Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2020 Aug 14;10:13811. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-70836-y

Association of self-regulation with white matter correlates in boys with and without autism spectrum disorder

Hsing-Chang Ni 1,2, Hsiang-Yuan Lin 3,4,5, Wen-Yih Isaac Tseng 6,7,, Susan Shur-Fen Gau 2,3,6,
PMCID: PMC7429820  PMID: 32796900

Abstract

Previous studies demonstrated distinct neural correlates underpinning impaired self-regulation (dysregulation) between individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and typically developing controls (TDC). However, the impacts of dysregulation on white matter (WM) microstructural property in ASD and TDC remain unclear. Diffusion spectrum imaging was acquired in 59 ASD and 62 TDC boys. We investigated the relationship between participants’ dysregulation levels and microstructural property of 76 WM tracts in a multivariate analysis (canonical correlation analysis), across diagnostic groups. A single mode of brain-behavior co-variation was identified: participants were spread along a single axis linking diagnosis, dysregulation, diagnosis-by-dysregulation interaction, and intelligence to a specific WM property pattern. This mode corresponds to diagnosis-distinct correlates underpinning dysregulation, which showed higher generalized fractional anisotropy (GFA) associated with less dysregulation in ASD but greater dysregulation in TDC, in the tracts connecting limbic and emotion regulation systems. Moreover, higher GFA of the tracts implicated in memory, attention, sensorimotor processing, and perception associated with less dysregulation in TDC but worse dysregulation in ASD. No shared WM correlates of dysregulation between ASD and TDC were identified. Corresponding to previous studies, we demonstrated that ASD and TDC have broad distinct white matter microstructural property underpinning self-regulation.

Subject terms: Neuroscience, Emotion, Neural circuits, Social neuroscience

Introduction

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder that encompasses the impairments in social interaction and communication and restricted, repetitive patterns of behaviors1. Besides the core symptoms, impaired self-regulation (namely dysregulation) in ASD is also common2,3. Generally, individuals’ optimal self-regulation can facilitate flexible modification of their interoceptive state and modulation of response to exteroceptive stimuli4. Suboptimal self-regulation is associated with heightened risk for affective psychopathology5 and maladaptive behaviors68. Dysregulation in ASD also links to higher use of psychiatric services, more social impairment, lower family quality of life, and more depressive and anxiety symptoms913. Dysregulation associated with ASD may be explained by co-occurring psychiatric disorders, maladaptive strategies, less-frequent use of cognitive reappraisal, and inherent autistic psychopathology2,14,15.

Self-regulation is a theoretically complex construct, which involves the affective, behavioral, and cognitive control16. Specifically, emotion regulation involves dual processes, which initiate regulation contingent on explicit and implicit goals as supported by controlled and automatic processes, repsectively17. Cognitive control interacts with motivation and may be treated as a domain of reward-based decision making18. Behavioral regulation is theoretically associated with belief salience measures, past behavior/habit, perceived behavioral control, self-efficacy, moral norms, self-identify, and affective beliefs19. However, such essential elements of self-regulation tend to work synergistically and show ongoing and dynamic modulation of each other16. Correspondingly and similarly, neural correlates of affect regulation implicate the amygdala, insula, ventromedial prefrontal (vmPFC)/orbitofrontal (OFC) cortex, ventrolateral (vlPFC), dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), and anterior cingulate cortex17. These prefrontal and cingulate components of affect regulation encompass the key regions involved in cognitive18 and behavioral control20. Of note, different striatal and subcortical regions may distinctly contribute to affect21, cognitive22, and behavioral control23.

To sum up, affect, behavioral, and cognitive control teams up with each other to facilitate self-regulation. Following this collective perspective, the level of dysregulation could potentially be measured by the Child Behavioral Checklist-Dysregulation Profile (CBCL-DP)24, which characterizes co-occurring elevations on the Anxiety-Depression (affect), Aggression (behavior), Attention (cognition) subscales on the CBCL in both clinical24 and non-clinical populations25. Previous study demonstrated that the CBCL-DP is different from CBCL total scores and specific for distinct adult outcomes26. The CBCL-DP profile has been shown to estimate the deficit emotional self-regulation and predict the functional impairment across several psychiatric disorders27. We also have applied this CBCL-DP to investigate structural28,29 and intrinsic functional correlates30 of dysregulation associated with ASD.

Despite the increasing understanding of the importance of self-regulation in ASD, the neural correlates underlying self-regulation in ASD are far from conclusive31. For example, using functional magnetic resonance image (fMRI), Richey et al.32 found children with ASD, relative to TDC, have less increase in the nucleus accumbens and amygdala and less change in the dlPFC activation during cognitive reappraisal of faces. Pitskel et al.33 found TDC show downregulation of bilateral insula and left amygdala while children with ASD show no modulation of insula and upregulation of the left amygdala on the decrease trials of emotional responses to disgusting images. Based on the CBCL-DP, our recent resting-state fMRI study demonstrated that ASD and TDC have distinct intrinsic functional connectivity in relation to dysregulation30. Overall, these fMRI studies have demonstrated that ASD and TDC appear to have different regional brain activities and connectivity in processing self-regulation. However, investigations based on structural MRI suggest the hypothesis asserting distinct neural mechanisms underpinning self-regulation in ASD and TDC remains contentious28,29.

As the development of white matter (WM) connectivity speaks to coordinated gray matter growth and functional network organization, WM tracts interconnecting the preceding regions may also distinctly relate with dysregulation in ASD. Diffusion-weighted MRI (dMRI) is a powerful method to characterize the organization and architecture of WM fibers34, by estimating the water diffusion profile in the brain. Although prior reports have applied dMRI to portray diagnosis-distinct WM microstructural properties in several psychiatric disorders with impaired self-regulation, such as major depressive disorder35, and bipolar disorders36, only one study has explored dimensional WM correlates of self-regulation in healthy adults37. Vandekerckhove et al.37 demonstrated that groups using high and low bottom-up emotional regulation have different WM microstructural properties in the tracts supporting emotion regulation, cognitive and motor control, and sensory affective processing information. Earlier literature suggests that the most consistent alterations of WM microstructural properties and organization in ASD involve corpus callosum and superior longitudinal fasciculus38,39, which might also be implicated in self-regulation given their respective roles in whole-brain information integration (corpus callosum) and communication within frontoparietal and frontotemporal systems (superior longitudinal fasciculus)40. Nonetheless, to our best knowledge, no study has yet investigated WM correlates underpinning dysregulation in ASD, which hampers a better understanding of this critical problem in ASD.

To investigate WM correlates of dysregulation in intellectually able boys with ASD and TDC boys, we leveraged recent advances in multivariate analysis, canonical correlation analysis (CCA)41, and the diffusion spectrum imaging (DSI) tractography42. DSI, relative to popular diffusion tensor imaging (DTI), was intentionally adopted to enable the detection of crossing WM fiber bundles, which have pronounced effects on tractography43. We hypothesized that the ASD and TDC groups would have distinct associations of dysregulation with microstructural property in widespread WM tracts, which interconnect regions involved in cognitive, affective, and behavioral controls.

Results

The ASD and TDC groups in the main sample (59 ASD and 62 TDC) had comparable demographic features, including age, handedness, intelligence, and in-scanner head motion levels (signal dropout counts) and DSI data signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) (Table 1). The psychiatric comorbidity and concurrent methylphenidate use is shown in Supplementary Table 1.

Table 1.

Demographic and clinical features of the main sample.

Mean (SD) ASD
(n = 59)
TDC
(n = 62)
Statistics
P value
Age (years) 12.5 (1.7) 12.0 (2.1) 0.199
Handedness, right (%) 55 (91.7) 60 (96.8) 0.432
Full-scale IQ 106.6 (14.3) 110.2 (11.3) 0.129
Verbal IQ 107.1 (14.1) 110.1 (10.9) 0.214
Performance IQ 105.1 (16.2) 109.1 (13.1) 0.150
Impaired self-regulation 204.8 (42.9) 151.0 (30.8) < 0.001
Autism diagnostic interview-reviseda
Social 9.7 (5.3)
Communication 8.8 (4.1)
Repetitive and stereotyped behaviors 5.1 (2.7)
Head motion and image quality
Signal-to-noise ratio 27.9 (2.9) 27.3 (2.6) 0.217
Signal dropout counts 13.7 (15.6) 10.8 (12.5) 0.259

aBased on the current behavior algorithms.

ASD, autism spectrum disorder; TDC, typically developing controls; IQ, intelligence quotient; SD, standard deviation.

Seventy-six white matter tracts of the whole brain were identified by the tract-based automatic analysis (TBAA) method42, and their generalized fractional anisotropy (GFA) values were calculated44. After controlling for age linear and squared terms, signal dropout counts, and SNR, the diagnostic differences in WM property did not survive correction for multiple tests by false discovery rate, FDR45 (Supplementary Table 2).

CCA applies a multivariate approach to identify latent linear brain-behavior relationships41 between sets of independent (behavioral measures) and dependent (brain measures) variables46. The first mode of CCA estimates the maximum co-variation between these two sets of the brain and behavioral measures. The maximum residual, orthogonal co-variation is represented by subsequent modes. One significant mode (r = 0.59, FWE-corrected p = 0.005) of interdependencies between WM property patterns and the diagnosis, diagnosis by dysregulation interaction and general cognitive function (full-scale intelligence quotient, FIQ) (Fig. 1A and Supplementary Table 3) was identified by CCA. Specifically, lower GFA values of 3 tracts, including the left uncinate fasciculus, left inferior longitudinal fasciculus (ILF) and genu of the corpus callosum, were positively associated with higher dysregulation in ASD, but lower dysregulation levels in TDC (Fig. 1B, Table 2 and Supplementary Table 4). FIQ and diagnosis (as expressed as ASD > TDC) were also negatively associated with WM property patterns of this set of tract bundles. The directions of the diagnostic differences which drove this set of WM tracts identified in the first CCA mode were compatible with that found in the conventional generalized linear model (Supplementary Table 2).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Canonical correlation analysis (CCA) mode relating microstructural property of 76 white matter tracts to dysregulation levels and cognitive measures across the whole cohort. (A) The CCA analysis identified a single significant (FWE-corrected p = 0.005) mode of associations between white matter microstructural property and the behavioral variables of interest. The strength and direction of the variance explained by the CCA mode are indicated in the figure by the vertical position and font size. (B) Higher dysregulation levels in ASD, while lower dysregulation levels in TDC, were negatively correlated with white matter property of a set of 3 tracts including the left uncinate fasciculus (UF), left inferior longitudinal fasciculus (ILF) and genu of corpus callosum (CC). (C) Higher dysregulation levels in ASD, while lower dysregulation levels in TDC, were positively correlated with white matter property of a set of 16 tracts including the main body and hippocampal components of the left cingulum (CG), hand and mouth part of the left corticospinal tract (CST), toe, trunk and throat part of bilateral corticospinal tracts, left thalamic radiation (TR) linking to the precentral gyrus and auditory nerve, right thalamic radiation linking to the postcentral gyrus, right frontal aslant tract, left medial lemniscus and the posterior commissure, left thalamic radiation linking to the precentral gyrus and auditory region, right thalamic radiation linking to the postcentral gyrus, right frontal aslant tract (FAT), left medial lemniscus and the posterior commissure (PC). The tracts depicted here are the tractogram reconstructed in the DSI template, which derived from the average of diffusion datasets of 122 healthy adults42. The exact delineation of the tracts varied somewhat between individuals (see Supplementary Fig. 2). R, right; L, left; GFA, generalized fractional anisotropy.

Table 2.

Similar and different associations between dysregulation and GFA values between autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and typically developing controls (TDC) based on the main sample.

Pattern Tract Connected ROIs Connected ROIs System
Similar association Not significant
Different association
(a) The lower GFA values with the worse regulation in ASD/the better regulation in TDC
Left ILF L_temporal pole Occipital lobe Emotion recognition and visual-affective integration
Left UF L_orbitofrontal gyrus L_superior temporal pole Emotion regulation
CC of genu L_frontal components, including orbitofrontal gyrus R_frontal components, including orbitofrontal gyrus high cortical function regulation
(b) The higher GFA values with the worse regulation in ASD/the better regulation in TDC
L_CST of mouth L_primary motor cortex of mouth component Brain stem Motor
L_CST of toe L_primary motor cortex of toe component Brain stem Motor
L_CST of geniculate fibers L_primary motor cortex of throat component Brain stem Motor
L_CST of trunk L_primary motor cortex of trunk component Brain stem Motor
L_CST of hand L_primary motor cortex of hand component Brain stem Motor
R_CST of geniculate fibers R_primary motor cortex of throat component Brain stem Motor
R_CST of trunk R_primary motor cortex of trunk component Brain stem Motor
R_CST of toe R_primary motor cortex of toe component Brain stem Motor
L_TR of auditory nerve L_thalamus L_Heschl’s gyrus Sensory processing
L_TR of precentral gyrus L_thalamus L_precentral gyrus Sensorimotor integration
R_TR of postcentral gyrus R_thalamus R_postcentral gyrus Sensory processing
L_cingulum of hippocampal component L_cingulate gyrus posterior part L_hippocampus Emotion, memory
L_cingulum of the main body component L_cingulate gyrus (anterior + middle part) L_cingulate gyrus posterior part Attention, emotion, cognitive control
Posterior commissure Dorsal aspect of the upper end of the cerebral aqueduct Bilateral cerebral hemispheres Pupillary light reflex and upward saccade, both related to automatic emotion perception
R_frontal aslant tract R_SMA R_inferior frontal gyrus opercular part Inhibition
L_medial lemniscus L_thalamus Brain stem Somatosensory

CC, corpus callosum; CST, corticospinal tract; ILF, inferior longitudinal fasciculus; L, left; R, right; SMA, supplementary motor area; TR, thalamic radiation; UF, uncinate fasciculus.

On the other hand, higher GFA values of 16 tracts were positively associated with higher dysregulation in ASD but lower dysregulation levels in TDC (Fig. 1C, Table 2, and Supplementary Table 4). These 16 tracts included the main body and hippocampal components of the left cingulum, the hand and mouth part of the left corticospinal tract, the toe, trunk and throat part of bilateral corticospinal tracts, the left thalamic radiation linking to the precentral gyrus and auditory nerve, the right thalamic radiation linking to the postcentral gyrus, the right frontal aslant tract, the left medial lemniscus, and the posterior commissure, the left thalamic radiation linking to the precentral gyrus and auditory region, the right thalamic radiation linking to the postcentral gyrus, the right frontal aslant tract, the left medial lemniscus and the posterior commissure. The other 3 modes did not capture any additional meaningful association between patterns of WM property and behaviors (p = 0.080, 0.229, 0.479, respectively).

To test the robustness of our results, we adopted two strategies. First, we implemented a “leave-five-out per diagnostic group” sensitivity analysis with 1,000 permutations. The average p value of the first CCA mode was 0.015, and 95.3% of p values among these 1,000 permuted CCA was smaller than 0.05. Besides, we implemented the same CCA in the originally recruited sample (87 ASD and 77 TDC), which exhibited significant between-group differences in age and intelligence (Supplementary Table 5). This additional CCA also identified one significant mode (r = 0.61, FWE-corrected p = 0.032; Supplementary Table 6) of interdependences between WM property patterns and diagnosis by dysregulation interaction and FIQ. Similarly distinct pattern of WM property and dysregulation in ASD and TDC was also found in this larger originally-recruited sample (Supplementary Fig. 1). The set of WM bundle tracts as identified in this additional CCA (Supplementary Table 7) overlapped with some of those from the initial results (8 out of 19), but showed largely consistent patterns in the involved functional systems (Supplementary Table 8).

Discussion

The current study is the first to investigate the neural correlates underpinning self-regulation in ASD and TDC based on WM microstructural property. By using the state-of-the-art DSI tractography and multivariate approach, we observed that ASD and TDC had distinct associations of self-regulation with microstructural property in diverse WM tracts, which may further contribute to altered strategies and the presentations of emotion regulation and cognitive control in ASD.

We identified that lower GFA values were associated with higher dysregulation in ASD but lower dysregulation in TDC in the left ILF, left uncinate fasciculus, and genu of the corpus callosum. The ILF interconnects the anterior temporal pole (affective) and occipital lobe (visual) and is involved in face recognition, semantic processing, visual memory, and emotion47. Earlier studies demonstrated the strong association of ILF and recognition of facial emotions48,49. Lesions corresponding to the ILF are associated with impaired processing of affective facial expressions50. The uncinate fasciculus is an association tract that connects the amygdala and hippocampus with the orbitofrontal cortex and supports emotional empathy51 in patients with hemispheric stroke, and emotion regulation52 in neurotypical adults. Moreover, the microstructural integrity of the uncinate fasciculus is significantly correlated with the performance on the emotionally reading the mind in the Eyes Task in neurotypical adults53 and can identify the at-risk group for bipolar disorder and the group with non-bipolar psychopathology (i.e., depression and ADHD)54. The genu of the corpus callosum is a commissure tract connecting the bilateral orbitofrontal cortex, which is critically implicated in emotion regulation55, as well as connecting medial and lateral surface of the frontal regions. Previous studies demonstrated that the microstructural integrity of the genu of CC correlated with suicidal behaviors in bipolar disorder56 and several emotional dysregulation associated psychiatric disorders including borderline personality disorder57, major depressive disorder58, bipolar disorder59, and post-traumatic stress disorder60.

Jointly, the WM tracts mentioned above interconnecting prefrontal components, the OFC and limbic, and visual and affective systems, are critical in the self-regulation of socio-emotional behaviors31. The result that higher GFA values of this set of circuitries were associated with a dysregulation in TDC is largely consistent with WM37, functional17 and morphometric correlates61 of self-regulation, as reported in earlier literature on neurotypical adults. Notably, the direction of this brain-behavior relationship reversed in the ASD group (diagnosis by dysregulation interaction), suggesting that ASD and TDC have different patterns of associations between dysregulation and white matter microstructural property32,33,62,63. Our results endorse the hypothesis that alterations in circuitries linking OFC and limbic systems contribute to dysregulatory socioemotional behaviors in ASD31.

Simultaneously identified in this significant CCA mode, higher GFA was associated with higher dysregulation in ASD but lower dysregulation in TDC in several major fiber traits, as described below. The central part of the cingulum bundle (dorsal cingulum) is implicated in attention64,65 and executive control, and emotion. The hippocampal part of the cingulum (ventral or parahippocampal cingulum) is closely linked to the learning, episodic memory66, and manage emotion67. The corticospinal tract, connecting the brain stem and primary motor cortex, mainly supports motor processing and voluntary movement68. The subdivisions of these corticospinal tracts are based on previous reports68,69. Interestingly, the microstructural property of the corticospinal tract is also suggested to be associated with understanding emotions67. The thalamic radiations of precentral and postcentral parts connect the thalamus with precentral and postcentral gyrus and are critical relays within the motor and somatosensory network70,71. The left medial lemniscus connecting the thalamus and brain stem is associated with motor planning and execution72. The right frontal aslant tract connecting the supplementary motor area and inferior frontal gyrus is associated with executive function especially inhibitory control73. The posterior commissure is a commissural connection between the cerebrums and is suggested to be implicated in upward saccade and pupillary responses, which are both related to automatic emotion perception74. Altogether, these 16 tracts work in parallel to support top-down regulation, such as attention and inhibition, as well as bottom-up processes including memory, sensory-motor integration, motor processing, and automatic perception. Beyond the facets related to emotion, the process of self-regulation intricately implicates in several neuropsychological constructs such as executive and attention control, response inhibition, motor planning, and emotion reactivity and regulation16. The coordinated interplay between top-down and bottom-up systems is suggested to support many cognitive functions included under the ‘self-regulation’ umbrella, such as attention75, perception76, behavioral inhibition77, and emotion generation78. Together with the aforementioned results of tracts connecting OFC, visual, and limbic systems, our findings echo an earlier hypothesis conjecturing that emotion dysregulation in ASD might be related to cognitive regulation, informative processing, perception, or altered physiological activity2.

Our speculation that dysregulation in ASD is contributed by a complex interplay between top-down and bottom-up systems was further supported by the sensitivity analysis based on the originally recruited sample. Despite potential confounding effects from between-group age, intelligence, and DSI SNR differences in this larger cohort, around half of the identified tracts in this additional CCA overlapped with that set using the main sample. Those seemingly inconsistent tracts identified in the additional CCA were actually also involved in the foregoing functional systems (Supplementary Table 8). For example, the left superior longitudinal fasciculus I was identified in the WM set from additional CCA, and is involved in regulating motor behaviors and voluntary oriented visuospatial attention79. The left perpendicular fasciculus was a similar case, given its critical role in attention and eye movement control80. These concordant findings also indirectly endorse the validity of using the CBCL-Dysregulation, which could exhaustively capture the miscellaneous features of the dysregulation, in the present study.

Besides the findings of distinct WM correlates underpinning self-regulation, we also observed interdependence between FIQ as well as diagnosis, and the aforementioned WM property patterns in the significant CCA mode. This result provides evidence to endorse the notion that the level of intelligence may be linked to alterations in neural circuitries that are involved in self-regulation in ASD31. The direction of diagnostic differences in this set of 19 tracts, which partly contributed to the first CCA mode, was compatible with the results from the conventional generalized linear model (Supplementary Table 2). However, these autism-associated WM alterations based on the univariate analysis did not survive correction for multiple tests, which are incompatible with the previous studies81. The reasons for this lack of diagnostic differences may be related to some issues. A major source of inconsistency is certainly the fact that sample characteristics82, especially sex effects83, are confounded.

On the one hand, our male-only sample may reduce sex-related heterogeneity; on the other hand, this approach may limit comparability with other studies. Methodologically, the present study applied the template-based whole-brain tractography based on DSI data (TBAA) to investigate microstructural WM properties, particularly GFA42. Despite its methodological sensitivity and specificity, the current findings should be complemented by other dMRI data, such as high angular resolution diffusion imaging and DTI, other template-based tract-specific analysis, e.g., tract-based spatial statistics84, and another approach estimating structural connectivity85. Moreover, despite a high correlation between GFA and fractional anisotropy86,87, we should refrain from directly comparing the findings from DSI and DTI without considering their different biophysical models and assumptions88. In addition, the present null diagnostic difference based on the univariate analysis may reflect our emphasis on comparable levels of head motion between ASD and TDC, as emerging evidence has indicated that in-scanner motion tends to produce spurious group differences89.

Some other limitations need to be mentioned. First, the current intellectually able sample of ASD may limit the generalization of the present findings to others on the spectrum90. In addition, considering common co-occurrence, we included children with ASD co-occurring with ADHD in the study. The ratio of psychiatric comorbidity in our ASD group (Supplementary Table 1) is largely compatible with the previous literature91. Nonetheless, this caveat regarding inclusion criteria may influence the present results, as ADHD is also highly associated with impaired self-regulation92,93. Furthermore, the concurrent use of methylphenidate might also have effects on the WM microstructure94. Future studies, including individuals with a broader functional presentation on the autism spectrum as well as individuals with ADHD-only, could help address this limitation. Second, we only used the CBCL to estimate dysregulation levels. Despite the validity of CBCL-Dysregulation24, this profile could only capture proxy dysregulation information cardinally containing affective, behavior, and cognitive domains. Other approaches, such as emotion dysregulation inventory95, could be adopted in the future study.

Our results provide the first evidence that ASD and TDC have distinct associations of dysregulation with the properties of WM tracts involved in emotion, attention control, sensorimotor processing, and visual-affective integration. Our results should be considered robust based on the consistent findings from two different strategies of sensitivity analyses. These findings not only support a notion that self-regulation encompasses multiple cognitive processes, but also suggest indispensable diagnosis-specific strategies when advancing therapeutics for dysregulation in individuals with ASD.

Methods and materials

Participants and procedures

We restricted the recruitment to males because of differences in white matter properties between ASD girls and boys, and also a relatively low prevalence of female patients with ASD96,97. Study participants hence included 104 Taiwanese boys with ASD from the child psychiatric outpatient clinic of National Taiwan University Hospital (NTUH) and 90 TDC boys from similar geographical areas. Structural MRI (T1-weighted) data of some participants have been published elsewhere28. The diagnosis of ASD was first assessed by the corresponding author (SSG) based on the DSM-IV-TR and ICD-10 criteria and further confirmed by interviewing the parents with the Chinese version of the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R). The age range of participants was 7–17 years, and their FIQ was higher than 70. The intellectual function was assessed Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–3rd Edition (for those younger than 16 years) and Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-3rd Edition (for the remaining older participants).

To confirm the psychiatric comorbid conditions, all the participants accepted the assessment of the Chinese version of the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children–Epidemiological Version (K-SADS-E) interview98100. The exclusion criteria for ASD and TDC were different. For ASD, we excluded the lifetime and current major psychiatric disorders such as schizophrenia, mood disorder, and substance use disorders, while autistic participants with the lifetime or current co-occurring attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, oppositional defiant disorder, learning disorder, and tic disorder were included in our study. To decrease the impact of anxiety on our results, the lifetime co-occurring anxiety disorder was included, but current co-occurring anxiety disorder was excluded. For TDC, we excluded any lifetime and current DSM-IV psychiatric disorders such as schizophrenia, mood disorder, anxiety disorder, substance use disorders, ADHD, learning disorder, and tic disorder. Participants with current or lifetime severe medical or neurological illness (e.g., epilepsy) and psychotropic medication, except methylphenidate, were also excluded from the study. The Research Ethics Committee at NTUH approved our study before implementation (200903062R, 201201006RIB; ClinicalTrials.gov number, NCT00916851, NCT01582256). Besides the ethical standards of the Committee at the NTUH on human experimentation, all procedures contributing to this work also comply with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. Written informed consent forms were obtained from the participants and their parents after the detailed face-to-face explanations of the current study objectives and procedures.

Assessments of dysregulation by the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL)

The CBCL is a parent-reported scale to evaluate the behavioral problems of children aged 4–18. Among 118 items, eight subscales were categorized including attention, anxiety/depression, aggression, delinquency, social problems, somatic symptoms, thought problems, and withdrawn101. Raw scores of each subscale were transformed to T-scores with a mean of 50 and a standard deviation of 10 based on a norm of typically developing children and adolescents. The level of dysregulation was assessed by the sum of the T-scores from 3 subscales including Attention, Aggression, and Anxiety-Depression, as defined and used in the previous studies24,102.

Image acquisition

This study adopted DSI, rather than DTI, for its better capacity to deal with the issues of crossing fibers and to unravel complex structural information103.

T1-weighted images and DSI were acquired on a 3 T MRI system (Siemens Magnetom Tim Trio, Germany) using a 32-channel phased arrayed head coil. High-resolution T1-weight imaging was performed using a 3D magnetization prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo sequence: Repetition time (TR)/Echo time (TE) = 2,000/2.98 ms; Inversion time = 900 ms; flip angle = 9°; field of view = 256 mm × 192 mm; matrix size = 256 × 192 × 208; voxel size = 1 mm3. DSI was performed using a pulsed-gradient spin-echo echo planner imaging with a twice-refocused balanced echo sequence104. The DSI sequence comprised 102 diffusion-encoding directions corresponding to the grid points within a half-sphere of the 3D diffusion encoding space (q-space) with the maximum diffusion sensitivity value (bmax) of 4,000 s/mm2105. The grid points had coordinates with equidistance of 1 unit, and the coordinates of the grid points (qx, qy, qz) satisfied the relationship: (qx2 + qy2 + qz2) ≤ r2, where r was the radius specified by the DSI scheme. To reduce the scan time, the grid points contained within the half-sphere with radius r were acquired. The grid points outside the half-sphere were filled with zeros. Moreover, owing to the symmetric property of q space signal about the origin, only half of the q space data in the + z direction were acquired. For DSI with diffusion data acquired at 102 grid points within the half q space, the radius r was set at 3.6. The b values at grid coordinates were scaled according to the corresponding r values, referenced to bmax = 4,000 s/mm2 at r = 3. The other parameters were: TR/TE = 9,600/130 ms, FOV = 200 mm × 200 mm, acquisition matrix = 80 × 80, in-plane spatial resolution = 2.5 × 2.5 mm, slice thickness = 2.5 mm, and slice number = 54.

For the control of head motion, all participants were requested to lie still on the table with head movement restricted by expandable foam cushions. Besides, the DSI data underwent a quality assurance procedure to ensure acceptable in-scanner head motion by counting the number of diffusion-weighted images with signal dropout in the DSI datasets89. DSI datasets with more than 90 signal dropout images were discarded42. The DSI images of each individual (5,508 images per person, 102 (directions / slice) × 54 (slices / head)) were scrutinized by calculating the signals in the central square (20 × 20 pixels) of each image. Signal dropout was defined as the average signal intensity of an image lower than two standard deviations from the mean of all images (after correcting for its b value)42. Besides, we calculated the SNR based on the signal statistics in two predefined regions of interest, one placed in the central slice of the brain and the other in the background106. DSI datasets with SNR lower than 20 were excluded from the analysis.

Seventeen ASD and 13 TDC were excluded from further imaging processing owing to excessive in-scanner head motion and low SNR, yielding a sample of 87 boys with ASD and 77 TDC boys (the henceforth ‘originally recruited sample’). Our preliminary analysis found significant differences in age and intelligence between ASD and TDC. Such differences may strongly confound the findings. Therefore, we further matched the age and intelligence between the two groups, yielding a final sample of 59 boys with ASD and 62 TDC boys (the ‘main sample’ henceforth) for the initial data analysis. The main results were analyzed based on this main sample of 59 ASD and 62 TDC. We ran a sensitivity analysis by implementing an additional CCA based on the originally recruited sample (87 ASD and 77 TDC). These additional results are shown in Supplementary Tables 58 and Supplementary Fig. 1.

DSI image reconstruction

The DSI data were reconstructed based on the Fourier relationship between the probability density function (PDF) and q-space signal107. Three-dimensional Fourier transform was performed on the q-space signal, applied with a Hanning filter of 17 units in width, to reconstruct the PDF. The orientation distribution function (ODF) was determined by computing the second moment of the PDF along each of the 362 radial directions in a sixfold tessellation108. Herein, WM microstructural properties, specifically diffusion anisotropy (i.e., direction dependence)86 were represented by the GFA value44, which was estimated with the formula: (standard deviation of the ODF)/(root mean square of the ODF)44. The GFA value is the most widely accepted measure in DSI and q-ball imaging and has a high linear correlation with the fractional anisotropy value derived based on the diffusion tensor model in DTI109. Based on the study investigating the plasticity in the motor network of stroke patients110, the observed decreased GFA values might link to disruption or loss of the axonal structures, whereas a GFA increase might be related to axonal sprouting or myelin growth. Despite this speculation, GFA (or any diffusion anisotropy measure such as fractional anisotropy) is only an indirect measures of axonal and myelin properties.

Whole-brain tract-based automatic analysis

For the whole-brain tract analysis, the TBAA method was used to enable efficient tract-based analysis of the major fiber tracts over the entire brain42. Briefly, all the DSI datasets were first registered to create a study-specific template (SST), which was then registered to the DSI template (NTU-DSI-122)111. The DSI template (NTU-DSI-122) is a DSI data set averaged over 122 registered DSI datasets of healthy adults. A total of 76 tracts have been constructed by deterministic tractography using open software (DSI Studio: https://dsi-studio.labsolver.org). We then transformed the predefined 76 fiber tract bundles from the DSI template into the individual DSI data sets by transforming the sampling coordinates from the DSI template to the SST and then to the individual DSI data sets. GFA values were sampled on the tract coordinates of the 76 tract bundles. In this study, the mean GFA value was calculated from the GFA profile sampled along with each tract bundle in each participant. Supplementary Fig. 3 depicts the sample GFA profiles which record the GFA variability of the sampled tracts.

Statistical analysis

Data analyses on group comparisons of demographic data and mean GFA values were conducted by using SAS 9.4 version (SAS Institute, Cary, NC). The alpha value was preselected at 0.05. The demographic data were compared by independent sample t-test (Table 1). The mean GFA values of the whole-brain white matter tracts between ASD and TDC were compared using the general linear model with a linear and square term of age, signal dropout counts, and SNR as covariates (Supplementary Table 2). To control for multiple tests in 76 tracts, a false discovery rate (FDR, q) correction was set at q < 0.05.

Canonical correlation analysis

We implemented CCA, in steps similar to those previously reported41,112,113, to identify modes that relate WM microstructural property of 76 tracts with dysregulation levels and cognitive measures across the whole cohort (ASD + TDC).

In detail, the WM property matrix N1 (76 × 164) was normalized and demeaned per the procedure reported in Smith et al.41 (https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/analysis/ HCP-CCA/hcp_cca.m), resulting in a matrix N2 for subsequent analyses. The potential confounding effects of head motion and age (i.e., signal dropout counts, SNR, and linear and square terms of age) were regressed from N2 to yield N3. A principal component analysis was then implemented using the FSLNets toolbox114 to reduce the dimensionality of the deconfounded WM property matrix (N3) to twenty eigenvectors (explaining 80.63% of the total variance in the N3 matrix). The data were reduced to this resolution to balance between maintaining the information within the datasets and avoiding overfitting the CCA. Notably, we acknowledge that no consensus exists for component number selection115.

Four subject measures were chosen to be included in the CCA: diagnosis, dysregulation levels, diagnosis by dysregulation interaction, and FIQ. Four CCA modes, which consisted of weighted linear combinations of orthogonalized non-imaging measures and WM property eigenvectors patterns, were generated. Each mode m stands for the maximum latent co-variation between these two combinations of the brain and behavioral variates in decreasing rank order. The vectors Um and Vm denoted each participant’ weights for subject measures and WM property matrices within mode m, respectively:

The vector Um represented the level of which each participant is (positively or negatively) correlated to population variation in subject measures within the mode m. The vector Vm is the extent to which each participant is correlated to population variation in WM property within the mode m. Rm represented the population covariation in the mode m, and was calculated by the correlation between Um and Vm. Familywise error (FWE)-corrected alpha < 0.05 was estimated via 10,000 per-mutations of the rows of one matrix relative to the other, to determine the statistical significance of each CCA mode.

We next assessed which WM tracts were most powerfully expressed by population variations in the original sets of WM property captured by CCA mode m. First, to obtain the relative weight (and directional signs) of each tract associated with the WM property patterns within mode m, we correlated Vm with the original WM property estimates in N3, resulting in a vector AFm. In line with that has been previously done41,112,113, the top 25% highest absolute values within AFm defined WM tracts, which were most strongly covaried, either positively or negatively, with subject behavioral measures.

The other sensitivity analysis for CCA based on the primary sample (59 ASD and 62 TDC) was implemented using the ‘leave-five-out per diagnosis group’ approach, with 1,000 permutations. The average p value of the first CCA mode from these permutations was calculated.

Supplementary information

Supplementary file1. (5.4MB, docx)

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by grants from National Science Council of Taiwan (NSC97-3112-B-002-009, NSC98-3112-B-002-004, NSC 99-2627-B-002-015, NSC 100-2627-B-002-014, NSC 101-2627-B-002-002, NSC 101-2314-B-002-136-MY3), National Taiwan University Hospital (NTUH101-S1910), National Health Research Institute (NHRI-EX104-10404PI, NHRI-EX105-10404PI, NHRI-EX106-10404PI, NHRI-EX104-10404PI), Taiwan and in part by the Department of Medical Imaging and 3T MRI Lab in National Taiwan University Hospital.

Author contributions

S.S.F.G. is the principal investigator of this study. H.C.N., H.Y.L., and S.S.F.G. generated the hypothesis. S.S.F.G. recruited and assessed the participants. S.S.F.G. and W.Y.I.T. participated in the study supervision and acquisition of imaging data. H.C.N., H.Y.L., W.Y.I.T., and S.S.F.G. analyzed and interpreted the data. H.C.N., H.Y.L., W.Y.I.T. and S.S.F.G. wrote the manuscript. All authors read and approved the manuscript.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher's note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Contributor Information

Wen-Yih Isaac Tseng, Email: wytseng@ntu.edu.tw.

Susan Shur-Fen Gau, Email: gaushufe@ntu.edu.tw.

Supplementary information

is available for this paper at 10.1038/s41598-020-70836-y.

References

  • 1.Association AP. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-V) 5. Philadelphia: American Psychiatric Association; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Mazefsky CA, et al. The role of emotion regulation in Autism Spectrum Disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2013;52:679–688. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2013.05.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Samson AC, Hardan AY, Podell RW, Phillips JM, Gross JJ. Emotion regulation in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. 2015;8:9–18. doi: 10.1002/aur.1387. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Heatherton TF. Neuroscience of self and self-regulation. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2011;62:363–390. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.121208.131616. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Strauman TJ. Self-regulation and psychopathology: toward an integrative translational research paradigm. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2017;13:497–523. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032816-045012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Zucker RA, Heitzeg MM, Nigg JT. Parsing the undercontrol/disinhibition pathway to substance use disorders: a multilevel developmental problem. Child Dev. Perspect. 2011;5:248–255. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00172.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Andover MS, Morris BW. Expanding and clarifying the role of emotion regulation in nonsuicidal self-injury. Can. J. Psychiatry. 2014;59:569–575. doi: 10.1177/070674371405901102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Anderson SE, Keim SA. Parent-child interaction, self-regulation, and obesity prevention in early childhood. Curr. Obes. Rep. 2016;5:192–200. doi: 10.1007/s13679-016-0208-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Croen LA, Najjar DV, Ray GT, Lotspeich L, Bernal P. A comparison of health care utilization and costs of children with and without autism spectrum disorders in a large group-model health plan. Pediatrics. 2006;118:e1203–1211. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-0127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Goldsmith SF, Kelley E. Associations between emotion regulation and social impairment in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2018;48:2164–2173. doi: 10.1007/s10803-018-3483-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Nuske HJ, Hedley D, Tseng CH, Begeer S, Dissanayake C. Emotion regulation strategies in preschoolers with autism: associations with parent quality of life and family functioning. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2018;48:1287–1300. doi: 10.1007/s10803-017-3391-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Rieffe C, De Bruine M, De Rooij M, Stockmann L. Approach and avoidant emotion regulation prevent depressive symptoms in children with an autism spectrum disorder. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 2014;39:37–43. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2014.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Swain D, Scarpa A, White S, Laugeson E. Emotion dysregulation and anxiety in adults with ASD: does social motivation play a role? J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2015;45:3971–3977. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2567-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Mazefsky CA, Borue X, Day TN, Minshew NJ. Emotion regulation patterns in adolescents with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder: comparison to typically developing adolescents and association with psychiatric symptoms. Autism Res. 2014;7:344–354. doi: 10.1002/aur.1366. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Samson AC, Hardan AY, Lee IA, Phillips JM, Gross JJ. Maladaptive behavior in autism spectrum disorder: the role of emotion experience and emotion regulation. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2015;45:3424–3432. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2388-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Nigg JT. Annual research review: on the relations among self-regulation, self-control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control, impulsivity, risk-taking, and inhibition for developmental psychopathology. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2017;58:361–383. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12675. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Etkin A, Buchel C, Gross JJ. The neural bases of emotion regulation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2015;16:693–700. doi: 10.1038/nrn4044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Botvinick M, Braver T. Motivation and cognitive control: from behavior to neural mechanism. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2015;66:83–113. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Mark Conner CJA. Extending the theory of planned behavior: a review and avenues for further research. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 1998;28:1429–1464. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1998.tb01685.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Yang Y, Raine A. Prefrontal structural and functional brain imaging findings in antisocial, violent, and psychopathic individuals: a meta-analysis. Psychiatry Res. 2009;174:81–88. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2009.03.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Cardinal RN, Parkinson JA, Hall J, Everitt BJ. Emotion and motivation: the role of the amygdala, ventral striatum, and prefrontal cortex. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2002;26:321–352. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(02)00007-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Cools R, D'Esposito M. Inverted-U-shaped dopamine actions on human working memory and cognitive control. Biol. Psychiatry. 2011;69:e113–e125. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.03.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Macpherson T, Morita M, Hikida T. Striatal direct and indirect pathways control decision-making behavior. Front. Psychol. 2014;5:1301. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01301. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Ayer L, et al. Child behavior checklist juvenile bipolar disorder (CBCL-JBD) and CBCL posttraumatic stress problems (CBCL-PTSP) scales are measures of a single dysregulatory syndrome. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2009;50:1291–1300. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02089.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Kim J, et al. Correlates of the CBCL-dysregulation profile in preschool-aged children. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2012;53:918–926. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02546.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Althoff RR, Verhulst FC, Rettew DC, Hudziak JJ, van der Ende J. Adult outcomes of childhood dysregulation: a 14-year follow-up study. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2010;49:1105–1116. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2010.08.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Biederman J, et al. Longitudinal course of deficient emotional self-regulation CBCL profile in youth with ADHD: prospective controlled study. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 2012;8:267–276. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S29670. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Ni HC, et al. Neural correlates of impaired self-regulation in male youths with autism spectrum disorder: a voxel-based morphometry study. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry. 2018;82:233–241. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2017.11.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Ni HC, Lin HY, Chen YC, Tseng WI, Gau SS. Boys with autism spectrum disorder have distinct cortical folding patterns underpinning impaired self-regulation: a surface-based morphometry study. Brain Imaging Behav. 2019 doi: 10.1007/s11682-019-00199-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Lin HY, Ni HC, Tseng WI, Gau SS. Characterizing intrinsic functional connectivity in relation to impaired self-regulation in intellectually able male youth with autism spectrum disorder. Autism. 2020 doi: 10.1177/1362361319888104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Bachevalier J, Loveland KA. The orbitofrontal-amygdala circuit and self-regulation of social-emotional behavior in autism. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2006;30:97–117. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.07.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Richey JA, et al. Neural mechanisms of emotion regulation in autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2015;45:3409–3423. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2359-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Pitskel NB, Bolling DZ, Kaiser MD, Pelphrey KA, Crowley MJ. Neural systems for cognitive reappraisal in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2014;10:117–128. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2014.08.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Taylor DG, Bushell MC. The spatial mapping of translational diffusion coefficients by the NMR imaging technique. Phys. Med. Biol. 1985;30:345–349. doi: 10.1088/0031-9155/30/4/009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Jiang J, et al. Microstructural brain abnormalities in medication-free patients with major depressive disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis of diffusion tensor imaging. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 2017;42:150–163. doi: 10.1503/jpn.150341. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Nortje G, Stein DJ, Radua J, Mataix-Cols D, Horn N. Systematic review and voxel-based meta-analysis of diffusion tensor imaging studies in bipolar disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 2013;150:192–200. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2013.05.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Vandekerckhove M, Van Hecke W, Quirin M, De Mey J. Neural networks in bottom up 'experiential emotion regulation'. Behav. Brain Res. 2018 doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2018.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Aoki Y, Abe O, Nippashi Y, Yamasue H. Comparison of white matter integrity between autism spectrum disorder subjects and typically developing individuals: a meta-analysis of diffusion tensor imaging tractography studies. Mol. Autism. 2013;4:25. doi: 10.1186/2040-2392-4-25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Urger SE, et al. The superior longitudinal fasciculus in typically developing children and adolescents: diffusion tensor imaging and neuropsychological correlates. J. Child Neurol. 2015;30:9–20. doi: 10.1177/0883073813520503. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Kamali A, Flanders AE, Brody J, Hunter JV, Hasan KM. Tracing superior longitudinal fasciculus connectivity in the human brain using high resolution diffusion tensor tractography. Brain Struct. Funct. 2014;219:269–281. doi: 10.1007/s00429-012-0498-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Smith SM, et al. A positive-negative mode of population covariation links brain connectivity, demographics and behavior. Nat. Neurosci. 2015;18:1565–1567. doi: 10.1038/nn.4125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Chen YJ, et al. Automatic whole brain tract-based analysis using predefined tracts in a diffusion spectrum imaging template and an accurate registration strategy. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2015;36:3441–3458. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22854. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Wedeen VJ, et al. Diffusion spectrum magnetic resonance imaging (DSI) tractography of crossing fibers. Neuroimage. 2008;41:1267–1277. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Tuch DS. Q-ball imaging. Magn. Reson. Med. 2004;52:1358–1372. doi: 10.1002/mrm.20279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B. 1995;57:289–300. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Krzanowski W. Principles of Multivariate Analysis. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Herbet G, Zemmoura I, Duffau H. Functional anatomy of the inferior longitudinal fasciculus: from historical reports to current hypotheses. Front. Neuroanat. 2018;12:77. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2018.00077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Phillips OR, et al. Fiber tractography reveals disruption of temporal lobe white matter tracts in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2009;107:30–38. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.10.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Genova HM, et al. Facial affect recognition linked to damage in specific white matter tracts in traumatic brain injury. Soc. Neurosci. 2015;10:27–34. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2014.959618. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Philippi CL, Mehta S, Grabowski T, Adolphs R, Rudrauf D. Damage to association fiber tracts impairs recognition of the facial expression of emotion. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:15089–15099. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0796-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Oishi K, et al. Critical role of the right uncinate fasciculus in emotional empathy. Ann. Neurol. 2015;77:68–74. doi: 10.1002/ana.24300. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Eden AS, et al. Emotion regulation and trait anxiety are predicted by the microstructure of fibers between amygdala and prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 2015;35:6020–6027. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3659-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Coad BM, et al. Structural connections support emotional connections: uncinate fasciculus microstructure is related to the ability to decode facial emotion expressions. Neuropsychologia. 2017 doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.11.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Versace A, et al. Diffusion imaging markers of bipolar versus general psychopathology risk in youth at-risk. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2018;43:2212–2220. doi: 10.1038/s41386-018-0083-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Bechara A, Damasio H, Damasio AR. Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2000;10:295–307. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.3.295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Matsuo K, et al. Anterior genu corpus callosum and impulsivity in suicidal patients with bipolar disorder. Neurosci. Lett. 2010;469:75–80. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2009.11.047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Lischke A, et al. Structural alterations in the corpus callosum are associated with suicidal behavior in women with borderline personality disorder. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2017;11:196. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00196. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Won E, et al. Association between reduced white matter integrity in the corpus callosum and serotonin transporter gene DNA methylation in medication-naive patients with major depressive disorder. Transl. Psychiatry. 2016;6:e866. doi: 10.1038/tp.2016.137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Barysheva M, Jahanshad N, Foland-Ross L, Altshuler LL, Thompson PM. White matter microstructural abnormalities in bipolar disorder: a whole brain diffusion tensor imaging study. Neuroimage Clin. 2013;2:558–568. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2013.03.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Rinne-Albers MA, et al. Abnormalities of white matter integrity in the corpus callosum of adolescents with PTSD after childhood sexual abuse: a DTI study. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2016;25:869–878. doi: 10.1007/s00787-015-0805-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Petrovic P, et al. Significant grey matter changes in a region of the orbitofrontal cortex in healthy participants predicts emotional dysregulation. Soc. Cogn. Affect Neurosci. 2016;11:1041–1049. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsv072. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Leung RC, Pang EW, Anagnostou E, Taylor MJ. Young adults with autism spectrum disorder show early atypical neural activity during emotional face processing. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2018;12:57. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Velasquez F, et al. Neural correlates of emotional inhibitory control in autism spectrum disorders. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2017;64:64–77. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2017.03.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Leech R, Sharp DJ. The role of the posterior cingulate cortex in cognition and disease. Brain. 2014;137:12–32. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt162. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Posner MI, Rothbart MK, Voelker P. Developing brain networks of attention. Curr. Opin. Pediatr. 2016;28:720–724. doi: 10.1097/MOP.0000000000000413. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Bubb EJ, Metzler-Baddeley C, Aggleton JP. The cingulum bundle: anatomy, function, and dysfunction. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2018;92:104–127. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.05.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Pisner DA, Smith R, Alkozei A, Klimova A, Killgore WD. Highways of the emotional intellect: white matter microstructural correlates of an ability-based measure of emotional intelligence. Soc. Neurosci. 2017;12:253–267. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2016.1176600. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Biga LM, Dawson S, Harwell A, Hopkins R, Kaufmann J, LeMaster M, Matern P, Morrison-Graham K, Quick D, Runyeon J, Whittier L. Anatomy & Physiology. Corvallis: Oregon State University; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Kandel ER, Schwartz JH, Jessell TM, Siegelbaum SA, Hudspeth AJ, editors. Principles of Neural Science. 5. New York: McGraw-Hill; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Barbagallo G, et al. Structural connectivity differences in motor network between tremor-dominant and nontremor Parkinson's disease. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2017;38:4716–4729. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23697. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Elias WJ, Zheng ZA, Domer P, Quigg M, Pouratian N. Validation of connectivity-based thalamic segmentation with direct electrophysiologic recordings from human sensory thalamus. Neuroimage. 2012;59:2025–2034. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Jang SH, Seo JP. Differences of the medial lemniscus and spinothalamic tract according to the cortical termination areas: a diffusion tensor tractography study. Somatosens. Mot. Res. 2015;32:67–71. doi: 10.3109/08990220.2014.966899. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Dick AS, Garic D, Graziano P, Tremblay P. The frontal aslant tract (FAT) and its role in speech, language and executive function. Cortex. 2019;111:148–163. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.10.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Bradley MM, Miccoli L, Escrig MA, Lang PJ. The pupil as a measure of emotional arousal and autonomic activation. Psychophysiology. 2008;45:602–607. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00654.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Katsuki F, Constantinidis C. Bottom-up and top-down attention: different processes and overlapping neural systems. Neurosci. Rev. J. Bring. Neurobiol. Neurol. Psychiatry. 2014;20:509–521. doi: 10.1177/1073858413514136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Sussman TJ, Jin J, Mohanty A. Top-down and bottom-up factors in threat-related perception and attention in anxiety. Biol. Psychol. 2016;121:160–172. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Amodio DM, Master SL, Yee CM, Taylor SE. Neurocognitive components of the behavioral inhibition and activation systems: implications for theories of self-regulation. Psychophysiology. 2008;45:11–19. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00609.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Ochsner KN, et al. Bottom-up and top-down processes in emotion generation: common and distinct neural mechanisms. Psychol. Sci. 2009;20:1322–1331. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02459.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Parlatini V, et al. Functional segregation and integration within fronto-parietal networks. Neuroimage. 2017;146:367–375. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.08.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Yeatman JD, et al. The vertical occipital fasciculus: a century of controversy resolved by in vivo measurements. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2014;111:E5214–5223. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1418503111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Ameis SH, Catani M. Altered white matter connectivity as a neural substrate for social impairment in Autism Spectrum Disorder. Cortex. 2015;62:158–181. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.10.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Vasa RA, Mostofsky SH, Ewen JB. The disrupted connectivity hypothesis of autism spectrum disorders: time for the next phase in research. Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging. 2016;1:245–252. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2016.02.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Lai MC, et al. Imaging sex/gender and autism in the brain: etiological implications. J. Neurosci. Res. 2017;95:380–397. doi: 10.1002/jnr.23948. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Smith SM, et al. Tract-based spatial statistics: voxelwise analysis of multi-subject diffusion data. Neuroimage. 2006;31:1487–1505. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Hagmann P, et al. Mapping human whole-brain structural networks with diffusion MRI. PLoS ONE. 2007;2:e597. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0000597. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Beaulieu C. Diffusion MRI. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2009. pp. 105–126. [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Yeh FC, Verstynen TD, Wang Y, Fernandez-Miranda JC, Tseng WY. Deterministic diffusion fiber tracking improved by quantitative anisotropy. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e80713. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0080713. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Jelescu IO, Budde MD. Design and validation of diffusion MRI models of white matter. Front. Phys. 2017 doi: 10.3389/fphy.2017.00061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Yendiki A, Koldewyn K, Kakunoori S, Kanwisher N, Fischl B. Spurious group differences due to head motion in a diffusion MRI study. Neuroimage. 2014;88:79–90. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Jack A, Pelphrey KA. Annual research review: understudied populations within the autism spectrum–current trends and future directions in neuroimaging research. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2017;58:411–435. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12687. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Lai MC, Lombardo MV, Baron-Cohen S. Autism. Lancet. 2014;383:896–910. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(13)61539-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.van Ewijk H, et al. The influence of comorbid oppositional defiant disorder on white matter microstructure in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2016;25:701–710. doi: 10.1007/s00787-015-0784-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Shaw P, Stringaris A, Nigg J, Leibenluft E. Emotion dysregulation in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2014;171:276–293. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.13070966. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Bouziane C, et al. White matter by diffusion MRI following methylphenidate treatment: a randomized control trial in males with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Radiology. 2019;293:186–192. doi: 10.1148/radiol.2019182528. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Mazefsky CA, Yu L, White SW, Siegel M, Pilkonis PA. The emotion dysregulation inventory: psychometric properties and item response theory calibration in an autism spectrum disorder sample. Autism Res. 2018;11:928–941. doi: 10.1002/aur.1947. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Irimia A, Torgerson CM, Jacokes ZJ, Van Horn JD. The connectomes of males and females with autism spectrum disorder have significantly different white matter connectivity densities. Sci. Rep. 2017;7:46401. doi: 10.1038/srep46401. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Lei J, et al. Altered neural connectivity in females, but not males with autism: preliminary evidence for the female protective effect from a quality-controlled diffusion tensor imaging study. Autism Res. 2019;12:1472–1483. doi: 10.1002/aur.2180. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Gau SS, Chong MY, Chen TH, Cheng AT. A 3-year panel study of mental disorders among adolescents in Taiwan. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2005;162:1344–1350. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.7.1344. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Gau SS, Chiang HL. Sleep problems and disorders among adolescents with persistent and subthreshold attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorders. Sleep. 2009;32:671–679. doi: 10.1093/sleep/32.5.671. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Gau SS, et al. Behavioral problems and parenting style among Taiwanese children with autism and their siblings. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2010;64:70–78. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2009.02034.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Achenbach TM. Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4–18 and 1991 profile. Burlington: Department of Psychiatry, University of Vermont; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Spencer TJ, et al. Toward defining deficient emotional self-regulation in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder using the Child Behavior Checklist: a controlled study. Postgrad. Med. 2011;123:50–59. doi: 10.3810/pgm.2011.09.2459. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Wedeen VJ, Hagmann P, Tseng WY, Reese TG, Weisskoff RM. Mapping complex tissue architecture with diffusion spectrum magnetic resonance imaging. Magn. Reson. Med. 2005;54:1377–1386. doi: 10.1002/mrm.20642. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Reese TG, Heid O, Weisskoff RM, Wedeen VJ. Reduction of eddy-current-induced distortion in diffusion MRI using a twice-refocused spin echo. Magn. Reson. Med. 2003;49:177–182. doi: 10.1002/mrm.10308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Kuo LW, Chen JH, Wedeen VJ, Tseng WY. Optimization of diffusion spectrum imaging and q-ball imaging on clinical MRI system. Neuroimage. 2008;41:7–18. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.02.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Dietrich O, Raya JG, Reeder SB, Reiser MF, Schoenberg SO. Measurement of signal-to-noise ratios in MR images: influence of multichannel coils, parallel imaging, and reconstruction filters. J. Magn. Reson. Imaging. 2007;26:375–385. doi: 10.1002/jmri.20969. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Callaghan PT, Coy A, MacGowan D, Packer KJ, Zelaya FO. Diffraction-like effects in NMR diffusion studies of fluids in porous solids. Nature. 1991;351:467–469. doi: 10.1038/351467a0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Yeh FC, Wedeen VJ, Tseng WY. Estimation of fiber orientation and spin density distribution by diffusion deconvolution. Neuroimage. 2011;55:1054–1062. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Fritzsche KH, Laun FB, Meinzer HP, Stieltjes B. Opportunities and pitfalls in the quantification of fiber integrity: what can we gain from Q-ball imaging? Neuroimage. 2010;51:242–251. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.02.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Granziera C, et al. A new early and automated MRI-based predictor of motor improvement after stroke. Neurology. 2012;79:39–46. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31825f25e7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Hsu YC, Lo YC, Chen YJ, Wedeen VJ, Isaac Tseng WY. NTU-DSI-122: a diffusion spectrum imaging template with high anatomical matching to the ICBM-152 space. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2015;36:3528–3541. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22860. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Lin HY, et al. Brain-behavior patterns define a dimensional biotype in medication-naive adults with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychol. Med. 2018;48:2399–2408. doi: 10.1017/S0033291718000028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Perry A, et al. The independent influences of age and education on functional brain networks and cognition in healthy older adults. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2017;38:5094–5114. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23717. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Smith SM, Hyvarinen A, Varoquaux G, Miller KL, Beckmann CF. Group-PCA for very large fMRI datasets. NeuroImage. 2014;101:738–749. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.07.051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Abdi Hervé WLJ. Principal component analysis. WIREs Comput. Stat. 2010;2:433–459. doi: 10.1002/wics.101. [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary file1. (5.4MB, docx)

Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES