., Mori T., Hirakata M., Imabayashi E. The neural network for the mirror system and mentalizing in normally developed children: an fMRI study. Neuroreport. 2004;15(9):1483–1487. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000127464.17770.1f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  171. Oostenbroek J., Suddendorf T., Nielsen M., Redshaw J., Kennedy-Costantini S., Davis J. Comprehensive longitudinal study challenges the existence of neonatal imitation in humans. Curr. Biol. 2016;26(10):1334–1338. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.03.047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  172. Over H., Carpenter M. Priming third‐party ostracism increases affiliative imitation in children. Dev. Sci. 2009;12(3):F1–F8. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00820.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  173. Over H., Carpenter M. Putting the social into social learning: explaining both selectivity and fidelity in children’s copying behavior. J. Comp. Psychol. 2012;126(2):182. doi: 10.1037/a0024555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  174. Over H., Carpenter M. The social side of imitation. Child Dev. Perspect. 2013;7(1):6–11. [Google Scholar]
  175. Paus T., Toro R., Leonard G., Lerner J.V., Lerner R.M., Perron M. Morphological properties of the action-observation cortical network in adolescents with low and high resistance to peer influence. Soc. Neurosci. 2008;3(3–4):303–316. doi: 10.1080/17470910701563558. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  176. Paus T., Keshavan M., Giedd J.N. Why do many psychiatric disorders emerge during adolescence? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2008;9:947. doi: 10.1038/nrn2513. Retrieved from. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  177. Pearce E., Launay J., Dunbar R.I.M. The ice-breaker effect: singing mediates fast social bonding. Open Science. 2015;2(10):150221. doi: 10.1098/rsos.150221. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  178. Pokorny J.J., Hatt N.V., Colombi C., Vivanti G., Rogers S.J., Rivera S.M. The action observation system when observing hand actions in autism and typical development. Autism Res. 2015;8(3):284–296. doi: 10.1002/aur.1445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  179. Pratt M., Singer M., Kanat-Maymon Y., Feldman R. Infant negative reactivity defines the effects of parent–child synchrony on physiological and behavioral regulation of social stress. Dev. Psychopathol. 2015;27(4pt1):1191–1204. doi: 10.1017/S0954579415000760. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  180. Provasi J., Anderson D.I., Barbu-Roth M. Rhythm perception, production, and synchronization during the perinatal period. Front. Psychol. 2014;5:1048. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  181. Quadrelli E., Turati C. Origins and development of mirroring mechanisms: a neuroconstructivist framework. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 2015;34(1):6–23. doi: 10.1111/bjdp.12110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  182. Rabinowitch T.-C., Knafo-Noam A. Synchronous rhythmic interaction enhances children’s perceived similarity and closeness towards each other. PLoS One. 2015;10(4):e0120878. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0120878. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  183. Rabinowitch T.-C., Meltzoff A.N. Joint rhythmic movement increases 4-year-old children’s prosocial sharing and fairness toward peers. Front. Psychol. 2017;8:1050. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  184. Rabinowitch T.-C., Meltzoff A.N. Synchronized movement experience enhances peer cooperation in preschool children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2017;160:21–32. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  185. Ramsey R. What are reaction time indices of automatic imitation measuring? Conscious. Cogn. 2018;65:240–254. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2018.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  186. Rauchbauer B., Majdandžić J., Hummer A., Windischberger C., Lamm C. Distinct neural processes are engaged in the modulation of mimicry by social group-membership and emotional expressions. Cortex. 2015;70:49–67. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  187. Rauchbauer B., Majdandžić J., Stieger S., Lamm C. The modulation of mimicry by ethnic group-membership and emotional expressions. PLoS One. 2016;11(8):e0161064. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0161064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  188. Rauchbauer B., Pfabigan D.M., Lamm C. Event-related potentials of automatic imitation are modulated by ethnicity during stimulus processing, but not during motor execution. Sci. Rep. 2018;8 doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-30926-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  189. Rauchbauer B., Dunbar R.I.M., Lamm C. Being mimicked affects inhibitory mechanisms of imitation. Acta Psychol. 2020;209 doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2020.103132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  190. Ray E., Heyes C. Imitation in infancy: the wealth of the stimulus. Dev. Sci. 2011;14(1):92–105. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00961.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  191. Rayson H., Bonaiuto J.J., Ferrari P.F., Murray L. Mu desynchronization during observation and execution of facial expressions in 30-month-old children. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2016;19:279–287. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2016.05.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  192. Reddish P., Fischer R., Bulbulia J. Let’s dance together: synchrony, shared intentionality and cooperation. PLoS One. 2013;8(8) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0071182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  193. Reddish P., Bulbulia J., Fischer R. Does synchrony promote generalized prosociality? Religion Brain Behav. 2014;4(1):3–19. [Google Scholar]
  194. Reddish P., Tong E.M.W., Jong J., Lanman J.A., Whitehouse H. Collective synchrony increases prosociality towards non‐performers and outgroup members. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 2016;55(4):722–738. doi: 10.1111/bjso.12165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  195. Reid V.M., Striano T., Iacoboni M. Neural correlates of dyadic interaction during infancy. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2011;1(2):124–130. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2011.01.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  196. Reindl V., Gerloff C., Scharke W., Konrad K. Brain-to-brain synchrony in parent-child dyads and the relationship with emotion regulation revealed by fNIRS-based hyperscanning. NeuroImage. 2018;178:493–502. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.05.060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  197. Rizzolatti G., Craighero L. The mirror neuron system. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2004;27(1):169–192. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  198. Rizzolatti G., Fabbri-Destro M. The mirror system and its role in social cognition. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2008;18(2):179–184. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2008.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  199. Ruysschaert L., Warreyn P., Wiersema J.R., Metin B., Roeyers H. Neural mirroring during the observation of live and video actions in infants. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2013;124(9):1765–1770. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2013.04.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  200. Ruysschaert L., Warreyn P., Wiersema J.R., Oostra A., Roeyers H. Exploring the role of neural mirroring in children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. 2014;7(2):197–206. doi: 10.1002/aur.1339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  201. Saby J.N., Marshall P.J., Smythe R., Bouquet C.A., Comalli C.E. An investigation of the determinants of motor contagion in preschool children. Acta Psychol. 2011;138(1):231–236. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.06.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  202. Saby J.N., Marshall P.J., Meltzoff A.N. Neural correlates of being imitated: an EEG study in preverbal infants. Soc. Neurosci. 2012;7(6):650–661. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2012.691429. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  203. Saby J.N., Meltzoff A.N., Marshall P.J. Infants’ somatotopic neural responses to seeing human actions: I’ve got you under my skin. PLoS One. 2013;8(10) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0077905. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  204. Salvia E., Süß M., Tivadar R., Harkness S., Grosbras M.-H. Mirror neurons system engagement in late adolescents and adults while viewing emotional gestures. Front. Psychol. 2016;7:1099. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01099. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  205. Santiesteban I., Banissy M.J., Catmur C., Bird G. Enhancing social ability by stimulating right temporoparietal junction. Curr. Biol. 2012;22(23):2274–2277. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.10.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  206. Sebastian C., Viding E., Williams K.D., Blakemore S.-J. Social brain development and the affective consequences of ostracism in adolescence. Brain Cogn. 2010;72(1):134–145. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.06.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  207. Shaw D.J., Grosbras M.-H., Leonard G., Pike G.B., Paus T. Development of functional connectivity during adolescence: a longitudinal study using an action–observation paradigm. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2011;23(12):3713–3724. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  208. Shaw D.J., Grosbras M.-H., Leonard G., Pike G.B., Paus T. Development of the action observation network during early adolescence: a longitudinal study. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2011;7(1):64–80. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  209. Shimada S., Hiraki K. Infant’s brain responses to live and televised action. NeuroImage. 2006;32(2):930–939. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.03.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  210. Simpson A., Carroll D.J. What’s so special about verbal imitation? Investigating the effect of modality on automaticity in children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2014;121:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  211. Somogyi E., Esseily R. Mimicry enhances observational learning in 16-Month-Old infants. PLoS One. 2014;9(12) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0113695. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  212. Southgate V., Johnson M.H., Osborne T., Csibra G. Predictive motor activation during action observation in human infants. Biol. Lett. 2009;5(6):769–772. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2009.0474. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  213. Sowden S., Catmur C. The role of the right temporoparietal junction in the control of imitation. Cereb. Cortex. 2013;25(4):1107–1113. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht306. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  214. Sowden Sophie, Catmur C. The role of the right temporoparietal junction in the control of imitation. Cereb. Cortex. 2015;25(4):1107–1113. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht306. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  215. Sparenberg P., Topolinski S., Springer A., Prinz W. Minimal mimicry: mere effector matching induces preference. Brain Cogn. 2012;80(3):291–300. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2012.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  216. Stapel J.C., Hunnius S., van Elk M., Bekkering H. ). Motor activation during observation of unusual versus ordinary actions in infancy. Soc. Neurosci. 2010;5(5–6):451–460. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2010.490667. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  217. Stephens G.J., Silbert L.J., Hasson U. Speaker–listener neural coupling underlies successful communication. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2010;107(32):14425–14430. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1008662107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  218. Su W.-C., Culotta M.L., Hoffman M.D., Trost S.L., Pelphrey K.A., Tsuzuki D., Bhat A.N. Developmental Differences in Cortical Activation During Action Observation, Action Execution and Interpersonal Synchrony: An fNIRS Study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2020 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00057. https://www.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fnhum.2020.00057 Retrieved from. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  219. Tarr B., Launay J., Dunbar R.I.M. Music and social bonding: “self-other” merging and neurohormonal mechanisms. Front. Psychol. 2014 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01096. https://www.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01096 Retrieved from. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  220. Tarr B., Launay J., Cohen E., Dunbar R. Synchrony and exertion during dance independently raise pain threshold and encourage social bonding. Biol. Lett. 2015;11(10):20150767. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2015.0767. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  221. Tarr B., Launay J., Dunbar R.I.M. Silent disco: dancing in synchrony leads to elevated pain thresholds and social closeness. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2016;37(5):343–349. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.02.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  222. Toppi J., Borghini G., Petti M., He E.J., De Giusti V., He B. Investigating cooperative behavior in ecological settings: an EEG hyperscanning study. PLoS One. 2016;11(4):e0154236. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0154236. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  223. Tunçgenç B., Cohen E. Movement synchrony forges social bonds across group divides. Front. Psychol. 2016;7:782. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00782. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  224. Tunçgenç B., Cohen E. Interpersonal movement synchrony facilitates pro‐social behavior in children’s peer‐play. Dev. Sci. 2018;21(1) doi: 10.1111/desc.12505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  225. Tunçgenç B., Cohen E., Fawcett C. Rock with me: the role of movement synchrony in infants’ social and nonsocial choices. Child Dev. 2015;86(3):976–984. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12354. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  226. Upshaw M.B., Bernier R.A., Sommerville J.A. Infants’ grip strength predicts mu rhythm attenuation during observation of lifting actions with weighted blocks. Dev. Sci. 2016;19(2):195–207. doi: 10.1111/desc.12308. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  227. Vacaru S.V., van Schaik J.E., Hunnius S. The modulation of facial mimicry by attachment tendencies and their underlying affiliation motives in 3-year-olds: an EMG study. PLoS One. 2019;14(7) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0218676. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  228. Valdesolo P., DeSteno D. Synchrony and the social tuning of compassion. Emotion. 2011;11(2):262. doi: 10.1037/a0021302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  229. Valdesolo P., Ouyang J., DeSteno D. The rhythm of joint action: synchrony promotes cooperative ability. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2010;46(4):693–695. [Google Scholar]
  230. Van Baaren R.B., Holland R.W., Kawakami K., Van Knippenberg A. Mimicry and prosocial behavior. Psychol. Sci. 2004;15(1):71–74. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01501012.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  231. Van Baaren R., Janssen L., Chartrand T.L., Dijksterhuis A. Where is the love? The social aspects of mimicry. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond., B, Biol. Sci. 2009;364(1528):2381–2389. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  232. van Elk M., van Schie H.T., Hunnius S., Vesper C., Bekkering H. You’ll never crawl alone: neurophysiological evidence for experience-dependent motor resonance in infancy. NeuroImage. 2008;43(4):808–814. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.07.057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  233. van Oosten J.M.F., Vandenbosch L. Sexy online self-presentation on social network sites and the willingness to engage in sexting: a comparison of gender and age. J. Adolesc. 2017;54:42–50. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2016.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  234. van Schaik J.E., Hunnius S. Little chameleons: the development of social mimicry during early childhood. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2016;147:71–81. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2016.03.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  235. van Schaik J.E., Endedijk H.M., Stapel J.C., Hunnius S. Young children’s motor interference is influenced by novel group membership. Front. Psychol. 2016;7:321. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00321. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  236. Van Schaik J.E., Hunnius S. Modulating mimicry: exploring the roles of inhibitory control and social understanding in 5-year-olds’ behavioral mimicry. PLoS One. 2018;13(3) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0194102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  237. Vink R., Wijnants M.L., Cillessen A.H.N., Bosman A.M.T. Cooperative learning and interpersonal synchrony. Nonlinear Dynamics Psychol. Life Sci. 2017;21(2):189–215. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  238. Virji-Babul N., Rose A., Moiseeva N., Makan N. Neural correlates of action understanding in infants: influence of motor experience. Brain Behav. 2012;2(3):237–242. doi: 10.1002/brb3.50. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  239. Virji‐Babul N., Rose A., Moiseeva N., Makan N. Neural correlates of action understanding in infants: influence of motor experience. Brain Behav. 2012;2(3):237–242. doi: 10.1002/brb3.50. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  240. Wadsworth H.M., Maximo J.O., Lemelman A.R., Clayton K., Sivaraman S., Deshpande H.D. The Action Imitation network and motor imitation in children and adolescents with autism. Neuroscience. 2017;343:147–156. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  241. Wang Y., Hamilton A.F.de C. Social top-down response modulation (STORM): a model of the control of mimicry in social interaction. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2012;6:160. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00153. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  242. Wang Y., Newport R., Hamilton A.F.D.C. Eye contact enhances mimicry of intransitive hand movements. Biol. Lett. 2011;7(1):7–10. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2010.0279. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  243. Warreyn P., Ruysschaert L., Wiersema J.R., Handl A., Pattyn G., Roeyers H. Infants’ mu suppression during the observation of real and mimicked goal‐directed actions. Dev. Sci. 2013;16(2):173–185. doi: 10.1111/desc.12014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  244. Watson-Jones R.E., Whitehouse H., Legare C.H. In-group ostracism increases high-fidelity imitation in early childhood. Psychol. Sci. 2016;27(1):34–42. doi: 10.1177/0956797615607205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  245. Weinstein D., Launay J., Pearce E., Dunbar R.I.M., Stewart L. Singing and social bonding: changes in connectivity and pain threshold as a function of group size. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2016;37(2):152–158. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2015.10.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  246. Wiltermuth S.S., Heath C. Synchrony and cooperation. Psychol. Sci. 2009;20(1):1–5. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02253.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  247. Xavier J., Magnat J., Sherman A., Gauthier S., Cohen D., Chaby L. A developmental and clinical perspective of rhythmic interpersonal coordination: from mimicry toward the interconnection of minds. J. Physiol. 2016;110(4):420–426. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2017.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  248. Xavier J., Gauthier S., Cohen D., Zahoui M., Chetouani M., Villa F. Interpersonal synchronization, motor coordination, and control are impaired during a dynamic imitation task in children with autism Spectrum disorder. Front. Psychol. 2018;9:1467. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01467. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  249. Yabar Y., Johnston L., Miles L., Peace V. Implicit behavioral mimicry: investigating the impact of group membership. J. Nonverbal Behav. 2006;30(3):97–113. [Google Scholar]
  250. Yoo K.H., Cannon E.N., Thorpe S.G., Fox N.A. Desynchronization in EEG during perception of means-end actions and relations with infants’ grasping skill. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 2015;34(1):24–37. doi: 10.1111/bjdp.12115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  251. Yu Y., Kushnir T. Social context effects in 2-and 4-year-olds’ selective versus faithful imitation. Dev. Psychol. 2014;50(3):922. doi: 10.1037/a0034242. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  252. Yun K., Watanabe K., Shimojo S. Interpersonal body and neural synchronization as a marker of implicit social interaction. Sci. Rep. 2012;2(1) doi: 10.1038/srep00959. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  253. Zmyj N., Daum M.M., Prinz W., Nielsen M., Aschersleben G. Fourteen-month-Olds’ imitation of differently aged models. Infant Child Dev. 2011;21(3):250–266. doi: 10.1002/icd.750. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  254. Zmyj N., Aschersleben G., Prinz W., Daum M. The peer model advantage in infants’ imitation of familiar gestures performed by differently aged models. Front. Psychol. 2012;3 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00252. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews are provided here courtesy of Elsevier

RESOURCES