Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2021 Sep 1.
Published in final edited form as: Cogn Emot. 2020 Mar 2;34(6):1143–1159. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2020.1731428

The Influence of Fear on Risk Taking: A Meta-Analysis

Sean Wake 1, Jolie Wormwood 2, Ajay B Satpute 1,*
PMCID: PMC7423744  NIHMSID: NIHMS1566951  PMID: 32116122

Abstract

A common finding in the study of emotion and decision making is the tendency for fear and anxiety to decrease risk taking. The current meta-analysis summarizes the strength and variability of this effect in the extant empirical literature. Our analysis of 136 effect sizes, derived from 68 independent samples and 9,544 participants, included studies that experimentally manipulated fear or measured naturally varying levels of fear or anxiety in both clinical and non-clinical samples, and studies measuring risky decision-making and risk estimation. A multilevel random effects model estimated a small to moderate average effect size (r = 0.22), such that fear was related to decreased risky decision making and increased risk estimation. There was also high heterogeneity in the effect sizes. Moderator analyses showed that effect sizes were greater when risk tasks that used tangible (e.g. monetary) outcomes, and when studies used clinically anxious participants. However, there also remained considerable variability in effect sizes the sources of which remain unknown. We posit several potential factors that may contribute to observed variability in this effect for future study, including factors concerning both the nature of fear experience and the risk taking context.

Keywords: meta-analysis, fear, anxiety, risk taking, decision making


A consensus has emerged among researchers that fear decreases risk taking (Raghunathan & Pham, 1999; Lerner & Keltner, 2001; Charpentier, Aylward, Roiser, & Robinson, 2017; Niedenthal, Krauth-Gruber, & Ric, 2006). This finding has motivated practices across multiple domains of social and industrial life. Economists apply it to understand market behavior (Reinhart & Rogoff, 2009), politicians and the media exploit it to influence public perception (Glassner, 2010), and health experts use it to more effectively communicate the health risks of certain behaviors (Brown & Walsh-Childers, 2002). Given the wide practical interest in the influence of fear on risk taking, it would be valuable to examine the strength and consistency of this relationship. Indeed, a closer examination of the literature suggests that there are studies that fail to report this effect (Hunt, Hopko, Bare, Lejuez, & Robinson, 2005; Charpentier, Hindocha, Roiser, & Robinson, 2016) or even observe the reverse effect whereby fear is associated with increased risk taking (Kugler, Connolly, & Ordóñez, 2012a; Zhang & Gu, 2018).

Examining the strength and consistency of the relationship between fear and risk taking may also contribute to ongoing theoretical debates concerning the nature of emotion. There is a large variety of theoretical models for fear that vary in the extent to which they propose that fear ought to have a more uniform (v. more heterogeneous) effect on behavior. In each model, fear is defined a priori by researchers in different ways. Fear is stipulated to be an affect program that drives specific facial behaviors or physiological responses (Ekman & Cordaro, 2011; Levenson, 2011), a set of stimulus appraisals (rapid evaluations of whether a stimulus is harmful, unexpected, etc.) that are either consistent across all different fear inductions or may involve different suites of appraisals in different moments of fear (Clore & Ortony, 2008; Ellsworth & Scherer, 2003), a single or suite of functional states that drives defensive behaviors like fleeing or fighting (Adolphs & Anderson, 2018; Mobbs, 2018; Fanselow, 1994), a personal schema (LeDoux, 2015), or a mental construction of prior experiences that are used to make meaning of current instances (Barrett, 2006; Lindquist & Barrett, 2008; Clore & Ortony, 2013; Satpute & Lindquist, 2019). These models vary in the extent to which they emphasise uniformity or non-uniformity in the relationship between fear and behavior, which may be informed by a comprehensive examination of the literature on fear and risk taking.

Here, we used meta-analytic methods to investigate the relationship between fear and risk taking. Meta-analysis provides a quantitative summary of the strength and consistency of an effect in the literature and the opportunity to test for potential moderators of effect size by statistically comparing groups of studies to one another. Our meta-analysis included two operationalizations of risk taking: risky decision making and risk estimation. Risky decision making refers to situations wherein an individual must decide between options that differ in the variability of their outcome. For example, participants may choose between a guaranteed $10 payoff or a 50% chance to win $20. This example specifies a 50% chance of success in the more variable, risky choice. Sometimes the risky choice can involve outcomes of unspecified odds as well (e.g. balloon analog risk task; Lejuez et al., 2002). Uncertainty refers to the situation in which the decision is risky, but the odds are unknown (Chua Chow & Sarin, 2002).

According to predominant theories of risky decision making, decision-makers deciding among risky options assign value to each option by estimating both the likelihood and subjective value associated with each outcome (Kahneman & Tversky, 2012). For example, participants may be asked to estimate the likelihood that their car would be broken into if left unlocked overnight, or how unpleasant experiencing a break-in would be. Such estimates are thus constitutive of the decision making process, and we consider them relevant to our investigation of risk-taking. We refer here to risk estimation as subjects’ perception of the likelihood and/or subjective valuation of risky outcomes.

The present meta-analysis assesses two overarching categories of moderators: (i) methods surrounding how fear was induced and/or measured, and (ii) methods for how risk taking was measured. Despite pervasive methodological variation in the measurement of these two constructs, justification for selecting certain protocols and speculation concerning the influence that these choices may have on outcomes is rare. One might expect, however, that different methodologies for inducing fear produce emotional experiences of variable nature and intensity (e.g., Condon, Wilson-Mendenhall, & Barrett, 2013), and that different risk taking tasks may vary in their susceptibility to the influence of emotion (Loewenstein, Weber, Hsee, & Welch, 2001; Slovic & Peters, 2006). Understanding their influence is of relevance for both fundamental and translational research questions in fear and risk taking.

Inducing and Measuring Fear and Anxiety

The relationship between emotion and decision-making in general has been shown to depend on several methodological factors (Angie, Connelly, Waples, & Kligyte, 2011; Lerner, Li, Valdesolo, & Kassam, 2015). Here, we identified several potential moderating variables relating to methodological variability in the induction and measurement of fear and anxiety. First, we assessed whether fear and anxiety was measured using a non-experimental design (i.e. examining how naturally varying trait fear or anxiety levels relate to risk taking) or an experimental design (i.e. experimentally-induced fear). In the present investigation, ‘naturally varying fear’ refers to trait measures of fear and anxiety. Conversely, as emotion induction experiments aim to manipulate fear in the moment, this category refers exclusively to state fear. Among non-experimental designs, we assessed the timing of fear inventory administration (e.g. whether a trait questionnaire was administered before or after the risk taking task), and the specificity of emotion inventories (i.e. whether emotions in addition to fear were measured). Among experimental designs, we identified whether or not a manipulation check was administered prior to participants completing the risk taking tasks as well as the medium of stimuli used to induce fear (e.g., pictures, sounds, movies). We also assessed whether the conceptual content of the fear induction was idiographic (i.e. tailed to each participant) or normative. Additionally, we compared studies that used neutral emotion as the control condition to those that used anger as the control condition. Finally, variation across studies may also be related to how strong the fear induction is (i.e., the intensity of the fear experience it produces). Thus, we examined whether the intensity of self-reported emotional experience predicted the strength of the relationship between fear and risk taking. One might expect greater self-reported fear experience to be associated with more pronounced decreases in risk taking (i.e. a dose-dependent relationship across studies) to the extent that the influence of fear on risk taking is uniform and monotonic.

Although applied and theoretical models vary in how they treat the constructs of fear and anxiety (Fanselow, 1991; Öhman, 2008; Reiss, 1991), in the risk taking literature these constructs are not clearly distinguished methodologically or empirically. Studies that use experimental inductions of fear rarely, if ever, ask participants to report on how the induction influences fear and anxiety separately. Similarly, studies using non-experimental designs use scores on scales and inventories that also do not clearly separate fear and anxiety; for example, the commonly used Anxiety Sensitivity Index includes items with the terms “scared”, “nervous”, “anxiety”, and “worried” (Reiss, Peterson, Gursky, & McNally, 1986). Thus, although researchers appear to focus on the impact of anxiety or fear separately, and do not intend to use the terms interchangeably, limited empirical distinction between these constructs is made in the existing literature. To examine whether effect sizes differed when considering studies ostensibly on fear v. anxiety, we coded and then analytically compared studies investigating anxiety and studies investigating fear on the basis of the language used by the authors. We note here that this dimension did not have an influence on the results; for ease, we use the term “fear” throughout the rest of the manuscript to refer to studies assessing fear and anxiety in this meta-analysis except where explicitly stated otherwise.

Assessing Risk Taking and Risk Estimation

We also identified moderating variables relating to the format of the risk taking measure to examine whether certain measures of risk taking are more susceptible to the influence of fear than others. We coded for whether studies measured risky decision-making (e.g., through responses on a gambling task) or risk estimation (e.g., through subjective reports of the perceived likelihood and/or value of various risky outcomes). Notably, we will use ‘risky decision-making’ to refer to studies using both conditions of risk and conditions of uncertainty. Whereas decision-making under risk involves the probabilities of each outcome being directly provided, uncertainty refers to the condition in which probabilities are unknown (or unknowable) to the decision-maker (De Groot & Thurik, 2018). Common tasks measuring risk include simple gambles devised by experimenters (such as a 50% chance of winning $10 vs. a 100% chance of winning $5), as well as the Game of Dice Task (Brand et al., 2002). Standard tasks involving uncertainty include the “balloon analog risk” task (BART; Lejuez et al., 2002) and the IOWA Gambling Task (Bechara, Damasio, Damasio, & Anderson, 1994). We included both types of tasks in our ‘risky decision-making’ category, but coded for and analyzed potential differences between decision-making tasks involving risk v. uncertainty in our moderator analyses. Additionally, among decision-making studies, we assessed whether tasks involved the framing of decisions in terms of gains or losses (for example, whether a decision-making scenario involving a disease epidemic is presented in terms of lives saved or lives lost (Tversky & Kahneman, 1981), and whether they used real (i.e. monetary) vs. hypothetical rewards (Irwin, McClelland, & Schulze, 1992). Among risk estimation studies, we compared the effect of fear on risk likelihood estimates to the effect on estimates of the subjective value of risky outcomes.

Methods

Literature Search

Manual searches of the psychological and medical literature were conducted to gather articles investigating the influence of fear on risk taking. Our search included terms for the emotion words ‘fear’ and ‘anxiety’, and terms relevant to measuring risk (‘risk aversion’, ‘risk seeking’, ‘risk taking’, ‘perception of risk’, ‘estimates of risk’, ‘risk estimates’, ‘decision-making’, ‘judgment of risk’, ‘risk judgment’, ‘risk-averse’, ‘risk sensitivity’, ‘risk preference’, ‘risk appraisal’, ‘risk perception’, ‘risk avoidance’, ‘risk avoidant’). We entered all combinations of one emotion word with one risk measure term into Pubmed and PsycInfo. Studies published prior to April 24, 2018, were included in the analysis. The literature search yielded an initial database of 2,097 studies (Figure 1).

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

PRISMA chart for study set. The database search yielded 2117 articles. Abstract screening identified 104 relevant articles, 55 of which met criteria for inclusion. In the case of studies reporting data insufficient for effect size calculation, 5 authors did not respond to our request for supplementary results. 50 studies were included in the current analysis, yielding 136 effect sizes from 68 independent samples. Search terms and criteria for inclusion are provided in methods section. The reduction from the number of articles in the database search to included articles is generally consistent with other meta-analyses that use broad and inclusive search terms for the initial database inquiry (see main text).

Criteria for Inclusion of Studies

Studies were assessed for inclusion based on the following two overarching criteria. First, studies were included if fear was treated as an independent variable or predictor. Here, studies varied in whether fear was experimentally-induced or measured in a non-experimental paradigm. Studies that experimentally induced fear used a variety of fear induction methods (e.g., presenting frightening movie clips, retrieving autobiographical memories; see coding scheme below for a complete list). Studies that implemented a non-experimental design were included if they measured naturally varying trait fear using established inventories: Fear Survey Schedule 2 (Bernstein & Allen, 1969), Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale (David Watson & Friend, 1969), Spielberger’s State-Trait Anxiety Scale (Spielberger, Gorsuch, & Lushene, 1970), Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (Sheehan et al., 1998), PANAS (D. Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988), Stanford Acute Stress Reaction Questionnaire (Anxiety Subscale) (Cardeña, Koopman, Classen, Waelde, & Spiegel, 2000), Beck Anxiety Inventory (Beck, Epstein, Brown, & Steer, 1988), GADQ-IV (Newman et al., 2002), Web-Based Depression and Anxiety Screen (Farvolden, McBride, Bagby, & Ravitz, 2003), Anxiety Sensitivity Index (Reiss, Peterson, Gursky, & McNally, 1986), Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children (March, Parker, Sullivan, Stallings, & Conners, 1997), Hospital Anxiety Scale (Zigmond & Snaith, 1983), Worry Domains Questionnaire (Tallis, Davey, & Bond, 1994)1, Taylor Manifest Anxiety Scale (Taylor, 1953), Brief Symptom Inventory (Anxiety Subscale) (Derogatis & Melisaratos, 1983).

Second, studies were included if they measured risk taking as a dependent or outcome variable. Here, studies varied in whether risk taking was operationalized in a decision-making task or in a risk-estimation task. Decision-making studies involved choice under conditions of risk or conditions of uncertainty. Studies could measure risk-taking by presenting participants with tasks to perform in lab (e.g. gambles, GDT, BART, IGT), or questionnaires aiming to quantify real-world risky decision making, such as the Domain Specific Risk Taking (DOSPERT) scale (Weber, Blais, & Betz, 2002). These common tasks are not an exhaustive list, however – decision-making studies using analogous measures were included as well, provided they involved conditions of risk or uncertainty. We also included risk-estimation tasks, in which participants provided estimates of the likelihood and/or subjective value of risky events. For example, participants might be asked what the likelihood is of being in a car accident in their lifetime or how undesirable they would estimate the experience to be. Notably, the definition of risk utilized in the present meta-analysis excludes distinct operationalizations of risk that are commonly used in epidemiology and health-related fields. Whereas we refer to risky decision-making as a choice between options that differ in the variability of possible outcomes, risk in the medical sense refers more strictly to the likelihood of incurring loss or damage (e.g., a negative health outcome; see Kaplan & Garrick, 1981).

Studies were excluded if the results reported were insufficient to compute an effect size. If an otherwise eligible study reported data insufficient for the calculation of an effect size, we contacted the corresponding author(s) requesting the missing component(s). However, this approach failed to yield necessary information in some cases. Since our interest is whether a person feels fear, studies were excluded if they did not induce fear experimentally or measure feelings of naturally varying fear via trait fear inventories in a non-experimental design. Some studies measured attitudes or belief about a specific event (e.g. do you fear terrorist threats? cancer? etc.), but if the experiment did not specifically induce or measure whether participants currently feeling fear or were trait anxious, they were excluded.

Of the 2,097 articles identified by the literature search, 50 met our inclusion criteria, which yielded 136 effect size estimates from 68 independent samples (see Figure 1). This reduction from the total search volume to the number of studies included is typical for meta-analyses that use broad and inclusive initial search strategies. For example, several prior studies using PRIMSA guidelines have final samples of 41 out of 28,585 in the search (Morina, Koerssen, & Pollet, 2016), 13 from 3,292 (Dowling et al., 2017), 6 from 634 (Piet & Hougaard, 2011), 101 from 10,894 (Mayo-Wilson et al., 2014), 40 from 5,384 (Bolier et al., 2013), etc.

Coding

A coding scheme was devised by surveying the literature and identifying features that varied across the studies. A summary of the features, their levels, and the numbers of studies contributing to them is provided in Table 1.

Table 1:

Effect sizes by Moderator Levels

Moderator and Levels N All Effect Sizes Mean Effect Size SD N Independent Samples Mean Effect Size SD
Emotion Label Used by Authors
Fear 46 0.15 0.17 22 0.19 0.22
Anxiety 90 0.20 0.23 46 0.22 0.25
Study Design
Experimental 52 0.15 0.20 28 0.16 0.23
Non-Experimental 65 0.16 0.19 30 0.16 0.20
Non-Experimental (Clinical) 19 0.33 0.27 10 0.30 0.37
Timing of Fear Inventory
In Lab, Prior to Risk Task 27 0.24 0.25 14 0.25 0.28
After Risk Task 9 0.18 0.08 3 0.22 0.10
Prior to Lab Visit 8 0.17 0.26 5 0.36 0.23
Presence of Additional Emotions Measured
No 13 0.18 0.24 6 0.23 0.35
Yes 11 0.41 0.26 7 0.30 0.25
Manipulation Check Prior to Risk-Taking Task
Yes 31 0.15 0.17 17 0.26 0.18
No 18 0.14 0.20 10 0.13 0.26
Control Group
Neutral 44 0.25 0.22 23 0.29 0.23
Angry 15 0.13 0.21 10 0.20 0.22
Fear Induction Medium
Participant-Imagined Scenario 6 0.11 0.07 3 0.16 0.10
Written Scenario (Provided by Experimenter) 6 0.32 0.16 4 0.34 0.17
Video Clip 6 0.08 0.22 3 0.06 0.34
Static Image 8 0.33 0.19 4 0.35 0.23
Musical Mood Induction 8 0.17 0.13 2 0.27 0.08
Autobiographical Recall 16 0.18 0.20 10 0.16 0.22
Social Evaluative Threat 1 0.71 NA 1 0.71 NA
Anticipation of Pain 1 0.02 NA 1 0.02 NA
Fear Induction Content
Normative 33 0.15 0.20 16 0.27 0.25
Idiographic 19 0.17 0.18 12 0.16 0.20
Risk-Taking Task
Risky Decision Making 73 0.19 0.27 44 0.21 0.29
Risk Estimation 63 0.17 0.12 24 0.22 0.11
Decision-Making Task
Simple Gamble 26 0.14 0.24 18 0.13 0.26
GDT 3 0.01 0.28 3 0.01 0.28
BART 14 0.37 0.40 8 0.38 0.40
IGT 2 0.33 0.01 2 0.33 0.01
DOSPERT 5 0.22 0.21 2 0.28 0.28
Estimation Task
Likelihood 43 0.17 0.12 19 0.22 0.12
Utility 20 0.18 0.11 5 0.23 0.06
Risk Type
Risk 40 0.19 0.28 22 0.16 0.29
Uncertainty 33 0.18 0.25 22 0.26 0.29
Frame
Gains 32 0.23 0.29 18 0.23 0.29
Losses 5 −0.05 0.28 4 0.14 0.32
Tangible Reward
Yes 42 0.30 0.28 30 0.31 0.27
No 94 0.16 0.16 38 0.14 0.20

Fear Measurements

We coded for studies with experimental designs (i.e. manipulating state fear) and non-experimental designs (i.e. naturally varying trait fear). For fear measured in non-experimental designs, we coded for whether the study was conducted within a neurotypical population or compared between neurotypical and clinically fearful groups. Clinically fearful participants were diagnosed with one or more of the following conditions: Generalized Anxiety Disorder (N = 241), Social Phobia (N = 78), Panic Disorder (N = 47), Obsessive Compulsive Disorder (N = 21), Separation Anxiety (N = 3), Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (N = 2), Adjustment Disorder with Anxious Mood (N = 1). Among experimental designs, we coded for the presence of a manipulation check prior to the task (as opposed to a manipulation check administered after the risk task or pre-tested on subjects not participating in the risk measure - or if no check was administered at all). Among non-experimental designs, we coded for the timing of fear inventory measures (i.e. whether fear was measured before participants arrived for the experiment, after arriving but prior to the risk task, or after completing the risk task) and the specificity of trait measures (i.e. the presence/absence of separate emotion trait measures in addition to fear). Additionally, among all studies we coded for the use of either a neutral baseline or anger as the control group condition.

Fear Intensity

Among studies that experimentally induced fear and also those that used non-experimental designs but directly compared two groups (i.e., one high and one low in fear), we recorded the strength of the fear induction (e.g. by recording the effect size of the difference in reported fear between conditions or groups). We then normalised these values (see below) to provide a more continuous assessment that could be used to examine how the strength of the fear inductions related to the strength of the differences in risk taking.

Fear Induction Stimulus Medium and Content Specificity

For fear inductions, we coded for the following features. First, we coded for stimulus medium; studies were coded for whether the fear induction involved video clips, static images, music, retrieval of autobiographical memories, social evaluative threat, anticipation of pain, scenarios imagined by participants, or written scenarios provided by experimenters. Second, these studies were grouped into those that used idiographic stimulus content (i.e. participants were asked to self-generate content that was fear-inducing) and normative stimulus content (i.e., all participants received the same experimenter-selected content).

Treatment of Fear and Anxiety

Fear and anxiety is rarely systematically distinguished, theoretically or experimentally, in the studies included in this meta-analysis or in the risk taking literature more generally. To provide an initial examination of differences between fear and anxiety, we grouped studies into those that referred to fear and those that referred to anxiety on the basis of the authors’ description. A preliminary comparison of studies using the term fear vs. using the term anxiety showed no significant differences in average effect size (see Table 1; F(1, 134) = 0.48, p = 0.49). Thus, the literature is currently inconclusive as to whether there are differences in risk taking that vary by fear and anxiety.

Risk Task

Studies were coded for whether risk was measured using risky decision-making or risk estimation. Studies using risk estimation were coded for whether they assessed perceived risk likelihood or the subjective value of risky outcomes. The subset of risky decision-making studies was coded for four moderators. Among decision-making tasks, we identified and coded for five commonly used protocols. These were simple gambles (e.g. a choice between a 50% chance of winning $10 and a 100% chance of winning $5), the Game of Dice Task (Brand et al., 2002), the Balloon Analog Risk Task (BART; Lejuez et al., 2002), the IOWA gambling task (Bechara, Damasio, Damasio, & Anderson, 1994), as well as the DOSPERT scale (Blais & Weber, 2006). Of 73 decision-making effect sizes, 50 used one of these standardized tasks. Additionally, we coded separately for the following variables: First, studies were placed either into the category of risk (i.e. odds of outcomes specified) or uncertainty (odds unspecified). Second, studies were coded based on whether the outcomes were framed in terms of gains, losses, or a mixture of both. Third, studies were coded based on whether the outcomes involved tangible (i.e. monetary) stakes or not (e.g., hypothetical outcomes).

Analysis

All effect size estimates (e.g., Cohen’s d for studies with group comparisons) were converted to correlation coefficients (r). For studies reporting multiple regression that did not provide corresponding r values (# of effect sizes = 4), we treated standardized beta coefficients (β) as correlation coefficients, consistent with recommendations in the literature (Peterson & Brown, 2005). Effect sizes were assigned a positive sign to indicate a relationship in the theoretically predicted direction (i.e., increased fear associated with decreased risk taking or increased risk estimation), or a negative sign to indicate a relationship in the opposite direction. Fifty articles generated a total of 136 effect sizes including independent and non-independent effects (i.e. multiple effects reported using the same subject sample) and 68 independent effect sizes, comprising 9,544 participants in total.

To control for non-independence, we conducted multilevel regression analyses of individual effect sizes nested within independent samples2, implemented in the Metafor package in R (Viechtbauer, 2010). All analyses treated sample as a random factor and utilised a variance components covariance structure. For each moderator, nominal categories/levels of the moderator were dummy-coded and linear multiple regression models were used to examine the association between varying levels of the moderator and effect size. Heterogeneity in the effect was computed using Cochran’s Q, which sums the squared deviations of each studies’ estimate from the mean effect size estimate produced by the overall meta-analysis (Cochran, 1954).

Additionally, to assess the problem of publication bias, a fail-safe N was computed – this value provides an estimate of the number of null findings that would need to be published to bring the significance of the average effect size (relative to 0) above the 0.05 significance threshold (one-tailed; Rosenthal, 1979). Additionally, p values associated with each effect size were calculated and input to a p curve, which provides another way of investigating publication bias by tracking the frequency of p values at intervals between 0.0 and 0.05. Clustering of p values near the p = 0.05 threshold presents evidence of potential publication bias or p-hacking, whereas clustering of p values approaching 0.0 suggests a true effect (Simonsohn, Nelson, & Simmons, 2014).

Results

Overall Strength and Consistency of Effect Size

The results of the random effects model are summarized in Figure 2. The estimated mean effect size for the relationship between fear and risk taking is r=0.219 (SD = 0.37, Z(136) = 8.65, p < 0.0001, two-tailed). These results suggest a significant relationship between increased fear and decreased risk taking (i.e. decreased risky decision-making and/or increased estimation of risk). Based on the criteria set forth by Cohen (1977), this effect is considered small-to-medium in strength. The fail-safe N analysis estimates that 10,264 studies producing null effects would need to be published to bring the significance of the analysis comparing this mean effect size above p = 0.05 (one-tailed). P curve analysis demonstrates that studies reporting significant results tended to increase in frequency as p-values decreased, suggesting there was limited evidence of any potential p-hacking or publication bias (Figure 3). The test for heterogeneity indicated a high degree of heterogeneity in the effect of fear on risk-taking, Q(135) = 1210.8, p < 0.001. The heterogeneity was not explained by statistical power in that the variability of effect sizes was high both for studies with high and low sample sizes (see funnel plot, Figure 4). Overall, these results suggest that, on average, there is a moderate and reliable effect of fear on risk taking across studies, but also that this relationship is not homogenous across studies.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Forest plot summarizing the distribution of effect sizes (N = 136). The weighted mean effect size is r = 0.22. Negative values indicate effects in the opposite direction (i.e. fear increases risk taking). The relationship between fear and risk taking exhibits high variability across studies with a range of [−0.52, 0.84].

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

P-Curve analysis. The figure illustrates the distribution of p values (N=136) for positive effect sizes (A and B) and negative effect sizes (C) . A) 76 positive effect sizes are statistically significant (p < 0.05), compared to 51 insignificant values. B) Among studies reporting significant decreases in risk-taking caused by fear (0 < p < 0.05), number of publications increase as p-values decrease. This trend suggests that publication bias and p-hacking are not major factors in the present distribution of studies investigating fear and risk taking. C Among negative effect sizes (i.e. fear increases risk taking) 5 are significant (p <0.05), and 4 are not (including one outlier not showed; p = 0.44). Among 4 significant values, 3 are below p = 0.01.

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

Funnel plot for relationship between effect sizes and standard error. If the effect of fear on risk taking is uniform, outcomes should converge towards the mean effect size (r = 0.22), as standard error decreases. Absence of this trend suggests heterogeneity is not explained by sampling error alone.

Moderator Analyses

We next examined whether heterogeneity in the influence of fear on risk taking could be explained by several moderators.

Fear Measurements

We first compared study designs that experimentally induced fear with those measuring trait fear (non-experimentally induced) in either clinical or non-clinical populations (see Table 1 for mean effect sizes). Significant differences were observed among the three groups – experimentally induced fear, non-experimentally measured fear (non-clinical population), and non-experimentally measured fear (clinical population), F(2, 133) = 4.76, p = 0.009. This effect appeared to be driven by differences between clinical and non-clinical populations, rather than experimentally-induced fear vs non-experimentally measured fear: combining the clinical and non-clinical categories of non-experimental fear, the comparison between experimentally-induced fear and non-experimentally measured fear was not significant, F(1, 134) = 0.01, p = 0.92. A direct comparison of non-experimental designs using clinical and non-clinical populations showed larger effect sizes in the studies with clinical populations than non-clinical populations, F(1, 82) = 4.81, p = 0.02. At the same time, studies with clinical populations also showed a high variability in effect sizes (SDs in Table 1).

Indeed, studies using clinical samples ranged in effect sizes from r = −0.15 (Lorian et al., 2011) and r = 0.00 (Nesse et al., 1994) on the low end to r = 0.67 (Charpentier et al., 2017) and 0.80 and 0.84 (Ortega et al., 2017) on the high end. We were unable to investigate differences among particular clinical diagnoses, as the majority of the studies grouped all clinical participants into one category, rather than keeping separate the individual diagnoses. Of the 16 effect sizes involving GAD patients, 9 included patients with conditions other than GAD. Among effect sizes using Panic Disorder (n = 6), Social Phobia (n =9), OCD (n=6), PTSD (n = 2), Separation Anxiety (n=2), and Adjustment Disorder with Anxious Mood (n=2), no analyses had fewer than two diagnoses combined within the clinical group.

Among non-experimental designs, it is possible that the relationship between fear and risk taking would depend on when inventories were used to measure fear and how many other inventories were included in the study. For example, demand characteristics could be greater when inventories are obtained beforehand, or if only a fear inventory was acquired in the study, making it more likely that participants will infer that the intent of the study is to examine the influence of fear on risk taking. Contrary to this idea, the effect of fear on risk taking did not significantly vary between studies that administered fear surveys prior to the day of the experiment, during the experiment but prior to the risk task, or after the risk task, F(2, 41) = 0.34, p = 0.71. Among studies that issued emotion surveys in lab and prior to the risk task, the presence of inventories measuring emotions other than fear did not impact the relationship between fear and risk-taking, F(1,22) = 0.28, p = 0.60.

Among studies that experimentally induced fear, it is possible that obtaining a manipulation check prior to the risk taking measurement may also influence the effect of fear on risk taking. A manipulation check may heighten awareness of study goals to examine fear and risk taking and increase demand characteristics. In contrast to this notion, the presence of a manipulation check prior to the risk task failed to significantly impact the relationship between fear and risk-taking and, if anything, trended in the opposite direction, F(1, 47) = 3.56, p = 0.06; see Table 1. A few studies (n=15) also used anger rather than neutral baselines as a control condition. The use of anger as a control condition produced slightly lower effect sizes than neutral controls, but this effect was not significant, F(1,57) = 1.66, p = 0.20.

Fear Induction Medium and Content Specificity

We also compared effect size across different fear induction mediums (e.g., movies, autobiographical recall). We found that, among the various mediums used to induce fear, a significant difference was observed with regard to risk taking effect sizes, F(7, 44) = 2.43, p = 0.02. This difference was likely driven by social evaluative threat, which produced an effect size far larger than the other induction mediums (of r = 0.71) whereas the average effect sizes of the other induction mediums were considerably lower and closer to the overall mean effect (rs < .33; see Table 1). However, only one study used social evaluative threat preventing any strong conclusions from being drawn from this finding at present. Finally, we examined the content specificity of fear inductions, or specifically, whether the effect of fear on risk taking differed when the conceptual content of the fear induction was determined by the experimenters (and thus more uniform across participants) versus when participants decided the induction content via their own conception of fear (i.e. autobiographical memories or imagery that was more unique or tailored to participants). Content specificity had no significant relationship with effect size, F(1, 50) = 0.43, p = 0.51.

Fear Intensity

It could be hypothesized that what might matter most in the strength of the relationship between fear and risk taking is not the specific experimental methods used to elicit or assess fear, but the relative intensity with which fear is experienced. If fear influences risk taking in a dose-dependent way (i.e. greater fear, greater risk aversion), then one might expect a monotonic relationship between increasing fear intensity and the extent of reduction in risk taking across studies. Here, we operationalized fear intensity as the magnitude of the self-reported difference in fear separating low-fear and high-fear groups both in experimental and non-experimental (trait fear) studies. For each of the studies included in this analysis, we calculated effect size (Z) for differences in fear and anxiety levels using methods consistent with those we used to calculate effect sizes for risk taking. Group differences in fear produced Cohen’s d values, which were converted to r before Fisher transforming. We then used a Pearson’s correlation analysis to examine associations between fear effect sizes and risk taking effect sizes (i.e., the difference in self-reported fear and the difference in risk-taking across conditions) across studies. As illustrated in Figure 5, we failed to find any significant relationship between the magnitude of the fear effect size and the magnitude of the risk taking effect size across studies, r(51) = 0.11, p = 0.44. Using fear intensity as a continuous moderator within the multi-level model, the analysis similarly found no significant effect, F(1, 51) = 0.88, p = 0.35. Thus, studies in which groups differed the most in self-reported fear did not systemically exhibit the greatest reductions in risk taking or increases in risk estimation.

Figure 5.

Figure 5.

Risk taking effect sizes (Z) do not significantly increase as fear intensity increases (Z) for A) all studies combined (r = 0.11, N = 53, p = 0.44, B) experimental designs (r = 0.16, N = 37, p = 0.36), or C) non-experimental designs (r = −0.014, N = 16, p = 0.96).

Risk Taking Task

Next, we focused on whether the various ways to measure risk taking also contributed to variability in effect sizes. Here, the only moderator that was significantly related with effect size was the consequences at stake for participants completing risky decision-making tasks. Among decision-making studies, fear induced greater risk-aversion in studies presenting tangible rewards (mean r = .31, SD = .27) compared to studies that offered no tangible reward (mean r = .14, SD = .20), F(1, 71) = 5.15, p = 0.02. However, the variability in effect sizes for studies with tangible rewards also remained high and spanned the full range from r = −0.52 (Zhang et al., 2018) and r = −0.38 (Jakuszkowiak-Wojten et al., 2017) on the low end to r = 0.72 (Ramirez et al., 2015) and 0.80 and 0.84 (Ortega et al., 2017) on the high end.

The remaining moderator variables relating to the measurement of risk taking were non-significant. Specifically, we found no significant differences between tasks requiring subjects to make decisions involving risk and those requiring them to estimate risk (see Table 1; F(1, 134) = 0.17, p = 0.69). Among risk estimation effect sizes, we observed no significant difference between estimates of likelihood and estimates of subjective value, F(1,61) = 0.08, p = 0.78. For studies using either simple gambles, GDT, BART, IGT or the DOSPERT scale, we observed no significant differences in the relationship between fear and risk-taking among these five tasks, F(4, 45) = 0.83, p = 0.50. The effect of fear on risk taking also failed to differ significantly between decision making tasks utilizing conditions of uncertainty vs conditions of risk, F(1, 71) = 0.66, p = 0.41. Furthermore, the effect of fear on risk taking did not significantly differ between studies where outcomes in the risky decision-making task were presented in a gain frame vs a loss frame, F(1,35) = 0.60, p = 0.44.

Discussion

In this study, we used meta-analytic methods to examine the strength and consistency of the relationship between fear and risk taking. Our goal was to provide a quantitative summary of extant results and a systematic examination of potential moderators of the relationship. Using a multi-level, random effects model across 136 effect sizes, we observed a highly reliable influence of fear on risk taking with an average effect size of r = 0.217, suggesting that fear is associated with decreased risk taking (i.e. decreased risky decision making and increased risk estimation). This relationship is considered to be “small to moderate” in strength (Cohen, 1977). At the same time, we observed substantial variability in effect sizes (quantified by Q(135) = 1210.8, and visualized in the funnel plot in Figure 4). In a small set of studies, fear was even associated with significantly greater risk taking (Figure 2; Lauriola, Russo, Lucidi, Violani, & Levin, 2005; Kugler, Connolly, & Ordóñez, 2012b; Bagneux, Bollon, & Dantzer, 2012; Jakuszkowiak-Wojten et al., 2017). These findings suggest that the relationship between fear and risk taking is also heterogeneous and underscores the importance of investigating moderating variables for this effect.

We coded for several features that varied across studies to investigate their role in moderating the relationship between fear and risk taking (Table 1). With regard to the measurement and induction of fear, there were larger effect sizes on average when the study used a non-experimental design with clinical samples (r = .32) than when it involved a non-experimental design with non-clinical samples (r = .16) or even an experimental induction of fear (r = .15). Yet, even just among the studies with clinical samples, the variability in effect sizes was quite high as indicated by the SD (Table 1) and range. With regard to the measurement of risk, there was a stronger effect when the risk task involved a tangible reward (e.g. monetary; r = .30) than a non-tangible reward (r = .16). Yet again, the variability in effect sizes was high for studies with tangible rewards (Table 1). These findings require a balanced conclusion. They suggest that studies with clinical populations and tangible rewards may demonstrate larger effects of fear on risk taking, but that even in these cases, the relationship between fear and risk taking is heterogeneous and requires further investigation.

Consistent with our finding that the effect of fear on risk taking was stronger in risk-taking contexts with tangible rewards, there is evidence to suggest that people are less risk-taking on average when dealing with real money than hypothetical money (Irwin et al., 1992). It has been theorized that hypothetical rewards do not provide adequate motivation to decision makers, and thus they tend to choose low-effort options by default over options that would optimize their gains (Smith & Walker, 1993). Perhaps adequate motivation is required for decision makers to detect and incorporate emotional cues in a robust way, and thus the process is less susceptible to the influence of fear when dealing with hypothetical rewards than tangible ones. Another possible explanation stems from the view that making a risky decision is itself affectively evocative (Loewenstein, Weber, Hsee, & Welch, 2001; Slovic & Peters, 2006). Decision making involving actual rewards may perhaps be more affectively evocative than hypothetical rewards. Future work may examine possible causal mechanisms to explain the increased influence of fear on risk taking when utilizing tangible vs. hypothetical outcomes. In doing so, it would be of importance to examine the mean influence of fear on risk across a wide variety of paradigms as well as the extent and possible causes of variability in effect sizes. For example, research using monetary decision making tasks has shown that the payout schedule (i.e. whether payouts are honored for all trials or a subset of trials) may also influence risk taking (Schmidt & Hewig, 2015); although we coded for this characteristic, there were too few studies that used only one (n=2) or a subset of trials (n=6) to determine payouts to conduct an informative moderator analysis.

Our meta-analytic review also makes clear several gaps in the literature on fear and risk taking. Different theoretical traditions in emotion research make competing suggestions for how the fear and risk taking relationship may depend on the specific nature of the fear induction. For example, constructionist models of emotion propose that the experience of fear may vary widely depending on both the individual and the situational context (Lindquist & Barrett, 2008; Barrett, 2017b; Satpute & Lindquist, 2019). Fear of a predator may involve different features (Mobbs & Kim, 2015) than fear of heights, fear of having a difficult conversation with a parent, or fear of test taking (Barrett, 2006; James, 1884; Satpute & Lindquist, 2019). Some fears are experienced as unpleasurable, but others as pleasurable (Condon, Wilson-Mendenhall, & Barrett, 2014). From this perspective, it would be of interest to measure the variety of features that constitute fear in different situations and examine how these features relate with risk taking (see also Baumann & DeSteno, 2012, for an example of how context impacts the influence of anger on risk taking).

Future work may also investigate the complexity of the risk situation. Most research studies emphasize a single dimension along which risk is manipulated (e.g. monetary risk), holding other sources of potential risk constant. Yet, in everyday situations, risk may be present along multiple dimensions (Lynn, Wormwood, Barrett, & Quigley, 2015). For example, Kugler et al. (2012) measured ‘person-based risk’, in which a safe monetary outcome required participants to take a risk in the social domain by forsaking cooperation and thus risking social repercussion. There has also been a call for developing risk taking tasks that are more naturalistic (Schonberg, Fox, & Poldrack, 2011). Future work that examines both fear and risk taking as complex, multifaceted constructs, may help uncover moderators that explain the heterogeneity we observed and also support generalization of extant findings to fear and risk taking in everyday life.

Our meta-analysis focused on incidental influences of fear and anxiety on risk. That is, we specifically examined situations in which fear and anxiety were incidental to (i.e., not directly related to) the content of the risky situations themselves. Presumably, such studies enable a better look at the underlying psychological mechanisms (e.g., by reducing demand characteristics or mitigating internal consistency goals). However, there is also a large body of work in applied contexts that examines fears and anxieties that are integral to (i.e., normatively relevant for or tailored to) the risk taking context (Consedine & Moskowitz, 2007; Ferrer & Klein, 2015). For example, researchers have examined how anticipated fear and regret when evaluating a gamble influences gambling decisions (Larrick & Boles, 1995; Mellers, Schwartz, Ho, & Ritov, 1997) and how fears and anxieties pertaining to cancer relate with obtaining screenings for breast cancer (Consedine, Magai, Krivoshekova, Ryzewicz, & Neugut, 2004). This work suggests that cancer-related fears and anxieties are not monolithic but heterogeneous. They include fear of pain during examination, fear of the uncertainty about whether one has cancer or not, fear of having cancer, and more dimensions that can interact to promote or prevent a person from obtaining a screening (Consedine, Magai, Krivoshekova, Ryzewicz, & Neugut, 2004). Such work dovetails with the idea that laboratory paradigms, too, may benefit from more finely deconstructing experiments in terms of the specific contents elicited by the fear induction and their relation to the attributes of particular risk taking situations.

While our focus was on behavioral research, there is also a sizeable literature on the neural basis of fear and risk. Most neural studies examine fear and anxiety separately from risk (Lindquist, Wager, Kober, Bliss-Moreau, & Barrett, 2012; Satpute & Lindquist, 2019; Vytal & Hamann, 2010), or risk separately from fear and anxiety (Mohr, Biele, & Heekeren, 2010; Wu, Sacchet, & Knutson, 2012), even though it has been noted across these literatures that fear and anxiety, risk perception and risk taking, frequently engage some of the same areas in common including the insula, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex/pre-supplementary motor area, and amygdala (Hartley & Phelps, 2012; Kuhnen & Knutson, 2005; Mohr et al., 2010). One study that specifically examined the neural mediators between trait anxiety and risk found that frontal midline theta power during the decision phase was associated with reduced risk taking and also mediated the relationship between trait anxiety and reduced risk (Schmidt, Kanis, Holroyd, Miltner, & Hewig, 2018). Frontal midline theta power has been previously linked with activity in the mid cingulate cortex (Cavanagh & Shackman, 2015), which in turn has been linked to both anxiety and cognitive control (Cavanagh & Shackman, 2015; Eisenberger, Lieberman, & Satpute, 2005). Physiological arousal also plays a role in risk seeking and risk avoidant behavior (Schmidt, Mussel, & Hewig, 2013) and may also involve some of these same neural loci (Critchley, Mathias, & Dolan, 2001). In relation to our meta-analytic findings, a better understanding of these neural and physiological components may help reveal when fear and anxiety are associated with increased risk aversion or at times with risk seeking.

In conclusion, our meta-analytic review suggests that many studies have observed a relationship between fear and risk taking, but this relationship is highly variable across studies. The literature in general involves a wide variety of heterogeneous methods for inducing and measuring fear, and also for measuring risk. This variance across studies was partially explained by some of our moderators. Studies with clinically anxious samples showed greater risk aversion on average, and studies using tangible rewards (e.g., monetary rewards) also showed greater effects of fear on risk aversion. Nevertheless, there remains high variability in effect sizes across studies suggesting that there is need to test the uniformity of the relationship between fear/anxiety and risk. Research that manipulates and measures fear and anxiety, risk perception and risk taking, using methods that systematically capture different underlying features, will be important to understand whether their functional relationship is singular or heterogeneous and also for which situations laboratory studies generalize to more naturalistic settings.

Acknowledgments

Research reported in this publication was supported by National Cancer Institute of the National Institutes of Health under award number U01CA193632, by the Department of Graduate Education of the National Science Foundation, NCS award number 1835309.

Footnotes

1

One non-independent effect size included self-reported scores on the Worry Domains Questionnaire in addition to STAI. Critically, we inspected the WDQ v. the STAI and found that both scales have similar items. In fact, the STAI uses ‘worry’ among its items in the questionnaire. Correlations between the two are also high: r = 0.71 and r = 0.73 in two separate studies (Davey, 1993; Rijsoort, Emmelkamp, & Vervaeke, 1999). Moreover, the WDQ provides no instructions to distinguish worry from anxiety. Because the WDQ includes questions analogous to anxiety surveys, but simply refers to the scale as ‘worry’, we considered it simply a linguistic distinction rather than a conceptual one. Notably, including this one non-independent effect had no impact on the results nor the conclusions of our meta-analysis.

2

Parallel analyses where non-independent effect sizes were aggregated to allow for standard multiple regression analyses revealed the same pattern of results.

References

  1. Adolphs R, & Anderson DJ (2018). The Neuroscience of Emotion: A New Synthesis. United Kingdom: Princeton University Press. [Google Scholar]
  2. Angie AD, Connelly S, Waples EP, & Kligyte V (2011). The influence of discrete emotions on judgement and decision-making: A meta-analytic review. Cognition & Emotion, 25(8), 1393–1422. 10.1080/02699931.2010.550751 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Barrett LF (2006). Solving the emotion paradox: Categorization and the experience of emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10(1), 20–46. 10.1207/s15327957pspr1001_2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Barrett LF (2017). The theory of constructed emotion: An active inference account of interoception and categorization. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(1), 1–23. 10.1093/scan/nsw154 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Baumann J, & DeSteno D (2012). Context explains divergent effects of anger on risk taking. Emotion, 12(6), 1196–1199. 10.1037/a0029788 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bechara A, Damasio AR, Damasio H, & Anderson SW (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50(1–3), 7–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Beck A, Epstein N, Brown G, & Steer R (1988). An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56(6), 893–897. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bernstein DA, & Allen GJ (1969). Fear survey schedule (II): Normative data and factor analyses based upon a large college sample. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 7(4), 403–407. 10.1016/0005-7967(69)90072-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  9. Blais A-R, & Weber EU (2006). A Domain-Specific Risk-Taking (DOSPERT) scale for adult populations. Judgment and Decision Making, 1(1), 33–47. [Google Scholar]
  10. Bolier L, Haverman M, Westerhof GJ, Riper H, Smit F, & Bohlmeijer E (2013). Positive psychology interventions: A meta-analysis of randomized controlled studies. BMC Public Health, 13, 1–20. 10.1186/1471-2458-13-119 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Brand M, Greco R, Schuster A, Kalbe E, Fujiwara E, Markowitsch HJ, & Kessler J (2002). The game of dice—A new test for the assessment of risktaking behavior. Neurorehabilitation & Neural Repair, 16, 142–143. [Google Scholar]
  12. Brown JD, & Walsh-Childers K (2002). Effects of media on personal and public health In Bryant J. & Mary Beth O. (Eds.), Media effects: Advances in theory and research, 2, 453–488. [Google Scholar]
  13. Cardeña E, Koopman C, Classen C, Waelde LC, & Spiegel D (2000). Psychometric properties of the Stanford Acute Stress Reaction Questionnaire (SASRQ): A valid and reliable measure of acute stress. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 13(4), 719–734. 10.1023/A:1007822603186 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Cavanagh JF, & Shackman AJ (2015). Frontal midline theta reflects anxiety and cognitive control: Meta-analytic evidence. Journal of Physiology-Paris, 109(1–3), 3–15. 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2014.04.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Charpentier CJ, Aylward J, Roiser JP, & Robinson OJ (2017). Enhanced risk aversion, but not loss aversion, in unmedicated pathological anxiety. Biological Psychiatry, 81(12), 1014–1022. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.12.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Charpentier CJ, Hindocha C, Roiser JP, & Robinson OJ (2016). Anxiety promotes memory for mood-congruent faces but does not alter loss aversion. Scientific Reports, 6, 24746 10.1038/srep24746 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Chua Chow C, & Sarin RK (2002). Known, unknown, and unknowable uncertainties. theory and decision, 52(2), 127–138. 10.1023/A:1015544715608 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  18. Clore GL, & Ortony A (2013). Psychological construction in the OCC model of emotion. Emotion Review, 5(4), 335–343. 10.1177/1754073913489751 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Cochran WG (1954). The combination of estimates from different experiments. Biometrics, 10(1), 101 10.2307/3001666 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  20. Cohen J (2013). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
  21. Condon P, Wilson-Mendenhall CD, & Barrett LF (2014). What is a positive emotion?: The psychological construction of pleasant fear and unpleasant happiness In Tugade MM, Shiota MN, & Kirby LD (Eds.), Handbook of positive emotions (p. 60–81). Guilford Press, New York, NY. [Google Scholar]
  22. Consedine NS, Magai C, Krivoshekova YS, Ryzewicz L, & Neugut AI (2004). Fear, anxiety, worry, and breast cancer screening behavior: A critical review. Cancer Epidemiology, Biomarkers & Prevention: A Publication of the American Association for Cancer Research, Cosponsored by the American Society of Preventive Oncology, 13(4), 501–510. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Consedine NS, & Moskowitz JT (2007). The role of discrete emotions in health outcomes: A critical review. Applied and Preventive Psychology, 12(2), 59–75. 10.1016/j.appsy.2007.09.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  24. Critchley HD, Mathias CJ, & Dolan RJ (2001). Neural activity in the human brain relating to uncertainty and arousal during anticipation. Neuron, 29(2), 537–545. 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00225-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Davey GC (1993). A comparison of three worry questionnaires. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31(1), 51–56. 10.1016/0005-7967(93)90042-S [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. De Groot K, & Thurik R (2018). Disentangling risk and uncertainty: when risk-taking measures are not about risk. Frontiers in Psychology, 9 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02194 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Derogatis LR, & Melisaratos N (1983). The Brief Symptom Inventory: An introductory report. Psychological Medicine, 13(3), 595–605. 10.1017/S0033291700048017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Dowling NA, Merkouris SS, Greenwood CJ, Oldenhof E, Toumbourou JW, & Youssef GJ (2017). Early risk and protective factors for problem gambling: A systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 51, 109–124. 10.1016/j.cpr.2016.10.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Eisenberger NI, Lieberman MD, & Satpute AB (2005). Personality from a controlled processing perspective: An fMRI study of neuroticism, extraversion, and self-consciousness. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 5(2), 169–181. 10.3758/CABN.5.2.169 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Ekman P, & Cordaro D (2011). What is meant by calling emotions basic. Emotion Review, 3(4), 364–370. 10.1177/1754073911410740 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  31. Ellsworth PC, & Scherer KR (2003). Appraisal processes in emotion In Davidson RJ, Scherer KR, & Goldsmith HH (Eds.), Series in affective science. Handbook of affective sciences (p. 572–595). New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  32. Fanselow MS (1991). The Midbrain Periaqueductal Gray as a Coordinator of Action in Response to Fear and Anxiety. In Depaulis A & Bandler R (Eds.), The Midbrain Periaqueductal Gray Matter: Functional, Anatomical, and Neurochemical Organization (pp. 151–173). New York: Plenum Press; 10.1007/978-1-4615-3302-3_10 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  33. Fanselow MS, & Lester LS (1988). A functional behavioristic approach to aversively motivated behavior: Predatory imminence as a determinant of the topography of defensive behavior In Bolles RC & Beecher MD (Eds.), Evolution and learning (pp. 185–212). Hillsdale, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. [Google Scholar]
  34. Farvolden P, McBride C, Bagby RM, & Ravitz P (2003). A Web-based screening instrument for depression and anxiety disorders in primary care. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 5(3), e23 10.2196/jmir.5.3.e23 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Ferrer R, & Klein WM (2015). Risk perceptions and health behavior. Current Opinion in Psychology, 5, 85–89. 10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.03.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Glassner B (2010). The Culture of Fear: Why Americans Are Afraid of the Wrong Things: Crime, Drugs, Minorities, Teen Moms, Killer Kids, Muta. Hachette UK: Basic Books. [Google Scholar]
  37. Hartley CA, & Phelps EA (2012). Anxiety and decision-making. Biological Psychiatry, 72(2), 113–118. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.12.027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Hunt MK, Hopko DR, Bare R, Lejuez CW, & Robinson EV (2005). Construct validity of the Balloon Analog Risk Task (BART): associations with psychopathy and impulsivity. Assessment, 12(4), 416–428. 10.1177/1073191105278740 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Irwin JR, McClelland GH, & Schulze WD (1992). Hypothetical and real consequences in experimental auctions for insurance against low-probability risks. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 5(2), 107–116. 10.1002/bdm.3960050203 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  40. James W (1884). II.—WHAT IS AN EMOTION ? Mind, os-IX(34), 188–205. 10.1093/mind/os-IX.34.188 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  41. Kahneman D, & Tversky A (1979). Prospect Theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47(2), 263–291. 10.2307/1914185 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  42. Kaplan S, & Garrick BJ (1981). On the quantitative definition of risk. Risk Analysis, 1(1), 11–27. 10.1111/j.1539-6924.1981.tb01350.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Kugler T, Connolly T, & Ordóñez LD (2012). Emotion, decision, and risk: betting on gambles versus betting on people. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 25(2), 123–134. 10.1002/bdm.724 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  44. Kuhnen CM, & Knutson B (2005). The neural basis of financial risk taking. Neuron, 47(5), 763–770. 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.08.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Larrick RP, & Boles TL (1995). Avoiding Regret in Decisions with Feedback: A Negotiation Example. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 63(1), 87–97. 10.1006/obhd.1995.1064 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  46. LeDoux JE (2015). Anxious: Using the Brain to Understand and Treat Fear and Anxiety. New York: Penguin. [Google Scholar]
  47. Lejuez CW, Read JP, Kahler CW, Richards JB, Ramsey SE, Stuart GL, … Brown RA (2002). Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: The Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). Journal of Experimental Psychology. Applied, 8(2), 75–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Lerner JS, & Keltner D (2001). Fear, anger, and risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(1), 146–159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Lerner Jennifer S., Li Y, Valdesolo P, & Kassam KS. (2015). Emotion and Decision Making. Annual Review of Psychology, 66(1), 799–823. 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115043 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Levenson RW (2011). Basic Emotion Questions. Emotion Review, 3(4), 379–386. 10.1177/1754073911410743 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  51. Lindquist KA, & Barrett LF (2008). Constructing Emotion: The Experience of Fear as a Conceptual Act. Psychological Science, 19(9), 898–903. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02174.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Lindquist KA, Wager TD, Kober H, Bliss-Moreau E, & Barrett LF (2012). The brain basis of emotion: A meta-analytic review. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(3), 121–143. 10.1017/S0140525X11000446 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Loewenstein GF, Weber EU, Hsee CK, & Welch N (2001). Risk as feelings. Psychological Bulletin, 127(2), 267–286. 10.1037/0033-2909.127.2.267 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Lynn SK, Wormwood JB, Barrett LF, & Quigley KS (2015). Decision making from economic and signal detection perspectives: Development of an integrated framework. Frontiers in Psychology, 6 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00952 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. March JS, Parker JD, Sullivan K, Stallings P, & Conners CK (1997). The Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children (MASC): Factor structure, reliability, and validity. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(4), 554–565. 10.1097/00004583-199704000-00019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Mayo-Wilson E, Dias S, Mavranezouli I, Kew K, Clark DM, Ades AE, & Pilling S (2014). Psychological and pharmacological interventions for social anxiety disorder in adults: A systematic review and network meta-analysis. The Lancet. Psychiatry, 1(5), 368–376. 10.1016/S2215-0366(14)70329-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Mellers BA, Schwartz A, Ho K, & Ritov I (1997). Decision Affect Theory: Emotional reactions to the outcomes of risky options. Psychological Science, 8(6), 423–429. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00455.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  58. Mobbs D (2018). The ethological deconstruction of fear(s). Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 24, 32–37. 10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.02.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Mobbs D, & Kim JJ (2015). Neuroethological studies of fear, anxiety, and risky decision-making in rodents and humans. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 5, 8–15. 10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.06.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Mohr PNC, Biele G, & Heekeren HR (2010). Neural Processing of Risk. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(19), 6613–6619. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0003-10.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Morina N, Koerssen R, & Pollet TV (2016). Interventions for children and adolescents with posttraumatic stress disorder: A meta-analysis of comparative outcome studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 47, 41–54. 10.1016/j.cpr.2016.05.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Newman MG, Zuellig AR, Kachin KE, Constantino MJ, Przeworski A, Erickson T, & Cashman-McGrath L (2002). Preliminary reliability and validity of the generalized anxiety disorder questionnaire-IV: A revised self-report diagnostic measure of generalized anxiety disorder. Behavior Therapy, 33(2), 215–233. 10.1016/S0005-7894(02)80026-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  63. Niedenthal PM, Krauth-Gruber S, & Ric F (2006). Psychology of emotion: Interpersonal, experimental and cognitive approaches. New York: Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
  64. Öhman A (2008). Fear and anxiety: Overlaps and dissociations In Handbook of emotions, 3rd ed (pp. 709–728). New York, NY, US: The Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
  65. Piet J, & Hougaard E (2011). The effect of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for prevention of relapse in recurrent major depressive disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(6), 1032–1040. 10.1016/j.cpr.2011.05.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Raghunathan R, & Pham MT (1999). All negative moods are not equal: motivational influences of anxiety and sadness on decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 79(1), 56–77. 10.1006/obhd.1999.2838 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Reinhart CM, & Rogoff KS (2009). This Time Is Different: Eight Centuries of Financial Folly. United Kingdom: Princeton University Press. [Google Scholar]
  68. Reiss S, Peterson RA, Gursky DM, & McNally RJ (1986). Anxiety sensitivity, anxiety frequency and the prediction of fearfulness. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 24(1), 1–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Reiss Steven. (1991). Expectancy model of fear, anxiety, and panic. Clinical Psychology Review, 11(2), 141–153. 10.1016/0272-7358(91)90092-9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  70. Rijsoort S van, Emmelkamp P, & Vervaeke G. (1999). The Penn State Worry Questionnaire and the Worry Domains Questionnaire: Structure, reliability and validity. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 6(4), 297–307. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  71. Rosenthal R (1994). Parametric measures of effect size In Cooper H, Hedges LV, & Valentine JC (Eds.), The handbook of research synthesis (pp. 231–244). New York, NY, US: Russell Sage Foundation. [Google Scholar]
  72. Satpute AB, & Lindquist KA (2019). The default mode network’s role in discrete emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23(10), 851–864. 10.1016/j.tics.2019.07.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Schmidt B, Kanis H, Holroyd CB, Miltner WHR, & Hewig J (2018). Anxious gambling: Anxiety is associated with higher frontal midline theta predicting less risky decisions. Psychophysiology, 55(10), e13210 10.1111/psyp.13210 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Schmidt B, Mussel P, & Hewig J (2013). I’m too calm-Let’s take a risk! On the impact of state and trait arousal on risk taking: I’m too calm-Let’s take a risk! Psychophysiology, 50(5), 498–503. 10.1111/psyp.12032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Schonberg T, Fox CR, & Poldrack RA (2011). Mind the gap: Bridging economic and naturalistic risk-taking with cognitive neuroscience. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(1), 11–19. 10.1016/j.tics.2010.10.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, … Dunbar GC (1998). The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): The development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59 Suppl 20, 22–33;quiz 34–57. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Simonsohn U, Nelson LD, & Simmons JP (2014). P-curve: A key to the file-drawer. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 143(2), 534–547. 10.1037/a0033242 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Slovic P, & Peters E (2006). Risk perception and affect. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15(6), 322–325. 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2006.00461.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  79. Smith VL, & Walker JM (1993). Monetary rewards and decision cost in experimental economics. Economic Inquiry, 31(2), 245–261. 10.1111/j.1465-7295.1993.tb00881.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  80. Spielberger CD (1970). STAI manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Self-Evaluation Questionnaire, 1–24. [Google Scholar]
  81. Tallis F, Davey GCL, & Bond A (1994). The Worry Domains Questionnaire. In Wiley Series in Clinical Psychology. Worrying: Perspectives on theory, assessment and treatment (pp. 285–297). Oxford, England: John Wiley & Sons. [Google Scholar]
  82. Taylor JA (1953). A personality scale of manifest anxiety. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 48(2), 285–290. 10.1037/h0056264 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Tversky A, & Kahneman D (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science, 211(4481), 453–458. 10.1126/science.7455683 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Viechtbauer W (2010). Conducting Meta-Analyses in R with the metafor Package. Journal of Statistical Software, 36(3), 1–48. [Google Scholar]
  85. Vytal K, & Hamann S (2010). Neuroimaging support for discrete neural correlates of basic emotions: A voxel-based meta-analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(12), 2864–2885. 10.1162/jocn.2009.21366 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Watson D, Clark LA, & Tellegen A (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063–1070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Watson D, & Friend R (1969). Measurement of social-evaluative anxiety. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 33(4), 448–457. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Weber EU, Blais AR, & Betz NE (2002). A domain‐specific risk‐attitude scale: Measuring risk perceptions and risk behaviors. Journal of behavioral decision making, 15(4), 263–290. [Google Scholar]
  89. Wu CC, Sacchet MD, & Knutson B (2012). Toward an Affective Neuroscience Account of Financial Risk Taking. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 6 10.3389/fnins.2012.00159 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Zhang D, & Gu R (2018). Behavioral preference in sequential decision-making and its association with anxiety. Human Brain Mapping, 39(6), 2482–2499. 10.1002/hbm.24016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Zigmond AS, & Snaith RP (1983). The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 67(6), 361–370. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Studies included in the analysis

  1. Bagneux V, Bollon T, & Dantzer C (2012). Do (un)certainty appraisal tendencies reverse the influence of emotions on risk taking in sequential tasks? Cognition and Emotion, 26(3), 568–576. 10.1080/02699931.2011.602237 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Broman-Fulks JJ, Urbaniak A, Bondy CL, & Toomey KJ (2014). Anxiety sensitivity and risk-taking behavior. Anxiety, Stress, & Coping, 27(6), 619–632. 10.1080/10615806.2014.896906 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Charpentier CJ, Aylward J, Roiser JP, & Robinson OJ (2017). Enhanced Risk Aversion, But Not Loss Aversion, in Unmedicated Pathological Anxiety. Biological Psychiatry, 81(12), 1014–1022. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.12.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Charpentier CJ, Hindocha C, Roiser JP, & Robinson OJ (2016). Anxiety promotes memory for mood-congruent faces but does not alter loss aversion. Scientific Reports, 6, 24746 10.1038/srep24746 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Chiou W-B, Chang M-H, & Chen C-L (2009). The Moderating Role of Personal Relevance on Differential Priming of Anxiety and Sadness on Perceived Travel Risk: A Replication. Psychological Reports, 104(2), 500–508. 10.2466/PR0.104.2.500-508 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Constans JI (2001). Worry propensity and the perception of risk. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39(6), 721–729. 10.1016/S0005-7967(00)00037-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Dorfman J, Rosen D, Pine D, & Ernst M (2016). Anxiety and Gender Influence Reward-Related Processes in Children and Adolescents. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 26(4), 380–390. 10.1089/cap.2015.0008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Foo M (2011). Emotions and Entrepreneurial Opportunity Evaluation. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice, 35(2), 375–393. 10.1111/j.1540-6520.2009.00357.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  9. Galván A, & Peris TS (2014). Neural Correlates of Risky Decision Making in Anxious Youth and Healthy Controls. Depression and Anxiety, 31(7), 591–598. 10.1002/da.22276 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Gambetti E, & Giusberti F (2014). The role of anxiety and anger traits in financial field. Mind & Society, 13(2), 271–284. 10.1007/s11299-014-0150-z [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  11. Giorgetta C, Grecucci A, Zuanon S, Perini L, Balestrieri M, Bonini N, … Brambilla P (2012). Reduced risk-taking behavior as a trait feature of anxiety. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 12(6), 1373–1383. 10.1037/a0029119 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Gu R, Wu R, Broster LS, Jiang Y, Xu R, Yang Q, … Luo Y-J (2017). Trait Anxiety and Economic Risk Avoidance Are Not Necessarily Associated: Evidence from the Framing Effect. Frontiers in Psychology, 8 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00092 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Habib M, Cassotti M, Moutier S, Houdé O, & Borst G (2015). Fear and anger have opposite effects on risk seeking in the gain frame. Frontiers in Psychology, 6 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00253 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Howlett JR, & Paulus MP (2017). Individual Differences in Subjective Utility and Risk Preferences: The Influence of Hedonic Capacity and Trait Anxiety. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 8 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00088 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Huh HJ, Baek K, Kwon J-H, Jeong J, & Chae J-H (2016). Impact of childhood trauma and cognitive emotion regulation strategies on risk-aversive and loss-aversive patterns of decision-making in patients with depression. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 21(6), 447–461. 10.1080/13546805.2016.1230053 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Hunt MK, Hopko DR, Bare R, Lejuez CW, & Robinson EV (2005). Construct Validity of the Balloon Analog Risk Task (BART): Associations With Psychopathy and Impulsivity. Assessment, 12(4), 416–428. 10.1177/1073191105278740 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Jakuszkowiak-Wojten K, Raczak A, Landowski J, Wiglusz MS, Gałuszko-Węgielnik M, Krysta K, & Cubała WJ (2017). Decision-making in panic disorder. Preliminary report. Psychiatria Danubina, 29(Suppl 3), 353–356. https://europepmc.org/abstract/med/28953790 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Juhasz G, Downey D, Hinvest N, Thomas E, Chase D, Toth ZG, … Anderson IM (2010). Risk-Taking Behavior in a Gambling Task Associated with Variations in the Tryptophan Hydroxylase 2 Gene: Relevance to Psychiatric Disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35(5), 1109–1119. 10.1038/npp.2009.216 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Kowert PA, & Hermann MG (1997). Who Takes Risks?: Daring and Caution in Foreign Policy Making. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 41(5), 611–637. 10.1177/0022002797041005001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  20. Kugler T, Connolly T, & Ordóñez LD (2012). Emotion, Decision, and Risk: Betting on Gambles versus Betting on People. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 25(2), 123–134. 10.1002/bdm.724 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  21. Lauriola M, Russo PM, Lucidi F, Violani C, & Levin IP (2005). The role of personality in positively and negatively framed risky health decisions. Personality and Individual Differences, 38(1), 45–59. 10.1016/j.paid.2004.03.020 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  22. Lee CJ, & Andrade EB (2011). Fear, Social Projection, and Financial Decision Making. Journal of Marketing Research, 48(SPL), S121–S129. 10.1509/jmkr.48.SPL.S121 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  23. Lench H, & Levine L (2005). Effects of fear on risk and control judgements and memory: Implications for health promotion messages. Cognition and Emotion, 19(7), 1049–1069. 10.1080/02699930500203112 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  24. Lerner JS, & Keltner D (2001). Fear, anger, and risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(1), 146–159. 10.1037//0022-3514.81.1.146 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Lerner Jennifer S., Gonzalez RM, Small DA, & Fischhoff B. (2003). Effects of Fear and Anger on Perceived Risks of Terrorism: A National Field Experiment. Psychological Science, 14(2), 144–150. 10.1111/1467-9280.01433 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Lerner Jennifer S., & Keltner D. (2000). Beyond valence: Toward a model of emotion-specific influences on judgement and choice. Cognition and Emotion, 14(4), 473–493. 10.1080/026999300402763 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  27. Lorian CN, & Grisham JR (2011). Clinical implications of risk aversion: An online study of risk-avoidance and treatment utilization in pathological anxiety. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 25(6), 840–848. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2011.04.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Lorian CN, Mahoney A, & Grisham JR (2012). Playing it safe: An examination of risk-avoidance in an anxious treatment-seeking sample. Journal of Affective Disorders, 141(1), 63–71. 10.1016/j.jad.2012.02.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Lucidi F, Giannini AM, Sgalla R, Mallia L, Devoto A, & Reichmann S (2010). Young novice driver subtypes: Relationship to driving violations, errors and lapses. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 42(6), 1689–1696. 10.1016/j.aap.2010.04.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Maner JK, & Gerend MA (2007). Motivationally selective risk judgments: Do fear and curiosity boost the boons or the banes? Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 103(2), 256–267. 10.1016/j.obhdp.2006.08.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  31. Maner JK, Richey JA, Cromer K, Mallott M, Lejuez CW, Joiner TE, & Schmidt NB (2007). Dispositional anxiety and risk-avoidant decision-making. Personality and Individual Differences, 42(4), 665–675. 10.1016/j.paid.2006.08.016 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  32. Maner JK, & Schmidt NB (2006). The Role of Risk Avoidance in Anxiety. Behavior Therapy, 37(2), 181–189. 10.1016/j.beth.2005.11.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Mitte K (2007). Anxiety and risky decision-making: The role of subjective probability and subjective costs of negative events. Personality and Individual Differences, 43(2), 243–253. 10.1016/j.paid.2006.11.028 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  34. Mueller EM, Nguyen J, Ray WJ, & Borkovec TD (2010). Future-oriented decision-making in Generalized Anxiety Disorder is evident across different versions of the Iowa Gambling Task. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 41(2), 165–171. 10.1016/j.jbtep.2009.12.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Nan X (2017). Influence of Incidental Discrete Emotions on Health Risk Perception and Persuasion. Health Communication, 32(6), 721–729. 10.1080/10410236.2016.1168004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Nesse RM, & Klaas R (1994). Risk perception by patients with anxiety disorders. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 182(8), 465–470. 10.1097/00005053-199408000-00008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Ortega AR, Ramírez E, Colmenero JM, & García-Viedma M del R. (2017). Negative Affect, Decision Making, and Attentional Networks. Journal of Attention Disorders, 21(3), 247–253. 10.1177/1087054712465336 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Panno A, Donati MA, Milioni M, Chiesi F, & Primi C (2018). Why Women Take Fewer Risk Than Men Do: The Mediating Role of State Anxiety. Sex Roles, 78(3), 286–294. 10.1007/s11199-017-0781-8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  39. Raghunathan R, & Pham MT (1999). All Negative Moods Are Not Equal: Motivational Influences of Anxiety and Sadness on Decision Making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 79(1), 56–77. 10.1006/obhd.1999.2838 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Ramírez E, Ortega AR, & Reyes Del Paso GA (2015). Anxiety, attention, and decision making: The moderating role of heart rate variability. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 98(3, Part 1), 490–496. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.10.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Richards JM, Patel N, Daniele-Zegarelli T, MacPherson L, Lejuez CW, & Ernst M (2015). Social anxiety, acute social stress, and reward parameters interact to predict risky decision-making among adolescents. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 29, 25–34. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2014.10.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Schulreich S, Gerhardt H, & Heekeren HR (2016). Incidental fear cues increase monetary loss aversion. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 16(3), 402–412. 10.1037/emo0000124 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Stöber J (1997). Trait anxiety and pessimistic appraisal of risk and chance. Personality and Individual Differences, 22(4), 465–476. 10.1016/S0191-8869(96)00232-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  44. Stone LA (1964). The Influence of Selected Individual-Difference Variables upon Utility for Risk. The Journal of General Psychology, 70(1), 29–32. 10.1080/00221309.1964.9920571 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Takács Á, Kóbor A, Janacsek K, Honbolygó F, Csépe V, & Németh D (2015). High trait anxiety is associated with attenuated feedback-related negativity in risky decision making. Neuroscience Letters, 600, 188–192. 10.1016/j.neulet.2015.06.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Tsai M-H, & Young MJ (2010). Anger, fear, and escalation of commitment. Cognition and Emotion, 24(6), 962–973. 10.1080/02699930903050631 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  47. Yang Q, Zhao D, Wu Y, Tang P, Gu R, & Luo Y (2018). Differentiating the influence of incidental anger and fear on risk decision-making. Physiology & Behavior, 184, 179–188. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.11.028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Zhang D, & Gu R (2018). Behavioral preference in sequential decision-making and its association with anxiety. Human Brain Mapping, 39(6), 2482–2499. 10.1002/hbm.24016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Zhang F, Xiao L, & Gu R (2017). Does Gender Matter in the Relationship between Anxiety and Decision-Making? Frontiers in Psychology, 8 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02231 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Zhang L, Wang K, Zhu C, Yu F, & Chen X (2015). Trait Anxiety Has Effect on Decision Making under Ambiguity but Not Decision Making under Risk. PLOS ONE, 10(5), e0127189 10.1371/journal.pone.0127189 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES