Abstract
Magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS) can be used in vivo to quantify neurometabolite concentration and provide evidence for the involvement of different neurotransmitter systems (e.g., inhibitory and excitatory) in sensory and cognitive processes. The relatively low signal-to-noise ratio of MRS measurements has shaped the types of questions that it has been used to address. In particular, temporal resolution is often sacrificed in MRS studies to achieve a signal sufficient to produce a reliable estimate of neurometabolite concentration. Here we apply novel analyses with large datasets from human participants (both sexes) to reveal the dynamics of GABA+ and Glx in visual cortex while participants are at rest (with eyes closed) and compare this with changes in posterior cingulate cortex from a previously collected dataset (under different conditions). We find that the dynamic concentration of GABA+ and Glx in visual cortex drifts in opposite directions; that is, GABA+ decreases while Glx increases over time. Further, we find that in visual, but not posterior cingulate cortex, the concentration of GABA+ predicts that of Glx 120 s later, such that a change in GABA+ is correlated with a subsequent opposite change in Glx. Together, these results expose novel temporal trends and interdependencies of primary neurotransmitters in visual cortex. More broadly, we demonstrate the feasibility of using MRS to investigate in vivo dynamic changes of neurometabolites.
Keywords: GABA glutamate, Glx, magnetic resonance spectroscopy, temporal dynamics, visual cortex
Significance Statement
Using large datasets of magnetic resonance spectroscopy acquisitions from human participants, we develop novel analyses to investigate the temporal dynamics of neurometabolite concentration in visual cortex. We find that, while participants are at rest, the concentration of GABA+ and Glx drifts in opposite directions; that is, GABA+ decreases while Glx increases over time. Further, we find that the concentration of GABA+ predicts that of Glx 120 s later, such that a change in GABA+ is correlated with a subsequent opposite change in Glx. We show that these phenomena are regionally localized to visual cortex.
Introduction
Magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS) can be used in vivo to measure the concentration of neurometabolites within the brain. The blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) signal measured using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) can provide evidence of neural activity; however, MRS can provide evidence that can be used to distinguish between different types of activity (e.g., excitatory and inhibitory). For the purpose of understanding neural mechanisms, identifying the involvement of neurotransmitter systems that support sensory/cognitive processes can be more informative than locating regions of neural representation.
Previous work using MRS to understand the role of different neurotransmitters in normal brain function has focused on neurometabolite activity in visual cortex; in particular, the function of γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA) and glutamate (Glu), the primary inhibitory and excitatory neurotransmitters in the central nervous system, respectively. A consistent finding from these studies is that Glu increases in visual cortex in response to visual stimulation, which has been interpreted as increased involvement of this neurotransmitter during the processing of visual stimuli (Mangia et al., 2007; Lin et al., 2012; Schaller et al., 2013; Bednařík et al., 2015, 2018; Ip et al., 2017; Kurcyus et al., 2018). The findings relating to GABA have been less consistent. One study found less GABA in visual cortex during visual stimulation compared with baseline (Mekle et al., 2017), while other studies have not replicated this result (Mangia et al., 2007; Schaller et al., 2013; Bednařík et al., 2015, 2018; Kurcyus et al., 2018).
The classic “static” fMRS approach (i.e., comparing the average neurometabolite concentration during one experimental condition with another) has implicated GABA and Glu in visual processing. However, the information provided using this approach is severely limited. In particular, by reducing the measure of neurometabolite concentration to a single estimate averaged across a viewing condition, dynamic changes in concentration that occur under different states of visual processing are obscured. By contrast, establishing the temporal dynamics in neurometabolite concentration will reveal the timing and magnitude of change in different neurotransmitters and novel relationships between neurotransmitters. This information is essential for a comprehensive understanding of the role of neurotransmitters in visual processing, but, more broadly, it may inform our understanding of the balance between excitation and inhibition, which is thought to be a key factor in multiple neurologic and psychiatric illnesses (Bradford, 1995; Rubenstein and Merzenich, 2003; Kehrer et al., 2008).
The temporal resolution of MRS is highly restrained by the signal-to-noise ratio of measurements acquired using the technique. Although the duration that restricts the temporal resolution of MRS (i.e., the relaxation time) can be similar to that of fMRI (i.e., ∼2 s), to yield a reliable measurement of neurometabolite concentration from within the brain, multiple transients must be combined to reach a sufficient signal-to-noise ratio (Mikkelsen et al., 2018). For example, it is common to combine between 200 and 300 transients (∼10 min) to produce a single measure of neurometabolite concentration (Kurcyus et al., 2018; Rideaux and Welchman, 2018). Here we overcome the signal-to-noise limitation of MRS by applying temporal analyses of neurometabolite concentration to a large dataset of participants. We measure the dynamic concentration of GABA and Glx in visual cortex of participants while at rest (with closed eyes). We assess the regional specificity of temporal dynamics by comparing these results with data from posterior cingulate cortex (Mikkelsen et al., 2017, 2019). To observe the temporal dynamics of neurometabolites, we analyze the data in two ways. We first take a moving average of an ∼6 min period, to reveal low-frequency trends in the data (Chen et al., 2017; Rideaux et al., 2019). Next, using a new technique, we combine data across participants (rather than time), which allows us to track the concentration of neurometabolites with relatively high temporal resolution (12 s) over a 13 min period.
Based on previous empirical evidence, we may expect Glx to decrease and GABA to increase under conditions of no visual stimulation. By contrast, our analyses reveal the opposite pattern of results: Glx increases while GABA decreases (in visual, but not posterior cingulate cortex). These results are broadly consistent with findings from visual deprivation studies (Boroojerdi et al., 2000; Lunghi et al., 2015) and may provide a link between the conflicting results from MRS studies that use relatively short periods of visual deprivation compared with those with longer periods. Further, we expose large changes in GABA and Glx, previously obscured by averaging over long durations, and reveal a striking relationship between GABA and Glx in visual cortex: a change in GABA predicts the opposite change in Glx ∼120 s later.
Materials and Methods
Participants
Fifty-eight healthy participants with normal or corrected-to-normal vision participated in the experiment. The mean age was 24.4 years (age range, 19.4–40.5 years; 31 women). Participants were screened for contraindications to MRI before the experiment. All experiments were conducted in accordance with the ethical guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and were approved by the university ethics committee, and all participants provided informed consent.
Data collection
Participants underwent an MR spectroscopic acquisition targeting visual cortex. During the acquisition, the lights in the room were turned off and participants were instructed to close their eyes. To compare these data with those measured from another brain region, we reanalyzed previously gathered MR spectroscopic data targeting posterior cingulate cortex (Mikkelsen et al., 2017, 2019).
Data acquisition
Magnetic resonance scanning targeting visual cortex was conducted on a 3 T Siemens Prisma equipped with a 32-channel head coil. Anatomical T1-weighted images were acquired for spectroscopic voxel placement with an MP-RAGE sequence. For the detection of GABA+ and Glx, spectra were acquired using a MEGA-PRESS sequence (Mescher et al., 1996, 1998): TE = 68 ms; TR = 3000 ms; 256 transients of 2048 data points were acquired in 13 min experiment time; a 14.28 ms Gaussian editing pulse was applied at 1.9 ppm (ON) and 7.5 ppm (OFF). Water suppression was achieved using variable power with optimized relaxation delays (Tkáč and Gruetter, 2005) and outer volume suppression. Automated shimming followed by manual shimming was conducted to achieve ∼12 Hz water linewidth.
The “Big GABA” dataset comprises a collection of MRS datasets collected by different groups using the same parameters on GE, Phillips, and Siemens scanners. This dataset was acquired separately for a previous study and targeted posterior cingulate cortex; thus, here it was reused as a region specificity control for the main visual cortex data. A detailed description of the data acquisitions for the Big GABA dataset can be found in studies by Mikkelsen et al. (2017, 2019). To summarize the procedure, magnetic resonance scanning targeting posterior cingulate cortex was conducted on 3 T Siemens, GE, and Phillips scanners equipped with 8-, 32-, or 64-channel head coils. Spectra were acquired using a MEGA-PRESS sequence: TE = 68 ms; TR = 2000 ms; 320 transients of either 2048 or 4096 data points were acquired in a 12 min experiment time; a 15 ms Gaussian editing pulse was applied at 1.9 ppm (ON) and 7.5 ppm (OFF). Note that, unlike the data collected in visual cortex, data collected in posterior cingulate cortex was not water suppressed, which can negatively impact the accurate quantification of other metabolites (e.g., sideband artifacts; for a review of issues relating to water suppression, see Dong, 2015). The Big GABA dataset comprises subdatasets collected by different research groups at different facilities. The following subdatasets from the Big GABA dataset were used in the current study: G4, G5, G7, G8, P1, P3, P4, P5, P6, P7, P8, P9, P10, S1, S6, and S8 (the letters in the subdatasets refer to the scanner make and the numbers refer to the group that collected the data). Each subdataset (e.g., G4) comprises data from between 8 and 12 participants; in total there were data from 196 participants. With the exception of G1 and G6, this includes all of the publicly available Big GABA datasets. We used the macromolecule unsuppressed transients from these datasets for closer comparison with visual cortex data, and we excluded data from G1 and G6 as these had fewer transients.
Spectra were acquired from a location targeting visual cortices (i.e., V1/V2; Fig. 1a), and posterior cingulate cortex (Fig. 1b); note that the posterior cingulate cortex data were acquired from the (previously described) publicly available Big GABA dataset. The voxel targeting visual cortex (3 × 3 × 2 cm) was placed medially in the occipital lobe; the lower face aligned with the cerebellar tentorium and was positioned so as to avoid including the sagittal sinus and to ensure that it remained within the occipital lobe. The voxel targeting posterior cingulate cortex (3 × 3 × 3 cm) was positioned in the medial parietal lobe and rotated in the sagittal plane to align with a line connecting the genu and splenium of the corpus callosum. The coordinates of the voxel location were used to draw a mask on the anatomic T1-weighted image to calculate the volume of gray matter, white matter, and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) within each voxel. Segmentation was performed using the Statistical Parametric Mapping toolbox for MATLAB (SPM12; http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/).
Data processing
Spectral quantification was conducted in MATLAB using GANNET version 3.1 (Edden et al., 2014) and in-house scripts. Frequency, phase, area, and full-width at half-maximum (FWHM) parameters of the creatine (Cr) peak at 3.0 ppm were estimated by fitting a Lorentzian peak to the data and individual spectra with parameter estimates >3 SDs from the mean were omitted from further analysis; the remaining spectra were frequency and phase corrected using these parameters. To ensure spectral alignment between transients across time and participants, spectra were aligned such that the Cr peak was centered at the same frequency (3.0 ppm). Total creatine (tCr) and total N-acetylaspartate (tNAA) signal intensity were determined by fitting a single Lorentzian peak to the mean OFF spectra at 3.0 and 2.0 ppm, respectively. ON and OFF spectra were subtracted to produce the edited spectrum (Fig. 1c,d), from which GABA+ (GABA and coedited macromolecules; 3 ppm) and Glx [a complex comprising Glu and glutamine (Gln); 3.8 ppm] signal intensity were modeled on single- and double-Gaussian peaks, respectively. All neurometabolite signal intensities were calculated as the area of the fitted peak or peaks.
Data in which the FWHM of any of the quantified neurometabolites (tCr, tNAA, GABA+, or Glx) was >3 SDs from the mean across each dataset (e.g., visual/posterior cingulate cortex) were omitted from further analysis. This resulted in omission of data from one participant in the visual cortex dataset and three participants in the posterior cingulate cortex dataset.
Intensities of GABA+, Glx, and tNAA were normalized to the commonly used internal reference tCr (Jansen et al., 2006), yielding relative concentration values (i.e., GABA+:tCr, Glx:tCr, and tNAA:tCr; Fig. 1e). The tCr signal is acquired within the same MEGA-PRESS transients as the target neurometabolites. Thus, normalization of GABA+, Glx, and tNAA to tCr minimizes the influence of changes that occur during the acquisition, which alter the entire spectrum (e.g., changes in signal strength, line width, chemical shift displacement, or dilution associated with changes in blood flow; Ip et al., 2017). For correlational analyses reported in Table 2, a CSF tissue correction (Harris et al., 2015) was applied to the neurometabolite measurements with the following equation:
(1) |
where and are the tissue-corrected and uncorrected neurometabolite concentrations (e.g., GABA+:tCr), respectively, and is the proportion of CSF within the voxel. All other analyses report concentrations as a proportion of their initial magnitude, and thus do not require tissue correction.
Table 2.
Location | GABA+and Glx | GABA+and tNAA | Glxand tNAA | ΔGABA+and ΔGlx | ΔGABA+and ΔtNAA | ΔGlxand ΔtNAA |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
VC | −0.16 | 0.33* | 0.33* | 0.06 | 0.10 | −0.17 |
PCC | 0.25*** | 0.50*** | 0.17* | 0.03 | −0.18* | 0.04 |
Correlation coefficients are Pearson linear partial coefficients after controlling for the common reference neurometabolite tCr. All neurometabolites are referenced to tCr and are tissue corrected. PCC, Posterior cingulate cortex; VC, visual cortex.
*p < 0.05; ***p < 0.001.
Low-resolution dynamic analysis
For the low-resolution dynamic analysis of the visual cortex data, we used a sliding window (width, 128 transients; step size, 2 transients) to measure average neurometabolite concentration as it changed over the course of the scan (256 transients/768 s). This window size was based on previous work using this analysis on similar data (Rideaux et al., 2019). For the posterior cingulate cortex data (320 transients/640 s), we matched the duration of the sliding window width to that used for the visual cortex data by including more transients (width, 192 transients; step size, 2 transients).
To determine whether the average change in neurometabolite concentration was significantly different from zero, a two-sided t test (α, 0.05) was performed at each time point. That is, for each brain region, each t test included a single estimate from each participant, which corresponded to the participant’s change in neurometabolite concentration at that time. As multiple tests (65) were conducted for each neurometabolite, to reduce the likelihood of spurious significant differences in the time course, a cluster correction was applied at the group level, where time was the clustered dimension. Clusters were defined by the sum of their constituent t values and compared with a null hypothesis distribution of clusters produced by shuffling the time labels (5000 permutations); positive and negative t value clusters were treated separately. Clusters below the 95th percentile of the null hypothesis distribution were disregarded.
High-resolution dynamic analysis
The temporal resolution of MRS is severely limited by the signal-to-noise ratio of individual spectra. That is, to achieve the signal-to-noise ratio required to yield an accurate neurometabolite measurement, many individual spectra must be combined. To achieve a sufficiently high signal in the low-resolution dynamic analysis, we combined multiple spectra (128) within the same subject. This method produces a dynamic trace for each participant; however, the smoothing produced by the sliding window approach may obscure both the true magnitude of metabolic change over time and dynamic changes occurring at higher frequencies. Thus, to achieve higher temporal resolution, we averaged individual ON and OFF spectra across participants to produce a single trace, with no smoothing, for each condition. As in the low-temporal resolution analysis, we matched the temporal resolution between datasets in the high-temporal resolution analysis (12 s) by using a resolution of four and six transients in the visual and posterior cingulate cortices, respectively.
To test for predictive relationships between GABA+ and Glx, we ran a cross-correlation analysis between the above-mentioned high-resolution neurometabolite traces. We tested for relationships in both directions; that is, whether GABA+ concentration predicts Glx concentration and vice versa. The neurometabolite traces comprise a limited number of time points (visual cortex, 64; posterior cingulate cortex, 53), and there is an inverse relationship between the lag separating the neurometabolites and the number of time points included in the cross-correlation analysis. This results in less reliable correlation values at lags close to the maximum duration of the neurometabolite traces due to insufficient sample sizes. To avoid these unreliable correlations, we only included lags with a minimum of 25 time points in the analysis (Bonett and Wright, 2000). This yielded a total of 40 correlations for each predictive direction between GABA+ and Glx in visual cortex and 29 for posterior cingulate cortex. Given that multiple tests were conducted, to reduce the likelihood of spurious significant correlation values in the cross-correlation analyses, a cluster correction was applied. Clusters were defined by the sum of their constituent t values and compared with a null hypothesis distribution of clusters produced by shuffling the time labels (5000 permutations); positive and negative t value clusters were treated separately. Clusters below the 95th percentile of the null hypothesis distribution were disregarded.
Significance testing
The significance of differences between data from different voxel locations was assessed using the independent-samples t test, and the significance of changes in neurometabolite concentration (from zero change) was assessed using the one-sample t test; all tests were two sided and used an α = 0.05. The normality assumption was tested with the Shapiro–Wilk test of normality. For data in which the assumption of normality was violated, significance was assessed using the Wilcoxon rank-sum test. The significance of correlations between neurometabolite concentrations was assessed using the Pearson linear correlation and the Pearson linear partial correlation; all tests used an α = 0.05.
Results
Spectra quality
Table 1 shows the average FWHM, frequency drift and fit error for measurements taken from visual and posterior cingulate cortices. For each subject, the FWHM was calculated from the average spectra [i.e., spectra averaged across all transients (256 of 320)]. Frequency drift was calculated as the SD of the position of the Cr peak across individual OFF spectra, before alignment; the frequency drift values shown in Table 1 reflect the average SD across participants. The fit errors for GABA+, Glx, tNAA, and tCr were divided by the amplitude of their fitted peaks to produce normalized measures of uncertainty. The average fit error for each neurometabolite was relatively low (Mullins et al., 2014; Table 1). Comparison between data from visual and posterior cingulate cortices revealed higher FWHM estimates for all quantified neurometabolites in visual cortex (GABA+: t(248) = 16.80, p = 8.4 × 10−43; Glx: z = 9.24, p = 9.4 × 10−20; tNAA: z = 2.06, p = 0.040; tCr: z = 7.07, p = 1.5 × 10−12). This suggests that shimming may have been more effective in the posterior cingulate dataset; however, differences in voxel size, voxel tissue composition, and the number of transients collected may have contributed to this difference. We also found that frequency drift (z = 5.16, p = 2.5 × 10−7), and fit errors for GABA+ (z = 2.59, p = 0.010) and tNAA (z = 4.89, p = 1.0 × 10−6) were higher in the visual cortex dataset. By comparison, fit error was lower in visual cortex for Glx (z = −5.15, p = 2.6 × 10−7) and tCr (z = −10.92, p = 9.3 × 10−28).
Table 1.
Location | FWHM (Hz) | Frequency drift (ppm SD) | Fit error (%) | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
GABA+ | Glx | tNAA | tCr | GABA+ | Glx | tNAA | tCr | ||
VC | 24.9 ± 1.7 | 17.8 ± 0.4 | 8.4 ± 0.9 | 8.5 ± 0.7 | 0.0088 ± 0.0044 | 7.6 ± 2.1 | 1.3 ± 0.3 | 3.3 ± 0.3 | 3.6 ± 0.2 |
PCC | 20.5 ± 1.7 | 16.8 ± 0.7 | 8.2 ± 0.8 | 7.6 ± 0.7 | 0.0065 ± 0.0061 | 6.9 ± 1.7 | 2.1 ± 1.5 | 3.1 ± 1.3 | 4.5 ± 0.5 |
Values indicate across-subject averages ± SD. PCC, Posterior cingulate cortex; VC, visual cortex.
Static analysis
Figure 2 shows the distribution of gray matter, white matter, and CSF voxel composition across participants for the visual and posterior cingulate cortex datasets. We found that visual cortex voxels were composed of less gray matter (t(248) = 7.53, p = 9.3 × 10−13) and more CSF (z = −6.27, p = 3.6 × 10−10) than those from posterior cingulate cortex; no significant difference was found between white matter (t(248) = 0.27, p = 0.787). We quantified the concentrations of GABA+, Glx, and tNAA using the classic static approach, in which all transients are averaged together to extract a single estimate. After applying a CSF tissue correction, we compared the average neurometabolite concentration across participants between visual and posterior cingulate cortices. We found lower concentrations of GABA+:tCr (t(248) = −6.53, p = 3.7 × 10−10) and Glx:tCr (z = −9.33, p = 1.0 × 10−20) in visual cortex, but a higher concentration of tNAA:tCr (t(248) = 4.62, p = 6.1 × 10−6; Fig. 2).
Low-resolution temporal dynamics of GABA and Glx
Using a sliding temporal window analysis, we quantified the change in the concentration of GABA+ and Glx measured from MRS voxels targeting visual and posterior cingulate cortices over the course of 13 and 12 min periods, respectively. We found that in visual cortex, GABA+ significantly decreased (maximum difference = −5.0%, t(56) = −3.25, p = 0.002), while Glx significantly increased (maximum difference = 2.7%, t(56) = 3.74, p = 4.4 × 10−4) over the course of the period (Fig. 3a). By comparison, we found that in posterior cingulate cortex there was no significant change in either GABA or Glx (Fig. 3b). The FWHM of the neurometabolite peaks indicates that spectra from posterior cingulate cortex had better signal quality; further, this dataset contained more than three times as many participants as the visual cortex dataset. Thus, it seems unlikely that the neurometabolite drift we observed in visual cortex was also present in posterior cingulate cortex, but that we failed to detect it because of poor signal quality or lack of statistical power. A more parsimonious explanation is that the neurometabolite drift in visual cortex was regionally specific. Given that we referenced GABA+ and Glx to tCr, a possible concern is that the changes in neurometabolite concentration observed in visual cortex reflect changes in tCr, as opposed to GABA+ or Glx. However, this is unlikely as we did not find any change in tNAA concentration, for either visual or posterior cingulate cortices, which was also referenced for tCr (Fig. 2a).
We next assessed whether the fit error of the GABA+, Glx, tNAA, and tCr peaks changed over the course of the acquisition. For visual cortex, there were no significant changes in the fit error of the tNAA or tCr peaks. However, there were significant increases in fit error for GABA+ between 360 and 394 s, and for Glx between 564 and 576 s. A possible concern is that the changes we observed in the concentrations of GABA+ and Glx in visual cortex were due to reduced neurometabolite quantification accuracy (i.e., increased fit error). To test this possibility, we assessed the interindividual relationship between change in fit error and change in neurometabolite concentration, for GABA+ and Glx in visual cortex. We found no evidence for a relationship between fit error and concentration for either neurometabolite (GABA+: n = 57, Pearson r = −0.14, p = 0.308; Glx: n = 57, Pearson r = 0.06, p = 0.676), indicating that change in quantification accuracy does not explain the neurometabolite drift we observed in visual cortex. For posterior cingulate cortex, we found no dynamic change in fit error for any of the neurometabolites, with the exception of the GABA+ peak, where there was a window between 196 and 208 s in which the fit error was significantly reduced.
Another possible concern is that changes observed in the difference spectra over time are related to scanner field drift due to gradient cooling (Lange et al., 2011) or participant motion (Bhattacharyya et al., 2007). In particular, if the scanner field drifts, the position of the editing pulse relative to the GABA and Glx peaks changes. This may change the efficiency with which the peaks are edited and thus their magnitude in the difference spectrum. As the frequency drift of the visual cortex spectra was higher than that in posterior cingulate cortex, where we did not observe any change in neurometabolite concentration, this may account for the changes in neurometabolite concentration observed in visual cortex. As a test of this possibility, we measured the negative tNAA signal in the difference spectra using an inverse Lorentzian. Like the magnitude of the GABA+ and Glx peaks, the magnitude of the negative tNAA signal reflects the efficiency of the editing pulse. Thus, if scanner drift is responsible for changes in the magnitude of the GABA+ or Glx peaks, we would expect to see corresponding changes in the amplitude of the edited negative tNAA signal. However, we found no evidence for change in the amplitude of the edited negative tNAA signal over time. As a further test of this possibility, we assessed whether there was an interindividual relationship between the degree of frequency drift and change in either GABA+ or Glx. If scanner drift produced the change in neurometabolite concentration we observed in visual cortex, we would expect these measures to be positively correlated. By contrast, we found no relationship between frequency drift and either change in GABA+ (n = 57, Pearson r = −0.10, p = 0.457) or Glx (n = 57, Pearson r = 0.06, p = 0.662). These results indicate that scanner drift did not contribute to the changes in neurometabolite concentration.
Correlational analyses of static and low-temporal resolution measurements
For each voxel location, we assessed whether there were interindividual relationships between different static neurometabolite concentrations or changes in concentration. For static measurements, we averaged across all 256 of 320 transients. The results of the analysis are shown in Table 2. For static neurometabolite concentrations, we found a positive relationship between Glx and tNAA measured from visual and posterior cingulate cortices. We also found a positive correlation between GABA+ and tNAA in visual cortex, and between GABA+ and Glx in posterior cingulate cortex. By contrast, the only relationship between changes in the metabolite concentration found was for GABA+ and tNAA in posterior cingulate cortex.
High-resolution temporal dynamics of GABA and Glx
To assess high-temporal resolution dynamics of neurometabolites, we next combined transients across subjects, rather than across time. In visual cortex, we found that the change in GABA+, relative from the first measurement, was primarily negative and ranged from ±15% from the average concentration, while the change in Glx was primarily positive and ranged from ±10% (Fig. 4a). These results are consistent with those from the previous analysis, except the higher temporal resolution obtained with the current approach revealed considerably larger changes in neurometabolite concentration than the previous estimates, which were likely obscured by smoothing measurements across the temporal window. In posterior cingulate cortex, we found that the changes in both GABA+ and Glx were primarily negative and similar in amplitude (Fig. 4b).
While GABA and Glu are thought to support opposing mechanisms in the CNS (i.e., inhibition and excitation), it seems reasonable to expect interactions between GABA and Glx, which comprises both Glu and Gln. For example, Gln is a primary source of GABA synthesis (Paulsen et al., 1988; Patel et al., 2001; Rae et al., 2003). Indeed, we found that the static concentration of GABA+ and Glx were positively related in posterior cingulate cortex. However, we found no evidence for a relationship between the overall changes in these neurometabolites in either the visual or posterior cingulate cortex (Table 2). One reason for this may be that the relationship between these neurometabolites may only be observed at a high temporal resolution, but not averaged across a 6 min period. To test this hypothesis, we used the high-resolution neurometabolite measurements to perform cross-correlation analyses on GABA+ and Glx concentrations.
For visual cortex, we found that the concentration of GABA+ predicted that of Glx between 108 and 132 s later [n(time points) = (55, 54, 53), Pearson r = (−0.36, −0.31, −0.39), p = (0.008, 0.024, 0.004); Fig. 4c]. This relationship was negative, that is, a positive/negative change in GABA+ predicted a later change in Glx in the opposite direction. By contrast, we found no periods of latency in which Glx predicted the concentration of GABA+. For posterior cingulate cortex, we found no periods of latency in which there was a significant relationship between GABA+ and Glx (Fig. 4d).
A possible concern is that the relationship between GABA+ and Glx was influenced by their common reference neurometabolite (tCr). In particular, as both GABA+ and Glx were referenced to tCr, the relationship between them could be explained by an autocorrelation in the concentration of tCr. However, we found the same pattern of results when tNAA, rather than tCr, was used as a reference neurometabolite (Fig. 5a). Another possible concern with this new analysis is that the signal-to-noise ratio is insufficient to yield valid measurements (e.g., due to reduced neurometabolite quantification accuracy). To assess the validity of the measurements, we compared the average FWHM and fit error of peaks produced in the high-resolution analysis with those produced in the low-resolution analysis. For visual cortex, we found no evidence for a difference in FWHM for any of the target neurometabolites (GABA+: z = −0.06, p = 0.948; Glx: t(119) = −5.08, p = 1.4 × 10−6; tCr: z = −1.10, p = 0.270). By comparison, fit error was lower for all target neurometabolites (GABA+: z = 2.38, p = 0.017; Glx: z = 4.21, p = 2.5 × 10−5; tCr: t(119) = 2.24, p = 0.027) modeled in the high-resolution analysis. Similarly, for posterior cingulate cortex, we found no evidence for a difference in either FWHM (GABA+: z = 1.62, p = 0.105; Glx: t(119) = 1.76, p = 0.080; tCr: z = 1.71, p = 0.087); however, while we found no evidence for a difference in fit error for GABA (z = 1.32, p = 0.187) and Glx (z = −0.55, p = 0.581), fit error was higher for tCr (z = −2.34, p = 0.019) in the high-resolution analysis. With the exception of the fit error of the tCr peak in posterior cingulate cortex, these results support the validity of the signal strength and quantification accuracy in the high-resolution analysis.
To further assess the validity of the measurements, we attempted to reproduce the results from the low-resolution analysis by applying a sliding window to the high-resolution neurometabolite trace. If the high-resolution measurements are valid, we would expect to find correspondence between the average results from the low-resolution analysis and those produced by applying a sliding window to the high-resolution measurements. For visual cortex, we found a high correspondence between the measurements produced by the two analyses for both GABA+ (n = 33, Pearson r = 0.995, p = 3.9 × 10−33; Fig. 5b, top) and Glx (n = 33, Pearson r = 0.995, p = 5.5 × 10−32). These results further validate the results of the high-resolution analysis in visual cortex. For posterior cingulate cortex, we found a correspondence between GABA+ measurements (n = 22, Pearson r = 0.655, p = 9.3 × 10−4; Fig. 5b, bottom), but not Glx measurements (n = 22, Pearson r = 0.182, p = 0.418). The weaker correspondence for data from posterior cingulate cortex may indicate that the measurements produced by the high-resolution analysis in this region are less reliable. However, it is also likely because there was less variability in neurometabolite concentration in the low-resolution analysis of posterior cingulate cortex; thus, unlike in visual cortex, there may be insufficient variability to cross-validate between the two measurements.
Discussion
MRS can be used in vivo to quantify neurometabolite concentration and provide evidence for the involvement of different neurotransmitter systems (e.g., inhibitory and excitatory) in sensory and cognitive processes. In MRS studies, temporal resolution is typically sacrificed to achieve a signal-to-noise ratio sufficient to produce a reliable estimate of neurometabolite concentration. Here we use novel analyses with large datasets to reveal the dynamics of GABA+ and Glx in visual and posterior cingulate cortices. We use a sliding window approach to show that when participants are at rest with their eyes closed, the concentration of GABA+ and Glx in visual cortex drifts in opposite directions; that is, GABA+ decreases while Glx increases over time. We then use a new method of combining MRS measurements across subjects, as opposed to time, to produce a high-temporal resolution index of neurometabolite concentration. Using this approach, we find that in visual cortex a change in the concentration of GABA+ predicts the opposite change in Glx ∼120 s later (e.g., an increase in GABA+ predicts a later reduction in Glx).
Dynamic response of GABA and Glx in visual cortex
Several studies have investigated GABA and/or Glx/Glu concentration in visual cortex in response to different viewing conditions. Mekle et al. (2017) found an ∼5% reduction in GABA concentration in response to visual stimulation. By contrast, while Kurcyus et al. (2018) reported that GABA was 16% lower when participants had their eyes open with no visual stimulation compared with when they had their eyes closed, they, like others (Mangia et al., 2007; Schaller et al., 2013; Bednařík et al., 2015, 2018), found no evidence for a difference in GABA in response to visual stimulation. Based on these somewhat inconsistent findings, one may infer that visual stimulation, or merely having the eyes open, leads to a reduction in the concentration of GABA in visual cortex. Extending this rationale, one could predict that closing the eyes should produce an increase in GABA. By contrast, we found the opposite result: during a 13 min period of resting in which participants’ eyes were closed, the concentration of GABA+ in visual cortex reduced on average by 5%. These results are consistent with previous work showing that monocular deprivation leads to reduced GABA concentration (∼8%) in visual cortex, but not posterior cingulate cortex, relative to a predeprivation baseline measurement (Lunghi et al., 2015). Thus, our finding that GABA is reduced when both eyes are closed may indicate that visual deprivation, either monocular or binocular, evokes a reduction in the concentration of GABA in visual cortex.
Previous observations of Glu concentration in visual cortex have been more consistent; several studies have shown Glx/Glu concentration increases (2–4%) in response to visual stimulation (Mangia et al., 2007; Lin et al., 2012; Schaller et al., 2013; Bednařík et al., 2015, 2018; Ip et al., 2017; Kurcyus et al., 2018). By contrast, here we found that Glx increased when participants’ eyes were closed. Increased Glu in visual cortex, evoked by visual stimulation, has been linked to increased blood oxygenation level-dependent responses (Ip et al., 2017). Here we measured Glx, a complex comprising Glu and Gln, and previous 7 T MRS work suggests that visual stimulation evoked changes in Glu, but not Gln, in visual cortex (Schaller et al., 2013; Bednařík et al., 2015, 2018). It is possible that the increase in Glx we found here, which occurred in the absence of visual stimulation, was driven by an alternative mechanism, one that is unrelated to BOLD activity and/or reflects changes in Gln rather than Glu. More work is needed to disambiguate changes in neurometabolite concentration that occur in visual cortex at different time scales and under different viewing conditions. For instance, future work could combine fMRI and MRS measurements to test whether the phenomenon observed here is related to BOLD activity (Ip et al., 2017). Additionally, future work could use a within-subject design to compare different voxel locations, and, separately, to assess Glu and Gln concentration drift.
Although the average concentrations of GABA+ and Glx drift in opposing directions, there were some participants who showed the opposite change in neurometabolite drift. One possible explanation for this is that the concentrations of these neurometabolites oscillate at a relatively slow frequency (e.g., wavelength = 15 min), and entering a state of rest (with eyes closed) introduces the linear trend of neurometabolite concentration change indicated by the average, which is summed with the larger oscillatory changes. If such oscillatory behavior was occurring, it would be unlikely to be detected in the high-resolution analysis, as troughs/peaks would be obscured by phase differences between participants. Another possible explanation for the variability between participants is that it relates to their state of wakefulness during the scan. Although subjects reported not falling asleep, it seems likely that some may have been more alert than others during the scan, and this may be related to the concentration drift observed in visual cortex. We do not have the data to examine this possibility; however, future work could concurrently collect metabolite concentration and a physiological marker of wakefulness (e.g., EEG) to assess this possibility.
Continuous unidirectional neurometabolite drift is not sustainable (i.e., neurometabolite concentration must maintain some degree of homeostasis). Thus, it seems likely that the change in concentration induced by resting only continues for a fixed period of time before stabilizing and possibly returning to “baseline” levels. Indeed, this appeared to occur for Glx, where there is no additional increase after ∼500 s, and the decrease in GABA+ appears to lessen after 400 s. Future work is needed to further understand dynamic neurometabolite activity over longer periods of time.
To produce a high-temporal resolution measure of neurometabolite concentrations, we applied a novel approach in which we combined MRS transients across subjects, rather than time. This approach yields a single measurement of neurometabolite concentration as a function of time; thus, we cannot test the statistical significance of the changes observed. However, it is reasonable to assume that the changes estimated using sliding window or static approaches underestimate the true magnitude of change, due to averaging. The measurements produced here by combining transients across subjects provide an indication of the true magnitude of change in GABA+ and Glx during the scan: up to 30% for GABA+ and 20% for Glx.
Dynamic relationship between GABA and Glx
A common finding among studies of GABA and Glu in visual cortex is that these neurometabolites change in opposite directions, and not in the same direction (Mekle et al., 2017; Kurcyus et al., 2018; Rideaux et al., 2019). This pattern would suggest an interdependence between the two neurometabolites. Given that Gln is a primary source of GABA synthesis (Paulsen et al., 1988; Patel et al., 2001; Rae et al., 2003), one may expect that a change in the concentration of GABA may result in a corresponding change in Glx, or vice versa. In line with this, our high-temporal resolution analysis of the data revealed a striking cross-correlation between these neurometabolites in visual cortex. Specifically, we found that the concentration of GABA+ predicts the concentration of Glx ∼120 s later. This relationship, which accounts for up to ∼20% of the variance of Glx, is obscured by conventional approaches of MRS analysis; indeed, we found no evidence for a relationship between the overall changes in GABA+ and Glx.
A limitation of MRS is that it measures the total concentration of neurometabolites within a localized region and cannot distinguish between intracellular and extracellular pools of GABA. It is generally thought that intracellular vesicular GABA drives neurotransmission (Belelli et al., 2009), whereas extracellular GABA maintains tonic cortical inhibition (Martin and Rimvall, 1993). Synthesis of intracellular GABA from Gln via the GABA–Gln cycle occurs on the scale of milliseconds, so it seems unlikely that this metabolic association could explain the predictive relationship between GABA and Glx concentration 120 s later. Instead, this relationship may reflect changes in the level of extracellular GABA, followed by relatively sluggish changes in the concentration of Glu to maintain the balance of inhibition and excitation.
Clinical relevance
An imbalance between excitation and inhibition, in particular between Glu and GABA, is thought to underlie a range of neurologic and psychiatric disorders including epilepsy (Bradford, 1995; Olsen and Avoli, 1997), autism (Rubenstein and Merzenich, 2003; Chao et al., 2010; Markram and Markram, 2010; Vattikuti and Chow, 2010), and schizophrenia (Kehrer et al., 2008). Here we demonstrated that it is possible to use MRS to measure changes in neurometabolite concentration at relatively high temporal resolution, and, in the process of doing so, we revealed a novel predictive relationship between GABA+ and Glx in visual cortex of healthy human participants. More broadly, this method could be used to reveal other trends and relationships between neurometabolites that have thus far been obscured by the classic static measurement approach. Uncovering the dynamics of the excitation–inhibition balance in this way may be instrumental in understanding unhealthy brain function. However, a caveat to this approach is that it requires a relatively large amount of MRS data. Here, we used a large number of subjects to meet this requirement; however, this may not be appropriate for clinical studies in which suitable subjects are more difficult to recruit. In this case, a modified event-related version of the method used here may be effective (Branzoli et al., 2013). That is, events (e.g., a 2 min stimulus presentation) could be repeated multiple times over a long period, separated by baseline resting intervals. Transients could then be aligned according to the time course of the event and averaged together. This would provide the same signal-to-noise improvement reached in the current study, while requiring fewer participants.
Conclusion
The relatively low signal-to-noise ratio of MRS measurements has shaped the types of questions that the technique has been used to address. Here we overcome this limitation by combining data from large cohorts to examine the dynamics of GABA and Glx concentrations in visual cortex. Through use of existing and novel analyses, we reveal opposing dynamic shifts in GABA and Glx in visual cortex while participants are at rest. Further, we demonstrate a predictive relationship between GABA and Glx in visual cortex. This study exposes temporal trends of primary neurotransmitters in visual cortex, and more generally, these findings demonstrate the feasibility of using MRS to investigate in vivo dynamic changes of neurometabolites.
Synthesis
Reviewing Editor: Satu Palva, University of Helsinki
Decisions are customarily a result of the Reviewing Editor and the peer reviewers coming together and discussing their recommendations until a consensus is reached. When revisions are invited, a fact-based synthesis statement explaining their decision and outlining what is needed to prepare a revision will be listed below. The following reviewer(s) agreed to reveal their identity: Tineke Grent-'t-Jong.
Reviewers were pleasantly surprised by a much improved manuscript compared to the previous one. Well done! In its current form, it is certainly a pleasant and interesting read, one much more acceptable for publication. In general, rationale, description of methods and results, and topics for discussion, are all more clearly formulated and are more complete. Caveats are indicated and where possible addressed. Furthermore, the removal of the K-dataset is not noticeable, which indicates that it could indeed be missed. Finally, adding a section on clinical relevance of this method and additionally including a potential event-related design that can be used for recordings of high-temporal resolution MRS data in clinical populations is an important addition.
Comments
• The abstract mentions that the effect was only found in the visual cortex. Given the differences between the studies I don't think this statement can be made without qualification that the data were collected under different conditions.
• One question relates to MRS specific inclusion criteria. Related to this, and quite important - can the authors be certain that participants did not fall asleep? Could that account for different patterns?
• In the new section lines 414-427 there are several issues:
a. Is P=3.9e-33 correct? Standard notation would be x10-33
b. Is the following sentence correct? Glx seems to have a significant t-value while GABA+ and tCr have z values.
For visual cortex, we found no evidence for a difference in FWHM for any of the target neurometabolites (GABA+, z=-0.06, P=.948; Glx, t(119)=-5.08, P=1.4e-6; tCr, z=-1.10, P=.270).
c. Throughout this section there is a mix of z and t and I don’t understand why it is not always the same test.
•Discuss potential impact of recording MRS data without water suppression (temporal stability of water signal can be an issue, isn’t it?).
•One could propose also future studies that use a within-subject design (minimally two ROIs) and/or separate Glutamate from Glutamine and look for replication of the current effects that are even better validated.
•Line 151: ‘and remove during’, should read ‘and removed during’
•Line 164: ‘were modelled off’, should read ‘were modelled on’
•Lines 288-294: ‘The FWHM of the neurometabolite peaks indicate spectra from posterior cingulate cortex had better signal quality; further, this dataset contained more than three times as many participants as the visual cortex dataset. Thus, it seems unlikely that the neurometabolite drift we observed in visual cortex was also present in posterior cingulate cortex, but we failed to detect it because of poor signal quality or lack of statistical power. A more parsimonious explanation is that the neurometabolite drift in visual cortex was regionally specific’. I’m not very convinced this statement is useful and/or correct. The whole reasoning can be turned upside down. With poorer signal quality and fewer participants in the VC data, compared to the PCC data, intersubject variability (i.e., measurement noise) could have been less sufficiently suppressed and thus revealed the effect reported. In other words, there is no guarantee that the VC effect would not disappear when as many datasets as in the PCC data were collected and included in the analyses. Only future studies with larger sample sizes can tell ...
•Line 390: ‘While GABA and Glx are thought to support opposing mechanisms ...’. Technically speaking, one could argue that this is not entirely correct, as it are GABA and Glutamate that are linked to inhibition and excitation, respectively, whereas Glutamine - also part of Glx - is seen as a source for GABA synthesis (i.e., not directly linked to excitation).
Author Response
The abstract mentions that the effect was only found in the visual cortex. Given the differences between the studies I don’t think this statement can be made without qualification that the data were collected under different conditions.
I thank the reviewer for this suggestion, I do not want to imply that the data were collected under the exact same conditions in the same study. I have noted this in the abstract of the revised manuscript as follows:
Lines 21-21
Here we apply novel analyses with large datasets from human participants (both sexes) to reveal the dynamics of GABA+ and Glx in visual cortex while participants are at rest (with eyes closed) and compare this with changes in posterior cingulate cortex from a previously collected dataset (under different conditions).
One question relates to MRS specific inclusion criteria. Related to this, and quite important - can the authors be certain that participants did not fall asleep? Could that account for different patterns?
Following the scan, we asked the participants if they fell asleep during the scan, and we did not have participants who admitted to this. Similarly, in the Big GABA dataset, participants’ wakefulness was not physiologically monitored, but there was an expectation that they would remain awake. Self-report is not ideal, and I suspect that participants varied in their wakefulness (possibly with some even briefly entering light sleep without realizing it). However, as we do not have an objective measure of this, e.g., EEG activity, we cannot conduct further analysis to determine whether it accounted for different patterns of metabolite drift. I agree that would be an interesting question and have included it in the Discussion of the revised manuscript.
Lines 539-545
Another possible explanation for the variability between participants is that it relates to their state of wakefulness during the scan. Although subjects reported not falling asleep, it seems likely that some may have been more alert than others during the scan, and this may be related to the concentration drift observed in visual cortex. We do not have the data to examine this possibility; however, future work could concurrently collect metabolite concentration and a physiological marker of wakefulness, e.g., EEG, to assess this possibility.
In the new section lines 414-427 there are several issues:
a. Is P=3.9e-33 correct? Standard notation would be x10-33
In scientific notation, “e” can be used to substitute “×10 to the power of”. However, to improve clarity, this has been converted to “×10” throughout the revised manuscript.
b. Is the following sentence correct? Glx seems to have a significant t-value while GABA+ and tCr have z values.
Yes - this is correct. As described in the Significance testing subsection added in the previous revision, for tests of data that violated the normality assumption, I applied the non-parametric Wilcoxon rank sum test, which produces a z-value, rather than a t-value.
c. Throughout this section there is a mix of z and t and I don’t understand why it is not always the same test.
See previous response.
Discuss potential impact of recording MRS data without water suppression (temporal stability of water signal can be an issue, isn’t it?).
My understanding of the issue is that water suppression was necessary in the past, because the hardware used to acquire the signal was not sufficiently sensitive and/or too susceptible to noise to yield accurate measures of other metabolites in the presence of the water peak. However, modern hardware does not have the same limitation and accurate quantification can be achieved with or without water suppression. There are arguments for and against using water suppression, and I agree with the reviewer that this is worth mentioning in the manuscript (as the VC dataset is water suppressed, while the PCC dataset is not). I have acknowledged this in the revised manuscript.
Lines 130-133
Note that unlike the data collected in visual cortex, data collected in posterior cingulate cortex was not water-suppressed, which can negatively impact accurate quantification of other metabolites (e.g., sideband artifacts; see (Dong, 2015) for a review of issues relating to water suppression).
One could propose also future studies that use a within-subject design (minimally two ROIs) and/or separate Glutamate from Glutamine and look for replication of the current effects that are even better validated.
I agree with the reviewer; this is a sensible suggestion. This has now been acknowledged in the Discussion section of the revised manuscript as follows:
Lines 529-531
Additionally, future work could use a within-subject design to compare different voxel locations, and separately assess Glu and Gln concentration drift.
Line 151: ‘and remove during’, should read ‘and removed during’
Line 164: ‘were modelled off’, should read ’were modelled on‘
I thank the Review for spotting these typos, they have been fixed in the revised manuscript.
Lines 288-294: ‘The FWHM of the neurometabolite peaks indicate spectra from posterior cingulate cortex had better signal quality; further, this dataset contained more than three times as many participants as the visual cortex dataset. Thus, it seems unlikely that the neurometabolite drift we observed in visual cortex was also present in posterior cingulate cortex, but we failed to detect it because of poor signal quality or lack of statistical power. A more parsimonious explanation is that the neurometabolite drift in visual cortex was regionally specific’. I’m not very convinced this statement is useful and/or correct. The whole reasoning can be turned upside down. With poorer signal quality and fewer participants in the VC data, compared to the PCC data, intersubject variability (i.e., measurement noise) could have been less sufficiently suppressed and thus revealed the effect reported. In other words, there is no guarantee that the VC effect would not disappear when as many datasets as in the PCC data were collected and included in the analyses. Only future studies with larger sample sizes can tell ...
I appreciate the Reviewer’s concern on this matter; however, as I mentioned in my previous response, I conducted robust statistical analyses on the likelihood that the difference in the VC data could have occurred in the scenario the reviewer has outlined, e.g., due to chance, and found that this was well below 1%. Further, the sample size of the VC data used in this study represents the largest of its kind to-date. Given these factors, I don’t believe that it is unreasonable to view the VC data findings as robust. If the VC results are to be accepted as robust, it then follows that it is reasonable to assert that the reason for not finding the same drift in the PCC data was not because of insufficient data quality or sample size.
Line 390: ‘While GABA and Glx are thought to support opposing mechanisms ...’. Technically speaking, one could argue that this is not entirely correct, as it are GABA and Glutamate that are linked to inhibition and excitation, respectively, whereas Glutamine - also part of Glx - is seen as a source for GABA synthesis (i.e., not directly linked to excitation).
I thank the Reviewer for this correction - it is important to be accurate and I have now updated this statement in the revised manuscript.
Lines 415-417
While GABA and Glu are thought to support opposing mechanisms in the central nervous system, i.e., inhibition and excitation, it seems reasonable to expect interactions between GABA and Glx, which comprises both Glu and Gln. For example, Gln is a primary source of GABA synthesis (Patel et al., 2001; Paulsen et al., 1988; Rae et al., 2003).
References
- Bednařík P, Tkáč I, Giove F, Dinuzzo M, Deelchand DK, Emir UE, Eberly LE, Mangia S (2015) Neurochemical and BOLD responses during neuronal activation measured in the human visual cortex at 7 Tesla. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 35:601–610. 10.1038/jcbfm.2014.233 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bednařík P, Tkáč I, Giove F, Eberly LE, Deelchand DK, Barreto FR, Mangia S (2018) Neurochemical responses to chromatic and achromatic stimuli in the human visual cortex. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 38:347–359. 10.1177/0271678X17695291 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Belelli D, Harrison NL, Maguire J, Macdonald RL, Walker MC, Cope DW (2009) Extrasynaptic GABAA receptors: form, pharmacology, and function. J Neurosci 29:12757–12763. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3340-09.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bhattacharyya PK, Lowe MJ, Phillips MD (2007) Spectral quality control in motion-corrupted single-voxel J-difference editing scans: an interleaved navigator approach. Magn Reson Med 58:808–812. 10.1002/mrm.21337 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bonett DG, Wright TA (2000) Sample size requirements for estimating Pearson, Kendall and Spearman correlations. Psychometrika 65:23–28. 10.1007/BF02294183 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Boroojerdi B, Bushara KO, Corwell B, Immisch I, Battaglia F, Muellbacher W, Cohen LG (2000) Enhanced excitability of the human visual cortex induced by short-term light deprivation. Cereb Cortex 10:529–534. 10.1093/cercor/10.5.529 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bradford HF (1995) Glutamate, GABA and epilepsy. Prog Neurobiol 47:477–511. 10.1016/0301-0082(95)00030-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Branzoli F, Techawiboonwong A, Kan H, Webb A, Ronen I (2013) Functional diffusion-weighted magnetic resonance spectroscopy of the human primary visual cortex at 7 T. Magn Reson Med 69:303–309. 10.1002/mrm.24542 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chao HT, Chen H, Samaco RC, Xue M, Chahrour M, Yoo J, Neul JL, Gong S, Lu HC, Heintz N, Ekker M, Rubenstein JLR, Noebels JL, Rosenmund C, Zoghbi HY (2010) Dysfunction in GABA signalling mediates autism-like stereotypies and Rett syndrome phenotypes. Nature 468:263–269. 10.1038/nature09582 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen C, Sigurdsson HP, Pépés SE, Auer DP, Morris PG, Morgan PS, Gowland PA, Jackson SR (2017) Activation induced changes in GABA: functional MRS at 7 T with MEGA-sLASER. Neuroimage 156:207–213. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.05.044 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dong Z (2015) Proton MRS and MRSI of the brain without water suppression. Prog Nucl Magn Reson Spectrosc 86-87:65–79. 10.1016/j.pnmrs.2014.12.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edden RAE, Puts NAJ, Harris AD, Barker PB, Evans CJ (2014) Gannet: a batch-processing tool for the quantitative analysis of gamma-aminobutyric acid-edited MR spectroscopy spectra. J Magn Reson Imaging 40:1445–1452. 10.1002/jmri.24478 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harris AD, Puts NAJ, Edden RAE (2015) Tissue correction for GABA-edited MRS: considerations of voxel composition, tissue segmentation, and tissue relaxations. J Magn Reson Imaging 42:1431–1440. 10.1002/jmri.24903 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ip IB, Berrington A, Hess AT, Parker AJ, Emir UE, Bridge H (2017) Combined fMRI-MRS acquires simultaneous glutamate and BOLD-fMRI signals in the human brain. Neuroimage 155:113–119. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.04.030 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jansen JFA, Backes WH, Nicolay K, Kooi ME (2006) 1H MR spectroscopy of the brain: absolute quantification of metabolites. Radiology 240:318–332. 10.1148/radiol.2402050314 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kehrer C, Maziashvili N, Dugladze T, Gloveli T (2008) Altered excitatory-inhibitory balance in the NMDA-hypofunction model of schizophrenia. Front Mol Neurosci 1:6. 10.3389/neuro.02.006.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kurcyus K, Annac E, Hanning NM, Harris AD, Oeltzschner G, Edden RAE, Riedl V (2018) Opposite dynamics of GABA and glutamate levels in the occipital cortex during visual processing. J Neurosci 38:9967–9976. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1214-18.2018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lange T, Zaitsev M, Buechert M (2011) Correction of frequency drifts induced by gradient heating in 1H spectra using interleaved reference spectroscopy. J Magn Reson Imaging 33:748–754. 10.1002/jmri.22471 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lin Y, Stephenson MC, Xin L, Napolitano A, Morris PG (2012) Investigating the metabolic changes due to visual stimulation using functional proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy at 7 T. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 32:1484–1495. 10.1038/jcbfm.2012.33 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lunghi C, Emir UE, Morrone MC, Bridge H (2015) Short-term monocular deprivation alters GABA in the adult human visual cortex. Curr Biol 25:1496–1501. 10.1016/j.cub.2015.04.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mangia S, Tkáč I, Gruetter R, Van de Moortele P-F, Maraviglia B, Uğurbil K (2007) Sustained neuronal activation raises oxidative metabolism to a new steady-state level: evidence from 1H NMR spectroscopy in the human visual cortex. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 27:1055–1063. 10.1038/sj.jcbfm.9600401 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Markram K, Markram H (2010) The intense world theory—a unifying theory of the neurobiology of autism. Front Hum Neurosci 4:224. 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00224 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin DL, Rimvall K (1993) Regulation of γ-aminobutyric acid synthesis in the brain. J Neurochem 60:395–407. 10.1111/j.1471-4159.1993.tb03165.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mekle R, Kühn S, Pfeiffer H, Aydin S, Schubert F, Ittermann B (2017) Detection of metabolite changes in response to a varying visual stimulation paradigm using short-TE 1H MRS at 7 T. NMR Biomed 30:e3672 10.1002/nbm.3672 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mescher M, Tannus A, Johnson MO, Garwood M (1996) Solvent suppression using selective echo dephasing. J Magn Reson A 123:226–229. 10.1006/jmra.1996.0242 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Mescher M, Merkle H, Kirsch J, Garwood M, Gruetter R (1998) Simultaneous in vivo spectral editing and water suppression. NMR Biomed 11:266–272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mikkelsen M, Barker PB, Bhattacharyya PK, Brix MK, Buur PF, Cecil KM, Chan KL, Chen DY-T, Craven AR, Cuypers K, Dacko M, Duncan NW, Dydak U, Edmondson DA, Ende G, Ersland L, Gao F, Greenhouse I, Harris AD, He N, et al. (2017) Big GABA: edited MR spectroscopy at 24 research sites. Neuroimage 159:32–45. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.07.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mikkelsen M, Loo RS, Puts NAJ, Edden RAE, Harris AD (2018) Designing GABA-edited magnetic resonance spectroscopy studies: considerations of scan duration, signal-to-noise ratio and sample size. J Neurosci Methods 303:86–94. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2018.02.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mikkelsen M, Rimbault DL, Barker PB, Bhattacharyya PK, Brix MK, Buur PF, Cecil KM, Chan KL, Chen DY-T, Craven AR, Cuypers K, Dacko M, Duncan NW, Dydak U, Edmondson DA, Ende G, Ersland L, Forbes MA, Gao F, Greenhouse I, et al. (2019) Big GABA II: water-referenced edited MR spectroscopy at 25 research sites. Neuroimage 191:537–548. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.02.059 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mullins PG, McGonigle DJ, O’Gorman RL, Puts NAJ, Vidyasagar R, Evans CJ, Edden RAE (2014) Current practice in the use of MEGA-PRESS spectroscopy for the detection of GABA. Neuroimage 86:43–52. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.12.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olsen RW, Avoli M (1997) GABA and Epileptogenesis. Epilepsia 38:399–407. 10.1111/j.1528-1157.1997.tb01728.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patel AB, Rothman DL, Cline GW, Behar KL (2001) Glutamine is the major precursor for GABA synthesis in rat neocortex in vivo following acute GABA-transaminase inhibition. Brain Res 919:207–220. 10.1016/s0006-8993(01)03015-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paulsen RE, Odden E, Fonnum F (1988) Importance of glutamine for gamma-aminobutyric acid synthesis in rat neostriatum in vivo. J Neurochem 51:1294–1299. 10.1111/j.1471-4159.1988.tb03099.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rae C, Hare N, Bubb WA, McEwan SR, Bröer A, McQuillan JA, Balcar VJ, Conigrave AD, Bröer S (2003) Inhibition of glutamine transport depletes glutamate and GABA neurotransmitter pools: further evidence for metabolic compartmentation. J Neurochem 85:503–514. 10.1046/j.1471-4159.2003.01713.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rideaux R, Welchman AE (2018) Proscription supports robust perceptual integration by suppression in human visual cortex. Nat Commun 9:1502. 10.1038/s41467-018-03400-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rideaux R, Goncalves NR, Welchman AE (2019) Mixed-polarity random-dot stereograms alter GABA and Glx concentration in the early visual cortex. J Neurophysiol 122:888–896. 10.1152/jn.00208.2019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rubenstein JLR, Merzenich MM (2003) Model of autism: increased ratio of excitation/inhibition in key neural systems. Genes Brain Behav 2:255–267. 10.1034/j.1601-183x.2003.00037.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schaller B, Mekle R, Xin L, Kunz N, Gruetter R (2013) Net increase of lactate and glutamate concentration in activated human visual cortex detected with magnetic resonance spectroscopy at 7 tesla. J Neurosci Res 91:1076–1083. 10.1002/jnr.23194 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tkáč I, Gruetter R (2005) Methodology of 1H NMR spectroscopy of the human brain at very high magnetic fields. Appl Magn Reson 29:139–157. 10.1007/BF03166960 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vattikuti S, Chow CC (2010) A computational model for cerebral cortical dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders. Biol Psychiatry 67:672–678. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.09.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]