Skip to main content
Elsevier Sponsored Documents logoLink to Elsevier Sponsored Documents
. 2020 Aug 18;32(7):108046. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2020.108046

Specific Hippocampal Interneurons Shape Consolidation of Recognition Memory

Jose F Oliveira da Cruz 1,2,3,9, Arnau Busquets-Garcia 1,2,4,9, Zhe Zhao 1, Marjorie Varilh 1,2, Gianluca Lavanco 1,2,5, Luigi Bellocchio 1,2, Laurie Robin 1, Astrid Cannich 1,2, Francisca Julio-Kalajzić 1,2, Thierry Lesté-Lasserre 1,2, Marlène Maître 1,2, Filippo Drago 4, Giovanni Marsicano 1,2,10,11,, Edgar Soria-Gómez 1,2,6,7,8,10,∗∗
PMCID: PMC7443618  PMID: 32814049

Summary

A complex array of inhibitory interneurons tightly controls hippocampal activity, but how such diversity specifically affects memory processes is not well understood. We find that a small subclass of type 1 cannabinoid receptor (CB1R)-expressing hippocampal interneurons determines episodic-like memory consolidation by linking dopamine D1 receptor (D1R) signaling to GABAergic transmission. Mice lacking CB1Rs in D1-positive cells (D1-CB1-KO) display impairment in long-term, but not short-term, novel object recognition memory (NOR). Re-expression of CB1Rs in hippocampal D1R-positive cells rescues this NOR deficit. Learning induces an enhancement of in vivo hippocampal long-term potentiation (LTP), which is absent in mutant mice. CB1R-mediated NOR and the associated LTP facilitation involve local control of GABAergic inhibition in a D1-dependent manner.

This study reveals that hippocampal CB1R-/D1R-expressing interneurons control NOR memory, identifying a mechanism linking the diversity of hippocampal interneurons to specific behavioral outcomes.

Keywords: CB1 receptor, novel object recognition memory, hippocampus, interneurons, dopamine, D1 receptor, GABA, long-term potentiation, endocannabinoid system

Graphical Abstract

graphic file with name fx1.jpg

Highlights

  • CB1Rs are present in hippocampal D1R-positive interneurons

  • CB1R/D1R-positive interneurons control the late phase of recognition memory

  • CB1R/D1R-positive interneurons control learning-induced facilitation of LTP


Oliveira da Cruz et al. show that a specific subpopulation of hippocampal CB1 and D1 receptor-positive neurons controls late consolidation of recognition memory and associated synaptic plasticity by moderating local inhibitory GABAergic activity in the hippocampus.

Introduction

Formation of episodic memory is a multistep brain process that requires activity of the medial temporal lobe (Squire et al., 2007). The hippocampus in particular participates in long-term storage of recently acquired events. Hippocampal circuits are regulated by a large variety of local inhibitory interneurons that are controlled by neuromodulatory systems ensuring their coordinated function to shape behavioral responses (Klausberger and Somogyi, 2008); the identities and functions of the interneurons are under intense scrutiny (Harris et al., 2018; Pelkey et al., 2017; Parra et al., 1998).

The endocannabinoid system is a brain-modulatory signaling hub formed mainly by type 1 cannabinoid receptors (CB1Rs), their endogenous ligands (endocannabinoids), and enzymes for their synthesis and degradation. In the hippocampus, CB1Rs are present in principal neurons and astroglial cells (Busquets-Garcia et al., 2015; Oliveira da Cruz et al., 2016). However, the largest expression of CB1Rs resides in GABAergic interneurons (Marsicano and Kuner, 2008; Katona and Freund, 2012), where they modulate local inhibition of hippocampal circuits. Particularly, the largest amount of CB1Rs is expressed in cholecystokinin (CCK)-positive interneurons, which are characterized by asynchronous neurotransmitter release (Harris et al., 2018; Katona et al., 1999; Marsicano and Lutz, 1999).

Hippocampal CB1Rs control episodic-like memory processes and synaptic plasticity (Robin et al., 2018; Hebert-Chatelain et al., 2016; Puighermanal et al., 2009). However, the specific locations where these receptors participate in the mechanisms underlying hippocampus-dependent memory are only partially known.

Activity-dependent long-term changes in hippocampal synaptic transmission are considered cellular correlates of memory consolidation (Nicoll, 2017; Whitlock et al., 2006), which involves local dopamine D1 receptor (D1R) signaling (Lisman et al., 2011; Yamasaki and Takeuchi, 2017). Exposure to hippocampus-dependent behavioral tasks induces changes in long-term potentiation (LTP) of synaptic transmission that require activation of D1-like receptors (Frey et al., 1990; Granado et al., 2008; Li et al., 2003; Lemon and Manahan-Vaughan, 2006). A novel subpopulation of hippocampal CB1R/CCK-positive interneurons containing D1R was recently described (Puighermanal et al., 2017; Gangarossa et al., 2012). However, the potential interactions between D1Rs and CB1Rs in regulating learning-induced plasticity, activity of hippocampal circuits, and memory processes remain unexplored.

Here we assessed the role of D1R/CB1R-positive cells in regulation of episodic-like novel object recognition (NOR) memory. We found that conditional deletion of the CB1R gene in hippocampal D1R-positive cells impairs long- but not short-term NOR memory and learning-induced LTP enhancement involving local control of GABAergic transmission. These intriguing results suggest that CB1R signaling provides a functional link between hippocampal dopaminergic and GABAergic control of synaptic plasticity and memory consolidation.

Results

CB1Rs in Hippocampal D1R-Positive Neurons Are Necessary for Consolidation of NOR Memory

Mutant mice bearing a deletion of the CB1R gene in cells expressing D1R (D1-CB1-knockout [KO] mice; Monory et al., 2007) displayed no phenotype in the short-term version (3 h post-training) of a NOR task (Figures 1A and 1B; Puighermanal et al., 2009; Busquets-Garcia et al., 2011; Robin et al., 2018). Conversely, they showed strong impairment in long-term (24 h) memory compared with their wild-type (WT) littermates (Figure 1C), with no changes in total exploration time (Figures S1A–S1D).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Hippocampal CB1Rs in D1R-Positive Cells Are Necessary for Late but Not Early Consolidation of NOR

(A) Schematic representation of the NOR memory task.

(B) Short-term (3 h) NOR memory performance of D1-CB1-WT mice (n = 10) and D1-CB1-KO littermates (n = 7).

(C) Long-term NOR (24 h) memory performance of D1-CB1-WT mice (n = 9) and D1-CB1-KO littermates (n = 8).

(D) Schematic representation of the experiment using viral re-expression of the CB1R gene in the striatum (STR) or the hippocampus (HPC) of D1-CB1-WT mice and D1-CB1-KO littermates.

(E) Representative images of Cre-expressing D1-CB1-KO mice injected with CB1R-myc in the STR using the same procedure as described in (D) (STAR Methods). Scale bar, 2 mm.

(F) NOR memory performance of mice with re-expression of the CB1R gene in the STR or HPC. Control, n (D1-CB1-WT) = 17 and n (D1-CB1-KO) = 5; STR-CB1-RS, n (D1-CB1-KO) = 6; HPC-CB1-RS, n (D1-CB1-KO) = 9.

(G) Immunofluorescence of cells expressing CB1R-myc in the HPC. Scale bar, 500 μm.

(H) Schematic representation of the experiment using viral expression of the Gi-DREADDs or mCherry in the HPC of D1-CB1-WT mice and D1-CB1-KO littermates. Clozapine N-oxide (CNO; 2 mg/kg) injections take place after the training phase of the NOR task.

(I) NOR memory performance of D1-CB1-WT mice injected intra-hippocampally with hM4D(Gi) virus or mCherry (n VEH = 16, n CNO = 21), D1-CB1-KO mice injected with mCherry (n VEH = 6, n CNO = 7), and D1-CB1-KO mice injected intra-hippocampally with hM4D(Gi) (n VEH = 11, n CNO = 14).

Data, mean ± SEM. p < 0.05, ∗∗∗p < 0.001, ∗∗∗∗p < 0.0001. ns, not significant. See also Figure S1 and Table S1.

The majority of CB1Rs in D1R-positive neurons have been characterized previously in striatonigral circuits (Monory et al., 2007). Considering the involvement of these circuits in NOR memory (Darvas and Palmiter, 2009), we tested the role of striatal CB1Rs. We infused an adeno-associated virus carrying a Cre-dependent expression of CB1Rs (pAAV-CAG-DIO-CB1) into the striatum of D1-CB1-KO mice to obtain re-expression (RS) of CB1Rs in cells where Cre is present (hereafter called D1R-positive) in this brain region (striatum [STR]-CB1-RS mice; Figures 1D and 1E), as revealed by immunodetection of a myc-tagged version of CB1Rs (CB1R-myc; STAR Methods; Figure 1E). This re-expression was not sufficient to rescue the phenotype of D1-CB1-KO mice in long-term NOR (Figures 1F, S1E, and S1F), suggesting that CB1Rs in striatal D1R-positive cells do not participate in this type of memory. Anatomical data indicate that a subset of hippocampal neurons contain D1Rs (Gangarossa et al., 2012), likely co-expressing CB1R protein (Puighermanal et al., 2017). Thus, we re-express the CB1R gene in the hippocampus of D1-CB1-KO mice to obtain hippocampus (HPC)-CB1-RS mice (Figures 1D and 1G). This manipulation fully rescued the phenotype of the mutant mice (Figure 1F, S1E, and S1F), indicating that hippocampal CB1Rs expressed in D1R-positive cells are required for NOR memory.

We recently reported that deletion of CB1Rs in hippocampal glial acidic fibrillary protein (GFAP)-positive cells (i.e., mainly astrocytes, GFAP-CB1-KO mice) also impaired NOR memory (Robin et al., 2018). Indeed, GFAP-CB1-KO mice were impaired in NOR (Figures S1G–S1I; Robin et al., 2018), but, in contrast to D1-CB1-KO mice, this phenotype extended to short-term NOR memory (Figures S1J–S1L). This difference suggests that CB1Rs expressed in hippocampal astrocytes or D1R-positive cells might control distinct phases of NOR memory consolidation.

The primary function of CB1R activation in neurons is to decrease neurotransmitter release (Castillo et al., 2012; Busquets-Garcia et al., 2017). Accordingly, deletion of CB1Rs from neurons often results in excessive neurotransmission. Thus, we reasoned that inhibition of hippocampal D1R-positive neurons during NOR consolidation should be able to rescue the memory impairment of D1-CB1-KO mice. Viral vectors carrying Cre-dependent expression of an inhibitory designer receptor exclusively activated by designer drugs (DIO-hM4DGi, Gi-DREADD; Robinson and Adelman, 2015) or control mCherry protein were infused into the hippocampi of D1-CB1-KO mice and WT littermates (Figure 1H). Post-training clozapine N-oxide (CNO) injections did not affect the NOR performance of D1-CB1-KO and WT mice injected with Gi-DREADD or mCherry, indicating that the drug or its metabolites had no effect per se (Gomez et al., 2017; Figures 1I, S1M, and S1N). Conversely, post-acquisition CNO treatment fully rescued the NOR impairment of D1-CB1-KO mice expressing Gi-DREADD (Figures 1I, S1M, and S1N). This strongly suggests that excessive activity of D1R-positive neurons during the consolidation process is responsible for the memory impairment observed in D1-CB1-KO mice.

CB1Rs in Hippocampal D1R-Positive Neurons Control Learning-Induced Changes of LTP In Vivo

Cellular and molecular mechanisms underlying activity-dependent changes in synaptic plasticity are proposed to underlie long-term memory (Aggleton and Morris, 2018). Previous studies showed that conditional and global deletion of CB1Rs in neuronal and glial cell populations induces deficits in learning and associated synaptic plasticity (Busquets-Garcia et al., 2017; Robin et al., 2018). To address the role of CB1Rs in hippocampal D1R-positive neurons in modulation of synaptic plasticity, we recorded in-vivo-evoked field excitatory postsynaptic potentials (fEPSPs) in the hippocampal CA3-CA1 pathway of anesthetized mice. High-frequency stimulation (HFS) induced similar long-lasting LTP of synaptic fEPSPs in D1-CB1-KO and WT littermates (Figures 2A and 2B), indicating that hippocampal D1R/CB1R-positive neurons are dispensable for expression of LTP in naive animals.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Learning-Induced Facilitation of In Vivo Hippocampal LTP Requires CB1Rs at D1R-Positive Neurons

(A and B) HFS in the dorsal hippocampal CA3 Schaffer collateral pathway induces in vivo LTP in the dorsal CA1 stratum radiatum.

(A) Summary plots of recorded evoked fEPSPs in anesthetized D1-CB1-WT (n = 8) and D1-CB1-KO (n = 8) mice.

(B) Bar histograms of normalized fEPSPs from (A), representing 30 and 60 min after HFS.

(C) Schematic representation of the experimental setup (STAR Methods).

(D and E) Learning modulates in vivo LTP.

(D) Summary plots of recorded evoked fEPSPs from mice exposed to control (n = 8) and NOR training (n = 11) conditions.

(E) Bar histograms of normalized of evoked fEPSPs from (D), representing 30 and 60 min after HFS.

(F and G) Learning-induced modulation of in vivo LTP is impaired in D1-CB1-KO mice.

(F) Summary plots of recorded fEPSPs in anesthetized D1-CB1-WT (n = 10) and D1-CB1-KO (n = 10) mice.

(G) Bar histograms of normalized of evoked fEPSPs from (F), representing 30 and 60 min after HFS.

Traces on the right side of the summary plots represent 150 superimposed evoked fEPSPs before HFS (1, gray) and 30 min (2, brown) and 60 min (3, black) after HFS. Data, mean ± SEM. p < 0.05. See also Table S1.

HPC-dependent memory-related processes such as LTP are sensitive to pharmacological and genetic modulation of hippocampal D1Rs, particularly after learning (Li et al., 2003; Lemon and Manahan-Vaughan, 2006; Takeuchi et al., 2016; Yamasaki and Takeuchi, 2017). Thus, we hypothesized that CB1Rs in D1R-positive neurons may modulate learning-dependent hippocampal synaptic plasticity. To explore whether acquisition of the NOR task modulates in vivo LTP, we recorded fEPSPs from C57Bl6/NRj mice after a NOR task (Figure 2C). HFS induced stronger LTP in animals exposed to NOR acquisition than in control mice (Figures 2D and 2E), showing that the training modulates hippocampal synaptic plasticity. Strikingly, D1-CB1-KO mice lacked this learning-induced enhancement of LTP (Figures 2F and 2G). Thus, physiological activation of CB1Rs in hippocampal D1R-positive neurons is required for learning-dependent facilitation of LTP.

CB1R in Hippocampal D1R-Positive Neurons Modulate NOR Memory Consolidation through a GABA-Dependent Mechanism

D1Rs are expressed in different hippocampal cells, including subsets of GABAergic and glutamatergic neurons (Gangarossa et al., 2012). Considering that CB1R signaling decreases the activity of hippocampal neurons (Busquets-Garcia et al., 2017; Castillo et al., 2012), we asked whether excessive glutamatergic or GABAergic neurotransmission might underlie the phenotype of D1-CB1-KO mice. Thus, we injected non-amnesic doses (Puighermanal et al., 2009) of the NMDA receptor blocker MK-801, the AMPA/kainate receptor antagonist NBQX (Figure S2A), or the GABAA receptor antagonist bicuculline into D1-CB1-KO and WT littermates immediately after NOR training. MK-801 and NBQX did not alter memory performance in WT mice, nor did it rescue the amnesic phenotype of D1-CB1-KO littermates (Figures 3A, S2B, and S2C). Conversely, bicuculline completely reversed the memory impairment of D1-CB1-KO mice when injected immediately after training or 1 h later without affecting WT littermates’ performance (Figures 3A, S2B, and S2C).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Hippocampal CB1R/D1R-Positive Interneurons Modulate Synaptic GABAergic Transmission

(A) NOR memory performance of mutant mice administered vehicle (n D1-CB1-WT = 14, n D1-CB1-KO = 14), MK-801 (0.1 mg/kg, intraperitoneally [i.p.]; n D1-CB1-WT = 7, n D1-CB1-KO = 7), NBQX (5 mg/kg, i.p.; n D1-CB1-WT = 8, n D1-CB1-KO = 5), or bicuculline immediately after (n D1-CB1-WT = 10, n D1-CB1-KO = 10) or 1 h after the training phase (n D1-CB1-WT = 10, n D1-CB1-KO = 8).

(B) Schematic representation of the experimental procedure to detect CB1R mRNA in D1R-positive cells.

(C and D) Representative images of CB1R mRNA (green) and mCherry protein (red) labeling in the hippocampal CA1 region of D1-Cre (C) and D1-CB1-KO (D) mice. White arrows indicate colocalization of CB1R-positive and D1R-positive cell bodies. Scale bar, 150 μm.

(E and F) Layer-specific distribution of the percentage of cell bodies expressing high (E) and low amounts (F) of CB1Rs, which colocalize with mCherry-positive (i.e., D1R-positive) in D1-Cre (n = 3) and D1-CB1-KO (n = 3).

Data, mean ± SEM. p < 0.05, ∗∗p < 0.01. See also Figure S2 and Table S1.

These data indicate that excessive GABAergic but not glutamatergic ionotropic receptor activity is involved in the phenotype of D1-CB1-KO mice. A large proportion of GABAergic hippocampal interneurons contain CB1R mRNA, which is expressed at different levels (high CB1R- and low CB1R-expressing cells; Marsicano and Lutz, 1999). Conversely, D1R mRNA is expressed at very low levels in the HPC (http://mouse.brain-map.org/experiment/show/35; data not shown), which makes it difficult to accurately quantify its expression above background. Therefore, to pinpoint which CB1R-positive interneurons in the HPC contain D1R, we combined fluorescence in situ hybridization for CB1R mRNA in D1-Cre and D1-CB1-KO mice carrying viral Cre-dependent expression of mCherry (STAR Methods; Figure 3B). As described (Marsicano and Lutz, 1999), detectable levels of CB1R mRNA were present throughout the HPC in pyramidal neurons and in GABAergic interneurons (Figure S2D). The distribution of mCherry-tagged D1-positive neurons in the dorsal CA1 region of D1-Cre mice was similar to previous findings (Puighermanal et al., 2017; Gangarossa et al., 2012). Double staining revealed that virtually no high CB1R-expressing interneurons in the strata oriens, pyramidale, radiatum, or lacunosum moleculare contain D1Rs (Figures 3C–3F and S2D). Conversely, D1Rs are present in a small subpopulation of low CB1R-expressing interneurons along the different hippocampal layers (Figures 3C and 3F). Importantly, this co-expression was virtually abolished in hippocampi of D1-CB1-KO mice (Figures 3C, 3D, and 3F).

Altogether, these data indicate that CB1R-dependent modulation of a small subpopulation of D1R-positive GABAergic interneurons is required during NOR memory consolidation.

Synaptic Mechanisms Underlying NOR Memory Consolidation and Associated Hippocampal Plasticity

The data collected so far show that reduction of GABAergic signaling prevents the deficits in D1-CB1-KO mice of NOR consolidation. Therefore, we tested whether inhibition of GABAA receptors could rescue the lack of learning-induced LTP enhancement observed in D1-CB1-KO mice. Trained mice received bicuculline or vehicle (VEH) before testing LTP induction in hippocampal circuits. In vehicle-treated animals, D1-CB1-KO mice showed no training-induced LTP enhancement (Figures 4A–4C). Strikingly, although bicuculline did not affect LTP in WT animals, it rescued the training-induced LTP of D1-CB1-KO mice (Figures 4A–4C).

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Cellular Mechanisms Linking D1R Signaling with GABAergic Activity during Learning-Induced Facilitation of In Vivo LTP and Memory Consolidation

(A) Effects of the GABAA receptor antagonist bicuculline and the D1/5R antagonist SCH-23390 on learning-induced modulation of in vivo LTP in D1-CB1-WT and D1-CB1-KO mice. Shown are summary plots of recorded evoked fEPSPs in vehicle (n D1-CB1-WT = 6, n D1-CB1-KO = 8), bicuculline (0.5 mg/kg, i.p.; n D1-CB1-WT = 9, n D1-CB1-KO = 11), and SCH-23390 (0.3 mg/kg, i.p.; n D1-CB1-WT = 6, n D1-CB1-KO = 6).

(B and C) Bar histograms of (A), representing normalized fEPSPs from 30 (B) and 60 (C) min after HFS.

(D) Memory performance D1-CB1-WT and D1-CB1-KO mice after being injected with vehicle (n D1-CB1-WT = 6, n D1-CB1-KO = 10) or SCH-23390 (0.3 mg/kg, i.p.; n D1-CB1-WT = 10, n D1-CB1-KO = 10).

Traces on the right side of the summary plot (A) represent 150 superimposed evoked fEPSPs before HFS (1, gray) and 30 min (2, brown) and 60 min (3, black) after HFS. Data, mean ± SEM. p < 0.05, ∗∗p < 0.01.

See also Figure S3 and Table S1.

Recent data suggest that hippocampal D1R-like receptors participate in memory formation, but little is known concerning the cell types involved (Lisman et al., 2011; Yamasaki and Takeuchi, 2017). Our data indicate that CB1R-dependent control of GABAergic transmission from a low number of hippocampal interneurons expressing D1R is required to guarantee late consolidation of NOR memory. Therefore, it is possible that endocannabinoid actions are secondary to activation of D1Rs in these cells. To address this issue, we first reasoned that partial inhibition of D1Rs should “replace” the lack of CB1R-dependent control of neurotransmission in D1-CB1-KO mice. Thus, we administered a sub-effective dose of the D1/5R antagonist SCH-23390 (Figures S3A–S3C) to D1-CB1-KO mice and WT littermates after NOR acquisition and analyzed the training-induced enhancement of in vivo LTP. This treatment slightly reduced the late phase of LTP in WT animals (Figures 4A–4C). However, the antagonist abolished the differences between D1-CB1-KO mice and WT littermates (Figures 4A–4C), indicating that reducing D1R activity counteracts the absence of CB1Rs in the mutants. If LTP is mechanistically linked to NOR consolidation, then the same treatment should rescue the memory impairment of D1-CB1-KO mice. Administration of SCH-23390 did not alter the behavior of WT mice (Figures 4D, S3D, and S3E), but, strikingly, it fully rescued the memory impairment of D1-CB1-KO littermates (Figures 4D, S3D, and S3E).

Altogether, these results indicate that endocannabinoid-dependent regulation of hippocampal D1R-positive interneurons is a necessary step in dopaminergic control of NOR memory consolidation and associated synaptic plasticity.

Discussion

The present study reveals that a specific subpopulation of hippocampal D1R/CB1R-positive neurons controls late consolidation of NOR memory and associated synaptic plasticity by moderating local inhibitory GABAergic activity in the HPC. Specifically, CB1Rs expressed in D1R-positive interneurons participate in learning-induced facilitation of in vivo LTP and are required for consolidation of NOR memory. Moreover, CB1Rs in D1R-positive neurons are necessary for physiological D1R-dependent modulation of memory processes, suggesting that cannabinoid signaling is part of a complex modulatory circuit regulated by dopamine transmission in the HPC. By determining cellular and behavioral functions of a specific CB1R-expressing interneuron subpopulation, these data uncover an unforeseen role of CB1Rs in the D1R-dependent control of long-term memory.

The endocannabinoid system regulates episodic-like recognition memory processes via CB1R-dependent control of different cell types in the HPC (Busquets-Garcia et al., 2017; Soria-Gomez et al., 2017; Busquets Garcia et al., 2016; Puighermanal et al., 2009; Robin et al., 2018). In the present study, we observed that the transition from short- to long-term memory processes is controlled by a functional interaction between D1Rs and CB1Rs in a specific subpopulation of hippocampal interneurons. In contrast, CB1R deletion from all body cells or in all forebrain GABAergic neurons does not reproduce the phenotype of D1-CB1-KO mice (Puighermanal et al., 2009; Hebert-Chatelain et al., 2016). These apparently contrasting observations can be explained by different possibilities. Long-term deletion of the CB1R gene starting from early developmental stages in CB1-KO and GABA-CB1-KO mice might induce compensatory mechanisms (El-Brolosy et al., 2019; El-Brolosy and Stainier, 2017), masking the functional role of the CB1R in NOR memory. An alternative or complementary explanation might point to the presence of different subpopulations of brain cells expressing CB1Rs and exerting opposite effects on memory processes. For instance, endocannabinoid signaling might promote or inhibit memory formation when acting at D1R-positive cells or at other neuronal subpopulations, respectively. We have shown previously that astroglial CB1Rs are necessary for consolidation of NOR memory by allowing D-serine availability at glutamatergic synapses (Robin et al., 2018). We cannot fully exclude that deletion of CB1Rs in D1R-positive cells does not also involve astrocytes (Nagatomo et al., 2017). However, so far, no conclusive anatomical evidence has been presented for expression of D1Rs in hippocampal astrocytes (Chai et al., 2017; Zhang et al., 2014; but see Jennings et al., 2017 for D1/5R pharmacological experiments). Moreover, our current and past results suggest that endocannabinoid control of astrocytes is likely involved in the initial phases of memory formation, whereas CB1R-dependent inhibition of D1R-positive hippocampal interneurons determines later phases of NOR memory consolidation. The time-course effects of pharmacological treatments indicate that D-serine can rescue memory performance of GFAP-CB1-KO mice only when administered immediately after learning (Robin et al., 2018). This idea is reinforced by the fact that these mutants do not express in vivo LTP even under basal “home cage” conditions (Robin et al., 2018), whereas D1-CB1-KO mice only lack the specific facilitation of LTP induced by learning. Altogether, these observations allow speculation that at least two distinct temporal windows exist in CB1R-dependent control of NOR. First, astroglial CB1R are necessary for the plastic processes to initiate the memory. Later, endocannabinoid-dependent regulation of D1R-positive interneurons is required to maintain the memory trace for longer periods.

Hippocampal D1R have been shown previously to be mainly on GABAergic interneurons, but lower levels were also detected on glutamatergic neurons (Gangarossa et al., 2012; Puighermanal et al., 2017; http://celltypes.brain-map.org/rnaseq/mouse_ctx-hip_smart-seq). Our data show that the D1-Cre mouse line used in the present study (Lemberger et al., 2007) induces recombination in a small sub-fraction of hippocampal interneurons containing low levels of CB1R mRNA but also in pyramidal neurons and mossy cells. Therefore, we cannot fully exclude that cell types other than hippocampal interneurons might participate in D1R/CB1R-dependent control of memory consolidation. However, our data show that partial blockade of GABAA receptors, but not of AMPA/kainate or NMDA glutamatergic ones, reverse the memory impairment of D1-CB1-KO mice. Therefore, our findings strongly suggest that CB1R control of GABA release from D1R-positive interneurons regulates late consolidation of NOR memory. However, recent data using emerging technologies suggest that hippocampal cells are more diverse and functionally segregated than previously thought (Harris et al., 2018; Soltesz and Losonczy, 2018). By identifying specific markers, future studies will extend our genetic and pharmacological evidence that a specific subpopulation of D1R/CB1R-positive hippocampal interneurons regulates consolidation of NOR memory.

LTP at the CA3-CA1 pathway is a potential molecular and cellular mechanism underlying behavioral expression of episodic-like memory processes (Morris, 2013). Interestingly, although deletion of CB1Rs from D1R-positive cells impairs NOR memory, the same manipulation does not impair in vivo LTP of hippocampal synaptic transmission in naive animals. In agreement with previous evidence under other experimental conditions (Li et al., 2003; Lemon and Manahan-Vaughan, 2006), WT mice exposed to the NOR learning task display facilitation of in vivo LTP compared with animals exposed to the same environment without any learning. Importantly, this facilitation is absent in D1-CB1-KO mice, suggesting that endocannabinoid control of D1R-positive hippocampal interneurons is recruited only after learning. The facilitation might be due to “real” stronger synaptic transmission after learning or a decrease in baseline synaptic activity (Lisman, 2017), which might be occluded in D1-CB1-KO mice. The fact that partial blockade of GABAA receptors in trained WT mice does not alter LTP facilitation suggests that this phenomenon is due to a genuine increase in LTP. In addition, our data indicate that reducing GABAergic transmission in D1R-positive neurons is required for this form of learning-induced synaptic plasticity. These results reinforce the idea that, to reveal relevant mechanisms, investigations of synaptic plasticity associated with memory processes should include not only naive animals but also behaviorally challenged ones (Lisman et al., 2011).

D1R activity in the HPC is necessary for long-term memory, synaptic plasticity, and network dynamics (Lisman et al., 2011; Yamasaki and Takeuchi, 2017; Kaufman et al., 2020; Bethus et al., 2010). Consistently, our results show that high doses of the D1/5R antagonist SCH-23390 impair memory performance in the NOR task. In addition, our data suggest that D1R/CB1R-positive hippocampal interneurons are one of the targets of dopaminergic control of learning and memory processes. Interestingly, it has been shown that parvalbumin (PV)-expressing interneurons require D1R activity for late phases of memory consolidation through coordinated control of the activity of hippocampal pyramidal neurons (Karunakaran et al., 2016). Particularly, the authors describe that this D1R activity modulates hippocampal network oscillations (i.e., sharp-wave ripples), which is a proposed correlate for synaptic plasticity and memory consolidation (Buzsáki, 2015). In addition, previous studies have shown that PV/CB1R-negative and CCK/CB1R-positive interneurons have complementary roles in ensuring such high oscillatory ripple events with consequent capacity to modulate synaptic plasticity (Klausberger et al., 2005; Buzsáki, 2015). Therefore, we speculate that the subpopulation of D1R/CB1R-positive interneurons described in our work could play a complementary role in maintaining a proper excitation/inhibition balance in the hippocampal network activity required for memory consolidation.

Although complete elucidation of the complex microcircuitry requires further characterization, our findings support the hypothesis that D1R/CB1R-positive hippocampal interneurons belong to a broader circuit participating in dopaminergic control of memory (Yamasaki and Takeuchi, 2017). Our data are compatible with a scenario where D1R activation during the learning/consolidation process potentiates GABAergic transmission. However, this D1R-dependent increase in inhibition is kept within adequate limits by activation of CB1Rs, allowing proper flow of information. In this sense, in the absence of CB1R-dependent control of D1R/CB1R-positive interneurons (i.e., D1-CB1-KO mice), partial inhibition of D1-like or GABAA receptors rescues the phenotype. In other words, although activation of D1Rs in interneurons seems to be necessary for the memory process, their abnormally high activity (e.g. in the absence of CB1Rs) impairs such functions. In this context, an interesting question relates to the functional link between endogenous activation of D1Rs and CB1Rs. Our results allow speculation about two potential scenarios based on autocrine or paracrine modes of action of endocannabinoid signaling (Busquets-Garcia et al., 2017). (1) General D1R-dependent dopaminergic signaling in the HPC might activate pyramidal neurons (Roggenhofer et al., 2013; Shivarama Shetty et al., 2016) targeted by D1R/CB1R-positive interneurons. This depolarization of pyramidal neurons would, in turn, induce canonical endocannabinoid-dependent retrograde inhibition of GABAergic release (Castillo et al., 2012), moderating, among others, activation of D1R/CB1R-positive interneurons. (2) Following D1R activation and consequent interneuron depolarization (Anastasiades et al., 2019; Gorelova et al., 2002), endocannabinoids might be mobilized locally and act in an autocrine manner to decrease the membrane potential and thereby moderate the activity of the neuron (Bacci et al., 2004). These two possibilities are not mutually exclusive, and they might reflect the effect of the mechanisms described on general network activity and/or on specific plastic cellular processes, respectively. Future studies will investigate these intriguing scenarios using adapted experimental approaches.

Altogether, these data reveal that functionally distinct cell types are present in the general population of hippocampal GABAergic interneurons expressing CB1Rs. In particular, D1R/CB1R-positive interneurons provide specific behavioral and hippocampal synaptic mechanisms sustaining the fine-tuned regulation of memory processes. The close interaction of CB1Rs and D1Rs in modulating recognition memory might provide novel therapeutic frameworks for treatment of cognitive diseases characterized by alterations of endocannabinoid or dopaminergic systems or both.

STAR★Methods

Key Resources Table

REAGENT or RESOURCE SOURCE IDENTIFIER
Antibodies

Rabbit antibody against the C-myc epitope tag BioLegend Cat# 906301; RRID:AB_2565064
Goat anti-rabbit antibody Alexa Fluor 488 Fisher Scientific Cat# A-11008; RRID:AB_143165
4’,6-diamidino-2-phenylindole Fisher Scientific Cat# D3571; RRID:AB_2307445
Rabbit polyclonal antibody against DsRed Takara Bio Cat# 632496; RRID:AB_10013483
Secondary antibody goat anti-rabbit conjugated to a horseradish peroxidase Cell signaling Cat#7074S; RRID:AB_2099233
Digoxigenin (DIG)-labeled riboprobe against mouse CB1 Marsicano and Lutz, 1999 N/A
Anti-DIG antibody conjugated to HRP Roche Cat#11207733910; RRID:AB_514500

Bacterial and Virus Strains

rAAV-CAG-DIO Lead contact lab rAAV-30
AAV-CAG-DIO-CB1 Lead contact lab rAAV-37
AAV-CAG-DIO-CB1-myc Lead contact lab rAAV-21
hSyn-DIO-hM4D(Gi)-mCherry Addgene 44361-AAV8
pAAV-hSyn-DIO-mCherry Addgene 50459-AAV8

Chemicals, Peptides, and Recombinant Proteins

2-methylbutane Sigma-Aldrich M32631-1L
TSA plus fluorescein system Perkin Elmer NEL741001KT
Streptavidin-Texas Red Perkin Elmer NEL721001EA
Normal donkey serum Merck S30-100ML
Sheep Serum Sigma Aldrich S3772-10ML
Formaldehyde 4% Sigma Aldrich HT501128-4L
Blocking reagent (to prepare NEN) Perkin Elmer FP1012
TSA Biotin Systems Perkin Elmer NEL700A001KT
SSC 20X Sigma 93017-10L-F
Fluoromount-GSlide Mounting Medium Electron microscopy sciences 17984-25
Isoflurane Virbac Vnr137317
Bicuculine Sigma-Aldrich 14343-50MG
SCH 23390 Sigma-Aldrich D054-10MG
MK-801 Sigma-Aldrich 77086-22-7
clozapine-N-oxide CNO Tocris 4936

Critical Commercial Assays

Avidin/Biotin Blocking Kit Vector Labs SP-2001

Experimental Models: Organisms/Strains

D1-CB1-WT and D1-CB1-KO Lead contact lab N/A
C57BL/6N Janvier Labs C57BL/6NRj
D1-Cre Lead contact lab Tg(Drd1a-cre)AGsc/KndlJ,
CB1flox/flox Lead contact lab Cnr1tm1.2Ltz, MGI:3045419
GFAP-CB1-WT and GFAP-CB1-KO Lead contact lab N/A

Software and Algorithms

Prism Graphpad Software V6.0
CED 1401 Spike2 Cambridge Electronic Design V6.18
ImageJ NIH V1.52

Resource Availability

Lead Contact

Further information and requests for resources and reagents should be directed to and will be fulfilled by the Lead Contact, Giovanni Marsicano (giovanni.marsicano@inserm.fr).

Materials Availability

Mouse lines generated and used in the current study are available from the lead contact upon request. We are glad to share the mouse lines with reasonable compensation by requestor for its processing and shipping.

Data and Code Availability

The data supporting the current study have not been deposited in a public repository but are available from the lead contact on request.

Experimental Model and Subject Details

Animal Model

All experimental procedures were approved by the ethical committee of the French Ministry of Higher Education, Research and Innovation (authorization APAFIS#18111). Maximal efforts were made to reduce the suffering of the animals. Male mice were used in this study.

D1-CB1-KO mice were generated as previously described (Monory et al., 2007; Terzian et al., 2011). Briefly, CB1 floxed mice (Marsicano et al., 2003) were crossed with D1-Cre line (Lemberger et al., 2007), in which the Cre recombinase was placed under the control of the D1 gene (Drd1a) regulatory sequences using transgenesis with modified bacterial artificial chromosomes. The pattern of Cre expression recapitulated the expression pattern of the endogenous Drd1a (Lemberger et al., 2007). Breeding was performed by mating male Cre-positive D1-CB1-KO mice with homozygous CB1-flox female mice deriving from a separate colony. In order to detect possible germline or ectopic recombination events, genotyping of tail samples from pups (PD10) was performed by genomic PCR using primers suited to identify WT, “floxed” and “recombined” bands. No germline or ectopic recombination was detected. Eight to 14 weeks-old naive male D1-CB1-KO and WT littermates were used. 8-14 weeks old male C57BL/6NRj mice purchased from Janvier (France). 8-12 weeks-old D1-Cre mice breed in the animal facilities of the U1215 we also used. Animals were housed collectively under standard conditions of temperature and humidity in a day/night cycle of 12/12 hours (light on at 7 am). Animals that underwent surgery were kept in individual cages after the procedures to avoid conflict with their littermates. Food and water were provided ad libitum. All the experiments were performed during the light phase. Behavioral experiments were performed from 9 am to 3 pm. Electrophysiology experiments were performed from 8 am to 7 pm.

Method Details

Drug preparation and administration

Bicuculline, MK-801, NBQX and SCH-23390 were purchased from Merck (formerly Sigma-Aldrich, France) and were dissolved to their final concentration in physiological saline (NaCl 0.9%). The exogenous DREADD ligand clozapine-N-oxide (CNO, 2 mg/kg) was purchased from Tocris Bioscience (Bristol, UK) and dissolved in saline after gently mixing with a vortex. All drugs were injected intraperitoneally in a volume of 10 ml/kg. Vehicle in all the conditions was composed of physiological saline (NaCl 0.9%) injections.

Novel object recognition memory

We used the novel object recognition (NOR) memory task in an L-maze (Busquets-Garcia et al., 2011, 2013; Hebert-Chatelain et al., 2016; Puighermanal et al., 2009, 2013; Robin et al., 2018).

The task took place in a L-shaped maze made of dark gray polyvinyl chloride made by two identical perpendicular arms (35 cm and 30 cm long respectively for external and internal L walls, 4.5cm wide and 15 cm high walls) placed on a white background (Busquets-Garcia et al., 2011; Puighermanal et al., 2009). The task occurred in a room adjacent to the animal house with a light intensity fixed at 50 lux. The maze was overhung by a video camera allowing the detection and offline scoring of animal’s behavior. The task consisted in 3 sequential daily trials of 9 minutes each. During the habituation phase (day 1), mice were placed in the center of the maze and allowed to freely explore the arms in the absence of any objects. The training phase (day 2) consisted in placing the mice again in the corner of the maze in the presence of two identical objects positioned at the extremities of each arm and left to freely explore the maze and the objects. The testing phase occurred 24 hours later (day 3): one of the familiar objects was replaced by a novel object different in its shape, color and texture and mice were left to explore both objects. The position of the novel object and the associations of novel and familiar were randomized. All objects were previously tested to avoid biased preference. Memory performance was assessed by the discrimination index (DI). The DI was calculated as the difference between the time spent exploring the novel (TN) and the familiar object (TF) divided by the total exploration time (TN+TF): DI = [TN-TF]/[TN+TF]. Memory was also evaluated by directly comparing the exploration time of novel and familiar objects, respectively. Object exploration was defined as the orientation of the nose to the object at less than 2 cm. Experienced investigators evaluating the exploration were blind of treatment and/or genotype of the animals. Pharmacological treatments were immediately administered after the training phase.

In vivo electrophysiology in anesthetized mice

Experiments were performed as described in Robin et al. (2018). Mice were anesthetized in a box containing 5% Isoflurane (Virbac, France) before being placed in a stereotaxic frame (Model 900, Kopf instruments, CA, USA) in which 1.0% to 1.5% of Isoflurane was continuously supplied via an anesthetic mask during the whole duration of the experiment. The body temperature was maintained at ± 36.5°C using a homeothermic system (model 50-7087-F, Harvard Apparatus, MA, USA) and the state of anesthesia was assessed by mild tail pinch. Before surgery, 100 mL of the local anesthetic lurocaine (vetoquinol, France) was injected in the scalp region. Surgical procedure started with a longitudinal incision of 1.5 cm in length aimed to expose Bregma and Lambda. After ensuring the correct alignment of the head, two holes were drilled in the skull for electrode placement. Glass recording electrodes were inserted in the CA1 stratum radiatum, and a concentric stimulating bipolar electrode (Model CBARC50, FHC, ME, USA) placed in the CA3 region. Coordinates were as follows: CA1 stratum radiatum: A/P 1.5, M/L 1.0, DV 1.20; CA3: A/P 2.2, M/L 2.8, D/V 1.3 (20 insertion angle). The recording electrode (tip diameter = 1–2 mm, 2-4 MΩ) was filled with a 2% pontamine sky blue solution in 0.5M sodium acetate. At first the recording electrode was placed by hand until it reached the surface of the brain and then to the final depth using a hydraulic micropositioner (Model 2650, KOPF instruments, CA, USA). The stimulation electrode was placed in the correct area using a standard manipulator. Both electrodes were adjusted to find the area with maximum response. In vivo recordings of evoked field excitatory postsynaptic potentials (fEPSPs) were amplified 1000 times and filtered (low-pass at 1Hz and high-pass 3000Hz) by a DAGAN 2400A amplifier (DAGAN Corporation, MN, USA). fEPSPs were digitized and collected on-line using a laboratory interface and software (CED 1401, SPIKE 2; Cambridge Electronic Design, Cambridge, UK). Test pulses were generated through an Isolated Constant Current Stimulator (DS3, Digitimer, Hertfordshire, UK) triggered by the SPIKE 2 output sequencer via CED 1401 and collected every 2 s at a 10 kHz sampling frequency and then averaged every 180 s. Test pulse intensities were typically between 40-250 μA with a duration of 50 ms. Basal stimulation intensity was adjusted to 30%–50% of the current intensity that evoked a maximum field response. All responses were expressed as percent from the average responses recorded during the 15 min before high frequency stimulation (HFS). HFS was induced by applying 3 trains of 100 Hz (1 s each), separated by 20 s interval. fEPSP were then recorded for a period of 60 min. C57BL6/NRj mice underwent this in vivo electrophysiology procedure after the training phase of NOR task. Also, where specified, D1-CB1-KO and D1-CB1-WT received an injection of Bicuculine (0.5 mg/kg, intraperitoneal) or SCH 23390 (0.3 mg/kg, intraperitoneal) or vehicle immediately after undergoing training in NORT and before being subjected to the in vivo electrophysiology procedure. At the end the experiment, the position of the electrodes was marked (recording area: iontophoretic infusion of the recording solution during 180 s at 20mA; stimulation area: continuous current discharge over 20 s at +20mA) and histological verification was performed ex vivo.

Surgery and viral administration

Mice were anesthetized in a box containing 5% Isoflurane (Virbac, France) before being placed in a stereotaxic frame (Model 900, Kopf instruments, CA, USA) in which 1.0% to 1.5% of Isoflurane was continuously supplied via an anesthetic mask during the whole duration of the experiment. For viral intra-HPC AAV delivery, mice were submitted to stereotaxic surgery (as above) and AAV vectors were injected with the help of a microsyringe (0.25 mL Hamilton syringe with a 30-gauge beveled needle) attached to a pump (UMP3-1, World Precision Instruments, FL, USA). Where specified, D1-CB1-WT and D1-CB1-KO mice were injected directly into the hippocampus (HPC) or striatum (STR) (0.5 μl per injection site at a rate of 0.5 μl per min), with the following coordinates: HPC, AP −1.8; ML ± 1; DV −2.0 and −1.5; Striatum: AP −1.34; ML ± 2.8; DV −1.84. Following virus delivery, the syringe was left in place for 1 minute before being slowly withdrawn from the brain. CB1 floxed mice were injected with rAAV-CAG-DIO (empty control vector), AAV-CAG-DIO-CB1 or AAV-CAG-DIO-CB1-myc to induce re-expression of the CB1 receptor gene in hippocampal or striatal D1-positive cells. To generate the aforementioned rAAVs, mouse CB1 receptor coding sequence (either native or fused to myc-tag at the C term) was cloned in rAAV-CAG-DIO vector using standard molecular cloning technology. The coding sequence was cloned inverted in orientation to allow Cre-dependent expression of CB1 receptors (Atasoy et al., 2008). In another experiment, and using the same procedure as described as above, D1-CB1-WT and D1-CB1-KO mice were injected intra hippocampally (AP −1.8; ML ± 1; DV −2.0 and −1.5), with pAAV-hSyn-DIO-hM4D(Gi)-mCherry or pAAV-hSyn-DIO-mCherry (addgene, USA). For anatomical experiments and using the same procedure as above, D1-Cre and D1-CB1-KO were injected intra hippocampally with pAAV-hSyn-DIO-mCherry. In this specific experiment, expression was allowed to take place for 2 weeks. For the remaining experiments, animals were used around 4-5 weeks after local infusions. Mice were weighed daily and individuals that failed to regain the pre-surgery body weight were excluded from the following experiments.

Immunohistochemistry on free-floating sections

Mice were anesthetized with pentobarbital (Exagon, Axience SAS, 400 mg/kg body weight), transcardially perfused with phosphate-buffered solution (PBS 0.1M, pH 7.4) before being fixed with 4% formaldehyde (Sigma-Aldrich). The The brains were extracted and incubated overnight at 4°C in the same fixative, then embedded with sucrose 30% for 3 days and finally frozen in 2-methylbutane (Sigma-Aldrich) at −80°C. Free-floating frozen coronal sections (40 μm) were cut out with a cryostat (Microm HM 500M Microm Microtech), collected collected in an antifreeze solution and conserved at −20°C. Sections were permeabilized in a blocking solution (in PBS: 10% donkey serum, 0.3% Triton X-100) for 1 hour at room temperature (RT). Then, sections were incubated with a rabbit primary antibody against the C-myc epitope tag (1:1000, BioLegend) overnight at 4°C. After several washes with PBS, slices were incubated for 2 hours with a secondary antibody goat anti-rabbit Alexa Fluor 488 (1:500, Fisher Scientific) and then washed in PBS at RT. Finally, sections were incubated with 4’,6-diamidino-2-phenylindole (DAPI 1:20000, Fisher Scientific) diluted in PBS for 5 minutes to visualize cell nuclei and then were washed, mounted and coverslipped. All the antibodies were diluted in blocking solution. The sections were imagedimaged with a slides scanner Hamamatsu Nanozoomer 2.0 HT.

Combined Fluorescent in situ hybridization (FISH)/ Immunohistochemistry (IHC) on free-floating frozen sections

Mice were anesthetized with pentobarbital (Exagon, Axience SAS, 400 mg/kg body weight), transcardially perfused with PBS (0.1M, pH 7.4) before being fixed with 4% formaldehyde (Sigma-Aldrich). The brains were extracted and incubated overnight at 4°C in the same fixative, then embedded with sucrose 30% for 3 days and finally frozen in 2-methylbutane (Sigma-Aldrich) at −80°C. Free-floating frozen coronal sections were cut out with a cryostat (30 μm, Microm HM 500M Microm Microtech) and collected in an antifreeze solution and conserved at −20°C.

Section were washed several times with PBS with diethyl pyrocarbonate (PBS-DEPC) to wash out the antifreeze solution. The endogenous peroxidases were inactivated by incubating the free-floating sections with 3% H2O2 in PBS-DEPC for 30 minutes. All endogenous biotin, biotin receptors, and avidin binding sites present in the tissue were blocked by using the Avidin/Biotin Blocking Kit (Vector Labs, USA). Then, the slices were incubated overnight at RT with a rabbit polyclonal primary antibody against DsRed (1:1000, Takara Bio) diluted in a blocking solution (0.3% Triton X-100 diluted in PBS-DEPC). The following day, after several washes, the sections were incubated with a secondary antibody goat anti-rabbit conjugated to a horseradish peroxidase (HRP) (1:500, Cell Signaling Technology) during 2 hours at RT followed by TSA Biotin System (Biotin TSA 1:100, PerkinElmer) for 10 minutes at RT. After several washes, the slices were fixed with 4% of formaldehyde (Sigma Aldrich) for 10 minutes and blocked with 0.2M HCl for 20 minutes at RT. Then, the section were acetylated in 0.1 M Triethanolamine, 0.25% Acetic Anhydride for 10 minutes. This step was performed to reduce non-specific probe binding. Sections were hybridized overnight at 60°C with Digoxigenin (DIG)-labeled riboprobe against mouse CB1 receptor (1:1000, prepared as described in Marsicano and Lutz, 1999). After hybridization, the slices were washed with different stringency wash buffers at 65°C. Then, the sections were incubated with 3% of H2O2 for 30 minutes at RT and blocked 1 hour with NEN blocking buffer prepared according to the manufacturer’s protocol (PerkinElmer). Anti-DIG antibody conjugated to HRP (1:2000, Roche) was applied for 2 hours at RT. The signal of CB1 receptor hybridization was revealed by a TSA reaction using fluorescein isothiocyanate (FITC)-labeled tyramide (1:80 for 12 minutes, Perkin Elmer). After several washes, the free-floating slices were incubated overnight at 4°C with Streptavidin-Texas Red (1:400, PerkinElmer). Finally, the slices were incubated with DAPI (1:20000; Fisher Scientific) diluted in PBS, following by several washes, to finally be mounted, coverslipped and imaged with an epifluorescence Leica DM 6000 microscope (Leica, Germany).

Quantification and Statistical Analysis

Data collection

No statistical methods were used to pre-determine sample sizes, but they are similar to those reported in previous publications. All data collection and/or analysis were performed blind to the conditions of the experimenter except for the in vivo electrophysiological experiments. All mice were assigned randomly to the different experimental conditions.

Fluorescence quantifications

Cells expressing mRNAs were quantified in the different layers (stratum oriens, stratum pyramidale, stratum radiatum and stratum lacunosum moleculare) of the dorsal hippocampus. CB1 receptor positive cells were classified according to the level of transcript visualized by the intensity of fluorescence (Marsicano and Lutz, 1999; Terral et al., 2019). ‘‘High-CB1’’ cells were considered to be round-shaped and intense staining covering the entire nucleus whereas ‘‘Low-CB1’’ cells were defined with discontinuous shape and lowest intensity of fluorescence allowing the discrimination of grains of staining.

Statistical analyses

Data were expressed as mean ± SEM or single data points and were analyzed with Prism 6.0 (Graphpad Software), using two-tails t test (paired, unpaired) or one-way ANOVA (Dunnett’s), two-way ANOVA (sidak’s). Sample sizes and p values can be found in figure legends and Table S1.

Acknowledgments

We thank Delphine Gonzales, Nathalie Aubailly, and the personnel of the Animal Facilities of the NeuroCentre Magendie for mouse care. We thank the Biochemistry Platform of Bordeaux NeuroCampus and the members of Marsicano’s lab for useful discussions. This work was funded by INSERM; the European Research Council (Endofood ERC-2010-StG-260515, CannaPreg ERC-2014-PoC-640923, and MiCaBra ERC-2017-AdG-786467 to G.M.); Fondation pour la Recherche Medicale (FRM; DRM20101220445 to G.M. and DT20160435664 to J.F.O.d.C.); the Human Frontiers Science Program, Region Aquitaine, Agence Nationale de la Recherche (ANR; NeuroNutriSens ANR-13-BSV4-0006, ORUPS ANR-16-CE37-0010-01, and CaCoVi ANR-18-CE16-0001-02 to G.M.); BRAIN ANR-10-LABX-0043 (to G.M.); NIH/NIDA (1R21DA037678-01); the European Regional Development Fund; the European Union Horizon 2020 Research and Innovation Program (grant agreement 686009); French State/Agence Nationale de la Recherche/IdEx (ANR-10-IDEX-03-02), Eu-Fp7 (FP7-PEOPLE-2013-IEF-623638), and MINECO/AEI (RYC-2017-21776) (to A.B.-G.); FRM (ARF20140129235 to L.B.); and Ikerbasque (The Basque Foundation for Science) and MINECO (Ministerio de Economía y Competitividad) PGC2018-093990-A-I00 (MICIU/AEI/FEDER, UE) (to E.S.-G.).

Author Contributions

J.F.O.d.C., A.B.-G., G.M., and E.S.-G. conceived and supervised the whole project. J.F.O.d.C. performed and analyzed in vivo electrophysiology and behavioral experiments. A.B.-G. and E.S.-G. performed and analyzed behavioral experiments. L.B. and A.C. contributed to experiments using viral vectors. L.R. and G.L. contributed to behavioral experiments. M.V., F.J.-K., T.L.-L., and M.M. performed cytochemical experiments. Z.Z. and M.V. helped with analysis of the data. J.F.O.d.C., A.B.-G., G.M., and E.S.-G. wrote the manuscript. F.D. contributed to writing. All authors edited and approved the manuscript.

Declaration of Interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Published: August 18, 2020

Footnotes

Supplemental Information can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2020.108046.

Contributor Information

Giovanni Marsicano, Email: giovanni.marsicano@inserm.fr.

Edgar Soria-Gómez, Email: edgarjesus.soria@ehu.eus.

Supplemental Information

Document S1. Figures S1–S3 and Table S1
mmc1.pdf (16MB, pdf)
Document S2. Article plus Supplemental Information
mmc2.pdf (19MB, pdf)

References

  1. Aggleton J.P., Morris R.G.M. Memory: Looking back and looking forward. Brain Neurosci. Adv. 2018;2 doi: 10.1177/2398212818794830. 2398212818794830. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Anastasiades P.G., Boada C., Carter A.G. Cell-Type-Specific D1 Dopamine Receptor Modulation of Projection Neurons and Interneurons in the Prefrontal Cortex. Cereb. Cortex. 2019;29:3224–3242. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy299. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Atasoy D., Aponte Y., Su H.H., Sternson S.M. A FLEX switch targets Channelrhodopsin-2 to multiple cell types for imaging and long-range circuit mapping. J. Neurosci. 2008;28:7025–7030. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1954-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bacci A., Huguenard J.R., Prince D.A. Long-lasting self-inhibition of neocortical interneurons mediated by endocannabinoids. Nature. 2004;431:312–316. doi: 10.1038/nature02913. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bethus I., Tse D., Morris R.G. Dopamine and memory: modulation of the persistence of memory for novel hippocampal NMDA receptor-dependent paired associates. J. Neurosci. 2010;30:1610–1618. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2721-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Busquets-Garcia A., Puighermanal E., Pastor A., de la Torre R., Maldonado R., Ozaita A. Differential role of anandamide and 2-arachidonoylglycerol in memory and anxiety-like responses. Biol. Psychiatry. 2011;70:479–486. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.04.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Busquets-Garcia A., Gomis-González M., Guegan T., Agustín-Pavón C., Pastor A., Mato S., Pérez-Samartín A., Matute C., de la Torre R., Dierssen M. Targeting the endocannabinoid system in the treatment of fragile X syndrome. Nat. Med. 2013;19:603–607. doi: 10.1038/nm.3127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Busquets-Garcia A., Desprez T., Metna-Laurent M., Bellocchio L., Marsicano G., Soria-Gomez E. Dissecting the cannabinergic control of behavior: The where matters. BioEssays. 2015;37:1215–1225. doi: 10.1002/bies.201500046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Busquets Garcia A., Soria-Gomez E., Bellocchio L., Marsicano G. Cannabinoid receptor type-1: breaking the dogmas. F1000Res. 2016;5:5. doi: 10.12688/f1000research.8245.1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Busquets-Garcia A., Bains J., Marsicano G. CB1 Receptors Signaling in the Brain: Extracting Specificity from Ubiquity. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2017;43:4–20. doi: 10.1038/npp.2017.206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Buzsáki G. Hippocampal sharp wave-ripple: A cognitive biomarker for episodic memory and planning. Hippocampus. 2015;25:1073–1188. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22488. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Castillo P.E., Younts T.J., Chávez A.E., Hashimotodani Y. Endocannabinoid signaling and synaptic function. Neuron. 2012;76:70–81. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Chai H., Diaz-Castro B., Shigetomi E., Monte E., Octeau J.C., Yu X., Cohn W., Rajendran P.S., Vondriska T.M., Whitelegge J.P. Neural Circuit-Specialized Astrocytes: Transcriptomic, Proteomic, Morphological, and Functional Evidence. Neuron. 2017;95:531–549.e9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.06.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Darvas M., Palmiter R.D. Restriction of dopamine signaling to the dorsolateral striatum is sufficient for many cognitive behaviors. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2009;106:14664–14669. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0907299106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. El-Brolosy M.A., Stainier D.Y.R. Genetic compensation: A phenomenon in search of mechanisms. PLoS Genet. 2017;13:e1006780. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1006780. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. El-Brolosy M.A., Kontarakis Z., Rossi A., Kuenne C., Günther S., Fukuda N., Kikhi K., Boezio G.L.M., Takacs C.M., Lai S.L. Genetic compensation triggered by mutant mRNA degradation. Nature. 2019;568:193–197. doi: 10.1038/s41586-019-1064-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Frey U., Schroeder H., Matthies H. Dopaminergic antagonists prevent long-term maintenance of posttetanic LTP in the CA1 region of rat hippocampal slices. Brain Res. 1990;522:69–75. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(90)91578-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Gangarossa G., Longueville S., De Bundel D., Perroy J., Hervé D., Girault J.A., Valjent E. Characterization of dopamine D1 and D2 receptor-expressing neurons in the mouse hippocampus. Hippocampus. 2012;22:2199–2207. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Gomez J.L., Bonaventura J., Lesniak W., Mathews W.B., Sysa-Shah P., Rodriguez L.A., Ellis R.J., Richie C.T., Harvey B.K., Dannals R.F. Chemogenetics revealed: DREADD occupancy and activation via converted clozapine. Science. 2017;357:503–507. doi: 10.1126/science.aan2475. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Gorelova N., Seamans J.K., Yang C.R. Mechanisms of dopamine activation of fast-spiking interneurons that exert inhibition in rat prefrontal cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 2002;88:3150–3166. doi: 10.1152/jn.00335.2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Granado N., Ortiz O., Suárez L.M., Martín E.D., Ceña V., Solís J.M., Moratalla R. D1 but not D5 dopamine receptors are critical for LTP, spatial learning, and LTP-Induced arc and zif268 expression in the hippocampus. Cereb. Cortex. 2008;18:1–12. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Harris K.D., Hochgerner H., Skene N.G., Magno L., Katona L., Bengtsson Gonzales C., Somogyi P., Kessaris N., Linnarsson S., Hjerling-Leffler J. Classes and continua of hippocampal CA1 inhibitory neurons revealed by single-cell transcriptomics. PLoS Biol. 2018;16:e2006387. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.2006387. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Hebert-Chatelain E., Desprez T., Serrat R., Bellocchio L., Soria-Gomez E., Busquets-Garcia A., Pagano Zottola A.C., Delamarre A., Cannich A., Vincent P. A cannabinoid link between mitochondria and memory. Nature. 2016;539:555–559. doi: 10.1038/nature20127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Jennings A., Tyurikova O., Bard L., Zheng K., Semyanov A., Henneberger C., Rusakov D.A. Dopamine elevates and lowers astroglial Ca2+ through distinct pathways depending on local synaptic circuitry. Glia. 2017;65:447–459. doi: 10.1002/glia.23103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Karunakaran S., Chowdhury A., Donato F., Quairiaux C., Michel C.M., Caroni P. PV plasticity sustained through D1/5 dopamine signaling required for long-term memory consolidation. Nat. Neurosci. 2016;19:454–464. doi: 10.1038/nn.4231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Katona I., Freund T.F. Multiple functions of endocannabinoid signaling in the brain. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2012;35:529–558. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150420. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Katona I., Sperlágh B., Sík A., Käfalvi A., Vizi E.S., Mackie K., Freund T.F. Presynaptically located CB1 cannabinoid receptors regulate GABA release from axon terminals of specific hippocampal interneurons. J. Neurosci. 1999;19:4544–4558. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-11-04544.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Kaufman A.M., Geiller T., Losonczy A. A Role for the Locus Coeruleus in Hippocampal CA1 Place Cell Reorganization during Spatial Reward Learning. Neuron. 2020;105:1018–1026.e4. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.12.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Klausberger T., Somogyi P. Neuronal diversity and temporal dynamics: the unity of hippocampal circuit operations. Science. 2008;321:53–57. doi: 10.1126/science.1149381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Klausberger T., Marton L.F., O’Neill J., Huck J.H., Dalezios Y., Fuentealba P., Suen W.Y., Papp E., Kaneko T., Watanabe M. Complementary roles of cholecystokinin- and parvalbumin-expressing GABAergic neurons in hippocampal network oscillations. J. Neurosci. 2005;25:9782–9793. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3269-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Lemberger T., Parlato R., Dassesse D., Westphal M., Casanova E., Turiault M., Tronche F., Schiffmann S.N., Schütz G. Expression of Cre recombinase in dopaminoceptive neurons. BMC Neurosci. 2007;8:4. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-8-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Lemon N., Manahan-Vaughan D. Dopamine D1/D5 receptors gate the acquisition of novel information through hippocampal long-term potentiation and long-term depression. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:7723–7729. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1454-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Li S., Cullen W.K., Anwyl R., Rowan M.J. Dopamine-dependent facilitation of LTP induction in hippocampal CA1 by exposure to spatial novelty. Nat. Neurosci. 2003;6:526–531. doi: 10.1038/nn1049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Lisman J. Criteria for identifying the molecular basis of the engram (CaMKII, PKMzeta) Mol. Brain. 2017;10:55. doi: 10.1186/s13041-017-0337-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Lisman J., Grace A.A., Duzel E. A neoHebbian framework for episodic memory; role of dopamine-dependent late LTP. Trends Neurosci. 2011;34:536–547. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2011.07.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Marsicano G., Kuner R. Anatomical distribution of receptors, ligands and enzymes in the brain and the spinal cord: circuitries and neurochemistry. In: Kofalvi A., editor. Cannabinoids and the brain. Springer; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  37. Marsicano G., Lutz B. Expression of the cannabinoid receptor CB1 in distinct neuronal subpopulations in the adult mouse forebrain. Eur. J. Neurosci. 1999;11:4213–4225. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00847.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Marsicano G., Goodenough S., Monory K., Hermann H., Eder M., Cannich A., Azad S.C., Cascio M.G., Gutiérrez S.O., van der Stelt M. CB1 cannabinoid receptors and on-demand defense against excitotoxicity. Science. 2003;302:84–88. doi: 10.1126/science.1088208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Monory K., Blaudzun H., Massa F., Kaiser N., Lemberger T., Schütz G., Wotjak C.T., Lutz B., Marsicano G. Genetic dissection of behavioural and autonomic effects of Delta(9)-tetrahydrocannabinol in mice. PLoS Biol. 2007;5:e269. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0050269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Morris R.G. NMDA receptors and memory encoding. Neuropharmacology. 2013;74:32–40. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.04.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Nagatomo K., Suga S., Saitoh M., Kogawa M., Kobayashi K., Yamamoto Y., Yamada K. Dopamine D1 Receptor Immunoreactivity on Fine Processes of GFAP-Positive Astrocytes in the Substantia Nigra Pars Reticulata of Adult Mouse. Front. Neuroanat. 2017;11:3. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2017.00003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Nicoll R.A. A Brief History of Long-Term Potentiation. Neuron. 2017;93:281–290. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.12.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Oliveira da Cruz J.F., Robin L.M., Drago F., Marsicano G., Metna-Laurent M. Astroglial type-1 cannabinoid receptor (CB1): A new player in the tripartite synapse. Neuroscience. 2016;323:35–42. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.05.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Parra P., Gulyás A.I., Miles R. How many subtypes of inhibitory cells in the hippocampus? Neuron. 1998;20:983–993. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80479-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Pelkey K.A., Chittajallu R., Craig M.T., Tricoire L., Wester J.C., McBain C.J. Hippocampal GABAergic Inhibitory Interneurons. Physiol. Rev. 2017;97:1619–1747. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00007.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Puighermanal E., Marsicano G., Busquets-Garcia A., Lutz B., Maldonado R., Ozaita A. Cannabinoid modulation of hippocampal long-term memory is mediated by mTOR signaling. Nat. Neurosci. 2009;12:1152–1158. doi: 10.1038/nn.2369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Puighermanal E., Busquets-Garcia A., Gomis-González M., Marsicano G., Maldonado R., Ozaita A. Dissociation of the pharmacological effects of THC by mTOR blockade. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2013;38:1334–1343. doi: 10.1038/npp.2013.31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Puighermanal E., Cutando L., Boubaker-Vitre J., Honoré E., Longueville S., Hervé D., Valjent E. Anatomical and molecular characterization of dopamine D1 receptor-expressing neurons of the mouse CA1 dorsal hippocampus. Brain Struct. Funct. 2017;222:1897–1911. doi: 10.1007/s00429-016-1314-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Robin L.M., Oliveira da Cruz J.F., Langlais V.C., Martin-Fernandez M., Metna-Laurent M., Busquets-Garcia A., Bellocchio L., Soria-Gomez E., Papouin T., Varilh M. Astroglial CB1 Receptors Determine Synaptic D-Serine Availability to Enable Recognition Memory. Neuron. 2018;98:935–944.e5. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.04.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Robinson S., Adelman J.S. A Method for Remotely Silencing Neural Activity in Rodents During Discrete Phases of Learning. J. Vis. Exp. 2015;(100):e52859. doi: 10.3791/52859. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Roggenhofer E., Fidzinski P., Shor O., Behr J. Reduced threshold for induction of LTP by activation of dopamine D1/D5 receptors at hippocampal CA1-subiculum synapses. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e62520. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0062520. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Shivarama Shetty M., Gopinadhan S., Sajikumar S. Dopamine D1/D5 receptor signaling regulates synaptic cooperation and competition in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons via sustained ERK1/2 activation. Hippocampus. 2016;26:137–150. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22497. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Soltesz I., Losonczy A. CA1 pyramidal cell diversity enabling parallel information processing in the hippocampus. Nat. Neurosci. 2018;21:484–493. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0118-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Soria-Gomez E., Metna M., Bellocchio L., Busquets-Garcia A., Marsicano G. The endocannabinoid system in the control of behavior. In: Tucci V., editor. Handbook of Neurobehavioral Genetics and Phenotyping. John Wiley & Sons; 2017. pp. 323–355. [Google Scholar]
  55. Squire L.R., Wixted J.T., Clark R.E. Recognition memory and the medial temporal lobe: a new perspective. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2007;8:872–883. doi: 10.1038/nrn2154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Takeuchi T., Duszkiewicz A.J., Sonneborn A., Spooner P.A., Yamasaki M., Watanabe M., Smith C.C., Fernández G., Deisseroth K., Greene R.W., Morris R.G. Locus coeruleus and dopaminergic consolidation of everyday memory. Nature. 2016;537:357–362. doi: 10.1038/nature19325. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Terral G., Busquets-García A., Varilh M., Achicallende S., Cannich A., Bellocchio L., Bonilla-Del Río I., Massa F., Puente N., Soria-Gómez E. CB1 receptors in the anterior piriform cortex control odor preference memory. Curr. Biol. 2019;29:2455–2464.e5. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.06.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Terzian A.L., Drago F., Wotjak C.T., Micale V. The Dopamine and Cannabinoid Interaction in the Modulation of Emotions and Cognition: Assessing the Role of Cannabinoid CB1 Receptor in Neurons Expressing Dopamine D1 Receptors. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2011;5:49. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2011.00049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Whitlock J.R., Heynen A.J., Shuler M.G., Bear M.F. Learning induces long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. Science. 2006;313:1093–1097. doi: 10.1126/science.1128134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Yamasaki M., Takeuchi T. Locus Coeruleus and Dopamine-Dependent Memory Consolidation. Neural Plast. 2017;2017:8602690. doi: 10.1155/2017/8602690. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Zhang Y., Chen K., Sloan S.A., Bennett M.L., Scholze A.R., O’Keeffe S., Phatnani H.P., Guarnieri P., Caneda C., Ruderisch N. An RNA-sequencing transcriptome and splicing database of glia, neurons, and vascular cells of the cerebral cortex. J. Neurosci. 2014;34:11929–11947. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1860-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Document S1. Figures S1–S3 and Table S1
mmc1.pdf (16MB, pdf)
Document S2. Article plus Supplemental Information
mmc2.pdf (19MB, pdf)

Data Availability Statement

The data supporting the current study have not been deposited in a public repository but are available from the lead contact on request.

RESOURCES