Abstract
Following arm amputation the region that represented the missing hand in primary somatosensory cortex (S1) becomes deprived of its primary input, resulting in changed boundaries of the S1 body map. This remapping process has been termed ‘reorganisation’ and has been attributed to multiple mechanisms, including increased expression of previously masked inputs. In a maladaptive plasticity model, such reorganisation has been associated with phantom limb pain (PLP). Brain activity associated with phantom hand movements is also correlated with PLP, suggesting that preserved limb functional representation may serve as a complementary process. Here we review some of the most recent evidence for the potential drivers and consequences of brain (re)organisation following amputation, based on human neuroimaging. We emphasise other perceptual and behavioural factors consequential to arm amputation, such as non-painful phantom sensations, perceived limb ownership, intact hand compensatory behaviour or prosthesis use, which have also been related to both cortical changes and PLP. We also discuss new findings based on interventions designed to alter the brain representation of the phantom limb, including augmented/virtual reality applications and brain computer interfaces. These studies point to a close interaction of sensory changes and alterations in brain regions involved in body representation, pain processing and motor control. Finally, we review recent evidence based on methodological advances such as high field neuroimaging and multivariate techniques that provide new opportunities to interrogate somatosensory representations in the missing hand cortical territory. Collectively, this research highlights the need to consider potential contributions of additional brain mechanisms, beyond S1 remapping, and the dynamic interplay of contextual factors with brain changes for understanding and alleviating PLP.
Keywords: Neuroimaging, Cortical reorganisation, Phantom limb pain, Preserved function, Multivariate analysis, Pain treatment, Use-dependent plasticity
Highlights
-
•
Technological advancements provide new insight into the neural basis of phantom pain.
-
•
Traditional mechanistic accounts of remapping in somatosensory cortex are incomplete.
-
•
Related contextual factors such as adaptive behaviour will contribute to brain plasticity.
-
•
A broader mechanistic focus beyond primary sensorimotor cortex is needed.
-
•
Plasticity and stability of the sensorimotor body maps may vary across time scales.
1. Introduction
Three decades ago it was first demonstrated that the sensory and motor maps in the adult primate brain can change as a consequence of injury as well as in response to training and stimulation (Jenkins et al., 1990; Kaas et al., 1990; Merzenich et al., 1983; Rajan et al., 1993; Sanes et al., 1988), and that these organisational changes are not limited to early brain development. This remapping has been attributed to an unmasking of normally inhibited connections between representational areas (Harding-Forrester and Feldman, 2018; Li et al., 2014), changes in subcortical projections to cortex (Jain et al., 2008) and even structural changes such as axonal sprouting (Florence et al., 1998; Jones and Pons, 1998) (though see (Chand and Jain, 2015) on changes after spinal cord injury). Subsequently, alterations in the organisation of sensory maps have been associated with a number of perceptual and behavioural changes, with many of them viewed as maladaptive, ranging from tinnitus to focal dystonia and phantom limb pain (PLP). However, these map changes have also been related to adaptive behaviors, such as improved sensory discrimination, advanced musical training and recovery from stroke (Flor and Diers, 2009). Here we review recent evidence on map changes in the primary somatosensory cortex (S1), and its association to PLP, based on recent neuroimaging studies in humans.
2. The sensorimotor homunculus – a neuroimaging perspective
Topographic body representation is one of the foundational organising principles in the brain. Multiple reports from the late 19th century observed that localised electric stimulation in animals evokes individualised movements of specific body parts (Ferrier, 1873). This work was refined by Penfield and colleagues, who reported a body-part map (somatotopy) along the human primary motor cortex (M1) (Penfield and Boldrey, 1937). Benefiting from verbal reports of their awake patients, Penfield and colleagues were able to infer that a second body map, relating to sensory perception, existed adjacently in the postcentral gyrus. These classical results, later elaborated in both animal (Romo et al., 1998, 2000; Tabot et al., 2013) and human cortical stimulation studies (Flesher et al., 2016; Hughes et al., 2020), established the role of S1 in eliciting key aspects of sensory bodily perception (e.g. stimulus modality, location, frequency and amplitude; note that S1 stimulation did not elicit painful sensations). More recent work, specifically focused on the causal role of S1, indicates that somatosensory perception might not be as heavily reliant on its processing (Medina and Rapp, 2014). Instead, S1 functioning may be particularly important for the consolidation of sensory and motor learning (Kumar et al., 2019; Mathis et al., 2017; Hong et al., 2018).
The consistent organisation of the sensorimotor maps was summarized in the canonical illustration of the human homunculus by Penfield and colleagues. However, despite this simplified illustration, the authors emphasised that the body map contains fuzzy boundaries, due to overlapping representations across body parts (Catani, 2017). Further work over the course of the 20th century revealed that although the motor map is best defined as crudely organised (Schieber, 2001), S1 contains detailed representations of specific body parts characterised by clearer boundaries (Kaas et al., 1979). The most striking, fine-grained organisation exists for the hand, where each of the digits and digit pads are represented separately and adjacently (Merzenich et al., 1983a), resulting in a detailed hand map.
The rise of neuroimaging allowed the investigation of somatotopic maps in the healthy human brain using PET (Fox et al., 1987), MEG (Nakamura et al., 1998), task-based fMRI (Glasser et al., 2016), resting-state functional connectivity (Yeo et al., 2011) and even structural neuroimaging techniques (e.g. DTI tractography (Behrens et al., 2003)). Unlike direct brain stimulation and recordings, human neuroimaging provided opportunities to identify body and hand representation beyond S1/M1 – namely in the cerebellum (Hahamy and Makin, 2019; Yeo et al., 2011), basal ganglia (Zeharia et al., 2015), operculum and insula (Brooks et al., 2005), supplementary motor cortex (Zeharia et al., 2012), occipitotemporal cortex (Orlov et al., 2010), parietal cortex (Huang et al., 2012; Zeharia et al., 2019) and more. Despite these findings indicating that body representation should not be studied in S1 in isolation (Longo et al., 2010), in the rest of this paper we will primarily focus on S1 body representation as identified with task-based fMRI (see Fig. 1 for an illustrating example). We will not consider the literature on M1 (re)mapping nor will we discuss the interactions of S1 changes with peripheral factors, although we acknowledge their importance in understanding S1 organisation and PLP.
There are several methodological considerations that have constrained the study of S1 somatotopy, which should be taken into consideration before interpreting this wealth of literature. First, due to the need for multiple repetitions of each condition, and the limited time-frame of a typical neuroimaging session, very few studies attempted to reconstruct the full homunculus. With few exceptions (e.g. (Saadon-Grosman et al., 2015; Tal et al., 2017; Zeharia et al., 2019, 2015; 2012)), most studies focused on the relative layout of several distinct body parts, and most prominently the foot, hand and mouth. This leaves potential gaps in the layout of the human somatotopy (e.g., it is still debated whether the hand representation neighbours the upper (Moulton et al., 2009) or lower (Kuehn et al., 2017) face representation). Other studies have focused on detailed investigations of one body part (most commonly the hand). Here the classical studies were restricted by limited resolution, making the dissociation of individual digits relatively noisy (Overduin and Servos, 2004). Recent advances in fMRI, and in particular techniques that provide increased signal to noise ratio and spatial resolution (multiband sequences and 7T MRI), offer new opportunities to overcome some of these technical issues. These new advancements allow for characterisation of the S1 hand maps with unprecedented detail: 7T fMRI permits identification of individual-digit finger maps with high intra- and inter-subject consistency (Kolasinski et al., 2016; Sanchez-Panchuelo et al., 2010). Structural techniques provide a first glimpse into the anatomical constraints of this organisation, for example, the hand-face border (Kuehn et al., 2017).
A further constraint for S1 mapping relates to the means of stimulation available, particularly when considering the restricted environment of MRI scanners, safety considerations, limited physical space and magnetic field distortions. Additionally, the peripheral nervous system has been shown to display high adaptation rates to repeated tactile stimuli (O’Mara et al., 1988). For these reasons, many researchers opted to using active paradigms, where participants are cued to move different body parts resulting in robust S1 activity (Makin et al., 2013a; Zeharia et al., 2015). However, this approach raises its own confounds, in particular unreliable delineation of S1 from M1 with standard acquisition resolution and pre-processing techniques. Moreover, movement-related S1 activity differs from passive sensory stimulation S1 activity, in that it recruits multiple additional inputs, including efferent signals from the motor system (London and Miller, 2013), deep cutaneous and proprioceptive peripheral signals (Grigg, 1994) and even cognitive or multisensory top-down modulatory inputs (Kuehn et al., 2018; Puckett et al., 2017; Shokur et al., 2013), though see (Berlot et al., 2019) and (Sanders et al., 2019) for similarities across multiple representational features of the hand and (Striem-Amit et al., 2018) for similarities in homunculus remapping between active and passive paradigms.
Non-invasive research in humans has also opened up new opportunities to study the role of S1 in pain processing, and specifically what are the organising principles of nociception in S1. Combined with electrophysiological research in both animals and humans, the evidence is inconclusive. While some studies demonstrated that painful stimuli are being processed in S1 (Tommerdahl et al., 1998; Vierck et al., 2013), other studies suggested that the observed activity does not reflect the nociceptive input per se, but rather other aspects relating to pain such as attention, salience or expectation (Bushnell et al., 1999; Mouraux and Iannetti, 2009). The spatiotemporal attributes of pain versus touch representation in S1 have also been debated, with some studies showing overlap in topographic finger representation (Mancini et al., 2012), while others identified differential organisation (Ploner et al., 2000). In the context of our review, it is important to note that nerve injury such as following amputation is known to trigger central sensitisation, whereby painful stimulation can activate S1 indirectly, due to plasticity in the dorsal pathway in the spinal cord (Devor and Wall, 1978).
3. Altered body representation following arm amputation
As highlighted above, the organising principles of the S1 somatotopy beyond nociception are highly ubiquitous. As such, S1 somatotopy provides an ideal model to address the question of brain remapping – can the properties of the map change, and in particular the boundaries between distinct body parts, once these have been established? Previous electrophysiology research in monkeys has identified extensive changes to the map features following amputation of a single digit (Merzenich et al., 1984), deafferentation of a nerve (Merzenich and Jenkins, 1993) or the entire arm (Pons et al., 1991). Here it was found that once neurons are deprived of their primary input, they become responsive to stimulation that activates the cortical neighbours of the deprived area. This activity change results in shifted boundaries of the body part map, termed cortical reorganisation. However, considering the key contribution of unmasking of already existing inputs in driving the shifted boundaries of the body map (Merzenich et al., 1983b), this description might be misleading. Perhaps the most dramatic demonstration of remapping occurs following arm deafferentation. Here the hand area of monkeys becomes responsive to inputs from the lower face – whose representation neighbours the hand area (though note that the activity in the deprived cortex is considerably smaller in amplitude to native face activity; see Figure 5 in (Kambi et al., 2014) for an example). Since hand and face inputs are segregated throughout the somatosensory hierarchy leading to S1, and since only sparse connections normally exist across the hand-face boundary (Chand and Jain, 2015), this process might reflect more profound changes to organising features of the body map.
In humans, research characterising lower face representation in unilateral arm amputees has not identified clear facial activity in the missing hand cortex (Kikkert et al., 2018; Makin et al., 2013b) (Fig. 3C). Instead, multiple studies examining activity associated with stimulation of the lower face (both actively (Foell et al., 2014; Lotze et al., 2001; Makin et al., 2015b; Raffin et al., 2016) and passively (Elbert et al., 1994; Karl et al., 2001a; Yang et al., 1994) have found that the centre (or spatial extent) of the lip cluster becomes medially shifted in the deprived hemisphere, particularly with respect to the opposite (intact) hemisphere. Critically, the extent of this lip activity shift associates with PLP intensity (Flor et al., 1995; Karl et al., 2001; Fig. 2). PLP is a sub-class of phantom sensations that amputees report as arising from their missing limb (Henderson and Smyth, 1948). While these sensations range in their specific characteristics (spanning an extensive range of tactile, proprioceptive and kinesthetic sensations) they are often also experienced as bothersome and painful (Kooijman et al., 2000), usually developing into a chronic condition, which is difficult to treat (Weeks et al., 2010). This is due to the fact that we still do not have a complete understanding of its neural basis (Aternali and Katz, 2019). As such, the observed correlation between S1 remapping and PLP intensity has opened up new avenues for exploring the mechanisms behind, and novel treatments for, PLP.
In particular, it has been proposed that the displaced facial inputs caused by the deprivation-triggered remapping prompt aberrant processing in the S1 hand area, which may in turn be interpreted as phantom sensation or pain arising from the missing hand (Flor et al., 2006; Yang et al., 1994). As mentioned above, it was found that people who experience worse PLP also show a greater shift of their lower face representation towards the missing hand cortex (Flor et al., 1995). A similar association was never established for non-painful phantom sensations, which have been in turn linked with frontal and parietal activity (Andoh et al., 2017). The finding of a positive association between S1 remapping and PLP has been replicated using multiple paradigms and participant groups (Diers et al., 2010; Lotze et al., 1999; MacIver et al., 2008). It provided first evidence that reorganisation of the S1 map bears functional consequences, maladaptive at that, with new opportunities for PLP treatment. However, it also opens up a host of questions about the potential relationship between brain remapping and PLP, in particular – how would displaced input to S1 interact with other pain mechanisms to manifest the pain sensation? What is the time-course of cortical remapping with respect to PLP? Is this relationship between remapping and PLP also found following remapping of other body parts into the missing hand cortex? (see (Mezue and Makin, 2017) for further considerations). Some of these questions are starting to unravel by neuroimaging studies that focus on neural changes following various treatment approaches to PLP, as described below.
Human neuroimaging studies have also examined the neural representation of the missing hand itself. As mentioned above, phantom sensations can manifest in the form of kinaesthesia – the sense of movement that amputees experience when volitionally trying to move their phantom hand (Weeks et al., 2010). Previous research has demonstrated that when amputees are instructed to move their phantom hand, this results in motor output to the denervated motor neurons as evidenced with EMG (Reilly et al., 2006), demonstrating engagement of the motor system. fMRI studies taking advantage of this simple manipulation have shown that phantom movements activate both M1 and S1 ((Raffin et al., 2012); Fig. 3A), albeit these original studies could not reliably differentiate activity profiles of these two neighbouring regions. It has been demonstrated that activity elicited by phantom hand movements in the sensorimotor missing hand cortex positively correlates with PLP – people who experience more chronic PLP also exhibit greater activity when moving their phantom hands (Kikkert et al., 2018; Makin et al., 2013b). This finding highlights that mechanisms pertaining to persistent hand representation may also be relevant for PLP, involving either bottom up (peripheral (Vaso et al., 2014)) or top down processes (e.g., from the motor system (Kikkert et al., 2017)). These associations, however, still await a mechanistic framework by which phantom movements relate to PLP and it is therefore currently not clear whether there is a direct causal relationship between persistent representation and PLP (Kikkert et al., 2018), see further discussion below).
4. Insights on remapping and phantom pain from clinical studies
The research on S1 remapping after amputation has yielded novel interventions that target the presumed maladaptive brain remapping. In mirror treatment, the intact hand is moved while the patient views it in the mirror and perceives the mirror image as the phantom hand, while the phantom hand moves along (Chan et al., 2007) or while no phantom hand movement was present (Foell et al., 2014). Diers et al. (2010) observed that not all amputees activated the cortical representation of the phantom hand during observation of phantom movements through mirror training, and those that did, experienced less PLP. In a later study (Foell et al., 2014), it was found that successful reduction of PLP by several sessions of mirror treatment normalised lip activity, as evidenced by the degree of representational shift away from the missing hand area in comparison to the intact hemisphere. Normalisation of lip activity was found to significantly correlate with treatment success. Interestingly, patients with telescoping – where the phantom hand moves towards the residual limb – did not significantly benefit from mirror treatment. This was potentially due to the disjunct of the intact hand viewed in the mirror and the telescoped phantom percept, creating a perceptual mismatch. Augmented reality applications can overcome some limitations of mirror treatment (Barbin et al., 2016), and have shown that patients with a telescoped phantom can also benefit from mirror training, as demonstrated by treatment-related reorganisation (Thogersen et al., 2020). In addition, activity of a region in the inferior parietal cortex, which has been associated with body image maintenance, was predictive of reduced PLP. The latter data suggest that the non-painful perception of the phantom also modulates brain changes and PLP in amputees. A further study (De Nunzio et al., 2018) used a combination of phantom limb movement and sensory feedback in a small sample of amputees, and observed a trend towards a change in lip representation after treatment. Similar changes were reported for motor imagery (MacIver et al., 2008). These studies show that S1 changes and treatment-induced reductions in PLP covary, but the direction of the relationship, i.e. if the brain changes are a cause or consequence of the change in PLP cannot be determined from these studies. Studies that can assess causal relationships are needed.
Further studies using brain stimulation techniques, such as transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) and direct current stimulation (tDCS), can begin to address the question of causality by inducing cortical change and potential alterations to PLP. TMS studies (as reviewed in (Nardone et al., 2019)) show that the phantom limb representation in motor cortex is shifted in amputees with PLP and that the representation of neighbouring body parts enlarged, in line with findings from S1. A recent tDCS study (Kikkert et al., 2019) investigated the consequences of successful PLP reduction and preserved phantom hand representation after stimulation over the S1/M1 missing hand area, involving a functional task (phantom hand movements). PLP reduction was found to correlate with reduced phantom hand activity, in contrast to the findings on mirror treatment. However, this could be attributed to different activation paradigms (active phantom movement versus mirrored movements), as well as use-dependent changes related to the intact hand which is engaged in the mirror paradigms (as discussed below). Importantly, activity of the posterior insula and additional regions involved in pain processing emerged as an important mediator of pain relief and activity changes relating to phantom hand representation. These pain-related regions have not yet been as actively studied in amputees with PLP, compared to patients with other types of chronic pain. For example, research of chronic musculoskeletal pain has been focused on widespread brain alterations and less on S1 (Lopez-Sola et al., 2017). In this context, the above mentioned activity could be due to a greater association of PLP with changes in sensory rather than affective processing (Fuchs et al., 2018; Larbig et al., 2019).
Other research studying the neural correlates of PLP relief has focused on peripheral contributions to PLP. For example, targeted muscle and sensory reinnervation (TMR) of the amputated limb, which involves connecting nerves leading to the amputated limb to specific muscles that can drive a prosthetic limb has been shown to reduce PLP ((Dumanian et al., 2018); though see (Aternali and Katz, 2019)) and there is similar initial evidence for intraneural stimulation (Petrini et al., 2019). Finally, brain computer interfaces (BCI) have been explored as modulators of brain activity and PLP (Yanagisawa et al., 2016). BCI’s are promising avenues for treatment as they can provide new insights by up- and downregulating brain activity in the phantom cortex and related areas, whilst examining associated pain changes. Here it is worth considering more global consequences of neuromodulation on somatosensation. To date, the relationship between amputation-triggered changes to S1 and altered somatosensation is far from clear, since no systematic studies exist that have related various sensory modalities to brain changes. Studies on psychophysical properties have found both enhanced and deficient sensory processing (Hunter et al., 2005; Li et al., 2015).
5. Additional contributors to remapping beyond deprivation
As mentioned above, remapping following amputation has been considered to be primarily triggered by the loss of sensory input, driving unmasking of weak or silent inputs (Merzenich et al., 1983b), and potentially promoting long-term potentiation mechanisms (Polley et al., 1999). According to this account, inputs that already have access to the deprived cortex, such as topographic neighbours, are more likely to be expressed in it following deprivation, resulting in a shift or expansion of an existing boundary. However, recent evidence in neuroimaging demonstrates violations of this rationale. For example, it was found that the residual arm, which is immediately neighbouring the missing hand cortex, does not remap into the deprived sensorimotor cortex in amputees, relative to controls (Makin et al., 2013a). Converseley, multiple studies have documented increased activity in the sensorimotor missing hand cortex for the intact hand (Bogdanov et al., 2012; Makin et al., 2013a; Philip and Frey, 2014; Raffin et al., 2016; Wesselink et al., 2019). Considering that the deprived cortex is normally inhibited by the intact hand cortex, it is difficult to resolve this finding with classical interhemispheric inhibition mechanisms (e.g. as suggested for stroke (Ward and Cohen, 2004)). Instead, it has been proposed that the remapping of the intact hand into the missing hand hemisphere relates to compensatory usage (Makin et al., 2013a).
It has long been observed that the receptive field (RF) properties of the remapped cortex gradually change over a long time-scale (Merzenich et al., 1983b). It has been suggested that the initial remapping triggered by deprivation will become refined by inputs due to daily hand usage involving compensatory behaviours (Churchill et al., 1998; Elbert et al., 1997). Since altered input is known to drive and shape brain organisation (Recanzone et al., 1992; Wang et al., 1995), it is reasonable to expect that adapted behaviour substituting for the missing hand function can shape remapping. This is akin to recent studies in individuals with congenital hand loss, who exhibit remapping of multiple body parts, which are used for compensatory behaviours, including the feet, residual arm and lips (Hahamy et al., 2017; Hahamy and Makin, 2019; Stoeckel et al., 2009) into the missing hand cortex. Considering that deprived cortex has been demonstrated to undergo network-level reorganisation in functional connectivity (Makin et al., 2015a), and that the connectome of the deprived cortex is wired to support hand function (Glasser et al., 2016; Graziano and Aflalo, 2007), inputs relating to typical hand function (e.g. manipulating objects) might consolidate more favourably. While still awaiting causal validation for a relationship between changed habitual behaviour and brain remapping in humans with congenital or acquired amputation (Dempsey-Jones et al., 2019), this potential process sheds new light on the classical findings. For example, consider the original studies in monkeys showing remapping of the mouth into the hand cortex. If these monkeys tended to use their mouth to substitute for their injured hand function, this could have contributed to the resulting remapping of the mouth into the hand area. It also provides new context to the aforementioned mirror-treatment studies which used intact hand movements to probe representations in the missing hand cortex (Diers et al., 2010). Currently, there is no systematic evidence that intact hand remapping associates with PLP, though few studies explored this potential link (Makin et al., 2013a; Philip and Frey, 2014; Raffin et al., 2016).
Prosthetic limb usage was also demonstrated to associate with cortical remapping and PLP (Lotze et al., 1999), but here again causality needs to be inferred with caution. It is known that individuals suffering from PLP are less likely to wear a prosthetic limb due to discomfort (Jang et al., 2011). It has also been demonstrated that individuals that use their prosthetic limb less (or more generally their residual arm (Makin et al., 2013a)), tend to use their intact hand more, presumably due to the need for alternative compensatory strategies. Any of these factors could potentially directly impact S1 organisation and beyond (van den Heiligenberg et al., 2018), or serve as an indirect moderating factor for a relationship between S1 remapping, PLP and persistent representation.
As already noted in section 4 on clinical studies, contextual variables also influence S1 remapping. For example, telescoping, which is associated with PLP and greater S1 remapping, was related to less PLP reduction and less changes in the mouth to hand cortical distance following mirror treatment (Foell et al., 2014). Other evidence for the instability of the S1 finger maps provide further considerations, which could impact the evidence for remapping observed in amputees. For example, acute pain (which will differ across amputees during the scanning session) has been shown to increase cortical distances in S1 (Buchner et al., 2000). Furthermore, Braun and colleagues (Braun et al., 2000, 2002) observed that the hand map can change depending on task requirements and attentional focus. These results confirm earlier suggestions that the fine features of the body maps in S1 are under-determined to an extent, and as such, could dynamically adapt to internal and external conditions within short time periods. Consequently, the context of the experimental paradigm and the details of the analysis could massively impact the boundaries of the body map being described by the researcher. This does not preclude high stability of finger maps under similar conditions (Kolasinski et al., 2016). Fig. 4 depicts some of the factors that can impact on remapping in S1.
6. Probing reorganisation using multivariate analysis techniques
Recent multivoxel analysis techniques offer a new lens through which somatotopic (re)mapping can be characterised by providing us with the means to quantify selectivity in terms of how distinct the representation of one body part is to another (for example, linear classifiers or the Mahalanobis distances observed between two representations in a given brain area). Moreover, these techniques provide the means to consider the type and extent of available information underlying the body map. The simple assumption here is that a brain area representing a given body part should contain distinct information about the functional features most relevant to the representation of that body part. For example, in S1 there are distinct neural representations of specific frequencies of tactile stimulus properties (Bensmaia, 2008), which might not be easily separable based on net activity levels, but result in distinct multivariate representational motifs (Kim et al., 2016). Representational similarity analysis (RSA) permits the consideration of functional attributes beyond selectivity of response. For example, it allows characterisation of inter-digit similarity or overlap (Diedrichsen and Kriegeskorte, 2017). Using this approach, it has been elegantly demonstrated that the canonical inter-digit representational structure in the S1 hand area can be best described in terms of daily hand use: those digits that form kinematic synergies to afford our daily interactions with objects and tools also show greater similarity in their representation (Ejaz et al., 2015).
7T fMRI and multivariate analysis approaches have been recently used to investigate S1 hand representation in amputees. With respect to persistent hand representation, 7T fMRI allowed us to uncover S1 digit maps, even up to three decades after amputation ((Kikkert et al., 2016); Fig. 3B). A complete digit map was also found in the missing hand cortex of an amputee with a brachial plexus avulsion (causing peripheral deafferentation), and was abolished when the participant was asked to imagine moving the phantom hand, rather than attempting to move it. As such, it appears that these maps reflect inputs from the motor system which are somatotopically structured. Multivoxel analysis techniques allow us to confirm that the representation of the missing hand, as probed using phantom hand movements (or at least attempted movements), shares the same representational features as a normal hand. In particular, we recently found that the unique configuration of inter-finger representational structure (previously linked with everyday experience), is entirely preserved after an average of 18 years since amputation (Wesselink et al., 2019). We found that across participants there was a significant relationship between the typicality of phantom hand representation and the sense of phantom kinaesthesia, specifically the number of digits that amputees perceived to be moving during the task. However, we found no significant correlations with either acute or chronic PLP, or the vividness of non-painful phantom sensations (as revealed in a linear regression model). In fact, typical hand representation was also found in the few amputees who did not experience any phantom sensations, suggesting it is a canonical organising principle, rather than a neural correlate of the phantom hand. A further study by Bruurmijn and colleagues, who used brain decoding to separate representations of multiple phantom hand gestures, was able to successfully decode the different gestures well above chance (Bruurmijn et al., 2017). Although decoding accuracy was reduced when compared with controls in the earlier sub-divisions of S1, this group difference was resolved in the higher sub-divisions (BA 1 and 2), indicating that the preserved representation of the phantom hand is at least partially maintained by higher order processing. Here again, no significant correlations were found with PLP (though considering the small sample size of n = 8, this null result should be regarded with caution).
Similar techniques have also been used to study the intact hand, specifically the increased ipsilateral activity that has been previously observed in the missing hand cortex. Previous research in two-handed adults demonstrated that the ipsilateral hand area contains finger-specific information which mirrors the contralateral representation of the same finger (Diedrichsen et al., 2013; Diedrichsen et al., 2018). This approach allows us to ask whether information content is greater for the ipsilateral representation of amputees’ intact hand relative to controls. In a recent study we found that ipsilateral inter-finger dissimilarity, underlying digit selectivity, was not significantly different between amputees and controls. This result indicates that the increased intact hand activity reported in amputees might not underlie increased functional processing (Wesselink et al., 2019). Similar evidence from stroke (Ejaz et al., 2018) and focal dystonia patients (Ejaz et al., 2016), also showed no changes in inter-finger representational features using RSA. This is in contrast to previous studies demonstrating that hand somatotopy is altered in these groups of interest (Flor and Diers, 2009), as mentioned in our introduction. Since this recent evidence demonstrates that information processing of inter-finger representation is unchanged, it is therefore possible that the previously documented map changes, which were based on measures of net activity changes, do not entail reorganisation of representations.
7. Concluding remarks
Our review shows that there is no simple relationship between somatosensory map reorganisation, PLP and preserved hand representation. This perspective converges with recent studies in related clinical conditions, for example complex regional pain syndrome (Mancini et al., 2019) and peripheral neuropathy (Maeda et al., 2014), showing that the previously assumed relationship between S1 remapping and pain requires further consideration. Although injury-related plasticity mechanisms may be the original driver of map changes, over time a number of additional factors such as use of a prosthesis, intact hand use, alterations in body representation as a consequence of amputation and processing in other regions, particularly with regards to pain processing, may interact with injury-related remapping. Furthermore, the close and bidirectional connections of somatosensory and motor cortex are also likely to impact S1 organisation. These interactions need to be examined and related to functional changes. Thirdly, the traditional focus on S1 remapping due to changed selectivity to particular body parts is probably incomplete, considering net activity changes might not adequetly reveal the underlying functional processing. Moreover, related research in monkeys (Kambi et al., 2014) and humans (Hahamy et al., 2019) indicate that sensorimotor cortical remapping might reflect changes in sub-cortical terminals (e.g. brainstem, basal ganglia) or cerebellum. Future research should aim to study PLP within this broader context, while taking into consideration the role of multiple brain networks and other contextual factors that can alter or stabilise the S1 body map at muliple time scales.
CRediT authorship contribution statement
Tamar R. Makin: Conceptualization, Writing - original draft. Herta Flor: Conceptualization, Writing - original draft.
Acknowledgements
TRM was supported by a Wellcome Trust Senior Research Fellowship (215575/Z/19/Z). The completion of this article was facilitated by a grant from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (SFB1158/B07) and European Research Council Advanced Grant (No. 230249) to H.F. We thank Victoria Root for assistance with proof-editing.
References
- Aternali A., Katz J. Recent advances in understanding and managing phantom limb pain. F1000Research. 2019;8:1167. doi: 10.12688/f1000research.19355.1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andoh J., Diers M., Milde C., Frobel C., Kleinbohl D., Flor H. Neural correlates of evoked phantom limb sensations. Biol. Psychol. 2017;126:89–97. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.04.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barbin J., Seetha V., Casillas J.M., Paysant J., Pérennou D. The effects of mirror therapy on pain and motor control of phantom limb in amputees: a systematic review. Ann Phys Rehabil Med. 2016;59:270–275. doi: 10.1016/j.rehab.2016.04.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Behrens T.E.J., Johansen-Berg H., Woolrich M.W., Smith S.M., Wheeler-Kingshott C.A.M., Boulby P.A., Barker G.J., Sillery E.L., Sheehan K., Ciccarelli O., Thompson A.J., Brady J.M., Matthews P.M. Non-invasive mapping of connections between human thalamus and cortex using diffusion imaging. Nat. Neurosci. 2003;6:750–757. doi: 10.1038/nn1075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bensmaia S. Tactile intensity and population codes. Behav. Brain Res. 2008;190:165–173. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.02.044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berlot E., Prichard G., O’Reilly J., Ejaz N., Diedrichsen J. Ipsilateral finger representations in the sensorimotor cortex are driven by active movement processes, not passive sensory input. J. Neurophysiol. 2019 doi: 10.1152/jn.00439.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bogdanov S., Smith J., Frey S.H. Former hand territory activity increases after amputation during intact hand movements, but is unaffected by illusory visual feedback. Neurorehabilitation Neural Repair. 2012 doi: 10.1177/1545968311429687. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braun C., Haug M., Wiech K., Birbaumer N., Elbert T., Roberts L.E. Functional organization of primary somatosensory cortex depends on the focus of attention. Neuroimage. 2002;17:1451–1458. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braun C., Schweizer R., Elbert T., Birbaumer N., Taub E. Differential activation in somatosensory cortex for different discrimination tasks. J. Neurosci. 2000;20:446–450. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-01-00446.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brooks J.C.W., Zambreanu L., Godinez A., Craig A.D.B., Tracey I. Somatotopic organisation of the human insula to painful heat studied with high resolution functional imaging. Neuroimage. 2005;27:201–209. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.03.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bruurmijn M.L.C.M., Pereboom I.P.L., Vansteensel M.J., Raemaekers M.A.H., Ramsey N.F. Preservation of hand movement representation in the sensorimotor areas of amputees. Brain. 2017;140:3166–3178. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx274. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buchner H., Richrath P., Grunholz J., Noppeney U., Waberski T.D., Gobbele R., Willmes K., Treede R.D. Differential effects of pain and spatial attention on digit representation in the human primary somatosensory cortex. Neuroreport. 2000;11:1289–1293. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200004270-00029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bushnell M.C., Duncan G.H., Hofbauer R.K., Ha B., Chen J.I., Carrier B. Pain perception: is there a role for primary somatosensory cortex? Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 1999;96:7705–7709. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.14.7705. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Catani M. A little man of some importance. Brain. 2017;140:3055–3061. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx270. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chan B.L., Witt R., Charrow A.P., Magee A., Howard R., Pasquina P.F., Heilman K.M., Tsao J.W. Mirror therapy for phantom limb pain. N. Engl. J. Med. 2007;357:2206–2207. doi: 10.1056/NEJMc071927. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chand P., Jain N. Intracortical and thalamocortical connections of the hand and face representations in somatosensory area 3b of macaque monkeys and effects of chronic spinal cord injuries. J. Neurosci. 2015;35:13475–13486. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2069-15.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Churchill J.D., Muja N., Myers W.A., Besheer J., Garraghty P.E. Somatotopic consolidation: a third phase of reorganization after peripheral nerve injury in adult squirrel monkeys. Exp. Brain Res. 1998;118:189–196. doi: 10.1007/s002210050271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Nunzio A.M., Schweisfurth M.A., Ge N., Falla D., Hahne J., Godecke K., Petzke F., Siebertz M., Dechent P., Weiss T., Flor H., Graimann B., Aszmann O.C., Farina D. Relieving phantom limb pain with multimodal sensory-motor training. J. Neural. Eng. 2018;15 doi: 10.1088/1741-2552/aae271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dempsey-Jones H., Themistocleous A.C., Carone D., Ng T.W.C., Harrar V., Makin T.R. Blocking tactile input to one finger using anaesthetic enhances touch perception and learning in other fingers. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2019;148:713–727. doi: 10.1037/xge0000514. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Devor M., Wall P.D. Reorganisation of spinal cord sensory map after peripheral nerve injury. Nature. 1978;276:75–76. doi: 10.1038/276075a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diedrichsen J., Kriegeskorte N. Representational models: a common framework for understanding encoding, pattern-component, and representational-similarity analysis. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2017;13 doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005508. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diers M., Christmann C., Koeppe C., Ruf M., Flor H. Mirrored, imagined and executed movements differentially activate sensorimotor cortex in amputees with and without phantom limb pain. Pain. 2010;149:296–304. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2010.02.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dumanian G.A., Potter B.K., Mioton L.M., Ko J.H., Cheesborough J.E., Souza J.M., Ertl W.J., Tintle S.M., Nanos G.P., Valerio I.L., Kuiken T.A., Apkarian A.V., Porter K., Jordan S.W. Targeted muscle reinnervation treats neuroma and phantom pain in major limb amputees: a randomized clinical trial. Ann. Surg. 2018 doi: 10.1097/SLA.0000000000003088. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ejaz N., Hamada M., Diedrichsen J.O.R. Hand use predicts the structure of representations in sensorimotor cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 2015:1–10. doi: 10.1038/nn.4038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ejaz N., Xu J., Branscheidt M., Hertler B., Schambra H., Widmer M., Faria A.V., Harran M.D., Cortes J.C., Kim N., Celnik P.A., Kitago T., Luft A.R., Krakauer J.W., Diedrichsen J. Evidence for a subcortical origin of mirror movements after stroke: a longitudinal study. Brain. 2018;141:837–847. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx384. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elbert T., Flor H., Birbaumer N., Knecht S., Hampson S., Larbig W. Extensive reorganization of the somatosensory cortex in adult humans after nervous system injury. Neuroreport. 1994 doi: 10.1097/00001756-199412000-00047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elbert T., Sterr A., Flor H., Rockstroh B., Knecht S., Pantev C., Wienbruch C., Taub E. Input-increase and input-decrease types of cortical reorganization after upper extremity amputation in humans. Exp. Brain Res. 1997;117:161–164. doi: 10.1007/s002210050210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferrier D. Experimental researches in cerebral physiology and pathology. Br. Med. J. 1873;1:457. doi: 10.1136/bmj.1.643.457. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flesher S.N., Collinger J.L., Foldes S.T., Weiss J.M., Downey J.E., Tyler-Kabara E.C., Bensmaia S.J., Schwartz A.B., Boninger M.L., Gaunt R.A. Intracortical microstimulation of human somatosensory cortex. Sci. Transl. Med. 2016;8 doi: 10.1126/scitranslmed.aaf8083. 361ra141–361ra141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flor H., Diers M. Sensorimotor training and cortical reorganization. NeuroRehabilitation. 2009;25:19–27. doi: 10.3233/NRE-2009-0496. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flor H., Elbert T., Knecht S., Wienbruch C., Pantev C., Birbaumer N., Larbig W., Taub E. Phantom-limb pain as a perceptual correlate of cortical reorganization following arm amputation. Nature. 1995;375:482–484. doi: 10.1038/375482a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flor H., Nikolajsen L., Staehelin Jensen T. Phantom limb pain: a case of maladaptive CNS plasticity? 2006;7:873–881. doi: 10.1038/nrn1991. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Florence S.L., Taub H.B., Kaas J.H. Large-scale sprouting of cortical connections after peripheral injury in adult macaque monkeys. Science. 1998;282:1117–1121. doi: 10.1126/science.282.5391.1117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foell J., Bekrater-Bodmann R., Diers M., Flor H. Mirror therapy for phantom limb pain: brain changes and the role of body representation. EJP. 2014;18:729–739. doi: 10.1002/j.1532-2149.2013.00433.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox P.T., Burton H., Raichle M.E. Mapping human somatosensory cortex with positron emission tomography. J. Neurosurg. 1987;67:34–43. doi: 10.3171/jns.1987.67.1.0034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fuchs X., Flor H., Bekrater-Bodmann R. Psychological factors associated with phantom limb pain: a review of recent findings. Pain Res. Manag. 2018;2018:5080123. doi: 10.1155/2018/5080123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glasser M.F., Coalson T.S., Robinson E.C., Hacker C.D., Harwell J., Yacoub E., Uğurbil K., Andersson J., Beckmann C.F., Jenkinson M., Smith S.M., Van Essen D.C. A multi-modal parcellation of human cerebral cortex. Nature. 2016;536:171–178. doi: 10.1038/nature18933. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Graziano M.S.A., Aflalo T.N. Mapping behavioral repertoire onto the cortex. Neuron. 2007;56:239–251. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.09.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grigg P. Peripheral neural mechanisms in proprioception. J. Sport Rehabil. 1994;3:2–17. doi: 10.1123/jsr.3.1.2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hahamy A., Macdonald S.N., van den Heiligenberg F., Kieliba P., Emir U., Malach R., Johansen-Berg H., Brugger P., Culham J.C., Makin T.R. Representation of multiple body parts in the missing-hand territory of congenital one-handers. Curr. Biol. 2017;27:1350–1355. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.03.053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hahamy A., Makin T. Remapping in cerebral and cerebellar cortices is not restricted by somatotopy. J. Neurosci. 2019;39:9328–9342. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2599-18.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harding-Forrester S., Feldman D.E. The Parietal Lobe, Handbook of Clinical Neurology. Elsevier; 2018. Somatosensory maps; pp. 73–102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henderson W.R., Smyth G.E. Phantom limbs. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 1948;11:88–112. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.11.2.88. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hong Y.K., Lacefield C.O., Rodgers C.C., Bruno R.M. Sensation, movement and learning in the absence of barrel cortex. Nature. 2018;561:542–546. doi: 10.1038/s41586-018-0527-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huang R.-S., Chen C.-F., Tran A.T., Holstein K.L., Sereno M.I. Mapping multisensory parietal face and body areas in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 2012;109:18114–18119. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1207946109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hughes C.L., Flesher S.N., Weiss J.M., Downey J.E., Collinger J.L., Gaunt R.A. Neural stimulation and recording performance in human somatosensory cortex over 1500 days. medRxiv. 2020 doi: 10.1101/2020.01.21.20018341. 2020.01.21.20018341. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hunter J.P., Katz J., Brain K.D. Dissociation of phantom limb phenomena from stump tactile spatial acuity and sensory thresholds. Brain. 2005;128:308–320. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh350. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jain N., Qi H.-X., Collins C.E., Kaas J.H. Large-scale reorganization in the somatosensory cortex and thalamus after sensory loss in macaque monkeys. J. Neurosci. 2008;28:11042–11060. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2334-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jang C.H., Yang H.S., Yang H.E., Lee S.Y., Kwon J.W., Yun B.D., Choi J.Y., Kim S.N., Jeong H.W. A survey on activities of daily living and occupations of upper extremity amputees. Ann Rehabil Med. 2011;35:907–921. doi: 10.5535/arm.2011.35.6.907. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jenkins W.M., Merzenich M.M., Ochs M.T., Allard T., Guic-Robles E. Functional reorganizaton of primary somatosensory cortex in adult owl monkeys after behaviorally controlled tactile stimulation. J. Neurophysiol. 1990;63:82–104. doi: 10.1152/jn.1990.63.1.82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones E.G., Pons T.P. Thalamic and brainstem contributions to large-scale plasticity of primate somatosensory cortex. Science. 1998;282:1121–1125. doi: 10.1126/science.282.5391.1121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaas J.H., Krubitzer L.A., Chino Y.M., Langston A.L., Polley E.H., Blair N. Reorganization of retinotopic cortical maps in adult mammals after lesions of the retina. Science. 1990;248:229–231. doi: 10.1126/science.2326637. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaas J.H., Nelson R.J., Sur M., Lin C.S., Merzenich M.M. Multiple representations of the body within the primary somatosensory cortex of primates. Science. 1979;204:521–523. doi: 10.1126/science.107591. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kambi N., Halder P., Rajan R., Arora V., Chand P., Arora M., Jain N. Large-scale reorganization of the somatosensory cortex following spinal cord injuries is due to brainstem plasticity. Nat. Commun. 2014;5:3602. doi: 10.1038/ncomms4602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karl A., Birbaumer N., Lutzenberger W., Cohen L.G., Flor H. Reorganization of motor and somatosensory cortex in upper extremity amputees with phantom limb pain. J. Neurosci. 2001;21:3609–3618. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-10-03609.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kikkert S., Johansen-Berg H., Tracey I., Makin T.R. Reaffirming the link between chronic phantom limb pain and maintained missing hand representation. Cortex. 2018;106:174–184. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.05.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kikkert S., Kolasinski J., Jbabdi S., Tracey I., Beckmann C.F., Johansen-Berg H., Makin T.R., Stephan K.E. Revealing the neural fingerprints of a missing hand. eLife. 2016;5 doi: 10.7554/eLife.15292. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kikkert S., Mezue M., O’Shea J., Henderson Slater D., Johansen-Berg H., Tracey I., Makin T.R. Neural basis of induced phantom limb pain relief. Ann. Neurol. 2019;85:59–73. doi: 10.1002/ana.25371. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kikkert S., Mezue M., Slater D.H., Johansen-Berg H., Tracey I., Makin T.R. Motor correlates of phantom limb pain. Cortex. 2017;95:29–36. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.07.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim J., Chung Y.G., Chung S.-C., Bülthoff H.H., Kim S.-P. Neural categorization of vibrotactile frequency in flutter and vibration stimulations: an fMRI study. IEEE Trans Haptics. 2016;9:455–464. doi: 10.1109/TOH.2016.2593727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kolasinski J., Makin T.R., Jbabdi S., Clare S., Stagg C.J., Johansen-Berg H. Investigating the stability of fine-grain digit somatotopy in individual human participants. J. Neurosci. 2016;36:1113–1127. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1742-15.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kooijman C.M., Dijkstra P.U., Geertzen J.H.B., Elzinga A., van der Schans C.P. Phantom pain and phantom sensations in upper limb amputees: an epidemiological study. Pain. 2000;87:33–41. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(00)00264-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuehn E., Dinse J., Jakobsen E., Long X., Schäfer A., Bazin P.-L., Villringer A., Sereno M.I., Margulies D.S. Body topography parcellates human sensory and motor cortex. Cerebr. Cortex. 2017;27:3790–3805. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuehn E., Haggard P., Villringer A., Pleger B., Sereno M.I. Visually-Driven maps in area 3b. J. Neurosci. 2018;38:1295–1310. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0491-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumar N., Manning T.F., Ostry D.J. Somatosensory cortex participates in the consolidation of human motor memory. PLoS Biol. 2019;17 doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3000469. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Larbig W., Andoh J., Huse E., Stahl-Corino D., Montoya P., Seltzer Z., Flor H. Pre- and postoperative predictors of phantom limb pain. Neurosci. Lett. 2019;702:44–50. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2018.11.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li L., Gainey M.A., Goldbeck J.E., Feldman D.E. Rapid homeostasis by disinhibition during whisker map plasticity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 2014;111:1616–1621. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1312455111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li S., Melton D.H., Li S. Tactile, thermal, and electrical thresholds in patients with and without phantom limb pain after traumatic lower limb amputation. J. Pain Res. 2015;8:169–174. doi: 10.2147/JPR.S77412. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- London B.M., Miller L.E. Responses of somatosensory area 2 neurons to actively and passively generated limb movements. J. Neurophysiol. 2013;109:1505–1513. doi: 10.1152/jn.00372.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Longo M.R., Azañón E., Haggard P. More than skin deep: body representation beyond primary somatosensory cortex. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48:655–668. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.08.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- López-Solà M., Woo C.W., Pujol J., Deus J., Harrison B.J., Monfort J., Wager T.D. Towards a neurophysiological signature for fibromyalgia. Pain. 2017;158:134–147. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000707. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lotze M., Flor H., Grodd W., Larbig W., Birbaumer N. Phantom movements and pain. An fMRI study in upper limb amputees. Brain. 2001;124:2268–2277. doi: 10.1093/brain/124.11.2268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lotze M., Grodd W., Birbaumer N., Erb M., Huse E., Flor H. Does use of a myoelectric prosthesis prevent cortical reorganization and phantom limb pain? Nat. Neurosci. 1999;2:501–502. doi: 10.1038/9145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maeda Y., Kettner N., Holden J., Lee J., Kim J., Cina S., Malatesta C., Gerber J., McManus C., Im J., Libby A., Mezzacappa P., Morse L.R., Park K., Audette J., Tommerdahl M., Napadow V. Functional deficits in carpal tunnel syndrome reflect reorganization of primary somatosensory cortex. Brain. 2014;137:1741–1752. doi: 10.1093/brain/awu096. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacIver K., Lloyd D.M., Kelly S., Roberts N., Nurmikko T. Phantom limb pain, cortical reorganization and the therapeutic effect of mental imagery. Brain. 2008;131:2181–2191. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Makin T.R., Cramer A.O., Scholz J., Hahamy A., Slater D.H., Tracey I., Johansen-Berg H. Deprivation-related and use-dependent plasticity go hand in hand. eLife. 2013;2 doi: 10.7554/eLife.01273. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Makin T.R., Filippini N., Duff E.P., Henderson Slater D., Tracey I., Johansen-Berg H. Network-level reorganisation of functional connectivity following arm amputation. Neuroimage. 2015;114:217–225. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Makin T.R., Scholz J., Filippini N., Henderson Slater D., Tracey I., Johansen-Berg H. Phantom pain is associated with preserved structure and function in the former hand area. Nat. Commun. 2013;4:1570. doi: 10.1038/ncomms2571. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Makin T.R., Scholz J., Henderson Slater D., Johansen-Berg H., Tracey I. Reassessing cortical reorganization in the primary sensorimotor cortex following arm amputation. Brain. 2015;138:2140–2146. doi: 10.1093/brain/awv161. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mancini F., Haggard P., Iannetti G.D., Longo M.R., Sereno M.I. Fine-grained nociceptive maps in primary somatosensory cortex. J. Neurosci. 2012;32:17155–17162. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3059-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mancini F., Wang A.P., Schira M.M., Isherwood Z.J., McAuley J.H., Iannetti G.D., Sereno M.I., Moseley G.L., Rae C.D. Fine-grained mapping of cortical somatotopies in chronic complex regional pain syndrome. J. Neurosci. 2019;39:9185–9196. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2005-18.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mathis M.W., Mathis A., Uchida N. Somatosensory cortex plays an essential role in forelimb motor adaptation in mice. Neuron. 2017;93:1493–1503. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.02.049. e6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Medina J., Rapp B. Rapid experience-dependent plasticity following somatosensory damage. Curr. Biol. 2014;24:677–680. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.01.070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merzenich M.M., Jenkins W.M. Reorganization of cortical representations of the hand following alterations of skin inputs induced by nerve injury, skin island transfers, and experience. J. Hand Ther. 1993;6:89–104. doi: 10.1016/S0894-1130(12)80290-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merzenich M.M., Kaas J.H., Wall J., Nelson R.J., Sur M., Felleman D. Topographic reorganization of somatosensory cortical areas 3b and 1 in adult monkeys following restricted deafferentiation. Neuroscience. 1983;8:33–55. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(83)90024-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merzenich M.M., Kaas J.H., Wall J.T., Sur M., Nelson R.J., Felleman D.J. Progression of change following median nerve section in the cortical representation of the hand in areas 3b and 1 in adult owl and squirrel monkeys. Neuroscience. 1983;10:639–665. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(83)90208-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merzenich M.M., Nelson R.J., Stryker M.P., Cynader M.S., Schoppmann A., Zook J.M. Somatosensory cortical map changes following digit amputation in adult monkeys. J. Comp. Neurol. 1984;224:591–605. doi: 10.1002/cne.902240408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mezue M., Makin T.R. Immutable body representations: lessons from phantoms in amputees. In: De Vignemont F., Alsmith A., editors. The Subject’s Matter: Self-Consciousness and the Body. MIT press; 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Moulton E.A., Pendse G., Morris S., Aiello-Lammens M., Becerra L., Borsook D. Segmentally arranged somatotopy within the face representation of human primary somatosensory cortex. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2009;30:757–765. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20541. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mouraux A., Iannetti G.D. Nociceptive laser-evoked brain potentials do not reflect nociceptive-specific neural activity. J. Neurophysiol. 2009;101:3258–3269. doi: 10.1152/jn.91181.2008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nakamura A., Yamada T., Goto A., Kato T., Ito K., Abe Y., Kachi T., Kakigi R. Somatosensory homunculus as drawn by MEG. Neuroimage. 1998;7:377–386. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0332. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nardone R., Versace V., Sebastianelli L., Brigo F., Christova M., Scarano G.I., Saltuari L., Trinka E., Hauer L., Sellner J. Transcranial magnetic stimulation in subjects with phantom pain and non-painful phantom sensations: a systematic review. Brain Res. Bull. 2019;148:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2019.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Mara S., Rowe M.J., Travin R.P.C. Neural mechanisms in vibrotactile adaptation. J. Neurophysiol. 1988;59:607–622. doi: 10.1152/jn.1988.59.2.607. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orlov T., Makin T.R., Zohary E. Topographic representation of the human body in the occipitotemporal cortex. Neuron. 2010;68:586–600. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.09.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Overduin S.A., Servos P. Distributed digit somatotopy in primary somatosensory cortex. Neuroimage. 2004;23:462–472. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Penfield W., Boldrey E. Somatic motor and sensory representation in the cerebral cortex of man as studied by electrical stimulation. Brain. 1937;60:389–443. doi: 10.1093/brain/60.4.389. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Petrini F.M., Valle G., Strauss I., Granata G., Di Iorio R., D’Anna E., Čvančara P., Mueller M., Carpaneto J., Clemente F., Controzzi M., Bisoni L., Carboni C., Barbaro M., Iodice F., Andreu D., Hiairrassary A., Divoux J.-L., Cipriani C., Guiraud D., Raffo L., Fernandez E., Stieglitz T., Raspopovic S., Rossini P.M., Micera S. Six-month assessment of a hand prosthesis with intraneural tactile feedback. Ann. Neurol. 2019;85:137–154. doi: 10.1002/ana.25384. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Philip B.A., Frey S.H. Compensatory changes accompanying chronic forced use of the nondominant hand by unilateral amputees. J. Neurosci. 2014;34:3622–3631. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3770-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ploner M., Schmitz F., Freund H.J., Schnitzler A. Differential organization of touch and pain in human primary somatosensory cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 2000;83:1770–1776. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.83.3.1770. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Polley D.B., Chen-Bee C.H., Frostig R.D. Two directions of plasticity in the sensory-deprived adult cortex. Neuron. 1999;24:623–637. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)81117-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pons T.P., Garraghty P.E., Ommaya A.K., Kaas J.H., Taub E., Mishkin M. Massive cortical reorganization after sensory deafferentation in adult macaques. Science. 1991;252:1857–1860. doi: 10.1126/science.1843843. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Puckett A.M., Bollmann S., Barth M., Cunnington R. Measuring the effects of attention to individual fingertips in somatosensory cortex using ultra-high field (7T) fMRI. Neuroimage. 2017;161:179–187. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.08.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raffin E., Mattout J., Reilly K.T., Giraux P. Disentangling motor execution from motor imagery with the phantom limb. Brain. 2012;135:582–595. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raffin E., Richard N., Giraux P., Reilly K.T. Primary motor cortex changes after amputation correlate with phantom limb pain and the ability to move the phantom limb. Neuroimage. 2016;130:134–144. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.01.063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rajan R., Irvine D.R., Wise L.Z., Heil P. Effect of unilateral partial cochlear lesions in adult cats on the representation of lesioned and unlesioned cochleas in primary auditory cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 1993;338:17–49. doi: 10.1002/cne.903380104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Recanzone G.H., Merzenich M.M., Jenkins W.M., Grajski K.A., Dinse H.R. Topographic reorganization of the hand representation in cortical area 3b owl monkeys trained in a frequency-discrimination task. J. Neurophysiol. 1992;67:1031–1056. doi: 10.1152/jn.1992.67.5.1031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reilly K.T., Mercier C., Schieber M.H., Sirigu A. Persistent hand motor commands in the amputees’ brain. Brain. 2006;129:2211–2223. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl154. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Romo Ranulfo, Hernández Adrián, Zainos Anótonio, Salinas Emilio. Somatosensory discrimination based on cortical microstimulation. Nature. 1998;392:387–390. doi: 10.1038/32891. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Romo Ranulfo, Hernández Adrián, Zainos Antonio, Brody Carlos D., Lemus Luis. Psychophysical performance based on cortical microstimulation. Neuron. 2000;26(1):273–278. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)81156-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saadon-Grosman N., Tal Z., Itshayek E., Amedi A., Arzy S. Discontinuity of cortical gradients reflects sensory impairment. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 2015;112:16024–16029. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1506214112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanchez-Panchuelo R.M., Francis S., Bowtell R., Schluppeck D. Mapping human somatosensory cortex in individual subjects with 7T functional MRI. J. Neurophysiol. 2010;103:2544–2556. doi: 10.1152/jn.01017.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanes J.N., Suner S., Lando J.F., Donoghue J.P. Rapid reorganization of adult rat motor cortex somatic representation patterns after motor nerve injury. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 1988;85:2003–2007. doi: 10.1073/pnas.85.6.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanders Z.-B., Wesselink D.B., Dempsey-Jones H., Makin T.R. bioRxiv; 2019. Similar Somatotopy for Active and Passive Digit Representation in Primary Somatosensory Cortex. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schieber M.H. Constraints on somatotopic organization in the primary motor cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 2001;86:2125–2143. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.86.5.2125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shokur S., O’Doherty J.E., Winans J.A., Bleuler H., Lebedev M.A., Nicolelis M.A.L. Expanding the primate body schema in sensorimotor cortex by virtual touches of an avatar. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 2013;110:15121–15126. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1308459110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stoeckel M.C., Seitz R.J., Buetefisch C.M. Congenitally altered motor experience alters somatotopic organization of human primary motor cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 2009;106:2395–2400. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0803733106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Striem-Amit E., Vannuscorps G., Caramazza A. Plasticity based on compensatory effector use in the association but not primary sensorimotor cortex of people born without hands. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 2018;115:7801–7806. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1803926115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tabot G.A., Dammann J.F., Berg J.A., Tenore F.V., Boback J.L., Vogelstein R.J., Bensmaia S.J. Restoring the sense of touch with a prosthetic hand through a brain interface. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 2013;110:18279–18284. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1221113110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tal Z., Geva R., Amedi A. Positive and negative somatotopic BOLD responses in contralateral versus ipsilateral Penfield homunculus. Cerebr. Cortex. 2017;27:962–980. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thøgersen M., Andoh J., Milde C., Graven-Nielsen T., Flor H., Petrini L. Individualized augmented reality training reduces phantom pain and cortical reorganization in amputees: a proof of concept study. J. Pain. 2020 doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2020.06.002. (in press) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tommerdahl M., Delemos K.A., Favorov O.V., Metz C.B., Vierck C.J., Whitsel B.L. Response of anterior parietal cortex to different modes of same-site skin stimulation. J. Neurophysiol. 1998;80:3272–3283. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.80.6.3272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van den Heiligenberg F.M.Z., Orlov T., Macdonald S.N., Duff E.P., Henderson Slater D., Beckmann C.F., Johansen-Berg H., Culham J.C., Makin T.R. Artificial limb representation in amputees. Brain. 2018;141:1422–1433. doi: 10.1093/brain/awy054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vaso A., Adahan H.-M., Gjika A., Zahaj S., Zhurda T., Vyshka G., Devor M. Peripheral nervous system origin of phantom limb pain. Pain. 2014;155:1384–1391. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2014.04.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vierck C.J., Whitsel B.L., Favorov O.V., Brown A.W., Tommerdahl M. Role of primary somatosensory cortex in the coding of pain. Pain. 2013;154:334–344. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2012.10.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang X., Merzenich M.M., Sameshima K., Jenkins W.M. Remodelling of hand representation in adult cortex determined by timing of tactile stimulation. Nature. 1995;378:71–75. doi: 10.1038/378071a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ward N.S., Cohen L.G. Mechanisms underlying recovery of motor function after stroke. Arch. Neurol. 2004;61:1844–1848. doi: 10.1001/archneur.61.12.1844. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weeks S.R., Anderson-Barnes V.C., Tsao J.W. Phantom limb pain: theories and therapies. Neurol. 2010;16:277–286. doi: 10.1097/NRL.0b013e3181edf128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wesselink D.B., van den Heiligenberg F.M., Ejaz N., Dempsey-Jones H., Cardinali L., Tarall-Jozwiak A., Diedrichsen J., Makin T.R. Obtaining and maintaining cortical hand representation as evidenced from acquired and congenital handlessness. eLife. 2019;8:611. doi: 10.7554/eLife.37227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yanagisawa T., Fukuma R., Seymour B., Hosomi K., Kishima H., Shimizu T., Yokoi H., Hirata M., Yoshimine T., Kamitani Y., Saitoh Y. Induced sensorimotor brain plasticity controls pain in phantom limb patients. Nat. Commun. 2016;7:13209. doi: 10.1038/ncomms13209. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang T.T., Gallen C., Schwartz B., Bloom F.E., Ramachandran V.S., Cobb S. Sensory maps in the human brain. Nature. 1994;368:592–593. doi: 10.1038/368592b0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeo B.T.T., Krienen F.M., Sepulcre J., Sabuncu M.R., Lashkari D., Hollinshead M., Roffman J.L., Smoller J.W., Zöllei L., Polimeni J.R., Fischl B., Liu H., Buckner R.L. The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J. Neurophysiol. 2011;106:1125–1165. doi: 10.1152/jn.00338.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeharia N., Hertz U., Flash T., Amedi A. Negative blood oxygenation level dependent homunculus and somatotopic information in primary motor cortex and supplementary motor area. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 2012;109:18565–18570. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1119125109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeharia N., Hertz U., Flash T., Amedi A. New whole-body sensory-motor gradients revealed using phase-locked analysis and verified using multivoxel pattern analysis and functional connectivity. J. Neurosci. 2015;35:2845–2859. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4246-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeharia N., Hofstetter S., Flash T., Amedi A. A whole-body sensory-motor gradient is revealed in the medial Wall of the parietal lobe. J. Neurosci. 2019 doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0727-18.2019. 0727–18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]