Skip to main content
PLOS Computational Biology logoLink to PLOS Computational Biology
. 2020 Jul 30;16(7):e1008075. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1008075

Electrical coupling controls dimensionality and chaotic firing of inferior olive neurons

Huu Hoang 1,*, Eric J Lang 2, Yoshito Hirata 3,4,5, Isao T Tokuda 6, Kazuyuki Aihara 3,5, Keisuke Toyama 1, Mitsuo Kawato 1,7,*, Nicolas Schweighofer 8,*
Editor: Chris De Zeeuw9
PMCID: PMC7419012  PMID: 32730255

Abstract

We previously proposed, on theoretical grounds, that the cerebellum must regulate the dimensionality of its neuronal activity during motor learning and control to cope with the low firing frequency of inferior olive neurons, which form one of two major inputs to the cerebellar cortex. Such dimensionality regulation is possible via modulation of electrical coupling through the gap junctions between inferior olive neurons by inhibitory GABAergic synapses. In addition, we previously showed in simulations that intermediate coupling strengths induce chaotic firing of inferior olive neurons and increase their information carrying capacity. However, there is no in vivo experimental data supporting these two theoretical predictions. Here, we computed the levels of synchrony, dimensionality, and chaos of the inferior olive code by analyzing in vivo recordings of Purkinje cell complex spike activity in three different coupling conditions: carbenoxolone (gap junctions blocker), control, and picrotoxin (GABA-A receptor antagonist). To examine the effect of electrical coupling on dimensionality and chaotic dynamics, we first determined the physiological range of effective coupling strengths between inferior olive neurons in the three conditions using a combination of a biophysical network model of the inferior olive and a novel Bayesian model averaging approach. We found that effective coupling co-varied with synchrony and was inversely related to the dimensionality of inferior olive firing dynamics, as measured via a principal component analysis of the spike trains in each condition. Furthermore, for both the model and the data, we found an inverted U-shaped relationship between coupling strengths and complexity entropy, a measure of chaos for spiking neural data. These results are consistent with our hypothesis according to which electrical coupling regulates the dimensionality and the complexity in the inferior olive neurons in order to optimize both motor learning and control of high dimensional motor systems by the cerebellum.

Author summary

Computational theory suggests that the cerebellum must decrease the dimensionality of its neuronal activity to learn and control high dimensional motor systems effectively, while being constrained by the low firing frequency of inferior olive neurons, one of the two major source of input signals to the cerebellum. We previously proposed that the cerebellum adaptively controls the dimensionality of inferior olive firing by adjusting the level of synchrony and that such control is made possible by modulating the electrical coupling strength between inferior olive neurons. Here, we developed a novel method that uses a biophysical model of the inferior olive to accurately estimate the effective coupling strengths between inferior olive neurons from in vivo recordings of spike activity in three different coupling conditions. We found that high coupling strengths induce synchronous firing and decrease the dimensionality of inferior olive firing dynamics. In contrast, intermediate coupling strengths lead to chaotic firing and increase the dimensionality of the firing dynamics. Thus, electrical coupling is a feasible mechanism to control dimensionality and chaotic firing of inferior olive neurons. In sum, our results provide insights into possible mechanisms underlying cerebellar function and, in general, a biologically plausible framework to control the dimensionality of neural coding.

Introduction

The cerebellum plays important roles in motor learning and motor control, although how it performs these roles is still unclear. In particular, the role of the inferior olive (IO) continues to be debated. On one hand, there is evidence that the olivo-cerebellar system conveys error signals into the cerebellum [14] and induces plasticity in parallel fiber-Purkinje cell synapses (e.g., [58]). Such error-driven plasticity is a central tenet of the original motor learning theory of the cerebellum [810], as it can allow learning of internal models for motor control [1116]. On the other hand, there is also evidence that olivo-cerebellar activity has a direct role in generating ongoing motor commands because of its ability to dynamically generate large ensembles of synchronously active Purkinje cells during movement that can affect downstream motor systems [1720]. Moreover, it has been shown that spontaneous olivo-cerebellar activity can directly influence ongoing spiking in cerebellar nuclear cells, which relay motor commands produced by the cerebellar cortex [21,22].

However, a fundamental and outstanding question that needs to be addressed by both theories is: how does the olivo-cerebellar system convey information, whether for learning or for controlling the high dimensional and nonlinear motor systems that generate movements, despite the low-firing rates of inferior olive neurons (typically ~1 Hz). Indeed, olivary neurons discharge at most one or two times during a typical movement [23]. For motor learning, compared to an artificial learning machine that can use high frequency errors [24], such a low firing rate significantly decreases the error transmission capability of the system, and thus its learning efficiency. Similarly, for motor control, the low firing rate presents a problem for the direct participation of the olivo-cerebellar system in the generation of high frequency and high dimensional motor commands across multiple muscles and joints.

An answer to this question is suggested by the unusual organization of the IO, in which neurons form the strongest electrically coupled neuronal network in the adult mammalian brain [2529]. This coupling underlines synchronization of complex spike activity in Purkinje cells [3033]. Moreover, the patterns and extent of synchronization are dynamically controlled by two types of synaptic inputs to the IO: GABAergic synapses whose activity reduces synchrony [33,34] and excitatory synapses whose activity alters the distribution of synchrony and enhances IO coupling of weakly coupled neurons [3537].

We have previously proposed that the capacity of the olivo-cerebellar system to adaptively control the dimensionality of the IO firing dynamics, defined as the minimal dimension required to provide a precise description of the neural dynamics, via modulation of electrical synapses between IO neurons is central to answering the above question [3840]. According to this idea, when coupling is high, IO synchrony is high, and groups of related neurons in the olivo-cerebellar system behave, in the limit, as a single-neuron chain, decreasing the dimensionality of the IO firing dynamics to one. For motor control, synchronous IO signals would induce synchronous activation of Purkinje cell ensembles, which, in turn, would tune the downstream systems to facilitate the initiation and coordination of fast and crude movements [19]. For motor learning, high synchrony in the early stages of learning induces strong plasticity at the parallel-fiber-Purkinje-cell synapses of large numbers of Purkinje cells simultaneously, resulting in fast but crude learning [39,40]. In contrast, in silico computer simulations show that chaotic resonance occurs when coupling is decreased, leading to a decrease in synchrony [4145]. Chaotic resonance can thereby allow both sophisticated learning and control, either for the final subtle corrections to optimize movements or for control of fine movements [45,46].

Here, we re-analyzed in vivo recordings of complex spikes recorded simultaneously from arrays of Purkinje cells [30,33,34] under three pharmacologically induced coupling conditions (low, control, high) to study the effect of coupling on the dimensionality of the IO code and on the induction of chaotic resonance. The low coupling condition was generated by intra-IO injection of the gap junction blocker carbenoxolone (CBX), which lowers complex spike synchrony [30], whereas the presumed high coupling condition was generated by intra-IO injection of the GABA-A blocker picrotoxin (PIX), which increases complex spike synchrony [33,34]. In the present study, we tested our two predictions that 1) increasing the synchrony level, via increased electrical coupling between IO neurons, decreases the dimensionality of IO firing dynamics and 2) intermediate coupling induces chaotic spiking.

Results

Estimation of the effective coupling between IO neurons in vivo

To examine the effect of electrical coupling on the dimensionality and chaotic dynamics of the IO code, we first need to determine the physiological range of effective coupling strengths between IO neurons under in vivo conditions. Direct quantitative measurement of electrical coupling between IO neurons has been obtained in slice preparations [4749]; however, such measurements remain technically impossible in vivo. We therefore used an indirect approach. Purkinje cell complex spikes, as opposed to simple spikes, bear a one-to-one relationship to IO discharges. Thus, complex spikes can be used as a proxy for IO spikes (see S1 Fig for examples of complex spike recordings in these three conditions; see Methods for experimental procedures). Note that an IO cell discharge can lead to one or several axonic spikes occurring with inter-spike intervals on the order of a millisecond [50]. Each such discharge, whether composed of one or several spikes, leads to a single complex spike in the Purkinje cells to which the IO cell projects. In this paper we consider each such IO discharge as a single 'spike' event. In vivo complex spike activity was compared with simulated activity generated by a biophysical model of a network of coupled IO neurons, whose parameters were estimated via a Bayesian method [51] that we modified by using Bayesian model-averaging to improve the robustness of its estimation of the coupling parameters. The coupling parameters that produced the spatiotemporal firing patterns that best matched those of the experimental data were used as the estimates (see below for details).

The biophysical IO model was adapted from a model that we previously developed to investigate the effect of PIX in modifying coupling via its action of blocking GABA inhibitory synapses. This model modified the original IO model [43,52] to include the modulation of electrical conductance between IO cells via inhibitory inputs from deep cerebellar nuclear cells (for review of IO anatomy and function, see [53]). Briefly, in the model, each IO neuron comprises a soma, a main dendrite, and four dendritic spine compartments, with each compartment having distinct ionic conductances. Most notably, the dendritic compartment has a high threshold calcium conductance and a calcium-activated potassium conductance, which are responsible for the after-depolarization and after-hyperpolarization sequence that follows each sodium spike and for the low firing rates of IO neurons [52,54,55]. Each neuron is coupled to its neighbors via electrical coupling conductances between the spine compartments. An inhibitory synaptic conductance in the spine compartment modulates the effective coupling strength (for a description of the model, see Methods for details and [51,56]; the code of the model is available for download, see Code Availability). In the present study, we increased chaotic dynamics by increasing the sodium conductance, as tests showed that these changes better accounted for the actual IO firing properties (see Methods for details).

Using the model, it is possible to derive a theoretical "effective" electrical coupling conductance geff as a function of the axial conductance of the spines gs, the electrical coupling conductance gc, and the GABAergic synaptic conductance gi (see [57] and Methods for details). Estimates of gc and gi, were obtained by comparing sixty-seven spatiotemporal features–including firing rates, local variation [58], minimal distance [57], auto-correlograms and cross-correlograms–of the model's spike activity to those of the complex spike data sets for different values of synaptic noise input frequencies (gs was held constant, see Methods for details). A final estimate of gi and gc for each condition (CBX, CON, and PIX) was obtained using Bayesian averaging of the model estimates for the different synaptic noise levels, weighted in proportion to the goodness-of-fit of the model for each noise level (see S3 Fig and S4 Fig, and Methods for details). Note that for this analysis, we used data from in vivo neurons whose activity was clearly affected by the drug treatments, based on changes in their firing rate from control levels (see S2 Fig and Methods for details). This was done because the lack of effect in some cells likely reflects the experimental limitation that the drug injection was localized to one part of the IO, whereas climbing fibers to the recording array arise from multiple IO regions (see [30] for results and discussion of this issue).

In order to compare the coupling across all three conditions, the two CON groups (CON–CBX and CON–PIX) were combined. As expected, the estimated inhibitory conductance gi in the CON condition (Fig 1A– 1.15 ± 0.21 mS/cm2, n = 100 neurons; all results reported as mean ± std) was significantly higher than in the PIX condition (0.72 ± 0.3 mS/cm2, n = 47 neurons; PIX vs CON: p < 0.0001). However, gi in the CBX condition (1.02 ± 0.13 mS/cm2, n = 53 neurons) was also significantly smaller than in both its own CON condition (1.21 ± 0.21 mS/cm2, CBX vs CON–CBX, p < 0.001) and the combined CON condition (CBX vs CON: p < 0.001) probably due to possible effects of CBX on inhibitory synapses [59]. Similarly, the estimated gap-junctional conductance gc in the CBX condition (Fig 1B– 0.88 ± 0.22 mS/cm2) was significantly smaller than in the CON condition (1.19 ± 0.25 mS/cm2, CBX vs CON: p < 0.0001), but there was no significant difference between the PIX (1.16 ± 0.21 mS/cm2) and CON conditions (PIX vs PIX–CON, p = 0.2; PIX vs CON: p = 0.5). As a result of these changes in gi and gc, the estimated effective coupling strength, geff, differed across the three conditions (one-way ANOVA: p < 0.0001). geff was smallest for the CBX condition (Fig 1Cgeff = 0.030 ± 0.002 mS/cm2, CBX–CON: p < 0.0001), intermediate for the CON condition (geff = 0.032 ± 0.002 mS/cm2) and largest for the PIX condition (geff = 0.036 ± 0.003 mS/cm2, PIX–CON: p < 0.0001).

Fig 1. Estimation of conductance and coupling coefficients in the three experimental conditions.

Fig 1

A-B: Values of the GABAergic synaptic conductance gi (A) and electrical coupling conductance gc (B) estimates for the three experimental conditions: carbenoxolone (CBX, green box), control (CON, black box) and picrotoxin (PIX, red box). C: The effective coupling coefficient geff computed from Eq (1) for the three conditions. D: The coupling coefficient (CC) estimated via simulations for the three conditions. Each boxplot shows white line as the mean, dark region as 95% CIs and light region as 1 std. Asterisks represent significance levels: ns p > 0.05, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001.

We confirmed these results without pooling the two CON groups. The best fit of the model to the data indicated PIX and CBX reduced mean gi and gc approximately 30% and 25% from their respective CON values, consistent with their known pharmacological effects. Specifically, gi was significantly decreased in the PIX condition from its CON value (1.06 ± 0.18 mS/cm2, CON–PIX vs PIX, p < 0.0001). Similarly, gc in the CBX condition was significantly smaller than in its CON condition (1.18 ± 0.28 mS/cm2, CON–CBX vs CBX, p < 0.0001).

Next, we examined whether the estimates of effective coupling strength, geff, were biologically realistic by computing the coupling coefficients (CCs) as the average ratio of the change in steady state membrane potentials of a master cell and its four neighboring cells in response to a current step (see S5 Fig and Methods for details). The calculated CCs for our data were similar to in vitro values [49]. As expected, CC was smaller in the CBX condition (Fig 1DCC = 0.008 ± 0.002, CBX vs CON: p < 0.0001) and larger in the PIX condition (CC = 0.019 ± 0.006, PIX vs CON: p < 0.0001) than in the CON condition (CC = 0.012 ± 0.003).

The estimated gi and gc parameters were then used to generate simulated spike trains under all three conditions. In each case, the spike trains were comparable to those of the recorded complex-spike activity (see Fig 2A–2C). Quantitatively, firing rates (model: 0.39 ± 0.30 and 1.43 ± 0.76 and 2.21 ± 0.53; data: 0.42 ± 0.27 and 1.34 ± 0.72 and 2.83 ± 1.20 for CBX, CON and PIX conditions, respectively) and cross-correlations (model: 0.01 ± 0.01 and 0.04 ± 0.03 and 0.11 ± 0.04; data: 0.02 ± 0.01 and 0.06 ± 0.02 and 0.18 ± 0.05; 10 ms time bin) increased in the PIX and decreased in the CBX condition. In contrast, auto-correlations (model: 0.69 ± 0.20 and 0.48 ± 0.17 and 0.35 ± 0.09; data: 0.75 ± 0.13 and 0.51 ± 0.17 and 0.32 ± 0.12; 50 ms time bin) showed the opposite change, being lower in the PIX and higher in the CBX condition (Fig 2D and 2E). The changes in those spike train measures reflect the changes in the firing dynamics, which became more synchronous across the IO neuronal ensemble under the PIX than the CON condition, and became less so under the CBX condition. The strong agreement of these measures between the experimental and model data confirms the goodness-of-fit of the model in all three data conditions.

Fig 2. Similarity between IO firing for model and data.

Fig 2

A–C: Raster plots of ten representative IO neurons of the model and the experimental complex spike data of three animals in the three conditions. Each row of tick marks represents the activity of a single neuron. A. Carbenoxolone (animal #1, irregular spiking). B. Control (animal #7, oscillatory spiking) C. Picrotoxin (animal #9, highly synchronous spiking). D–E: Three major spatiotemporal features extracted from the spike trains–firing rates, auto-correlations and cross-correlations–in the three data conditions of the model (D) and the data (E). FR: firing rates. ACor: auto-correlation. CCor: cross-correlation. The ordinates of D–E are scaled so that the mean value of the CON is 1.

Dimensionality is inversely related to synchrony and effective coupling levels

Next, we examine the relationship between dimensionality and synchrony in an identical time bin of those two measures. The dimensionality d is defined as the minimal number of principal components accounting for approximately 90% of the variability in the covariance data (see Eq 5 in Methods), as proposed by [60]. We extracted the average firing rates of neurons in 50-second long periods and applied principal component analysis (PCA) to compute the covariance of these firing rate vectors for each animal. d values were then normalized by the number of neurons simultaneously recorded (see S6 Fig and Methods for details). Synchrony was measured by calculating the zero-lag cross-correlation coefficient of the two spike trains (Eq 4). In the literature, time bins of 1–10 ms have been commonly used [22,6163] (see [22] for a detailed justification of this range from the perspective of the impact on cerebellar nuclear cells). Thus, in our study, we chose a time bin of 10 ms for computing both the synchrony and the dimensionality of Purkinje complex spike activity (see Methods for details).

For the present dataset, and consistent with the parent datasets [30,33,34], synchrony levels increased three-fold in the PIX condition (synchrony = 0.186 ± 0.05, PIX vs CON, p = 0.012) and decreased about three-fold in the CBX condition (synchrony = 0.018 ± 0.009, CBX vs CON, p = 0.0015) compared to the CON condition (synchrony = 0.061 ± 0.015)–Fig 3A. We then assessed whether the dimensionality changed with the drug condition. As predicted, d differed across the three conditions (one-way ANOVA, p = 0.02), being smaller in the PIX condition (d = 0.15 ± 0.06, PIX vs CON, p = 0.07) and larger in the CBX condition (d = 0.40 ± 0.16, CBX vs CON, p = 0.07) than in the CON condition (d = 0.25 ± 0.11)–Fig 3B. Although change in the dimensionality was large between conditions (approximately 2-fold from CBX to CON and from CON to PIX), there was no statistical difference between both the PIX and CBX conditions with the control group. This is probably due to the small number of samples as well as the large variance in the dimensionality across the animals in each condition.

Fig 3. The synchrony and the dimensionality in IO firings moderated by effective coupling.

Fig 3

A-B: The synchrony (A) and the dimensionality (B) for each of the three data conditions. Significance level, ns p > 0.05, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01. C: the change of synchrony by drugs (either CBX–green bars, or PIX–red bars, left axis) is coupled with change in the dimensionality (right axis) compared with the control level. Each connected pair of data points is from one animal. D–E: The synchrony (D) and the dimensionality (E) as functions of effective coupling strength averaged for selected neurons in individual animals confirming that effective coupling is a control parameter to optimize the synchrony and thus the dimensionality of IO firings. Each type of symbol represents the data of an individual animal. The cyan solid lines show results of the linear regression models and shaded regions are of 95% CIs.

The relationship between changes in synchrony and dimensionality was consistent and was seen in each animal (Fig 3C). Relative to CON, a 2-fold decrease of synchrony in the CBX condition was associated with an ~70% increase in d, whereas a 3-fold increase of synchrony in the PIX condition was associated with a 40% reduction in dimensionality. Furthermore, we divided the control periods of the complex spike data (n = 9 animals) into short time segments, to assess the relationship between synchrony level and dimensionality for levels that are within the same range as occurs in the awake animal [63]. Synchrony levels fluctuated between these segments and these fluctuations were negatively correlated with changes in the dimensionality (S7 Fig). Thus, variations in synchrony within physiological range are associated with significant changes in dimensionality.

Next, we quantitatively investigated the effect of effective coupling on synchrony and dimensionality. As expected, there was a positive correlation (r = 0.76; p = 0.0002, Fig 3D) between synchrony and effective coupling averaged for each animal. Note that we found an outlier (animal #9 in the PIX condition, the red diamond in Fig 3D) by computing Cook’s distances with a threshold of five times the mean value [64]. But even when that outlier was removed, the correlation was still significant (r = 0.75; p = 0.0004). In addition, there was a negative correlation between geff and d (r = -0.5; p = 0.03, no outlier detected with the same criteria above, Fig 3E). These results support our hypothesis that synchronization is a feasible mechanism for dimensionality reduction in IO neurons and that effective coupling is a control parameter that the IO uses to optimize the dimensionality of the olivo-cerebellar system.

Inverted U-shaped relationship between complexity entropy and effective coupling

We next addressed the question of whether intermediate, physiological coupling strengths maximize the chaotic level of IO activity. Lyapunov exponents quantify the sensitivity of a dynamical system to the initial conditions [65,66], and are thus often used as indicators of chaos. However, methods to compute Lyapunov exponents from time series data [67,68] are not applicable to our spike data sets, because the computation requires access to continuous variables. We therefore computed the complexity entropy, which is applicable to spike train data and approximates the largest Lyapunov exponents in simulations of IO neurons [69,70] (see S8 Fig and Methods for details).

For both the simulated IO spike and the experimental complex spike data sets, we investigated whether the relationship between complexity entropy and effective coupling formed an inverted U-shape, as previously shown in simulations [43,45]. For each of the experimental IO neurons, we computed the complexity entropy from the simulated spike data that was generated with the estimated coupling values that best fit the data in terms of the PCA error (difference between experimental and simulated spike data in the PCA space, S4A Fig). For the IO model (Fig 4A), the second order model (regression model in Wilkinson notation [71]: entropy ~ 1 + geff + geff2, Bayesian information criterion (BIC): -1263.4) where entropy is the complexity entropy, had a negative coefficient for the second order term (mean ± sem, -157 ± 36), and better fit the simulated spikes in the three conditions than the first-order linear model (entropy ~ 1 + geff, BIC: -646.8; Log likelihood ratio test (LLR): p < 0.0001). For the IO data (Fig 4B), a mixed effect regression model analysis, with Animal as the random intercept accounting for repeated measures within the same animal, showed that the second order model (entropy ~ 1 + geff + geff2 + (1 | Animal), BIC: -1319.5), where (1 | Animal) is the random intercept, had a negative fixed-effect coefficient of the second order term (mean ± sem, -75 ± 30), and provided a better fit than the linear model (entropy ~ 1 + geff + (1 | Animal), BIC: -1318; LLR: p = 0.01). We further conducted a Gaussian-Process regression, which does not assume an explicit relationship between the coupling and the complexity entropy. The result also showed an inverted U-curve that peaks at around geff = 0.033 mS/cm2 for both the model and the data (S9 Fig). These results all indicate that intermediate coupling strengths induce chaotic behavior in both the model and the data. Note that the relatively small changes in the complexity entropy that we observed in the model and data correspond to large changes in the largest Lyapunov exponent λ1 (see Methods and S8B Fig), from synchronous and rhythmic firings (in both the model and the data, entropy = 0.21, λ1 = 5 bits/second) to chaotic firings (entropy = 0.24, λ1 = 40 bits/second).

Fig 4. Inverted U-shaped relationship of complexity versus effective coupling, model and data.

Fig 4

A-B: Complexity entropy versus effective coupling. Upper panel: chaotic levels measured by the complexity entropy of the spike data as a function of effective coupling strength for the model (A) and real IO neurons (B) confirming that moderate couplings induce chaos. Each value in (A) (open symbols) is given by the model neuron that best fits to the actual IO neuron in terms of the PCA error. The right ordinates of A-B represent the first Lyapunov exponents approximated from the simulation data (S8B Fig). Each type of symbol in (B) represents the data of an individual animal. The cyan solid lines indicate the second-order of linear model (A) and mixed-effects model (B) and shaded regions are of 95% CIs. Lower panel: spike trains of the representative neurons (located at dark arrows in the upper panel of Fig 4B) which show periodic and synchronous firings for either low or high couplings but exhibits chaotic firings for intermediate couplings.

Discussion

Estimation of the physiological electrical coupling between IO neurons from complex spike data

We developed a novel technique that combines computational modeling, Bayesian inference and model-averaging to estimate the effective coupling among IO neurons from rat in vivo complex spike data and to investigate the effects of changes in this coupling on the dynamics of olivo-cerebellar activity.

To estimate the effective coupling among IO neurons, we determined the gi and gc values that allowed our model to best match the experimental complex spike data in conditions of normal (CON), high (PIX), and low (CBX) synchrony levels, which are assumed to correspond to normal, high, and low coupling among IO neurons. These values were then used to calculate the effective coupling and coupling coefficients between IO neurons under these conditions. In addition, we adopted a Bayesian model-averaging approach to examine the effect of synaptic input strength on the effective coupling. As the result, we obtained an estimate of the physiological range over which the coupling between IO neurons may vary.

The validity of our methods to estimate coupling strengths is supported by several observations. First, the direction of the changes in gc and gi between the control and each drug condition determined by the model matched the known effects of the drugs. Specifically, PIX is a GABAa receptor antagonist and the model best simulated the complex spike patterns in PIX by decreasing gi. In contrast, CBX spiking patterns were best reproduced when gc and gi were both reduced. These changes match the pharmacology of CBX, which is generally known as a gap junction blocker, but also blocks GABAa receptors [59]. Second, reflecting these changes gc and gi, geff rose with PIX and fell with CBX, leading to increased and decreased coupling coefficients, respectively, which is again consistent with the observed experimental changes in complex spike synchrony caused by these drugs. Third, our coupling coefficient results agree with those of an in vitro slice study in which the effect of GABA on IO coupling coefficients was directly measured [49]. Specifically, baseline coupling coefficients in IO slices (CC = 0.021 ± 0.02, cf. S1 Table in [49]) closely match the values we obtained for the PIX condition (CC = 0.019 ± 0.006, Fig 1D), which is consistent with the fact that in both cases there is little to no GABAergic activity (the lack of spontaneous GABAergic activity in the slice was confirmed by the lack of effect of applying gabazine, a GABAa antagonist, on slice activity). Moreover, when GABAergic fibers were activated optogenetically in the study of [49] (CC = 0.012 ± 0.013), this created a situation analogous to the CON condition in our study (CC = 0.012 ± 0.003), because deep-cerebellar cells are spontaneously active in the anesthetized animal. In both studies, an approximate doubling of the coupling coefficients was found for the conditions where GABAergic activity was reduced or blocked. Thus our estimated coupling strengths values fall within the range of physiologically realistic values.

The validity of our approach also rests on the assumption that complex spike synchrony observed in the recordings is primarily due to the electrical coupling of IO neurons as opposed to some other source, such as correlated activity in afferents [72]. Indeed, in awake animals, synaptic input to the IO can limit the impact of gap junctional coupling within the IO [73]. Furthermore, the synchrony is reduced under isoflurane anesthesia compared to the awake state [20]. However, the evidence suggests that in our recordings, and probably under multiple physiological conditions, complex spike synchrony patterns largely reflect the effective coupling among IO neurons. We note that significant levels of complex spike synchrony remain after blocking GABAergic and/or glutamatergic afferents to the IO [3335]. Moreover, complex spike synchrony depends on gap junction coupling, as it is lost or greatly decreased by pharmacological block of the gap junctions and is absent in connexin36 knockout mice in which IO neurons are not coupled [30,32,74]. Thus, synchrony in spontaneous complex spike activity requires electrical coupling of IO neurons and can occur in the absence of IO afferent activity. While these experiments were obtained in anesthetized animals, similar patterns of synchrony are found for spontaneous complex spike activity in awake animals [63]. Thus, the relationship between synchrony and electrical coupling seems to be broadly valid. Of course, complex spike synchrony could be driven by highly synchronized afferent activity in certain situations. In fact, complex spike activity driven by electrical stimulation of the motor cortex does show higher levels of synchrony. However, even in this case, the spatial distribution of synchrony still matches the spontaneous distribution set by electrical coupling [34]. In sum, the basic patterns of complex spike synchrony seem to strongly reflect the coupling pattern within the IO, even in the face of highly correlated afferent activity.

Synchrony as a mechanism for controlling dimensionality

Our analysis of complex-spike data shows that controlling the effective coupling between IO neurons may be a mechanism for controlling the dimensionality of olivo-cerebellar activity. In particular, we found that increased electrical coupling between IO neurons decreased the dimensionality of IO firing dynamics. Dimensionality reduction has long been considered one of the core computations in the brain [7579]. Our study provides direct evidence that electrical coupling among neurons can control the dimensionality of the population activity by modulating the synchrony of the neural code. Quantitatively, the approximately two-fold reduction in dimensionality from the PIX to the CON condition was highly comparable to that of stimulus-evoked activity of cortical neurons under different stimulus conditions and in varied tasks [76,79]. We note, however, that additional mechanisms could work in parallel to effectively control the dimensionality, such as pruning of irrelevant inputs [80]. In the olivo-cerebellar system, in particular, climbing fiber-Purkinje-cell synapses are gradually eliminated based on IO activity during development [81,82]. We further note that our proposal of dimension reduction of an oscillatory system via coupling-induced synchronization contrasts other neural networks (e.g., auto-encoders) in that we propose a framework of neural communication among neurons for transmitting information rather than approximating a function that maps the data from high-dimensional space to low-dimensional space (i.e, encoding/decoding). In most artificial neural networks, such encoding/decoding scheme creates a black box on their mechanisms in reducing the dimension in the data. For instance, there is no meaningful link between the weights and the function being approximated or which variables in the data are irrelevant is an open problem. In contrast, the core idea of our proposal is that coupling provides a biologically plausible mechanism to achieve stable and reliable transitions between different oscillatory regimes [83]. Such framework allows the olivo-cerebellar system to dynamically control of the dimensionality depending upon the required task.

Intermediate coupling strengths induce chaotic firings in inferior olive neurons

Our results also show that intermediate ranges of electrical coupling maximize chaotic dynamics. The model suggests that low complexity entropies found at weak and strong coupling levels are due to two different mechanisms. In the first mechanism, strong couplings synchronize the neurons and thus reduce the entropy of the network. In the second mechanism, weak couplings enhance asynchrony in the network. However, in the limit of no coupling, IO neurons do not interact. In this case, the whole network possesses a quasi-periodic solution when natural frequencies of different IO neurons have irrational ratios. It is known that the maximum Lyapunov exponent of quasi-periodic solutions is zero [84]. Thus, no chaotic behavior is expected in the no coupling condition, and very weak coupling should lead to similar dynamics. In contrast to the scenario of strong and weak couplings, moderate interactions of the neurons via intermediate coupling strengths induce asynchronous and irregular spiking activity and thus maximize the entropy [85]. We note that, in addition to the coupling strengths, there exists several factors that may affect chaotic dynamics of IO neurons. We found, in simulations, that the landscape of the complexity entropy, with respect to inhibitory conductance gi and gap-junctional conductance gc, changes as the synaptic input level varies (S10A Fig). However, an inverted-U curve of complexity entropy as a function of effective coupling was observed in all the synaptic input levels tested, indicating the robustness of our finding that intermediate coupling strengths induce chaos (S10B Fig).

The finding of an inverted U curve in both the model and experimental data are consistent with the “chaotic resonance” hypothesis, according to which chaotic firing increases information transmission despite the low firing rates of IO neurons [43]. We have previously proposed, and shown in simulations, that such chaotic firing may be useful to enhance cerebellar learning by increasing the error transmission capability of the olivocerebellar system [45]. In agreement with this view, a previous combined in vitro and in vivo study of balanced excitatory/inhibitory cortical networks showed that the entropy of neural activity and mutual information between stimulus and response are maximized [86].

Modulation of electrical coupling is a key parameter of olivocerebellar activity

Our results support the view that the efficacy of coupling between IO neurons is regulated by synaptic inputs to the IO, particularly those that terminate on the gap junction-coupled spines [25]. Both excitatory and inhibitory synapses are present and may provide complementary mechanisms for controlling the strength of coupling between IO cells [87]. In the first mechanism, presynaptic GABAergic terminals control the efficacy of electrical coupling [25,26,49,56,88]. Note that GABAergic sources may also have an inhibitory effect on IO activity in general, and on their subthreshold oscillations in particular [89]. Thus, the changes in complex spike activity probably reflect both changes in coupling strength and excitability of IO neurons. We examined that possibility by varying the levels of the inhibitory synaptic inputs in the model. The effective coupling strengths estimated from the IO spiking data was slightly varied across the individual models, indicating the possible effect of GABAergic sources on excitability of IO neurons. However, we also found consistent effects of the drugs on the coupling strengths (i.e. increased by PIX and decreased by CBX), suggesting that the effect on complex spike activity reflects the drugs' effects on effective coupling strength directly (S4C Fig). In the second mechanism, glutamatergic synapses may control the strength of coupling between IO cells through both AMPA and NMDA-receptor mediated actions. It has been shown that blocking of AMPA receptors restricts and modifies the distribution of complex spike synchrony [34,35] and that activation of NMDA receptor strengthens coupling between weakly-coupled IO neurons thereby expanding the coupled IO network [36,37]. Thus, both of these mechanisms suggest that the olivo-cerebellar system can dynamically control the synchrony level of their corresponding climbing fiber inputs through regulating the coupling strength between IO neurons.

Changes in dimensionality enable changes in modes of motor learning and control

The view that olivo-cerebellar axons carry error-based signals that induce plasticity at the parallel-fiber Purkinje cell synapses has received extensive experimental support since it was proposed on theoretical grounds by Marr and Albus (see Introduction). In addition, animal and human experimental data support the importance of electrical coupling for cerebellar learning [90,91]. Our results are compatible with the view that by modulating electrical coupling of IO neurons the dimensionality of olivo-cerebellar activity is adaptively optimized for different modes of motor learning. While plasticity of parallel fiber synapses with any particular PC may not necessarily depend on the dimensionality of IO activity, dimensionality and synchrony may have an influence on the distribution of plasticity across PCs, and this may be a key parameter of motor learning. For example, in the early phase of learning, motor commands are far from the desired ones. As a result, the Purkinje cells would be strongly modulated by large sensory inputs due to large error signals and their activity would inhibit cerebellar nuclei-IO neurons, thereby decreasing GABA release in the IO. Thus, IO neurons would be strongly coupled, and the dimensionality of olivo-cerebellar activity would be low. Because of this low dimensionality, the IO network would respond only to low-frequency components of the error signals, which would convey only the gross features of the motor commands. However, the strong coupling would allow widespread synchrony among IO neurons and potentially lead to parallel-fiber-Purkinje-cell synaptic plasticity (LTP or LTD) among large numbers of PCs simultaneously, resulting in fast but coarse learning. In contrast, in the late phase of learning, as the motor error becomes smaller, error-driven Purkinje cell activity would decrease, allowing increased activity of cerebellar nucleo-olivary neurons. This would result, in turn, in reduced IO coupling, lower synchrony, and thereby higher dimensionality. Furthermore, the decreased synchrony potentially would allow the occurrence of chaotic resonance to enhance information transmission of the error signals [4145], which would overcome the constraint of low IO firing rates [55,92]. De-synchronized, high-dimensional IO activity may optimally select the sites at which LTD/LTP occurs, and thereby allow more sophisticated learning, resulting in fine tuning of motor commands [45,46,93].

In addition to its effect on plasticity and learning, it is thought that the IO activity contributes directly to ongoing motor outputs throughout the learning process because changes in synchrony levels affect cerebellar nuclear cell activity directly [21,61,94]. Early on, highly synchronous activity, perhaps triggered by error signals, would trigger relatively crude corrective movements. Later on, more restricted synchrony patterns would convey high dimensional signals to be used for the fine grain motor control commands that are needed for precise motor coordination [19]. Note that the motor learning and control functions of the IO are not mutually exclusive, and the current consensus is that the olivo-cerebellar system contributes to both functions. Indeed, conceptual proposals have been made to integrate these two functional roles [39,95,96]. Additional modeling studies, in which IO and cerebellar networks are embedded into realistic motor systems with multiple muscles and joints will be needed to fully integrate and understand the role of the IO with its varying coupling strengths into motor learning and control.

Methods

The recording experiments were performed in accordance with the National Institute of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals. Experimental protocols were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of New York University School of Medicine.

Experimental data

The analyses were performed on a subset of data obtained in two prior series of experiments in ketamine/xylazine anesthetized female, Sprague-Dawley rats that involved either injection of picrotoxin (PIX) or carbenoxolone (CBX) to the IO to block GABA-A receptors or gap junctions, respectively [30,33,34]. The specific experiments were chosen primarily on the basis of having typical complex spike activity in control and a large change in activity in response to the drug injection.

Details of the experimental procedures can be found in the original reports. In brief, a rectangular array of glass microelectrodes was implanted into the apical surface of crus 2a. The arrays typically contained 3–4 mediolaterally running rows and up to 10 rostro-caudally running columns, with an interelectrode spacing of ~250 μm. Electrodes were implanted to a depth of ~100 μm below the brain surface such that complex spikes from individual Purkinje cells were recorded. In each experiment, spontaneous complex spike activity was recorded during an initial control period. Following the control (CON) period, the IO was located by lowering a microelectrode through the brainstem under stereotaxic guidance until activity characteristic of IO neurons was observed. The microelectrode was then replaced by an injection pipette containing the drug solution that was lowered to the same location as the site where IO activity was found. A slow injection of drug solution was then performed (~1 μl over 5–10 min). The drug conditions analyzed were recorded after completion of the injection and a clear change in activity was observed. The multielectrode arrays recorded from 10–30 Purkinje cells in each of the CBX experiments (n = 6 animals), and from 16–42 Purkinje cells in the PIX experiments (n = 3 animals).

The effect of CBX and PIX on complex spike activity often varied among cells within an experiment. This was likely due to the Purkinje cells in different parts of the array receiving climbing fibers from different regions of the IO, that the drugs were injected at a single point within the IO, and that drug concentration (and therefore efficacy) will fall with distance from the injection site. Indeed, the IO is an extended structure (particularly in the rostrocaudal axis where it is ~2 mm long). We therefore considered the effects of the drugs when selecting the neurons for analysis. That is, Purkinje cells that exhibited significant changes in complex spike firing rate, measured as the mean number of spikes per second, between the control and drug conditions were selected. For CBX, the criterion was a 50% decrease and for PIX it was a 50% increase (S2 Fig). In total, we analyzed spike train data from 500-second long periods for the control and drug conditions for each neuron (neurons/condition: control, n = 100; PIX, n = 47; CBX, n = 53).

IO network model

The IO neuron model is a conductance-based model [52] extended via addition of glomerular compartments comprising electrically coupled spines [56]. The network model consisted of an array of 3x3 IO neurons, each of which was mutually connected to its four neighboring neurons by a gap junction from one of its spines to one of its neighbor’s represented by the gap-junctional conductance gc, whose strength was drawn from a uniform distribution with the maximum deviation set at ± 20% of the mean (S3A Fig).

We simulated spike data of the nine cells with stepwise changes of two model parameters: inhibitory synaptic conductance gi, and coupling conductance gc. These two parameters were both varied in the range of 0–2.0 mS/cm2 with an increment of 0.05 mS/cm2. We generated a total of 41x41 = 1681 sets of 500-second long simulated spike trains. The simulated spike data for each variation of gi and gc was then compared with the actual spike data, and the parameters whose firing dynamics best fit to that of individual neurons in the control, PIX, and CBX conditions were selected as the estimated values (see below for details). Because the effect of the axial conductance of the spines, gs, is equivalent to that of the gap-junctional conductance, gc, in determining the amount of current will flow across the gap junction, gs does not need to be estimated from the data and thus was fixed at 0.1 mS/cm2 [56]. To better account for excitability of the neurons in vivo, the inward sodium current conductance gNa was set as 110 mS/cm2, which has been shown to induce robust chaos in the model [43]. All of the soma, dendrite, and spine compartments respectively receive 10, 80, and 10 excitatory and inhibitory synapses driven by Poisson spike generators [56]. The numbers of synapses are roughly proportional to the surface areas of the three compartments.

The segmental Bayes inference for estimating the effective coupling from a single model

Under simplified assumptions, the effective coupling, geff, between two IO neurons was calculated from the axial conductance of the spines gs, inhibitory conductance gi and gap-junctional conductance gc as in [57]:

geff=gs2gc+gi+gsgc. (1)

This equation implies that to estimate the effective coupling geff, we need to estimate both the coupling conductance gc and the GABA conductance gi reliably for each of the three datasets CBX, CON, and PIX. For that purpose, we previously developed a Bayesian method that contains two steps [51]. In the first step, the parameters are estimated for each 50-second time-segment of individual neurons, allowing the parameter values to vary in time. This compensates for inevitable mismatch in the firing patterns between the model and the data. In the second step, a single set of parameter values is estimated for the entire set of time-segments of individual neurons by a hierarchical Bayes framework. Below, we outline the segmental Bayes method (for a detailed description, see [51]).

First, the firing dynamics of the spike data were characterized by a feature vector composed of a total of sixty-seven spatiotemporal features, e.g., firing rate, local variation [58], cross-correlation, auto-correlation, and minimal distance [97]. Principal component analysis (PCA) was then conducted to remove the redundancy of those features. The Bayesian inference aims to inversely estimate the conductance values from the top three principal components, which accounted for 55% of the data variance (S3B Fig). To compensate for the modeling errors, i.e. differences in the complexity of firing patterns between the model and actual neurons, we divided the spike data of each neuron into short time-segments under the assumption that segmental estimates of individual neurons fluctuated around a single neuronal estimate with a normal (Gaussian) distribution. The conductance values of individual neurons can be estimated by a hierarchical Bayesian framework (S3C Fig). Here, the segment size, 50 seconds, was optimized so that the variance of firing frequency across segments was minimal [56]. We have shown that the segmental Bayes algorithm minimizes the fitting between experimental and simulated spike data [51], and further confirmed, by simulations, that it indeed minimizes the estimation errors compared to other conventional methods–including the non-segmental Bayes inference, which finds the estimates once across the entire spike data, and the minimum-error algorithm, which directly finds the closest match in the feature space [98].

Model-averaging estimation of the effective coupling between IO neurons

We found that the firing frequency of inhibitory synaptic noise inputs significantly affects the spiking behavior of the IO model and thus the estimation results. To reduce the uncertainty in estimates of gc and gi, we therefore adopted the segmental Bayes algorithm by using a model-averaging approach as follows (for review, see [99]). We first simulated four models with the firing frequency of inhibitory synaptic inputs of 10, 20, 50 and 70 Hz, which are observed in slices of cerebellar nucleo-olivary neurons [100]. Next, we conducted the segmental Bayes to estimate posterior probability of gi and gc for each model.

P(g|y,mi)P(y|g,mi)P(g|mi), (2)

where P(g | y, mi) is the posterior probability of the conductance g = (gi, gc), y is the feature vectors extracted from the spike data, and mi is the ith selected model (i = 1…4). We then mixed the posterior probabilities with the weights proportional to the model evidence as follows:

P(g|y)=i=1:4P(g|y,mi)P(mi|y),P(mi|y)P(y|mi)P(mi),P(y|mi)=gP(y|g,mi)P(g|mi)dg,P(mi)1, (3)

where P(g | y) is the mixed probability for an individual neuron and P(y | mi) is the evidence of the ith model. Here, all models are treated equally with a non-preference prior P(mi). Finally, the point estimates of gi and gc were computed by marginalizing the mixed posterior probabilities (S4B Fig).

Calculation of the synchrony for individual neurons

The spike train of a neuron was binned into X(i), where i represents the time step (i = 1,,T), with X(i) = 1 if the spike occurs in the ith time bin; otherwise, X(i) = 0. The synchrony of two different neurons, x and y, was calculated as the cross-correlation coefficient at zero-time lag:

Cx,y=i=1TX¯(i)Y¯(i)i=1TX¯(i)2i=1TY¯(i)2, (4)
X¯(i)=X(i)1Tj=1TX(j),Y¯(i)=Y(i)1Tj=1TY(j),

where X¯(i) and Y¯(i) are the normalized forms of X(i) and Y(i) to account for the firing frequency. Here, the two spikes were considered synchronous if their onsets occur in the same 10 milli-second bin. The synchrony level of an individual neuron x was computed as the mean of Cx,y for all neurons yx in the same animal.

Estimation of the coupling coefficient by simulations

To examine whether the estimates of effective coupling strengths were biologically realistic, we computed the coupling coefficients (CCs) for the model neurons as follows. After hyperpolarizing all neurons to -69 mV by injection of Ihyp = -1 μA/cm2 to increase responsiveness, we injected a step current Icmd = -1 μA/cm2 in the soma of the center neuron. We computed the CCs as the average ratio of change in steady state membrane potentials of this “master” cell and its four neighboring cells (S5A Fig). We generated and computed the CCs for hundreds of gi and gc values over the range that the estimated conductance of the data was distributed and found a strong positive correlation between the effective coupling and the CC (S5B Fig, R2 = 0.8, p < 0.0001). CC was determined by transforming the geff value with the fitted model of geff vs. CC (S5B Fig).

Estimation of the dimensionality of neural firings

The dimensionality can be considered as the minimal dimensions necessary to provide an accurate description of the neural dynamics. Principal component analysis (PCA) has become the most widely used approach for determining this, because it enables neural dynamics to be represented in a lower dimensional space [79]. Here, we adopted this approach for estimating the dimensionality of the firing activity of a small number of neurons, like the numbers in the recording arrays.

We first sampled 50-s long spike trains using sampling intervals of 10 milli-seconds, from which the firing rate vectors of all neurons were computed (S6A Fig). Firing rate vector in each sampled window corresponds to an observation in the N-dimensional space, where N is the number of ensemble neurons. Then, PCA was applied to estimate the dimensionality as [60]:

d=1i=1Nλ˜i2, (5)

where λ˜i=λi/(jλj) are the principal eigenvalues expressed as the amount of variance explained (S6A Fig), and λi is the ith eigenvalue of the covariance matrix of the firing rate vectors.

To test whether dimensionality was sensitive to the duration of the sampled window, windows with duration of 10–50 seconds were analyzed. No significant different values were found (S6B Fig), probably because IO firing rates are stable across each condition. However, it has been shown that dimensionality estimation depends on the number of ensemble neurons N. Specifically, d is underestimated for small N but becomes independent of N for sufficiently large N [79]. After data selection (see above), the number of IO neurons in each animal is N = 4–20, which is likely to suffer from the under-sampling bias. Thus, to compare dimensionality among the animals, we normalized it by the number of selected IO neurons in individual animals (i.e normalized d = d/N).

Computation of the complexity entropy

The Lyapunov exponents quantify the sensitivity of a dynamical system to initial conditions, and thus are often used as indicators of chaos [65,66]. A number of methods have been developed to compute the Lyapunov exponents from time series with a fixed sampling interval [67,68]. Those methods, however, are not applicable for our IO data because computation of Lyapunov exponents requires access to continuous variables, which is not the case in our discrete IO spike sets. We therefore adopted a previously proposed approach [70] that approximates the Lyapunov exponents via a recurrence plot by using the edit distance of spike trains [101]. Our method requires computing the modified edit distance of the spike trains [97] and its recurrence plot [102,103]. The edit distance of two derived windows is defined by a total minimal cost for converting one window to the other [101]. Allowed operations include deletion or insertion of events (both cost 1 for each event), and shift of events (cost 20% the amount of shifting in second for each event). The complexity entropy [69] was computed from the distribution of the length of diagonal lines in the recurrence plot (see S8 Fig for illustration of the complexity method).

Specifically, we first sampled the spikes trains in windows of 50 seconds and computed the edit distance for all pairs of sampled windows. To resolve the issue of discontinuity induced by the difference in the number of spikes in two sampled windows, we adopted a modified version of edit distance computation as in [97]. Briefly, for each sampled window, we took into account the spikes that occur immediately before and/or after the time window, thus resulting in four derived windows. We then computed the edit distance for a total of 16 (4x4) derived pairs of the two sampled windows and temporarily assigned the minimum value as edit distance between them. The edit distance for all pairs of sampled windows of 50 seconds with an interval of 2 seconds constitutes a two-dimensional distance matrix. We then updated the edit distance matrix by the shortest distance connecting any two sampled windows–S8A Fig. The recurrence plot is constructed by binarizing the edit distance matrix, with the distance values smaller than a predefined threshold as 1, and the others else as 0 [102]. The threshold was determined so that 10% of data points in the distance matrix were 1, as in [103]. Next, we extracted the frequency distribution of the length of the points 1 that form diagonal lines in the recurrence plot. The Shannon entropy of that distribution has been shown to be inversely proportional to the largest Lyapunov exponent [69]. We thus used the inverse of Shannon entropy as a measure of chaos for the spike data.

To validate that complexity entropy is an indicator of chaos, we generated noise-free simulation data and computed the correlations between complexity entropy and the Lyapunov indexes (S8B and S8C Fig). Note that this approach is possible for the simulation data because we have access to the continuous trace of the membrane potential. Specifically, we first removed the noise in the synaptic inputs, and simulated 500-second spike trains for more than 100 conductance values (gi varied in 0–1.0 mS/cm2 and gc in 0–2.0 mS/cm2) and estimated the complexity entropy from the simulated spike trains. Next we computed the Lyapunov exponents of the IO model by the method of [104], and then extracted the largest component, λ1, as well as the Lyapunov dimension, DKY, as these are two direct indicators of chaos [105].

Statistical analysis

Unless specifically stated elsewhere, all data is reported as mean ± std. The non-parametric Kruskal-Wallis one-way analysis of variance was used to test whether data groups of different sizes originate from the same distribution.

Code availability

The simulation code of the IO network model is hosted publicly on github, accessible via https://github.com/hoang-atr/io_model. The MATLAB implementations of the segmental Bayes algorithm [51] and the complexity entropy method [70] are available upon request from the corresponding authors.

Supporting information

S1 Fig. Inferior olive firing data set for all animals.

A: Spike data in 50 second of 10 representative neurons in 9 animals with the physiological conditions (CBX and PIX) in the right and the control condition (CON) in the left columns.

(TIF)

S2 Fig. Data selection by changes in the firing rate.

A: the histograms of firing rate change by drug treatments (decreased by CBX and increased by PIX) compared to the CON condition. The red lines indicate the thresholds (50%) for selecting the neurons for analysis. B: pseudo-color maps show the firing rate change by drug treatments of the neurons in the micro-electrode arrays for six CBX animals (top two rows) and three PIX animals (bottom row). Red asterisks indicated the selected neurons, whose firing rate changes exceed the thresholds.

(TIF)

S3 Fig. Estimation of the conductances from spiking data using IO model and segmental Bayes algorithm.

A: left, electrical circuit equivalents of the soma (S), dendrite (D) and spine compartments (SP) of a model IO neuron. middle, the connection of two IO neurons via a gap junctional conductance gc that connects the spine compartments. right, The IO network, which consists of 3x3 neurons, each of which is connected to its four neighboring neurons as shown. B: Left, five major features (FR = firing rate, ACG1 = auto-correlogram in 50 ms bin, CCG1 = cross-correlogram in 50 ms bin, MD1 = the first fraction of the minimal distance distribution, LV = local variation) extracted from spiking data of the three conditions (see [56] for detailed definitions of the features). Each feature was normalized by the mean value of the CON level. Right, the top two principal components of the extracted features. C: Flow chart of conductance estimation for each neuron. To account for highly non-stationary of the spike patterns in the three data condition, we divided the spike data of each neuron into small time-segments, applied the Bayesian inference to estimate gi and gc for every segment under the assumption that segmental estimates were drawn from a single neuronal estimate of a normal (Gaussian) distribution with unknown mean and the variance as a prior (left). The variance was optimized so as to maximize the fit of the data and the model in the PCA space. The posterior estimation for a representative neuron's gi and gc conductances (right). A broad probability distribution was found when the variance was relaxed but a much smaller distribution resulted when optimized variance was used [51].

(TIF)

S4 Fig. Improving the parameter estimates via Bayesian model-averaging.

A: PCA error rates of the gi and gc estimates by the segmental Bayesian inference averaged for the entire IO neurons for CBX, CON, and PIX conditions for four different models (color bars) in comparison with the previous model (black bar, [56]). The error bars are of 95% CIs. B: Posterior probabilities of a representative IO neuron by individual models and the mixed posterior probability with the weights determined by the evidence of Bayesian inference. C: boxplots show estimates of the effective coupling geff of the three data conditions in the four individual models.

(TIF)

S5 Fig. Estimation of the coupling coefficient (CC) by simulation.

A: We injected a current pulse of -1 μA/cm2 to a cell and recorded the steady-state voltage change of this “master” cell and its four post-junctional cells. B: We computed the CCs for hundreds of gi and gc values in the range over which the estimated conductances of the data distributed, and found a strong positive correlation between the effective coupling and the CC (R2 = 0.8, p < 0.0001). Note that the non-linear fit represents the nature of deriving geff from gi and gc following Eq (1).

(TIF)

S6 Fig. Dimensionality estimation for the spike data of ensemble neurons.

A: Illustration of the principal component analysis (PCA) for the firing rate vectors extracted from 50-second windows of three neurons of Animal #6 in the CON condition. The estimated dimensionality d = 1.86 (dashed dark line, Eq 5), indicates that the approximately 2-dimensional subspace (shaded gray plane) can explain more than 90% of the variance of neural firing dynamics. B: Estimating dimensionality (Eq 5) with varied window lengths from 10–50 seconds for 9 animals in the three data conditions showing the robustness of dimensionality estimation against the window length. The error bars are of 95% CIs.

(TIF)

S7 Fig. Change in synchrony is linearly coupled with change in dimensionality.

We first divided the complex spike data of 9 animals in the control condition into a series of shorter segments using a moving window of length 5, 10, 20, or 50 s, whose start incremented in 1 second steps. Next, we computed the dimensionality and the synchrony (both in 10 milli-second timescale) for each segment (A) and measured the correlation coefficient between those two metrics (B). The correlation coefficients were negative for almost all animals and window lengths (C). These results provide clear evidence that variations of synchrony within the physiological range are negatively correlated with changes in dimensionality. Note that the control data of spontaneous complex spike activity in our study is within the range of physiologically occurring synchrony levels. A: time-varying synchrony (left ordinate) and dimensionality (right ordinate) in successive, overlapped 10-s sliding windows of the control recording from animal #3. B: Scatterplot of correlation versus dimensionality values shown in A shows the presence of a significant negative correlation (r = -0.6). C: synchrony vs. dimensionality correlation coefficients for 9 control animals and all tested window lengths.

(TIF)

S8 Fig. Computation and validation of the complexity entropy method.

A: Illustration of edit distance computation between two sampled spike windows shows a sequence of elementary steps that convert the spike window (a) into (b). Each bar represents one spike. Allowed operations include deletion of a spike (shown in red), insertion of a spike (shown in green), or shifting a spike in time (blue arrows). Computation of edit distance for continuous sampling windows for the entire spike train constitutes the edit distance matrix. Then, the recurrent plot is constructed by binarizing the edit distance matrix. The points, whose values are smaller than the threshold, were plotted as white dots, otherwise as black dots. Complexity entropy is computed as the inverse of Shannon entropy, in terms of frequency distribution of the length of the diagonal lines of white dots [69]. B–C: Complexity entropy measured for a total of a hundred of parameter values (black crosses) in noise-free simulations showed strong positive correlations with the largest Lyapunov exponent λ1 (regression model: λ1 ~ 1 + entropy, R2 = 0.4, F-test: p < 0.0001, S8B Fig) and the Lyapunov dimension DKY (DKY ~ 1 + entropy, R2 = 0.48, F-test: p < 0.0001, S8C Fig). Solid cyan lines represent the fit of linear models with 95% CIs (dashed cyan lines).

(TIF)

S9 Fig. Validation of the inverted U-shaped curves.

We investigated whether intermediate couplings maximize the complexity entropy by applying a non-parametric Gaussian Process regression model, which does not assume an explicit relationship between the coupling and the complexity entropy. Still, we observed inverted U-shaped curves maximized at around geff = 0.033 mS/cm2 for both the model (A) and the data (B). In sum, these results support the inverted U-shaped relationship between the effective coupling and complexity entropy. The right ordinates of A–B represent the first Lyapunov exponents approximated from the simulation data (see S8B Fig), indicating that intermediate couplings induce chaos. The shaded regions are of ±sem.

(TIF)

S10 Fig. Effect of inhibitory synaptic input on complexity entropy.

A: the pseudo-color heatmap of the complexity entropies, averaged across 9 model neurons, for all pairs of (gi, gc) in the range of 0–2 mS/cm2. The frequency of inhibitory synaptic input noise was varied in the range of 10–70 Hz. B: complexity entropy vs. effective coupling geff. In each model, a second-order regression (entropy ~ 1 + geff + geff2) fit was shown by black thick line. The second-order coefficient was negative and significant for all the models indicating the robustness of the inverted-U curve.

(TIF)

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting Information files.

Funding Statement

HH, KT and MK were supported by a contract with the National Institute of Information and Communications Technology entitled ‘Development of network dynamics modeling methods for human brain data simulation systems’ (Grant No.173) and ‘Analysis of multi-modal brain measurement data and development of its application for BMI open innovation’ (Grant No.209). HH and KT are partially supported by ImPACT Program of Council of Science, Technology and Innovation (Cabinet Office, Government of Japan). HH, ITT and KT are partially supported by MEXT Kakenhi (No. 17H06313). HH and KT are partially supported by JST ERATO (JPMJER1801, “Brain-AI hybrid”). KA is partially supported by MEXT Kakenhi (No. 15H05707) and AMED under Grant Number JP20dm0307009. EJL acknowledges grants NIH NS095089 and NS37028. NS acknowledges grants NSF BCS-1031899 and 1R56NS100528-01. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Bazzigaluppi P, Ruigrok T, Saisan P, de Zeeuw CI, de Jeu M. Properties of the Nucleo-Olivary Pathway: An In Vivo Whole-Cell Patch Clamp Study. PLoS ONE. 2012. September 27;7(9): e46360 10.1371/journal.pone.0046360 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Keating JG, Thach WT. Nonclock behavior of inferior olive neurons: interspike interval of Purkinje cell complex spike discharge in the awake behaving monkey is random. J Neurophysiol. 1995. April;73(4):1329–40. 10.1152/jn.1995.73.4.1329 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Kitazawa S, Kimura T, Yin PB. Cerebellar complex spikes encode both destinations and errors in arm movements. Nature. 1998. April 2;392(6675):494–7. 10.1038/33141 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Kobayashi Y, Kawano K, Takemura A, Inoue Y, Kitama T, Gomi H, Kawato M. Temporal firing patterns of Purkinje cells in the cerebellar ventral paraflocculus during ocular following responses in monkeys II. Complex spikes. J Neurophysiol. 1998. August;80(2):832–48. 10.1152/jn.1998.80.2.832 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.D’Angelo E, Mapelli L, Casellato C, Garrido JA, Luque N, Monaco J, et al. Distributed Circuit Plasticity: New Clues for the Cerebellar Mechanisms of Learning. Cerebellum. 2016. April;15(2):139–51. 10.1007/s12311-015-0711-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Hansel C, Linden DJ, D’Angelo E. Beyond parallel fiber LTD: the diversity of synaptic and non-synaptic plasticity in the cerebellum. Nat Neurosci. 2001. May;4(5):467–75. 10.1038/87419 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Kuroda S, Schweighofer N, Kawato M. Exploration of signal transduction pathways in cerebellar long-term depression by kinetic simulation. J Neurosci. 2001. August 1;21(15):5693–702. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-15-05693.2001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Ito M. Cerebellar long-term depression: characterization, signal transduction, and functional roles. Physiol Rev. 2001. July;81(3):1143–95. 10.1152/physrev.2001.81.3.1143 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Marr D. A theory of cerebellar cortex. J Physiol. 1969. June;202(2):437–70. 10.1113/jphysiol.1969.sp008820 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Albus JS. The theory of cerebellar function. Math Biosci. 1971. February;10(1–2):25–61. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Bastian AJ. Learning to predict the future: the cerebellum adapts feedforward movement control. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2006. December;16(6):645–9. 10.1016/j.conb.2006.08.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Kawato M, Gomi H. The cerebellum and VOR/OKR learning models. Trends Neurosci. 1992. November;15(11):445–53. 10.1016/0166-2236(92)90008-v [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Schweighofer N, Spoelstra J, Arbib MA, Kawato M. Role of the cerebellum in reaching movements in humans. II. A neural model of the intermediate cerebellum. Eur J Neurosci. 1998. January;10(1):95–105. 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1998.00007.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Tseng Y-W, Diedrichsen J, Krakauer JW, Shadmehr R, Bastian AJ. Sensory prediction errors drive cerebellum-dependent adaptation of reaching. J Neurophysiol. 2007. July;98(1):54–62. 10.1152/jn.00266.2007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Vinueza Veloz MF, Zhou K, Bosman LWJ, Potters JW, Negrello M, Seepers RM, et al. Cerebellar control of gait and interlimb coordination. Brain Struct Funct. 2015. November;220(6):3513–36. 10.1007/s00429-014-0870-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Herzfeld DJ, Kojima Y, Soetedjo R, Shadmehr R. Encoding of error and learning to correct that error by the Purkinje cells of the cerebellum. Nat Neurosci. 2018. May;21(5):736–43. 10.1038/s41593-018-0136-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Llinás R. The noncontinuous nature of movement execution. In: Humphrey DR, Freund H-J, editors. Motor control: concepts and issues. New York: Wiley; 1991. p. 223–42. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Welsh JP, Lang EJ, Sugihara I, Llinás R. Dynamic organization of motor control within the olivocerebellar system. Nature. 1995. March 30;374(6521):453–7. 10.1038/374453a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Hoogland TM, De Gruijl JR, Witter L, Canto CB, De Zeeuw CI. Role of synchronous activation of cerebellar Purkinje cell ensembles in multi-joint movement control. Curr Biol. 2015. May 4;25(9):1157–65. 10.1016/j.cub.2015.03.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Mukamel EA, Nimmerjahn A, Schnitzer MJ. Automated analysis of cellular signals from large-scale calcium imaging data. Neuron. 2009. September 24;63(6):747–60. 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.08.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Blenkinsop TA, Lang EJ. Synaptic action of the olivocerebellar system on cerebellar nuclear spike activity. J Neurosci. 2011. October 12;31(41):14708–20. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3323-11.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Tang T, Blenkinsop TA, Lang EJ. Complex spike synchrony dependent modulation of rat deep cerebellar nuclear activity. eLife. 2019. January 9;8:e40101 10.7554/eLife.40101 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Thach WT. Discharge of Purkinje and cerebellar nuclear neurons during rapidly alternating arm movements in the monkey. J Neurophysiol. 1968. September;31(5):785–97. 10.1152/jn.1968.31.5.785 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Gomi H, Kawato M. Neural network control for a closed-loop system using feedback-error-learning. Neural Netw. 1993;6(7):933–46. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Llinás R, Baker R, Sotelo C. Electrotonic coupling between neurons in cat inferior olive. J Neurophysiol. 1974. May;37(3):560–71. 10.1152/jn.1974.37.3.560 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Sotelo C, Llinás R, Baker R. Structural study of inferior olivary nucleus of the cat: morphological correlates of electrotonic coupling. J Neurophysiol. 1974. May;37(3):541–59. 10.1152/jn.1974.37.3.541 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.De Zeeuw CI, Hertzberg EL, Mugnaini E. The dendritic lamellar body: a new neuronal organelle putatively associated with dendrodendritic gap junctions. J Neurosci. 1995. February;15(2):1587–604. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-02-01587.1995 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Belluardo N, Mudò G, Trovato-Salinaro A, Le Gurun S, Charollais A, Serre-Beinier V, et al. Expression of connexin36 in the adult and developing rat brain. Brain Res. 2000. May 19;865(1):121–38. 10.1016/s0006-8993(00)02300-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Condorelli DF, Parenti R, Spinella F, Trovato Salinaro A, Belluardo N, Cardile V, et al. Cloning of a new gap junction gene (Cx36) highly expressed in mammalian brain neurons. Eur J Neurosci. 1998. March;10(3):1202–8. 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1998.00163.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Blenkinsop TA, Lang EJ. Block of Inferior Olive Gap Junctional Coupling Decreases Purkinje Cell Complex Spike Synchrony and Rhythmicity. J Neurosci. 2006. February 8;26(6):1739–48. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3677-05.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Marshall SP, Lang EJ. Local changes in the excitability of the cerebellar cortex produce spatially restricted changes in complex spike synchrony. J Neurosci. 2009. November 11;29(45):14352–62. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3498-09.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Long MA, Deans MR, Paul DL, Connors BW. Rhythmicity without synchrony in the electrically uncoupled inferior olive. J Neurosci. 2002. December 15;22(24):10898–905. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-24-10898.2002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Lang EJ, Sugihara I, Llinás R. GABAergic modulation of complex spike activity by the cerebellar nucleoolivary pathway in rat. J Neurophysiol. 1996. July;76(1):255–75. 10.1152/jn.1996.76.1.255 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Lang EJ. GABAergic and glutamatergic modulation of spontaneous and motor-cortex-evoked complex spike activity. J Neurophysiol. 2002. April;87(4):1993–2008. 10.1152/jn.00477.2001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Lang EJ. Organization of olivocerebellar activity in the absence of excitatory glutamatergic input. J Neurosci. 2001. March 1;21(5):1663–75. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-05-01663.2001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Mathy A, Clark BA, Häusser M. Synaptically induced long-term modulation of electrical coupling in the inferior olive. Neuron. 2014. March 19;81(6):1290–6. 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.01.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Turecek J, Yuen GS, Han VZ, Zeng XH, Bayer KU, Welsh JP. NMDA receptor activation strengthens weak electrical coupling in mammalian brain. Neuron. 2014. March 19;81(6):1375–88. 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.01.024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Kawato M, Kuroda S, Schweighofer N. Cerebellar supervised learning revisited: Biophysical modeling and degrees-of-freedom control. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2011. October;21(5):791–800. 10.1016/j.conb.2011.05.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Schweighofer N, Lang EJ, Kawato M. Role of the olivo-cerebellar complex in motor learning and control. Front Neural Circuits. 2013. May 28;7:94 10.3389/fncir.2013.00094 2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Tokuda IT, Hoang H, Kawato M. New insights into olivo-cerebellar circuits for learning from a small training sample, Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2017. October;46:58–67. 10.1016/j.conb.2017.07.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Makarenko V, Llinas R. Experimentally determined chaotic phase synchronization in a neuronal system. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 1998. December 22;95(26):15747–52. 10.1073/pnas.95.26.15747 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Masuda N, Aihara K. Spatiotemporal spike encoding of a continuous external signal. Neural Comput. 2002. July;14(7):1599–628. 10.1162/08997660260028638 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Schweighofer N, Doya K, Fukai H, Chiron JV, Furukawa T, Kawato M. Chaos may enhance information transmission in the inferior olive. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2004. March 30;101(13):4655–60. 10.1073/pnas.0305966101 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Nobukawa S, Nishimura H. Chaotic resonance in coupled inferior olive neurons with the Llinás approach neuron model. Neural Comput. 2016. November;28(11):2505–32. 10.1162/NECO_a_00894 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Tokuda IT, Han CE, Aihara K, Kawato M, Schweighofer N. The role of chaotic resonance in cerebellar learning. Neural Netw. 2010. Sep;23(7):836–42. 10.1016/j.neunet.2010.04.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Tokuda IT, Hoang H, Schweighofer N, Kawato M. Adaptive coupling of inferior olive neurons in cerebellar learning. Neural Netw. 2013. November;47:42–50. 10.1016/j.neunet.2012.12.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Devor A, Yarom Y. Electrotonic coupling in the inferior olivary nucleus revealed by simultaneous double patch recordings. J Neurophysiol. 2002. June;87(6):3048–58. 10.1152/jn.2002.87.6.3048 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Hoge GJ, Davidson KG, Yasumura T, Castillo PE, Rash JE, Pereda AE. The extent and strength of electrical coupling between inferior olivary neurons is heterogeneous. J Neurophysiol. 2011. March;105(3):1089–101. 10.1152/jn.00789.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Lefler Y, Yarom Y, Uusisaari M. Cerebellar inhibitory input to the inferior olive decreases electrical coupling and blocks subthreshold oscillations. Neuron. 2014. March 19;81(6):1389–400. 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.02.032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Schmolesky MT, Weber JT, Zeeuw CI, Hansel C. The making of a complex spike: ionic composition and plasticity. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2002. December;978:359–90. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2002.tb07581.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Hoang H, Yamashita O, Tokuda IT, Sato M-A., Kawato M, Toyama K. Segmental Bayesian estimation of gap-junctional and inhibitory conductance of inferior olive neurons from spike trains with complicated dynamics. Front Comput Neurosci. 2015. May 21;9:56 10.3389/fncom.2015.00056 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Schweighofer N, Doya K, Kawato M. Electrophysiological properties of inferior olive neurons: A compartmental model. J Neurophysiol. 1999. August;82(2):804–17. 10.1152/jn.1999.82.2.804 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.De Zeeuw CI, Simpson JI, Hoogenraad CC, Galjart N, Koekkoek SKE, Ruigrok TJH. Microcircuitry and function of the inferior olive. Trends Neurosci. 1998. September;21(9):391–400. 10.1016/s0166-2236(98)01310-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Llinás R, Yarom Y. Electrophysiology of mammalian inferior olivary neurones in vitro. Different types of voltage-dependent ionic conductances. J Physiol. 1981. June;315:549–67. 10.1113/jphysiol.1981.sp013763 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Llinás R, Yarom Y. Properties and distribution of ionic conductances generating electroresponsiveness of mammalian inferior olivary neurones in vitro. J Physiol. 1981. July;315:569–84. 10.1113/jphysiol.1981.sp013764 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Onizuka M, Hoang H, Kawato M, Tokuda IT, Schweighofer N, Katori Y, et al. Solution to the inverse problem of estimating gap-junctional and inhibitory conductance in inferior olive neurons from spike trains by network model simulation. Neural Netw. 2013. November;47:51–63. 10.1016/j.neunet.2013.01.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Katori Y, Lang EJ, Onizuka M, Kawato M, Aihara K. Quantitative Modeling of Spatio-Temporal Dynamics of Inferior Olive Neurons With a Simple Conductance-Based Model. Int J Bifurcat Chaos 2010. March;20(3):583–603. 10.1142/S0218127410025909 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Shinomoto S, Miura K, Koyama S. A measure of local variation of inter-spike intervals. BioSystems. Jan-Mar 2005;79(1–3):67–72. 10.1016/j.biosystems.2004.09.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Tovar KR, Maher BJ, Westbrook GL. Direct Actions of Carbenoxolone on Synaptic Transmission and Neuronal Membrane Properties. J Neurophysiol. 2009. August;102(2):974–8. 10.1152/jn.00060.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Abbott LF, Rajan K, Sompolinsky H. “Interactions between intrinsic and stimulus-evoked activity in recurrent neural networks,” in The Dynamic Brain: An Exploration of Neuronal Variability and its Functional Significance, eds Glanzman D. L. and Ding M. (New York, NY: Oxford University Press; ); 2011. p. 65–82. [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Tang T, Suh CY, Blenkinsop TA, Lang EJ. Synchrony is Key: Complex Spike Inhibition of the Deep Cerebellar Nuclei. Cerebellum. 2016. February;15(1):10–3. 10.1007/s12311-015-0743-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Sasaki K, Bower JM, Llinás R. Multiple Purkinje Cell Recording in Rodent Cerebellar Cortex. Eur J Neurosci. 1989. January;1(6):572–86. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1989.tb00364.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Lang EJ, Sugihara I, Welsh JP, Llinás R. Patterns of Spontaneous Purkinje Cell Complex Spike Activity in the Awake Rat. J Neurosci. 1999. April 1;19(7):2728–39. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-07-02728.1999 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Cook RD. Detection of Influential Observation in Linear Regression. Technometrics. 1977. January 5;19(1):15–8. [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Sano M, Sawada Y. Measurement of the lyapunov spectrum from a chaotic time series. Phys Rev Lett. 1985. September 2;55(10):1082–5. 10.1103/PhysRevLett.55.1082 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Farmer JD, Sidorowich JJ. Predicting chaotic time series. Phys Rev Lett. 1987. August 24;59:845–8. 10.1103/PhysRevLett.59.845 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Rosenstein MT, Collins JJ, De Luca CJ. A practical method for calculating largest Lyapunov exponents from small data sets. Phys D. 1993. May;65(1–2):117–34. [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Kantz H. A robust method to estimate the maximal Lyapunov exponent of a time series. Phys Lett A. 1994. January 31;185(1):77–87. [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Letellier C. Estimating the Shannon entropy: Recurrence plots versus symbolic dynamics. Phys Rev Lett. 2006. June 30;96(25):254102 10.1103/PhysRevLett.96.254102 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Hirata Y, Horai S, Aihara K. Reproduction of distance matrices and original time series from recurrence plots and their applications. Eur Phys J Spec Top. 2008;164:13–22. [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Wilkinson GN, Rogers CE. Symbolic description of factorial models for analysis of variance. Appl Stat. 1973;22:392–9. [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Litwin-Kumar A, Oswald A-MM, Urban NN, Doiron B. Balanced Synaptic Input Shapes the Correlation between Neural Spike Trains. PLoS Comput Biol. 2011. December;7(12):e1002305 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002305 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Negrello M, Warnaar P, Romano V, Owens CB, Lindeman S, Iavarone E, et al. Quasiperiodic rhythms of the inferior olive. PLoS Comput Biol. 2019. May 6;15(5):e1006475 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1006475 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Marshall SP, van der Giessen RS, de Zeeuw CI, Lang EJ. Altered olivocerebellar activity patterns in the connexin36 knockout mouse. Cerebellum. 2007;6(4):287–99. 10.1080/14734220601100801 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Pillow JW, Shlens J, Paninski L, Sher A, Litke AM, Chichilnisky EJ, et al. Spatio-temporal correlations and visual signaling in a complete neuronal population. Nature. 2008. August 21;454(7207):995–9. 10.1038/nature07140 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Churchland MM, Yu BM, Cunningham JP, Sugrue LP, Cohen MR, Corrado GS, et al. Stimulus onset quenches neural variability: a widespread cortical phenomenon. Nat Neurosci. 2010. March;13(3):369–78. 10.1038/nn.2501 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Rigotti M, Barak O, Warden MR, Wang XJ, Daw ND, Miller EK, et al. The importance of mixed selectivity in complex cognitive tasks. Nature. 2013. May 30;497(7451):585–90. 10.1038/nature12160 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Cunningham JP, Yu BM. Dimensionality reduction for large-scale neural recordings. Nat Neurosci. 2014. November;17(11):1500–9. 10.1038/nn.3776 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Mazzucato L, Fontanini A, La Camera G. Stimuli Reduce the Dimensionality of Cortical Activity. Front Syst Neurosci. 2016. February 17;10:11 10.3389/fnsys.2016.00011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Cortese A, De Martino B, Kawato M. The neural and cognitive architecture for learning from a small sample. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2019. April;55:133–41. 10.1016/j.conb.2019.02.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Schweighofer N. A model of activity-dependent formation of cerebellar microzones. Biol Cybern. 1998. August;79(2):97–107. 10.1007/s004220050462 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Good J-M, Mahoney M, Miyazaki T, Tanaka KF, Sakimura K, Watanabe M, et al. Maturation of Cerebellar Purkinje Cell Population Activity during Postnatal Refinement of Climbing Fiber Network. Cell Reprts. 2017. November 21;21(8):2066–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Williamson RC, Cowley BR, Litwin-Kumar A, Doiron B, Kohn A, Smith MA, et al. Scaling Properties of Dimensionality Reduction for Neural Populations and Network Models. PLoS Comput Biol. 2016. December 7;12(12):e1005141 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005141 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Pikovsky A, Rosenblum M, Kurths J. Synchronization: A Universal Concept in Nonlinear Science. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, England; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Bertschinger N, Natschläger T. Real-time computation at the edge of chaos in recurrent neural networks. Neural Comput. 2004. July;16(7):1413–36. 10.1162/089976604323057443 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Shew WL, Yang H, Yu S, Roy R, Plenz D. Information Capacity and Transmission Are Maximized in Balanced Cortical Networks with Neuronal Avalanches. J Neurosci. 2011. January 5;31(1):55–63. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4637-10.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.De Gruijl JR, Sokół PA, Negrello M, De Zeeuw CI. Modulation of electrotonic coupling in the inferior olive by inhibitory and excitatory inputs: Integration in the glomerulus. Neuron. 2014. March 19;81(6):1215–17. 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.03.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Best AR, Regehr WG. Inhibitory Regulation of Electrically Coupled Neurons in the Inferior Olive Is Mediated by Asynchronous Release of GABA. Neuron. 2009. May 28;62(4):555–65. 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.04.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.De Gruijl JR, Bazzigaluppi P, de Jeu MTG, De Zeeuw CI. Climbing Fiber Burst Size and Olivary Sub-threshold Oscillations in a Network Setting. PLoS Comput Biol. 2012. December 13;8(12):e1002814 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002814 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Van Der Giessen RS, Koekkoek SK, van Dorp S, De Gruijl JR, Cupido A, Khosrovani S, et al. Role of Olivary Electrical Coupling in Cerebellar Motor Learning. Neuron. 2008. May 22;58(4):599–612. 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.03.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Van Essen TA, Van der Giessen RS, Koekkoek SKE, VanderWerf F, De Zeeuw CI, van Genderen PJJ, et al. Anti-malaria drug mefloquine induces motor learning deficits in humans. Front Neurosci. 2010. November 19;4:191 10.3389/fnins.2010.00191 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Eccles JC, Llinás R, Sasaki K. The excitatory synaptic action of climbing fibres on the purkinje cells of the cerebellum. J Physiol. 1966. January;182(2):268–96. 10.1113/jphysiol.1966.sp007824 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Shaikh AG, Wong AL, Optican LM, Zee DS. Impaired Motor Learning in a Disorder of the Inferior Olive: Is the Cerebellum Confused? Cerebellum. 2017. February;16(1):158–67. 10.1007/s12311-016-0785-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Lang EJ, Blenkinsop TA. Control of cerebellar nuclear cells: a direct role for complex spikes? Cerebellum. 2011. December;10(4):694–701. 10.1007/s12311-011-0261-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Gao Z, Van Beugen BJ, De Zeeuw CI. Distributed synergistic plasticity and cerebellar learning. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2012. September;13(9):619–35. 10.1038/nrn3312 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Lang EJ, Apps R, Bengtsson F, Cerminara NL, De Zeeuw CI, Ebner TJ, et al. The roles of the olivocerebellar pathway in motor learning and motor control. A consensus paper. Cerebellum. 2017. February;16(1):230–52. 10.1007/s12311-016-0787-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Hirata Y, Aihara K. Representing spike trains using constant sampling intervals. J Neurosci Methods. 2009. October 15;183(2):277–86. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2009.06.030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Hoang H, Tokuda IT. Verification of parameter estimation techniques from spike train data. NOLTA2015: 2015 International Symposium on Nonlinear Theory and its Applications, Kowloon, Hong Kong, China, December 1–4, 2015; p. 648–51.
  • 99.Grueber CE, Nakagawa S, Laws RJ, Jamieson IG. Multimodel inference in ecology and evolution: Challenges and solutions. J Evol Biol. 2011. April;24(4):699–711. 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2010.02210.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Najac M, Raman IM. Integration of Purkinje Cell Inhibition by Cerebellar Nucleo-Olivary Neurons. J Neurosci. 2015. January 14;35(2):544–9. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3583-14.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Victor JD, Purpura KP. Metric-space analysis of spike trains: theory, algorithms and application. Net Comput Neural Syst. 1997. May 1;8(2):127–64. [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Eckmann J-P., Kamphorst SO, Ruelle D. Recurrence Plots of Dynamical Systems. Europhys Lett. 1987;4:973–7. [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Marwan N, Carmen Romano M, Thiel M, Kurths J. Recurrence plots for the analysis of complex systems. Phys Rep. 2007. January;438(5–6):237–329. [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Wolf A, Swift JB, Swinney HL, Vastano JA. Determining Lyapunov exponents from a time series. Phys D Nonlinear Phenom. 1985. July;16(3):285–317. [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Kaplan JL, Yorke JA. Chaotic behavior of multidimensional difference equations. In H. O. Walter & H. Peitgen (Eds.). In Lecture Notes in Mathematics: Vol. 730. Functional Differential Equations and Approximations of Fixed Points, 1970; p. 204–207.
PLoS Comput Biol. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1008075.r001

Decision Letter 0

Chris De Zeeuw, Daniele Marinazzo

3 Jan 2020

Dear Dr Hoang,

Thank you very much for submitting your manuscript 'Electrical coupling controls dimensionality and chaotic firing of inferior olive neurons' for review by PLOS Computational Biology. Your manuscript has been fully evaluated by the PLOS Computational Biology editorial team and in this case also by independent peer reviewers. The reviewers appreciated the attention to an important problem, but raised some substantial concerns about the manuscript as it currently stands. While your manuscript cannot be accepted in its present form, we are willing to consider a revised version in which the issues raised by the reviewers have been adequately addressed. We cannot, of course, promise publication at that time.

Please make sure to address the concerns of all three reviewers. In addition, I would like you to add at least in the discussion your view on the impact of dimensionality in olivary activity on the induction of LTD (by increase in CF activity) as well as that of LTP (by a decrease in CF activity) in the context of learning (see also the comments from reviewer 3).

Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out.

Your revisions should address the specific points made by each reviewer. Please return the revised version within the next 60 days. If you anticipate any delay in its return, we ask that you let us know the expected resubmission date by email at ploscompbiol@plos.org. Revised manuscripts received beyond 60 days may require evaluation and peer review similar to that applied to newly submitted manuscripts.

In addition, when you are ready to resubmit, please be prepared to provide the following:

(1) A detailed list of your responses to the review comments and the changes you have made in the manuscript. We require a file of this nature before your manuscript is passed back to the editors.

(2) A copy of your manuscript with the changes highlighted (encouraged). We encourage authors, if possible to show clearly where changes have been made to their manuscript e.g. by highlighting text.

(3) A striking still image to accompany your article (optional). If the image is judged to be suitable by the editors, it may be featured on our website and might be chosen as the issue image for that month. These square, high-quality images should be accompanied by a short caption. Please note as well that there should be no copyright restrictions on the use of the image, so that it can be published under the Open-Access license and be subject only to appropriate attribution.

Before you resubmit your manuscript, please consult our Submission Checklist to ensure your manuscript is formatted correctly for PLOS Computational Biology: http://www.ploscompbiol.org/static/checklist.action. Some key points to remember are:

- Figures uploaded separately as TIFF or EPS files (if you wish, your figures may remain in your main manuscript file in addition).

- Supporting Information uploaded as separate files, titled Dataset, Figure, Table, Text, Protocol, Audio, or Video.

- Funding information in the 'Financial Disclosure' box in the online system.

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email us at figures@plos.org.

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see here

We are sorry that we cannot be more positive about your manuscript at this stage, but if you have any concerns or questions, please do not hesitate to contact us.

Sincerely,

Chris De Zeeuw

Guest Editor

PLOS Computational Biology

Daniele Marinazzo

Deputy Editor

PLOS Computational Biology

A link appears below if there are any accompanying review attachments. If you believe any reviews to be missing, please contact ploscompbiol@plos.org immediately:

[LINK]

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Authors:

Please note here if the review is uploaded as an attachment.

Reviewer #1: It is an interesting study that provides new insights about how olivo-cerebellar loop processes information and how the timing and learning hypothesis of cerebellar function can be reconciled. However, I have some points that I would like you to address:

1) During the whole study (Introduction, results and discussion). It was not mentioned at all the contribution of excitatory synapses to the modulation of coupling strength in the IO (Turececk et al 2014, Mathy et al 2014). For example, I saw you mentioned the study of Turececk et al, 2014. However, you seem to bypass the main findings of that paper which is that the activation of NMDA receptors enhance electrical coupling in weakly coupled neurons in the IO. Taking those results into account, you could also say that the observed enhancement of electrical coupling during the application of PIX in rats is due to an enhancement of excitation rather than absence of inhibition. I think both introduction and discussion sections should be rephrased in such a way that also should be considered the possibility of the effect of excitatory synapses on coupling strength in the IO. This is very crucial to keep in mind because it is known the close proximity of excitatory and inhibitory terminals to the electrically coupled dendritic spines in the glomeruli (De Zeeuw, 1990).

2) Figure 1, panel A: Do you think that the possible significant difference found between CBX and control conditions can be due to the possible direct actions of carbenoxolone on GABAa receptors (Tovar et al, 2009. Journal of Neurophysiology).

3) Figure 1, panel B: How can you explain the non-significant difference found in the gap junctional conductance between the control and PIX group?

Reviewer #2: The inferior olive is crucial for setting the activity level of the cerebellum, affecting (motor) execution as well as (motor) learning. The authors hypothesize that adjusting the coupling strength between inferior olivary neurons changes the dimensionality of inferior olivary output. The authors integrate (previous) experimental data with computational modeling to demonstrate that their ideas are biologically plausible.

Compared to other brain regions, neurons of the inferior olive (IO) have extraordinarily strong gap junctional coupling. This strong coupling has long been considered to be the basis of synchronization of IO output, and thus of complex spike firing by the postsynaptic Purkinje cells. However, it is now clear that the impact of coupling depends on the behavioral state of the animal and is strongly affected by anesthesia (see e.g. Mukamel et al., Neuron 2009). In awake animals, synaptic input to the IO strongly limits the impact of gap junctional coupling within the IO (see e.g. Negrello et al., PLoS Comp Biol 2019). Indeed, the authors noticed this impact of synaptic input (lines 159-160), but largely ignored this observation during their study. While their idea – controlling the dimensionality by adjusting coupling strength within the IO – is interesting and appealing, the use of experimental data from anesthetized animals restricts the interpretational value of this study. Several studies, using predominantly optical recording techniques, but there are some electrophysiological studies as well, have published complex spike recordings of multiple Purkinje cells in awake animals and related to specific tasks. Relating the ideas of the current manuscript to (at least) one of the awake datasets would strengthen the study.

In Fig. 3, the authors suggest – but do not show directly – that synchrony is linearly related to dimensionality. This seems an intuitive, if not trivial, result. It would help to show whether changes in synchrony that are physiologically relevant are indeed coupled to changes in dimensionality. This would demonstrate whether IO synchrony is indeed modulated during different phases of motor adaptation and learning, as suggested by the last paragraph of the Discussion.

The authors present direct comparisons of biological data and modeling data (e.g., Fig. 2). This comparison would benefit from providing more details (also in the Figure; not only summarized in the text). While the impact of the drugs on firing rate is obvious, more relevant measures like synchrony are not illustrated in a direct comparison.

In short, the authors present an interesting hypothesis: that the degree of gap junctional coupling controls the dimensionality / entropy of the IO output via modulation of IO synchrony. Their data suggest that the amplitude of changes in gap junctional coupling required for having an impact on dimensionality / entropy are realistic given experimental data. However, the experimental data are recorded under anesthesia, which is known to affect IO synchrony, and variations in coupling are only linked to pharmacological intervention and not to behaviorally relevant changes, clearly limiting the relevance of this study in its current form.

Reviewer #3: I apologize in advance if the review sounds harsh.

The paper puts forward a story about the putative functional consequences of an adaptive modulation of gap junctional plasticity in the inferior olivein the course of learning. It extends a well-validated model of inferior olivary cells to a network in order to study the effect of gap junctional modulation on ‘dimensionality’ of the system.

I have multiple problems with the paper in its current form, and I am afraid I cannot recommend publication in PLoS Comp. Bio. The argumentation in the paper is rather shallow and repetitive, mostly recapitulating earlier arguments but with scant new evidence.

The whole introduction reads as a discussion of the functional consequences of an assumption. It heavily depends on a particular theoretical framework, that which upholsters the view that cerebellar function is to perform a basic form of supervised learning. It uses the word dimensionality profusely, but confusingly. It identifies reduction of dimensionality in the control problem with the reduction of dimensionality in synchronizing coupled oscillators, though the logics for this step are not spelled out. Furthermore, the authors argue here (and previously) that a reduction of dimensionality is a desirable end state for motor control, but that is not at all a given, and depends very much on the type of control problem (for example, smooth grasp requires higher correlation between output control variables).

Major points:

1. Heavy usage of abstract terms with vague and weak links to actual problems of motor control:

- The use of the word ‘dimension’ is too loose. There seems to be a confused use of the term ‘reduction of dimensionality’, where the authors seem to assume that the reduction of dimensionality of an oscillatory system due to synchrony is akin to what happens in neural networks (such as in auto-encoders). This identification is highly problematic, because it is not clear which are the variables being encoded/decoded.

- Substantial differences are expected in coupling conductances in real networks. Small perturbations would be sufficient to create mounting phase disparities in the cell populations, unless these perturbations are correlated.

- In an oscillatory system driven by noise, the use of chaos as a functional property is contentious. The time scales where sensitivity to initial conditions would have a measurable effect are long with respect to motor behavior. More plausible sources of phase variability exist which are ‘transient’, rather than asymptotic, as required by the use of the term ‘chaos’.

- Chaos in a slowly oscillating system would only be measurable in longer time scales (>>100ms) . But in long time scales the sensitive dependence of initial conditions will likely be swamped under the effects of (1) input noise (2) intrinsic plasticity (3) short term synaptic plasticity (4) changes of rectifying properties in gap junctions. In the absence of parameter space analysis

- Many processes influence and drive synchrony, most importantly, input correlations. The level of synchrony induced by coupling is likely smaller than the correlations from GABAergic and Glutamatergic sources.

- “Low dimension components of error signal” Usually the error signal is assumed to be a coarse representation of an organismic-level event.

- Similarly, it is not always clear what the DoF that the paper is referring to. Motor synergies highly constrain the number of DoF in motor output systems. Only twitches and the like fit into ‘reduction of DoF’.

2. Cerebellum as supervised learning machine: tacit acceptance of the theory that supervised learning in the cerebellum is driven primarily by the CF related LTD.

- The idea is that reduction of coupling conductance and synchrony co-occurs with training in the system presumes that the main learning mechanism in the cerebellum is CS driven PF plasticity, i.e., supervised learning. Though this is a common belief, there are continued and substantial problems with the idea. Particularly as one attempts to create more complex control systems with more degrees of freedom in 3D space, the requirements for a motor error signal rise substantially. Though this is not the place for a full discussion of the problems with this assumption, given the number of years that the hypothesis had to prove its ability to explain high level control and acquisition, a more careful presentation of the theory is called for.

- The paper uses the literature to justify the assumption that GABAergic sources reduce coupling between cells. In the paper mentioned, they have not controlled for the effects of inhibition in the measured reduction of coupling conductance. It remains highly plausible that reduction of coupling is simply a consequence of shunting of oscillations rather than a direct effect on the coupling values. Hence, adaptation of coupling is not induced by GABAergic sources, but simply a momentary reduction of oscillatory amplitude.

**********

Have all data underlying the figures and results presented in the manuscript been provided?

Large-scale datasets should be made available via a public repository as described in the PLOS Computational Biology data availability policy, and numerical data that underlies graphs or summary statistics should be provided in spreadsheet form as supporting information.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No: At least not in the current submission.

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

Reviewer #3: No

PLoS Comput Biol. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1008075.r003

Decision Letter 1

Chris De Zeeuw, Daniele Marinazzo

15 May 2020

Dear Dr. Hoang,

Thank you very much for submitting your manuscript "Electrical coupling controls dimensionality and chaotic firing of inferior olive neurons" for consideration at PLOS Computational Biology. As with all papers reviewed by the journal, your manuscript was reviewed by members of the editorial board and by several independent reviewers. The reviewers appreciated the attention to an important topic. Based on the reviews, we are likely to accept this manuscript for publication, providing that you modify the manuscript according to the review recommendations.

Please prepare and submit your revised manuscript within 30 days. If you anticipate any delay, please let us know the expected resubmission date by replying to this email. 

When you are ready to resubmit, please upload the following:

[1] A letter containing a detailed list of your responses to all review comments, and a description of the changes you have made in the manuscript. Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out

[2] Two versions of the revised manuscript: one with either highlights or tracked changes denoting where the text has been changed; the other a clean version (uploaded as the manuscript file).

Important additional instructions are given below your reviewer comments.

Thank you again for your submission to our journal. We hope that our editorial process has been constructive so far, and we welcome your feedback at any time. Please don't hesitate to contact us if you have any questions or comments.

Sincerely,

Chris De Zeeuw

Guest Editor

PLOS Computational Biology

Daniele Marinazzo

Deputy Editor

PLOS Computational Biology

***********************

A link appears below if there are any accompanying review attachments. If you believe any reviews to be missing, please contact ploscompbiol@plos.org immediately:

[LINK]

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Authors:

Please note here if the review is uploaded as an attachment.

Reviewer #1: This new version of the manuscript has been improved substantially. You have answered all my questions and include my suggestions. Moreover, You have clarified some important concepts such as dimensionality which provides the reader with a better understating of the main message of this study.

I only have minor remarks that I would like you to address:

Legend of Figure 2: Should the CBX animal show ‘chaotic’ firing instead of ‘repetitive’ whereas the control animal should exhibit ‘repetitive’ firing and not ‘chaotic?.

Figure 3: Would you expect a different relationship between dimensionality and synchrony if you would extract the coefficient of variation of adjacent interspike intervals (CV2) instead of the average firing frequency in order to calculate the dimensionality of complex spike activity?.

Figure 3B: Can you explain why is there no significant difference between both the PIX and control group and CBX and control group?.

Discussion: The last part of the discussion is redundant when it’s mentioned the role of synchrony at early and late stages of learning. It can be more concise.

Reviewer #2: I would like to thank the Authors for taking the time to substantiate their manuscript.

Synchrony within the inferior olive, and thereby between complex spikes of downstream Purkinje cells, is an abundant phenomenon. In this paper, the authors argue that gap junctional coupling between IO cells can affect synchrony and that this in turn affects the dimensionality of the olivary-cerebellar system. Intuïtively, there has to be a relation between synchrony and dimensionality: all cells firing in perfect synchrony minimize and all cells firing chaotically maximize entropy. In the first round of review, I mainly referred to the biological relevance of the current study. In response to my question regarding the relation between synchrony and dimensionality (which was suggested, but not shown), the authors now demonstrate this relation in the new Fig. S7.

I am afraid that my concerns were not fully addressed. I remarked that such a relation between synchrony and dimensionality is (almost) trivial. I indicated that it would help to provide evidence that the synchrony varies under physiologically relevant situations to such a degree that changes in dimensionality become apparent. Related to this issue, I have also encouraged the inclusion of awake data.

In response to the latter remark (on the inclusion of awake data) the authors have expanded the Discussion, giving a better representation of the value of the anesthetized data used for this study. I would accept this solution. However, the question remains whether "IO synchrony is indeed modulated durig different phases of motor adaptation and learning" (see my remarks during the previous round). I do not feel that the new Fig. S7 helps to address this.

In other words, this study argues that IO dimensionality is related to IO synchrony (via gap junctions), and I agree with these conclusions, but leave me with the question to what extent this happens during real life. I would like to encourage the authors to address the expected variation in synchrony more explicitly.

**********

Have all data underlying the figures and results presented in the manuscript been provided?

Large-scale datasets should be made available via a public repository as described in the PLOS Computational Biology data availability policy, and numerical data that underlies graphs or summary statistics should be provided in spreadsheet form as supporting information.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

Figure Files:

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email us at figures@plos.org.

Data Requirements:

Please note that, as a condition of publication, PLOS' data policy requires that you make available all data used to draw the conclusions outlined in your manuscript. Data must be deposited in an appropriate repository, included within the body of the manuscript, or uploaded as supporting information. This includes all numerical values that were used to generate graphs, histograms etc.. For an example in PLOS Biology see here: http://www.plosbiology.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pbio.1001908#s5.

Reproducibility:

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, PLOS recommends that you deposit laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see http://journals.plos.org/ploscompbiol/s/submission-guidelines#loc-materials-and-methods

PLoS Comput Biol. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1008075.r005

Decision Letter 2

Chris De Zeeuw, Daniele Marinazzo

18 Jun 2020

Dear Dr. Hoang,

We are pleased to inform you that your manuscript 'Electrical coupling controls dimensionality and chaotic firing of inferior olive neurons' has been provisionally accepted for publication in PLOS Computational Biology.

Before your manuscript can be formally accepted you will need to complete some formatting changes, which you will receive in a follow up email. A member of our team will be in touch with a set of requests.

Please note that your manuscript will not be scheduled for publication until you have made the required changes, so a swift response is appreciated.

IMPORTANT: The editorial review process is now complete. PLOS will only permit corrections to spelling, formatting or significant scientific errors from this point onwards. Requests for major changes, or any which affect the scientific understanding of your work, will cause delays to the publication date of your manuscript.

Should you, your institution's press office or the journal office choose to press release your paper, you will automatically be opted out of early publication. We ask that you notify us now if you or your institution is planning to press release the article. All press must be co-ordinated with PLOS.

Thank you again for supporting Open Access publishing; we are looking forward to publishing your work in PLOS Computational Biology. 

Best regards,

Chris De Zeeuw

Guest Editor

PLOS Computational Biology

Daniele Marinazzo

Deputy Editor

PLOS Computational Biology

***********************************************************

PLoS Comput Biol. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1008075.r006

Acceptance letter

Chris De Zeeuw, Daniele Marinazzo

22 Jul 2020

PCOMPBIOL-D-19-01931R2

Electrical coupling controls dimensionality and chaotic firing of inferior olive neurons

Dear Dr Hoang,

I am pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been formally accepted for publication in PLOS Computational Biology. Your manuscript is now with our production department and you will be notified of the publication date in due course.

The corresponding author will soon be receiving a typeset proof for review, to ensure errors have not been introduced during production. Please review the PDF proof of your manuscript carefully, as this is the last chance to correct any errors. Please note that major changes, or those which affect the scientific understanding of the work, will likely cause delays to the publication date of your manuscript.

Soon after your final files are uploaded, unless you have opted out, the early version of your manuscript will be published online. The date of the early version will be your article's publication date. The final article will be published to the same URL, and all versions of the paper will be accessible to readers.

Thank you again for supporting PLOS Computational Biology and open-access publishing. We are looking forward to publishing your work!

With kind regards,

Laura Mallard

PLOS Computational Biology | Carlyle House, Carlyle Road, Cambridge CB4 3DN | United Kingdom ploscompbiol@plos.org | Phone +44 (0) 1223-442824 | ploscompbiol.org | @PLOSCompBiol

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Fig. Inferior olive firing data set for all animals.

    A: Spike data in 50 second of 10 representative neurons in 9 animals with the physiological conditions (CBX and PIX) in the right and the control condition (CON) in the left columns.

    (TIF)

    S2 Fig. Data selection by changes in the firing rate.

    A: the histograms of firing rate change by drug treatments (decreased by CBX and increased by PIX) compared to the CON condition. The red lines indicate the thresholds (50%) for selecting the neurons for analysis. B: pseudo-color maps show the firing rate change by drug treatments of the neurons in the micro-electrode arrays for six CBX animals (top two rows) and three PIX animals (bottom row). Red asterisks indicated the selected neurons, whose firing rate changes exceed the thresholds.

    (TIF)

    S3 Fig. Estimation of the conductances from spiking data using IO model and segmental Bayes algorithm.

    A: left, electrical circuit equivalents of the soma (S), dendrite (D) and spine compartments (SP) of a model IO neuron. middle, the connection of two IO neurons via a gap junctional conductance gc that connects the spine compartments. right, The IO network, which consists of 3x3 neurons, each of which is connected to its four neighboring neurons as shown. B: Left, five major features (FR = firing rate, ACG1 = auto-correlogram in 50 ms bin, CCG1 = cross-correlogram in 50 ms bin, MD1 = the first fraction of the minimal distance distribution, LV = local variation) extracted from spiking data of the three conditions (see [56] for detailed definitions of the features). Each feature was normalized by the mean value of the CON level. Right, the top two principal components of the extracted features. C: Flow chart of conductance estimation for each neuron. To account for highly non-stationary of the spike patterns in the three data condition, we divided the spike data of each neuron into small time-segments, applied the Bayesian inference to estimate gi and gc for every segment under the assumption that segmental estimates were drawn from a single neuronal estimate of a normal (Gaussian) distribution with unknown mean and the variance as a prior (left). The variance was optimized so as to maximize the fit of the data and the model in the PCA space. The posterior estimation for a representative neuron's gi and gc conductances (right). A broad probability distribution was found when the variance was relaxed but a much smaller distribution resulted when optimized variance was used [51].

    (TIF)

    S4 Fig. Improving the parameter estimates via Bayesian model-averaging.

    A: PCA error rates of the gi and gc estimates by the segmental Bayesian inference averaged for the entire IO neurons for CBX, CON, and PIX conditions for four different models (color bars) in comparison with the previous model (black bar, [56]). The error bars are of 95% CIs. B: Posterior probabilities of a representative IO neuron by individual models and the mixed posterior probability with the weights determined by the evidence of Bayesian inference. C: boxplots show estimates of the effective coupling geff of the three data conditions in the four individual models.

    (TIF)

    S5 Fig. Estimation of the coupling coefficient (CC) by simulation.

    A: We injected a current pulse of -1 μA/cm2 to a cell and recorded the steady-state voltage change of this “master” cell and its four post-junctional cells. B: We computed the CCs for hundreds of gi and gc values in the range over which the estimated conductances of the data distributed, and found a strong positive correlation between the effective coupling and the CC (R2 = 0.8, p < 0.0001). Note that the non-linear fit represents the nature of deriving geff from gi and gc following Eq (1).

    (TIF)

    S6 Fig. Dimensionality estimation for the spike data of ensemble neurons.

    A: Illustration of the principal component analysis (PCA) for the firing rate vectors extracted from 50-second windows of three neurons of Animal #6 in the CON condition. The estimated dimensionality d = 1.86 (dashed dark line, Eq 5), indicates that the approximately 2-dimensional subspace (shaded gray plane) can explain more than 90% of the variance of neural firing dynamics. B: Estimating dimensionality (Eq 5) with varied window lengths from 10–50 seconds for 9 animals in the three data conditions showing the robustness of dimensionality estimation against the window length. The error bars are of 95% CIs.

    (TIF)

    S7 Fig. Change in synchrony is linearly coupled with change in dimensionality.

    We first divided the complex spike data of 9 animals in the control condition into a series of shorter segments using a moving window of length 5, 10, 20, or 50 s, whose start incremented in 1 second steps. Next, we computed the dimensionality and the synchrony (both in 10 milli-second timescale) for each segment (A) and measured the correlation coefficient between those two metrics (B). The correlation coefficients were negative for almost all animals and window lengths (C). These results provide clear evidence that variations of synchrony within the physiological range are negatively correlated with changes in dimensionality. Note that the control data of spontaneous complex spike activity in our study is within the range of physiologically occurring synchrony levels. A: time-varying synchrony (left ordinate) and dimensionality (right ordinate) in successive, overlapped 10-s sliding windows of the control recording from animal #3. B: Scatterplot of correlation versus dimensionality values shown in A shows the presence of a significant negative correlation (r = -0.6). C: synchrony vs. dimensionality correlation coefficients for 9 control animals and all tested window lengths.

    (TIF)

    S8 Fig. Computation and validation of the complexity entropy method.

    A: Illustration of edit distance computation between two sampled spike windows shows a sequence of elementary steps that convert the spike window (a) into (b). Each bar represents one spike. Allowed operations include deletion of a spike (shown in red), insertion of a spike (shown in green), or shifting a spike in time (blue arrows). Computation of edit distance for continuous sampling windows for the entire spike train constitutes the edit distance matrix. Then, the recurrent plot is constructed by binarizing the edit distance matrix. The points, whose values are smaller than the threshold, were plotted as white dots, otherwise as black dots. Complexity entropy is computed as the inverse of Shannon entropy, in terms of frequency distribution of the length of the diagonal lines of white dots [69]. B–C: Complexity entropy measured for a total of a hundred of parameter values (black crosses) in noise-free simulations showed strong positive correlations with the largest Lyapunov exponent λ1 (regression model: λ1 ~ 1 + entropy, R2 = 0.4, F-test: p < 0.0001, S8B Fig) and the Lyapunov dimension DKY (DKY ~ 1 + entropy, R2 = 0.48, F-test: p < 0.0001, S8C Fig). Solid cyan lines represent the fit of linear models with 95% CIs (dashed cyan lines).

    (TIF)

    S9 Fig. Validation of the inverted U-shaped curves.

    We investigated whether intermediate couplings maximize the complexity entropy by applying a non-parametric Gaussian Process regression model, which does not assume an explicit relationship between the coupling and the complexity entropy. Still, we observed inverted U-shaped curves maximized at around geff = 0.033 mS/cm2 for both the model (A) and the data (B). In sum, these results support the inverted U-shaped relationship between the effective coupling and complexity entropy. The right ordinates of A–B represent the first Lyapunov exponents approximated from the simulation data (see S8B Fig), indicating that intermediate couplings induce chaos. The shaded regions are of ±sem.

    (TIF)

    S10 Fig. Effect of inhibitory synaptic input on complexity entropy.

    A: the pseudo-color heatmap of the complexity entropies, averaged across 9 model neurons, for all pairs of (gi, gc) in the range of 0–2 mS/cm2. The frequency of inhibitory synaptic input noise was varied in the range of 10–70 Hz. B: complexity entropy vs. effective coupling geff. In each model, a second-order regression (entropy ~ 1 + geff + geff2) fit was shown by black thick line. The second-order coefficient was negative and significant for all the models indicating the robustness of the inverted-U curve.

    (TIF)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: IO_Response_to_Reviewers.docx

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: IO_2R_Reply.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting Information files.


    Articles from PLoS Computational Biology are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES