Skip to main content
Brain and Behavior logoLink to Brain and Behavior
. 2020 Jun 12;10(8):e01704. doi: 10.1002/brb3.1704

Functional and cognitive capacity differ in dystonic motor subtypes when compared to choreatic and hypokinetic‐rigid motor subtypes in Huntington's disease

Jannis Achenbach 1,, Sarah Maria von Hein 1, Carsten Saft 1
PMCID: PMC7428484  PMID: 32530575

Abstract

Background

Motor phenotypes in Huntington's disease vary manifold. Phenotype classification is essential to adapt treatment. The aim of this study was to classify a dystonic subtype closer.

Methods

A total of 7,512 manifest ENROLL‐HD participants were subdivided into mainly choreatic (N = 606), dystonic (N = 402), and hypokinetic‐rigid (N = 369) subjects. Cognitive (verbal fluency, symbol digit, stroop color, trail making, Mini‐Mental State Examination), functional (total functional capacity, Independence Scale), and psychiatric (problem behaviors assessment, Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale) performance was evaluated at baseline visit.

Results

Symptoms onset for dystonic were similar to hypokinetic‐rigid, but earlier compared to choreatic subjects (p < .001). Cognition was better in both groups compared to hypokinetic rigid (all p < .001). Functionality differed between all groups (all p < .001). Differences remained (all p < .001) after controlling for CAP score, CAG, age, disease duration, and education.

Conclusions

Motor subtypes differ in functional and cognitive capacities but less in psychiatric. We identified better cognitive and functional capacities and similar onsets in predominant dystonic compared to hypokinetic‐rigid patients.

Keywords: dystonia, Huntington's disease, movement disorders


The research confirmed that motoric phenotypes are relevant for functional and cognitive capacities in HD. Onsets in dystonic patients are similar to hypokinetic‐rigids. Cognitive capacity is better in dystonic and choreatic than hypokinetic‐rigids patients.

graphic file with name BRB3-10-e01704-g001.jpg

1. INTRODUCTION

Neurodegenerative Huntington's disease (HD) is caused by CAG trinucleotide expansion in the huntingtin gene (HTT; Walker, 2007). Symptomatic manifestation accompanies with heterogeneous motoric, cognitive, and psychiatric symptoms. Although dominant motor symptoms are hyperkinetic, they can vary manifold with bradykinesia, dystonia, myoclonus, or as hypokinetic‐rigid (Roos, 2010; Saft, Lauter, Kraus, Przuntek, & Andrich, 2006).

Several studies investigated different aspects of bradykinesia, respectively, hypokinetic‐rigidity or chorea in HD (Girotti, Marano, Soliveri, Geminiani, & Scigliano, 1988; Mahant, McCusker, Byth, & Graham, 2003; Reedeker, Van Der Mast, Giltay, Van Duijn, & Roos, 2010; Sanchez‐Pernaute et al., 2000; Thompson et al., 1988; Young et al., 1986). More recently, Hart et al. (2013) focused on aspects of predominant choreatic versus hypokinetic‐rigid motor phenotypes and described a better capacity for global and cognitive functioning of their choreatic group.

So far only a few smaller studies investigated the role of dystonia in HD (Mahant et al., 2003; Zande et al., 2017). Zande et al. (2017) described a high prevalence of dystonia as being a late symptom correlating with increasing HD stage and disease duration. In addition, a strong relationship with functional status of dystonia and rigidity was described (Carlozzi et al., 2019). To our knowledge, no study compared three different phenotypes with predominant dystonia, chorea, and hypokinetic‐rigidity which are essential for symptomatic treatments (Saft, von Hein, & Lucke, 2018; Wojtecki, Groiss, & Ferrea, 2015; Zittel et al., 2018). For a better understanding, we investigated whether there are collectives in the European Huntington's Disease Network ENROLL‐HD study classified as predominant choreatic, dystonic, and hypokinetic‐rigid in order to compare cognitive, functional, and psychiatric performance data (Landwehrmeyer, Fitzer‐Attas, & Giuliano, 2017). We hypothesize a better cognitive functioning of a choreatic and dystonic group when compared to a hypokinetic‐rigid group and possibly more depression due to pain in our dystonic subgroup.

2. PATIENTS AND METHODS

A total of 7,512 subjects (>18 years) with manifest HD (diagnostic confidence level: 4; >36 CAG; total motor score (TMS): >5) participating in the ENROLL‐HD study were analyzed (Kieburtz et al., 1996).

Enroll‐HD is a global clinical research platform designed to facilitate clinical research in Huntington's disease. Core datasets are collected annually from all research participants as part of this multicentre longitudinal observational study. Data are monitored for quality and accuracy using a risk‐based monitoring approach. All sites are required to obtain and maintain local ethical approval.

For subdividing predominant motor phenotypes, sum scores were calculated according to Hart et al. (2013) Scores had to differ by at least one standard deviation compared to the entire cohort. Classification of predominant choreatic (N = 606) due to chorea scale and hypokinetic‐rigid (N = 369) due to hypokinetic‐rigidity with finger taps, pronate–supinate hands, bradykinesia, and rigidity, phenotypes were identified in UHDRS motor assessment (Hart et al., 2013). Equally onset and classification of dystonia were evaluated due to items for dystonia in trunk, right, and left upper and lower extremities as part of the UHDRS motor score, and (N = 402) subjects were classified as predominant dystonic. Interference with predominant hypokinetic‐rigidity and dystonia (n = 661) or smaller deviations were regarded as mixed phenotypes (n = 6,135) and excluded.

Differences among group means were assessed using ANOVA with post hoc Tukey's HSD tests in IBM SPSS Statistics V.25. Afterward, one‐way ANCOVAs were conducted determining differences between subtypes on functional and cognitive assessments controlling for CAP score (Zhang et al., 2011), CAG, age, disease duration, and education.

3. RESULTS

Significant mean differences were detected between choreatic, dystonic, and hypokinetic‐rigid groups regarding all demographic parameters (all p < .001) (Table 1). Post hoc comparisons indicated CAP score was higher, education level lower, and disease duration longer in hypokinetic compared to dystonic and choreatic group (all p < .001). The CAG mean was lower in the choreatic versus dyston and hypokinetic group (all p < .001). Comparison of age indicated that the dystonic group was younger than choreatic and hypokinetic‐rigid (all p < .001).

TABLE 1.

Analysis of demographics, motoric, cognitive, functional, and psychiatric capacities in different motor subtypes

Choreatic (1)

N = 606

Dyston (2)

N = 402

Hypokinetic‐rigid (3)

N = 369

F p Part. η 2 p (1) versus (2) p (1) versus (3) p (2) versus (3)
Demographic parameters
Age (years); M (SD) 56.1 (12.0) 51.2 (13.5) 55.8 (14.2) 14.10 <.001 0.020 <.001 .929 <.001
CAG high 43.6 (3.4) 45.3 (5.3) 45.4 (5.8) 23.87 <.001 0.034 <.001 <.001 .974
Disease duration (years) 8.6 (5.3)

7.9 (5.5)

n = 400

10.3 (5.0)

n = 361

20.96 <.001 0.030 .054 <.001 <.001
Education level (ISCED 0–6) 3.4 (1.2)

3.4 (1.2)

n = 399

3.0 (1.3)

n = 366

13.92 <.001 0.020 .874 <.001 <.001
CAP score 526.2 (83.2) 548.1 (92.1) 589.4 (110.4) 52.15 <.001 0.071 <.005 <.001 <.001
Timed onsets
Age of onset motor symptoms (years) 47.5 (11.6)

44.0 (13.8)

n = 400

45.6 (14.1)

n = 361

8.84 <.001 0.013 <.001 .072 .216
Rater's estimate of symptom onset (years)

47.25 (11.9)

n = 572

43.2 (13.2)

n = 378

44.8 (13.5)

n = 340

12.18 <.001 0.019 <.001 .013 .215
Date of clinical HD diagnosis (years)

51.2 (12.0)

n = 601

46.7 (13.6)

n = 396

48.6 (13.9)

n = 362

14.71 <.001 0.021 <.001 .009 .102
Symptoms first noticed by participant (years)

48.3 (12.1)

n = 577

44.8 (13.4)

n = 374

45.8 (13.8)

n = 338

9.09 <.001 0.014 <.001 .014 .566
Symptoms first noticed by family (years)

47.2 (11.9)

n = 564

43.5 (13.5)

n = 375

45.2 (13.7)

n = 343

9.41 <.001 0.014 <.001 .072 .163
At what age did cognitive impairment first start to have an impact on daily life? (years)

50.1 (12.4)

n = 381

46.2 (13.9)

n = 274

48.8 (14.5)

n = 302

6.76 <.005 0.014 <.005 .442 .050
At what age did perseverative obsessive behavior begin? (years)

52.1 (13.5)

n = 333

48.1 (14.4)

n = 221

49.9 (14.3)

n = 219

5.76 <.005 0.015 <.005 .167 .340
At what age did irritability begin? (years)

47.9 (13.3)

n = 413

44.2 (14.6)

n = 255

47.6 (15.5)

n = 262

5.86 <.005 0.012 <.005 .951 .020
At what age did depression begin? (years)

47.1 (13.5)

n = 427

42.1 (14.4)

n = 298

45.5 (14.8)

n = 264

10.95 <.001 0.022 <.001 .337 .012
At what age did apathy begin? (years)

51.8 (13.1)

n = 386

46.8 (14.2)

n = 251

51.1 (14.7)

n = 268

10.92 <.001 0.024 <.001 .780 <.005
At what age did violent or aggressive behavior begin? (years)

47.7 (14.4)

n = 223

44.3 (14.8)

n = 134

47.2 (16.1)

n = 189

2.26 .105 0.008 .101 .939 .210
At what age did psychosis (hallucinations or delusions) begin? (years)

49.6 (12.7)

n = 61

45.9 (15.6)

n = 60

49.9 (15.8)

n = 93

1.47 .231 0.014 .359 .992 .239
Motoric parameter
TMS (UHDRS) 50.4 (11.4) 52.2 (10.3) 63.5 (10.7) 180.33 <.001 0.208 .028 <.001 <.001
Cognitive parameters
Verbal fluency test (letters): Total correct (in 60 s)

10.7 (4.7)

n = 595

9.9 (4.6)

n = 394

5.7 (3.8)

n = 319

136.85 <.001 0.173 .021 <.001 <.001
Verbal fluency test (letters): Total correct (in 180 s)

18.1 (10.5)

n = 472

16.1 (9.4)

n = 295

8.0 (6.4)

n = 183

75.66 <.001 0.138 <.005 <.001 <.001
SDMT: total correct (in 90 s)

19.3 (10.3)

n = 555

16.7 (9.0)

n = 377

7.0 (7.4)

n = 257

154.71 <.001 0.207 <.001 <.001 <.001
Stroop color naming test: total correct (in 45 s)

37.3 (14.1)

n = 592

35.5 (14.1)

n = 387

20.2 (13.7)

n = 311

164.78 <.001 0.203 .113 <.001 <.001
Stroop word reading test: total correct (in 45 s)

49.6 (18.6)

n = 581

46.3 (18.2)

n = 389

25.8 (18.2)

n = 297

173.35 <.001 0.215 .016 <.001 <.001
Stroop interference test: total correct (in 45 s)

20.3 (9.8)

n = 512

18.9 (8.6)

n = 330

10.1 (8.2)

n = 198

91.84 <.001 0.150 .076 <.001 <.001
Trail making test: Part A time to complete (in seconds)

82.7 (51.0)

n = 437

91.9 (54.1)

n = 271

151.4 (73.4)

n = 126

75.08 <.001 0.153 .084 <.001 <.001
Trail making test: Part B time to complete (in seconds)

173.0 (68.1)

n = 415

188.9 (63.7)

n = 253

223.0 (45.7)

n = 116

28.27 <.001 0.068 .005 <.001 <.001
MMSE: total correct

24.5 (4.0)

n = 409

23.3 (4.0)

N = 253

17.8 (6.1)

n = 156

128.42 <.001 0.240 .005 <.001 <.001
Functional parameters
TFC (UHDRS) 7.3 (2.9)

6.6 (3.1)

n = 401

3.2 (2.6)

n = 368

242.07 <.001 0.261 <.005 <.001 <.001
IS (%)

73.7 (12.8)

70.2 (14.2)

n = 400

51.0 (17.5) 295.26 <.001 0.301 <.005 <.001 <.001
Psychiatric parameters
PBA‐s: sum score apathy

3.6 (4.3)

n = 604

3.9 (4.5)

n = 400

6.6 (5.8)

n = 358

49.71 <.001 0.068 .604 <.001 <.001
PBA‐s: sum score executive function

3.8 (5.8)

n = 599

4.3 (6.1)

n = 399

5.7 (7.1)

n = 348

9.93 <.001 0.015 .463 <.001 .007
PBA‐s: sum score depression

4.9 (6.5)

n = 603

5.0 (6.0)

n = 401

4.8 (6.2)

n = 356

0.093 .911 0.000 .981 .956 .903
PBA‐s: sum score irritability, aggression

3.5 (5.1)

n = 604

2.9 (4.7)

n = 401

4.1 (6.2)

n = 368

4.90 .008 0.007 .199 .190 .05
PBA‐s: sum score psychosis

0.4 (2.2)

n = 603

0.4 (1.9)

n = 398

0.5 (2.4)

n = 355

0.516 .597 0.001 .984 .665 .617
HADS‐SIS: depression subscore

6.2 (3.9)

n = 386

6.6 (4.4)

n = 200

7.4 (4.6)

n = 135

3.96 .019 0.011 .511 .014 .226
HADS‐SIS: irritability subscore

6.0 (4.5)

n = 385

5.7 (5.0)

n = 200

5.0 (4.1)

n = 134

2.42 .090 0.007 .716 .073 .360
HADS‐SIS: outward irritability subscore

3.6 (2.7)

n = 387

3.3 (2.8)

n = 201

3.3 (2.8)

n = 135

0.776 .461 0.002 .553 .599 .998
HADS‐SIS: inward irritability subscore

2.4 (2.4)

n = 387

2.4 (2.9)

n = 201

1.7 (2.1)

n = 135

4.36 .013 0.012 .974 .011 .042
HADS‐SIS: anxiety subscore

6.0 (4.2)

n = 387

6.0 (4.7)

n = 200

5.2 (3.8)

n = 132

1.94 .144 0.005 .989 .133 .239

Statistical data given as a comparison of mean values (standard deviation) between choreatic, dystonic, and hypokinetic‐rigid groups using ANOVA and post hoc tests (Tukey's HSD).

Abbreviations: F, F value; HADS‐SIS, Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale; IS, Independence Scale; M, mean; MMSE, Mini‐Mental State Examination; p, p value; Part η 2, effect size; PBA‐s, problem behaviors assessment; SD, standard deviation; SDMT, symbol digit modality test; TFC, total functional score; TMS, total motor score.

Analysis of motoric manifestation verified a higher UHDRS‐TMS for hypokinetic subjects when compared to other groups (all p < .001). The dystonic group had earlier motoric onset when compared to choreatic (p < .001) but not to hypokinetic‐rigid subjects (p = .216). Comparisons of choreatic versus hypokinetic did not differ in means (p = .072). Timed onsets for the rater's estimation date of HD diagnosis and symptoms noticed by participant and family differed in same manner as described for motoric manifestation.

Differences regarding cognitive capacity revealed lower performance of hypokinetic if compared to choreatic or dystonic participants (all p < .001). Differences of the choreatic versus dystonic group were not significant except for better performance in SDMT (p < .001) and verbal fluency (180 s; p < .005).

Functional capacities differed for the UHDRS‐total functional capacity (TFC) score and Independence Scale (IS) (all p < .001) detecting choreatic with better capacities compared to dystonic (all p < .005) and hypokinetic (all p < .001). Post hoc tests indicated better functionality for the dystonic versus the hypokinetic group (all p < .001).

Beside higher apathy scores for hypokinetic versus. dystonic and choreatic (all p < .001) and executive function (choreatic vs. hypokinetic; p < .001), the psychiatric scores revealed no group differences.

One‐way ANCOVAs determined effects in cognitive and functional scores remained significant (all p < .001) after controlling for covariates (Table 2). Controlling for the covariate CAP score indicated highest effect‐ changes (in partial η 2) compared to other covariates.

TABLE 2.

Analysis of cognitive and functional capacities in choreatic, dystonic, and hypokinetic‐rigid motor subtypes controlling for covariates

ANCOVA Mot. Phenotype (choreatic, dystonic, hypokinetic) With covariate CAP score With covariate CAG high With covariate age With covariate disease duration With covariate education
F p Part. η 2 F p Part. η 2 F p Part. η 2 F p Part. η 2 F p Part. η 2 F p Part. η 2
SDMT 154.71 <.001 0.207 128.26 <.001 0.178 156.44 <.001 0.209 156.73 <.001 0.209 142.24 <.001 0.195 141.51 <.001 0.193
Verbal fluency (letters) 136.85 <.001 0.173 111.95 <.001 0.147 136.60 <.001 0.173 137.38 <.001 0.174 124.81 <.001 0.162 124.64 <.001 0.161
Stroop color naming 164.28 <.001 0.203 141.91 <.001 0.181 172.56 <.001 0.212 166.38 <.001 0.206 150.18 <.001 0.190 150.43 <.001 0.190
Stroop word reading 173.35 <.001 0.215 145.63 <.001 0.187 176.54 <.001 0.218 175.23 <.001 0.217 157.69 <.001 0.201 158.29 <.001 0.201
Stroop interference 91.84 <.001 0.150 82.05 <.001 0.137 104.12 <.001 0.167 98.47 <.001 0.160 83.24 <.001 0.139 82.04 <.001 0.137
TFC 242.08 <.001 0.261 202.88 <.001 0.228 240.65 <.001 0.260 242.69 <.001 0.261 215.31 <.001 0.240 225.16 <.001 0.249
IS 295.27 <.001 0.301 248.82 <.001 0.266 294.40 <.001 0.300 296.50 <.001 0.302 266.36 <.001 0.281 278.75 <.001 0.290

Comparison of mean values.

Abbreviations: F, F value; IS, Independence Scale; p, p value; Part η 2, effect size; SDMT, symbol digit modality test; TFC, total functional score.

4. DISCUSSION

To our knowledge, this is the first study comprising data coming from a large cohort of manifest HD patients analyzing subjects with motor symptoms in terms of predominantly dystonia compared to choreatic and hypokinetic‐rigid regarding their cognitive, functional, and psychiatric performance.

If compared to hypokinetic‐rigids, no differences regarding onsets were observed in the dystonic group except for earlier apathy. Contrary dystonic participants showed earlier onsets compared to choreatic. In contrast to earlier publication, no difference was observed for the choreatic compared to the hypokinetic‐rigid group even if CAG repeats and the CAP score were significantly lower in the choreatic group (Hart et al., 2013). It is reasonably possible that in previous research, dystonic patients were implemented partly within a hypokinetic‐rigid cohort. As mentioned, we identified a large cohort (n = 661) of patients with interfering dystonic and hypokinetic‐rigid motor subtypes which were excluded from our analysis. Former research might have implemented these patients in their analysis which might explain the different effects regarding onsets.

Former research described dystonia as a symptom which is related to increased HD stages with longer motor disease duration (Zande et al., 2017). In our large cohort, dystonic patients were younger than predominant choreatic and hypokinetic‐rigid patients (all p < .001). Additionally, if compared to choreatic subjects (p = .054), the disease duration of dystonic participants did not differ. Hence, we assumed that dystonia may occur early and is not solely accompanied with longer disease durations, which is unexpected and partly in opposite to earlier findings.

Moreover dystonia seems to have an early impact since patients and families notice symptoms earlier than in the choreatic group (all p < .001). These results go along with dystonia research data additionally describing patients suffering from pain and emotional harm caused by dystonia with a high impact on the quality of life (Bernstein et al., 2016; Torres & Rosales, 2017).

Regarding most tasks for cognition and TMS, there was no difference between the dystonic and the choreatic group, except for SDMT which might be due to a motor impairment and verbal fluency (180 s) for letters. Dystonic patients showed better cognitive performance in all categories when compared to hypokinetic‐rigid patients (all p < .001).

Comparison of TFC was different in all groups, whereas dystonic patients ranged in‐between choreatic with higher and hypokinetic‐rigid with lower capacities. Thus, recent research describing greater impact of dystonia and rigidity compared to chorea on functionality can be confirmed in a large cohort (Carlozzi et al., 2019).

Group differences were found for CAG repeats, disease duration, education, TMS, and CAP score, with lower CAG repeats in the choreatic group versus the hypokinetic‐rigid (p < .001) but also for the choreatic versus the dystonic group (p < .001). Since differences might have an impact, we focused on controlling these which resulted in no change in findings for cognitive and functional capacities. Thus, group differences are not because of underlying effects from varying covariates.

Apart from an increased apathy sum score in the hypokinetic‐rigid group versus the dystonic and choreatic group, we did not observe any differences in the severity of psychiatric symptoms between groups. Besides, less executive function was observed in the hypokinetic versus the choreatic group (PBA score). This is remarkable because patients with focal dystonia complained about increased psychiatric symptoms with depression, anxiety, and obsessive‐compulsive disorders (Jahanshahi, 2017). We therefore initially hypothesize that dystonia may lead to more depression in HD which is however not supported by our data. Hereby, unawareness in HD patients might be a possible explanation (Hergert, Haaland, & Cimino, 2019; Hergert, Sanchez‐Ramos, & Cimino, 2019; McCusker & Loy, 2014; Sitek, Thompson, Craufurd, & Snowden, 2014). According to our data, functional status and cognition seem not primary influenced by psychiatric status.

5. CONCLUSION

In summary, we were able to confirm better cognitive capacity of choreatic versus hypokinetic‐rigid patients and extend this knowledge to dystonic patients showing a cognitive capacity similar to choreatic. In this study, we identified and characterized a large cohort of dystonic HD patients for the first time. Motoric phenotypes are relevant for functional and cognitive capacities independent from described co‐variates. On the one hand, dystonic patients seem to have a similar onset to hypokinetic‐rigid patients, but on the other hand, a cognitive performance is similar to choreatic patients, which is not explained by higher CAG repeats, CAP scores, age, disease duration, or education.

CONFLICT OF INTEREST

Jannis Achenbach, Sarah Maria von Hein, and Dr. Carsten Saft reported no conflict of interest that relate to the research covered in this article.

AUTHORS' CONTRIBUTION

Jannis Achenbach involved in conception, organization, and execution of research project; design and execution of statistical analysis; writing of the first draft of manuscript. Sarah Maria von Hein involved in conception of research project; review and critique of statistical analysis; review and critique of manuscript. Carsten Saft involved in conception and organization of research project; review and critique of statistical analysis; review and critique of manuscript.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

Enroll‐HD is a clinical research platform and longitudinal observational study for Huntington's disease families intended to accelerate progress toward therapeutics; it is sponsored by the CHDI Foundation, a nonprofit biomedical research organization exclusively dedicated to collaboratively developing therapeutics for HD. Enroll‐HD would not be possible without the vital contribution of the research participants and their families. We acknowledge the individuals (https://www.enroll‐hd.org/acknowledgments/) who contributed to the collection of the Enroll‐HD data. We acknowledge support by the DFG Open Access Publication Funds of the Ruhr‐Universität Bochum.

Achenbach J, von Hein SM, Saft C. Functional and cognitive capacity differ in dystonic motor subtypes when compared to choreatic and hypokinetic‐rigid motor subtypes in Huntington's disease. Brain Behav. 2020;10:e01704 10.1002/brb3.1704

The peer review history for this article is available at https://publons.com/publon/10.1002/brb3.1704

Funding information

Jannis Achenbach and Sarah Maria von Hein did not receive any funding for the last 12 months. Dr. Saft reports personal fees/honoraria from Teva Pharma GmbH, as well as nonfinancial support and other support from ENROLL‐HD study (CHDI), PRIDE‐HD (TEVA), LEGATO (TEVA), and Amaryllis (Pfizer), ASO (IONIS Pharmaceuticals and Roche AG) for conduction of studies, and grants from Biogen all outside the submitted work and without relevance to the manuscript.

DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon request.

REFERENCES

  1. Bernstein, C. J. , Ellard, D. R. , Davies, G. , Hertenstein, E. , Tang, N. K. Y. , Underwood, M. , & Sandhu, H. (2016). Behavioural interventions for people living with adult‐onset primary dystonia: A systematic review. BMC Neurology, 16, 40 10.1186/s12883-016-0562-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Carlozzi, N. E. , Schilling, S. G. , Boileau, N. R. , Chou, K. L. , Perlmutter, J. S. , Frank, S. , … Dayalu, P. (2019). How different aspects of motor dysfunction influence day‐to‐day function in huntington's disease. Movement Disorders, 34, 1910–1914. 10.1002/mds.27866 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Girotti, F. , Marano, R. , Soliveri, P. , Geminiani, G. , & Scigliano, G. (1988). Relationship between motor and cognitive disorders in Huntington's disease. Journal of Neurology, 235, 454–457. 10.1007/BF00314246 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Hart, E. P. , Marinus, J. , Burgunder, J. M. , Bentivoglio, A. R. , Craufurd, D. , Reilmann, R. , … Roos, R. A. C. (2013). Better global and cognitive functioning in choreatic versus hypokinetic‐rigid Huntington's disease. Movement Disorders, 28, 1142–1145. 10.1002/mds.25422 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Hergert, D. C. , Haaland, K. Y. , & Cimino, C. R. (2019). Evaluation of a performance‐rating method to assess awareness of cognitive functioning in Huntington's disease. Clinical Neuropsychology, 34, 477–497. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Hergert, D. C. , Sanchez‐Ramos, J. , & Cimino, C. R. (2019). Awareness of chorea in Huntington's disease. Journal of Huntington's Disease, 9, 99–103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Jahanshahi, M. (2017). Neuropsychological and neuropsychiatric features of idiopathic and DYT1 dystonia and the impact of medical and surgical treatment. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 32, 888–905. 10.1093/arclin/acx095 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Kieburtz, K. , Penney, J. B. , Corno, P. , Ranen, N. , Shoulson, I. , Feigin, A. , … Huntington Study Group (1996). Unified Huntington's disease rating scale: Reliability and consistency. Movement Disorders, 11, 136–142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Landwehrmeyer, G. B. , Fitzer‐Attas, C. J. , Giuliano, J. D. , Gonçalves, N. , Anderson, K. E. , Cardoso, F. … Sampaio, C. (2017). Data analytics from Enroll‐HD, a global clinical research platform for Huntington's disease. Movement Disorders Clinical Practice, 4, 212–224. 10.1002/mdc3.12388 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Mahant, N. , McCusker, E. A. , Byth, K. , & Graham, S. (2003). Huntington's disease: Clinical correlates of disability and progression. Neurology, 61, 1085–1092. 10.1212/01.WNL.0000086373.32347.16 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. McCusker, E. , & Loy, C. T. (2014). The many facets of unawareness in huntington disease. Tremor and Other Hyperkinetic Movements, 4, 257. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Reedeker, N. , Van Der Mast, R. C. , Giltay, E. J. , Van Duijn, E. , & Roos, R. A. (2010). Hypokinesia in Huntington's disease co‐occurs with cognitive and global dysfunctioning. Movement Disorders, 25, 1612–1618. 10.1002/mds.23136 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Roos, R. A. (2010). Huntington's disease: A clinical review. Orphanet Journal of Rare Diseases, 5, 40 10.1186/1750-1172-5-40 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Saft, C. , Lauter, T. , Kraus, P. H. , Przuntek, H. , & Andrich, J. E. (2006). Dose‐dependent improvement of myoclonic hyperkinesia due to Valproic acid in eight Huntington's Disease patients: A case series. BMC Neurology, 6, 11 10.1186/1471-2377-6-11 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Saft, C. , von Hein, S. M. , Lucke, T. , Thiels, C. , Peball, M. , Djamshidian, A. , … Seppi, K. (2018). Cannabinoids for treatment of dystonia in Huntington's disease. Journal of Huntington's Disease, 7, 167–173. 10.3233/JHD-170283 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Sanchez‐Pernaute, R. , Kunig, G. , del Barrio, A. A. , de Yebenes, J. G. , Vontobel, P. , & Leenders, K. L. (2000). Bradykinesia in early Huntington's disease. Neurology, 54, 119–125. 10.1212/WNL.54.1.119 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Sitek, E. J. , Thompson, J. C. , Craufurd, D. , & Snowden, J. S. (2014). Unawareness of deficits in Huntington's disease. Journal of Huntington's Disease, 3, 125–135. 10.3233/JHD-140109 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Thompson, P. D. , Berardelli, A. , Rothwell, J. C. , Day, B. L. , Dick, J. , Benecke, R. , & Marsden, C. D. (1988). The coexistence of bradykinesia and chorea in Huntington's disease and its implications for theories of basal ganglia control of movement. Brain, 111(Pt 2), 223–244. 10.1093/brain/111.2.223 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Torres, J. , & Rosales, R. L. (2017). Nonmotor symptoms in dystonia. International Review of Neurobiology, 134, 1335–1371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. van de Zande, N. A. , Massey, T. H. , McLauchlan, D. , Pryce Roberts, A. , Zutt, R. , Wardle, M. , … Peall, K. J. (2017). Clinical characterization of dystonia in adult patients with Huntington's disease. European Journal of Neurology, 24, 1140–1147. 10.1111/ene.13349 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Walker, F. O. (2007). Huntington's disease. The Lancet, 369, 218–228. 10.1016/S0140-6736(07)60111-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Wojtecki, L. , Groiss, S. J. , Ferrea, S. , Elben, S. , Hartmann, C. J. , Dunnett, S. B. , … Vesper, J. (2015). A prospective pilot trial for Pallidal deep brain stimulation in Huntington's disease. Frontiers in Neurology, 6, 177 10.3389/fneur.2015.00177 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Young, A. B. , Shoulson, I. , Penney, J. B. , Starosta‐Rubinstein, S. , Gomez, F. , Travers, H. , … Wexler, N. S. (1986). Huntington's disease in Venezuela: Neurologic features and functional decline. Neurology, 36, 244–249. 10.1212/WNL.36.2.244 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Zhang, Y. , Long, J. D. , Mills, J. A. , Warner, J. H. , Lu, W. , & Paulsen, J. S. (2011). Indexing disease progression at study entry with individuals at‐risk for Huntington disease. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 156b, 751–763. 10.1002/ajmg.b.31232 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Zittel, S. , Tadic, V. , Moll, C. K. E. , Bäumer, T. , Fellbrich, A. , Gulberti, A. , … Münchau, A. (2018). Prospective evaluation of Globus pallidus internus deep brain stimulation in Huntington's disease. Parkinsonism & Related Disorders, 51, 96–100. 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2018.02.030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon request.


Articles from Brain and Behavior are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES