Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2021 Mar 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Sch Health. 2020 Jan 5;90(3):182–193. doi: 10.1111/josh.12863

School Climate as an Intervention to Reduce Academic Failure and Educate the Whole Child: A Longitudinal Study

Shay M Daily a, Michael J Mann b, Christa L Lilly c, Angela M Dyer d, Megan L Smith e, Alfgeir L Kristjansson f
PMCID: PMC7427837  NIHMSID: NIHMS1614426  PMID: 31903632

Abstract

BACKGROUND

Preventing student academic failure is crucial to student health and life success. Previous studies suggest a positive school climate may reduce students’ risk for academic failure and contribute to academic success. The purpose of this study was to determine the longitudinal associations between school climate and academic grades in a group of middle school students who transition into high school.

METHODS

Parallel latent growth curve modeling was used to examine changes among study variables longitudinally using a sample of 2604 in 6th, 7th, and 8th-grade students across 16 regional schools located in 3 counties in West Virginia.

RESULTS

Students with higher perceptions of a positive school climate exhibited sustained or improved academic achievement over time (β = 0.22 to 0.30, p < .01). Higher positive perceptions of school climate appear to sustain students who earn As/Bs (β = 0.20 to 0.27, p < .01) and strengthen students who earn Cs/Ds/Fs (β = −0.16 to −0.46, p < .05).

CONCLUSIONS

Positive student perceptions of school climate may sustain high academic performance while strengthening students who earn Cs/Ds/Fs. School climate may be useful as an intervention to support school-based health promotion to reduce the achievement gap in the United States.

Keywords: middle school, early adolescents, achievement, parallel latent growth analysis


Academic success and earning a high school diploma is strongly associated with health throughout the lifespan. Research suggests that a higher prevalence of chronic disease and early death disproportionately occurs among the least educated.24 In addition, people who leave school early are more likely to have occupational issues, social dysfunction, and engage in criminal behavior.58 Economic and social demands for an educated labor force underscore the value of at least completing a high school education, which is often the minimum requirement for gainful employment and college admission.9,10 Beyond the potential economic cost, educational attainment is also often required to open avenues for social mobility.11,12 Preventing student academic failure is therefore crucial to student health and to support success in adulthood.7,8,13

Preventing risky youth behaviors that may lead to less than desirable student outcomes continues to be a challenge.14,15 The US public (non-charter) school system serves an estimated 85% (47.3 million) of the child and adolescent population.1618 The public school system is, therefore, a promising location to implement large-scale primary prevention initiatives because of its broad capacity to provide health promotion and education to children and youth.19,20 In addition, social-ecological theories suggest learning and modeling stems from interactions within an environment while sharing experiences with others while in school.2124 As a result, schools are places where most children and adolescents develop behavioral patterns that may follow into adulthood.2528 Therefore, a natural partnership exists between educators and health promotion researchers making public schools potentially ideal locations to implement population health interventions and research.19,20 By working together, collaborative partnerships between public health and public education may have practical implications toward a shared and collective impact that may help accomplish national academic and health goals.

An Overview of School Climate and Academic Achievement

A positive school climate provides an excellent example of how a positive social environment may shape student behaviors.20,29 However, definitions of school climate tend to diverge.30 Tangible definitions are based on behavioral patterns of school life that are observed through norms, values, practices, and relationships.31 Theoretical definitions describe school climate as the “spirit” or “heart beat” of a school.32 Definition aside, most research has shown social relationships such as bonding with teachers and peers; order and safety such as respect for school rules; academic opportunities such as a sense of accomplishment and satisfaction with school; and school connectedness such as attachment and building social bonds to school tend to be essential constructs found in conceptual models of school climate.30,33,34

Current school climate research, with an emphasis on ecological approaches, suggests school climate can shape the interactions between students, teachers, families, and the broader community.35,36 From a school climate point-of-view, shaping may occur through high-quality classroom management and an emphasis on valued social norms where learning materializes, such as a classroom.31,33,37 A collective goal of most school climate research has been to empirically highlight the importance of non-academic factors in lieu of an overemphasis on curriculum and instruction to support student success.38 Studies focused on safe and positive school environments suggests strategies that foster a positive school climate may safeguard students from social inequalities like poverty toward improved academic performance.3941 Although, we know many factors associated with school climate occur while students are in school, the temporal nature of understanding how school climate shapes students’ academic success is far from complete.30 Longitudinal studies are needed to establish the dynamic process of school climate and its association to academic outcomes.42 If school climate and academic achievement change together, such evidence may further support the improvement of school-based interventions.

An Opportunity for School Climate to Promote Academic Success

Clarifying relationships between school climate and student outcomes is especially relevant now as two large-scale policy initiatives advocate for the evaluation of school climate to support alignment with academics to reduce educational and health disparities. First, the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) has developed the Whole School, Whole Child, Whole Community (WSCC) model to intersect health and education.43 WSCC encourages schools to engage students using a platform that best meets their health and developmental needs to achieve successful academic and health outcomes.44 Second, the US legislature put forth the Every Student Succeeds Act (ESSA).45 This legislation is meant to empower schools and encourages the integration of WSCC.46 The changes brought on by ESSA emphasize the use of school climate as an added measure of school safety and quality to compliment standardized metrics of school performance.47 This recommendation is important because school climate information may provide useful guidance for school management and pedagogical strategies (ie, bonding relationships48) that may impact educational and health outcomes.39 Hence, studies that illustrate longitudinal associations of school climate and academic achievement may have systemic implications that support decisions on how to reduce academic risk and potentially prevent negative student outcomes.

The purpose of this study was to determine the longitudinal associations between school climate and academic grades across 3 annual data collections (3 waves) from a group of middle school students who transition into high school. Due to the exploratory nature of this study, 3 research questions guided our methodology: (1) perceptions of school climate and self-reported academic grades would independently change over time, (2) measures of school climate would demonstrate positive associations to self-reported academic grades over time, and (3) students’ academic achievement would demonstrate positive associations with measures of school climate.

METHODS

Participants, Procedures, and Handling of Missing Data

Annual data collections (3 waves) occurred between 2015, 2016, and 2017 from moderately uniform student groups in 6th (37.8%), 7th (32.5%), and 8th (29.7%) grades across 16 regional schools from 3 counties located in West Virginia (WV) at baseline. For clarity, students in 6th grade in 2015 would be in 8th grade in 2017 and students in 7th grade would be in 9th grade and be in their first year of high school in WV. School size ranged from 130 (smallest) to 648 (largest) students from rural and suburban districts. Students from each county represent a spectrum of diverse characteristics from families living in severe isolation/poverty to modest privilege/affluence with 50% to 64% of children considered eligible for free or reduced price lunch.4952

A letter was sent to parents to provide an opportunity to exclude their children (parental optout rate<1%).53,54 Surveys were administered by classroom teachers with oversight from a school contact agent to ensure response confidentiality and data collection protocols. Participation was voluntary and made available to all students. Students were free to answer all or part of the survey and elect to stop participation at any time. For additional examples on the data source and collection procedures, see Kristjansson et al.5557

In 2015 (time 1), students at baseline provided 6364 eligible observations (response rate = 82.6%). In 2016 (time 2), students provided 6336 observations (response rate = 82.0%). In 2017 (time 3), students provided 6278 observations (response rate = 81.3%). Student data were then matched over time using a unique self-reported identification number yielding a final sample of 2604 retained cases (3 data points per participant). With participant dropout rates common for longitudinal studies this level of attrition was anticipated.58,59 To ensure data quality and estimate accuracy, omnibus tests under the Unrestricted Latent Class Indicator models for data missing completely at random (MCAR) were shown to be non-significant (all p > .05).60,61 Preliminary tests additionally demonstrated less than 5% missing on dependent variables under pairwise techniques on retained cases, which has been shown to produce stable estimates.62 Conservatively, we assumed missing data patterns as a function of missing at random (MAR).60 Missing data were then handled using full information maximum likelihood estimation (FIML).63,64 This strategy was chosen because FIML yields unbiased estimates under MAR hypotheses and is often equivalent to computationally heavy imputation techniques.6569

Measures

Academic grades

Self-reported grades were captured using a single question, “What were your FINAL grades in the following subjects LAST year?” for Mathematics and English. Responses were combined to represent students’ overall grades at each time point.7072 Academic grades were then pooled into ordered categories, Mostly As/Bs (coded 2), Mostly Cs (coded 1), and Mostly Ds/Fs (coded 0). Mostly As/Bs signified higher academic performance.

School climate

Three sub-scales (positive student-teacher relationships, order and safety, and opportunities for student engagement) were selected from the School Climate Measure (SCM) developed by Zullig et al.34,73,74 The 3 sub-scales were chosen because they have demonstrated the most robust psychometric support within the SCM and are common measures among other school climate instruments.73 SCM questions use a 5-point Likert type scale with response options “strongly disagree” (coded 1) to “strongly agree” (coded 5). Higher scores indicate a more positive perception of school climate.

Covariates

Study covariates were selected and supported using citations based on a review of school and student-based outcomes literature.

Biological sex

Biological sex57,75,76 was assessed by asking respondents “Are you a boy or girl?” Male (coded 0) and female (coded 1) were represented as a dichotomous time-invariant covariate.

Family structure

Respondents were asked to indicate their family structure57,75 using a 19-item multi-response question, “Which of the following persons live in your home?” For analysis, the question was dichotomized into “lives with both biological parents” (coded 1) and “other arrangements” (coded 0) and represented as a dichotomous time-invariant covariate.

Maternal education

Maternal education75,77 was captured by asking students to select 1 of 9 response options from a singular question “What is the highest level of schooling your mother has completed?” Responses were pooled into categories to simplify analyses, “college graduate” (coded 3), “high school graduate” (coded 2), “less than high school” (coded 1), and “I don’t know” (coded 0) and represented as a nominal categorical time-invariant covariate.

Data Analysis

Descriptive statistics were analyzed using SAS 9.4® (Cary, NC)78 and include frequencies, means, standard deviations, scale reliability, and confirmatory factor analysis estimates. Parallel latent growth curve modeling (PLGM, Figure 1) was selected to examine linear changes in school climate sub-scales and academic grades while controlling for sex, family structure, and maternal education.79 All PLGM analyses were performed in Mplus 8.0© (Los Angeles, CA)80 using a 2-tailed distribution with p-values equal to or less than .05. The complex option was used in Mplus to accommodate for statistical anomalies such as non-normality and non-independence of data with robust standard errors and probit transormation.8082 These options were used to account for children clustered in schools, which when overlooked, may produce inaccurate results. Numerical integration with unconstrained residuals80 was also used to appropriately handle the interaction between latent categorical (non-normal) and continuous (normal) intercepts (start-point) and slope (growth trajectory). The deviance statistic (−2LL),83,84 Akaike Information Criteria (AIC), and Bayesian Information Criteria (BIC) were used to judge which model best fit our sample data. Latent regression path estimates (B) and standard errors (SE) indicate the relationship between school climate and academic grades over time. Standardized regression estimates, represented by beta (β), were used to demonstrate the practical importance of the relationship between school climate and academic achievement. Lastly, to support this study’s findings, we used a sensitivity analysis and tested the same models on a middle school only sample of students who started in 6th grade and ended in 8th grade (students who did not transition into high school, N = 966) and a sample of high school only students (N = 531) who started in 9th and 10th grade and ended in 11th and 12th grade.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Parallel Latent Growth Model for School Climate and Academic Grades

RESULTS

Eighty percent of students reported earning As/Bs at time 1 and 74.9% at time 3. On average, students perceptions of school climate was slightly positive and ranged 3.3 (SD = 0.9) at time 1 and 3.7 (SD = 0.8) at time 3. All school climate sub-scales indicated acceptable reliability (α = 0.85 to 0.94) and factor analysis measurement fits between time 1 and time 3: χ2 = 929.17–950.61 (all df = 157, p ≤ .01), comparative fit index = 0.97–0.98, Tucker-Lewis index = 0.96–0.97, standardized root mean square residual = 0.02–0.03, root mean square error of approximation = all 0.044 (±CI 90% 0.041, 0.047). Table 1 reports additional descriptive statistics.

Table 1.

Sample Frequencies, Scale Means, Standard Deviations, and Internal consistency, N = 2604

2015
2016
2017
Variable N % N % N %

Academic grades
 Mostly As/Bs 1797 80.5 1689 77.5 1702 74.9
 Mostly Cs 327 14.7 362 16.6 399 17.6
 Mostly Ds/Fs 106 4.8 129 5.9 171 7.5
Biological sex
 Female/male 1138/950 54.5/45.5
Race
 White/all other races 1817/280 86.7/13.3
Maternal education
 Coll grad/HSgrad 696/615 34.2/30.2
 Less than HS/not sure 146/581 7.2/28.4
Family structure (lives with)
 Biological parents/other arrangement 1195/902 57.0/43.0
Scale variable M(SD) α M(SD) α M(SD) α
 Student-teacher relationships 3.6 (0.8) 0.91 3.4 (0.9) 0.93 3.3 (0.9) 0.94
 Order, safety, and discipline 3.7 (0.8) 0.85 3.6 (0.9) 0.88 3.5 (0.9) 0.90
 Student engagement 3.8 (0.8) 0.86 3.7 (0.9) 0.88 3.6 (0.9) 0.89

Coll, College; HS, High School; Grad, Graduate; α, Cronbach’s alpha coefficient.105

Missing observations were handled using pairwise techniques.

Race has been included to show sample homogeneity.

Figure 1 depicts a conceptual model of the PLGMs with results reported in Tables 24. For reporting parsimony and clarity please refer to Tables 24 for additional model statistics such as between-person intercept (start-point) and slope (growth) means.

Table 2.

Parallel Latent Growth Model Intercepts, Slopes, and Unstandardized/Standardized Regression Estimates for Student Teacher Relationships, N = 2511

Acad. Grades SC1 As/Bs SC1 Cs SC1 Ds/Fs SC1

Parameter B (SE) B (SE) B (SE) B (SE) B (SE) B (SE) B (SE) B (SE)

Intercept 0.47 (.13)** 3.42 (.06)** 0.47 (.13)** 3.42 (.06)** −0.73 (.09)** 3.42 (.05)** −1.86 (.21)** 3.42 (.06)**
Slope −0.92 (.21)** 0.18 (.15) −0.93 (.18)** 0.19 (.11) 0.33 (.11)** 0.21 (.14) 0.90 (.26)** 0.19 (.04)
Int. → slope −0.10 (.05)* −0.09 (.04)* −0.11 (.06) −0.09 (.04)* −0.11 (.12) −0.10 (.04)** −0.13 (.07)* −0.09 (.04)*
SC1 X grade −0.01 (.01) 0.23 (.06)** −0.01 (.01) 0.23 (.05)** 0.03 (.06) −0.11 (.02)* 0.01 (.02) −0.27 (.08)**
Slope ↔ slope 0.03 (.01)** 0.04 (.01)** −0.02 (.01)* −0.01 (.02)

Std Est β (SE) β (SE) β (SE) β (SE) β (SE) β (SE) β (SE) β (SE)

Int. → slope −0.33 (.16)* −0.25 (.09)** −0.35 (.20) −0.25 (.09)** −0.33 (.31) −0.26 (.09)** −0.48 (.24)* −0.24 (.10)*
SC1 X grade −0.08 (.07) 0.30 (.08)** −0.09 (.06) 0.27 (.08)** 0.10 (.06) −0.20 (.01)* 0.04 (.12) −0.45 (.11)**
Slope ↔ glope 0.28 (.13)* 0.30 (.10)** −0.29 (.15)* −0.09 (.23)
−2LL (FP) 24,558.14(29) 22,978.26 (28) 22,978.26 (28) 19,786.04 (28)
AIC/BIC 24,616.14/24,785.17 23,034.26/23,197.46 22,584.06/22,747.26 19,842.04/20,005.24
**

p < .01

*

p < .05.

−2LL, Deviance, FP, Free Parameters, Std. Est., Standardized Estimate, SC1, Student-Teacher Relationships, Int., Intercept, Acad. Grades, Academic Grades.

Estimates include covariates and pairwise adjustment. School Clusters = 16.

Table 4.

Parallel Latent Growth Model Intercepts, Slopes, and Unstandardized/Standardized Regression Estimates for Student Engagement, N = 2512

Acad. Grades SC3 As/Bs SC3 Cs SC3 Ds/Fs SC3

Parameter B (SE) B (SE) B (SE) B (SE) B (SE) B (SE) B (SE) B (SE)

Intercept 0.48 (.12)** 3.72 (.05)** 0.47 (.13)** 3.72 (.05)** −0.73 (.09)** 3.72 (.05)** −1.90 (.22)** 3.72 (.06)**
Slope −0.75 (.25)** 0.35 (.13)** −0.77 (.24)** 0.35 (.13)** 0.35 (.30)** 0.36 (.14)** 0.63 (.30)* 0.33 (.09)*
Int. → slope −0.09 (.05)* −0.14 (.03)** −0.11 (.06) −0.14 (.03)** −0.11 (.12) −0.14 (.03)** −0.14 (.07)* −0.14 (.03)**
SC3 X grade −0.01 (.01) 0.16 (.07)* −0.01 (.01) 0.17 (.06)** 0.02 (.02) −0.11 (.04)** −0.01 (.02) −0.17 (.08)*
Slope ↔ slope 0.03 (.01)** 0.03 (.01)** −0.01 (.01) −0.02 (.02)

Std Est β (SE) β (SE) β (SE) β (SE) β (SE) β (SE) β (SE) β (SE)

Int. → slope −0.32 (.16)* −0.29 (.06)** −0.35 (.20) −0.30 (.06)** −0.33 (.30) −0.30 (.06)** −0.54 (.02)* −0.29 (.06)**
SC1 Xdrug −0.03 (.06) 0.22 (.09)** −0.04 (.06) 0.20 (.08)** 0.06 (.07) −0.19 (.07)** −0.03 (.10) −0.30 (.13)*
Slope ↔ slope 0.26 (.10)** 0.24 (.10)* −0.15 (.15) −0.29 (.18)
−2LL(FP) 24,587.87 (28) 23,008.97 (28) 22,550.21 (28) 19,808.34(28)
AIC/BIC 24,645.87/24,814.90 23,064.97/23,228.17 22,606.21/22,769.42 19,864.34/20,027.55
**

p < .01

*

p < .05.

−2LL, Deviance, FP, Free Parameters, Std. Est., Standardized Estimate, SC2, Order and Safety, Int., Intercept, Acad. Grades, Academic Grades.

Estimates include covariates and pairwise adjustment. School Clusters = 16.

Table 2 reports the associations between student-teacher relationships (SC1) and academic grades. Changes in growth for student-teacher relationships across all academic grade groups were significant and ranged β = −0.26 to −0.24 (SE = .09 to .10, all p < .01). Changes in growth between student-teacher relationships on academic grades (cross-lagged paths) were significant and ranged −0.45 to 0.30 (SE = .08 to .11, all p < .05). Growth trajectories between academic grades on student-teacher relationships demonstrated no significant associations. Correlations between slopes ranged −0.29 to 0.30 (SE = .10 to .15, all p < .05).

Table 3 describes the associations between order and safety (SC 2) and academic grades. Growth trajectories for order and safety across all grade models were significant and reported relatively consistent changes β = −0.25 (all SE = .09, p < .01). Changes in growth for academic grades found significant associations for all-academic grades (ie, A-F) grouped together (−0.31, SE = .16, p < .05) and mostly Ds/Fs (−0.55, SE = .26, p < .05). The growth relationship between order and safety on academic grades were significant and ranged −0.46 to 0.29 (SE = .01 to .11, all p < .05). Growth trajectories between academic grades on order and safety indicated significant associations with the mostly As/Bs (−0.11, SE = .05, p < .05) and mostly Cs (−0.15, SE = .07, p < .05) groups. Significant correlations between slopes were found for the all-academic grades (ie, A-F) grouped together and the As/Bs group (0.25, SE = .11, p < .05).

Table 3.

Parallel Latent Growth Model Intercepts, Slopes, and Unstandardized/Standardized Regression Estimates for Order and Safety, N = 2512

Acad. Grades SC2 As/Bs SC2 Cs SC2 Ds/Fs SC2

Parameter B (SE) B (SE) B (SE) B (SE) B (SE) B (SE) B (SE) B (SE)
Intercept 0.47 (.12)** 3.64 (.06)** 0.47 (.18)** 3.64 (.06)** −0.74 (.18)** 3.64 (.05)** −1.89 (.22)** 3.64 (.06)**
Slope −0.96 (.25)** 0.28 (.18) −0.90 (.25)** 0.28 (.19) 0.27 (.14)* 0.30 (.19) 0.98 (.26)** 0.27 (.19)
Int. → slope −0.09 (.04)* −0.11 (.05)* −0.11 (.06) −0.12 (.05)* −0.12 (.05) −0.12 (.04)** −0.14 (.06)* −0.11 (.05)*
SC2 X grade −0.02 (.01) 0.23 (.07)** −0.02 (.01)* 0.21 (.07)** 0.04 (.02)* −0.09 (.04)* 0.01 (.02) −0.27 (.07)**
Slope ↔ slope 0.03 (.01)* 0.03 (.01)* −0.02 (.01) −0.01 (.02)
Std Est β (SE) β (SE) β (SE) β (SE) β (SE) β (SE) β (SE) β (SE)
Int. → slope −0.31 (.16)* −0.25 (.09)** −0.36 (.20) −0.25 (.09)** −0.32 (.31) −0.25 (.09)** −0.55 (.26)* −0.25 (.04)**
SC2 X grade −0.10 (.05) 0.29 (.09)** −0.11 (.05)* 0.24 (.01)** −0.15 (.07)* −0.16 (.08)* −0.01 (.04) −0.46 (.11)**
Slope ↔ slope 0.25 (.11)* 0.25 (.11)* −0.23 (.17) −0.15 (.22)
−2LL (FP) 24,681.74(29) 23,104.74(28) 22,650.31 (28) 19,902.18 (28)
AIC/BIC 24,739.74/24,908.77 23,160.74/23,323.94 22,706.31/22,869.51 19,958.18/20,121.38
**

p < .01

*

p < .05.

−2LL, Deviance, FP, Free Parameters, Std. Est., Standardized Estimate, SC2, Order and Safety, Int., Intercept, Acad. Grades, Academic Grades.

Estimates include covariates and pairwise adjustment. School Clusters = 16.

Table 4 summarizes the associations between student engagement (SC 3) and academic grades. Student engagement growth trajectories were all found to be significant and ranged −0.25 to −0.29, SE=.04 to .06. Similar to order and safety, only significant associations were found for the all-academic grades grouped together (−0.32, SE=.16, p < .05) and mostly Ds/Fs (−0.54, SE=.27, p < .05). All growth associations between student engagement on academic grades ranged −0.46 to 0.29 (SE=.09 to .11). Mostly Cs (−0.15, SE=.07, p < .05) demonstrated the only significant growth associations between student engagement on academic grades. Lastly, the all-academic grades (ie, A-F) grouped together (0.26, SE=.10, p < .01) and As/Bs group (0.24, SE=.10, p < .05) demonstrated significant correlations between slopes.

DISCUSSION

Previous empirical evidence suggests schools that foster a positive school climate are more likely to deliver academically prepared and well-rounded students.32,85 As a result, schools become places where students want to spend their time because it enriches their life, which ultimately supports success in school and preparation for adulthood.8688 In this study, we sought to determine the associated growth trajectories between school climate and academic grades in a sample of students who started in middle and transitioned into high school. The study presents 5 main findings: (1) academic grades changed over time, (2) school climate changed over time, (3) students with more positive perceptions of school climate sustained As/Bs and improved Cs/Ds/Fs over time, (4) teacher relationships demonstrated the most robust effects on academic grades, and (5) sensitivity analysis revealed a few notable differences among independent samples of middle and high school students. Although the findings are by no means definitively causal, the study design and analytical techniques suggest a possible directional relationship and support the importance of a positive school climate throughout the context of schooling.

First, the findings suggest grades independently changed over time. Research focused on changes in grades as students transition tends to be mixed.89 A study by West and Schwerdt90 using longitudinal achievement data found standardized test scores declined as students transitioned from elementary into middle school. Another study by Bellmore91 tested the effects of interpersonal social position in schools on academic indicators. Her findings additionally demonstrated declines in early adolescent achievement over time. Alternatively, a meta-analytic review by Lee92 suggests that the transitionary effects on middle school students are likely to cause academic achievement to stays the same (ie, plateau) instead of decline. Furthermore, a study by Akos, Rose, Orthner93 suggests the “transition effect” is more of an interruption in student achievement growth that can be mended as students acclimate to a new school environment. This study’s findings support the general premise of this literature and provide a mild extension by being able to model changes between As/Bs, Cs, and Ds/Fs. However, a deeper understanding of specific student characteristics that may impact academic grades in this area is warranted.

Second, student perceptions of school climate declined across 3 selected sub-scales and academic groups over time. Research suggests students’ perceptions of school climate may be fluid and subject to change.94 This study’s findings add value to using longitudinal school climate studies, as cross-sectional relationships are less likely to describe such patterns.30 By reviewing growth model slopes, we additionally were able to better understand if changes in school climate occur individually and at different time points. This study’s findings suggest that students’ perceptions of school climate appear to diminish as a group instead of individually. However, this result was not found for the student engagement sub-scale. Practical implications of these findings may suggest that perceptions of school climate may shift due to underlying transitional modifiers.95 The findings may also suggest conceptually different sub-scales of school climate may have added significance to students’ maturity and growth. A longitudinal study by Wang and Eccles96 outlined a multi-dimensional model that demonstrated when the school environment supports prosocial norms, students were more likely to engage in school life. This study’s findings may support this position and suggest school climate is a multidimensional and socially generated phenomenon.26,97 However, this is a mild speculation and further research is needed.

Third, perceived school climate appears to change in parallel with self-reported grades. The findings suggest students averaging As/Bs and a high positive perception of school climate maintained their academic achievement over time. Additionally, students averaging Cs/Ds/Fs with a positive school climate also demonstrated better academic grades. These findings may suggest that when positive perceptions of school climate are improved, students averaging Cs/Ds/Fs may also improve. These results are important because school climate appears to support the academic success of all students and not only the majority who tend to be less at risk.13 If this is the case, improving students’ perceptions of school climate may be a useful primary prevention strategy for large-scale school-based health promotion.98 For example, if schools focus on improving perceptions of school climate that are supportive of health and academic-related outcomes in a collective manner, rather than separately, the results may be more impactful at the individual- and system- levels.

Fourth, although all school climate sub-scales demonstrated positive effects on academic grades, student-teacher relationships illustrated the strongest effects overall. Cross-sectional research has consistently shown nurturing relationships are a salient contributor to academic outcomes.34,73,74 This study’s results support the cross-sectional literature but also suggests that as students grow older, a positive school climate may be important for them to prospectively do well academically.99 On the other hand, academic grades did not appear to influence school climate sub-scales except for order and safety. Among the As/Bs/Cs, academic grades demonstrated a diminishing effect on order and safety. This finding was unanticipated and further supports the dynamic interplay between school climate sub-scales and academic achievement. Similar to study findings by Peguero, Bracy,100 these results may indicate unintended consequences with school disciplinary procedures. Studies have shown when school rules are too strict, defiance can be an unintentional result.95 Furthermore, higher-achieving students in this sample may possess unobserved characteristics that support their unenthusiastic perception of school rules and authority. However, this concept requires further investigation.

Lastly, group comparisons from the sensitivity analysis revealed a few notable differences between middle and high school students. In the middle school only group, school climate and academic grades were found to be stable and did not significantly decline over time. This may suggest students’ perceptions of school climate and academic grades stay relatively similar throughout middle school. Comparatively, high school students’ perceptions of school climate appear to be just as important to all levels of academic achievement. However, high school students’ presented much steeper declines in perceptions of school climate over time. Hence, perceptions of school climate may shift as students get older. This finding may suggest the presence of a “transition effect” as students adjust to high school.101 Therefore, from a developmental standpoint, high school students may benefit more from subject specialization than a focus on school connection as they prepare for adulthood.102 Although, the findings allude to the idea of transitional effects, more studies are needed to better understand how a transition between middle and high school may disrupt the social-hierarchy and impact school-based outcomes.

Conclusion

Study results demonstrated longitudinal associations between school climate and academic achievement. Findings support the value and promotion of a positive school climate, especially across sub-scales. In addition, positive perceptions of school climate may sustain high academic performance, while strengthening the grades of average and low academic performers. Given the modest variation between As/Bs, Cs, and Ds/Fs future studies should examine the mechanisms that exist between more robust models of school climate and contextual characteristics of these groups. Preliminary comparisons between middle and high school students suggest there may be differences between these groups, but further investigation is required. Future studies that support longitudinal and more generalizable findings are needed to provide recommendations for the delivery of instruction and professional practice that promotes the whole child.

Limitations

First, the sample was drawn from a predominantly homogenous group of White middle school students from 16 schools in WV. Findings may not be representative or generalizable to other regions of the United States. Second, the potential issue of non-matched students may exacerbate issues with representativeness. However, the mechanism that limits matching procedures may be difficult to distinguish. Fortunately, even when participant attrition is high, parameter estimates are still likely to be accurate.58 Third, student self-reported information is subject to acquiescence and recall bias. Fourth, the study only modeled 3 aspects of the school climate. The literature around the school climate is diverse and considers many aspects of the socio-organizational structures of schools. However, the 3 sub-scales chosen in this study are consistently found in other school climate instruments and make-up half of the items on the SCM. Fifth, combing academic achievement into groups (ie, As/Bs) may diminish measurement sensitivity. Using measures of academic achievement as an uncombined measure may yield different information. Sixth, Mathematics and English are different subjects and therefore may also demonstrate unique independent information. However, combining these outcomes as a singular measure to represent overall academic performance is common in educational research. Seventh, maternal education may not fully represent students’ SES (Berkowitz, et al35). Eighth, the diminished indicator sensitivity and sophisticated statistical techniques using a mixture of categorical and continuous measurement suggest caution should be used when interpreting the results. Despite this limitation, this study represents an exploration in the parallel growth of school climate and academic achievement over time. Lastly, we were unable to rule out unaccounted variance from other confounding variables that may substantially impact school climate sub-scales and academic grades over time.

IMPLICATIONS FOR SCHOOL HEALTH

How schools support students to achieve academic and life success will continue to be a topic for researchers and educators alike.103 This study provides evidence that taking the time to care for students social-emotional needs does not hurt their grades, but helps them. However, non-academic factors like school climate still require research to better understand their importance to support frameworks like WSCC to achieve desired student outcomes.104 Although this study does not account for teacher performance or classroom management, helping students achieve good grades will likely be shaped by dedicated administrators, educators, and parents but as this study’s findings suggest:

  • Academic grades are related to non-academic factors.

  • A positive school climate, especially positive relationships with teachers, may support academic success over time.

  • School climate may be used to support organizational behavior in school as a way to inform and improve academic and health promotion practice.

  • Schools focused on integrating a positive school climate may increase the potential to enrich students’ lives and strengthen their academic success.

  • Considering average academic performers (“C” students) less important than lower performers may undermine the likelihood of a student being able to cross the achievement gap toward success.

Lastly, administrators who support and encourage teachers to form bonding relationships may help students in ways which may be evident in their academic behavior. This study’s findings support this idea as students who perceived relationships with teachers as positive were as likely to sustain high academic performance compared to students with a less positive perception. In addition, school climate may support upward academic mobility for average and lower student performers. A seminal work by Allensworth and Easton13 highlights this importance from what they deem as “personalism,” (pp 32) which accounts for students’ initial trust of teachers and feelings of personal support. This idea is not to infer that each student requires scheduled amounts of time to meet their needs. Rather, the authors’ state:

Teachers working together in a coordinated way—taking responsibility for the whole school; providing relevant, coherent instruction; and developing strong relationships with students—most strongly distinguishes schools with above-expected student performance in their courses.13 (pp 33–34).

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the West Virginia Department of Education (WVDOE) in Charleston, WV and the Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (SAMHSA) in Washington, DC; Project AWARE 2015–2020. The authors would like to extend their gratitude to the funders for their generous support. The authors would like to thank the dedicated personnel who made Project AWARE 2015–2020 data collection possible. Lastly, the authors would like to recognize Dr. Thomas K. Bias and Dr. Danielle M. Davidov for their thoughtful reviewand commentary for this article.

Human Subjects Approval Statement

All aspects of each data collection year in this study, including participant involvement based on passive parental consent, were accomplished in accordance with West Virginia University’s Institutional Review Board guidelines for the protection of research participants. The following are the IRB protocols for each corresponding data collection year, 2015 (# 1406345394), 2016 (# 1406345394R002), and 2017 (# 1406345394R004).

Footnotes

Conflict of Interest

All authors of this article declare they have no conflicts of interest.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Basch CE. Healthier students are better learners: a missing link in school reforms to close the achievement gap. J Sch Health. 2011;81(10):593–598. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Vaughn MG, Salas-Wright CP, Maynard BR. Dropping out of school and chronic disease in the United States. J Public Health. 2014;22(3):265–270. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Woolf SH, Johnson RE, Phillips RL, Philipsen M. Giving everyone the health of the educated: an examination of whether social change would save more lives than medical advances. Am J Public Health. 2007;97(4):679–683. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Zimmerman E, Woolf SH. Understanding the relationship between education and health. Roundtable on Population Health Improvement; June 5, 2014; Washington, DC: Available at: https://nam.edu/wp-content/uploads/2015/06/BPH-UnderstandingTheRelationship1.pdf. Accessed May 5, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Bowers AJ, Sprott R. Why tenth graders fail to finish high school: a dropout typology latent class analysis. J Educ Stud Placed Risk. 2012;17(3):129–148. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Bowers AJ, Sprott R, Taff SA. Do we know who will dropout? A review of the predictors of dropping out of high school precision, sensitivity, and specificity. High Sch J. 2013;96(2):77–100. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Lansford JE, Dodge KA, Pettit GS, Bates JE. A public health perspective on school dropout and adult outcomes: a prospective study of risk and protective factors from age 5 to 27 years. J Adolesc Health. 2016;58(6):652–658. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Lleras-Muney A The relationship between education and adult mortality in the United States. Rev Econ Stud. 2005;72(1):189–221. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Freeman J, Simonsen B. Examining the impact of policy and practice interventions on high school dropout and school completion rates: a systematic review of the literature. Rev Educ Res. 2015;85(2):205–248. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Freudenberg N, Ruglis J. Reframing school dropout as a public health issue. Prevent Chronic Dis. 2007;4(4):A107. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Myers DJ. Epidemiologists, our patient is society. New Solut. 2008;18(2):107–109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Myers DJ. Education and health disparities: a macro, not micro, phenomenon. New Solut. 2010;20(2):175–177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Allensworth EM, Easton JQ. What Matters for Staying On-Track and Graduating in Chicago Public Highs Schools: A Close Look at Course Grades, Failures, and Attendance in the Freshman Year. Research Report. Chicago, IL: Consortium on Chicago School Research; 2007. Available at: https://consortium.uchicago.edu/sites/default/files/2018-10/07%20What%20Matters%20Final.pdf. Accessed May 5, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Blum RW, Bastos FI, Kabiru CW, Le LC. Adolescent health in the 21st century. Lancet. 2012;379(9826):1567–1568. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Resnick MD, Catalano RF, Sawyer SM, Viner R, Patton GC. Seizing the opportunities of adolescent health. Lancet. 2012;379(9826):1564–1567. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.US Department of Education NCES. Enrollment Trends. Digest of Education Statistics, 2015 (NCES 2016–014); 2016. Available at: https://nces.ed.gov/fastfacts/display.asp?id=65. Accessed December 3, 2017.
  • 17.US Department of Education NCES. Fast Facts: Elementary and Secondary Education Enrollment; 2017. Available at: https://nces.ed.gov/fastfacts/display.asp?id=372. Accessed December 3, 2017.
  • 18.US Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics, Common Core of Data (CCD). Public Elementary/Secondary School Universe Survey 2000–01 through 2015–16. 2017. Available at: https://nces.ed.gov/programs/digest/d17/tables/dt17_216.30.asp. Accessed May 2, 2018.
  • 19.Svirydzenka N, Aitken J, Dogra N. Research and partnerships with schools. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2016;51(8):1203–1209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Allensworth D, Lewallen TC, Stevenson B, Katz S. Addressing the needs of the whole child: what public health can do to answer the education sector’s call for a stronger partnership. Prev Chronic Dis. 2011;8(2):A44. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Bandura A Health promotion by social cognitive means. Health Educ Behav. 2004;31(2):143–164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Bandura A, Barbaranelli C, Pastorelli C, Caprara GV, Zimbardo PG. Prosocial foundations of children’s academic achievement. Psychol Sci. 2000;11(4):302–306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Zimmerman BJ, Bandura A, Martinez-Pons M. Self-motivation for academic attainment: the role of self-efficacy beliefs and personal goal setting. Am Educ Res J. 1992;29(3): 663–676. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Bandura A Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall; 1986. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Silvers JA, McRae K, Gabrieli JDE, Gross JJ, Remy KA, Ochsner KN. Age-related differences in emotional reactivity, regulation, and rejection sensitivity in adolescence. Emotion. 2012;12(6):1235–1247. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Eccles JS, Roeser RW. An ecological view of schools and development In: Meece JL, Eccles JS, eds. Handbook of Research on Schools, Schooling, and Human Development. New York, NY: Routledge; 2010. Available at: https://www.routledgehandbooks.com/doi/10.4324/9780203874844.ch2. Accessed February 2, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Eccles JS, Roeser RW. Schools as developmental contexts during adolescence. J Res Adolesc. 2011;21(1):225–241. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Spengler M, Roberts BW, Lüdtke O, Martin R, Brunner M. Student characteristics and behaviours in childhood predict self-reported health in middle adulthood. Eur J Pers. 2016;30(5):456–466. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Murray NG, Low BJ, Hollis C, Cross AW, Davis SM. Coordinated school health programs and academic achievement: a systematic review of the literature. J Sch Health. 2007;77(9):589–600. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Wang MT, Degol JL. School climate: a review of the construct, measurement, and impact on student outcomes. Educ Psychol Rev. 2016;28(2):315–352. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Cohen J, McCabe L, Michelli NM, Pickeral T. School climate: research, policy, practice, and teacher education. Teach Coll Rec. 2009;111(1):180–213. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Freiberg HJ. School Climate: Measuring, Improving and Sustaining Healthy Learning Environments. London, UK: Taylor & Francis; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Thapa A, Cohen J, Guffey S, Higgins-D’Alessandro A. A review of school climate research. Rev Educ Res. 2013;83(3): 357–385. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Zullig KJ, Koopman TM, Patton JM, Ubbes VA. School climate: historical review, instrument development, and school assessment. J Psychoeduc Assess. 2010;28(2):139–152. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Berkowitz R, Moore H, Astor RA, Benbenishty R. A research synthesis of the associations between socioeconomic background, inequality, school climate, and academic achievement. Rev Educ Res. 2016;87(2):425–469. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Durlak JA, Weissberg RP, Dymnicki AB, Taylor RD, Schellinger KB. The impact of enhancing students’ social and emotional learning: a meta-analysis of school-based universal interventions. Child Dev. 2011;82(1):405–432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Cohen J Measuring and improving school climate: A prosocial strategy that recognizes, educates and supports the whole child and the whole school community In: Brown PM, Corrigian MW and Higgins-D’Alessandro A, eds. The Handbook of Prosocial Education. 1st ed. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield, 2012:226–249. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Piscatelli J, Lee C. State Policies on School Climate and Bully Prevention Efforts: Challenges and Opportunities for Deepening State Policy Support for Safe and Civil Schools. New York, NY: National School Climate Center; 2011. Available at: https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED566375.pdf. Accessed May 5, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Michael SL, Merlo CL, Basch CE, Wentzel KR, Wechsler H. Critical connections: health and academics. J Sch Health. 2015;85(11):740–758. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Bradley RH, Corwyn RF. Socioeconomic status and child development. Annu Rev Psychol. 2002;53(1):371–399. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Myhr A, Lillefjell M, Espnes GA, Halvorsen T. Correction: do family and neighbourhood matter in secondary school completion? A multilevel study of determinants and their interactions in a life-course perspective. PloS One. 2017;12(8):e0184231. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Voight A, Hanson T. How Are Middle School Climate and Academic Performance Related across Schools and over Time? (REL 2017–212). Washington, DC: US Department of Education, Institute of Education Sciences, National Center for Education Evaluation and Regional Assistance, Regional Educational Laboratory West; 2017; Available at: https://ies.ed.gov/ncee/edlabs/regions/west/pdf/REL_2017212.pdf. Accessed May 15, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.US Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Whole School, Whole Community, Whole Child 2016. Available at: https://www.cdc.gov/healthyyouth/wscc/. Accessed October, 14, 2017.
  • 44.Rasberry CN, Slade S, Lohrmann DK, Valois RF. Lessons learned from the whole child and coordinated school health approaches. J Sch Health. 2015;85(11):759–765. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Senate Committee on Health Education Labor and Pensions. The every child achieves act of 2015. 2016. Available at: https://www.help.senate.gov/imo/media/The_Every_Child_Achieves_Act_of_2015--summary.pdf. Accessed July 23, 2017.
  • 46.Healthy School Campaign. Leveraging New Education Law to Support Health and Learning. 2017. Available at: https://healthyschoolscampaign.org/policy/essa/. Accessed December 3, 2017.
  • 47.Every Student Succeeds Act (ESSA). December 10, 2015. Available at: https://www.ed.gov/essa?src=rn. Accessed July 23, 2017.
  • 48.Wentzel KR. Student motivation in middle school: the role of perceived pedagogical caring. J Educ Psychol. 1997;89(3):411–419. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Appalachian Regional Commission (ARC). Person Below Poverty Level, 2011–2015. 2011–2015. Available at: https://www.arc.gov/reports/custom_report.asp?REPORT_ID=70. Accessed May 15, 2018.
  • 50.University of Wisconsin Population Health Institute. County Health Rankings: 2018 West Virginia State Report 2018. Available at: http://www.countyhealthrankings.org/explorehealth-rankings/reports/state-reports/2018/west-virginia. Accessed May 13, 2018.
  • 51.United Health Foundation. America’s Health Rankings: 2017 West Virginia Annual Report Summary 2017. Available at: https://www.americashealthrankings.org/learn/reports/2017-annual-report/state-summaries-west-virginia. Accessed May 13, 2018.
  • 52.University of Wisconsin Population Health Institute. County Health Rankings: 2018 West Virginia State Report, Children eligible for free or reduced lunch 2018. Available at: http://www.countyhealthrankings.org/app/west-virginia/2018/measure/factors/65/data. Accessed January 27, 2019.
  • 53.Chartier M, Stoep AV, McCauley E, Herting JR, Tracy M, Lymp J. Passive versus active parental permission: implications for the ability of school-based depression screening to reach youth at risk. J Sch Health. 2008;78(3):157–164; quiz 184–156. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Levine RJ. Research involving children: an interpretation of the new regulations. Ethics Hum Res. 1983;5(4):1–5. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Kristjansson AL, Sigfusson J, Sigfusdottir ID, Allegrante JP. Data collection procedures for school-based surveys among adolescents: the youth in Europe study. J Sch Health. 2013;83(9):662–667. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Kristjansson AL, Kogan SM, Mann MJ, et al. Does early exposure to caffeine promote smoking and alcohol use behavior? A prospective analysis of middle school students. Addiction. 2018;113:1706–1713. 10.1111/add.14261. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Kristjansson AL, Mann MJ, Smith ML, Sigfusdottir ID. Social profile of middle school-aged adolescents who use electronic cigarettes: implications for primary prevention. Prev Sci. 2017;19(6):805–812. 10.1007/s11121-017-0825-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Gustavson K, von Soest T, Karevold E, Roysamb E. Attrition and generalizability in longitudinal studies: findings from a 15-year population-based study and a Monte Carlo simulation study. BMC Public Health. 2012;12:918. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Tambs K, Ronning T, Prescott CA, et al. The Norwegian Institute of Public Health twin study of mental health: examining recruitment and attrition bias. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2009;12(2):158–168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Little RJA, Rubin DB. Statistical Analysis with Missing Data. 2nd ed. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Little RJA. A test of missing completely at random for multivariate data with missing values. J Am Stat Assoc. 1988;83(404):1198–1202. [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Marshall A, Altman DG, Holder RL. Comparison of imputation methods for handling missing covariate data when fitting a cox proportional hazards model: a resampling study. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2010;10:112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Mazza GL, Enders CK, Ruehlman LS. Addressing item-level missing data: a comparison of proration and full information maximum likelihood estimation. Multivariate Behav Res. 2015;50(5):504–519. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Schafer JL, Graham JW. Missing data: our view of the state of the art. Psychol Methods. 2002;7(2):147–177. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Cham H, Reshetnyak E, Rosenfeld B, Breitbart W. Full information maximum likelihood estimation for latent variable interactions with incomplete indicators. Multivariate Behav Res. 2017;52(1):12–30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Graham JW. Missing data analysis: making it work in the real world. Annu Rev Psychol. 2009;60(1):549–576. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Graham JW. Adding missing-data-relevant variables to FIML-based structural equation models. Struct Equ Modeling. 2003;10(1):80–100. [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Collins LM, Schafer JL, Kam CM. A comparison of inclusive and restrictive strategies in modern missing data procedures. Psychol Methods. 2001;6(4):330–351. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Graham JW, Olchowski AE, Gilreath TD. How many imputations are really needed? Some practical clarifications of multiple imputation theory. Prev Sci. 2007;8(3): 206–213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Harwell MR, Gatti GG. Rescaling ordinal data to interval data in educational research. Rev Educ Res. 2001;71(1): 105–131. [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Kuncel NR, Credé M, Thomas LL. The validity of self-reported grade point averages, class ranks, and test scores: a meta-analysis and review of the literature. Rev of Educ Res. 2016;75(1):63–82. [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Sticca F, Goetz T, Bieg M, Hall NC, Eberle F, Haag L. Examining the accuracy of students’ self-reported academic grades from a correlational and a discrepancy perspective: evidence from a longitudinal study. PLoS One. 2017;12(11):e0187367. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Zullig KJ, Collins R, Ghani N, et al. Preliminary development of a revised version of the school climate measure. Psychol Assess. 2015;27(3):1072–1081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Zullig KJ, Collins R, Ghani N, Patton JM, Huebner ES, Ajamie J. Psychometric support of the school climate measure in a large, diverse sample of adolescents: a replication and extension. J Sch Health. 2014;84(2):82–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Kristjánsson ÁL, Sigfúsdóttir ID. The role of parental support, parental monitoring, and time spent with parents in adolescent academic achievement in Iceland: a structural model of gender differences. Scand J Educ Res. 2009;53(5):481–496. [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Marcenaro-Gutierrez O, Lopez-Agudo LA, Ropero-García MA. Gender differences in adolescents’ academic achievement. Young. 2017;26(3):250–270. [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Holmes EK, Holladay HM, Hill EJ, Yorgason JB. Are mothers’ work-to-family conflict, school involvement, and work status related to academic achievement? J Child Fam Stud. 2018;27(6):1881–1898. [Google Scholar]
  • 78.SAS Institute. Base SAS 9.4 Procedures Guide: Statistical Procedures. 2nd ed. Cary, NC: SAS Institute; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Cheong J, MacKinnon D, Khoo ST. Investigation of mediational processes using parallel process latent growth curve modeling. Struct Equ Modeling. 2003;10(2):238–262. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Muthen L, Muthen BO. Mplus User’s Guide. 8th ed. Los Angeles, CA: Muthen and Muthen; 2017. [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Liao TF. Interpreting Probability Models: Logit, Probit, and Other Generalized Linear Models. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Masyn KE, Petras H, Liu W. Growth curve models with categorical outcomes In: Encycl Criminol Crim Justice. New York, NY: Springer; 2014:2013–2025. Available at: https://link.springer.com/referenceworkentry/10.1007/978-1-4614-5690-2_404. Accessed December 6, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Lee TK, Wickrama K, O’Neal CW. Application of latent growth curve analysis with categorical responses in social behavioral research. Struct Equ Modeling. 2018;25(2):294–306. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Geiser C Data Analysis with Mplus. New York, NY: The Guilford Press; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Maxwell LE. School building condition, social climate, student attendance and academic achievement: a mediation model. J Environ Psychol. 2016;46:206–216. [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Huebner ES, Funk BA, Gilman R. Cross-sectional and longitudinal psychosocial correlates of adolescent life satisfaction reports. Can J Sch Psychol. 2000;16(Part 1):53–64. [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Huebner ES, McCullough G. Correlates of school satisfaction among adolescents. J Educ Res. 2000;93(5):331–335. [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Morse LL, Allensworth DD. Placing students at the center: the whole school, whole community, whole child model. J Sch Health. 2015;85(11):785–794. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Goldstein SE, Boxer P, Rudolph E. Middle school transition stress: links with academic performance, motivation, and school experiences. Contemp Sch Psychol. 2015;19(1):21–29. [Google Scholar]
  • 90.West MR, Schwerdt G. The middle school plunge: achievement tumbles when young students change schools. Educ Next. 2012;12(2):63–68. [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Bellmore A Peer rejection and unpopularity: associations with GPAs across the transition to middle school. J Educ Psychol. 2011;103(2):282–295. [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Lee J Tripartite growth trajectories of reading and math achievement. Am Educ Res J. 2010;47(4):800–832. [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Akos P, Rose RA, Orthner D. Sociodemographic moderators of middle school transition effects on academic achievement. J Early Adolesc. 2014;35(2):170–198. [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Wang MT, Dishion TJ. The trajectories of adolescents’ perceptions of school climate, deviant peer affiliation, and behavioral problems during the middle school years. J Res Adolesc. 2012;22(1):40–53. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Way SM. School discipline and disruptive classroom behavior: the moderating effects of student perceptions. Sociol Q. 2011;52(3):346–375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Wang MT, Eccles JS. School context, achievement motivation, and academic engagement: a longitudinal study of school engagement using a multidimensional perspective. Learn Instruc. 2013;28:12–23. [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Hopson LM, Lee E. Mitigating the effect of family poverty on academic and behavioral outcomes: the role of school climate in middle and high school. Child Youth Serv Rev. 2011;33(11):2221–2229. [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Mann MJ, Kristjansson AL, Smith ML, Daily SM, Thomas S, Murray S. From tactics to strategy: creating and sustaining social conditions that demand and deliver effective school health programs. J Sch Health. 2018;88(5):333–336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Payton J, Weissberg RP, Durlak JA, et al. The positive impact of social and emotional learning for kindergarten to eighth-grade students: findings from three scientific reviews. Collab Acad Soc Emot Learn. 2008:2–12. Available at: https://www.casel.org/wp-content/uploads/2016/08/PDF-4-the-positive-impact-of-social-and-emotional-learning-for-kindergarten-to-eighth-grade-students-executive-summary.pdf. Accessed December 15, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Peguero AA, Bracy NL. School order, justice, and education: climate, discipline practices, and dropping out. J Res Adolesc. 2015;25(3):412–426. [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Madjar N, Cohen-Malayev M. Perceived school climate across the transition from elementary to middle school. Sch Psychol Q. 2016;31(2):270–288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Scott TM, Hirn RG, Alter PJ. Teacher instruction as a predictor for student engagement and disruptive behaviors. Prev Sch Fail. 2014;58(4):193–200. [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Birch DA, Priest HM, Mitchell QP. Advocacy for quality school health education: the role of public health educators as professionals and community members. Health Educ. 2015;47(1):38–44. [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Rooney LE, Videto DM, Birch DA. Using the whole school, shole community, whole child model: implications for practice. J Sch Health. 2015;85(11):817–823. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Cronbach LJ, Meehl PE. Construct validity in psychological tests. Psychol Bull. 1955;52(4):281–302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES