Abstract
Cooperative breeding may be selected for in animals when, on average, it confers greater benefits than solitary breeding. In a number of eusocial insects (i.e., ants, bees, wasps, and termites), queens join together to co-create new nests, a phenomenon known as colony co-founding. It has been hypothesised that co-founding evolved because queens obtain several fitness benefits. However, in ants, previous work has suggested that co-founding is a random process that results from high queen density and low nest-site availability. We experimentally examined nest-founding behaviour in the black garden ant, Lasius niger. We gave newly mated queens the choice between two empty nesting chambers, and compared their distribution across the two chambers with that expected under random allocation. We found that queens formed associations of various sizes; in most instances, queens group together in a single chamber. Across all experiments, the frequency of larger groups of queens was significantly higher than expected given random assortment. These results indicate colony co-founding in ants may actually be an active process resulting from mutual attraction among queens. That said, under natural conditions, ecological constraints may limit encounters among newly mated queens.
Subject terms: Behavioural ecology, Animal behaviour
Introduction
Cooperative breeding is a social system in which organisms create communal nests, and it has evolved repeatedly in a range of taxa, including insects, fish, birds, and mammals1–7. In cooperative breeding, several adults engage in social behaviours that benefit both themselves individually and the group as a whole. This system may be selected for when ecological constraints (e.g., nest-site limitation, predation, parasitism, unpredictable resource availability) and competition greatly diminish the expected fitness payoff of solitary breeding. Cooperative breeding can result in greater nesting success because it enhances survival and reproduction, alloparental care, and/or collective nest defence8–13. Related individuals may nest together because they obtain fitness benefits, either directly or indirectly (i.e., via kin selection)14. Unrelated individuals may also nest together because they derive benefits arising from mutualism, reciprocity, and/or group selection15–19.
Ecological constraints on solitary breeding appear to be major drivers of collaborative colony founding in the four main groups of eusocial insects—ants, bees, wasps and termites20–25. In the majority of ant species, foundation of a colony is the deadliest phase of the life cycle because newly mated queens are exposed to predation, starvation, disease, competition, and adverse environmental conditions (e.g. desiccation). Colony founding events have a very high failure rate, as high as 99% in some species [26–29 and references therein]. Although new colonies are created by single queens (haplometrosis) in most ants, the process can involve multiple queens (pleometrosis) in several species20,26,30. Founding associations have been documented across a dozen genera from three different ant subfamilies20. Collaborative colony founding, hereafter referred to as colony co-founding, is usually carried out by unrelated queens; therefore, it is unlikely to have evolved as a result of indirect fitness benefits20,26,28,30.
In ants, colony co-founding enhances the productivity and success of incipient colonies because it increases queen survival during the early founding phase31–33; promotes faster brood development31–39; and boosts the size of the initial workforce, providing greater protection against intraspecific brood raiding, predation, and/or adverse abiotic conditions32,36,37,40–43. However, there is a cost associated with colony co-founding. In most species, the collaboration among queens is transient, and, after the first workers emerge, all but one of the queens are usually eliminated via queen fighting and/or culling by workers20,42. Co-founding a colony is therefore a risky endeavour: while the surviving queen will reap the full reproductive benefits of the colony, the defeated queens will have zero fitness. Thus, co-founding should be selected for when, on average, queens achieve higher fitness than they could have as solitary foundresses; conversely, it should be selected against when fitness benefits are significantly lower. Then, co-founding would result from random encounters when co-founding and solitary founding provide roughly equal benefits.
Although considerable attention has been paid to the benefits of colony co-founding in eusocial insects, the proximate factors underlying the phenomenon have remained largely unexplored. In particular, it is unclear whether co-founding results from a random process in which queens are simply tolerant of one another (i.e., there is neither attraction nor repulsion) or whether it results from attraction among queens. Studies have shown that group size increases with increasing queen density in some ant species43–45. However, whether or not such associations were random was unclear. A laboratory study of co-founding in the ant Lasius pallitarsis suggested that queen association resulted from random allocation, but mutual attraction and active co-founding could occur with large queen density45. In the tree-nesting ant Crematogaster scutellaris, the number of groups formed by queens under natural conditions did not differ from that expected based on random allocation46, suggesting that newly mated queens were not actively co-founding colonies. However, this study did not take into account spatial variation in nest-site availability or the density of newly mated queens.
Here, we examined whether colony co-founding could result from queens actively grouping together. We used the black garden ant, Lasius niger, as a model system (Fig. 1a,b). In this species, mating occurs during large-scale nuptial flights, where thousands of sexuals from many colonies gather for a few hours. Once mated, queens land in an unknown environment, lose their wings, and quickly find a nesting site (small burrows in the open soil or under stones). In about 18–25% of cases, groups of 2–5 unrelated queens co-found colonies42,47. However, after the first workers emerge, queens start fighting with each other. Ultimately, only one queen survives, and she alone benefits from the colony’s future reproductive success. An experimental study of colony founding in L. niger offered newly mated queens an asymmetrical binary choice of nesting chambers: queens could settle either in an empty chamber or in a chamber containing another newly mated queen42. The study found that queens did not display a preference for either scenario, supporting the conclusion that colony co-founding was likely a random process promoted by high queen densities. To better understand the forces driving colony co-founding, we explored whether newly mated queens actively nested in groups. To this end, we presented newly mated queens with a symmetrical binary choice between two nesting chambers that were both initially unoccupied. We investigated how queen number affected the grouping patterns of queens across the two chambers by carrying out experimental trials involving two, four, and eight queens. Queens were allowed to move freely between the two chambers. We compared the observed grouping patterns of the queens across the two chambers after 24 h with the expected grouping patterns given random allocation based on stochastic simulations. Under conditions of random allocation, there would be no attraction among queens, and the queens would have an equal probability of ending up in either chamber (p = 0.5). If queens were actively grouping together, frequencies of larger groups of queens would be higher than expected based on random allocation. Conversely, if queens were actively avoiding each other, frequencies of larger groups of queens would be lower than expected based on random allocation.
Figure 1.
Grouping patterns of L. niger founding queens. (a) Virgin winged L. niger queens embarking on their mating flight from their nest of origin. In the centre of the image is a male standing on the wings of a queen. Picture: Q. Willot. (b) After mating, queens land and then lose or tear off their wings. They subsequently search for small burrows in the ground in which they found new colonies, either alone or with other queens. Picture: H. Darras. (c) Queen grouping during one of the experimental trials: 8 newly mated queens have clustered in a single nesting chamber. (d) Proportion of observations as a function of the number of queens in the largest group sheltering within a chamber (dark grey), and theoretical distribution (light grey) based on the assumption of random assortment (see “Methods”). Experimental trials were performed with N = 2 queens (n = 34), 4 queens (n = 23), and 8 queens (n = 25). For example, three situations were possible in trials with 4 queens: 2 queens in each chamber (2); 3 queens in one chamber and 1 queen in the other chamber (3); and all 4 queens in a single chamber (4). The graphs only show results for experimental trials in which all the queens were sheltered (i.e., none remained in the arena).
Results
In the three types of experimental trials (N = 2, 4, or 8 queens), the vast majority of queens ended up in one of the two chambers (Table 1 and Supplementary Table S1). The mean proportion of sheltered queens did not differ among trials (Kruskal–Wallis one-way analysis of variance, P > 0.05).
Table 1.
Description of experiment and queen grouping patterns.
Experimental trial | n | # sheltered queens (S) (# trials) | Proportion of sheltered queens ± SD across all trials | % trials where queens sheltered in the same chamber | MWSE ± SD | P |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
2 queens | 38 | 0.92 ± 0.24 | ||||
2 (34) | 91% | 1.91 ± 0.28 | < 0.0001 | |||
1 (2) | – | – | – | |||
0 (2) | – | – | ||||
4 queens | 38 | 0.76 ± 0.36 | ||||
4 (23) | 78% | 3.78 ± 0.41 | < 0.0001 | |||
3 (5) | 60% | 2.6 ± 0.49 | < 0.034 | |||
2 (2) | 100% | 2.0 ± 0.0 | ||||
1 (4) | – | – | ||||
0 (4) | – | – | ||||
8 queens | 41 | 0.84 ± 0.26 | ||||
8 (25) | 76% | 7.76 ± 0.43 | < 0.0001 | |||
7 (5) | 80% | 6.40 ± 1.20 | < 0.0001 | |||
6 (0) | – | – | ||||
5 (2) | 50% | 4.50 ± 0.50 | ||||
4 (5) | 60% | 3.20 ± 0.98 | ||||
3 (3) | 66% | 2.66 ± 0.47 | ||||
2 (0) | – | – | ||||
1 (0) | – | – | ||||
0 (1) | – | – |
The table indicates the results of the different types of experimental trials (N = 2, 4, or 8 queens released in the arena at the start of the trial); the total number of trials of each type (n); the total number of sheltered queens (S) and the number of trials in which different values of S occurred (# trials); the mean proportion of sheltered queens ± SD across all the experimental trials of a certain type; the percentage of experimental trials in which sheltered queens were all in the same chamber; the mean number of sheltered queens in the largest group (MWSE) ± SD; and the probability of obtaining MWSE by chance, based on 10,000 simulations of random allocation outcomes. Experimental trials in which S = 0 or S = 1 were excluded when calculating MWSE and P.
First, we evaluated the results for the trials in which all the queens were sheltered (2 queens: 34/38 trials [89%)]; 4 queens: 23/38 trials [61%]; 8 queens: 25/41 trials [61%]; Table 1). Remarkably, queens grouped together in a single chamber in 91% of the trials with 2 queens (n = 34), in 78% of the trials with 4 queens (n = 23), and in 76% of the trials with 8 queens (n = 25) (Table 1). The mean proportion of sheltered queens found in the largest group was significantly greater than that expected given random assortment (P < 0.0001 for all three trial types; Fig. 1d).
Second, we evaluated the results for the trials in which some queens remained in the arena. After excluding trials with 0–1 sheltered queens, we were left with two situations: trials with 4 queens in which there were 2–3 sheltered queens (7/38 trials [18%]) and trials with 8 queens in which there were 2–7 sheltered queens (15/41 [37%]) (Table 1). As previously, the mean proportion of sheltered queens found in the largest group was significantly greater than that expected given random assortment (P < 0.034 and P < 0.0001 for trials with 4 and 8 queens, respectively).
Discussion
We show that newly mated queens actively formed groups when given the choice between two empty nesting chambers. This suggests that colony co-founding in L. niger is an active process that results from mutual attraction among queens.
Our results contrast with those from the few previous studies that have examined colony co-founding in ants, which assumed that the phenomenon resulted from queens simply being drawn to the safety of an enclosed nesting place rather than being drawn by the presence of other queens [see “Introduction”42,46,48,49]. One study specifically stated that there was no attraction or repulsion between L. niger foundresses42. The discrepancy between their findings and our findings may stem from differences in methodology. In our study, queens were given a symmetrical choice between two empty nesting chambers. In contrast, in Sommer and Hölldobler’s study42, queens were given an asymmetrical choice: they could shelter in an empty chamber or in a chamber that already contained a queen. However, the study did not make clear how the latter queen was kept in the chamber or whether the potential retention method affected the queen’s behaviour. Moreover, the sample size was small, so the probability of making a type II error (wrongly failing to reject the null hypothesis of random allocation) was high. Another possibility is that queens from different populations differ in their colony founding strategies, as has been observed in the seed-harvester ant Pogonomyrmex californicus: in some populations, queens found colonies solitarily, whereas, in other populations, unrelated queens co-found colonies30,50. In the latter case, the associations persist as the colony matures, which means that colonies are headed by several reproductive queens (i.e., primary polygyny)33. Colony founding strategy is correlated with aggressiveness in P. californicus queens, and aggressiveness and tolerance phenotypes are strongly influenced by genetics35,50–53. Although it cannot entirely be excluded, this scenario seems unlikely in L. niger since (i) queens sampled in different parts of Europe have been observed to group together in the laboratory [e.g.42,54–60] and (ii) collaboration among queens is unstable and always transforms into intense fighting when the first workers emerge, a phase that only a single queen survives.
Our study was time limited and restricted to the grouping patterns of queens after 24 h. Clearly, additional studies should help decipher the mechanisms involved in the nesting choice of founding queens. Among these, is the probability for a queen to enter a chamber a function of the number of foundresses already present? Does the time spent searching for a shelter influence the probability for a queen to join other queens? What are the exact behavioural interactions among co-founding queens? Do queens move between shelters under laboratory or natural conditions and, if so, does the probability of leaving a shelter vary with the number of congeners in the same chamber? Also, the density of newly mated queens was probably much greater than in the field, a situation that increased the likelihood of queens clustering in the same chamber. However, our results clearly show that the queens’ grouping patterns were not random; they indicate that there was mutual attraction among queens.
In L. niger, colony co-founding has been shown to confer clear demographic advantages, since multiple-foundress colonies have a higher rate of worker production than do single-foundress colonies47,55. The creation of a larger workforce within a shorter time period presumably enhances colony survival under natural conditions. Altogether, these findings suggest that the low frequency of colony co-founding in L. niger in nature (18–25% of incipient colonies)42,47 is due to a lower likelihood of queens encountering each other. This encounter frequency could be diminished by low local densities of newly mated queens, high abundances of nest sites, and/or the need for queens to move into the first nest site they find to avoid desiccation or predation. In addition, the propensity of queens to co-found could depend on intrinsic factors, such as body weight or size, metabolic resources and, ultimately, the probability of surviving the conflict during reversion to single-queen colonies20,45,54,61,62. Joining behaviour indeed appears to be influenced by queen condition in the ant Lasius pallitarsis, where heavier queens are significantly more likely to join others than lighter queens, consistent with predictions of improved competitive ability45. So far, queen condition was however not shown to affect co-founding in the black garden ant42.
In short, and in contrast to previous studies, this study shows that colony co-founding in the black garden ant, L. niger, is an active process likely rooted in strong mutual attraction among newly mated queens. It is possible that the same is true in other Formicidae. Identifying the specific mechanisms mediating this mutual attraction may be challenging from a technical perspective, but they should be explored in future research.
Methods
Newly mated and dealated L. niger queens were collected within a few hours of a large mating flight, after the queens had landed, in June 2011 in Brussels, Belgium. The next day, groups of N = 2 (n = 38 experiments), N = 4 (n = 38), and N = 8 (n = 41) queens were created via random assignment, then released into an arena (diameter: 9 cm) formed by a layer of plaster. Within the arena, at equal distances from the centre, were two identical circular nesting chambers (diameter: 3.5 cm, area: 9.62 cm2) (Fig. 1c). Both chambers were equally accessible. They were covered by transparent red filters (LEE Filters, absorption spectrum: 550–850 nm) that limited the light entering the chambers while still allowing the ants to be observed; a hole (diameter: 5 mm) was created in each filter so that queens could freely enter and exit the chambers. During the experiment, the temperature and relative humidity were 24 °C and 50%, respectively. The queens did not need to be fed during the experiment because colony founding is claustral in L. niger : queens seal themselves up in small burrows and rear their first brood by metabolising their wing muscles and stored fat.
After 24 h, we counted the number of queens in each chamber. In nature, queens that do not find a shelter within such a timeframe would most likely not survive.
Queen grouping patterns
To define the expected frequencies of grouping patterns under conditions of random allocation, we performed stochastic simulations in which queens were distributed across the two chambers (see Supplementary Methods for a detailed description of statistical procedure). We were interested in the variable S, which was the number of sheltered queens (defined as queens that had taken shelter in a chamber). For example, in an experimental trial with 4 queens, S could be equal to 4 (all 4 queens were sheltered), 3 (3 queens sheltered, 1 queen in the arena), or 2 (2 queens sheltered, 2 queens in the arena) (Table 1). Experimental trials where S = 1 or S = 0 were discarded since they were not informative. These simulations made it possible to calculate the mean number of queens expected under random allocation in the larger group (this group can be in the left of right chamber) of sheltered queens, for the experimental trials with N = 2, 4 or 8 queens. Using this approach, we examined two sets of data. First, we considered the data from experimental trials in which all the queens were sheltered (S = N). For each sample (N = 2, 4 or 8 queens), we calculated the mean number of queens in the larger group. Then, 10,000 simulations with random allocation of queens across the two chambers were performed, and we calculated the proportion of means simulated that were equal to or higher than the experimental mean. The null hypothesis that the queens’ grouping patterns arose from random allocation was rejected when this proportion was ≤ 0.05. Second, we considered the data from the experimental trials in which some of the queens remained in the arena (S < N). In this case, the small sample sizes for each grouping pattern (see Table 1) made it impossible to compare observed and expected queen grouping patterns. We summed the number of queens in the larger group of sheltered queens across the experimental trials for the different values of S. Therefore, simulations were weighted based on the number of sheltered queens observed (S) (Supplementary Methods). Comparisons between the results of the simulations and those of the experimental trials were realized as described above.
Ethics
Research on the species in this study does not require ethical licences. All experiments were performed in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations in Belgium.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by the Belgian Fonds National pour la Recherche Scientifique (grant numbers # J.0063.14, J.0151.16, and T.0140.18 to SA). We thank Florence Tourneur for her help with the experimental trials and J. Pearce-Duvet for her language editing services.
Author contributions
S.A. and J.L.D. designed the study, carried out the work, the data analysis and the statistical analysis, and wrote the manuscript. Both authors gave final approval for the publication.
Data availability
All the datasets supporting this article are included in the present work and the Supplementary Information.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
is available for this paper at 10.1038/s41598-020-70497-x.
References
- 1.Bourke AFG, Heinze J. The ecology of communal breeding: The case of multiple-queen Leptothoracine ants. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 1994;345:359–372. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Bourke, A. F. G. Principles of Social Evolution. Oxford Series in Ecology and Evolution (2011).
- 3.Cockburn A. Evolution of helping behavior in cooperatively breeding birds. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 1998;29:141–177. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Jennions M. Cooperative breeding in mammals. Trends Ecol. Evol. 1994;9:89–93. doi: 10.1016/0169-5347(94)90202-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Lukas D, Clutton-Brock T. Life histories and the evolution of cooperative breeding in mammals. Proc. R. Soc. B. 2012;279:4065–4070. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2012.1433. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Purcell J. Geographic patterns in the distribution of social systems in terrestrial arthropods. Biol. Rev. 2011;86:475–491. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-185X.2010.00156.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Wong M, Balshine S. The evolution of cooperative breeding in the African cichlid fish, Neolamprologus pulcher. Biol. Rev. 2011;86:511–530. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-185X.2010.00158.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Dugatkin L. Animal cooperation among unrelated individuals. Naturwissenschaften. 2002;89:533–541. doi: 10.1007/s00114-002-0379-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Emlen S. The evolution of helping. An ecological constraints model. Am. Nat. 1982;119:29–39. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Nichols HJ, Amos W, Bell MBV, Mwanguhya F, Kyabulima S, Cant MA. Food availability shapes patterns of helping effort in a cooperative mongoose. Anim. Behav. 2012;83:1377–1385. [Google Scholar]
- 11.Riehl C, Strong MJ. Stable social relationships between unrelated females increase individual fitness in a cooperative bird. Proc. R. Soc. B. 2018;285:20180130. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2018.0130. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Sharp SP, English S, Clutton-Brock TH. Maternal investment during pregnancy in wild meerkats. Evol. Ecol. 2012;27:1033–1044. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Taborsky M. Broodcare helpers in the cichlid fish Lamprologus brichardi: Their costs and benefits. Anim. Behav. 1984;32:1236–1252. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Hamilton WD. The genetical evolution of social behaviour. J. Theor. Biol. 1964;7:1–52. doi: 10.1016/0022-5193(64)90038-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Bshary R. Cooperation between unrelated individuals—a game theoretic approach. In: Kappeler P, editor. Animal Behaviour: Evolution and Mechanisms. Berlin: Springer; 2010. pp. 213–240. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Dugatkin LA, Mesterton-Gibbons M. Cooperation among unrelated individuals: Reciprocal altruism, by-product mutualism and group selection in fishes. Biosystems. 1996;37:19–30. doi: 10.1016/0303-2647(95)01542-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Keller L. Queen Number and Sociality in Insects. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Matsuura K, Fujimoto M, Goka K, Nishida T. Cooperative colony foundation by termite female pairs: Altruism for survivorship in incipient colonies. Anim. Behav. 2002;64:167–173. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Mesterton-Gibbons M, Dugatkin LA. Cooperation among unrelated individuals: Evolutionary factors. Q. Rev. Biol. 1992;67:267–281. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Bernasconi G, Strassmann JE. Cooperation among unrelated individuals: The ant foundress case. Trends Ecol. Evol. 1999;14:477–482. doi: 10.1016/s0169-5347(99)01722-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Itô Y. Behaviour and Social Evolution of Wasps. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Packer L. Multiple-foundress associations in sweat bees. In: Keller L, editor. Queen Number and Sociality in Insects. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1993. pp. 215–233. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Schwarz MP, Bull NJ, Hogendoorn K. Evolution of sociality in the allodapine bees: A review of sex allocation, ecology and evolution. Insectes Soc. 1998;45:349–368. [Google Scholar]
- 24.Shellman-Reeve JS. The spectrum of eusociality in termites. In: Choe JC, Crespi BJ, editors. The Evolution of Social Behavior in Insects and Arachnids. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1997. pp. 52–93. [Google Scholar]
- 25.Thorne BL. Evolution of eusociality in termites. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 1997;28:27–54. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Hölldobler B, Wilson EO. The Ants. Berlin: Springer; 1990. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Schmid-Hempel P. Parasites in Social Insects. Princeton: Princeton University Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Tschinkel WR. The Fire Ants. Cambridge: Harvard University Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Cole BJ. The ecological setting of social evolution. In: Gadau J, Fewell J, editors. Organization of Insect Societies. Cambridge: Harvard University Press; 2009. pp. 74–104. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Johnson RA. Colony founding by pleometrosis in the semi-claustral seed-harvester ant Pogonomyrmex calfornicus (Hymenoptera: Formicidae) Anim. Behav. 2004;68:1189–1200. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Tschinkel WR. An experimental study of pleometrotic colony founding in the fire ant, Solenopsis invicta: What is the basis for association? Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1998;43:247–257. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Jerome CA, McInnes DA, Adams ES. Group defense by colony-founding queens in the fire ant Solenopsis invicta. Behav. Ecol. 1998;9:301–308. [Google Scholar]
- 33.Helms Cahan S, Julian GE. Fitness consequences of cooperative colony founding in the desert leaf-cutter ant Acromyrmex versicolor. Behav. Ecol. 1999;10:585–591. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1999.1184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Adams ES, Tschinkel WR. Effects of foundress number on brood raids and queen survival in the fire ant Solenopsis invicta. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1995;37:233–242. [Google Scholar]
- 35.Clark RM, Fewell JH. Social dynamics drive selection in cooperative associations of ant queens. Behav. Ecol. 2014;25:117–123. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Offenberg J, Peng R, Nielsen M. Development rate and brood production in haplo- and pleometrotic colonies of Oecophylla smaragdina. Insectes Soc. 2012;59:307–311. [Google Scholar]
- 37.Rissing SW, Pollock GB. An experimental analysis of pleometric advantage in the desert seed-harvester ant Messor pergandei (Hymenoptera; Formicidae) Insectes Soc. 1991;38:205–211. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Sasaki K, Jibiki E, Satoh T, Obara Y. Queen phenotype and behaviour during cooperative colony founding in Polyrhachis moesta. Insectes Soc. 2005;52:19–25. [Google Scholar]
- 39.Waloff N. The effect of the number of queens of the ant Lasius flavus (Fab.) (Hym. Formicidae) on their survival and on the rate of development of the first brood. Insectes Soc. 1957;4:391–408. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Bartz SH, Hölldobler B. Colony founding in Myrmecocystus mimicus Wheeler (Hymenoptera, Formicidae) and the evolution of foundress associations. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1982;10:137–147. [Google Scholar]
- 41.Helms Cahan S. Ecological variation across a transition in colony-founding behavior in the ant Messor pergandei. Oecologia. 2001;129:629–635. doi: 10.1007/s004420100761. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Sommer K, Hölldobler B. Colony founding by queen association and determinants of reduction in queen number in the ant Lasius niger. Anim. Behav. 1995;50:287–294. [Google Scholar]
- 43.Tschinkel WR, Howard DF. Colony founding by pleometrosis in the fire ant, Solenopsis invicta. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1983;12:103–113. [Google Scholar]
- 44.Herbers JM. Nest site limitation and facultative polygyny in the ant Leptothorax longispinosus. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1986;19:115–122. [Google Scholar]
- 45.Nonacs P. Queen condition and alate density affect pleometrosis in the ant Lasius pallitarsis. Insectes Soc. 1992;39:3–13. [Google Scholar]
- 46.Masoni A, Frizzi F, Mattioli M, Turillazzi S, Ciofi C, Santini G. Pleometrotic colony foundation in the ant Crematogaster scutellaris (Hymenoptera: Formicidae): Better be alone than in bad company. Myrmecol. News. 2016;25:51–59. [Google Scholar]
- 47.Sommer K, Hölldobler B. Pleometrosis in Lasius niger. In: Billen J, editor. Biology and Evolution of Social Insects. Leuven: Leuven University Press; 1992. pp. 47–50. [Google Scholar]
- 48.Pfennig DW. Absence of joint nesting advantage in desert seed harvester ants: Evidence from a field experiment. Anim. Behav. 1995;49:567–575. [Google Scholar]
- 49.Tschinkel WR. Brood raiding and the population dynamics of founding and incipient colonies of the fire ant Solenopsis invicta. Ecol. Entomol. 1992;17:179–188. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Helms Cahan S, Fewell JH. Division of labor and the evolution of task sharing in queen associations of the harvester ant Pogonomyrmex californicus. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2004;56:9–17. [Google Scholar]
- 51.Helmkampf M, Mikheyev AS, Kang Y, Fewell J, Gadau J. Gene expression and variation in social aggression by queens of the harvester ant Pogonomyrmex californicus. Mol. Ecol. 2016;25:3716–3730. doi: 10.1111/mec.13700. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Overson RP, Gadau J, Clark RM, Pratt SC, Fewell JH. Behavioral transitions with the evolution of cooperative nest founding by harvester ant queens. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2014;68:21–30. [Google Scholar]
- 53.Shaffer Z, Sasaki T, Haney B, Janssen M, Pratt SC, Fewell JH. The foundress's dilemma: Group selection for cooperation among queens of the harvester ant, Pogonomyrmex californicus. Sci. Rep. 2016;6:29828. doi: 10.1038/srep29828. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Aron S, Steinhauer N, Fournier D. Influence of queen phenotype, investment and maternity apportionment on the outcome of fights in cooperative foundations of the ant Lasius niger. Anim. Behav. 2009;77:1067–1074. [Google Scholar]
- 55.Brütsch T, Avril A, Chapuisat M. No evidence for social immunity in co-founding queen associations. Sci. Rep. 2017;7:16262. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-16368-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Chérasse S, Aron S. Measuring inotocin receptor gene expression in chronological order in ant queens. Horm. Behav. 2017;96:116–121. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2017.09.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Dreier S, d'Ettorre P. Social context predicts recognition systems in ant queens. J. Evol. Biol. 2009;22:644–649. doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2008.01668.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Holman L, Dreier S, d'Ettorre P. Selfish strategies and honest signalling: Reproductive conflicts in ant queen associations. Proc. R. Soc. B. 2010;277:2007–2015. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2009.2311. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Pull CD, Cremer S. Co-founding ant queens prevent disease by performing prophylactic undertaking behaviour. BMC Evol. Biol. 2017;17:219. doi: 10.1186/s12862-017-1062-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Pull CD, Hughes WHO, Brown MJF. Tolerating an infection: An indirect benefit of co-founding queen associations in the ant Lasius niger. Naturwissenschaften. 2013;100:1125–1136. doi: 10.1007/s00114-013-1115-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Bernasconi G, Keller L. Phenotype and individual investment in cooperative foundress associations of the fire ant, Solenopsis invicta. Behav. Ecol. 1998;9:478–485. [Google Scholar]
- 62.Bernasconi G, Keller L. Effect of queen phenotype and social environment on early queen mortality in incipient colonies of the fire ant, Solenopsis invicta. Anim. Behav. 1999;57:371–377. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1998.0955. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All the datasets supporting this article are included in the present work and the Supplementary Information.